Spectral enhancement of Polish vowels to improve their identification by hearing impaired listeners

11
Spectral enhancement of Polish vowels to improve their identification by hearing impaired listeners E. Ozimek * , A. Se z k, A. Wicher, E. Skrodzka, J. Konieczny Institute of Acoustics, A. Mickiewicz University, Pozna n, Poland Received 23 March 2003; received in revised form 10 November 2003; accepted 17 November 2003 Abstract Abnormalities in the cochlear function usually cause broadening of the auditory filters which reduces the speech intelligibility. An attempt to apply a spectral enhancement algorithm has been undertaken to improve the identification of Polish vowels by subjects with cochlear- based hearing-impairment. The identification scores of natural (unprocessed) vowels and spectrally enhanced (processed) vowels has been measured for hearing-impaired subjects. It has been found that spectral enhancement improves vowel scores by about 10% for those subjects, however, a wide variation in individual performance among subjects has been ob- served. The overall vowels identification scores obtained were 85% for natural vowels and 96% for spectrally enhanced vowels. Ó 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Spectral enhancement; Vowels; Identification scores; Cochlear function 1. Introduction Broadening of the auditory filters resulting from the abnormalities in the cochlear function [13] is usually related to worsening of frequency resolution and spectral contrast (difference in amplitude between spectral peaks and troughs of the succes- sive formants) of speech sounds. This leads to a smear of internal auditory repre- sentations (perceptual spectra) of those sounds worsening their identification. * Corresponding author. E-mail address: [email protected] (E. Ozimek). 0003-682X/$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2003.11.004 www.elsevier.com/locate/apacoust Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

Transcript of Spectral enhancement of Polish vowels to improve their identification by hearing impaired listeners

www.elsevier.com/locate/apacoust

Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

Spectral enhancement of Polish vowelsto improve their identification by

hearing impaired listeners

E. Ozimek *, A. Sezk, A. Wicher, E. Skrodzka, J. Konieczny

Institute of Acoustics, A. Mickiewicz University, Pozna�n, Poland

Received 23 March 2003; received in revised form 10 November 2003; accepted 17 November 2003

Abstract

Abnormalities in the cochlear function usually cause broadening of the auditory filters

which reduces the speech intelligibility. An attempt to apply a spectral enhancement algorithm

has been undertaken to improve the identification of Polish vowels by subjects with cochlear-

based hearing-impairment. The identification scores of natural (unprocessed) vowels and

spectrally enhanced (processed) vowels has been measured for hearing-impaired subjects. It

has been found that spectral enhancement improves vowel scores by about 10% for those

subjects, however, a wide variation in individual performance among subjects has been ob-

served. The overall vowels identification scores obtained were 85% for natural vowels and 96%

for spectrally enhanced vowels.

� 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Spectral enhancement; Vowels; Identification scores; Cochlear function

1. Introduction

Broadening of the auditory filters resulting from the abnormalities in the cochlear

function [13] is usually related to worsening of frequency resolution and spectral

contrast (difference in amplitude between spectral peaks and troughs of the succes-

sive formants) of speech sounds. This leads to a smear of internal auditory repre-

sentations (perceptual spectra) of those sounds worsening their identification.

*Corresponding author.

E-mail address: [email protected] (E. Ozimek).

0003-682X/$ - see front matter � 2003 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.apacoust.2003.11.004

474 E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

The question arises to what extent the changes in the internal representations of

vowels lead to a reduction of their identification for hearing-impaired subjects. It is

usually assumed that such changes effect at least partly the vowel recognition ability

of those subjects. The identification of vowels may be facilitated by spectral en-

hancement which leads to increase of their spectral contrast. The spectral enhance-

ment, causing a greater than normal concentration of the spectrum energy aroundformant frequencies, may improve recognition of vowels because increasing of

spectral contrast partly compensates for the poorer than normal frequency resolu-

tion accompanying a sensorineural hearing impairment [18]. Some researchers have

attempted to change the spectral contrast of speech sounds by changing their for-

mant bandwidths [4,17,20]. Increasing formant bandwidths reduces formant peak-

to-trough differences and consequently reduces spectral contrast, while decreasing

formant bandwidths causes opposite effect. Boers [4] found only insignificant effect

of narrowed bandwidths on sentence intelligibility. Van Veen and Houtgast [20]found that decreasing formant bandwidths has small effect on the judged similarity

of vowels. Summerfield et al. [17] varied spectral contrast by varying formant

bandwidths in the synthetic CVC syllables. They found little improvement in iden-

tification of stop consonants resulting from narrowed bandwidths. Leek et al. [12]

examined a minimum spectral contrast for identification of synthesized vowels by

normal-hearing and hearing-impaired subjects. They measured identification ability

in noise as a function of peak-to-trough differences in the spectrum of those vowels

and found that the peak-to-trough amplitude differences required for 75% identifi-cation accuracy amounted to 1–2 dB for normal-hearing subjects and 6–7 dB for

hearing-impaired subjects. Franck et al. [9] investigated effects of the reduced dy-

namic range on vowel perception by compression and compensation of the reduced

frequency resolution by spectral enhancement. They found in some measurable

conditions that spectral enhancement produced improvements of vowel scores but

this was counteracted by deterioration of the consonant scores.

The basic purpose of this study was to examine the influence of spectral en-

hancement on the identification of Polish vowels by hearing-impaired subjects. Thevowels were modified by means of the spectral enhancement algorithm developed at

Cambridge University by Baer and co-workers [3,2], which increases the peak-to-

trough differences in the spectral envelope.

2. Concept of the enhancement algorithm

The applied algorithm assumes that the vowels sounds supplied to the impaired-hearing system should be transformed in such a way that it would produce a similar

excitation as the non-transformed signal in the normal-hearing system. If PN and FNstand for the matrices representing the excitation patterns and auditory filters in the

normal-hearing system, PS and FS in the impaired-hearing system, and X is the vector

representing the spectrum of the signal, then

PN ¼ FNX ð1Þ

E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483 475

and

PS ¼ FSX : ð2Þ

The signal whose spectrum would be represented by the vector #ðX Þ, that is the

signal which passed through the broadened filters would stimulate the same activityof the hearing system as the signal X in the normal-hearing system, should satisfy the

equation:

PN ¼ FS#ðX Þ ð3Þ

and finally

#ðX Þ ¼ F �1S FNX : ð4Þ

The principle of the algorithm is as follows. The vowels recorded on audio CD were

sampled at a 24-kHz rate and low-pass filtered at 4.5 kHz. Each vowel, was divided

by the Hamming window into short time segments (256 points, 10 ms long) andconverted to the frequency domain (X vector) by the fast Fourier transform (FFT).

Obtained in this way short-term spectra were processed by the enhancement pro-

cedure in which only the magnitude (not phase) spectra were processed. In the en-

hancement process, the spectra were subjected to a function which enhanced their

valleys. In the inverted FFT, the adapted magnitude data were combined with the

original phase data and retransformed into the time domain. Resulting time seg-

ments were added using an overlap-add procedure to get the final output signal. The

algorithm was implemented using the TDT system (Tucker–Davies Technology) anda PC computer. The exact value of the contrast weight used for the enhanced stimuli

was chosen individually for subjects with hearing impairment, depending on their

absolute threshold elevations versus frequency.

3. Experiment

3.1. Subjects

Three hearing-impaired subjects (age range 24–40 years) who participated in

the experiment showed bilaterally symmetrical hearing impairment within the range

35–75 dB HL. No air-gaps were observed for any subjects. Basic audiological tests

(air and bone conductions, speech audiometry and SISI test) indicated sensorineural

impairment of cochlear origin. The air audiograms for hearing-impaired subjects (1,

2, 3) participating in the study are shown in Fig. 1. Two subjects 1 and 3 showed

hearing losses of between 30 and 35 dB at 125 Hz and between 60 and 65 dB at 4kHz. The hearing loss of subject 2 increased from 40 dB at 125 Hz to approximately

75 dB at 4 kHz.

For subjects with sensorineural hearing losses characteristics of the auditory filters

centered at 1, 2 and 4 kHz were also determined using a notched-noise method [16].

It was found that the shapes of the obtained auditory filters, for the frequencies for

which hearing loss were observed were asymmetric, had lower dynamic range and

Fig. 1. Audiograms of the three hearing-impaired subjects at octave intervals. The subjects are indicated

as 1–3.

476 E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

were much broader than the corresponding filters of the normal-hearing subjects.

The broadening of the auditory filters deteriorates the frequency selectivity of the

auditory system and impairs vowels identification. Amplification of the signal

reaching the ear of a subject with broadened filters cannot improve this identifica-

tion, because the signal-to-noise ratio remains the same.

3.2. Stimuli and procedure

Six Polish vowels (/o/, /a/, /i/, /e/, /u/, /y/) were used as the stimuli. Each vowel was

approximately of 500 ms duration, including 50 ms linear rise-fall times. They were

stored on a computer disk and presented to subjects in random sequence. The stimuli

were presented in 8 blocks of 4 trials to each subject. Each block of trials consisted of

eight randomly ordered presentations of six vowels. Correct answer feedback was

not given. Subjects were asked to listen to each of the blocks of stimuli a minimum offour times on different days. After each stimulus presentation, the subject wrote on a

list which vowel was heard. All testing was completed in five sessions, each lasting

3 h. The spectral enhancement was done off-line. Subjects were seated in a sound-

treated booth. They were listening to stimuli via Sennheiser 580 headphones. The

desired level for each subject was adjusted, so the subject could hear the stimuli at the

most comfortable level (MCL). The MCL levels were within a range from 85 to 100

dB SPL. To avoid distortions, a high-pass (cutoff frequency 50 Hz) and a low-pass

filter (cutoff frequency 4500 Hz) were implemented.

3.3. Results for unprocessed (natural) vowels

Fig. 2 shows example spectra of three natural (unprocessed) vowels (i, e, y) (solidlines) and the same vowels processed by the enhancement procedure (broken lines).

1 2 3 4 5-80

-60

-40

-20

0

20

/i/ /i/

Enh

1 2 3 4 5-80

-60

-40

-20

0

20

/y/ /y/

Enh

1 2 3 4 5

-80

-60

-40

-20

0

20

/e/ /e/

Enh

LE

VE

L [

dB]

FREQUENCY [kHz]

Fig. 2. Spectra of three Polish vowels produced by female speaker (solid line) and vowels processed by the

enhancement procedure (broken line).

E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483 477

Fig. 3. Individual and mean correct identification (in percent) for the natural vowels obtained by the

hearing-impaired subjects.

478 E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

Individual data on identification scores of natural vowels by hearing-impaired

subjects are shown in Fig. 3. Mean percent correct identification of tested vowels is

given by blank bars. The accuracy of identification is indicated by standard deviation.

As follows from Fig. 3 the identification score varies depending on the vowel. The

best hearing-impaired subject�s score equaled as an average across vowels 97%-correct vowel identification and the worst subject scored 75%. Clear decrease in

identification is observed for vowel /o/. Van Tasell et al. [19] testing hearing-impaired

subjects found that one of their three subjects identified the seven synthetic vowel

stimuli well (93%) while the other two performed with a nearly 70% accuracy.

Nabelek et al. [14] reported a range of vowel-identification performance of 68–93%

for subjects with a mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss.

The data in Fig. 3 display not only the individual identification scores of the

successive vowels, but also a tendency to use some responses more frequently thanothers. To check this tendency, the confusion matrices [6] were calculated to estimate

the effect of response bias and the pairwise discriminability of the vowels. The ob-

tained data are presented in Table 1. The percent of correct identifications of par-

ticular vowels is given at the diagonal of the confusion matrices obtained for

particular subjects.

Table 1

Vowel confusion matrices and overall percent-correct vowel identification for natural vowels

Subject 1 Subject 2 Subject 3

Vowel o a i e u y o a i e u y o a i e u y

o 66 0 0 28 0 6 98 0 0 2 0 0 2 0 0 98 0 0

a 3 45 0 49 3 0 2 97 0 1 0 0 2 98 0 0 0 0

i 0 0 94 3 3 0 0 0 98 2 0 0 0 0 97 3 0 0

e 6 0 3 91 0 0 0 2 1 97 0 0 0 2 0 98 0 0

u 0 3 0 6 63 28 0 0 0 0 96 4 0 3 0 0 97 0

y 0 0 3 3 3 91 0 3 0 0 3 96 0 0 0 0 2 98

Mean (%) 75 97 82

E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483 479

The data from Table 1 show that the easiest identifiable were the vowels /e/ /i/ (97%

mean value) characterized by a significant difference in frequencies between the first and

second formant. The vowels showing small frequency difference between the formants

F1 and F2 (e.g., /o/ and /u/) weremuchworse identifiable. These results indicate that the

frequency information provided by the spectrum in the region of the first two formants

is important for recognition of vowels for subjects with moderate hearing loss.

3.4. Results for spectrally enhanced vowels

In the second stage of the experiment, identification scores were determined for

spectrally enhanced vowels whose spectra are shown in Fig. 2 (broken line). The

weight of spectral contrast used for the enhanced stimuli was chosen individually for

each subject with hearing impairment, depending on his (her) threshold elevation

and auditory filter shape. As can be seen the enhanced vowels are characterized by alarger level differences of peak and trough of successive formants and better peak

resolution relative to natural (unprocessed) vowels.

Individual (hatched bars) and average (black bars) data on vowel identification are

shown in Fig. 4. The accuracy of identification is indicated by standard deviation.

As follows from Fig. 4, the subject performance for the spectrally enhanced

vowels is better across all vowels than it is for the unprocessed vowels. Averaged

identification score across vowels and hearing-impaired subjects is equal to about

93% (83% was for unprocessed vowels). The highest identification improvement dueto spectral enhancement, is observed for vowels /o/. The best hearing-impaired

subject�s score equaled as an average 100%-correct vowel identification (97% was for

unprocessed vowels) and the worst subject scored 97% (75%).

In order to check a possible tendency among subjects to use some responses more

frequently than others, confusion matrices (similar, as in the first part of the ex-

periment) were calculated. The data are presented in Table 2.

The data from Table 2, show that the most frequently confused vowel was /o/,

whereas the least confused vowel was /e/. In the first stage of this experiment the

Fig. 4. Individual and mean correct identification (in percent) for the spectrally enhanced vowels obtained

by hearing-impaired subjects (blank bars refer to unprocessed vowels).

Table 2

Vowel confusion matrices and overall percent-correct vowel identification for spectrally enhanced vowel

Subject 1 Subject 2 Subject 3

Vowel o a i e u y o a i e u y o a i e u y

o 100 0 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 0 19 0 3 78 0 0

a 6 88 0 6 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 0 94 3 3 0 0

i 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 3 0 94 3 0 0

e 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 3 97 0 0

u 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 0 3 3 94 0

y 0 0 0 0 6 94 0 0 0 0 0 100 0 0 6 0 0 94

Mean

(%)

97 100 82

480 E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

most and the least confused vowels were /o/ and /i/, respectively. The vowel /e/ was

the only one that was rarely confused with any of the others.

The results shown in Fig. 4 were subjected to the variance analysis (ANOVA) in

order to check the statistical significance of the improvement of the vowel identifi-cation due to spectral enhancement. It was found that the combined effects of

spectral enhancement and subject was statistically significant [F ð1; 30Þ ¼ 6; 5,p < 0:05]. No statistical significance was found for the dependence between the

identification improvement due to spectral enhancement and the shape of the au-

ditory filters.

4. Discussion

The experimental data showed that the mean percent correct identification for

hearing-impaired subjects averaged across vowels was 85% with SD¼ 12%. For the

sake of comparison, a vowel identification study was also conducted for five un-

trained normal-hearing subjects. It was found that the vowel identification score for

those subjects equaled 100% except for the vowel /o/ whose identification score

amounted to 99.5% (data not presented in the paper). Such a high score for normal-

hearing subjects is similar to that obtained by Van Tasell et al. [19] who found thatnatural vowels were identifiable by untrained normal subjects with an average

identification score of 98.2%.

Worsening of the performance of the hearing-impaired subjects relative to that of

the normal-hearing generally supports the assumption that broadening of the au-

ditory filters associated with sensorineural hearing impairment reduces peak-to-

trough amplitude differences in the internal auditory representation of vowel spectra.

The low identification scores for some vowels (/o/ and /u/) is probably due to the fact

that hearing-impaired subjects have some difficulty to discriminate closely spacedformant peaks corresponding to those vowels, which was caused by a smoothing of

the their internal representation by broadened auditory filters.

In the literature, it is generally assumed that to get good vowel identification, the

internal representations of vowels should clearly exhibit spectral peaks. However,

E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483 481

there are some discrepancies in the role of the formant structure. Dubno and

Dorman [8] stressed a special role of the first formant in this identification. Coughlin

et al. [7] suggested that vowel identification is partially predicted by reduced ability

to discriminate spectral differences in the F2 region. However, Chistovich [5] found

that poorly resolved formant peaks would not necessarily predict poor vowel iden-

tification performance. Other researchers suggest that accurate identification ofvowels is dependent on combined information of such acoustic properties as spectral

cues, vowel duration, and formant dynamics [1,10]. Our data suggest that the fre-

quency information provided by the spectrum in the region of the first two formants

is important for recognition of Polish vowels for subjects with moderate hearing loss.

In the second stage of the experiment, it was found that the spectral contrast

enhancement generally improved identification of Polish vowels for hearing-im-

paired subjects. The positive effect of spectral enhancement stated in this experiment

is similar to that of Leek et al. [12], who found that spectral contrast in vowelsprovides a useful cue to vowel identification for persons with moderate hearing

impairment. However, it is not an obvious outcome since it is not in agreement with

the Klatt [11] finding who stated that overriding importance in vowel identification

are the formant frequencies and not specially important are formant amplitude,

peak-to-valley differences and overall spectral slope. Our data suggest that the vowel

identification requires only gross estimation of formant peaks rather than resolution

of details across the spectrum.

The increase in the spectral contrast of vowels might partly compensate for thereduced frequency resolution of hearing-impaired subjects, so as to produce an in-

ternal representation similar to that produced in the normal-hearing subjects. The

contrast enhancement was not equally successful in improving the vowel identifi-

cation for tested hearing-impaired subjects. The improvement is clearly seen for

subjects 1 but less for 3. Several authors have examined the effect of spectral contrast

on the identification of speech sounds and some of them rather failed to demonstrate

its beneficial effects for the hearing-impaired subjects [4,8,17]. Boers [4] found only

insignificant effect of narrowed bandwidths on the sentence intelligibility. But forsome experimental conditions it was stated that increased spectral contrast resulted

in poorer speech-reception threshold scores for hearing-impaired subjects. Sum-

merfield et al. [17] varied spectral contrast by varying formant bandwidth in the

synthetic CVC syllables and found little improvement in identification of stop

consonants resulting from narrowed bandwidths. Franck et al. [9] stated the positive

effect of spectral enhancement on the vowels identification, however, it was coun-

teracted by the negative effect on the consonants.

These inconsistencies in the literature on vowel identification can have severalpossible reasons. The spectral contrast in the internal representation can be signifi-

cantly reduced but it is still sufficient for vowel identification. Leek et al. [12] showed

that normal-hearing subjects required only a 1–2 dB difference in the amplitude of

harmonics at spectral peaks and troughs to achieve greater than 75% accuracy in

identification of some synthetic vowels. Moreover, hearing-impaired subjects may

use some additional cues such as, vowel duration and pitch [15], formant transition

[21] or some linguistic factors which may improve identification performance. One

482 E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483

can also assume that vowels are characterized by a unique patterns in the internal

representations. Such assumption would help to explain why vowels identification is

not seriously impaired for mild and moderate hearing loss.

It should be added that the study presented in this paper concern only Polish

language vowels. Their specific spectral–temporal character is rather significantly

different from that in other languages, e.g., English or German. For instance Englishhas central vowels unknown in Polish or retroflexion vowels. German has a pro-

found contribution of front vowels as does French. Because of these differences the

amplitude and frequency relations of the formants are different in these languages

and are idiosyncratic for each language. In view of the above results on the effect of

the spectral enhancement procedure on identification of vowels should not be ap-

plied to any other language as it might lead to some errors. Besides at this stage of

the study aimed just to test the enhancement procedure we decided to make mea-

surements on only three hearing-impaired subjects only. In further studies aimed atimplementation of the spectral enhancement algorithm in the hearing aids, at least

several dozen subjects with hearing-impairment of cochlear origin will participate.

Moreover, besides natural vowels other speech tests will be used.

Generally one can state that the results obtained in this paper indicated the in-

crease of vowel identification when spectral enhancement was applied. But one has

to remember that the enhancement procedure also produced some distortions that

could be not easily acceptable by some people with hearing impairment. Thus further

investigation, for a much wider range of speech stimuli and a greater number ofhearing-impaired subjects is needed before the spectral enhancement procedure finds

any application to hearing aids.

5. Conclusions

The following conclusions come out from this study:

The hearing-impaired subjects showed slight-to-moderate difficulty with identifi-cation of the natural Polish vowels. The averaged identification scores for tested

subjects ranged from 75% to 97%.

The applied spectral enhancement algorithm improved vowel identification an

average by about 8%. In the individual results, it was found that this improvement

was strongly dependent on type of vowel. The highest identification improvement

was observed for vowels /o/ (18%), the lowest for vowel /i/ (2%). The best hearing-

impaired subject�s score was 97% averaged across vowels and the worst subject

scored 82% (99% and 75% for unprocessed vowels respectively).

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Grant # 8 T11E 017 17 from State Committee

for Scientific Research (KBN). Permission from Cambridge University (B. Mooreand T. Baer) for the use of their spectral enhancement algorithm is gratefully

acknowledged.

E. Ozimek et al. / Applied Acoustics 65 (2004) 473–483 483

References

[1] Andruski J, Nearey T. On the sufficiency of compound target specification of isolated vowel and

vowels in /bVb/ syllables. J Acoust Soc Am 1992;91:390–410.

[2] Baer T, Moore BCJ, Gatehouse S. Spectral contrast enhancement of speech in noise for listeners with

sensorineural hearing impairment: effects on intelligibility, quality and response times. J Rehabil Res

Dev 1993;30:49–72.

[3] Baer T, Moore BCJ. Spectral enhancement to compensate for reduced frequency selectivity. J Acoust

Soc Am 1994;95:2992.

[4] Boers PM. Formant enhancement of speech for listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. IPO Ann

Prog Rep 1980;15:21–8.

[5] Chistovich LA. Central auditory processing of peripheral vowel spectra. J Acoust Soc Am

1985;77:789–805.

[6] Clarke FR. Constant-ratio rule for confusion matrices in speech communication. J Acoust Soc Am

1957;29:715–20.

[7] Coughlin M, Kewley-Port D, Humes LE. The relation between identification and discrimination of

vowels in young and elderly listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 1998;104:3597–607.

[8] Dubno JR, Dorman FM. Effects of spectral flattening on vowel identification. J Acoust Soc Am

1987;82:1503–11.

[9] Franck BAM, Sidonne C, van Kreveld-Bos GM, Dreschler WA. Evaluation of spectral enhancement

in hearing aids combined with phonemic compression. J Acoust Soc Am 1999;106:1452–64.

[10] Jenkins JJ, Strange W, Miranda S. Vowel identification in mixed-speaker silent-center syllables. J

Acoust Soc Am 1994;95:1030–43.

[11] Klatt DH. Prediction of perceived phonetic distance from critical-band spectra: A first step. Proc

IEEE Int Conf Speech Acoust Signal Process 1982;129:1278–81.

[12] Leek MR, Dorman MF, Summerfield Q. Minimum spectral contrast for vowel identification by

normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 1987;81:148–54.

[13] Moore BCJ. Parallels between frequency selectivity measured psychophysically and in cochlear

mechanism. Scand Audiol Suppl 1986;25:139–52.

[14] N�ab�elek AK, Czyzewski Z, Krishnan LA. The influence of talker differences on vowel identification

by norm hearing and hearing impaired people. J Acoust Soc Am 1992;92:1228–46.

[15] Peterson GE, Lehiste I. Duration of syllable nuclei in English. J Acoust Soc Am 1960;32:693–703.

[16] Skrodzka E, Wicher A, Ozimek E, Sezk A. Auditory filters in sensorineural hearing-impaired subjects.

Arch Acoust 2002;27:159–74.

[17] Summerfield Q, Foster J, Tyler R, Bailey P. Influences of formant bandwidths and auditory frequency

selectivity on identification of place of articulation in stop consonants. Speech Commun 1985;4:213–

29.

[18] Tyler RS, Fernande M, Wood EJ. Masking, temporal integration and speech intelligibility in

individuals with noise-induced hearing loss. In: Taylor I, Markides A, editors. Disorder of auditory

function, vol. 3. London: Academic Press; 1980.

[19] Van Tasell D, Fabry DA, Thibodeau LM. Vowel identification and vowel masking patterns of

hearing-impaired subjects. J Acoust Soc Am 1987;81:1586–97.

[20] Van Veen TM, Houtgast T. Spectral sharpness and vowel dissimilarity. J Acoust Soc Am

1985;77:628–34.

[21] Verbrugge R, Strange W, Shankweiler D, Edman T. What information enables a listener to map a

talker�s vowel space? J Acoust Soc Am 1976;60:198–212.