HAVING, GIVING & TAKING - UvA Scripties

25
HAVING, GIVING & TAKING BIG DATA ON THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIAL CLASS AND PROSOCIAL BEHAVIOR In the current research it is examined how social class is related to prosocial behavior. Whereas previous research has found inconsistent results using experimental lab settings and survey approaches, we analysed actual lending and borrowing behavior in a natural setting. Study 1 (N=16.251) found that there is no meaningful relation between social class and lending. Study 2 (N=98), combined behavioral data with survey data and found, contrastingly, that higher social class is associated with more lending. Higher social class was also associated with more borrowing. We conclude that the theoretical field on prosocial behavior highly benefits from a more natural research approach. Statistical challenges regarding analysing big data are discussed. Student name: Mayra Kapteyn Student number: 10002758 Supervisor: Gerben van Kleef Secondary assessor: Eftychia Stamkou Date: January 8th, 2016

Transcript of HAVING, GIVING & TAKING - UvA Scripties

HAVING,  G IV ING  &  TAK ING  

BIG  DATA  ON  THE  RELATIONSHIP  BETWEEN  SOCIAL  CLASS  AND  PROSOCIAL  BEHAVIOR  

 

In  the  current  research  it  is  examined  how  social  class  is  related  to  prosocial  behavior.  

Whereas  previous  research  has  found  inconsistent  results  using  experimental  lab  settings  and  

survey  approaches,  we  analysed  actual  lending  and  borrowing  behavior  in  a  natural  setting.  

Study   1   (N=16.251)   found   that   there   is   no   meaningful   relation   between   social   class   and  

lending.   Study   2   (N=98),   combined   behavioral   data   with   survey   data   and   found,  

contrastingly,  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  more  lending.  Higher  social  class  was  

also   associated   with  more   borrowing.  We   conclude   that   the   theoretical   field   on   prosocial  

behavior   highly   benefits   from   a   more   natural   research   approach.   Statistical   challenges  

regarding  analysing  big  data  are  discussed.  

 

 

 

 

 

 

Student  name:  Mayra  Kapteyn  

Student  number:  10002758  

Supervisor:  Gerben  van  Kleef  

Secondary  assessor: Eftychia  Stamkou    

Date:  January  8th,  2016  

  2  

Participative  societies  thrive  on  prosocial  behavior.  People  help  each  other  out  by  

sharing,  volunteering  and  many  other  kinds  of  behavior  that  are  intended  to  benefit  

another.  They  do  this  either  because  it  brings  them  some  kind  of  reward  in  exchange  and/or  

because  they  feel  an  empathetic  and  compassionate  response  to  someone  else’s  need  

(Batson  &  Shaw,  1991).  Helping  somebody  brings  prosperity  for  the  other,  but  how  does  

one’s  own  prosperity  relate  to  the  willingness  to  help  the  other?  Do  people  who  have  more,  

give  more,  or  is  it  the  other  way  around?  

In  this  research  project  we  assessed  how  social  class  is  related  to  prosocial  behavior.  

Social  class  can  be  measured  by  material  resources,  such  as  education  (Snibbe  &  Markus,  

2005),  income  (Kraus  &  Keltner,  2009)  and  occupational  status  (Oakes  &  Rossi,  2003),  as  well  

as  social  class  rank,  which  is  a  subjective  perception  of  rank  in  comparison  to  others  (Kraus,  

Piff  &  Keltner,  2009).  Different  social  classes  experience  different  levels  of  opportunity,  

which  shapes  the  way  they  think  (Johnson  &  Kreuger,  2006).  We  will  explain  how  these  

cognitions  differ  and  how  that  might  impact  prosocial  behavior.    

Two  viewpoints  suggest  contrasting  hypotheses  regarding  social  class  and  prosocial  

behavior.  First,  the  negative  relation  hypothesis  stems  from  the  social  cognitive  theory  of  

class  (Kraus,  Piff,  Mendoza-­‐Denton,  Rheinschmidt  &  Keltner,  2012),  which  suggests  that  

lower  class’  act  more  prosocial  than  upper  class  individuals  because  they  are  more  attuned  

to  their  environment  (Piff,  Kraus,  Côté,  Cheng  &  Keltner,  2010).  Contrastingly,  a  positive  

relation  hypothesis  stems  from  the  concept  of  Noblesse  Oblige.  This  viewpoint  predicts  that  

prosocial  behavior  is  higher  among  upper  class  individuals  because  their  relative  cost  of  

helping  is  lower  in  comparison  to  lower  class  individuals  (Dovidio,  Piliavin,  Schroeder  &  

Penner,  2006;  Batson  &  Shaw,  1991).

Negative  relation  hypothesis:  Social  class  and  Contextualism  

Recent  research  has  suggested  that  there  are  cognitive  differences  between  lower  

and  upper  class  individuals  that  influence  people’s  prosocial  behavior  (Piff  et  al.,  2010).  

Lower  class  individuals  are  suggested  to  have  a  more  contextual  social  cognition,  which  

means  they  draw  on  external  forces  to  explain  personal,  social  and  political  events  (Kraus  et  

al.,  2012).  This  contextual  thinking  implies  paying  a  lot  of  attention  to  other  people’s  

thoughts  and  actions;  therefore,  Piff  et  al.,  (2010)  hypothesized  that  people  with  contextual  

  3  

social  cognition  generate  behavior  that  is  highly  influenced  by  other  people.  Upper  class  

individuals,  on  the  other  hand,  tend  to  have  what  is  called  a  solipsistic  relation  to  the  outside  

world  (Kraus  et  al.,  2012).  This  refers  to  an  individualistic  orientation  to  the  environment,  

motivated  by  internal  states,  goals  and  emotions.  It  implies  a  higher  sense  of  personal  

control  over  one’s  life  outcomes.    

Indeed,  in  comparison  to  their  upper  class  counterparts,  lower  class  individuals  are  

more  dependent  on  their  external  world  for  their  personal  outcomes  (Argygle,  1994),  and  

experience  less  control  over  their  lives  (Johnson  &  Krueger,  2006;  Lachman  &  Weaver,  

1998).  This  lack  of  personal  control  drives  lower  class  individuals  to  explain  success  to  

situational  factors,  while  upper  class  individuals  attribute  success  to  internal  traits  (Kraus  et  

al.,  2009).    

This  reduced  sense  of  personal  control  and  dependency  on  other  people  (Kraus  et  al.,  

2009)  may  entice  lower  class  people  to  engage  more  with  one  another.  In  personal  

interactions,  lower  class  individuals  show  a  more  socially  engaged  non-­‐verbal  style  than  

upper  class  individuals,  who  show  relatively  more  impolite  behaviors  such  as  self-­‐grooming  

(Kraus  &  Keltner,  2009).  It  is  theorised  that  due  to  lower  class’  reduced  sense  of  personal  

control,  they  think  in  a  contextual  way,  leading  them  to  be  socially  engaged.  

Consequentially,  their  contextual  cognition  may  result  in  them  being  more  helpful  towards  

other  people  (Piff  et  al.,  2010;  Kraus  et  al.,  2012).  

The  question  remains  if  lower  class’  contextual  focus  leads  them  to  be  more  prosocial  

than  upper  class  individuals.  Research  on  prosocial  behavior  has  shown  that  lower  class  

individuals  are  better  at  judging  other  people’s  emotions,  signifying  more  empathic  accuracy  

(Kraus,  Côté  &  Keltner,  2010).  They  also  report  higher  levels  of  compassion  in  response  to  

seeing  someone  else  suffering,  which  is  also  reflected  in  their  decreased  heart  rate,  a  

symptom  associated  with  feeling  compassion  (Stellar,  Manzo,  Kraus  &  Keltner,  2012).  This  

enhanced  empathetic  accuracy  and  feelings  of  compassion  may  lead  lower  class  individuals  

to  act  more  prosocially  than  upper  class  individuals.  

To  examine  this  hypothesis,  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  conducted  four  studies  and  found  

evidence  that  social  class  is  negatively  related  to  displays  of  prosocial  behavior.  However,  

their  methodology  is  flawed  in  the  sense  that  they  don’t  measure  real-­‐world  prosocial  

  4  

behavior.  The  measures  of  prosocial  behavior  are  either  attitude  measures  (Study  2)  or  

experimental  measures  in  a  lab  setting  (Study  1,  3  and  4).  We  will  address  each  study  and  

explain  how  these  measures  lack  external  validity.  

In  Study  1,  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  found  that  subjective  social  rank  is  related  to  decreased  

generosity  in  the  Dictator  Game.  The  Dictator  Game  is  an  adequate,  highly  controllable  

measure  of  prosocial  behavior.  It  is  however  a  very  simplified  reconstruction  of  reality,  

because  the  situation  depicted  the  Dictator  Game  –having  to  distribute  points  between  

oneself  and  a  stranger-­‐  is  one  that  does  not  (often)  present  itself  often  in  real  life.  Therefore,  

additional  methods  using  measures  closely  related  to  real  life  is  needed.  

Second,  they  found  that  manipulated  social  rank  and  income  negatively  predict  

attitudes  on  the  amount  of  money  people  should  donate  to  charity.  This  attitudinal  measure  

is  flawed  because  it  does  not  control  for  social  desirability  bias  (Randall  &  Fernandes,  1991).  

This  is  a  serious  problem  to  validity  because  different  social  classes  may  be  more  or  less  

triggered  to  respond  socially  desirable.  Especially  lower  class  individual’s  contextual  

cognition  (Kraus  et  al.,  2012)  may  make  them  more  susceptible  to  social  desired  responding  

because  they  may  be  more  attuned  to  leaving  a  good  impression  with  others.  The  second  

problem  with  the  attitudinal  measure  is  that  attitudes  generally  don’t  predict  behavior  very  

well:  only  when  the  timing,  context,  action  and  target  of  the  attitude  measure  and  the  

behavior  are  similar  (Ajzen  &  Fishbein,  1977).  In  Study  2,  prosocial  behavior  is  measured  by  

the  question  “what  portion  of  one’s  salary  should  be  allocated  to  charitable  donations”.  

Because  there  are  no  real  costs  involved,  answering  this  question  in  a  prosocial  manner  is  

much  easier  said  than  done,  so  the  action  in  the  attitude  measure  does  not  resemble  the  

actual  action.  Therefore,  the  measured  attitudes  on  donations  arguably  do  not  predict  class-­‐

driven  behavior  well.  

Third,  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  reported  a  negative  relationship  between  social  class  and  

prosocial  behavior,  mediated  by  egalitarian  values.  However,  they  assessed  prosocial  

behavior  using  the  Trust  Game,  which  is  not  a  valid  measure  of  prosocial  behavior.  The  

participant  namely  allocates  points  to  another  participant,  while  the  other  participant  has  

the  chance  to  return  the  favour  with  increased  value  of  the  points.  This  task  does  not  

measure  prosocial  behavior;  rather,  it  measures  whether  participants  choose  a  risky,  

  5  

cooperative  but  potentially  rewarding  strategy,  or  a  safe  individualist  strategy,  with  less  

potential  rewards.  Prosocial  behavior  is  defined  as  behavior  intended  to  benefit  the  other  

(Brief  &  Motowidlo,  1986),  but  in  this  case,  the  ultimate  intention  of  the  participant  may  just  

be  to  receive  the  maximum  points  for  themselves.  Upper  class’  solipsistic  cognitions  may  

lead  them  to  choose  a  more  individualistic  strategy,  but  that  does  not  mean  they’re  less  

prosocial.  Therefore,  this  is  not  an  accurate  measure  of  prosocial  behavior.  

Fourth  and  finally,  it  was  reported  that  compassion  moderates  the  negative  

relationship  between  social  class  and  helping  behavior.  This  experiment  was  the  only  explicit  

behavioral  measure  used  in  this  research.  The  measure  of  prosocial  behavior  was  the  time  

the  participant  took  to  help  a  female  confederate  who  arrived  late  to  do  her  task.  This  

measure  is  biased  by  social  norm  rigidity,  because  the  help  recipient’s  distress  (and  thus,  

need  for  help)  is  a  direct  consequence  of  her  own  lack  of  punctuality.  Lack  of  punctuality  is  

something  people  can  disapprove  strongly  of,  especially  towards  women  (Kanekar  &  Vaz,  

1993).  Bowles  and  Gelfand  (2010)  found  that  when  a  low-­‐status  individual  (operationalised  

as  “lacking  a  high-­‐status  track  record”,  as  is  the  case  with  the  confederate)  violates  a  norm,  

upper  class  individuals  punish  more  heavily  than  lower  class  individuals.  In  a  subsequent  

study,  they  found  that  men  are  more  eager  to  punish  female  norm  violators  than  male  norm  

violators.  These  findings  seriously  question  the  conclusion  drawn  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010),  

because  the  supposedly  direct  effect  of  social  class  on  prosocial  behavior  may  be  

confounded  by  norm  rigidity  towards  the  female,  norm-­‐violating  confederate.  

Thus,  although  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  may  have  a  solid  theoretical  background  to  

hypothesize  that  social  class  inhibits  prosocial  behavior,  their  measures  of  prosocial  behavior  

lack  external  validity.  The  Dictator  Game  in  Study  1  is  accurate  but  simplified,  the  measure  in  

Study  2  is  merely  an  attitude  measure,  study  3  measures  strategy  instead  of  helping  and  the  

measure  in  study  4  is  confounded  by  norm  rigidity.  These  flawed  measures  of  prosocial  

behavior  imply  that  these  experimental  results  may  not  be  valid  in  the  real  world.  Therefore,  

we  cannot  conclude  on  a  negative  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  

Positive  relation  hypothesis:  Noblesse  Oblige  

A  contrasting  hypothesis  arises  from  the  Noblesse  Oblige  concept:  those  who  have  

more,  will  give  more.  Because  lower  class  individuals  have  less  resources,  the  relative  cost  of  

  6  

being  prosocial  is  higher.  The  higher  the  cost  compared  to  rewards,  the  lower  the  probability  

that  somebody  will  help  (Dovidio  et  al.,  2006;  Batson  &  Shaw,  1991).  So  simply  because  

lower  class  individuals  have  relatively  less  to  give,  they  may  act  less  prosocial  than  upper  

class  individuals.  

Korndörfer,  Egloff  and  Schmukle  (2015)  tested  whether  there  is  a  positive  or  a  

negative  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  They  conducted  eight  survey  

studies  and  reported  mixed  results.  For  example,  Study  1,  which  was  conducted  in  Germany,  

reported  no  significant  relationship  between  social  class  and  relative  amount  of  money  

spent  on  charity  among  donating  households.  Contrastingly,  Study  2,  which  was  conducted  

in  the  U.S.,  reported  a  negative  relationship  between  social  class  and  relative  amount  of  

money  spent  on  charity  among  donating  households.  Then  Study  3  –also  using  U.S.  data  on  

donating-­‐  reported  a  positive  relationship.  Studies  4  (conducted  in  Germany)  and  5  

(conducted  in  the  US)  also  reported  positive  relationships  between  social  class  and  reported  

volunteering.  The  results  from  Study  6  reported  a  meaningless  (b=.06)  but  significant  

positive  association  between  social  class  and  volunteering,  among  37.000+  participants  

internationally.  Study  7  found  a  significant  positive  association  between  objective  social  class  

and  everyday  helping,  but  no  significant  association  between  subjective  social  class  and  

everyday  helping.  Study  8  found  a  significant  positive  relationship  between  social  class  and  

allocated  points  in  the  Trust  Game.  These  results  are  not  consistent,  but  seem  to  point  in  the  

direction  that  there  is  a  positive  relationship  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior,  

contrary  to  the  findings  from  Piff  et  al.  (2010).    

However,  the  methodology  used  in  this  study  also  lacks  external  validity.  Seven  out  of  

eight  studies  conducted  were  survey  measures.  Survey  measures  on  past  behavior  (such  as  

volunteering  and  donating  behavior)  are  biased  by  socially  desirable  responding  and  recall  

bias  (Coughlin,  1990;  Randal  &  Fernandes,  1991).  Especially  prosocial  behavior  is  sensitive  to  

social  desirability  bias  because  prosocial  behavior  is  very  socially  desired  in  definition.  

Therefore,  survey  measures  are  not  adequate  measures  of  prosocial  behavior.  

The  only  behavioral  measure  used  in  this  research  is  the  Trust  Game  in  Study  8,  

which  (as  described  above)  measures  behavior  intended  to  benefit  the  self,  not  the  other.  So  

in  eight  studies,  none  of  the  measures  of  prosocial  behavior  contain  actual  behavior.  

  7  

Therefore,  these  results  don’t  provide  sufficient  evidence  that  there  is  a  positive  relation  

between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior,  or  to  state  that  the  negative  relation  hypothesis  

from  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  is  invalidated.  There  is  behavioral  research  needed  in  a  natural  setting  

in  order  to  assess  a  valid  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  

Current  Research  

The  current  literature  on  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  has  used  only  

experimental  or  survey  data,  but  no  behavioral  data  in  a  natural  setting,  and  therefore  it  

may  not  be  valid  in  the  real  world.  Therefore,  we  tested  whether  social  class  is  positively  or  

negatively  related  to  prosocial  behavior  with  actual  behavioral  measures  in  a  natural  setting.  

We  tested  if  participant’s  social  class  is  associated  with  lending  household  items  through  an  

online  sharing  platform  called  Peerby.  Hypothesis  1a  is  that  social  class  is  negatively  

associated  with  lending  and  hypothesis  1b  is  that  social  class  is  positively  associated  with  

lending.  

Balancing  Giving  and  Taking  

Aside  from  the  lack  of  behavioral  measures,  another  missing  element  in  the  current  

literature  regarding  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  is  the  balance  between  giving  and  

receiving  help.  Thus  far,  we  have  a  unilateral  understanding  of  prosocial  behavior,  because  

we  only  have  information  on  how  much  people  give,  and  none  on  how  much  people  take.  

The  social  cognitive  theory  on  social  class  (Kraus  et  al.,  2012)  predicts  that  upper  class  

individuals  experience  more  personal  control  and  are  therefore  less  attuned  to  their  

environment.  This  may  have  different  implications  for  requesting  help.  Hypothesis  2a  is  that  

upper  class’  elevated  sense  of  personal  control  (Johnson  &  Krueger,  2006;  Kraus  et  al.,  2009)  

reduces  their  tendency  to  ask  for  help  because  of  elevated  levels  of  individualism  (see  Kraus  

et  al.,  2012).  Contrastingly,  and  alternative  explanation  is  that  upper  class’  elevated  sense  of  

personal  control  triggers  assertiveness  –  not  being  afraid  to  ask.  Following  this  reasoning,  

hypothesis  2b  is  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  asking  for  help  more  often.    

By  combining  the  measures  between  giving  and  receiving  help,  we  can  get  a  sense  

whether  there  is  truly  a  relation  between  social  class  and  prosociality,  or  that  there  is  only  a  

relation  between  social  class  and  activity  on  Peerby.  Namely,  if  upper  or  lower  social  class  

  8  

individuals  would  both  borrow  and  lend  more  than  the  other,  it  wouldn’t  necessarily  mean  

that  they  are  more  prosocial  –  it  could  also  just  mean  that  they  are  more  participative  on  

Peerby.  Therefore,  we  tested  the  relation  between  social  class  and  a  prosociality  ratio  of  

lending  minus  borrowing.  Hypothesis  3a  is  that  the  relationship  between  social  class  and  

prosocial  behavior  upholds  when  subtracting  borrowing  behavior  from  lending  behavior.  

This  would  signal  strong  differences  in  prosociality  among  social  classes.  Contrastingly,  

hypothesis  3b  is  that  there  is  no  relation  between  social  class  and  prosociality  when  

subtracting  borrowing  behavior  from  lending  behavior.  This  would  signal  that  a  difference  

between  social  classes  may  be  due  to  different  levels  of  activity  on  the  Peerby  platform,  and  

not  due  to  a  difference  in  prosociality.      

In  the  second  study,  in  order  to  validate  that  self-­‐report  measures  are  indeed  

inadequate  measures  of  prosocial  behavior,  we  also  assessed  how  the  self-­‐report  measures  

on  borrowing  and  lending  relate  to  the  behavioral  measures  on  borrowing  and  lending.  

Therefore,  hypothesis  4  is  that  there  is  a  low  correlation  between  self-­‐report  lending  and  

actual  lending,  and  hypothesis  5  is  that  that  there  is  a  low  correlation  between  self-­‐report  

borrowing  and  actual  borrowing.  

Study  1  uses  a  large  dataset  (N=16.251)  in  order  to  assess  robust  general  findings  on  

the  relations  between  social  class,  borrowing,  lending  and  prosociality  ratio.  We  measured  

social  class  by  combining  average  street  income  and  average  house  value  of  the  participant’s  

street.  In  Study  2,  we  enriched  the  street  level  social  class  measures  and  behavioral  data  

from  Peerby  with  survey  data  on  income,  age,  gender  and  self-­‐reported  borrowing  -­‐and  

lending.    

Study  1  

In  study  1,  big  data  is  used  to  assess  if  social  class  is  positively  or  negatively  related  to  

lending  (hypothesis  1)  and  borrowing  (hypothesis  2)  on  the  Peerby  platform.  Third,  we  

tested  if  the  relation  between  social  class  and  lending  would  uphold  when  subtracting  

borrowing  from  the  lending  score.  

The  behavioral  data  is  gathered  from  Peerby,  an  online  sharing  platform  where  

neighbors  lend  each  other  household  items  for  free.  Peerby  saves  button  clicks  on  the  

  9  

website  and  app  onto  their  database.  The  measure  of  lending  is  somewhat  determined  by  

what  objects  people  have  in  their  homes,  so  it  is  important  that  these  are  not  luxury  items  

that  only  upper  class  individuals  possess.  The  most  requested  items  on  Peerby  are:  (1)  drill,  

(2)  ladder,  (3)  standing  tables,  (4)  bike,  (5)  trailer  and  (6)  car.  There’s  no  way  of  knowing  

what  items  participants  exactly  have  in  their  home,  but  the  top  items  clearly  aren’t  exclusive  

to  higher  social  classes.  Therefore,  it  should  not  confound  the  measure  of  prosocial  

behavior.    

Method  

Participants  

A  dataset  containing  92.679  participants  was  provided  by  Peerby.  49.182  participants  

were  excluded  because  of  missing  values  for  all  of  the  social  class  measures,  namely  a)  they  

did  not  provide  their  full  6-­‐digit  zip  code  area  or  b)  there  was  no  data  on  income  or  house  

value  available  for  their  specific  zip  code  area.  To  account  for  the  high  number  of  people  

who  just  signed  up  for  Peerby  to  ‘take  a  look  around’,  we  selected  members  who  at  least  

lent  out  once.  After  excluding  inactive  members,  16.167  active  members  were  left  in  the  

dataset.  These  participants  were  members  for  510.36  days  on  average  (SD  =  273.67).  

Procedure  

FIGURE  1.  A  PEERBY  REQUEST  

  10  

The  Peerby  platform  works  demand  based:  when  someone  needs  something,  they  

send  out  a  request  to  their  neighbors  with  a  personal  message,  see  Figure  1.  The  receiver  of  

the  message  can  then  click  ‘Help  neighbor  X’;  ‘Not  now’  or  ‘I  don’t  have  it’.  If  someone  clicks  

“Help  neighbor  X”,  the  two  neighbors  enter  a  chat  page  where  they  can  make  arrangements  

to  pick  up  the  item.  As  there  are  often  more  than  one  neighbor  offering  the  item,  the  

requesting  neighbor  chooses  one  of  the  offering  neighbors,  then  picks  the  item  up  at  their  

home  address  and  returns  it  after  use.    

Measures  

Lending.    Lending  was  measured  by  the  total  number  of  ‘Help’  clicks  per  member,  see  

Figure  1.  This  is  the  total  number  of  help  clicks  on  both  the  app  and  the  web  platform  (M  =  

4.20,  SD  =  7.37).  The  recipient  sees  the  date,  a  photograph  of  the  requester,  the  distance  to  

the  recipient,  their  name,  the  item  that  they  need  and  a  personal  message  they  provide.  

They  can  choose  to  click  ‘Help’;  ‘Not  now’  or  ‘I  don’t  have  it’.  There  is  also  a  flag  button,  in  

case  the  request  is  inappropriate  or  unwanted.  

Borrowing.    Requested  help  was  measured  by  the  total  amount  of  requests  the  participant  

has  placed  on  the  website,  see  Figure  2.  In  order  to  request  an  item  on  the  Peerby  platform,  

the  participant  describes  the  item  that  they  need  and  a  short  story  to  describe  what  they  

need  it  for  (see  Figure  2).  Then  Peerby  sends  the  request  to  max.  250  of  the  participant’s  

neighbors.  It  is  communicated  that  people  receive  an  offer  from  their  neighbors  in  thirty  

minutes  on  average.  The  average  total  number  of  borrowing  is  1.42  times  (SD  =  2.27).  

 

 

 

 

 

  11  

 

FIGURE  2.  A  PEERBY  REQUEST  FORM  

Prosociality  index.    A  prosociality  index  was  computed  by  standardizing  borrowing  

and  lending,  and  then  subtracting  borrowing  from  lending.  Thus  the  higher  the  score,  the  

more  prosocial  the  behavior  on  Peerby.  

Social  Class.    Social  class  was  measured  by  standardizing  the  average  income  and  the  

average  house  value  in  the  participant’s  street.  These  two  variables  are  highly  correlated,  

r(7550)=.68,  p<.001,  thus  predict  social  class  reliably.  If  one  of  the  two  values  was  missing,  

only  the  other  variable  was  used  as  proxy  for  social  class.  There  was  more  data  available  on  

income  (N=16167)  than  on  house  value  (N=7550).    

This  data  is  obtained  from  the  Dutch  national  databank  (CBS,  2012),  who  published  it  

as  customized  data  by  the  request  of  Sinfore  and  the  Jan  van  Es  Institute.  House  value  is  only  

published  when  there  are  at  least  20  venues  in  the  6-­‐digit  zip  area,  rounded  off  and  reported  

in  units  of  thousand.  The  average  income  is  only  published  when  there  are  at  least  10  

income  receivers  in  the  6-­‐digit  zip  area  and  rounded  off  to  values  of  one  hundred.  When  the  

  12  

average  monthly  income  was  below  the  minimal  (€500)  or  above  the  maximum  (€10.000)  

value,  the  minimal  (€500)  or  maximum  (€10.000)  value  was  reported.  

Results  

The  first  step  was  to  assess  how  social  class  relates  to  prosocial  behavior.  Hypothesis  

1a  was  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  less  lending  (see  Piff  et  al.,  2010),  while  

hypothesis  1b  predicted  a  positive  relation  between  social  class  and  lending  (see  Korndörfer  

et  al.,  2015).    

Due   to   the   non-­‐parametric   distribution   of   the   data,   we   ranked   the   values   and  

computed  Spearman’s  Rho  correlation.  Table  1  shows  that,  supporting  the  positive  relation  

hypothesis,  a  very  weak  but  significant  positive  relationship  was  found  between  social  class  

and  prosocial  behavior.  Hypothesis  2a  was  that  social  class  would  be  negatively  associated  

with  social  class,  while  hypothesis  2b  predicted  a  positive  association.  We  found  that  higher  

social  class  was  associated  with  less  borrowing,  therefore  confirming  hypothesis  2a.  We  thus  

confirmed  hypothesis  3a,  which  suggested  that  the  found  relationship  between  social  class  

and   prosocial   behavior   is   indeed   caused   by   higher   prosocial   behavior   among   upper   class  

individuals,  and  not  by  increased  Peerby  activity  overall.    

Table  1  

Spearman’s  rho  Correlations  between  measures,  (N)  

Measure   Lending   Borrowing   Prosociality  ratio  

Social  Class   .03*  (16251)   -­‐.04*  (16251)   .05*  (16251)  

Note.  *p<.001  

Although   these   results   suggest   significant   relations,   the   coefficients   are   extremely  

weak.   The  explained  variance  of   social   class  on   lending   is  only  ρ2=.001,   and   the  explained  

variance  of  social  class  on  prosociality  is  ρ2=.003.  Therefore,  we  cannot  conclude  there  is  a  

meaningful  relationship  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  

Discussion  

  13  

In  Study  1,  we  did  not  find  a  meaningful  relationship  between  social  class  and  

prosocial  behavior.  This  indicates  that  either  the  assumed  relationship  between  social  class  

and  prosocial  behavior  is  non-­‐existent  in  a  natural  setting,  or  that  the  relationship  is  more  

complex  than  could  be  measured  in  this  research  design.    

In  the  current  study,  it  was  impossible  to  account  for  demographic  data  such  as  

gender  and  age.  Moreover,  we  could  not  assess  participant’s  social  class  directly,  but  used  

proxy  data  from  the  street  the  participant  lived  in.  Additional  research  is  needed  to  verify  

the  quality  of  the  social  class  measure  and  to  control  for  age  and  gender.  That  way  we  can  

draw  conclusions  on  the  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  

Study  2  

Our  second  study  investigated  the  relationship  between  social  class  and  borrowing  

and  lending  among  a  smaller  sample  of  Peerby  members,  with  direct  measures  of  income,  

education  level,  age  and  gender.  However,  unfortunately,  we  weren’t  able  to  control  for  age  

and  gender  because  the  data  did  not  meet  parametric  assumptions.  

Taking  the  results  of  Study  1  in  account,  we  predicted  that,  following  hypothesis  1b,  

higher  social  class  would  be  associated  with  more  lending  and  borrowing.  The  findings  in  

Study  one  also  set  direction  for  the  second  hypothesis,  namely  that  higher  social  class  would  

be  associated  with  less  borrowing.  Third,  we  expected  that  following  hypothesis  3a,  the  

relation  between  social  class  and  lending  would  uphold  when  accounting  for  borrowing  

behavior.    Fourth,  we  hypothesized  that  self-­‐report  measures  on  lending  would  weakly  

correlate  with  actual  lending.  Similar  to  that  account,  our  fifth  hypothesis  was  that  self-­‐

report  measures  on  borrowing  would  be  weakly  correlated  with  actual  borrowing.      

Method  

Participants.  Participants  are  members  of  Peerby  who  responded  to  a  survey  

regarding  participation  on  sharing  platforms,  conducted  by  Stipo  (N=  180).  Stipo  is  a  

consultancy  firm  that  published  a  report  on  participative  Internet  Platforms  (Stipo,  2015).  

Participants  were  contacted  through  e-­‐mail  and  asked  to  complete  a  58  item  survey  on  their  

Peerby  behavior.  A  €50  voucher  was  allotted  among  the  participants.  71  participants  were  

excluded  because  of  missing  data.  We  also  excluded  11  participants  because  their  lending  

  14  

count  was  above  40,  so  they  were  clear  outliers  who  impacted  results  disproportionately.  

Two  of  these  participants  were  employees  of  Peerby  or  Stipo.  This  left  99  participants  in  the  

final  analysis  (57  female,  42  male)  who  were  members  for  539.85  days  on  average  (SD  =  

262.62).  Participants  ranged  in  age  from  25  to  65  years  old  (M  =  44.09,  SD  =  11.35).  97%  of  

the  respondents  reported  a  Dutch  nationality.    

Measures  

Self-­‐report  income  measure.  The  self  report  measure  inquired  “what  is  your  income  

level  on  a  yearly  basis”  using  six  categories:  (1)  <€20.000,  (2)  €20.000-­‐€30.000,  (3)  €30.000-­‐

€50.000,  (4)  €50.000-­‐€70.000,  (5)  €70.000-­‐€100.000,  or  (6)  >€100.000.    

This  manner  of  questioning  is  suboptimal  because  it  remained  unclear  whether  it  

referred  to  personal  or  household  income,  and  before  or  after  tax  deductions.  However,  

because  it  is  not  likely  that  there  is  an  effect  of  social  class  on  the  way  people  respond  to  this  

question;  we  assume  that  the  inaccuracy  is  distributed  equally  among  social  classes.  In  order  

to  calculate  correlations  using  income  as  a  scale,  participant’s  income  levels  were  recoded  

into  numerical  scale  variables  (1)  €18.000*,  (2)  €25.000,  (3)  €40.000  (4)  €60.000,  (5)  €85.000,  

and  (6)  €120.000.  Participants  reported  a  median  income  level  of  €40.000.    

Street  income.  The  street  income  measure  was  (similar  to  Study  1)  obtained  from  the  

CBS  data  regarding  mean  income  per  month  in  the  participant’s  street  of  residence  (CBS,  

2012).  The  mean  monthly  street  income  was  €2840,  with  a  standard  deviation  of  €989.    

In  order  to  assess  the  validity  of  the  income  proxy  measure,  we  calculated  Pearson  

correlation  between  the  and  the  self-­‐report  income  measure.  The  correlation  was  moderate,  

r(74)=.37,  p<.01.  So  even  though  one  measure  is  direct  and  the  other  indirect,  they  are  

moderately  associated  with  each  other.    

Street  house  value.  The  average  street  house  value  was  obtained  from  CBS  data  

regarding  average  house  values  per  street,  following  the  same  procedure  to  study  1.  The  

average  house  value  was  €212.450  (SD  =  €87.468).    

                                                                                                                         

*  €18.000  is  chosen  because  it  is  the  minimum  wage  for  people  above  23  years  old  in  The  Netherlands  

  15  

Social  class.  A  composite  measure  of  social  class  was  computed  by  standardizing  the  

self-­‐report  income  measure,  the  street  income  measure  and  the  street  house  value  

measure,  then  averaging  the  scores  on  these  three  measures.  If  one  of  the  measures  was  

missing,  the  average  of  the  other  measures  was  taken.  A  Crohnbach’s  Alpha  (using  

standardized  values)  of  α=.73  showed  this  composite  of  social  class  was  a  reliable  predictor.    

Education  level.  Education  level  was  assessed  using  six  categories:  (1)  lbo  (lower  craft  

education),  (2)  vmbo  (high  school),  (3)  mavo  (high  school),  (4)  havo  (high  school),  (5)  vwo  

(high  school),  (6)  mbo  (college),  (7)  hbo  (college),  and  (8)  university.  The  median  education  

level  was  HBO  (college).    

Interestingly,  education  level  and  self-­‐reported  income  were  not  correlated,  

r(96)=.17,  p=.10.  Therefore,  education  level  was  not  combined  with  the  other  social  class  

measures  to  represent  social  class.  This  lack  of  reliability  in  using  education  as  a  measure  of  

social  class  may  be  explained  by  the  egalitarian  education  model  in  the  Netherlands.  Higher  

education  is  government  funded  with  additional  funding  for  lower  class  individuals.  Thus,  

even  though  education  has  previously  been  used  as  a  measure  of  social  class  (Snibbe  &  

Markus,  2006),  this  measure  may  not  be  valid  in  highly  egalitarian  educational  systems.    

Lending  behavior.  Lending  behavior  was,  similar  to  Study  1,  tracked  by  Peerby.  The  

measure  represents  the  total  number  of  times  the  participant  has  clicked  “Help  neighbor  X”,  

in  order  to  lend  something  to  another  member  of  Peerby,  see  Figure  1.  Participants  lent  9.37  

times  on  average  (SD  =  8.5).    

Self-­‐reported  lending.  Participants  self-­‐reported  how  often  they  lent  things  per  year  

using  six  categories:  (1)  once  per  week,  (2)  twice  or  more  per  week,  (3)  once  a  month,  (4)  a  

few  times  per  month,  (5)  a  few  times  per  year,  or  (6)  once  a  year.  In  order  to  calculate  

correlations,  these  scores  were  recoded  into  scale  values  of  lending  frequency  per  year:  (1)  

once  per  week  into  52,  (2)  twice  or  more  per  week  into  104,  (3)  once  a  month  into  12,  (4)  a  

few  times  per  month  into  24,  (5)  a  few  times  per  year  into  2,  and  (6)  once  a  year  into  1.    

Participants  reported  an  average  lending  of  4.64  times  per  year  (SD  =  5.95).  The  

discrepancy  between  mean  of  the  self-­‐report  data  and  the  behavioural  measure  can  be  

  16  

explained  by  the  fact  that  offering  to  lend  does  not  always  result  in  actual  lending.  Often,  

help  requesters  receive  multiple  lending  offers  and  choose  one  neighbor  to  borrow  it  from.    

Borrowing  behavior.  Borrowing  behavior  was  also  tracked  by  Peerby,  just  like  done  

in  Study  1  (M  =  2.75,  SD  =  3.39).  The  count  number  represents  the  total  number  of  times  the  

participant  has  requested  to  borrow  something  from  the  other  members  during  their  

membership.  

Self-­‐reported  borrowing.  In  the  survey,  participants  were  asked  how  often  they  

borrowed  things  per  year,  using  six  categories:  (1)  once  per  week,  (2)  twice  or  more  per  

week,  (3)  once  a  month,  (4)  a  few  times  per  month,  (5)  a  few  times  per  year,  or  (6)  once  a  

year.  This  scores  were  recoded  into  scale  values  of  lending  frequency  per  year:  (1)  once  per  

week  into  52,  (2)  twice  or  more  per  week  into  104,  (3)  once  a  month  into  12,  (4)  a  few  times  

per  month  into  24,  (5)  a  few  times  per  year  into  2,  and  (6)  once  a  year  into  1.  The  average  

frequency  was  3.21  times  per  year  (SD  =  4.77).    

Prosociality  ratio.  The  prosociality  ratio  was  computed  by  subtracting  the  

standardized  borrowing  score  from  the  standardized  lending  score.  This  represents  the  

balance  between  providing  for  others  and  receiving  help.  The  higher  the  score,  the  more  

lending  in  comparison  to  borrowing.  

Results  

In  this  study,  we  assessed  if  higher  social  class  is  related  to  more  prosocial  behavior.  

We  used  behavioral  data  on  lending  and  borrowing  to  measure  prosocial  behavior,  in  

combination  with  proxy  social  class  measures  and  survey  measures  on  income,  gender,  age  

and  self-­‐reported  lending  and  borrowing.  Because  the  behavioral  data  does  not  meet  

parametric  assumptions,  we  ranked  the  data  and  computed  Spearman’s  rho.  Table  2  shows  

a  summary  of  the  correlations  between  the  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior,  age  and  

gender.    

Hypothesis  1  was  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  more  lending.  Indeed,  

Spearman’s  rho  reported  that  there  is  a  moderate  positive  association  between  social  class  

and  lending,  r(96)=.23,  p<.05.  This  indicated  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  more  

lending,  and  that  the  negative  relation  found  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  is  falsified.    

  17  

Hypothesis  2  was  that  higher  social  class  is  associated  with  less  borrowing.  In  contrast  

to  that  prediction,  borrowing  was  marginally  significant  associated  with  social  class  in  a  

positive  direction,  r(98)=.16,  p=.06.  This  means  that  upper  class  individuals  seem  to  borrow  

more  than  lower  class  individuals.    

So  upper  class  participants  both  lend  and  borrow  more,  relative  to  lower  class  

participants.  Hypothesis  3  was  that  the  relation  between  social  class  and  prosociality  would  

uphold  when  accounting  for  both  borrowing  and  lending.  Because  of  the  unexpected  

positive  relation  between  social  class  and  borrowing,  there  was  no  significant  relation  

between  social  class  and  prosociality  ratio,  r(98)=-­‐.03,  p=.40.  This  suggests  that  the  

difference  in  lending  activity  found  among  social  classes  may  be  explained  by  different  levels  

of  participation  on  Peerby  in  general,  and  not  by  different  levels  of  prosociality.  However,  in  

order  to  conclude  this,  we  must  conduct  mediational  analysis,  which  is  not  possible  using  

Spearman’s  rho.  

As  for  hypothesis  4,  we  predicted  that  self-­‐reported  lending  would  be  weakly  

associated  with  actual  lending.  We  found  that  indeed,  self-­‐reported  lending  was  moderately  

correlated  with  actual  lending,  r(80)=.22,  p<.05.  This  indicates  that  what  participants  report  

on  lending  is  not  accurate  in  representing  actual  behavior.  Similarly,  hypothesis  5  predicted  

that  self-­‐reported  borrowing  would  also  be  weakly  associated  with  actual  borrowing.  Our  

results  showed  that  this  correlation  is  indeed  weak  and  only  borderline  significant,  r(58)=.22,  

p=.051.  So  in  borrowing  too,  participants’  self  reports  are  not  accurate.  Due  to  this  

inaccuracy,  as  can  be  seen  in  Table  2,  self-­‐reported  lending  is  not  significantly  associated  

with  social  class,  while  actual  lending  is  significantly  higher  among  upper  class  participants,  

compared  to  lower  class  participants.  This  informs  us  that  self-­‐report  measures  don’t  

accurately  represent  people’s  actual  behavior.    

Finally,  we  found  that  age  is  moderately  correlated  with  lending,  self-­‐reported  

lending  and  prosociality  ratio.  This  implies  that  age  may  be  a  factor  that  can  explain  the  

positive  relation  between  social  class  and  lending.  But,  similar  to  the  other  possible  

confounds,  there  is  mediational  analysis  needed  before  we  can  conclude  that  the  relation  

between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  is  indeed  mediated  by  participant’s  age.    

Table  2  

  18  

Correlation  Matrix  between  measures  of  Prosocial  behavior,  Social  Class  and  Demographic  

Measures  (N)  

Measure   Lending   Self-­‐reported  

Lending  

Borrowing   Self-­‐reported  

Borrowing  

Prosociality  

ratio  

Social  Class   .23  (98)**   .06  (79)   .16  (98)*   -­‐.12  (57)   -­‐.03  (98)  

Age   .29  (99)***   .19  (80)**   -­‐.15  (99)   -­‐.12  (58)   .32***  (99)  

Gender   .11  (99)   .10  (80)   .02  (99)   -­‐.05  (58)   .08  (99)  

Note.  *p<.10,  **p<.05,  ***p<.01  

Discussion  

The  results  of  Study  2  show  that,  confirming  hypothesis  1b,  there  is  indeed  a  positive  

relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  This  clearly  falsifies  the  negative  

relation  hypothesis  stipulated  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010),  who  suggests  that  due  to  lower  class’  

highly  contextual  cognition,  they  are  more  prosocial  than  lower  class  people.  In  this  study,  

were  able  to  connect  survey  data  to  actual  behavioral  data  gathered  in  a  natural  setting.  This  

gives  our  findings  great  external  validity,  while  also  ensuring  internal  validity  on  our  social  

class  measure,  because  we  were  able  to  measure  income  directly.  

 In  contrast  to  the  predicted  hypothesis  2a,  there  was  also  a  (borderline  significant)  

positive  relation  found  between  social  class  and  requesting  help.  So  upper  class  participants  

were  more  likely  to  both  lend  and  borrow  on  Peerby.  When  subtracting  borrowing  from  

lending  in  one  prosociality  ratio  (hypothesis  3),  there  was  no  relationship  found  between  

social  class  and  prosociality  ratio.    

These  results  signal  the  possibility  that  the  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  

behavior  may  not  be  be  caused  by  upper  class’  enhanced  prosociality,  but  on  their  increased  

tendency  to  participate  on  Peerby  in  general.  Due  to  statistical  limitations  in  analysing  the  

non-­‐parametric  dataset,  we  could  not  conduct  mediation  or  control  analyses  for  this  

alternative  explanation.  Our  results  do  however  send  a  clear  signal  that  prosocial  behavior  

needs  to  be  put  in  perspective  of  help  giving  as  well  as  giving  requesting.  

  19  

Also  concerning  correct  measuring  of  prosocial  behavior,  we  assessed  the  validity  of  

self-­‐report  data.  Confirming  hypothesis  4,  we  found  a  weak  correlation  between  self-­‐

reported  lending  and  actual  lending  behavior.  Similarly,  confirming  hypothesis  5,  we  found  a  

(borderline  significant)  weak  relationship  between  self-­‐reported  borrowing  and  actual  

borrowing  requests.  We  do  need  to  be  careful  interpreting  these  results.  Apart  from  to  the  

response  errors  such  as  recall  bias  and  socially  desired  responding  (Coughlin,  1990;  Randall  

&  Fernandes,  1991),  this  low  correlation  may  be  caused  by  the  rigid  categories  participants  

had  to  answer  to.  For  example,  65%  of  participants  reported  borrowing  “a  few  times  per  

year”.  This  categorical  way  of  asking  is  necessary  because  people  aren’t  able  provide  an  

exact  number  how  often  they  did  something.  However,  this  does  lead  categorical  data  to  

lose  much  of  the  variance  that  natural  data  has,  especially  when  events  are  not  frequent  and  

spread  out  over  a  long  period  of  time  (Coughlin,  1990).  So,  our  results  show  that  categorical  

self-­‐report  measures  specifically  don’t  predict  real-­‐world  behavior  accurately.    

Finally,  an  alternative  hypothesis  could  be  that  the  relation  between  social  class  and  

lending  is  confounded  by  participant’s  age.  We  found  a  moderate  correlation  between  age  

and  lending  on  Peerby.  Because  the  data  was  not  distributed  normally,  it  was  impossible  to  

control  for  age.  The  found  relationship  between  social  class  and  lending  may  be  caused  by  

the  fact  that  older  people  have  more  material  resources  and  lend  more  things.  However,  the  

positive  relation  between  social  class  and  borrowing  can  not  be  explained  by  age,  because  

older  people  do  not  seem  to  borrow  more  than  younger  people.  Future  research  using  

parametric  datasets  (or  new  statistical  methods)  are  needed  to  assess  if  age  mediates  the  

relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.    

General  Discussion  

The  current  research  investigated  whether  having  resources  is  related  to  giving  

resources  to  somebody  else.  Our  two  studies  found  mixed  results.  Study  1  found  an  

extremely  weak  correlation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior,  signalling  that  there  

is  no  meaningful  relation  between  how  much  people  have  and  how  much  they  give.  Study  2  

found  a  moderate  positive  relationship  between  social  class  and  lending.  We  also  found  a  

marginally  significant  positive  relation  between  social  class  and  borrowing.    

  20  

The  two  studies  signalled  very  different  strengths  regarding  how  higher  social  class  is  

related  to  more  prosocial  behavior.  This  discrepancy  might  be  caused  by  the  large  number  of  

unengaged  members  of  Peerby  in  Study  1,  whereas  the  sample  who  responds  to  a  survey  

(Study  2)  consists  of  people  who  are  more  committed  to  the  platform.  The  Study  2  sample  

indeed  had  an  average  lending  count  of  9.37  times,  which  is  more  than  double  in  

comparison  to  the  average  of  4.20  times  in  Study  1.  An  alternative  explanation  could  be  that  

the  relation  between  social  class  and  lending  only  arises  among  very  active  members.  We  

tested  this  possibility  by  only  selecting  very  active  members  (lending  >  10)  in  Study  1.  This  

did  not  result  in  a  significant  correlation.  So  the  lack  of  result  in  Study  1  can’t  be  explained  by  

the  large  group  of  members  who  are  only  sporadically  active.  Most  probably,  the  data  in  

Study  1  was  simply  too  complex  to  be  correctly  analysed  with  the  current  statistical  

programs.  The  dataset  of  Study  1  contains  a  lot  of  participants  who  are  in  some  way  

different  than  the  participants  of  Study  2,  but  we  don’t  know  exactly  in  what  way.  This  is  the  

challenge  that  psychological  research  has  facing  the  opportunities  of  big  data.  

The  use  of  big  data  in  the  current  study  does  enable  this  research  to  be  the  first  on  

social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  to  use  behavioral  data  in  a  natural  setting.  This  has  the  

advantage  that,  whereas  previous  research  on  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  lacked  

external  validity  (Piff  et  al.,  2010;  Korndörfer  et  al.,  2015),  there  is  no  risk  of  experiment  

constructs,  socially  desired  responding  or  other  responding  errors  (Randall  &  Fernandes,  

1991;  Coughlin,  1990).  We  found  evidence  that  indeed,  what  people  report  on  how  much  

they  lend  and  borrow,  does  not  reflect  what  how  much  they  actually  lend  and  borrow.  So  

using  natural  behavior  data,  these  results  robustly  contradict  the  negative  relation  

hypothesis  postulated  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010).    

Balancing  Giving  and  Receiving  Help  

While  upper  class  people  may  be  more  likely  to  help  in  comparison  to  lower  class  

people,  they’re  also  more  likely  to  request  help.  This  high  level  of  activity  among  upper  class  

individuals  may  be  explained  by  the  possibility  that  due  to  their  elevated  personal  control  

(Johnson  &  Krueger,  2006;  Kraus  et  al.,  2009),  upper  class  individuals  adopt  innovations  

(such  as  Peerby;  see  Rogers,  2010)  sooner  than  lower  class  individuals,  and  are  therefore  

more  active  on  Peerby  both  in  borrowing  as  well  as  in  lending.  Additional  research  must  

  21  

assess  whether  personal  control  and  innovation  adoption  mediate  the  positive  relationship  

of  social  class  with  borrowing  and  lending.    

Another  way  to  experimentally  assess  how  giving  and  receiving  help  is  related,  is  to  

reverse  the  Dictator  Game.  A  Reversed  Dictator  Game  could  communicate  to  participants  

that  an  experiment  partner  has  received  10  points  and  is  free  to  decide  how  to  distribute  it.  

The  participant  would  get  the  possibility  to  ask  their  experiment  partner  for  a  portion  of  the  

points.  Requesting  help  would  then  be  measured  with  by  the  amount  of  points  asked  by  the  

participant.  

Economic  Inequality  as  Moderator  of  Upper  Class  Prosociality  

Apart  from  methodological  problems  with  the  evidence  provided  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010),  

there  is  a  new  theoretical  explanation  for  the  contrasting  evidence  in  the  literature  so  far.  

The  contrasting  evidence  between  the  current  study  and  those  published  by  Piff  et  al.  (2010)  

may  be  explained  by  the  amount  of  economic  inequality  in  the  country  where  the  research  

took  place.  While  the  data  analysis  of  the  current  research  was  in  progress,  Côté  et  al.  (2015)  

published  findings  reporting  that  economical  inequality  moderates  the  effect  of  income  on  

generosity  in  the  Dictator  Game.    

They  found  that  the  negative  effect  of  social  class  on  prosocial  behavior  only  emerges  

when  there  is  great  economic  inequality,  specifically,  when  the  Gini  coefficient  (representing  

economic  inequality)  is  .485  or  higher.  Contrastingly,  when  the  Gini  coefficient  is  .454  or  

lower,  they  found  that  social  class  is  associated  with  equal  or  more  prosocial  behavior.  This  

pattern  also  arises  when  participants  are  experimentally  induced  to  think  there  is  high  vs.  

low  economic  inequality  in  their  home  state.  This  explains  why  Piff  et  al.  (2010),  who  

conducted  their  studies  in  California  (one  of  the  most  unequal  US  states;  Wilkinson  &  

Pickett,  2009),  found  a  negative  relationship  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior,  

while  the  current  research  found  neutral  and  positive  relationships,  conducting  research  in  

the  Netherlands.  Here,  the  Gini  coefficient  is  .251,  signifying  high  economic  equality.  

Therefore,  in  the  current  study,  the  model  proposed  by  Côté  et  al.  (2015)  holds  up.  However,  

in  order  to  conclude  that  the  positive  relationship  between  social  class  and  prosocial  

behavior  was  indeed  moderated  by  economic  equality,  we  need  to  do  follow  up  research  on  

borrowing  and  lending  under  unequal  economic  circumstances.  Peerby  has  around  500  

  22  

members  in  the  United  States,  so  this  data  could  be  analysed  in  order  to  conclude  if  indeed  

there  is  a  negative  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior.  

The  findings  of  Côté  et  al.  (2015)  combined  with  those  from  this  research,  raise  

questions  on  how  different  social  classes  are  motivated  towards  prosocial  behaviour.  The  

reason  why  the  current  study  found  a  positive  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  

behaviour  may  be  because  in  contexts  of  economic  equality,  people  are  more  motivated  by  

potential  rewards  for  acting  prosocial,  and  less  by  compassion  (Batson  &  Shaw,  1991).  

Compassion  is  an  emotion  induced  as  a  reaction  to  the  suffering  of  others  (Goetz,  Keltner,  

Simon-­‐Thomas,  2010).  Where  there  is  more  economic  equality,  people  might  be  less  

triggered  to  feel  compassion  because  they’re  confronted  with  less  suffering  than  in  

situations  with  high  economic  inequality.  So  acting  prosocially  may  be  more  driven  by  

rewards  (such  as  being  able  to  ask  for  help  in  return)  than  by  compassion.  This  drive  for  

rewards  is  reflected  in  our  finding  that  upper  class  individuals  seem  to  request  more  help  

than  lower  class  individuals.  In  contrast,  in  a  context  with  high  economic  inequality,  Piff  et  al  

(2010)  found  that  compassion  mediates  the  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  

behavior  (Piff  et  al.,  2010).  This  may  explain  why  unequal  economic  situations  elicit  more  

prosocial  behavior  among  lower  class  people  and  relatively  less  among  upper  class  people.  

Upper  class  individuals  may  be  more  reward  focused,  while  lower  class  individuals  are  

motivated  to  help  another  by  compassion  (see  van  Kleef  et  al.,  2008;  Batson  &  Shaw,  1991;  

Clark,  Mills  &  Powel,  1998).  Future  research  should  assess  how  compassion  and  reward-­‐

seeking  impact  the  relation  between  social  class  and  prosocial  behavior  in  circumstances  

with  equal  –and  unequal  economic  situations.    

Challenges  in  using  Big  Data  in  Psychological  Research  

Concerning  big  data  research,  the  most  critical  methodological  challenge  research  is  

how  to  conduct  control  analysis  when  data  does  not  meet  parametric  assumptions.  We  

could  not  conduct  control  analyses  for  the  amount  of  requests  received  and  age,  raising  

methodological  issues.  For  example,  the  amount  of  requests  received  may  confound  the  

relation  between  social  class  and  lending.  Members  who  live  in  rural  areas  receive  fewer  

requests  in  their  neighborhood,  and  therefore  click  ‘help’  less  often,  than  members  in  urban  

areas.  We  cannot  exclude  the  possibility  that  Peerby  members  from  one  social  class  are  

  23  

more  likely  to  live  in  a  densely  populated  area  than  members  from  another  social  class,  and  

this  may  have  repercussions  for  the  validity  of  the  current  conclusions.  Second,  we  could  not  

control  for  age,  even  though  Study  2  shows  that  age  strongly  predicts  lending  on  Peerby.  It  is  

possible  that  the  relation  between  lending  and  social  class  is  (partially)  mediated  by  age.  

Older  people  have  built  up  more  resources,  and  may  be  generally  more  prosocial  than  

younger  people.  Therefore,  it  is  important  to  assess  whether  the  relation  between  social  

class  and  prosocial  behavior  exists  when  controlling  for  age  and  number  of  received  

requests.  

The  current  research  explored  new  possibilities  using  big  data  to  understand  human  

interactions.  It  is  clear  that  statistical  programs  need  to  adapt  to  this  new  possibility.  Still  

now,  the  potential  of  analysing  actual  human  behaviour  instead  of  lab  measures  proves  

valuable,  especially  in  fields  sensitive  to  socially  desired  responding  like  prosocial  behavior.  

In  this  article,  we  provide  evidence  that  social  class  is  not  (Study  1)  or  positively  (Study  2)  

related  to  prosocial  behavior,  contrasting  the  negative  relation  hypothesis  from  the  social  

cognitive  theory  on  social  class  (Piff  et  al.,  2010;  Kraus  et  al.,  2012).  It  seems  that  having  

more  does  lead  to  giving  more,  and  at  the  same  time,  taking  more.    

Literature  

Ajzen,  I.,  &  Fishbein,  M.  (1977).  Attitude-­‐behavior  relations:  A  theoretical  analysis  

and  review  of  empirical  research.  Psychological  bulletin,  84(5),  888.  

Argyle,  M.  (1994).  The  psychology  of  social  class.  Psychology  Press.  

Batson,  C.  D.,  &  Shaw,  L.  L.  (1991).  Evidence  for  altruism:  Toward  a  pluralism  of  

prosocial  motives.  Psychological  Inquiry,  2(2),  107-­‐122.  

Bowles,  H.  R.,  &  Gelfand,  M.  (2009).  Status  and  the  evaluation  of  workplace  

deviance.  Psychological  Science.  

Brief,  A.  P.,  &  Motowidlo,  S.  J.  (1986).  Prosocial  organizational  behaviors.  Academy  of  

management  Review,  11(4),  710-­‐725.  

Bowles,  H.  R.,  &  Gelfand,  M.  (2009).  Status  and  the  evaluation  of  workplace  deviance.  

Psychological  Science.  

  24  

Coughlin,  S.  S.  (1990).  Recall  bias  in  epidemiologic  studies.  Journal  of  clinical  

epidemiology,  43(1),  87-­‐91.  

 

Census  (2010).  The  Asian  Population.  Gathered  from:  

http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-­‐11.pdf.  

Census  (2010).  The  African  Population.  Gathered  from:  

https://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-­‐06.pdf  

Côté,  S.,  House,  J.,  &  Willer,  R.  (2015).  High  economic  inequality  leads  higher-­‐income  

individuals  to  be  less  generous.  Proceedings  of  the  National  Academy  of  Sciences,  

201511536.  

Clark,  M.  S.,  Mills,  J.,  &  Powell,  M.  C.  (1986).  Keeping  track  of  needs  in  communal  and  

exchange  relationships.  Journal  of  personality  and  social  psychology,  51(2),  333.  

Dovidio,  J.  F.,  Piliavin,  J.  A.,  Schroeder,  D.  A.,  &  Penner,  L.  (2006).  The  social  

psychology  of  prosocial  behavior.  Lawrence  Erlbaum  Associates  Publishers.  

Goetz,  J.  L.,  Keltner,  D.,  &  Simon-­‐Thomas,  E.  (2010).  Compassion:  an  evolutionary  

analysis  and  empirical  review.  Psychological  bulletin,  136(3),  351.  

Johnson,  W.,  &  Krueger,  R.  F.  (2006).  How  money  buys  happiness:  genetic  and  

environmental  processes  linking  finances  and  life  satisfaction.  Journal  of  personality  and  

social  psychology,  90(4),  680.  

Kanekar,  S.,  &  Vaz,  L.  (1993).  Effects  of  gender  and  status  upon  punctuality  norms.  

The  Journal  of  social  psychology,  133(3),  377-­‐384.  

Korndörfer,  M.,  Egloff,  B.,  &  Schmukle,  S.  C.  (2015).  A  Large  Scale  Test  of  the  Effect  of  

Social  Class  on  Prosocial  Behavior.  PloS  one,  10(7),  e0133193.  

Kraus,  M.  W.,  Côté,  S.,  &  Keltner,  D.  (2010).  Social  class,  contextualism,  and  empathic  

accuracy.  Psychological  Science.  

Kraus,  M.  W.,  &  Keltner,  D.  (2009).  Signs  of  socioeconomic  status  a  thin-­‐slicing  

approach.  Psychological  Science,  20(1),  99-­‐106.  

  25  

Kraus,  M.  W.,  Piff,  P.  K.,  &  Keltner,  D.  (2009).  Social  class,  sense  of  control,  and  social  

explanation.  Journal  of  personality  and  social  psychology,  97(6),  992.  

Kraus,  M.  W.,  Piff,  P.  K.,  Mendoza-­‐Denton,  R.,  Rheinschmidt,  M.  L.,  &  Keltner,  D.  

(2012).  Social  class,  solipsism,  and  contextualism:  how  the  rich  are  different  from  the  poor.  

Psychological  review,  119(3),  546.  

Lachman,  M.  E.,  &  Weaver,  S.  L.  (1998).  The  sense  of  control  as  a  moderator  of  social  

class  differences  in  health  and  well-­‐being.  Journal  of  personality  and  social  psychology,  74(3),  

763.  

Oakes,  J.  M.,  &  Rossi,  P.  H.  (2003).  The  measurement  of  SES  in  health  research:  

current  practice  and  steps  toward  a  new  approach.  Social  science  &  medicine,  56(4),  769-­‐784.  

Piff,  P.  K.,  Kraus,  M.  W.,  Côté,  S.,  Cheng,  B.  H.,  &  Keltner,  D.  (2010).  Having  less,  giving  

more:  the  influence  of  social  class  on  prosocial  behavior.  Journal  of  personality  and  social  

psychology,  99(5),  771.  

Randall,  D.  M.,  &  Fernandes,  M.  F.  (1991).  The  social  desirability  response  bias  in  

ethics  research.  Journal  of  Business  Ethics,  10(11),  805-­‐817.  

Rogers,  E.  M.  (2010).  Diffusion  of  innovations.  Simon  and  Schuster.  

Snibbe,  A.  C.,  &  Markus,  H.  R.  (2005).  You  can't  always  get  what  you  want:  

educational  attainment,  agency,  and  choice.  Journal  of  personality  and  social  

psychology,  88(4),  703.  

Stellar,  J.  E.,  Manzo,  V.  M.,  Kraus,  M.  W.,  &  Keltner,  D.  (2012).  Class  and  compassion:  

socioeconomic  factors  predict  responses  to  suffering.  Emotion,12(3),  449.  

STIPO,  2015.  Nieuwe  Rijkdommen.  gathered  from    

http://issuu.com/stipoteam/docs/2015_stipo___nieuwe_rijkdommen_in_d_225d1e3336593

5/1  

Trautmann,  S.  T.,  van  de  Kuilen,  G.,  &  Zeckhauser,  R.  J.  (2013).  Social  Class  and  (Un)  

Ethical  Behavior  A  Framework,  With  Evidence  From  a  Large  Population  Sample.  Perspectives  

on  Psychological  Science,  8(5),  487-­‐497.  

Wilkinson  R,  Pickett  K  (2009)  The  Spirit  Level:  Why  Greater  Equality  Makes  Societies  

Stronger  (Bloomsbury  Publishing,  New  York).