Infants’ sensitivity to shading and line junctions

7
Infants’ sensitivity to shading and line junctions Tomoko Imura a,b, * , Masami K. Yamaguchi c,d , So Kanazawa e , Nobu Shirai b,f , Yumiko Otsuka b,g , Masaki Tomonaga a , Akihiro Yagi h a Section of Language and Intelligence, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, 41-2, Inuyama, Aichi, 484-8506, Japan b Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, 6 Ichibancho, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 102-8471, Japan c Department of Psychology, Chuo University, 742-1 Higashinakano, Hachioji, Tokyo, 192-0393, Japan d PRESTO, Japan Science and Technology Agency, 4-1-8 Honcho Kawaguchi, Saitama, 332-0012, Japan e Department of Psychology, Faculty of Integrated Art and Social Sciences, Japan Women’s University, Nishi-ikuta 1-1-1, Tama, Kawasaki, Kanagawa, 214-8565, Japan f Department of Psychology, Tokyo Metropolitan University, 1-1 Minamiohsawa Hachiohji, Tokyo, 192-0397, Japan g Department of Infants’ Brain & Cognitive Development, Tokyo Women’s Medical University, 8-1, Kawata, Shinjuku, Tokyo 162-8666, Japan h Department of Integrated Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University, Uegahara, Nishinomiya, Hyogo, 662-8501, Japan article info Article history: Received 7 February 2007 Received in revised form 5 March 2008 Keywords: Shading Line junctions Pictorial depth cue Infants abstract We examined the sensitivity to shading and line junction cues in human infants aged 5–8 months using computer-generated displays containing a rectangular-wave grating and a serrated aperture. In Experi- ment 1, infants were presented with a pair of displays: a two-dimensional to three-dimensional (2D– 3D) display, alternating between 2D and 3D images, and a 2D–2D display, alternating between two 2D images. The 3D image consisted of black-and-white borders aligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture, creating the appearance of a 3D folded surface. The 2D image consisted of the black-and-white borders misaligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture, which does not create a 3D impression for adults. Seven- and 8-month-old infants looked longer at the 2D–3D display than the 2D–2D display. In contrast, 5- and 6-month-old infants did not exhibit a looking preference. In Experiment 2, we used images with double-cycle rectangular-wave gratings to impair shading information. These images consisted of black- and-white borders aligned with half of the peaks and misaligned with latter half of the peaks of a serrated aperture, giving the appearance of surface markings. Seven- and 8-month-old infants did not exhibit a significant difference in preference between the two test displays. These results could not be explained by the young infant’s failure of discrimination due to the experimental procedure (Experiment 3). These results showed that the sensitivity to shading and line junctions change between 5–6 and 7–8 months of age. Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction The adult visual system utilizes various kinds of depth cues, such as motion parallax, binocular disparity, shading, and texture gradients to perceive the three-dimensional (3D) shape of an ob- ject (e.g., Gibson, 1950), and several psychophysical studies sug- gest that the visual system integrates information from these depth cues (e.g., Bruno & Cutting, 1988; Curran & Johnston, 1994; Maloney & Landy, 1989). Recent neurophysiological studies also showed that monocular depth cues interact with other mon- ocular cues and binocular disparity. A visual evoked potential (VEP) study demonstrated that stimuli with consistent shading and line junctions elicited larger evoked response than those with inconsistent cues (Hou, Pettet, Vildavski, & Norcia, 2006). fMRI studies revealed a group of neurons that are selective to the 3D surface orientation and sensitive to multiple cues, such as shading, texture gradients, and binocular disparity, in the caudal intrapari- etal (CIP) area (Taira, Nose, Inoue, & Tsutsui, 2001; Tsutsui, Jiang, Yara, Sakata, & Taira, 2001; Tsutsui, Sakata, Naganuma, & Taira, 2002; Tsutsui, Taira, & Sakata, 2005). Although many studies have reported that the sensitivity to various depth cues emerges in early in life, there are only a few studies about sensitivity to multiple depth cues in infants. In the present study, we examined the sen- sitivity to shading and line junction cues in human infants. There is a variety of opinions about the development of sensitiv- ity to pictorial depth cues. Some studies suggest that 3-month-old infants show sensitivity to differences in 3D features that are per- ceived automatically and effortlessly by preattentive visual sys- tems in adults (e.g., Enns, 1992; Enns & Rensink, 1990, 1991; Kleffner & Ramachandran, 1992). Bhatt and Waters (1998) showed that 3-month-old infants detected an odd item among an array consisting of cubes defined by shading and line junctions (Y-junc- tions). Bhatt and Bertin (2001) showed that 3-month-old infants detect orientation changes based on line junctions and other 2D cues using a visual search display. In contrast, Bertin and Bhatt 0042-6989/$ - see front matter Ó 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.011 * Corresponding author. Fax: +81 568 63 0549. E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Imura). Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Vision Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/visres

Transcript of Infants’ sensitivity to shading and line junctions

Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Vision Research

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate /v isres

Infants’ sensitivity to shading and line junctions

Tomoko Imura a,b,*, Masami K. Yamaguchi c,d, So Kanazawa e, Nobu Shirai b,f, Yumiko Otsuka b,g,Masaki Tomonaga a, Akihiro Yagi h

a Section of Language and Intelligence, Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, 41-2, Inuyama, Aichi, 484-8506, Japanb Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, 6 Ichibancho, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo 102-8471, Japanc Department of Psychology, Chuo University, 742-1 Higashinakano, Hachioji, Tokyo, 192-0393, Japand PRESTO, Japan Science and Technology Agency, 4-1-8 Honcho Kawaguchi, Saitama, 332-0012, Japane Department of Psychology, Faculty of Integrated Art and Social Sciences, Japan Women’s University, Nishi-ikuta 1-1-1, Tama, Kawasaki, Kanagawa, 214-8565, Japanf Department of Psychology, Tokyo Metropolitan University, 1-1 Minamiohsawa Hachiohji, Tokyo, 192-0397, Japang Department of Infants’ Brain & Cognitive Development, Tokyo Women’s Medical University, 8-1, Kawata, Shinjuku, Tokyo 162-8666, Japanh Department of Integrated Psychological Science, Kwansei Gakuin University, Uegahara, Nishinomiya, Hyogo, 662-8501, Japan

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Received 7 February 2007Received in revised form 5 March 2008

Keywords:ShadingLine junctionsPictorial depth cueInfants

0042-6989/$ - see front matter � 2008 Elsevier Ltd. Adoi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.03.011

* Corresponding author. Fax: +81 568 63 0549.E-mail address: [email protected] (T. Imura

a b s t r a c t

We examined the sensitivity to shading and line junction cues in human infants aged 5–8 months usingcomputer-generated displays containing a rectangular-wave grating and a serrated aperture. In Experi-ment 1, infants were presented with a pair of displays: a two-dimensional to three-dimensional (2D–3D) display, alternating between 2D and 3D images, and a 2D–2D display, alternating between two 2Dimages. The 3D image consisted of black-and-white borders aligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture,creating the appearance of a 3D folded surface. The 2D image consisted of the black-and-white bordersmisaligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture, which does not create a 3D impression for adults.Seven- and 8-month-old infants looked longer at the 2D–3D display than the 2D–2D display. In contrast,5- and 6-month-old infants did not exhibit a looking preference. In Experiment 2, we used images withdouble-cycle rectangular-wave gratings to impair shading information. These images consisted of black-and-white borders aligned with half of the peaks and misaligned with latter half of the peaks of a serratedaperture, giving the appearance of surface markings. Seven- and 8-month-old infants did not exhibit asignificant difference in preference between the two test displays. These results could not be explainedby the young infant’s failure of discrimination due to the experimental procedure (Experiment 3). Theseresults showed that the sensitivity to shading and line junctions change between 5–6 and 7–8 months ofage.

� 2008 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

The adult visual system utilizes various kinds of depth cues,such as motion parallax, binocular disparity, shading, and texturegradients to perceive the three-dimensional (3D) shape of an ob-ject (e.g., Gibson, 1950), and several psychophysical studies sug-gest that the visual system integrates information from thesedepth cues (e.g., Bruno & Cutting, 1988; Curran & Johnston,1994; Maloney & Landy, 1989). Recent neurophysiological studiesalso showed that monocular depth cues interact with other mon-ocular cues and binocular disparity. A visual evoked potential(VEP) study demonstrated that stimuli with consistent shadingand line junctions elicited larger evoked response than those withinconsistent cues (Hou, Pettet, Vildavski, & Norcia, 2006). fMRIstudies revealed a group of neurons that are selective to the 3Dsurface orientation and sensitive to multiple cues, such as shading,

ll rights reserved.

).

texture gradients, and binocular disparity, in the caudal intrapari-etal (CIP) area (Taira, Nose, Inoue, & Tsutsui, 2001; Tsutsui, Jiang,Yara, Sakata, & Taira, 2001; Tsutsui, Sakata, Naganuma, & Taira,2002; Tsutsui, Taira, & Sakata, 2005). Although many studies havereported that the sensitivity to various depth cues emerges in earlyin life, there are only a few studies about sensitivity to multipledepth cues in infants. In the present study, we examined the sen-sitivity to shading and line junction cues in human infants.

There is a variety of opinions about the development of sensitiv-ity to pictorial depth cues. Some studies suggest that 3-month-oldinfants show sensitivity to differences in 3D features that are per-ceived automatically and effortlessly by preattentive visual sys-tems in adults (e.g., Enns, 1992; Enns & Rensink, 1990, 1991;Kleffner & Ramachandran, 1992). Bhatt and Waters (1998) showedthat 3-month-old infants detected an odd item among an arrayconsisting of cubes defined by shading and line junctions (Y-junc-tions). Bhatt and Bertin (2001) showed that 3-month-old infantsdetect orientation changes based on line junctions and other 2Dcues using a visual search display. In contrast, Bertin and Bhatt

T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426 1421

(2006) suggested that 3-month-olds are not sensitive to line junc-tion cues alone in the absence of shading information. These find-ings suggest that 3-month-old infants are sensitive to shading andline junction cues in the context of preattentive visual processingsuch as occurs in visual search task (Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Bhatt& Bertin, 2001; Bhatt & Waters, 1998).

On the other hand, other studies reported that sensitivity toshading and line junction cues develops between 5 and 7 months(shading: Granrud, Yonas, & Opland, 1985; T-junctions: Granrud& Yonas, 1984; Y-junctions: Yonas & Arterberry, 1994; curved Y-junctions: Kavšek, 1999). Granrud et al. (1985) investigated theperception of shape from shading in infants aged 5 and 7 monthsusing infants’ reaching response as a dependent measure. Infantswere presented a display consisting of a photographic convexityand a photographic concavity defined by shading cues. The 7-month-olds reached more for the convexity display than for theone showing concavity under monocular viewing conditions, eventhough the display was flat. The 5-month-olds showed no reachingpreference. Granrud and Yonas (1984) examined the effect of inter-position from T-junctions in a preferential reaching task. The 5-and 7-month-old infants were presented three overlapping sur-faces whose order was defined by T-junctions, and their reachingresponses to each surface were measured. All parts of the displaywere coplanar and equidistance from the infants. Only the 7-month-old infants showed a reaching preference for the apparentlynearer surface under monocular viewing conditions. These studiesindicate that 7-month-old infants discriminate shape from shadingand line junctions. Yonas and Arterberry (1994) explored infants’ability to discriminate between lines indicating a border and linesindicating a surface marking in a habituation–dishabituation pro-cedure. Infants were habituated to the display containing a surfacemarking and a border defined by a T-junction and tested with twodisplays, one lacking the surface marking and one lacking the bor-der. The 7.5-month-old infants looked longer at the display with-out the border than at the display that lacked the surfacemarking, suggesting that the infants discriminated between sur-face markings and borders. Furthermore, Yonas and Arterberry(1994) asserted that interposition information was more salientfor infants than surface markings. Kavšek (1999) extended theirfindings to curved Y-junctions by using cylinders as stimuli, andsuggested that the ability to perceive depth from pictorial cuessuch as shading and line junctions develops at 7 months. Furtherstudies have shown evidence that infants’ sensitivities for otherkinds of pictorial depth cues, such as cast shadows (Imura et al.,2006; Yonas & Granrud, 2006), texture gradients (Arterberry, Yo-nas, & Bensen, 1989), relative size (Yonas, Granrud, & Pettersen,1985), and familiar size (Granrud, Haake, & Yonas, 1985) also de-velop between 5 and 7 months.

Although the findings from previous studies suggest that sensi-tivity to pictorial depth cues develops by 7 months of age earliersensitivity has been reported with other experimental procedures.Bhatt and Waters (1998) found that 3-month-old infants showedsignificant looking responses to differences defined by shadingand line junctions. In a series of studies Bhatt and his colleagues(Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Bhatt & Bertin, 2001; Bhatt & Waters,1998) have used visual search displays. Such types of stimuli havebeen used to examine preattentive visual processing in adults.Thus, the findings might reflect the sensitivity to pictorial depthcues on lower-level visual processing such as feature detection.The previous studies suggesting the sensitivity to pictorial depthcues only at 7 months used different types of stimuli from visualsearch display and it is possible that these studies reflect the sen-sitivity to pictorial depth cues at a higher-level of processing.

Given that studies using a visual search display might relate tolower-level visual processing as already stated, further studiesusing different kinds of stimuli might clarify developmental pro-

cess of sensitivity to multiple depth cues a later developmentalphases. In the present studies, we examined the sensitivity toshading and line junction cues using the different stimuli from vi-sual search display used in a series of Bhatt and his colleagues’studies.

The stimuli used here were originally developed by Hou et al.(2006). They created a display containing rectangular grating anda serrated aperture as shading and line junction (Y-junction) cues.The 3D interpretation of the display was varied temporally bymanipulating the spatial relationship between the rectangulargrating and the serrated aperture. When the black-and-white bor-ders were aligned with the peaks of the aperture, the display ap-peared as a 3D folded surface. When the borders weremisaligned with the peaks of the aperture, the display appearedto be flat. If infants are sensitive to the relationship between theshading and line junction cues, they should show a discriminativeresponse to the display only when these cues are consistent.

In Experiment 1, we presented displays with and without depthchange, defined by shading and line junction cues, to infants andtested whether they preferred to look at displays containing depthchanges over those without them. In Experiment 2, we exploredwhether the inconsistency of these cues interfered with infants’discrimination between the two displays. Finally, we demonstratedthe validity of this procedure in younger groups of infants in Exper-iment 3.

2. Experiment 1: Shading and line junction cues

2.1. Method

2.1.1. ParticipantsThe sample consisted of fifteen 5- and 6-month-old infants

(mean age = 168.87 days; SD = 20.22 days) and fifteen 7- and 8-month-old infants (males and females, mean age = 223.8 days,SD = 19.51 days). All infants were full-term at birth and healthyat the time of testing. An additional two 5- and 6-month-oldsand one infant in the 7- and 8-month-old group were tested butexcluded from the sample due to fussiness. The infants were re-cruited through newspaper advertisements. The sample consistedof infants whose parents responded to a letter and volunteeredto participate. Parents gave informed consent before the experi-mental session.

2.1.2. ApparatusStimuli were displayed on a 21-in. CRT monitor (TOTOKU, Calix

CDT2141A). The resolution of the CRT was set at 1024 � 768 pixelswith an 8-bit color mode. The refresh rate of the CRT was 75 Hz.Two loud speakers were located on either side of the CRT. Theexperimenter could see the infant’s looking behavior via a videocamera placed at the bottom of the monitor. The presentation ofstimulus and sound was controlled by a personal computer (AONEVXMSL2MeC/09183).

2.1.3. StimuliThe test stimuli were created with Adobe Illustrator and con-

trolled by a compiled program written by Microsoft Visual Basic6.0 with DirectX 7.0. The test displays are shown in Fig. 1. We used0.18 cpd rectangular-wave grating as a shading cue (e.g., Bhatt &Waters, 1998; Hou et al., 2006). The luminance of the white areawas 93.93 cd/m2, and that of the black area was 1.88 cd/m2. TheMichelson’s contrast of the grating was 96.1%, sufficient for thecontrast sensitivity in young infants (Atkinson, Braddick, & Brad-dick, 1974). The rectangle-wave grating was occluded by a mid-gray serrated aperture (29.37 cd/m2). The overall size of the aper-ture was 8.39� � 17.78�, and the distance between each peak was

Fig. 1. Schematic representation of stimuli and procedure in Experiment 1. The 2D–3D display comprised the alternation of 2D and 3D images. The 2D–2D display consistedof the alternation of two 2D images. The images were updated every 1 s, and alternations continued for 20 s.

1422 T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426

8.39�. When the peaks of the aperture were aligned with the black-and-white borders, five Y-junctions were formed. We created 3Dand two-dimensional (2D) images by manipulating the spatialrelationship between the black-and-white borders and the peaksof the aperture. The 3D image consisted of the black-and-whiteborders aligned with the peaks of the aperture, giving the appear-ance of a 3D folded surface. The 2D image consisted of black-and-white borders misaligned with the peaks of the aperture, whichdoes not create a 3D appearance.

Two kinds of test displays were made from these images. Thefirst display, the 2D–3D display, contained repeated alternationsof 2D and 3D images. In this display, the location of the black-and-white borders was modulated by 90� between the 2D and3D images, and the aligned (3D image) and misaligned (2D image)displays were alternated every 1 s. An adult observer could detectthe depth change within the 2D–3D display. The second display,the 2D–2D display, contained alternations of two different 2Dimages. In this display, the modulations of the black-and-whiteborders were identical to those of the 2D–3D display, but the peaksof the aperture were out of alignment by 0.70� from the bottom ofthe screen. The 2D–2D display was perceived as flat by adultobservers.

2.1.4. ProcedureThe infants sat on their parents’ laps, 40 cm from a CRT monitor.

Before the experimental session started, parents were instructed toclose their eyes during trials to prevent their looking behavior fromaffecting the data coding. At the beginning of each trial, a picture(13.99� � 17.34�) was presented at the center of the monitor witha sound to attract the infants’ attention. When the infants looked atthe center of the CRT, the trials were started. Two test displayswere presented side by side for 20 s. The experimental session con-

sisted of four trials. In half of the trials, the 2D–3D display ap-peared on the right side of the CRT. In the other half of the trials,the 2D–3D display appeared on the left side of the CRT. The orderof the four trials was pseudorandomized. Each infant received oneexperimental session.

2.1.5. Data coding and analysisOne observer measured the infants’ looking time for the 2D–3D

and 2D–2D displays based on an off-line video movie. The observerwas naive to the locations of the two test displays. The observer re-corded the infants’ looking time for the right or left presentationfield by pressing one of two keys when the infant was looking atthe relevant field. When the infant looked away from the presenta-tion field, no observation was recorded. Infants’ preference scorefor the 2D–3D display was calculated based on the ratio of thetimes they looked at the 2D–3D display relative to the times theylooked at both the 2D–3D and the 2D–2D displays. To examine thesignificance of the mean preference score for the 2D–3D display,we performed a two-tailed t-test versus chance (50%). We also con-ducted two-tailed t-test between age groups. To control the exper-imentwise error rates across Experiments 1 and 2, we correctedalpha levels by using Bonferroni procedure.

2.2. Results and discussion

Fig. 2 shows each age group’s mean preference score for the 2D–3D display. As this figure illustrates, 7- to 8-month-olds lookedlonger at the 2D–3D display than the 2D–2D display, whereas 5-to 6-month-olds did not exhibit a looking preference for the 2D–3D display. We conducted two-tailed t-tests versus chance (50%)for each age group. The results revealed that 7- and 8-month-oldinfants exhibited a significant preference for the 2D–3D display

Fig. 2. The mean preference score for the 2D–3D displays in Experiment 1 (a) andthe control 2D–3D displays in Experiment 2 (b). Error bars indicate the SEM acrossinfants.

T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426 1423

(t(14) = 4.50, p < .01), while 5- and 6-month-olds showed no pref-erence (t(14) = 0.36, n.s.). In addition, comparisons between youn-ger and older groups revealed significant differences in preferencebetween age groups (t(28) = 4.43, p < .05).

Our results indicate that 7- and 8-month-old infants prefer tolook at the display containing depth change defined by shadingand line junction cues. These results suggest that 7- and 8-month-old infants can use shading and line junction informationto discriminate two test displays. In contrast, the study found noevidence of this ability in 5- and 6-month-old infants.

Our findings are consistent with previous infant studies on per-ception of shape from shading and line junctions in a preferentialreaching task (shading: Granrud et al., 1985; T-junctions: Granrud& Yonas, 1984; Y-junctions: Yonas & Arterberry, 1994; curved Y-junctions: Kavšek, 1999). On the other hand, our findings seemto conflict with those that have found that 3-month-old infantscan detect a target defined by 3D features such as shading and linejunctions in visual search displays (Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Bhatt &Bertin, 2001; Bhatt & Waters, 1998). The current study used differ-ent stimuli that might require infants to process higher-level of 3Dstructure. Taken together, these findings suggest that there mightbe different levels of processing of pictorial depth in early infancy,and that the ability to process multiple depth cues changes be-tween 5 and 7 months of age.

However, there are two possible interpretations of these find-ings. One is that 7- and 8-month-old infants might simply detectthe Y-junctions as a depth cue without using shading information.We explored whether infants integrate information from bothshading and line junctions cues in Experiment 2. The other is thatthe procedure used in this experiment might not be valid for youn-ger groups of infants. To rule out this possibility, we examinedtheir ability to discriminate the spatial frequency of rectangular-wave gratings using the same procedure in Experiment 3.

3. Experiment 2: Inconsistency in shading and line junctioncues

Experiment 2 examined whether infants’ preference for 2D–3Ddisplay disappears when shading and line junction information areincompatible. We used double-cycle rectangular-wave gratings asa shading cue to impair the relationship between shading and linejunction cues. In the 3D image, half of the black-and-white borderswere aligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture, while the otherhalf of the borders and peaks were misaligned by 90�. Such an im-age produces weak depth impressions for adult observers,

although line junctions still appear. If infants indeed integrate 3Dinformation from shading and line junction cues, inconsistentinformation between cues would not benefit the infants’ discrimi-nation of displays.

3.1. Method

3.1.1. ParticipantsThe sample consisted of fifteen 7- and 8-month-old infants

(mean age = 220.3 days, SD = 19.75 days). All infants were full-term at birth and healthy at the time of testing. Participants wererecruited through newspaper advertisements, and the sample con-sisted of infants whose parents responded to a subsequent letterand volunteered to participate with their infant. Parents gave in-formed consent before the experimental sessions.

3.1.2. ApparatusThe apparatus in this experiment was identical to that used in

Experiment 1.

3.1.3. StimuliThe test displays are shown in Fig. 3. The test stimuli were iden-

tical to those used in Experiment 1 except that in Experiment 2, a0.35-cpd rectangular-wave grating was used as a shading cue. TheMichelson’s contrast of the grating was 96.1%, sufficient for thecontrast sensitivity in young infants (Atkinson et al., 1974). Inthe control 3D image, half of the black-and-white borders werealigned with the peaks of a serrated aperture, while the other halfof the borders were misaligned with the peaks. Such an image pro-duces weak depth impressions for the adult observer because theline junction cue is inconsistent with shading information. Thecontrol 2D image consisted of black-and-white borders misalignedwith the peaks of a serrated aperture. Based on the control 3D andthe control 2D images, we created a control 2D–3D display and acontrol 2D–2D display. If infants can use shading and line junctioninformation, and integrate these cues, they would not discriminatebetween the control 2D–3D display and the control 2D–2D dis-plays. If infants merely detect Y-junctions from the 2D–3D display,they would also look longer at the control 2D–3D display than atthe control 2D–2D display.

3.1.4. Procedure and data codingThe procedure and method of data coding were identical to

those of Experiment 1. We conducted two-tailed t-tests versuschance (50%) to demonstrate the infants’ discrimination of dis-plays. We also performed an unpaired t-test between the older in-fants group between Experiments 1 and 2 to compare the meanpreference score for the control 2D–3D display (Experiment 2) tothe 2D–3D display (Experiment 1).

3.2. Results and discussion

The results supported our prediction. As Fig. 2 illustrates, 7- and8-month-old infants did not look longer at the control 2D–3D dis-play than at the control 2D–2D display. We conducted two-tailed t-tests versus chance (50%). The results revealed that infants exhib-ited no significant preference for the control 2D–3D display(t(14) = �1.152, n.s.). In addition, 7- and 8-month-old infantsshowed a significant preference for the 2D–3D display in Experi-ment 1, while they showed no preference for the control 2D–3Ddisplay in Experiment 2. We conducted unpaired t-tests betweenthe 2D–3D display and the control 2D–3D display. The between-experiment comparisons in older age groups revealed that the 7-and 8-month-olds showed a significant preference for the 2D–3Ddisplay in Experiment 1 (t(28) = 4.48, p < .01). These results indi-cate that 7- and 8-month-old infants did not discriminate between

Fig. 3. Schematic representation of stimuli and procedure in Experiment 2. The control 2D–3D display comprised the alternation of 2D and 3D images. The control 2D–2Ddisplay consisted of the alternation of two 2D images. These images were identical to those used in Experiment 1 except for the addition of a double-cycle rectangular-wavegrating. The images were updated every 1 s, and alternations continued for 20 s.

1424 T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426

the control 2D–3D display and the control 2D–2D display, suggest-ing that line junction with inconsistent shading did not provide 3Dinformation for 7- and 8-month-old infants. These results suggestthat 7- and 8-month-old infants are sensitive to the relationshipbetween shading and line junction cues.

4. Experiment 3: The validity of the procedure in younger groupof infants

In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-month-old infants did not exhibit alooking preference. However, there is a possibility that the proce-dure used in Experiment 1 is not valid for younger infants. Weexamined younger infants’ ability to discriminate spatial frequencyof the rectangular-wave grating between two stimuli using thesame method as Experiments 1 and 2.

4.1. Method

4.1.1. ParticipantsThe sample consisted of fifteen 5- and 6-month-old infants

(mean age = 165.6 days, SD = 16.54 days). All infants were full-term at birth and healthy at the time of testing. Participantswere recruited through newspaper advertisements, and the sam-ple consisted of infants whose parents responded to a subse-quent letter and volunteered to participate with their infant.Parents gave informed consent before the experimental sessions.

4.1.2. ApparatusThe apparatus in this experiment was identical to that used in

Experiment 1.

4.1.3. StimuliWe used the 2D–2D display used in Experiment 1 and the con-

trol 2D–2D display used in Experiment 2 as the test displays. Theyconsisted of the images whose black-and-white borders misa-ligned with the peaks of a serrated aperture. Thus, both of themdid not create the three-dimensional impression, but the rectangu-lar-wave gratings of the control 2D–2D display had twice as highspatial frequency as that of the 2D–2D display. If infants can dis-criminate spatial frequency of displays under the current experi-mental procedure, they would expect to look longer at the higherspatial frequent display, that is, control 2D–2D display (used inExperiment 2) than at the 2D–2D display (used in Experiment 1).

4.1.4. Procedure and data codingThe procedure and method of data coding were identical to

those of Experiment 1. We conducted two-tailed t-tests versuschance (50%) to demonstrate the infants’ discrimination ofdisplays.

4.2. Results and discussion

The results supported our prediction. Five- and 6-month-old in-fants looked longer at the control 2D–2D display than at the 2D–2Ddisplay (57.34% ± 3.10% SEM). We conducted two-tailed t-tests ver-sus chance (50%). The results revealed that infants exhibited signif-icant preference for the control 2D–2D display (t(14) = 2.56,p < .05). These results indicate that 5- and 6-month-old infants dis-criminate between the 2D–2D display and the control 2D–2D dis-play, suggesting that the procedure used in this experiment worksin the case of younger group of infants. These results suggest that

T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426 1425

the failure of 5- and 6-month-old infants in Experiment 1 is notdue to the procedure.

5. General discussion

In the present study, we examined the sensitivity to shadingand line junction cues in human infants aged 5–8 months using anew type of stimulus. In Experiment 1, we presented a display con-taining depth change (2D–3D display) and a display without depthchange (2D–2D display) defined by shading and line junction cues.The 7- and 8-month-old infants discriminated between the twodisplays, while 5- and 6-month-old infants did not. In Experiment2, we used the double-cycle grating to impair shading informationas depth cues. We presented two displays without depth change(control 2D–3D display and control 2D–2D display) with shadinginformation inconsistent with line junction. The 7- and 8-month-old infants did not discriminate between the control 2D–3D dis-play and the control 2D–2D display. The findings of Experiment 3suggest that younger infants’ failure in Experiment 1 is not dueto the procedure used in this experiment. Summarizing the resultsof Experiments 1, 2, and 3, infants’ sensitivity to shading and linejunctions changes remarkably at around 5–8 months of age, and7- and 8-month-old infants discriminate displays based on shadingand line junction cues.

The fact that the 5- and 6-month-olds in Experiment 1 did notexhibit a preference for the 2D–3D display might be due to shortstimuli alternation duration. The young infants might not be ableto extract 3D structure from the test displays having rapid alterna-tion rate (1 Hz). However, Hou et al. (2006) showed that in adults,information about the relative alignment of the shading and linejunctions is processed in as quickly as 100 ms. Although no devel-opmental studies have been conducted on the temporal aspects ofpictorial depth perception in infants, the stimuli duration usedhere (500 ms) might be enough long to process 3D information.

The main finding of the present study is that the sensitivity toshading and line junctions dynamically change at around 5–8 months of ages. These results are consistent with most of previ-ous studies that examined pictorial depth perception, such asshading (Granrud et al., 1985), cast shadows (Imura et al., 2006;Yonas & Granrud, 2006), line junctions (T-junctions: Granrud & Yo-nas, 1984; Y-junctions: Yonas & Arterberry, 1994; curved Y-junc-tions: Kavšek, 1999), texture gradients (Arterberry et al., 1989),relative size (Yonas et al., 1985), and familiar size (Granrud et al.,1985). These previous and present results suggest that sensitivityto pictorial depth cues develops between 5 and 8 months of ages.

On the other hand, other studies have reported that 3- and 4-month-old infants are sensitive to differences in pictorial depth cues,such as line junction, shading, linear perspective, interposition, and acombination of those cues (line junction: Bhatt & Bertin, 2001; linearperspective: Durand, Lécuyer, & Frichtel, 2003; line junctions andinterposition: Shuwairi, Albert, & Johnson, 2007; line junction andshading: Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Bhatt & Waters, 1998). These resultsare obviously inconsistent with the present results. Here we discussabout possible interpretations for this inconsistency as below.

(1) Difference of experimental method

The 5- to 6-month-olds in the present study might show signif-icant sensitivity to depths cues, if they were tested with more sen-sitive experimental method such as familiarity/novelty preferenceprocedure or habituation/dishabituation method than the methodused here (preferential looking method). This may be one of theconvincing explanations for the inconsistency with the previousstudies. However, it should be noticed that some studies reportedsensitivities to various depth cues develop around 7–8 months of

age; even they used these sensitive methods (Granrud & Yonas,1984; Imura et al., 2006; Kavšek, 1999; Yonas & Arterberry,1994). Additionally, Shuwairi et al. (2007) have shown that 4-month-olds are sensitive to pictorial depth cues, even when the in-fants were tested by preferential looking procedure.

(2) Difference of processing levels for pictorial depth cues

Most studies reporting sensitivity to 3D features such as shad-ing and line junction in younger age used visual search displays(Bertin & Bhatt, 2006; Bhatt & Bertin, 2001; Bhatt & Waters,1998). Because the adult visual system processes 3D feature insuch display automatically and preattentively (e.g., Enns, 1992;Enns & Rensink, 1990, 1991; Kleffner & Ramachandran, 1992),the younger infants’ sensitivity reported in the visual search exper-iments might represent automatic and preattentive processing ofpictorial depth cues (see also Bhatt & Waters, 1998). The inconsis-tent results between the previous and present studies might reflectthat such automatic and preattentive processing of depth cues maydevelop faster than the other higher processing. Recent neural datashowed that there are multiple bases for processing pictorial depthcues in the human visual system (Taira et al., 2001). It is plausiblethat the lower visual areas which show sensitivity to visual searchdisplay (such as V1, V2, V3: e.g., see Humphrey et al., 1997; Lee,Yang, Romero, & Mumford, 2002) develop faster than the higher vi-sual areas (Intraparietal sulcus: see Taira et al., 2001). This ideaseems to well explain the inconsistency between the previousstudies. However, a recent study using no visual search display alsoshowed that 4-month-old infants could discriminate between apair of a possible and an impossible figure defined by line junctionsand interposition cues (Shuwairi et al., 2007). Thus, we cannot ex-plain the whole inconsistencies by differences in processing levelsbetween stimuli.

(3) Difference of 3D percept between stimuli

Another possible factor for the inconsistency between the pres-ent and previous studies is difference in 3D percept derived fromthe stimuli. The ‘‘2D–3D display” used in the current experimentscould provide ambiguous bi-stable 3D perception (see Hou et al.,2006). The stimuli can be perceived as both ‘‘appearance and disap-pearance of 3D corrugation” or ‘‘lateral motion of flat black-and-white borders behind an occluding gray aperture” (Hou et al.,2006). Such ambiguity of 3D percept might disturb the younger in-fants’ preference to 3D cues in the present study. Although the incon-sistent developmental periods reported in the present and previousstudies might be due to the experimental procedure and the differ-ences in visual processing (see the discussions above), the inconsis-tency might reflect difficulty of the younger infants to extract 3Dshape from the bi-stable stimuli used in our study.

As discussed above, there are several possibilities for the incon-sistency between the studies reporting developmental periodaround 7–8 months and that around 3–4 months. One or some ofthese possible factors might be responsible for the inconsistentdevelopmental periods reported in the previous studies. The mostplausible explanation in the above three may be the last one, becausethere are counter evidence given by Shuwairi et al. (2007) for theother possibilities. Therefore, the present results might reflect that7–8 month-old infants can discriminate between 2D and 3D displaysdefined by pictorial depth cues even from ambiguous stimuli.

References

Arterberry, M., Yonas, A., & Bensen, A. S. (1989). Self-produced locomotion and thedevelopment of responsiveness to linear perspective and texture gradients.Developmental Psychology, 25, 976–982.

1426 T. Imura et al. / Vision Research 48 (2008) 1420–1426

Atkinson, J., Braddick, O. J., & Braddick, F. (1974). Acuity and contrast sensitivity ofinfant vision. Nature, 247, 403–404.

Bertin, E., & Bhatt, R. S. (2006). Three-month-olds’ sensitivity to orientation cues inthe three-dimensional depth plane. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93,45–62.

Bhatt, R. S., & Bertin, E. (2001). Pictorial cues and three-dimensional informationprocessing in early infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80,315–332.

Bhatt, R. S., & Waters, S. E. (1998). Perception of three-dimensional cues in earlyinfancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 207–224.

Bruno, N., & Cutting, J. E. (1988). Minimodularity and the perception of layout.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 161–170.

Curran, W., & Johnston, A. (1994). Integration of shading and texture cues: Testingthe linear model. Vision Research, 34, 1863–1874.

Durand, K., Lécuyer, R., & Frichtel, M. (2003). Representation of the third dimension:The use of perspective cues by 3- and 4-month-old infants. Infant Behavior andDevelopment, 26, 151–166.

Enns, J. T. (1992). Sensitivity of early human vision to 3-D orientation in line-drawings. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 143–169.

Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (1990). Influence of scene-based properties on visualsearch. Science, 247, 721–723.

Enns, J. T., & Rensink, R. A. (1991). Preattentive recovery of threedimensional orientation from line drawings. Psychological Review, 98,335–351.

Gibson, J. J. (1950). Perception of the visual world. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Granrud, C. E., Haake, R. J., & Yonas, A. (1985). Infants’ sensitivity to familiar size:

The effect of memory on spatial perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 37,459–466.

Granrud, C. E., & Yonas, A. (1984). Infants’ perception of pictoriallyspecified interposition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37,500–511.

Granrud, C. E., Yonas, A., & Opland, E. A. (1985). Infants’ sensitivity to the depth cueof shading. Perception & Psychophysics, 37, 415–419.

Hou, C., Pettet, M. W., Vildavski, V. Y., & Norcia, A. M. (2006). Neural correlates ofshape-from-shading. Vision Research, 46, 1080–1090.

Humphrey, G. K., Goodale, M. A., Bowen, C. V., Gati, J. S., Vilis, T., Rutt, B. K., et al.(1997). Differences in perceived shape from shading correlate with activity inearly visual areas. Current Biology, 7, 144–147.

Imura, T., Yamaguchi, M. K., Kanazawa, S., Shirai, N., Otsuka, Y., Tomonaga, M., et al.(2006). Perception of motion trajectory of object from the moving cast shadowin infants. Vision Research, 46, 652–657.

Kavšek, M. J. (1999). Infants’ responsiveness to line junctions in curved objects.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 177–192.

Kleffner, D. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1992). On the perception of shape fromshading. Perception & Psychophysics, 52, 18–36.

Lee, T. S., Yang, C. F., Romero, R. D., & Mumford, D. (2002). Neural activity in earlyvisual cortex refracts behavioral experience and higher-order perceptualsaliency. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 589–597.

Maloney, L. T., & Landy, M. S. (1989). A statistical framework for robust fusion ofdepth information. Proceedings of the SPIE: Visual Communications and ImageProcessing, 1199(Part 2), 1154–1163.

Shuwairi, S. M., Albert, M. K., & Johnson, S. P. (2007). Discrimination of possible andimpossible objects in infancy. Psychological Science, 18, 303–307.

Taira, M., Nose, I., Inoue, K., & Tsutsui, K. (2001). Cortical areas related to attention to3D surface structures based on shading: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 14,959–966.

Tsutsui, K., Jiang, M., Yara, K., Sakata, H., & Taira, M. (2001). Integration ofperspective and disparity cues in surface-orientation-selective neurons of areaCIP. Journal of Neurophysiology, 86, 2856–2867.

Tsutsui, K., Sakata, H., Naganuma, T., & Taira, M. (2002). Neural correlates forperception of 3D surface orientation from texture gradient. Science, 298,409–412.

Tsutsui, K., Taira, M., & Sakata, H. (2005). Neural mechanisms of three-dimensionalvision. Neuroscience Research, 51, 221–229.

Yonas, A., & Arterberry, M. E. (1994). Infants perceive spatial structure specified byline junctions. Perception, 23, 1427–1435.

Yonas, A., & Granrud, C. E. (2006). Infants’ perception of depth from cast shadows.Perception & Psychophysics, 68, 154–160.

Yonas, A., Granrud, C. E., & Pettersen, L. (1985). Infants’ sensitivity to relative sizeinformation for distance. Developmental Psychology, 21, 161–167.