Predictability modulates motor–auditory interactions in self-triggered audio–visual apparent...

12
RESEARCH ARTICLE Predictability modulates motor–auditory interactions in self-triggered audio–visual apparent motion Mikhail Zvyagintsev Andrey R. Nikolaev Krystyna A. Mathiak Hans Menning Ingo Hertrich Klaus Mathiak Received: 9 September 2007 / Accepted: 6 May 2008 / Published online: 26 May 2008 Ó Springer-Verlag 2008 Abstract We studied an effect of predictability in an audio–visual apparent motion task using magnetoenceph- alography. The synchronous sequences of audio–visual stimuli were self-triggered by subjects. The task was to detect the direction of the apparent motion in experimental blocks in which the motion either started from the side selected by subjects (predictable condition) or was random (unpredictable condition). Magnetic fields yielded three patterns of activity in the motor, auditory, and visual areas. Comparison of the dipole strength between predictable and unpredictable conditions revealed a significant difference of the preparatory motor activity in the time interval from -450 to -100 ms before self-triggering the stimulus. Perception of the audio–visual apparent motion was also modulated by predictability. However, the modulation was found only for the auditory activity but not for the visual one. The effect of predictability was selective and modu- lated only the auditory component N1 (100 ms after stimulus), which reflects initial evaluation of stimulus meaning. Importantly, the preparatory motor activity cor- relates with the following auditory activity mainly in the same hemisphere. Similar modulation by predictability of the motor and auditory activities suggests interactions between these two systems within an action–perception cycle. The mechanism of these interactions can be under- stood as an effect of anticipation of the own action outcomes on the preparatory motor and perceptual activity. Keywords Magnetoencephalography Predictability Apparent motion Self-triggering Motor Auditory Visual systems Introduction Our response to environmental changes highly depends on how well we can anticipate them. An unpredictable event may bring vitally important information and should get priority in processing compared to a predictable event. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1423-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. M. Zvyagintsev (&) K. A. Mathiak K. Mathiak Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen, Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany e-mail: [email protected] M. Zvyagintsev Institute of Neuroscience and Biophysics, Research Center Ju ¨lich GmbH, 52425 Ju ¨lich, Germany A. R. Nikolaev Laboratory for Perceptual Dynamics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako 351-0198, Japan A. R. Nikolaev Institute of Higher Nervous Activity, Butlerov str. 5a, 117485 Moscow, Russia K. A. Mathiak Department of Psychology, University of Warsaw, ul. Stawki 5/7, 00-183 Warsaw, Poland H. Menning Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Scheuchzerstr. 21, 8006 Zu ¨rich, Switzerland I. Hertrich Center for Neurology, University of Tu ¨bingen, Hoppe-Seyler-Str. 3, 72076 Tu ¨bingen, Germany K. Mathiak Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK 123 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 DOI 10.1007/s00221-008-1423-8

Transcript of Predictability modulates motor–auditory interactions in self-triggered audio–visual apparent...

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Predictability modulates motor–auditory interactionsin self-triggered audio–visual apparent motion

Mikhail Zvyagintsev Æ Andrey R. Nikolaev ÆKrystyna A. Mathiak Æ Hans Menning Æ Ingo Hertrich ÆKlaus Mathiak

Received: 9 September 2007 / Accepted: 6 May 2008 / Published online: 26 May 2008

� Springer-Verlag 2008

Abstract We studied an effect of predictability in an

audio–visual apparent motion task using magnetoenceph-

alography. The synchronous sequences of audio–visual

stimuli were self-triggered by subjects. The task was to

detect the direction of the apparent motion in experimental

blocks in which the motion either started from the side

selected by subjects (predictable condition) or was random

(unpredictable condition). Magnetic fields yielded three

patterns of activity in the motor, auditory, and visual areas.

Comparison of the dipole strength between predictable and

unpredictable conditions revealed a significant difference

of the preparatory motor activity in the time interval from

-450 to -100 ms before self-triggering the stimulus.

Perception of the audio–visual apparent motion was also

modulated by predictability. However, the modulation was

found only for the auditory activity but not for the visual

one. The effect of predictability was selective and modu-

lated only the auditory component N1 (100 ms after

stimulus), which reflects initial evaluation of stimulus

meaning. Importantly, the preparatory motor activity cor-

relates with the following auditory activity mainly in the

same hemisphere. Similar modulation by predictability of

the motor and auditory activities suggests interactions

between these two systems within an action–perception

cycle. The mechanism of these interactions can be under-

stood as an effect of anticipation of the own action

outcomes on the preparatory motor and perceptual activity.

Keywords Magnetoencephalography � Predictability �Apparent motion � Self-triggering � Motor � Auditory �Visual systems

Introduction

Our response to environmental changes highly depends on

how well we can anticipate them. An unpredictable event

may bring vitally important information and should get

priority in processing compared to a predictable event.

Electronic supplementary material The online version of thisarticle (doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1423-8) contains supplementarymaterial, which is available to authorized users.

M. Zvyagintsev (&) � K. A. Mathiak � K. Mathiak

Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, RWTH Aachen,

Pauwelsstr. 30, 52074 Aachen, Germany

e-mail: [email protected]

M. Zvyagintsev

Institute of Neuroscience and Biophysics, Research Center

Julich GmbH, 52425 Julich, Germany

A. R. Nikolaev

Laboratory for Perceptual Dynamics, RIKEN Brain Science

Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako 351-0198, Japan

A. R. Nikolaev

Institute of Higher Nervous Activity,

Butlerov str. 5a, 117485 Moscow, Russia

K. A. Mathiak

Department of Psychology, University of Warsaw,

ul. Stawki 5/7, 00-183 Warsaw, Poland

H. Menning

Department of Psychology, University of Zurich,

Scheuchzerstr. 21, 8006 Zurich, Switzerland

I. Hertrich

Center for Neurology, University of Tubingen,

Hoppe-Seyler-Str. 3, 72076 Tubingen, Germany

K. Mathiak

Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London,

De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, UK

123

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

DOI 10.1007/s00221-008-1423-8

Predictable and unpredictable events differ in their per-

ception, as well as in corresponding motor response. For

example, activity of the pre-motor and supplementary

motor areas reflects predictability of the externally trig-

gered movements (Dassonville et al. 1998; Berndt et al.

2002). Predictability of tracking tactile stimuli was found

to be reflected in the joint activation of the somatosensory

and frontal motor regions evidencing for the interactions

between sensory and motor systems (Nelson et al. 2004).

The common finding in these studies is an increased brain

activation associated with unpredictable stimulation in

comparison with the predictable one.

Unequal predictability may be a reason of difference

between perceptual consequences of externally and self-

produced actions. Both behavioral and electroencephalog-

raphy (EEG)/magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies

showed that the brain responds weaker to the self-produced

than to the external stimuli. For example, Blakemore et al.

(2000) found that tickling oneself is felt less intensively

compared to being tickled by somebody else. Schafer and

Marcus (1973) showed that cortical potentials evoked by

self-produced tones have significantly smaller amplitudes

than those produced externally. Neuromagnetic response to

one’s own speech was smaller than to the same recorded

utterances (Curio et al. 2000). Martikainen et al. (2005)

observed that the amplitude of the auditory N1 MEG

response was weaker when a tone was self-triggered than

when the same tone was listened to passively. Interestingly,

if the self-produced stimuli are rare enough, i.e. become

unpredicted, the brain response can increase overriding the

external stimulation: the amplitude of the ERP component

P3 evoked by the rare deviant target was higher during self-

triggered stimulation in comparison with the external one

(McCarthy and Donchin 1976; Nittono and Ullsperger

2000, Nittono 2006).

The factors of predictability and whether perceptual

events occur in consequence of self-generated actions can

be dissociated. In a PET study Blakemore et al. (1998)

observed an interaction between predictability and self-

produced action in cingulate cortex, insula, thalamus, and

right temporal cortex and concluded that the detection of

predictable events and detection of self-produced actions

are functionally distinct processes. However, this study

cannot show temporal relationship of interactions between

motor and perceptual systems because of low temporal

resolution of PET.

In the current work, we study time course of the pre-

dictability effect on the motor and perceptual activity

during self-produced stimulation using MEG. We manip-

ulated the probability of stimulus appearance from the side

of a button press which triggered a stimulus: the stimuli

might appear at this side with 100% (predictable condi-

tion) or with 50% (unpredictable condition) probability.

The first aim of the current study was to reveal how the

predictability affects the motor preparation to self-trig-

gering and subsequent perception of the self-triggered

stimuli.

Another goal of our study was to evaluate the effect of

predictability on integration of the auditory and visual

stimuli during audio–visual apparent motion. For this aim,

we used a combined audio–visual stimulation which led to

an apparent motion illusion. Previously it was found that

the direction of the visual motion in the bimodal audio–

visual stimulation can affect the direction of the auditory

motion, but not vice versa (Allen and Kolers 1981;

Mateeff et al. 1985; Ohmura 1987; Soto-Faraco et al.

2002, 2004; Strybel and Vatakis 2004). Such visual

dominance in detection of motion direction is evidence for

asymmetry between the auditory and the visual modalities

in the audio–visual apparent motion. This asymmetry

originates from integration of modalities. Predictability

may separately affect the visual and auditory modalities

(Johnson and Shapiro 1989), but it is not clear whether

this factor modulates them after integration. Thus, the

second aim of our study was to figure out whether the

predictability would change the auditory and visual

modalities after their integration in audio–visual apparent

motion.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Twenty paid healthy subjects (age range 22–36 years, mean

age 27.1; 12 females) participated in the study. All the

subjects were right-handed, had no audiological abnor-

malities, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects

prior to participation in the study. The study was approved

by the ethics committee of the University of Tubingen.

Visual stimuli

We combined auditory and visual stimuli to elicit the

audio–visual apparent motion illusion. The visual stimuli

were prepared and presented using MATLAB 6.5 software

(The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and the Psychophysics

toolbox for MATLAB. The visual stimuli were two white

1� circles back-projected on the screen at the distance

0.8 m from the subject (Fig. 1). The stimuli were located

on horizontal with the distance of 15� of visual angle at

either side of the screen center. The apparent motion illu-

sion was elicited by presenting the stimuli for 67 ms at the

one side and then after 67 ms delay—for 67 ms at the

opposite side.

290 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123

Auditory stimuli

Auditory stimuli were presented to both ears using an

Enhanced Digital Sound Processor RX2 (Tucker-Davis

Technologies, TDTTM

, Alachua, FL) at 70 dB SPL via non-

magnetic air-conducting plastic tubes (E-A-R-tone 3A,

Cabot Safety Corporation, Indianapolis, IN).

The auditory stimuli were the white noise bursts pro-

cessed in such a way that the sound was spatially perceived

at the position of the visual stimuli (+15� and -15�). For

this aim the bursts were, first, convoluted with spatial

impulse responses using individualized head-related

transfer functions (Algazi et al. 2001). We applied ear

models that represented the head-related impulse response

(HRIR) which was recorded with spatial resolution of 15�horizontal and 10� vertical (Djelani et al. 2000). We chose

the most appropriate ear model individually for each sub-

ject by testing his(her) perception of the virtual auditory

locations as follows: for each subject we presented a set of

100-ms long sounds that were generated by convolution of

HRIRs of each head models obtained by Djelani et al.

(2000) and white noise. As HRIR we used responses

obtained from 24 positions with each of 15� horizontal at

0� vertical. Subjects were asked to locate the perceived

sound on the monitor screen. We selected the ear model for

which the tested angles were recognized maximally cor-

rectly (the mean correct recognition rate across subjects

was 79 ± 6% SD). Second, using a ‘‘shoebox’’ room

simulation, we created an echo train to make an effect of

external sound, as we did in the previous experiments

(Mathiak et al. 2003). We located two virtual sound sour-

ces in such a way that the noise bursts were perceived as

originating from one of the two locations corresponding to

the visual stimuli. The onset of the auditory and visual

stimuli was synchronized using Psychophysics toolbox.

The measured jitter of the onset of auditory and visual

stimuli was strictly less than 2 ms. Sound duration was

67 ms (including 5 ms rise and fall times), that is the same

as duration of the visual stimuli.

Procedure

Each trial consisted of three parts (see Fig. 1). First, the

fixation point (a circle with a diameter of 0.1� in the screen

center) switched its color from red to green. This switch

signaled to subjects to press a button starting the apparent

motion stimuli. Subjects were instructed to press buttons in

random order either by the left or right index finger. If the

subject did not press a button within 1000 ms, the direction

was chosen randomly, but the trial was discarded from

further analyses. Second, the first audio–visual stimulus

was presented for 67 ms at one side, followed by a 67 ms

pause and the second audio–visual stimulus of the same

duration was presented at the other side. Third, after

500 ms interval the fixation point turned red, signaling to

indicate a perceived direction of the apparent motion by

pressing the button on the corresponding to the second

stimulus side. The duration of the third part was random-

ized so that the stimulus onset asynchrony was jittered

from 2700 to 2900 ms. Subjects were instructed to look at

the fixation point during the entire stimulation interval.

Altogether, each subject participated in the four sessions

separated with 2–3 min breaks. Each session consisted of

240 trials divided into four blocks: two blocks with

unpredictable and two blocks with predictable condition. In

the predictable condition, the side of appearance of the first

apparent motion stimulus coincided with the side of the

button pressed, i.e., the subjects determined themselves the

direction of apparent motion. In the unpredictable condi-

tion, the apparent motion started equiprobably from either

side. The type of the first block in each session was chosen

randomly, then the blocks alternated. Thus, subjects were

not aware about blocks sequence in advance. To make the

expectancy the only experimental factor and to keep the

Stimuli self-triggering

1st-stim delay 2nd-stimApparent motion stimuli

ResponsePause

< 1000 ms 67 ms 67 ms 67 ms 1000-1200 ms500 ms

Pause

Fig. 1 The time scheme of the

experiment. A subject pressed a

button which triggered an

audio–visual apparent motion

stimulus. After the stimulus the

subject indicated the perceived

direction of apparent motion

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 291

123

physical properties of stimulation in predictable and

unpredictable conditions identical, we applied further

analyses only on the trials in which the side of first stimulus

coincided with the button pressed: all trials in the

predictable and approximately half of trials in the unpre-

dictable condition. To compensate the lack of such trials in

the unpredictable condition the amount of trials in the

unpredictable and predictable blocks was unequal (70 and

50 trials, respectively).

MEG recording

Neuromagnetic responses were recorded in a magnetically

shielded booth using a 151-sensors whole-head gradiome-

ter (CTF Systems Inc., Vancouver, Canada). Measurements

were performed in a sitting position of the subjects. The

MEG signals were sampled at 312.5 Hz.

Data processing

MEG data were band-pass filtered at 0.1–25 Hz. The data

were segmented into epochs 1400 ms long: -800 ms

before and 600 ms after the onset of the first audio–visual

stimulus. Baseline correction was done relative to the

interval between -800 and -500 ms before the onset of

the first stimulus to ensure the use of the interval preceded

the preparatory motor activity.1 Trials containing artifacts

due to eye blinks (criterion: eye dipole moment[80 nAm)

were discarded in an automatic procedure. We analyzed the

data using custom software in MATLAB 6.5.

As we mentioned earlier, we used in the analysis only

those trials which were in agreement with the two criteria:

(1) the first button was pressed timely (i.e., within

1000 ms) and (2) the first stimulus was at the same side as

the button pressed: all trials in the predictable condition

and approximately half of trials in the unpredictable one.

Moreover, we excluded the first 10 trials of each block, as

according to the subject’s reports it took them about 10

trials to realize that they may control the motion direction

in this block. The data for each subject were separately

averaged relative to the first stimulus onset for the pre-

dictable and the unpredictable conditions, as well as left

and right sides of the first stimulus appearance.

Dipole pattern and fitting

Grand average of the magnetic field revealed three con-

sistent dipole patterns in the time interval from -20 to

180 ms after the first stimulus onset. The first pattern (from

-20 to 40 ms) reflected motor activity due to the button

press. The activity was consistent with the right motor area

for conditions with the left button press and in the left

motor area for conditions with the right button press (Fig. 2

upper panels). The second pattern (from 50 to 110 ms)

represented bilateral auditory activity (Fig. 2, middle

panels). The third pattern (from 120 to 180 ms) reflected

the activity in the right visual cortex when the first stimulus

was at the left side (left-to-right motion) and in the left

visual area when the first stimulus was at the right side

(right-to-left motion; Fig. 2, lower panels).

The sources of neuromagnetic activity were modeled as

equivalent current dipoles. Dipole fitting was performed as

follows: first, we calculated reference position and direc-

tion of the motor, auditory, and visual dipoles based on

grand averages. To that end, magnetic fields were averaged

over time in the corresponding intervals (from -20 to

40 ms for motor, from 50 to 110 ms for auditory, and from

120 to 180 ms for visual dipole). Then, to reduce outliers’

effect of distant sensors, we selected 35–80 sensors for

dipole fitting. The semiautomatic sensor selection included

two parts: the automatic procedure selected the sensors

which had the amplitude not lower than 75% of maximal

amplitude; then, we manually adjusted the selection by

choosing the sensors located over each dipolar pattern

Left-to-Right motionRight-to-Left motion

ytivitcaroto

Ms

m04

–02-

ytivitcayrotidu

As

m011

–05

ytivitcalausiV

sm

081–

021

150

75

0

-75

-150

fT

100

50

0

-50

-100

fT

200

100

0

-100

-200

fT

Fig. 2 Grand averaged maps of magnetic activity within specified

time windows. The upper panel shows magnetic activity in the

interval -20 to +40 ms relative to the triggering button press and the

first stimulus onset. The dipole in the motor areas explains the major

part of the field variance. The middle panel shows magnetic activity

in the interval 50-110 ms after the first stimulus onset. The dipole in

the auditory areas explains the major part of the field variance. The

lower panel shows magnetic activity in the interval 120–180 ms after

the first stimulus onset. The dipole in the visual areas explains the

major part of the field variance. The sensors selected for the dipole

fitting are marked

1 See Electronic supplementary material for the control of the

baseline interval selection.

292 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123

(Fig. 2; for the similar procedures, see Curio et al. 2000,

Martikainen et al. 2005).

Fitting the auditory dipoles was performed in the

average of all conditions (motion from left-to-right and

from right-to-left, predictable and unpredictable) together.

Dipole fitting for the motor and visual activities was

performed in the averages for the right and left dipoles

separately. Since different modalities manifested their

activity in different time intervals and motor and visual

activities precisely lateralized the source model used for

dipole fitting that included a single dipole model for motor

and visual activities and two dipoles model for auditory

activity. We applied the standard spherical head model for

the source localization.

Second, the obtained dipole positions and directions

were used as starting points for dipole fitting individual

MEG data. The procedure was performed similar to fitting

on grand averages. The obtained dipoles were projected on

individual MRI (see Fig. 3). Then we calculated dipole

moments in the respective areas during the entire time

interval and based on all sensors. We used spatio-temporal

dipole model proposed by Scherg and Von Cramon (1985).

For the subjects with low explained variance values the

calculation was based on the dipoles’ positions and direc-

tions obtained from dipole fitting in the grand averages.

Since the audio–visual stimuli were applied at both sides,

but the motor activity occurred only on one side, in the

conditions with the left-to-right motion (left button was

pressed) the dipole model included two auditory, two

visual, and the right motor dipoles; in the conditions with

the right-to-left motion (right button was pressed), the

dipole model contained two auditory, two visual, and the

left motor dipole. We calculated dipole moments for each

subject. Then we filtered the obtained auditory and visual

dipole moments using 3–25 Hz band-pass filter. Motor

activity was filtered using two different filter ranges: the

preparatory activity before button press was band-pass

filtered in 0.1–7 Hz, in the same way as in EEG readiness

potential (RP) studies (see below), and the activity after

button press was band-pass filtered in 3–25 Hz, similarly to

filtering the activity in other modalities2 (see Fig. 4a, b).

The time course of the motor, auditory, and visual

source activities was reflected in the dipole strength curves

(Fig. 4). The curves represent activity contralateral to the

stimulus for the motor and the visual dipoles and contra-

lateral and ipsilateral for the auditory dipole.

Motor dipole strength

We analyzed the preparatory motor dipole moment in the

interval from -5503 to -100 ms (Fig. 5a). The ramp-like

shape and time course of this wave (Fig. 4a) suggests that it

is an MEG analog of EEG readiness potential (Kornhuber

and Deecke 1965). We divided this wave into ten 50-ms

windows and estimated weighted mean amplitude within

each window. Then the amplitude in each window was

averaged across the left-to-right and the right-to-left

motions, excluding the factor of motion direction from the

further analysis.

The curves representing the strength of the motor dipole

after the button press are shown in Fig. 4b. The dipole

moment disclosed two peaks. The first negative peak was

found about the point of the first stimulus onset—it was

marked as M0. Its minimal amplitude and its latency were

measured in the time window from -20 to 40 ms. The

second positive peak emerging at about 100 ms after the

first stimulus onset was marked as M100 and was measured

in the time window from 70 to 120 ms. We estimated both

Fig. 3 Location and orientation of motor (pink, the leftmost map), auditory (green, two central maps) and visual (red, the rightmost map) dipoles

projected on individual MRI of one representative subject

2 The obtained motor activity was used in the control analysis of

possible propagation of activity between the auditory and motor

dipoles which were located close to each other (see Results).3 Here and below all the latencies are given relatively presentation of

the first stimulus.

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 293

123

the peaks at sides contralateral to the pressed button. We

averaged the amplitude and latency of each peak across the

left-to-right and the right-to-left motions, excluding the

factor of motion direction from the further analysis.

Auditory dipole strength

The auditory dipole revealed two sequential pairs of peaks:

the first pair was evoked by the first stimulus and the

second pair of peaks resulted from the activity evoked by

the second stimulus overlapped with the tail of the activity

evoked by the first stimulus (Fig. 4c). Therefore, we used a

prime to mark the second pair of peaks. We marked the

first pair of peaks P50 (in the 50–80 ms) and N1 (90–

130 ms) and the second pair of peaks P500 (180–230 ms)

and N10 (230–280 ms). For each of the four peaks, we

measured the maximal amplitude and the latency for right

and left auditory dipoles, right-to-left and left-to-right

apparent motion directions, and predictable and unpre-

dictable conditions. Then for each dipole side, we averaged

amplitude and latency of each peak across the left-to-right

and the right-to-left motion directions obtaining the activ-

ities in contralateral and ipsilateral sides in relation to the

first stimulus appearance.

Visual dipole strength

The dipole moments of the visual dipoles revealed three

pairs of peaks—each three peaks in contralateral and

ipsilateral hemispheres in relation to the first stimulus

appearance (Fig. 4d). We marked the peaks in the same

manner as the auditory ones: related to the first stimulus

N75 (in the 60–90 ms time interval), P1 (90–120 ms),

N145 (130–160 ms) and related to the second stimulus

N750 (180–230 ms), P10 (210–260 ms), N1450 (260–

310 ms). We measured amplitudes and latency for each

peak for the right-to-left and left-to-right motion directions

for the predictable and the unpredictable conditions. Then,

we averaged the amplitudes and the latencies of each peak

across the right-to-left and the left-to-right motion direc-

tions obtaining the activities in contralateral and ipsilateral

sides in relation to the first stimulus appearance (Fig. 4d).

For statistical evaluation of amplitude and latency, we

used a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA).

Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test was applied

for the post hoc analyses. Statistical analysis was per-

formed using SPSS 12.0 software (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL).

We quantified localization accuracy of dipole fitting

using a crosstalk metric (Liu et al. 1998). The crosstalk

metric estimates how the activity at the current location

depends on the activity at other locations. We calculated

the crosstalk value according to the formula:

time [ms]

Mot

orac

tivity

dipo

lem

omen

t [nA

m]

dipo

lem

omen

t [nA

m]

dipo

lem

omen

t [nA

m]

Aud

itory

activ

ityV

isua

lac

tivity

predictable contralateral unpredictable contralateral

predictable ipsilateral unpredictable ipsilateral

M100

P50

N1

P50’

N1’

N75

P1

N145

N75’

P1’

N145’

M0

Mot

orac

tivity

dipo

lem

omen

t[n

Am

]RP

predictable unpredictable

predictable unpredictable

a

b

c

d

Fig. 4 Grand averaged dipole strength of preparatory motor activity

(a), motor activity after button press (b), auditory (c), and visual (d)

dipoles. The red lines indicate the temporal position of the first and

the second stimuli. All evoked components under study are marked.

The laterality of auditory (c) and visual (d) components is considered

relatively to the first stimulus

294 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123

n2ij ¼jwiajj2

jwiaij2; ð1Þ

where wi represents linear inverse estimator of dipole i, ai

represents a leadfield matrix of dipole i. A crosstalk value

of 0% means that the estimated activity at one location is

completely insensitive to activity at another location. A

crosstalk value of 100% means that the activity at one

location is equally sensitive to activity at this location and

another location. We estimated crosstalk between all pairs

of six dipoles in each subject. Thereto, we located the

dipoles on individual head models. Then we simulated the

activity of each dipole with elementary dipole moment and

projected it onto the remaining five dipoles according to the

formula (1). The dipole crosstalk values obtained for each

were averaged between subjects. We also evaluated the

distance between all pairs of the dipoles.

Results

All subjects reported that they perceived the presented

stimuli as an audio–visual apparent motion. The direction

of the motion was detected correctly in 86.7 and 83.7% of

the predictable and unpredictable trials, respectively, that is

more often in the predictable than in the unpredictable con-

dition (Mann–Whitney U test: Z value = 4.66, P \ 0.01).

Moreover, the detection of the motion direction in the

predictable condition was significantly faster than during

the unpredictable one [856 ms (SEM = 5 ms) vs. 924 ms

(SEM = 6 ms); Z = 8.76, P \ 0.01].

Triggering the stimulus and the apparent motion illusion

emerged the sequential activation of the brain areas as follows:

• In the time window from 20 ms before to 40 ms after

the first stimulus onset we observed the neuromagnetic

activity at in the contralateral to the pressed button

motor areas (Fig. 2, upper panels). In grand averages,

the single dipole model explained 95 and 97% of the

field variance across the selected sensors for the left-to-

right (right motor dipole) and the right-to-left (left

motor dipole) conditions, respectively. In the data of 19

subjects, the motor dipole explained always more than

85% of variances (Fig. 3). Only in one subject the

explained variance was lower (48%).

• In the time window 50–110 ms, we found the expected

pattern of auditory evoked fields (Fig. 2, middle

panels). Two dipoles explained 97% of the field

variance across the selected sensors (Fig. 3). In indi-

vidual data of 16 subjects, the explained variances were

always higher than 75%. In the other four subjects, the

mean of explained variances was 54% (SD ± 7%).

• In the time window 120–180 ms, the topographical

distribution of the activity for the left-to-right and

the right-to-left apparent motion was in accordance

with the right and the left sources in the occipital

areas, respectively (Fig. 2, lower panels). The dipole

explained 93 and 95% of field variance across the

selected channels for the left-to-right and the right-to-

left conditions, respectively (Fig. 3). In individual data

of 14 subjects, the explained variances were always

higher than 75%. In the other six subjects, the mean of

explained variances was 57% (SD ± 9%).

In the motor dipole moment, we observed three compo-

nents: RP, M0, M100 (Fig. 4a, b). To compare time course

of RP in the predictable and unpredictable conditions we

applied a repeated-measures ANOVA on the RP amplitude

with factors of predictability (two levels: predictable vs.

unpredictable) and time window (10 levels) from –550 to

-100 ms. ANOVA revealed significant effects of predict-

ability (F1,19 = 10.7, P \ 0.01) and time window

time windows ms[ ]

dipo

lem

omen

t [nA

m]

predictable

unpredictable

unpredictablepredictable

contralateral N1

contralateral N1’

ipsilateral N 1

ipsilateral N 1’

dipo

lem

omen

t[n

Am

]

**

* **

* *

*

*

*

**

*

*

a bFig. 5 a The mean (±SEM)

amplitudes of preparatory motor

activity in ten 50-ms time

windows from -550 to

-100 ms before the stimulus

onset. b The mean (±SEM)

amplitude of the N1 and N10

components of the contralateral

and the ipsilateral auditory

response for the predictable and

the unpredictable conditions.

*P \ 0.05, **P \ 0.01

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 295

123

(F9,171 = 13.1, P \ 0.001, Huynh–Feldt e = 0.26). The

interaction was not significant (F9,171 = 2.1, P [ 0.1,

Huynh–Feldt e = 0.53). Post hoc LSD test revealed that

the difference between the predictable and unpredictable

conditions was significant from -450 ms and lasted until

-100 ms relative to the button press (Fig. 5a).

To be sure that the baseline interval (-800 to -500 ms),

which might include the early phase of RP (Kristeva et al.

1991), does not differ between conditions, we made a

control analysis of the preparatory motor activity using as a

baseline the interval -1000 to -900 ms (see Electronic

supplementary material and Fig. S1 for details). We found

that the time course of the difference between the pre-

dictable and unpredictable conditions remained to be the

same as in the analysis described earlier. This evidences

that the baseline interval (-800 to -500 ms) does not

contain the differential activity which could affect the

observed differences between the conditions.

The components M0 and M100 reflect the executive

stage of the motor activity and exhibit a similar latency as

the auditory components P50 and N1. The mean amplitude

and the latency of M0 and M100 are given in Table 1. We

evaluated an effect of predictability on the M0 and M100

motor components. A repeated-measures ANOVA with

factor of predictability (predictable vs. unpredictable) did

not show any significant difference between the predictable

and unpredictable conditions in both the amplitude and the

latency of these components (F1,19 always less than 1.7,

P [ 0.1). This evidences that predictability modulating the

preparatory motor activity does not effect on the execution

of the motor program. Moreover, taking into account the

predictability effect found in the post-stimulus interval in

the auditory domain (described later) this suggests inde-

pendence of auditory and motor activities.4

The auditory dipole moments revealed P50 and N1

components to the first and second apparent motion stimuli.

These components emerged in both the contralateral and

ipsilateral to the stimulus hemispheres (Fig. 4c). Their

mean amplitudes and latencies are given in Table 2. A

2 9 2 repeated-measures ANOVA evaluated the effect of

predictability (predictable vs. unpredictable) and the effect

of laterality (contralateral vs. ipsilateral) for each compo-

nent. For the N1 amplitude, we found the effect of

predictability (F1,19 = 6.8, P \ 0.05) with more negative

amplitude in the unpredictable than predictable condition

and the near significant effect of laterality (F1,19 = 4.2,

P = 0.06) with larger amplitude in ipsilateral than the

contralateral side. Interestingly, the interaction between the

factors was highly significant (F1,19 = 16.3, P \ 0.01).

The post hoc LSD test revealed that the predictability

effect was prominent only in the contralateral to the first

stimulus side (P \ 0.001). For the N10 component, we also

found the significant effect of predictability (F1,19 = 15.7,

P \ 0.01) with larger amplitude in the unpredictable than

predictable condition; however, for this component the

effect was observed for both the ipsilateral and contralat-

eral activities (Fig. 5b). Latency of the components was not

influenced by either laterality or predictability (F1,19

always less than 2.3, P [ 0.1).

The time course of the visual dipole moments exhibited

N75, P1, and N145 components in the hemisphere con-

tralateral to each stimulus onset side (Fig. 4d). The mean

values of the amplitude and the latency of the visual

components are presented in Table 3. Predictability did not

influence the amplitude of the visual components (F1,19

always less than 2.1, P [ 0.1). The only significant effect

of the predictability was found on the latency of N145

component: it emerged earlier in the unpredictable condi-

tion (F1,19 = 4.39, P \ 0.05).

The predictability effect found for both the auditory and

the motor activities suggests a systematic relationship. To

further investigate this issue we correlated the differences

(predictable minus unpredictable) between the amplitude

of the RP and two auditory peaks N1 and N10 in the pre-

dictable and the unpredictable conditions (DRP vs. DN1

and DRP vs. DN10). We considered DRP on the side con-

tralateral to the first stimulus onset for 10 time intervals

within the time window from -550 to -100 ms, with

interval duration 50 ms. The DN1 and DN10 differences

were considered in both contralateral and ipsilateral sides.

In each time interval, we computed Pearson correlation

coefficient between DN1 (DN10) and DRP. We found sig-

nificant positive correlations for both DN1 and DN10 on the

side contralateral to the first stimulus within time intervals

-300 to -150 ms (r [ 0.45, P \ 0.05) (except -200 ms;

Fig. 6). Correlation of the DRP and DN10 on the ipsilateral

side was significant in the time interval -100 ms before

the button press. Correlation of the DRP and DN1 on the

ipsilateral side was not significant. Thus, we found that the

effect of predictability was accompanied with selective

correlations between activities in auditory and motor

domains.

Table 1 Mean amplitude and latency (±SEM) of M0 and M100

motor components

Amplitude [nAm] Latency (ms)

P U P U

M0 -7.1 ± 0.8 -7.2 ± 0.8 23 ± 2 22 ± 2

M100 8.9 ± 0.7 9.2 ± 0.8 123 ± 4 122 ± 3

Statistical analysis did not show any influence of predictability on the

amplitude and the latency of these components

P predictable, U, unpredictable conditions

4 The absence of propagation of activity between auditory and motor

dipoles was also confirmed by evaluation of crosstalk (Table S1).

296 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123

Crosstalk between each pair of dipoles did not exceed

35% (Table S1). The distance between the dipoles was

always larger than 40 mm which evidences for the good

spatial source separation (Fujimaki et al. 2002). Taking

into account that the timing of preparatory motor activity

and auditory components was different (see Fig. 4) it is

unlikely that the correlation between preparatory motor and

auditory activity was influenced by a propagation of

activity between auditory and motor areas. In the post-

stimulus interval the interaction between motor M100 and

auditory N1 components could not be excluded, however,

we found no evidence of influence of predictability on the

M100 (see earlier).

Discussion

Our study demonstrates how predictability effects on the

motor preparation to stimulus self-triggering and on the

following perception of an audio–visual apparent motion.

We found that predictability modulated the motor and

auditory but not visual activity. The preparatory motor

Table 3 The mean of the amplitudes and the latencies (±SEM) of

the visual components

Amplitude (nAm) Latency (ms)

P U P U

N75 -0.3 ± 0.1 -0.3 ± 0.1 60 ± 2 58 ± 1

P1 1.0 ± 0.1 1.0 ± 0.2 101 ± 2 102 ± 2

N145 -1.2 ± 0.2 -1.2 ± 0.2 151 ± 3 146 ± 2

N750 -0.7 ± 0.2 -0.8 ± 0.2 196 ± 5 199 ± 4

P10 1.3 ± 0.2 1.3 ± 0.2 235 ± 6 234 ± 5

N1450 -1.4 ± 0.2 -1.4 ± 0.2 278 ± 7 277 ± 6

Statistically significant difference between predictable and unpre-

dictable conditions is marked bold

P predictable, U unpredictable conditions

∆N1

Con

tral

ater

alco

rrel

atio

n

Ipsi

late

ral

corr

elat

ion

time windows [ms] time windows [ms]

∆N1′Fig. 6 Correlation coefficients

between DRP and DN1 (DN10)in time windows from -550 to

-100 ms. DN1 and DN10 are

derived from the contralateral

and ipsilateral side relative to

the stimulus onset. DRP is

derived only from the

contralateral side. The asterisksindicate the significance of the

correlations: *P \ 0.05,

**P \ 0.01

Table 2 Mean amplitude and latency (±SEM) of auditory components for the contra- and ipsilateral activity

Contralateral activity Ipsilateral activity

Amplitude (nAm) Latency (ms) Amplitude (nAm) Latency (ms)

P U P U P U P U

P50 9.5 ± 1.0 8.8 ± 1.0 65 ± 2 68 ± 2 10.4 ± 1.1 10.1 ± 1.0 66 ± 2 68 ± 3

N1 -12.8 ± 1.4 -14.9 ± 1.5 111 ± 3 110 ± 3 -15.6 ± 1.5 -15.4 ± 1.7 109 ± 3 111 ± 3

P500 12.4 ± 1.1 13.2 ± 1.5 180 ± 5 180 ± 4 11.0 ± 1.0 11.6 ± 1.1 185 ± 5 184 ± 5

N10 -11.3 ± 1.3 -12.6 ± 1.4 242 ± 6 239 ± 5 -10.9 ± 1.0 -12.2 ± 1.0 249±6 251 ± 6

P50 and N1 components evoked by the first stimulus and P500 and N10 by the second stimulus

Statistically significant difference between predictable and unpredictable conditions is marked bold

P predictable, U unpredictable conditions

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 297

123

component (‘RP’) was affected within the time interval

from -450 to -100 ms before button press triggering the

stimulus. Analyzing the components related to perception

of the audio–visual apparent motion we found larger

amplitude in the unpredictable than predictable conditions

in accordance with the previous studies (Schafer and

Marcus 1973; Curio et al. 2000; Martikainen et al. 2005).

This predictability effect was found for the auditory com-

ponent N1 (100 ms): for N1 evoked by the first event of the

apparent motion sequence the effect was observed on the

side contralateral to the stimulus appearance, whereas for

N10 evoked by the second event the effect was observed on

both sides. The predictable–unpredictable difference of the

N1 and N10 correlated with that of the preparatory motor

activity before the button press. The correlation was more

prominent on the side contralateral to the stimulus

appearance. These findings suggest that predictability

mediates interactions between auditory and motor systems

in self-triggered stimulation.

Self-triggering of apparent motion

Predictability affected the amplitude of the preparatory

motor component (an analog of EEG ‘RP’) on the side

contralateral to the pressed button in the time interval from

-450 to -100 ms. From EEG RP studies it is known that

more negative amplitude of the RP is the evidence for more

effortful preparation process (Deecke 1996). But we cannot

extrapolate this fact to our preparatory motor component as

MEG measures the tangential component of the source,

whereas EEG picks its radial component, and therefore the

comparison of MEG and EEG polarity is problematic.

However, this leaves intact our finding that the predictable

and unpredictable conditions differ already in motor

preparation.

Despite that the predictable and unpredictable conditions

were presented in the separate blocks, the effect of pre-

dictability on the RP was observed only in the limited time

interval -450 to -100 ms. The predictability had no effect

on the motor components M0 and M100 that accompanied

the execution of the button press. This suggests that the

predictability information becomes relevant for the motor

system right before movement execution and does not

constitute a constant background shift of the motor activity.

Perception of apparent motion

The effect of predictability on the perception of apparent

motion appeared to be highly selective: we observed it only

on the auditory peak N1, but neither on another auditory

peak nor on the visual activity. Therefore, it is unlikely that

the predictability is a general factor which steadily shifted

the level of excitability of the brain systems in our blocked

design. The predictability rather modulates certain percep-

tual stages crucial for evaluation of stimulus meaning. This

idea is supported by the fact that predictability has no effect

on the early sensory P50 components evoked by both the

first and second auditory events, but its effect starts from the

N1 component which is ‘‘flanked’’ by P50 components

related to the first and second events. The latency of the N1

component is 100 ms or higher, which is sufficient for

initial evaluation of meaning of the auditory stimulus

(Naatanen and Picton 1987). Moreover, it was shown that

the brain activity starting from about 100 ms may be

modulated by top–down influences, e.g., attention (Hillyard

et al. 1998). This suggests that predictability modulates the

activity of the auditory cortex in top–down manner.

The absence of the predictability effect on the visual

activity in our study may be explained by asymmetry of the

multisensory interactions in perception of apparent motion

direction. The dominance of the visual motion information

over the auditory one in perception of motion direction in

multisensory apparent motion was repeatedly reported

(Allen and Kolers 1981; Mateeff et al. 1985; Ohmura 1987;

Soto-Faraco et al. 2004; Strybel and Vatakis 2004). This

dominance is especially prominent when the visual and

auditory information exactly coincides spatially and is

synchronized temporally (Soto-Faraco et al. 2002), i.e.,

when the audio–visual integration is facilitated, that was

the case in our study. The visual dominance in perception

of motion direction is explained by the higher accuracy of

vision in detection of spatial attributes in comparison with

audition (Soto-Faraco et al. 2004). Thus, we may conclude

that the audio–visual integration makes the visual compo-

nent of the apparent motion more robust against influence

of external factors, such as predictability.

Motor–auditory interaction

The effect of predictability was initially observed on the

motor activity between -450 and-100 ms before stimulus

triggering; then, about 200 ms later on the auditory com-

ponents N1 for the first stimulus and again after some

delay, on the N10 for the second stimulus. These changes

were limited topographically: the effect of predictability on

the auditory N1 after first stimulus was observed only on

the contralateral to the stimulus side. This side corresponds

to the motor area, which participated in triggering the

stimulus. This evidences that predictability affects the

auditory and motor systems within the same brain hemi-

sphere. We also found that the differences between the

predictable and unpredictable conditions for the motor and

auditory activity were correlated. The correlations of the

motor activity with auditory component N1 evoked by the

first and second stimuli were also mainly observed con-

tralateral to the stimulus (Fig. 6). Such correlations limited

298 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123

to one hemisphere suggest interactions of motor and

auditory systems during the preparation and the perception

of outcomes of self-generated actions. These interactions

may be either direct projections from the motor to the

sensory system or be driven by other high-order brain

mechanisms, such as anticipation (see later).

The difference between sensory outcomes of self-gen-

erated vs. external actions is often explained by an internal

forward model (Wolpert 1997; Blackmore and Decety

2001; Martikainen et al. 2005). Our results can be only

partially modeled in terms of this concept as follows: in the

predictable condition the forward model is built, which

accounts an intention to launch the stimulus from a certain

side. The model predicts the sensory consequences of the

movements. The prediction is then compared with the actual

consequences of the motor act to determine the source of

sensory events. The correct prediction in the predictable

condition attenuates the response of the auditory system. In

the unpredictable condition such a forward model is not

built. Therefore, the auditory response is not attenuated.

However, the internal forward model cannot embrace all

of our results: in our experiment predictability also affected

the preparatory motor activity in the interval -450 to

-100 ms before self-triggering. It cannot be explained by

the forward model which accounts for the causal relation-

ship only between actions and their outcomes. The

predictability-related changes of the preparatory motor

activity might have resulted from anticipation of action

outcomes. In our study we used block design in which the

consequences of own actions can be easily predicted.

Anticipation of action consequences represented in mem-

ory play a powerful role in planning and selection of an

action (Hommel et al. 2001). Moreover, during preparation

of the motor program anticipation of its outcomes pre-tunes

the brain to the perception of action effects, which was

found to be reflected in early perceptual components of

event-related potentials (Nikolaev et al. 2008). The current

study provides evidence that anticipation of own action

outcomes selectively modulates the preparatory motor

activity, as well as its perceptual consequences.

Acknowledgments This research was supported by the BICW

(Brain Imaging Center West, Clinical MEG) of the BMBF and the

German Research Foundation (SFB 550/B1). K.M. is supported by an

AstraZeneca foundation. The authors thank Maike Borutta for her

excellent technical assistance and Argiro Vatakis and Zoe Kourtzi for

helpful discussion.

References

Algazi VR, Duda RO, Thompson DM, Avendano C (2001) The

CIPIC HRTF database. In: Proc. 2001 IEEE workshop on

applications of signal processing to audio and electroacoustics,

pp 99–102

Allen PG, Kolers PA (1981) Sensory specificity of apparent motion.

J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 7:1318–1326

Berndt I, Franz VH, Bulthoff HH, Wascher E (2002) Effects of

pointing direction and direction predictability on event-related

lateralizations of the EEG. Hum Mov Sci 21:387–410

Blakemore S-J, Rees G, Frith CD (1998) How do we predict the

consequences of our actions? A functional imaging study.

Neuropsychologia 25:410–418

Blakemore S-J, Wolpert DM, Frith CD (2000) Why can’t you tickle

yourself? Neuroreport 11:11–16

Curio G, Neuloh G, Numminen J, Jousmaki V, Hari R (2000)

Speaking modifies voice-evoked activity in the human auditory

cortex. Hum Brain Mapp 9:183–191

Dassonville P, Lewis SM, Zhu X-H, Ugurbil K, Kim S-G, Ashe J

(1998) Effects of movement predictability on cortical motor

activation. Neurosci Res 32:65–74

Deecke L (1996) Planning, preparation, execution, and imagery of

volitional action. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 3:59–64

Djelani T, Porschmann C, Sahrhage J, Blauert J (2000) An interactive

virtual-environment generator for psychoacoustic research II:

collection of head related impulse responses and evaluation of

auditory localization. ACUSTICA/acta acustica 86:1046–1053

Fujimaki N, Hayakawa T, Nielsen M, Knosche TR, Miyauchi S (2002)

An fMRI-constrained MEG source analysis with procedures for

dividing and grouping activation. Neuroimage 17:324–43

Hillyard SA, Vogel EK, Luck SJ (1998) Sensory gain control

(amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: Electro-

physiological and neuroimaging evidence. Philos Trans R Soc

Lond B Biol Sci 353:1257–1270

Hommel B, Musseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory

of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action

planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–878

Johnson TL, Shapiro KL (1989) Attention to auditory and peripheral

visual stimuli: effects of arousal and predictability. Acta Psychol

(Amst) 72:233–45

Kornhuber HH, Deecke L (1965) Hirnpotentialanderungen bei

Willkiirbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Menschen:

Bereitscbaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale. Pfluegers Arch

248:1–17

Kristeva R, Cheyne D, Deecke L (1991) Neuromagnetic fields

accompanying unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements:

topography and analysis of cortical sources. Electroencephalogr

Clin Neurophysiol 81:284–298

Liu AK, Belliveau JW, Dale AM (1998) Spatiotemporal imaging of

human brain activity using functional MRI constrained magne-

toencephalography data: Monte Carlo simulations. Proc Natl

Acad Sci USA 95:8945–8950

Martikainen MH, Kaneko K, Hari R (2005) Suppressed responses to

self-triggered sounds in the human auditory cortex. Cereb Cortex

15:299–302

Mateeff S, Hohnsbein J, Noack T (1985) Dynamic visual capture:

apparent auditory motion induced by a moving visual target.

Perception 14:721–727

Mathiak K, Hertrich I, Kincses WE, Riecker A, Lutzenberger W,

Ackermann H (2003) The right supratemporal plane hears the

distance of objects: neuromagnetic correlates of virtual reality.

Neuroreport 14:307–311

McCarthy G, Donchin E (1976) The effects of temporal and event

uncertainty in determining the waveforms of the auditory event

related potential (ERP). Psychophysiology 13:581–590

Naatanen R, Picton T (1987) The N1 wave of the human electric and

magnetic response to sound: a review and an analysis of the

component structure. Psychophysiology 24:375–425

Nelson AJ, Staines WR, McIlroy WE (2004) Tactile stimulus

predictability modulates activity in a tactile-motor cortical

network. Exp Brain Res 154:22–32

Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300 299

123

Nikolaev AR, Ziessler M, Dimova K, van Leeuwen C (2008)

Anticipated action consequences as a nexus between action and

perception: evidence from event-related potentials. Biol Psychol

78:53–65

Nittono H (2006) Voluntary stimulus production enhances deviance

processing in the brain. Int J Psychophysiol 59:15–21

Nittono H, Ullsperger P (2000) Event-related potentials in a self-

paced novelty oddball task. Neuroreport 11:1861–1864

Ohmura H (1987) Intersensory influences on the perception of

apparent movement. Jpn Psychol Res 29:1–19

Schafer EWP, Marcus M (1973) Self-stimulation alters human

sensory brain responses. Science 181:175–177

Scherg M, Von Cramon D (1985) Two bilateral sources of the late

AEP as identified by a spatio-temporal dipole model. Electro-

encephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 62:32–44

Soto-Faraco S, Lyons J, Gazzaniga M, Spence C, Kingstone A (2002)

The ventriloquist in motion: illusory capture of dynamic

information across sensory modalities. Cogn Brain Res

14:139–146

Soto-Faraco S, Spence C, Kingstone A (2004) Moving multisensory

research along: motion perception across sensory modalities.

Curr Dir Psychol Sci 13:29–32

Strybel TZ, Vatakis A (2004) A comparison of auditory and visual

apparent motion presented individually and with crossmodal

moving distractors. Perception 33:1033–1048

Wolpert DM (1997) Computational approaches to motor control.

Trends Cogn Sci 1:209–216

300 Exp Brain Res (2008) 189:289–300

123