Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approach to buffering role conflict and...

19
This article was downloaded by: [Erasmus University] On: 27 May 2013, At: 05:10 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The International Journal of Human Resource Management Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rijh20 Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approach to buffering role conflict and physical stressors Margarita Mayo a , Juan I. Sanchez b , Juan C. Pastor a & Alfredo Rodriguez c a IE Business School, Madrid, Spain b Florida International University, Miami, FL, USA c Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain Published online: 18 Apr 2012. To cite this article: Margarita Mayo , Juan I. Sanchez , Juan C. Pastor & Alfredo Rodriguez (2012): Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approach to buffering role conflict and physical stressors, The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 23:18, 3872-3889 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585192.2012.676930 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Transcript of Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approach to buffering role conflict and...

This article was downloaded by: [Erasmus University]On: 27 May 2013, At: 05:10Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

The International Journal of HumanResource ManagementPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rijh20

Supervisor and coworker support:a source congruence approach tobuffering role conflict and physicalstressorsMargarita Mayo a , Juan I. Sanchez b , Juan C. Pastor a & AlfredoRodriguez ca IE Business School, Madrid, Spainb Florida International University, Miami, FL, USAc Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid,Madrid, SpainPublished online: 18 Apr 2012.

To cite this article: Margarita Mayo , Juan I. Sanchez , Juan C. Pastor & Alfredo Rodriguez (2012):Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approach to buffering role conflict andphysical stressors, The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 23:18, 3872-3889

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585192.2012.676930

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representationthat the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of anyinstructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primarysources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Supervisor and coworker support: a source congruence approachto buffering role conflict and physical stressors†

Margarita Mayoa*, Juan I. Sanchezb, Juan C. Pastora and Alfredo Rodriguezc

aIE Business School, Madrid, Spain; bFlorida International University, Miami, FL, USA;cFacultad de Psicologıa, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

The buffering effects of supervisor support on the stressor–strain relationship haveproven elusive in prior research (Beehr, Farmer, Glazer, Gudanowski and Nair (2003),‘The Enigma of Social Support and Occupational Stress: Source Congruence andGender Role Effects,’ Journal of Occupational and Health Psychology, 8, 220–231).We built on emerging work on source congruence and conservation of resource theoryto test a series of hypotheses intended to clarify these mixed findings. Using a sample of768 employees from 45 organizations in North America, results from moderatedregression analyses, showed that the effects of supervisor support on the stressor–strainrelationship depended on source congruence. In accordance with our predictions,although we found buffering effects for the physical stressors–strain relationship, wefound a reverse buffering effect for the role conflict–strain relationship. Thesedifferential buffering effects did not emerge when considering coworker support. Wediscuss the implications of our results for shedding light on the mixed evidenceregarding buffering work stressors reported in prior research.

Keywords: coworker support; physical stressors; role conflict; source congruence;stress; supervisor support

Introduction

Social support has been heralded as a resource that helps employees cope with work

stressors, which are the stimuli that induce the stress process and ultimately provoke

employee strains (Sosik and Godshalk 2000; Searle, Bright and Bochner 2001; van

Dierendonck, Haynes, Borrill and Stride 2004). Theoretical models involving both main

and interaction effects have been advanced to explain the role that supervisor support plays

in the work stressor–strain relationship (Carlson and Perrewe 1999; Cooper, Dewe and

O’Driscoll 2001). According to the main effects model, supervisor support directly

reduces the level of tension, dissatisfaction and other forms of strain, regardless of the

level of stressors. On the other hand, the moderating or buffering hypothesis postulates

an interaction effect whereby supervisor support attenuates the impact of stressors on

strains. In other words, there should be a strong stressor–strain relationship when

supervisor support is low and a weak relationship when supervisor support is high.

Unlike the main effects model, which has held up in individual studies (e.g. Blau 1981;

Beehr 1985; Ganster, Fusilier and Mayer 1986; Fried and Tiegs 1993) as well as in a

meta-analysis of the work stress literature (Viswesvaran, Sanchez and Fisher 1999),

ISSN 0958-5192 print/ISSN 1466-4399 online

q 2012 Taylor & Francis

http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09585192.2012.676930

http://www.tandfonline.com

†An earlier version of this paper was published in the Best Paper Proceedings of the Academy ofManagement Conference in 2007.

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

The International Journal of Human Resource Management,

Vol. 23, No. 18, October 2012, 3872–3889

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

evidence of the moderating effects of supervisor support has been, in the words of Beehr

et al. (2003, p. 220), ‘rather tenuous, inconsistent and unclear’. That is, although several

studies have supported this ‘buffering effect’ suggesting that supervisor support reduces

the impact of stressors on strain (e.g. Beehr, King and King 1990; Dorman and Zapf 1999;

Sargent and Terry 2000; Beehr et al. 2003), others have either failed to detect it (e.g. Blau

1981; Ganster et al. 1986; Fried and Tiegs 1993; Beehr, Jex, Stacy and Murray 2000) or

found rather weak support (Dorman and Zapf 1999). Furthermore, still other studies have

reported an intriguing ‘reverse buffering’ effect, which suggested that support may at

times exacerbate, rather than weaken, the impact of stressors on strains (e.g. Kobasa and

Puccetti 1983; Kaufmann and Beehr 1986; Glaser, Tatum, Nebeker, Sorenson and Aiello

1999; Kickul and Posig 2001; Deelstra et al. 2003). Our study attempts to reconcile these

apparent contradictory results by investigating how the congruence between the sources of

stressors and of social support makes a difference in determining the ameliorating effect of

social support on the relationship between stressors and strain. The notion of source

congruence, first advanced by Blau (1981) and later Kaufmann and Beehr (1986), refers to

the congruence between sources of the stressor and of social support. In particular, we

examine two sources of stressors – role conflict and physical stressors – and two sources

of support – supervisor and coworker. From a both theoretical and practical standpoint, it

seems important to understand why support from the supervisor, a frequently offered

prescription in the practitioners press, does not alleviate employee strain in every situation.

Researchers have called for theoretically guided efforts to disentangle the seemingly

inconsistent findings concerning the buffering effects of supervisor support (Beehr et al.

2003). Although Cohen and Wills (1985) contended that, to be effective, the kind of

support should have a good fit with the focal stressor, the extant research does not seem to

provide a convincing, unambiguous set of criteria capable of explaining when and how

stressors and support fit each other. Indeed, Viswesvaran, Sanchez and Fisher’s meta-

analysis (1999) concluded that the type of support thought to fit the specific type of stressor

did not yield larger moderator effects than types of support that did not seemingly fit the

stressor. Beehr et al. (2003) also noted that researchers have had a great deal of difficulty

predicting whether or not a specific type of support will buffer a given stressor. Along

these same lines, de Jonge and Dormann (2006) concluded that ‘inconsistencies in

demonstrating interaction effects between stressors and resources represent one of the

major threats to prominent models on organizational stress’ (p. 1369).

The study described herein relies on emerging work on source congruence and

conservation of resource (COR) theory to predict the extent to which the buffering effect of

supervisor support depends on the congruence between the resource (e.g. supervisor

support) and the source of the specific stressor. In particular, we examined a work stressor

that is likely to be congruent with or emanate from primarily the supervisor (e.g. role

conflict), together with a stressor (e.g. physical stressors), which tends to be incongruent

with the supervisor because it typically falls beyond the supervisor’s control. Then, we

compared the results of supervisor support with those of coworker support to provide a more

robust test of the source congruence hypothesis. An additional strength of our study is that, in

contrast to prior studies of buffering effects, it employed a relatively large sample size that

afforded us sufficient statistical power to detect even small stressor by support interactions.

Theoretical background

Social support generally refers to the individual belief that help would be available from

other people in difficult situations (Cobb 1976). Support may come from different sources.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3873

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

There are work-related and non-work related sources of support, such as supervisors,

coworkers and family (Halbesleben 2006). Here, we focus on work-related sources of

support because they are more closely related to the stressors of interest – work conflict

and poor physical conditions. Family and non-work related sources might be most relevant

when considering buffering stressors such as work–family conflict. Traditionally, it was

thought that social support, independently of the source, was a strong remedy against job

stressors. However, Blau (1981) was the first to suggest that sources of support may need

to be independent of sources of stressors because support coming from the same source

that is provoking the stressor might be awkward and unproductive. Similarly, Kaufmann

and Beehr (1986) also called for further examination of the congruence between the source

of the stressor and the source of support as an explanation for the contradictory buffering

effects of social support. Beehr et al. (2003) further explicated the theoretical rationale

behind this source congruence hypothesis by arguing that, when both support and stressors

originate in the same source (e.g. the supervisor), two conflicting or dissonant cognitions

may arise about this source, hence augmenting rather than buffering the impact of stressors

on strains. However, Beehr et al. (2003) did not find consistent support for the source

congruence hypothesis, which had anticipated that such congruence would be more

harmful than helpful, which led them to call for additional research on alternative forms of

support–stressor congruence. In line with this call for further research, recent revisions of

the COR theory (Hobfoll 2001; Hobfoll and Kay 2007) focusing on employee appraisals of

resources may help us sort out the boundary conditions under which supervisor support has

a buffering effect from those under which it has a reverse buffering effect.

The basic tenet of COR theory (Hobfoll 1989, 1998; Hobfoll and Shirom 2000) is that

strain is most likely to occur when (a) there is a perceived threat to one’s resources, (b) there

is an actual resource loss or (c) the anticipated return on investment of one’s resources does

not materialize. This theory proposes that people seek to acquire and maintain resources and

stress arises because there is a threat of a loss of resources, an actual loss in resources or lack

of an expected gain in resources. The term ‘resources’ refers to objects, conditions, personal

characteristics and energies that are valued by the individual (Hobfoll 1989). Thus, stressors

threaten valuable employee resources such as status, position, self-esteem and energy.

Hobfoll (1998) designated social support as one of the most important resources for dealing

with stressors, since it can both widen one’s pool of available resources and can replace or

reinforce other resources that have been lacking. That is, supervisor support may promote a

positive sense of self and a sense of security that in turn facilitates the preservation of these

and other valued resources, thereby preventing strain despite the threat represented by work

stressors (Hobfoll 1989). COR theory also incorporates the notion of resource appraisal

(Hobfoll 2001; Hobfoll and Kay 2007), suggesting that employees assess the extent to

which the resource (e.g. source of support) matches the stressor. Thus, for supervisor

support to have a buffering effect on the stressor–strain relationship, it needs to be

perceived by the subordinate as fitting the specific type of stressor. In particular, job-related

strain involves both mental and physical health over a relative long period of time (6 or 12

months). Following previous studies on strain (e.g. Spreitzer, Kizilos and Nason 1997;

Sprigg, Jackson and Parker 2000), we consider two aspects of job-related strain, namely

job-related tension or anxiety and medical physical symptoms.

To test this argument regarding fit, we focused on two stressors that have been

traditionally associated with employee strain, namely role conflict and physical working

conditions. On the one hand, role conflict, which has received a great deal of attention for

over four decades (e.g. Katz and Kahn 1966; Rizzo, House and Lirtzman 1970; Perrewe

et al. 2004), occurs when individuals experience two or more sets of incompatible role

M. Mayo et al.3874

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

demands so that fulfilling one impedes the fulfilment of the other(s). Research has shown

that role conflict is closely related to the supervisor (O’Driscoll and Beehr 1994) and it is

associated with a number of dysfunctional outcomes, such as job dissatisfaction and

psychological strains (e.g. Jackson and Schuler 1985). On the other hand, poor working

conditions (Sutton and Rafaeli 1987; McCoy and Evans 2005), such as extreme

temperatures, inappropriate lighting, noise, crowding, and lack of space and privacy,

according to ergonomics research, are the result of occupational and organizational design,

which are typically beyond the control of the supervisor (Humphrey, Nahrgang and

Morgeson 2007; Dierdorff and Ellington 2008; Fritz and Sonnentag 2009). We have

focused on these two specific resources because they are viewed to have their origin at

differing sources, hence allowing us to test the source congruence hypothesis. In particular,

we expect role conflict to be originated from the supervisor, whereas physical stressor

would be originated from the organization. Thus, role conflict is source-congruent with

supervisor support, but physical stressors are not.

Hypotheses

Because one of the supervisor’s main responsibilities is to shape the employee’s role

expectations, employees are therefore likely to perceive their supervisor as the main source

of conflicting role demands (O’Driscoll and Beehr 1994). This line of thinking is consistent

with the fact that ‘the boss’ dominates anecdotal reports of complaints about conflicting

work assignments (e.g. Kellerman 2004). Thus, because role conflict and supervisor

support are likely to emanate from the same source (i.e. the supervisor), supervisor support

seems particularly prone to magnify the negative impact of this stressor (e.g. Rizzo, House

and Lirtman 1970; Perrewe et al. 2004). According to Kaufmann and Beehr (1986), when a

supervisor who is also the source of stress approaches employees to offer his/her support,

‘even this “friendly” approach may be experienced as stressful’ (p. 524). Under such

circumstances, supervisor support may be perceived as inconsistent and insincere (Kickul

and Posig 2001), and it may even provoke self-perceptions of incompetence in the

subordinate (Blaine, Crocker and Major 1995). Thus, we propose the following source

congruence hypothesis between supervisor support and role conflict.

Hypothesis 1: Supervisor support will have a reverse buffering effect on the relationship

between role conflict and strain so that role conflict will be more strongly

associated with strains (i.e. job tension and medical symptoms) when

supervisor support is high than when it is low.

In contrast, when stressors are typically attributed to the nature of the job, as it is the

case for poor physical working conditions (Sutton and Rafaeli 1987; McCoy and Evans

2005; Dierdorff and Ellington 2008), employees attribute less responsibility to supervisors

for the hardships of the physical environment. That is, even though managers may

influence employees’ individual working conditions through differential task assignments,

they are unlikely to control the fundamental physical conditions that define the job. Also,

supervisors know about these conditions and, therefore, may provide tips and support to

cope with this type of physical stressor (Beehr et al. 2003). Thus, we propose a source

incongruence hypothesis between supervisor support and physical stressors.

Hypothesis 2: Supervisor support will have a buffering effect on the relationship

between physical stressors and strain so that physical stressors will be

less strongly associated with strains (job tension and medical symptoms)

when supervisor support is high than when it is low.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3875

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

To provide a more comprehensive test of our theory, we compared the impact of

supervisor support with that of coworker support. Our reasoning for including coworker

support was twofold. First, support from coworkers is an important source of social

support for employees and therefore it is often included in this literature (Viswesvaran

et al. 1999). Second, although coworkers are certainly important role senders and can

influence the speed and coordination of work tasks, they do not typically have the authority

to shape employee work assignments in the manner that supervisors do. Therefore,

because employees are unlikely to attribute stressors such as role conflict or working

conditions to coworkers, one should not expect a reverse buffering effect for coworkers’

support. Instead, supportive coworkers may indeed buffer the effects of role conflict on

strain because they help employees vent their frustrations, provide fresh ideas that help

employees cope with conflicting role demands and coach employees to develop new

coping mechanisms. In line with these arguments, Beehr et al. (2003) found that coworker

support moderated the relationship between workload and psychological strain. Thus, we

expect a buffering effect.

Hypothesis 3: Coworker support will have a buffering effect on the relationship

between role conflict and strain so that role conflict will be less strongly

associated with strain (i.e. job tension and medical symptoms) when

coworker support is high than when it is low.

In addition, support from coworkers should also help neutralize the negative

consequences of physical stressors. Even though coworkers might influence working

conditions, they are hardly responsible for the general physical conditions at work, such as

the size of work stations, amount of storage space, visual privacy, air quality and

temperature. Coworkers can certainly be a source of noise and distraction (Oldham

and Rotchford 1983), but even if that is the case, work settings in which people encounter

frequent contact with others facilitate communication and interpersonal bonds (Oldham

and Brass 1979), which help reduce strain. For instance, there is ample evidence that

frequent communication with peers is related to higher levels of job satisfaction (Brass,

Galaskiewicz, Greve and Tsai 2004; Balkundi and Harrison 2006). Furthermore, a

supportive group of coworkers should help alleviate physical stressors because coworkers

share a common understanding of the same physical environment. Thus, we expect the

following buffering effect.

Hypothesis 4: Coworker support will have a buffering effect on the relationship

between physical stressors and strain so that physical stressors will be less

strongly associated with strain (i.e. job tension and medical symptoms)

when coworker support is high than when it is low.

Method

Participants

The data for this study were collected from 45 service and manufacturing organizations of

various sizes in the northeastern USA and Canada. In total, 768 completed surveys were

returned out of the 1291 distributed to employees at all job levels, yielding a response rate

of 59%. About half of participants were male (55.6%). Thirty percent were between 20 and

30 years of age, and 27% were between 30 and 40. Twenty-seven percent had an average

tenure with their organization of between 2 and 5 years, and 41% had their current

supervisors for the last 2–5 years.

M. Mayo et al.3876

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Data collection procedure

After initial contacts (via students enrolled in an MBA program at a large university in the

northeastern USA), requests to participate in a large-scale study were sent to supervisors

at 45 companies. These supervisors were asked to request their subordinates to participate

in the study and distribute the questionnaires. Completed surveys were returned in a

sealed envelope to the researchers. Employees received an explanation of the objectives

of the study and instructions for filling out the survey at a meeting of their work group

held during working hours. The work groups were defined as those employees reporting to

the same supervisor. Following this introduction, participants were asked to participate in

the study voluntarily by completing the survey, and they were also assured of the

anonymity of their responses. The purpose and procedure of the study were stated as

follows:

We are interested in learning more about the job situations of people working in variousindustries located within the Western New York and Southern Ontario Region. This surveyfocuses on three areas: (1) the potentially stressful aspects of your job, (2) your own senseof health and well-being, and (3) your supervisor’s style of interacting with you. To makethings easier and less time consuming, you will be asked to respond to a series of standardquestions and statements concerning each of these three areas. In all cases, you need onlywrite in the number which comes closest to providing the most accurate picture of you,your job and its context. Rest assured that your responses will remain completelyconfidential and anonymous. Your completed survey will be turned over to researchersat the University for analyses. No one will ever be able to connect your name with yourresponses.

We used a survey design because we were interested in targeting attitudinal outcomes

across a large sample of organizations in order to increase the generalizability of our

results. In addition, a survey design allowed us to collect data via well-tested scales in the

literature that provide reliable and valid measures of the theoretical constructs.

Measures

Social support. We assessed social support via the social support scale developed by

Caplan, Cobb, French, Van Harrison and Pinneau (1975). Items asked participants to

evaluate the extent to which their supervisor and coworkers provided social support.

The four items are: (1) How much does each of these people go out of their way to do

things to make your life easier for you? (2) How easy is it to talk with each of the following

people? (3) How much can each of these people be relied on when things get tough at

work? And (4) How much is each of the following people willing to listen to your personal

problems? For each question, participants evaluated the extent to which their supervisor

and their coworkers provided support, using a 5-point Likert scale ranging from ‘not at all’

(1) to ‘almost always’ (5). Each set of four items was summed into a global index of

support for each of the two sources. Cronbach’s a reliabilities were 0.87 and 0.82 for

supervisor and coworker support, respectively.

Role conflict. We assessed role conflict through the stress diagnostic survey developed

by Ivancevich and Matteson (1980). The five items included in this scale are as follows:

(1) I work on unnecessary tasks or projects, (2) I get caught in the middle between my

supervisor and my subordinates, (3) The formal chain of command is not adhered to,

(4) I do things on the job that are accepted by one person and not by others and (5) I receive

conflicting requests from two or more people. Respondents indicated the frequency of this

stressor using a 5-point scale ranging from ‘never’ (1) to ‘frequently if not always’ (5).

The scale’s Cronbach’s a was 0.73.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3877

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Physical stressors. We measured physical stressors using Sutton and Rafaeli’s (1987)

18-item scale, which assesses the degree to which employees perceive their working

conditions (such as temperature, quality of light and space) as adequate. Answers ranged

from ‘very inadequate’ (1) to ‘very adequate’ (5). Cronbach’s a was 0.87.

Strain. Two forms of strain were assessed, namely job tension and medical symptoms.

Job tension was measured using the seven-item scale included in House and Rizzo’s

(1972) anxiety–stress questionnaire. This scale measures individuals’ feelings of irritation

and tension (e.g. ‘My job tends to directly affect my health’, ‘I work under a great deal of

tension’). The Cronbach’s a for the scale in this study was 0.85. To assess the degree to

which employees experienced medical symptoms, we provided participants with the list of

physical ailments in the Quick and Quick’s (1984) stress survey, which includes

headaches, nervousness, poor appetite, constipation, restlessness, cold sweats, heart

beating hard and hands trembling. Participants reported the frequency with which they had

experienced these medical symptoms in the past 12 months using a 5-point scale ranging

from ‘never’ (1) to ‘frequently if not always’ (5). The Cronbach’s a for the scale in this

study was 0.80.

Control variables. We controlled for employees’ age, gender, years of experience at

the company, years worked with the supervisor and dispositional optimism. Age and years

of experience at the company might provide employees with solid and stable social

support networks, including the relationship with their supervisors. For example, age has

been positively related to job and mental well-being (Siu, Spector, Cooper and Donald

2001). Gender is another variable that has been related to stress and social support. Men

and women often choose different career paths (Stroth and O’Reilly 1999) that lead to

different kinds of work stressors (Alvesson and Billing 1997) and develop different social

support networks (Ibarra 1992, 1993; McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Cook 2001).

Similarly, we expect the number of years working with the supervisor to have a positive

effect on the quality of the relationship, as interpersonal problems between leaders and

followers will result in attrition. Finally, we included dispositional optimism to control for

the potential effect of negative affectivity (Watson, Pennebaker and Folger 1987).

Dispositional optimism has been found to buffer the relationship between stressors and

strain (Scheier and Carver 1985, 1992), and it has also shown a positive relationship with

social support (Brissette, Scheier and Carver 2002). We measured dispositional optimism

using the nine-item life orientation test (Scheier and Carver 1985). Each item is scored on

a 5-point scale ranging from ‘not at all true’ (1) to ‘frequently if not always true’ (5), and

the scale yielded a Cronbach’s a of 0.68.

Measurement model

To evaluate the extent to which our findings were influenced by common method

variance, we followed the procedure recommended by Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, and

Podsakoff (2003) and employed by Ahuja, Chudoba, Kacmar, McKnight and George

(2007) and by Carlson and Kacmar (2000). Following this approach, we estimated three

models: (1) a null measurement model, (2) a multifactor trait measurement model with

the proposed latent constructs and (3) a trait measurement model with an additional

method factor. If a common method effect existed, model 3 would fit the data

significantly better than model 2. Then we would need to determine the amount of

variance in the model contributed by the single method factor. To do that, we computed

the average variance extracted (AVE) for the latent constructs against the method factor.

It has been argued that, to rule out the presence of pervasive method variance, the

M. Mayo et al.3878

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

variance explained by the method factor should be less than 25% of the total variance

(Williams, Cote and Buckley 1989).

Three models were estimated through confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) using AMOS

7.0 (Arbuckle 2006). The results of the CFA were as follows: null model, x 2 ¼ 9125,

df ¼ 1540, p , 0.001; Tucker–Lewis index, TLI ¼ 0.38, comparative fit index,

CFI ¼ 0.40, root mean square error of approximation, RMSEA ¼ 0.10; multifactor trait

model with eight factors, x 2 ¼ 3342, df ¼ 1488, p , 0.001; TLI ¼ 0.90, CFI ¼ 0.91,

RMSEA ¼ 0.05; and multifactor trait model with a single method factor, x 2 ¼ 3123,

df ¼ 1480, p , 0.001; TLI ¼ 0.92, CFI ¼ 0.93, RMSEA ¼ 0.04. The eight-factor model

provided a good fit to the data, with RMSEA scores below 0.06, whereas TLI and CFI were

above 0.90 (Bentler and Bonnet 1980). The loadings from model 3 were then used to

compute the AVE for each latent construct, including the method factor. The results

indicated that, although model 3 fitted the data better than model 2 (Dx 2 ¼ 219, df ¼ 8),

the method factor accounted for only 16% of the total variance – less than the 25% cut-off

recommended by Williams et al. (1989). Thus, we concluded that common method

variance did not significantly contaminate the results.

Data analyses

To test the hypotheses, we used moderated hierarchical regression analyses computed in

accordance with the procedure delineated by Aiken and West (1991). To test the ‘reverse

buffering effect’, we first computed the interaction term or cross-product between

supervisor support and role conflict. We expected that the sign of this interaction would be

positive when predicting strain, such that the relationship between role conflict and strain

would be stronger under high supervisor support. In contrast, to test the ‘buffering effect’,

we computed three interaction or cross-product terms: supervisor support and physical

stressors, coworker support and role conflict, and coworkers support and physical stressors.

According to our hypotheses, we expected a negative sign of these interactions when

predicting strain. The statistical significance of the interaction or moderation effect was

assessed after controlling for all main effects. Control variables (demographic variables,

optimism) were entered first in the models, followed by the main effects of physical

stressors and role conflict. The two sources of social support were entered in the next step,

and the interaction terms in the final step.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics, reliabilities and correlations for all variables are reported in Table 1.

The bivariate correlations indicated that the relationships between sources of social support

and strain were negative. Supervisor support had negative and statistically significant

correlations with job tension (r ¼ 20.27, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms (r ¼ 20.27,

p , 0.01). The same correlation pattern emerged for coworker support, although

correlations with job tension (r ¼ 20.21, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms (r ¼ 20.21,

p , 0.01) were smaller. Role conflict had positive and statistically significant correlations

with job tension (r ¼ 0.46, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms (r ¼ 0.36, p , 0.01).

Physical stressors had positive and statistically significant correlations with job tension

(r ¼ 0.26, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms (r ¼ 0.24, p , 0.01). In addition, optimism

had a negative correlation with job tension (r ¼ 20.25, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms

(r ¼ 20.36, p , 0.01).

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3879

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Tab

le1

.D

escr

ipti

ve

stat

isti

cs,

reli

abil

ity

and

corr

elat

ion

sam

on

gst

ud

yv

aria

ble

s.

Variables

MSD

12

34

56

78

910

11

1.

Ag

e2

.24

1.0

9–

2.

Gen

der

(1m

;2

f)1

.44

0.5

00

.01

–3

.Y

ears

atth

eco

mp

any

2.9

31

.40

0.6

12

0.1

2–

4.

Yea

rsw

ith

sup

erv

iso

r1

.88

0.9

20

.32

0.1

00

.32

–5

.O

pti

mis

m3

.58

0.6

62

0.1

42

0.0

70

.09

0.0

2(0

.68

)6

.P

hy

sica

lst

ress

ors

2.7

10

.66

20

.11

0.0

82

0.0

72

0.0

42

0.1

8(0

.87

)7

.R

ole

con

flic

t2

.13

0.7

22

0.0

62

0.0

60

.04

20

.02

20

.20

0.3

3(0

.73

)8

.S

up

erv

iso

rysu

pp

ort

3.4

21

.02

0.0

92

0.0

12

0.0

00

.08

0.2

22

0.2

92

0.3

6(0

.87

)9

.C

ow

ork

ers

sup

po

rt3

.56

0.8

60

.09

0.0

40

.03

0.1

00

.20

20

.27

20

.25

0.4

6(0

.82

)1

0.

Job

ten

sio

n2

.21

0.8

00

.07

0.0

30

.18

0.0

92

0.2

50

.26

0.4

62

0.2

72

0.2

1(0

.85

)1

1.

Med

ical

sym

pto

ms

1.8

00

.59

20

.14

0.1

72

0.0

42

0.0

82

0.3

60

.24

0.3

62

0.2

72

0.2

10

.52

(0.8

0)

No

te.

Co

effi

cien

ta

’sap

pea

ral

ong

the

dia

go

nal

inp

aren

thes

es.

Co

rrel

atio

ns

gre

ater

than

0.0

7ar

esi

gn

ifica

nt

atp,

0.0

5;

corr

elat

ion

sg

reat

erth

an0

.10

are

sig

nifi

can

tat

,0

.01.

Ag

e,1¼

20

or

less

,2¼

30

s,3¼

40

s,4¼

50

s,5¼

60

so

rm

ore

;g

end

er,

mal

e,2¼

fem

ale;

yea

rsat

the

com

pan

yan

dy

ears

wo

rked

wit

hsu

per

vis

or,

less

than

2,

2–

5,

6–

10

,4¼

11

–1

5,

more

than

15

.

M. Mayo et al.3880

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Tests of hypotheses

Table 2 shows that the interaction terms predicted incremental variance beyond main

effects for the two dependent variables (job tension and medical symptoms). To minimize

the threat of multicollinearity, we centered all independent variables before entering them

into the regression equations (Aiken and West 1991). No evidence of multicollinearity was

detected, according to Kleinbaum, Kupper and Mueller’s (1988) criteria for variance

inflation factor (VIF) and tolerance criteria. Also, the Durbin–Watson statistics for all

analyses indicated that errors were not correlated (Ryan 1997).

Buffering effects of supervisor support. Hypothesis 1 predicted a reverse buffering

effect of supervisor support on the relationship between role conflict and strain.

The supervisor support £ role conflict interaction emerged as a positive and statistically

significant predictor of job tension (b ¼ 0.12, p , 0.01) and medical symptoms

(b ¼ 0.07, p , 0.05). To further examine whether the nature of this interaction conformed

to the reverse buffering effects of supervisor support predicted by our hypotheses, we

plotted the interactions using Aiken and West’s (1991) procedures. Figure 1(a), (b) shows

that, as Hypothesis 1 predicted, the association of role conflict with both job tension and

medical symptoms was stronger when supervisory support was high than when it was low.

In contrast, Hypothesis 2 predicted a positive buffering effect for supervisor support

when the stressor is the physical environment. As we expected, the unfavorable or reverse

buffering effect of supervisor support was limited to the experience of role conflict and did

not apply to the effects of physical stressors on strain. Indeed, there was a significant

buffering effect of supervisor support on the relationship between physical stressors and

medical symptoms (b ¼ 20.08, p , 0.05, Figure 1(c)). That is, the negative consequences

of physical stressors on medical symptoms were weaker when supervisory support was high

than when it was low. Although statistically non-significant, a similar trend was detected for

job tension.

Buffering effects of coworker support. Hypotheses 3 and 4 predicted that support from

coworkers would buffer the negative effects of both role conflict and physical stressors.

Table 2 shows that the coworker support £ role conflict interaction term had a negative

and statistically significant relationship with job tension (b ¼ 20.10, p , 0.05) and

medical symptoms (b ¼ 20.09, p , 0.05). Again, we further examined the direction of

the moderating effects by plotting the interactions using Aiken and West’s (1991)

procedures. As we expected, role conflict had significantly weaker associations with job

tension (Figure 2(a)) and medical symptoms (Figure 2(b)) when employees reported high

rather than low coworker support, thereby supporting Hypothesis 3. However, the

coworker support £ physical stressors interaction did not reach statistical significance

predicting either job tension or medical symptoms. These results failed to support

Hypothesis 4.

We repeated the regression analyses and examined the moderating effect of optimism

on the relationship between role conflict and strain after controlling for social support.

The results failed to yield statistically significant interactions between role conflict and

optimism or between physical stressors and optimism, hence strengthening the argument

that social support, and not optimism, accounted for the buffering effects between work

stressors and strain.

A post hoc test of stressor–support source congruence

COR theory underscores the importance of source congruence between stressors and

support (Hobfoll 2001). We have argued that role conflict is more likely to be attributed to

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3881

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Tab

le2

.R

esu

lts

of

hie

rarc

hic

alre

gre

ssio

nan

aly

ses

on

stra

in.

Jobtension

Medicalsymptoms

Stepsandvariables

12

31

23

1.

Co

ntr

ols

Ag

e0

.03

0.0

30

.03

20

.11*

*2

0.1

0*

*2

0.1

0*

*G

end

er0

.02

0.0

20

.02

0.2

0*

*0

.20

**

0.2

0*

*Y

ears

atth

eco

mp

any

0.1

6*

*0

.15

**

0.1

6*

*0

.06

0.0

60

.07*

Yea

rsw

ith

sup

erv

iso

r0

.05

0.0

50

.05

20

.04

20

.03

20

.02

Op

tim

ism

20

.16

**

20

.15

**

20

.15

**

20

.28*

*2

0.2

7*

*2

0.2

7*

*R

ole

con

flic

t0

.39

**

0.3

7*

*0

.37

**

0.2

6*

*0

.24

**

0.2

4*

*P

hy

sica

lst

ress

ors

0.1

1*

*0

.10

**

0.0

9*

*0

.07*

0.0

6*

0.0

6*

2.

Su

pp

ort

Su

per

vis

or

sup

po

rt2

0.0

6*

20

.07

*2

0.0

52

0.0

7*

Co

wo

rker

ssu

pp

ort

20

.06

*2

0.0

42

0.0

6*

20

.04

3.

Inte

ract

ion

sS

up

erv

iso

rsu

pp

ort£

role

con

flic

t0

.12

**

0.0

7*

Su

per

vis

or

sup

po

rt£

ph

ysi

cal

stre

sso

rs2

0.0

22

0.0

8*

Co

wo

rker

ssu

pp

ort£

role

con

flic

t2

0.1

0*

*2

0.0

9*

*C

ow

ork

ers

sup

po

rt£

ph

ysi

cal

stre

sso

rs2

0.0

52

0.0

2R

20

.28

**

0.2

9*

*0

.31

**

0.2

8*

*0

.29

**

0.3

0*

*DR

20

.01

*0

.02

**

0.0

1*

*0

.01*

No

te.A

ge,

20

or

less

,2¼

30

s,3¼

40

s,4¼

50

s,5¼

60

so

rm

ore

;g

end

er,1

¼m

ale,

fem

ale;

yea

rsat

the

com

pan

yan

dy

ears

wo

rked

wit

hsu

per

vis

or,

less

than

2,2

¼2

–5

,3¼

6–

10

,4¼

11

–1

5,

mo

reth

an1

5.

*p,

0.0

5;

**p,

0.0

1.

M. Mayo et al.3882

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

supervisors than to peers, customers and other organizational stakeholders. To test the

assumption that employees are more likely to perceive supervisors as the source of their

role conflict, but not as the source of physical stressors, we conducted a separate

supplementary study using a different sample. Participants used a 5-point scale ranging

from 1 (not at all) to 5 (almost always) to answer the following six questions: ‘To what

extent do you find inadequate physical conditions at work emanating from your (1)

supervisors, (2) coworkers and (3) organization?’ and ‘To what extent do you find role

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8(a)

(b)

(c)

Role Conflict

Job

Ten

sion

Low Supervisor Support

High Supervisor Support

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

Role Conflict

Med

ical

Sym

ptom

s

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

Physical Stressors

Med

ical

Sym

ptom

s

Low Supervisor Support

High Supervisor Support

Low Supervisor Support

High Supervisor Support

Figure 1. Reverse buffering effects of supervisory support for (a) role conflict predicting jobtension, (b) role conflict predicting medical symptoms and (c) physical stressors predicting medicalsymptoms.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3883

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

conflict at work emanating from your (1) supervisors, (2) coworkers and (3) organization?’

A sample of 131 employed individuals enrolled in an executive MBA program in Spain

completed the questionnaire. The average age of sample members was 32, the average

work experience was 9 years and 74% of this sample was male.

As we expected, role conflict was primarily attributed to the supervisor, and not to the

coworkers (Msupervisor ¼ 3.37 vs. Mcoworker ¼ 1.57, d ¼ 1.80; t(130) ¼ 24.14, p , 0.001) or

to the organization (Msupervisor ¼ 3.37 vs. Morganization ¼ 2.24, d ¼ 1.13; t(130) ¼ 12.79,

p , 0.001). Also as we expected, physical stressors were attributed primarily to the

organization, and not to the supervisors (Morganization ¼ 2.55 vs. Msupervisor ¼ 1.36,

d ¼ 1.19; t(130) ¼ 12.59, p , 0.001) or to the coworkers (Morganization ¼ 2.55 vs.

Mcoworker ¼ 1.19, d ¼ 1.36; t(130) ¼ 12.17, p , 0.001). These results are consistent with

our assumption that role conflict is source congruent primarily with supervisor rather than

coworker support.

Discussion

Contributions to theory

This study contributes to clarify the mixed evidence concerning the buffering and reverse

buffering effects of supervisor support in regard to the impact of work stressors on

employee strain. The results are largely consistent with our source congruence hypotheses,

suggesting that supervisor support may be beneficial when it counteracts stressors that are

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8(a)

(b)

Role Conflict

Job

Ten

sion

Low Coworker Support

High Coworker Support

–0.8

–0.6

–0.4

–0.2

0

0.2

0.4

0.6

0.8

Role Conflict

Med

ical

Sym

ptom

s

Low Coworker Support

High Coworker Support

Figure 2. Buffering effects of coworker support for role conflict predicting (a) job tension and (b)medical symptoms.

M. Mayo et al.3884

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

originated outside of the supervisor’s realm, such as physical stressors. In contrast,

supervisor support may increase subordinates’ strains when stressors deemed to be

originated in the supervisor, such as role conflict.

Our finding concerning the simultaneous occurrence of opposite buffering effects of

supervisor support is noteworthy, because such a finding has proven elusive in prior

studies. In what was possibly the first study explicitly designed to identify the conditions

under which reverse buffering occurs. Beehr et al. (2003) found no support for the

hypothesis that a reverse buffering effect occurs when the source of supervisor support

coincides with the source of the stressor. Beehr et al. (2003) speculated that the lack of

empirical support might have been due, in part, to a small sample size. They also noted that

there are multiple interpretations of the concept of source congruence, and called for

additional research on alternative forms of fit between work stressors and social support.

Based on the COR framework, we found support for the notion that role conflict and

physical conditions are two stressors likely to have differential effects when interacting

with supervisor support. Our results hint that although supervisor support alleviates

medical symptoms originated by physical stressors, it magnifies the unfavorable effects of

role conflict on both job tension and medical symptoms.

An alternative causal explanation for the reverse buffering effect of social support has

been that stressors lead to strain, which in turn leads employees (as proactive agents) to

seek social support (Kaufmann and Beehr 1986). We addressed this issue by examining

the effects of support from coworkers. If employees actively seek support when stressors

are related to strain, then we should find reverse buffering effects for both supervisor and

coworker support. However, we found that only supervisor support exacerbated the

association between role conflict and strain. This finding supports the source congruence

explanation.

Lastly, our results indicated that supervisor support had stronger direct associations

with strains than did coworker support: correlations between supervisor support and

strains (job tension and medical symptoms were 20.27 and 20.27 vs. correlations of

20.21, 20.21, between coworker support and the same strains). The magnitude of these

differences is similar to that reported in Viswesvaran et al.’s (1999) meta-analysis. When

compared to the relatively modest effect sizes associated with both positive and negative

buffering uncovered here and elsewhere (Viswesvaran et al. (1999) reported similar DR 2

ranging from 0.01 to 0.03), these correlations suggest that supervisor support is still a more

powerful reducer of employee strain than coworker support is, despite the former’s

potential for reverse buffering.

Limitations and future research

In spite of the large sample size and the high response rate, our design had some

limitations. First, associations among self-reported variables might have been inflated by

common method variance, but our CFAs suggested that such inflation was minimal in our

case. In addition, nonlinear effects such as the interactions that captured the buffering

effects are hardly attributable to method bias (Aiken and West 1991). Second, our cross-

sectional design precluded cause-and-effect determinations, and future longitudinal and

experimental research may help clarify the causal direction of these findings. More

nuanced relationships between stressors and support should be examined in future

research. For instance, Harris and Kacmar (2006) found a U-shape curvilinear relationship

between the quality of the relationship between leaders and followers so that employees in

high-quality leader member exchange (LMX) relationships with their supervisors

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3885

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

paradoxically experienced more stress than did their colleagues in moderate-quality LMX

relationships. Finally, we examined solely self-reported measures of strain. Our reliance

on self-reports undoubtedly facilitated gathering data from a large sample, thus increasing

the statistical power needed to detect the kind of interaction or buffering effects that we

sought. Even though there is a well-documented empirical association between

psychological and physiological measures of strains (Fox, Dwyer and Ganster 1993;

Sprigg, Stride, Wall, Holman and Smith 2007), a broader set of strain measures that also

encompasses directly measurable physiological outcomes such as cortisol levels, muscular

disorders and blood pressure should be included in future studies.

Implications and conclusions

The most straightforward implication of our study is that managers should be aware of the

sources of stressors impinging on their subordinates before they possibly overwhelm them

with their support. For this reason, it is important that they develop observation and

diagnostic skills to assess the origin of stressors affecting their subordinates, and then offer

the kind of support that is most likely to match the stressor. When the source of employees’

stressors is largely unrelated to their supervisors (e.g. physical conditions), supervisors

may be in the best position of truly alleviating the negative consequences of stressors.

However, if the source of stressors is perceived to be linked to the supervisor, as it appears

to be the case of role conflict, supervisor support may make matters worse. Employees

should also remain aware of instances when ‘talking to the boss’ may not be the best

approach to reduce tension (Harris and Kacmar 2006). Our results suggest that this

scenario is likely when the source of stressors is also the supervisor, who may end up

aggravating the stressor–strain relationship.

In short, while supervisor support has undeniable beneficial effects to alleviate

employee strain, it also has the potential for making matters worse when support comes

from the same source as stressor at hand. We hope that our findings stimulate further

research on the boundary conditions influencing the extent to which social support and

other resources adequately match work stressors (Beehr et al. 2003; de Jonge and

Dormann 2006).

Acknowledgement

This research was partially funded by a research grant from the Ministry of Science and Innovationof Spain # ECO2009-12952 to Margarita Mayo.

References

Ahuja, M.K., Chudoba, K.M., Kacmar, C.J., McKnight, D.H., and George, J.F. (2007), ‘IT RoadWarriors: Balancing Work–Family Conflict, Job Autonomy, and Work Overload to MitigateTurnover Intentions,’ Management Informational System Quarterly, 31, 1–17.

Aiken, L.S., and West, S.G. (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interaction,Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Alvesson, M., and Billing, Y.D. (1997), Understanding Gender and Organizations, London: Sage.Arbuckle, J.L. (2006), Amos (Version 7.0), Chicago, IL: SPSS.Balkundi, P., and Harrison, D.A. (2006), ‘Ties, Leaders and Time in Teams: Strong Inference About

Network Structure’s Effects on Team Viability and Performance,’ Academy of ManagementJournal, 49, 49–68.

Beehr, T.A. (1985), ‘The Role of Social Support in Coping with Organizational Stress,’in Human Stress and Cognition in Organizations: An Integrated Perspective, eds. T.A. Beehrand T.S. Bhagat, New York: Wiley, pp. 375–398.

M. Mayo et al.3886

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Beehr, T.A., Farmer, S., Glazer, S., Gudanowski, D., and Nair, V. (2003), ‘The Enigma of SocialSupport and Occupational Stress: Source Congruence and Gender Role Effects,’ Journal ofOccupational and Health Psychology, 8, 220–231.

Beehr, T.A., Jex, S.M., Stacy, B.A., and Murray, M.A. (2000), ‘Work Stressors and CoworkerSupport as Predictors of Individual Strain and Job Performance,’ Journal of OrganizationalBehavior, 21, 391–405.

Beehr, T.A., King, L.A., and King, D.W. (1990), ‘Social Support and Occupational Stress: Talkingto Supervisors,’ Journal of Vocational Behavior, 36, 61–81.

Bentler, P.M., and Bonnet, D.G. (1980), ‘Significance Test and Goodness of Fit in the Analysis ofCovariance Structures,’ Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606.

Blaine, B., Crocker, J., and Major, B. (1995), ‘The Unintended Negative Consequences of Sympathyfor the Stigmatized,’ Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 25, 889–905.

Blau, G. (1981), ‘An Empirical Investigation of Job Stress, Service Length and Job Strain,’Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 27, 279–302.

Brass, D.J., Galaskiewicz, J., Greve, H.R., and Tsai, W. (2004), ‘Taking Stock of Networks andOrganizations: A Multilevel Perspective,’ Academy of Management Journal, 47, 795–819.

Brissette, I., Scheier, M.F., and Carver, C.S. (2002), ‘The Role of Optimism in Social NetworkDevelopment, Coping, and Psychological Adjustment During a Life Transition,’ Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 82, 1, 102–111.

Caplan, R.D., Cobb, S., French, J.R.P., Harrison, R.V., and Pinneau, S.R. Jr (1975), Job Demandsand Worker Health, Washington, DC: NIOSH.

Carlson, D.S., and Kacmar, K.M. (2000), ‘Work–Family Conflict in the Organization: Do Life RolesValues make a Difference?’ Journal of Management, 26, 1031–1054.

Carlson, D.S., and Perrewe, P. (1999), ‘The Role of Social Support in the Stressors–StrainRelationship: An Examination of Work–Family Conflict,’ Journal of Management,25, 513–540.

Cobb, S. (1976), ‘Social Support as Moderator of Life Stress,’PsychosomaticMedicine, 38, 300–314.Cohen, S., and Wills, T.A. (1985), ‘Stress, Social Support, and the Buffering Hypothesis,’

Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.Cooper, C.L., Dewe, P., and O’Driscoll, M.P. (2001), Organizational Stress: A Review and Critique

of Theory, Research, and Applications (Foundations for Organizational Science), London: Sage.de Jonge, J., and Dormann, C. (2006), ‘Stressors, Resources, and Strain at Work: A Longitudinal

Test of the Triple-Match Principle,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1359–1374.Deelstra, J.T., Peeters, M., Schaufeli, W.B., Stroebe, W., Zijlstra, F.R.H., and van Doornen, L.P.

(2003), ‘Receiving Instrumental Support at Work: When Help is not Welcome,’ Journal ofApplied Psychology, 88, 324–331.

Dierdorff, E.C., and Ellington, J.K. (2008), ‘It’s the Nature of Work: Examining Behavior-BasedSources of Work–Family Conflict Across Occupations,’ Journal of Applied Psychology,93, 883–892.

Dorman, C., and Zapf, D. (1999), ‘Social Support, Social Stressors at Work, and DepressiveSymptoms: Testing for main and Moderating Effects with Structural Equations in a Three WaveLongitudinal Study,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 874–884.

Fox, M.L., Dwyer, D.J., and Ganster, D.C. (1993), ‘Effects of Stressful Job Demands and Control onPhysiological and Attitudinal Outcomes in a Hospital Setting,’ Academy of ManagementJournal, 36, 289–318.

Fried, Y., and Tiegs, R.B. (1993), ‘The Main Effect Model Versus Buffering Model of Shop StewardSocial Support: A Study of Rank-and-File Auto Workers in the USA,’ Journal of OrganizationalBehavior, 14, 481–493.

Fritz, C., and Sonnentag, S. (2009), ‘Antecedents of Day-level Proactive Behavior: A Look at JobStressors and Positive Affect Experienced during the Workday,’ Journal of Management, 35,94–111.

Ganster, D.C., Fusilier, M., and Mayes, B. (1986), ‘The Role of Social Support in the Experience ofStress at Work,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 102–110.

Glaser, D.N., Tatum, B.C., Nebeker, D.M., Sorenson, R.C., and Aiello, J.R. (1999), ‘Workload andSocial Support: Effects on Performance and Stress,’ Human Performance, 12, 155–176.

Halbesleben, J.R. (2006), ‘Sources of Social Support and Burnout: A Meta-analytic Test of theConservation of Resources Model,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1134–1145.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3887

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Harris, K.J., and Kacmar, K.M. (2006), ‘Too Much of a Good Thing? The Curvilinear Effect ofLeader-Member Exchange on Stress,’ Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 65–84.

Hobfoll, S.E. (1989), ‘Conservation of Resources: A New Attempt at Conceptualizing Stress,’American Psychologist, 44, 3, 513–524.

Hobfoll, S.E. (1998), Stress, Culture and Community: The Psychology and Philosophy of Stress,New York: Plenum.

Hobfoll, S.E. (2001), ‘The Influence of Culture, Community, and the Nested-Self in the StressProcess: Advancing Conservation of Resource Theory,’ Applied Psychology: An InternationalReview, 50, 337–421.

Hobfoll, S.E., and Kay, J.S. (2007), ‘Conservation of Resources Theory,’ in The Encyclopedia ofStress (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.), ed. G. Fink, New York: Elsevier, pp. 562–567.

Hobfoll, S.E., and Shirom, A. (2000), ‘Conservation of Resources Theory: Applications toStress and Management in the Workplace,’ in Handbook of Organizational Behavior,ed. R.T. Golembiewski, New York: Dekker, pp. 57–81.

House, R.J., and Rizzo, J.R. (1972), ‘Role Conflict and Ambiguity as Critical Variables in a Model ofOrganizational Behavior,’ Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 7, 467–505.

Humphrey, S.E., Nahrgang, J.D., and Morgeson, F.P. (2007), ‘Integrating Motivational, Social, andContextual Work Design Features: A Meta-Analytic Summary and Theoretical Extension of theWork Design Literature,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1332–1356.

Ibarra, H. (1992), ‘Homophily and Differential Returns: Sex Differences in Network Structure andAccess in an Advertising Firm,’ Administrative Science Quarterly, 37, 422–447.

Ibarra, H. (1993), ‘Personal Networks of Women and Minorities in Management: A ConceptualFramework,’ Academy of Management Review, 18, 56–87.

Ivancevich, J.M., and Matteson, M.T. (1980), Stress at Work, Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman.Jackson, S., and Schuler, R. (1985), ‘A Meta-Analysis and Conceptual Critique of Research on Role

Ambiguity and Role Conflict in Work Settings,’ Organizational Behavior and Human DecisionProcesses, 36, 16–78.

Katz, D., and Kahn, R.L. (1966), The Social Psychology of Organizations, New York: Wiley.Kaufmann, G.M., and Beehr, T.A. (1986), ‘Interactions Between Job Stressors and Social Support:

Some Counterintuitive Results,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 522–526.Kellerman, B. (2004), Bad Leadership: What it is, How it Happens, Why it Matters, Boston, MA:

Harvard Business School Press.Kickul, J., and Posig, M. (2001), ‘Supervisory Emotional Support and Burnout: An Explanation of

Reverse Buffering Effects,’ Journal of Managerial Issues, 13, 328–344.Kleinbaum, D.G., Kupper, L.L., and Mueller, K.E. (1988), Applied Regression Analysis and Other

Multivariable Methods, Boston, MA: PWS-KENT Publishing Company.Kobasa, S.C.Q., and Puccetti, M.C. (1983), ‘Personality and Social Resources in Stress Resistance,’

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 839–850.McCoy, J.M., and Evans, G.W. (2005), ‘Physical Work Environment,’ in Handbook of Work Stress,

eds. Julian Barling, E. Kevin Kelloway and Michael Frone, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.McPherson, J.M., Smith-Lovin, L., and Cook, J.M. (2001), ‘Birds of a Feather: Homophily in Social

Networks,’ in Annual Review of Sociology (Vol. 27), eds. J. Hagan and K.S. Cook, Palo Alto,CA: Annual Reviews, pp. 415–444.

O’Driscoll, M.P, and Beehr, T.A. (1994), ‘Supervisor Behaviors, Role Stressors and Uncertainty asPredictors of Personal Outcomes for Subordinates,’ Journal of Organizational Behaviour,15, 141–155.

Oldham, G.R., and Brass, D.J. (1979), ‘Employee Reactions to an Open-Plan Office: A NaturallyOccurring Quasi-Experiment,’ Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 2, 267–284.

Oldham, G.R., and Rotchford, N.L. (1983), ‘Relationships Between Office Characteristics andEmployee Reactions: A Study of the Physical Environment,’ Administrative Science Quarterly,28, 542–556.

Perrewe, P.L., Zellars, K.L., Ferris, G.R., Rossi, A.M., Kacmar, C.J., and Ralston, D.A. (2004),‘Neutralizing Job Stressors: Political Skill as an Antidote to the Dysfunctional Consequences ofRole Conflict,’ Academy of Management Journal, 47, 141–152.

Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J., and Podsakoff, N.P. (2003), ‘Common Method Biases inBehavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and Recommended Remedies,’Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 879–903.

M. Mayo et al.3888

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3

Quick, J.C., and Quick, J.D. (1984), Organizational Stress and Preventative Management,New York: McGraw-Hill.

Rizzo, J.R., House, R.J., and Lirtzman, S.I. (1970), ‘Role Conflict and Ambiguity in ComplexOrganizations,’ Administrative Science Quarterly, 15, 150–163.

Ryan, T. (1997), Modern Regression Methods, New York: Wiley.Sargent, L.D., and Terry, D.J. (2000), ‘The Moderating Role of Social Support in Karasek’s Job

Strain Model,’ Work & Stress, 14, 3, 245–261.Scheier, M.F., and Carver, C.S. (1985), ‘Optimism, Coping, and Health: Assessment and

Implications of Generalized Outcome Expectancies,’ Health Psychology, 4, 219–247.Scheier, M.F., and Carver, C.S. (1992), ‘Effects of Optimism on Psychological and Physical

Well-Being: Theoretical Overview and Empirical Update,’ Cognitive Therapy and Research,16, 201–228.

Searle, B., Bright, J.E.H., and Bochner, S. (2001), ‘Helping People to Sort it Out: The Role of SocialSupport in the Job Strain Model,’ Work & Stress, 15, 4, 328–346.

Siu, O.L., Spector, P.E., Cooper, C.L., and Donald, I. (2001), ‘Age Differences in Coping and Locusof Control: A Study of Managerial Stress in Hong Kong,’ Psychology and Aging, 16, 4,707–710.

Sosik, J.J., and Godshalk, V.M. (2000), ‘Leadership Styles, Mentoring Functions Received, andJob-Related Stress: A Conceptual Model and Preliminary Study,’ Journal of OrganizationalBehavior, 21, 365–390.

Spreitzer, G.M., Kizilos, M., and Nason, S. (1997), ‘A Dimensional Analysis of the Relationshipbetween Psychological Empowerment and Effectiveness, Satisfaction, and Strain,’ Journal ofManagement, 23, 5, 679–704.

Sprigg, C., Jackson, P.R., and Parker, S.K. (2000), ‘Production Teamworking: The Importance ofInterdependence and Autonomy for Employee Strain and Satisfaction,’ Human Relations, 53,1519–1543.

Sprigg, C.A., Stride, C.B., Wall, T.D., Holman, D.J., and Smith, P.R. (2007), ‘Work Characteristics,Musculoskeletal Disorders and the Mediating Role of Psychological Strain: A Study of CallCenter Employees,’ Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 5, 1456–1466.

Stroth, L.K., and Reilly, A.H. (1999), ‘Gender and Careers: Present Experiences and EmergingTrends,’ in The Handbook of Gender & Work, ed. G. Powell, Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Sutton, R.I., and Rafaeli, A. (1987), ‘Characteristics of Work Stations as Potential OccupationalStressors,’ Academy of Management Journal, 30, 260–276.

van Dierendonck, D., Haynes, C., Borrill, C., and Stride, C. (2004), ‘Leadership Behavior andSubordinate Wellbeing,’ Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 9, 165–175.

Viswesvaran, C., Sanchez, J.I., and Fisher, J. (1999), ‘The Role of Social Support in the Process ofWork Stress: A Meta-Analysis,’ Journal of Vocational Behavior, 54, 314–334.

Watson, D., Pennebaker, J.W., and Folger, R. (1987), ‘Beyond Negative Affectivity: MeasuringStress and Satisfaction in the Workplace,’ Journal of Organizational Behavior Management,8, 141–157.

Williams, L.J., Cote, J.A., and Buckley, M.R. (1989), ‘Lack of Method Variance in Self-ReportedAffect and Perceptions of Work: Reality or Artefact?’ Journal of Applied Psychology,74, 462–468.

The International Journal of Human Resource Management 3889

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Era

smus

Uni

vers

ity]

at 0

5:10

27

May

201

3