Diurnal rodents as an advantageous model for affective disorders: novel data from diurnal degu...

11
PSYCHIATRY AND PRECLINICAL PSYCHIATRIC STUDIES - ORIGINAL ARTICLE Diurnal rodents as an advantageous model for affective disorders: novel data from diurnal degu (Octodon degus) Tal Ashkenazy-Frolinger Haim Einat Noga Kronfeld-Schor Received: 21 October 2013 / Accepted: 6 December 2013 Ó Springer-Verlag Wien 2013 Abstract Circadian rhythms are strongly associated with affective disorders and recent studies have suggested uti- lization of diurnal rodents as model animal for circadian rhythms-related domains of these disorders. Previous work with the diurnal fat sand rat and Nile grass rat demonstrated that short photoperiod conditions result in behavioral changes including anxiety- and depression-like behavior. The present study examined the effect of manipulating day length on activity rhythms and behavior of the diurnal degu. Animals were housed for 3 weeks under either a short photoperiod (5-h:19-h LD) or a neutral photoperiod (12-h:12-h LD) and then evaluated by sweet solution test and the forced swim test for depression-like behavior, and in the light/dark box and open field for anxiety-like behavior. Results indicate that short photoperiod induced depression-like behavior in the forced swim test and the sweet solution preference test and anxiety-like behavior in the open field compared with animals maintained in a neutral photoperiod. No effects were shown in the light/ dark box. Short photoperiod-acclimated degu showed reduced total activity duration and activity was not restricted to the light phase. The present study further supports the utilization of diurnal rodents to model circadian rhythms-related affective change. Beyond the possible diversity in the mechanisms underlying diurnality in different animals, there are now evidences that in three different diurnal species, the fat sand rat, the grass Nile rat and the degu, shortening of photoperiod results in the appearance of anxiety- and depression-like behaviors. Keywords Animal model Á Circadian rhythm Á Anxiety Á Depression Á Diurnal Á Nocturnal Introduction Circadian rhythms abnormalities are clearly involved in the pathology of mood disorders such as major depressive disorder (MDD), bipolar disorder (BPD) and seasonal affective disorder (SAD) (Kronfeld-Schor and Einat 2012; McClung 2007; Nestler et al. 2002; Lenox et al. 2002; Lam and Levitan 2000). Affective disorder symptoms show diurnal variations (Gordijn et al. 1994), and treatments directed to affect the circadian system, such as bright light therapy, phase-advance therapy and sleep deprivation, contribute to the remission on these illnesses, sometimes even in drug-resistant patients (Dietzel et al. 1986; Kripke 1998; Yamada et al. 1995; Wirz-Justice and Van den Hoofdakker 1999; Giedke and Schwa ¨rzler 2002; Wehr et al. 1979; Wu et al. 2009; Moscovici and Kotler 2009; Benedetti et al. 2001). Manipulating light exposure affects several neurotransmitter systems related to mood including dopamine (Abı ´lio et al. 1999; Abı ´lio et al. 2003), serotonin (Zawilska et al. 1997) and noradrenaline (Zubidat et al. 2007). Antidepressants and mood stabilizers are known to affect pathways related to the circadian system including melatonin receptors, serotonin levels, sleep architecture and hormone secretion (Jawed et al. 2007; Sonntag et al. T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger Á N. Kronfeld-Schor (&) Department of Zoology, Tel-Aviv University, Ramat-Aviv, 69978 Tel Aviv, Israel e-mail: [email protected] H. Einat School of Behavioral Sciences, Tel Aviv-Yaffo Academic College, Tel Aviv, Israel H. Einat Department of Clinical Biochemistry and Pharmacology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel 123 J Neural Transm DOI 10.1007/s00702-013-1137-3

Transcript of Diurnal rodents as an advantageous model for affective disorders: novel data from diurnal degu...

PSYCHIATRY AND PRECLINICAL PSYCHIATRIC STUDIES - ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Diurnal rodents as an advantageous model for affective disorders:novel data from diurnal degu (Octodon degus)

Tal Ashkenazy-Frolinger • Haim Einat •

Noga Kronfeld-Schor

Received: 21 October 2013 / Accepted: 6 December 2013

� Springer-Verlag Wien 2013

Abstract Circadian rhythms are strongly associated with

affective disorders and recent studies have suggested uti-

lization of diurnal rodents as model animal for circadian

rhythms-related domains of these disorders. Previous work

with the diurnal fat sand rat and Nile grass rat demonstrated

that short photoperiod conditions result in behavioral

changes including anxiety- and depression-like behavior.

The present study examined the effect of manipulating day

length on activity rhythms and behavior of the diurnal

degu. Animals were housed for 3 weeks under either a

short photoperiod (5-h:19-h LD) or a neutral photoperiod

(12-h:12-h LD) and then evaluated by sweet solution test

and the forced swim test for depression-like behavior, and

in the light/dark box and open field for anxiety-like

behavior. Results indicate that short photoperiod induced

depression-like behavior in the forced swim test and the

sweet solution preference test and anxiety-like behavior in

the open field compared with animals maintained in a

neutral photoperiod. No effects were shown in the light/

dark box. Short photoperiod-acclimated degu showed

reduced total activity duration and activity was not

restricted to the light phase. The present study further

supports the utilization of diurnal rodents to model

circadian rhythms-related affective change. Beyond the

possible diversity in the mechanisms underlying diurnality

in different animals, there are now evidences that in three

different diurnal species, the fat sand rat, the grass Nile rat

and the degu, shortening of photoperiod results in the

appearance of anxiety- and depression-like behaviors.

Keywords Animal model � Circadian rhythm �Anxiety � Depression � Diurnal � Nocturnal

Introduction

Circadian rhythms abnormalities are clearly involved in the

pathology of mood disorders such as major depressive

disorder (MDD), bipolar disorder (BPD) and seasonal

affective disorder (SAD) (Kronfeld-Schor and Einat 2012;

McClung 2007; Nestler et al. 2002; Lenox et al. 2002; Lam

and Levitan 2000). Affective disorder symptoms show

diurnal variations (Gordijn et al. 1994), and treatments

directed to affect the circadian system, such as bright light

therapy, phase-advance therapy and sleep deprivation,

contribute to the remission on these illnesses, sometimes

even in drug-resistant patients (Dietzel et al. 1986; Kripke

1998; Yamada et al. 1995; Wirz-Justice and Van den

Hoofdakker 1999; Giedke and Schwarzler 2002; Wehr

et al. 1979; Wu et al. 2009; Moscovici and Kotler 2009;

Benedetti et al. 2001). Manipulating light exposure affects

several neurotransmitter systems related to mood including

dopamine (Abılio et al. 1999; Abılio et al. 2003), serotonin

(Zawilska et al. 1997) and noradrenaline (Zubidat et al.

2007). Antidepressants and mood stabilizers are known to

affect pathways related to the circadian system including

melatonin receptors, serotonin levels, sleep architecture

and hormone secretion (Jawed et al. 2007; Sonntag et al.

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger � N. Kronfeld-Schor (&)

Department of Zoology, Tel-Aviv University, Ramat-Aviv,

69978 Tel Aviv, Israel

e-mail: [email protected]

H. Einat

School of Behavioral Sciences, Tel Aviv-Yaffo Academic

College, Tel Aviv, Israel

H. Einat

Department of Clinical Biochemistry and Pharmacology,

Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beersheba, Israel

123

J Neural Transm

DOI 10.1007/s00702-013-1137-3

1996; Wirz-Justice et al. 1980; Tan et al. 2007). Addi-

tionally, human genetic association studies implicate clock

genes in mood disorders (Benedetti et al. 2003; Johansson

et al. 2003; Partonen et al. 2007) and animal studies

demonstrate affective-like behavioral changes related to

targeted mutations in clock genes (Bunney and Bunney

2000; Roybal et al. 2007; Le-Niculescu et al. 2008).

Recently, a disruption of the circadian pattern of gene

expression in several brain areas was described in major

depressive disorder (Li et al. 2013).

Circadian rhythms are driven by an internal master

circadian clock, which in mammals is located in the sup-

rachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus, and

subsidiary clocks in nearly every body cell (Dibner et al.

2010). The SCN is entrained to the environment, with light

acting as the strongest synchronizer. A disruption of the

circadian clock results in misalignment between the inter-

nal circadian clock and the activity pattern, which may

have adverse consequences such as metabolic syndrome,

obesity, insomnia, increased risk of cancer, as well as other

physiological and mental disorders (Albrecht 2010; Arble

et al. 2009; Karatsoreos et al. 2011; Scheer et al. 2009).

Animal models are critical in our attempts to understand

the pathophysiology of affective disorders and develop

better medications (Einat 2010; Tallman 1999; Tecott and

Nestler 2004). Significant advances in research were made

by selecting homologous model animal, including the use

of non-traditional model animals, to explore a variety of

biological systems in the context of health and disease

(Kara and Einat 2013; Smale et al. 2005; Bolker 2012).

Previous studies from our laboratories suggest that it is

possible that some of the difficulty in the identification and

development of good animal models for circadian rhythm

disturbances-related psychiatric diseases in humans is that

the standard animals used for modeling neuropsychiatric

research are nocturnal rodents and that there might be an

advantage in developing models utilizing diurnal animals

(Einat and Kronfeld-Schor 2009; Kronfeld-Schor and Einat

2012).

Despite the extraordinary advancement in our under-

standing of the circadian clock mechanism, it is still unclear

how the temporal signals from the clock are translated into

activity patterns, and how they differ in diurnal and nocturnal

mammals (Cohen et al. 2009; 2010a; Hagenauer and Lee

2008; Smale et al. 2008). Nevertheless, it is clear that some

fundamental differences exist between nocturnal and diurnal

mammals (Challet 2007; Cuesta et al. 2008; Smale et al. 2008),

which may be crucial for the study of circadian rhythms-

related diseases. For example, like humans, diurnal species are

active when melatonin levels are low, while nocturnal mam-

mals are active when melatonin levels are high (Nowak and

Zawilska 1998). Melatonin decreases anxiety-like behavior in

nocturnal species, while in diurnal species it decreases it, or

has no effect on anxiety (Bilu and Kronfeld-Schor 2013).

Another important component of the circadian system is the

masking effect of light. Specifically, light increases activity in

diurnal mammals (positive masking) and suppresses it in

nocturnal ones (negative masking), while darkness acts in

opposite ways (Cohen et al. 2010b; Hagenauer and Lee 2008;

Rotics et al. 2011; Barak and Kronfeld-Schor 2013; Shuboni

et al. 2012). Because of these significant differences, we pre-

viously suggested the utilization of diurnal rodents as model

animals to decipher the mechanisms underlying the relation-

ship between circadian rhythms and affective behavior

(Kronfeld-Schor and Einat 2012). In accordance with this

suggestion, several studies found that, indeed, a model based

on diurnal rodents demonstrates relatively strong validity for

the exploration of photoperiod-related affective changes.

Specifically, sand rats (Psammommys obesus) and Nile grass

rats (Arvicanthis Niloticus) exposed to 3 weeks or more of

short photoperiod (5-h:19-h LD) show anxiety- and depres-

sion-like behaviors (Einat et al. 2006; Ashkenazy et al. 2009b;

Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009; Leach et al. 2013a). More-

over, similar anxiety- and depression-like symptoms were

induced by administration of melatonin in a regimen that

mimics short photoperiod (Ashkenazy et al. 2009b), and

ameliorated after clinically relevant treatment with bright light

or antidepressants (Ashkenazy et al. 2009a; Krivisky et al.

2011, 2012). These results strongly support the possible use of

diurnal animals to model circadian rhythms-related affective

disorders in humans.

Because the ancestral state of mammals is nocturnality and

diurnality has evolved in several independent events (Roll

et al. 2006), it is possible that diurnality, and the mechanisms

underlying it, are diverse. Therefore, to fully understand di-

urnality and its relation to affective disorders, it is important to

study several unrelated species of diurnal rodents. To date, the

fat sand rat and the Nile grass rat were studied in this context

and the present study was designed to examine a third diurnal

rodent, the degu (Octodon degus), a highly social, diurnal, rat-

sized rodent from central Chile. Degus have been used in a

number of study areas including circadian rhythms research

(Lee 2004), separation distress, social attachment and play

(Colonnello et al. 2011a), but not in the context of depression.

The present study therefore evaluated the effects of short

photoperiod conditions on activity rhythms and affective-like

behavioral tests in the degu.

Materials and methods

Animals

Adult male degu (*4–6 mo old), weighing approximately

200 ± 20 g at the start of the experiment from our

breeding colony at the I. Meier Segals Garden for

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

123

Zoological Research at Tel Aviv University were used for

the experiments. Degus were divided into two groups

(n = 8 per group) and individually housed in standard

plastic cages (21 9 31 9 13 cm) in two different temper-

ature-controlled rooms under 12-h:12-h LD (lights on at

08:00 h, neutral photoperiod, NP, 800 Lux,). Under labo-

ratory conditions degus are diurnal when maintained at

17–22 �C. When housed at higher temperatures, many

animals are crepuscular and some are even nocturnal

(Kenagy et al. 2002b; Lee 2004). Therefore, we kept them

at 20 �C. Degus were provided with ad libitum rodent

chow (product 19560 by Harlen, Jerusalem, Israel) and

water, and were weighed 1 week after arrival (week 1) and

once a week thereafter for a period of 6 weeks. All pro-

cedures were conducted in accordance with and approved

by the Institutional Animal Ethics Committee (13-004-L).

All efforts were made to minimize the number of animals

used and their discomfort.

Experimental procedure

Photoperiod manipulation

After a week of acclimatization to the housing conditions,

the photoperiod of the test group was changed to 5-h:19-h

LD (lights on at 08:00 h, short photoperiod, SP, 800 Lux).

This lighting regime was set based on previous results with

fat sand rats (Einat et al. 2006; Ashkenazy et al. 2009b, b)

and Nile grass rats (Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009; Leach

et al. 2013b). Degus remained under these conditions for

3 week prior to onset of experiments, a time that is suffi-

cient for physiological acclimation (Kronfeld-Schor et al.

2000) and synchronization of circadian rhythms (Cohen

and Kronfeld-Schor 2006; Kronfeld-Schor et al. 2001) and

was previously shown to be adequate for the induction of

anxiety- and depression-like behaviors in the fat sand rat

(Einat et al. 2006; Ashkenazy et al. 2009b) and in the

Nilegrass rat (Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009). The initial

number of animals per group was eight, but two degus from

the SP group were not tested because they lost over 20 %

of body weight during acclimation time.

Behavioral testing

Behavioral experiments: general Animals were tested in

a battery of behavioral tests as detailed below. To minimize

the effects of behavioral experience on results, experiments

were conducted from the less to the more intrusive. The

order of experiments was: (1) home cage continuous

activity monitoring; (2) sweet solution preference; (3)

light/dark box; (4) large open field exploration; (5) forced

swim test. With small variations, this battery was previ-

ously used with the fat sand rats and the grass Nile rats

(Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009). Animals were tested

one test per day (except the sweet solution preference test

which is longer) as detailed below. All behavioral tests

were performed during the light hours of both groups and

in a group balanced manner. Experiments started at least

30 min after ‘‘lights on’’ and were terminated no later than

30 min before ‘‘lights off’’. For the SP group, experiments

were performed between 08:30 and 12:30 h, well within

the light/activity phase of the two groups and were there-

fore preformed in the light (800 lx).

Activity monitoring Activity of the two groups was con-

tinuously recorded using an infrared detector (Intrusion

detector model MH10, Crow group, Kiriat Reufa, Israel)

placed above their cages and connected to a PC. Data were

collected at 6-min intervals using software designed for

this purpose (ICPC, Ntanya, Israel) from the beginning of

the acclimation period until the end of all experiments.

Sweet solution preference Anhedonia is a core symptom

of depression (Sadock and Kaplan 2007) and can be

evaluated in animals using the sweet solution preference

test (Papp et al. 1991). Animals were supplied with a bottle

of 2 % sucrose solution (SIGMA, Rehovot, Israel) dis-

solved in tap water, on top of the regular supply of water

and food. The sucrose concentration was selected based on

previous experiments with degus (Colonnello et al. 2011b).

The weights of sucrose solution and water bottles were

taken at the beginning of the experiment and every 24 h

thereafter, approximately 4 h after ‘‘lights on’’. Sweet

solution preference was calculated daily as a ratio of

sucrose solution consumption out of total liquid (sucrose

solution ? water) consumption. This computation is nec-

essary to overcome weight variability between individual

animals or possible generalized effects on liquid con-

sumption (Flaisher-Grinberg et al. 2009). The schedule

included 5 days of continuous exposure to sucrose (‘‘day

1’’ representing preference for hours 0–24, ‘‘day 2’’ rep-

resenting preference for hours 24–48, ‘‘day 3’’ representing

preference for hours 48–72, ‘‘day 4’’ representing prefer-

ence for hours 72–96, ‘‘day 5’’ representing preference for

hours 96–118).

Light/dark box The light/dark box test is a frequently

used test for anxiety-like behavior (Crawley 2007). For the

current experiment, the light/dark box was constructed

from Plexiglas box (60 cm 9 25 cm with 30 cm walls)

and divided into two compartments: black, covered com-

partment (dark, one-third of the box) and white, uncovered

compartment (light, two thirds of the box) with a separat-

ing wall that is slightly ajar to allow free transitions

between the compartments. Each degu was placed in the

light part of the box and allowed to freely move for a 5-min

Short photoperiod in diurnal degu

123

session. Behavior was digitally recorded from an overhead

camera interfaced with a computer for later analysis and

manually scored from recordings for measures of time and

frequency of visits to the white part of the box. An animal

was considered to be in one of the compartments when its

head and the two front paws were in that area of the box. At

the end of the session, animals were returned to their home

cages and the box was wiped clean with a 10 % alcohol

solution.

Open field The open field is a widely used test in animal

psychobiology research (Walsh and Cummins 1976). In this

test, an animal (usually a rodent) is introduced into a plain

and illuminated arena (Genaro and Schmidek 2000) and its

behavior in the arena is regarded as a fundamental index of

general activity (Walsh and Cummins 1976) and anxiety

(Prut and Belzung 2003). Each degu underwent a 20-min

open field test in a 1 m 9 1 m arena with 40-cm high walls.

A video camera was mounted above the arena and inter-

faced with a video tracking computer program (Ethovision,

Noldus, The Netherlands). This program provides a number

of behavioral measures including measures of the amount

and the distribution of activity. At the end of each session,

animals were returned to their home cages and the area was

wiped clean with a 10 % alcohol solution.

Forced swim test The forced swim test is possibly the

most frequently used test for antidepressant effects of

manipulations and treatments (Martijena et al. 1996; Cryan

and Holmes 2005) and is a commonly used test for

depression-like behavior in both rats and mice, and was

also utilized in other rodents with some methodological

difference in application across species (Porsolt et al. 1978;

Wallace-Boone et al. 2008; Ashkenazy et al. 2009a, b;

Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009). For the current study, the

test was performed in a white, opaque plastic cylinder,

30 cm in diameter and 45 cm in height. The cylinder was

filled with water (21–23 �C) to a depth of 25 cm, pre-

venting the degus from touching the floor or escaping. A

digital video camera was mounted above the cylinder and

recorded files were later used to score behaviors. Animals

were tested in alternating order of the two groups, with

each animal individually placed in the water by the

experimenter and left there to swim. Similarly to what we

previously observed in the sand rats (Einat et al. 2006;

Ashkenazy et al. 2009a, b; Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

2009), degus are not as good swimmers as rats and mice

and they cannot float well. Therefore, when they reach the

state when they do not swim or struggle, they sink into the

water. Because this behavior is similar to what we previ-

ously reported in the sand rats, we followed the previously

established sand rat protocol (Einat et al. 2006; Ashkenazy

et al. 2009a, b; Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009). In this

protocol, the main measure is the first and second events of

‘time to sink’ where sinking is defined as a time when the

entire body of the animal (including snout) is immersed in

water. After two sinking events animals were taken out of

water, dried with a paper towel and replaced in their cages.

According to this protocol, in the rare event that an animal

sinks and stays under water for 5 s, it is immediately

removed from the water by the experimenter; however, no

such events happened in the present experiment.

Statistical analysis Continuous activity data were col-

lected at 6-min intervals and analyzed for phase preference,

activity profile (averages of activity levels in 6-min inter-

vals over a 24-h periods), activity onset and offset (esti-

mated using a template matching algorithm, searching for

6-h period of inactivity followed by a 6-h period of high

activity for activity onset, and 6-h period of high activity

followed by a 6-h period of inactivity for activity offset)

using the Clocklab program (Actimetrics Wilmette, IL)

that was also used to generate all actograms. Activity onset

and offset values were compared between the groups using

an unpaired t test, and within the same group at different

time points using a paired t test, using Statistica 7.0 soft-

ware (Statsoft, Tulsa, OK, USA). Duration of activity was

defined as the time between activity onset and offset.

Data from all other experiments were analyzed using an

analysis of variance (ANOVA) with Statistica 7.0 software

(Statsoft, Tulsa, OK, USA). Data for the weights, sweet

solution preference test and FST were analyzed with

repeated-measures ANOVA with photoperiod length as the

main factor and week (weight), day (sweet solution pref-

erence) or ‘‘sink’’ (FST) as a repeated-measure factor.

When the ANOVA resulted in a significant effect, it was

followed by a separate t test analysis for each repetition.

Specifically, for weight the data were analyzed separately

for each weighing, for the sweet solution preference test

the data were analyzed separately for each day and for the

FST the data were analyzed separately for each ‘‘Sink’’

event (Sink 1 and Sink 2).

The light/dark box and open field experiments were ana-

lyzed using t test analysis with photoperiod as the main

factor. For all experiments, the level of statistical significance

was set at p \ 0.05. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.

Results

Continuous measure of activity rhythms

All degus demonstrated an entrained and diurnal activity

pattern (Fig. 1). In the NP-acclimated group (Fig. 1a, c)

activity started with a peak just before ‘‘light-on’’ (08:00 h),

indicating that the animals were entrained to the NP

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

123

schedule, and offset was entrained to ‘‘lights off’’ (20:00 h).

After the SP group was transferred to SP by advancing

lights offset, activity onset did not change [t(62) = 0.6,

p = 0.53], and activity offset began advancing toward the

new ‘‘lights off’’ time (Fig. 1b, c). At the end of the accli-

mation period, activity of the SP group ended significantly

earlier compared with the NP group [t(62) = 6.6,

p \ 0.0001]. As a result, total activity duration of the NP

group was 13.7 h while the duration of activity of the SP

degus decreased to 12.1 h per day (t(62) = 5.19,

p = 0.0001) and was not restricted to the light phase.

Weights

Animals maintained under NP conditions gained on aver-

age 8.7 g during the 6-week period of acclimatization and

experiment, whereas animals maintained under SP condi-

tions lost on average 8.5 g, but the difference between the

groups did not reach the level of statistical significance

(Fig. 2; t(12) = 1.77, p = 0.1).

Sweet solution preference

Degu-acclimated to NP gradually increased their sucrose

preference, while in the animals that were maintained

under SP conditions no such change was observed. The

difference between the groups became significant on days 4

and 5 (Fig. 3). Repeated-measures ANOVA across days,

Photoperiod effect: F(1, 12) = 1.68, p = 0.22. Day effect:

F(4, 48) = 1.12, p = 0.36. Photoperiod X Day interaction:

F(4, 48) = 2.62, p = 0.0466. Following comparisons for

separate days, photoperiod effect: day 1: t(12) = 0.69,

Fig. 1 Double daily plots of activity during the experiment. a Rep-

resentative actogram of an individual acclimated to 12-h:12-h NP.

b Representative actogram of an individual acclimated to 12-h:12-h

NP and transferred to 5:9 SP conditions on day 0. c Percentage of

daily activity during the last 5 days of the acclimation period to

12-h:12-h NP/5-h:19-h SP. (n = 8, mean ± SE). The NP group degus

were active during light hours with total activity duration of 13.7 h

with activity peaks occurring at the beginning and end of the dark

phase, while SP degu activity duration decreased to 12.1 h per day.

[t(62) = 5.19, p = 0.0001] and was not restricted to the light phase.

Activity onset of the SP animals did not change (c; t(62) = 0.6,

p = 0.53), but it ended earlier (c; t(62) = 6.6, p \ 0.0001). Black and

white bars at the top of the plot represent the light schedules in the

two treatments

Short photoperiod in diurnal degu

123

p = 0.5; day 2: t(12) = 0.59, p = 0.6; day 3: t(12) = 1.69,

p = 0.1; day 4: t(12) = 2.21, p = 0.046; day 5:

t(12) = 2.29, p = 0.04.

Light/dark box

Day length schedule had no effect on the time spent in the

light area of the box or on the number of entries to this area

(Table 1).

Open field

Photoperiod length did not affect distance travelled in the

open field (Table 1). However, animals from the SP group

spent 45 % less time than animals from the NP group in the

central area of the arena, suggesting an anxiety-like

behavior (Table 2; t(12) = 2.35, p = 0.037). Increased

anxiety-like behavior of the SP group in the open field is

also supported by additional measures: increased latency to

first entry into the central zone and increased time spent in

the corners of the arena (see Table 2 for data and statistics).

Forced swim test

A number of animals were excluded from the analysis in

the FST. Specifically, two animals from the NP group were

excluded because they escaped immediately before the test

and were captured only after some minutes. One animal

from the SP group was excluded because it demonstrated a

generalized sickness behavior in its cage after the first day

in the FST and therefore was not tested on the second day.

Hence, the final number of animals analyzed in the FST

was six for the NP group and five for the SP group. As

expected based on previous work with the fat sand rat, the

manipulation of photoperiod length had no effects on the

behavior on the first day of testing (Table 1); however,

despite the small number of animals, a near significant

difference between groups appeared on the second day of

1 2 3 4 50.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

NPSP

**

Day

Pre

fere

nce

rat

io

Fig. 3 Sucrose preference (mean ± SE) of neutral (NP, n = 8) and

short (SP, n = 6) photoperiod groups across the 5 days of testing.

Asterisk indicates a significant difference between groups (p \ 0.05).

Repeated-measures ANOVA across days, Photoperiod effect: F(1,

12) = 1.68, p = 0.22. Day effect: F(4, 48) = 1.12, p = 0.36. Pho-

toperiod X Day interaction: F(4, 48) = 2.62, p = 0.0466. Following

comparisons for separate days, photoperiod effect: day 1:

t(12) = 0.69, p = 0.5; day 2: t(12) = 0.59, p = 0.6; day 3:

t(12) = 1.69, p = 0.1; day 4: t(12) = 2.21, p = 0.046; day 5:

t(12) = 2.29, p = 0.04

Table 1 Some behavioral measures (mean ± SEM) not clearly

influenced by photoperiod length

Measure NP animals

(N = 8)

SP animals

(N = 6)

Statistics

Distance in a

large open

field (cm)

1,9103 ± 2,102 14,743 ± 2,067 t(12) = 1.44,

p = 0.17,

NS

Light/dark box,

time in light

part (s)

174 ± 14 150 ± 20 t(12) = 1.0,

p = 0.32,

NS

Light/dark box,

entries to light

part (#)

11.8 ± 2.0 12.3 ± 0.8 t(12) = 0.24,

p = 0.81,

NS

FST Day 1 (s to

sink)

Sink 1:

102.1 ± 23.1

Sink 1:

80.6 ± 21.6

t(9) = 0.67,

p = 0.52,

NS

Sink 2:

124.2 ± 27.3

Sink 2:

120.6 ± 13.1

t(9) = 0.11,

p = 0.91,

NS

Table 2 Effects of photoperiod manipulations on anxiety-related

measures (mean ± SEM) in the large open field

Measure NP animals

(N = 8)

SP animal

(N = 6)

Statistics

Time in center area

(s)

137.3 ± 21.6 75.0 ± 9.6 t(12) = 2.35,

p = 0.037

Latency to enter the

center area (s)

26.4 ± 3.6 80.6 ± 18.2 t(12) = 3.36,

p = 0.006

Time in corners (s) 515 ± 33 681 ± 38 t(11) = 3.3,

p = 0.007*

* One animal from the NP group was excluded from the analysis as

an outlier, being more than 2XSTD away from the mean

NP SP-20

-10

0

10

20

Wei

gh

t ch

ang

e (g

)

Fig. 2 Change in body weight (mean ± SE) in degus maintained

under neutral (NP, n = 8) or short (SP, n = 6) photoperiod condi-

tions during the 6-week experiment (weight at week 6-week at the

start of the behavioral tests). The different trends of the two groups

did not reach statistical significance t(12) = 1.77, p = 0.1

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

123

testing with the SP group sinking approximately 30 %

faster than the NP animals (Fig. 4; time to second sink:

t(9) = 2.21, p = 0.054). Whereas the statistical analysis

shows only a near significant effect (p = 0.054), it is

suggested that with the small number of animals and the

expectation for faster sinking based on the previous results

in sand rats, the effects can be considered to reflect a real

difference.

Discussion

The results of the present study show that short photope-

riod conditions result in behavioral changes in the diurnal

degus that are generally similar to previous findings with

the diurnal fat sand rat and Nile grass rat and include

components of anxiety- and depression-like behavior.

Under both photoperiod schedules, at 20 �C and with no

running wheels available, all degus were diurnal and

entrained. These results are in accordance with their diur-

nal activity in their natural habitat as well as with findings

from other laboratories where degus are kept under similar

conditions (Kenagy et al. 2002b; Lee 2004; Kas and Edgar

1999; Goel and Lee 1995; Woods and Boraker 1975; Re-

finetti 1996). The degus were also previously reported to

show reduced activity during midday when ambient tem-

peratures are high or when kept under long photoperiods

(Kas and Edgar 1998; Kenagy et al. 2002a; Bacigalupe and

Rezende 2003; Kenagy and Vasquez 2004), which was also

evident in our results.

When transferred to SP conditions by advancing lights

offset, the degus significantly advanced their activity off-

set, but only by 1.6 h, resulting in activity continuing well

into the dark phase. These results are similar to ones

reported in the Nile grass rats, which did not respond

effectively to short photoperiod with respect to either the

duration of their active phase (Leach et al. 2013a; Refinetti

2004) or the elevated phase of PER1 and PER2 rhythms

within the SCN (Leach et al. 2013b).

We found that similar to previous work in diurnal

rodents, which demonstrated the development of anxiety-

and depression-like behavioral changes when maintained

under short photoperiod conditions (Einat et al. 2006;

Ashkenazy et al. 2009a, b; Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

2009; Leach et al. 2013a), degus maintained at 5-h:19-h

LD demonstrated anxiety-like behavior in the open field

test (Table 2; changes in measures related to the center of

the area), depression-like behavior in the sweet solution

preference (Fig. 3) and in the FST (Fig. 4), compared with

animals maintained under 12-h:12-h LD. It is important to

note that these behavioral changes are not the conse-

quences of a generalized reduction in activity, because no

effects of the photoperiod manipulation were shown in

locomotion levels in the large open field test (Table 1).

Despite the higher anxiety levels demonstrated by the

SP-acclimated animals in the open field test, such differ-

ences were not found in another test for anxiety-like

behavior, the light/dark box (Table 1). The lack of effect of

altered photoperiods in the light/dark box may indicate that

this test is unsuitable for diurnal animals. The main anxiety

provoking stimulus in this test is light. In nocturnal rodents,

light has a negative effect on activity levels and increases

anxiety and glucocorticoids levels (Barak and Kronfeld-

Schor 2013; Bilu and Kronfeld-Schor 2013; Cohen et al.

2010b; Eilam et al. 1999; Gutman et al. 2011; Hendrie

et al. 1996, 1998; Mandelik and Dayan 2000; Rotics et al.

2011). However, these effects are absent and sometime

opposite in diurnal rodents (Barak and Kronfeld-Schor

2013; Bilu and Kronfeld-Schor 2013; Cohen et al. 2010a;

Rotics et al. 2011). These findings are not different from

previous findings showing that diurnal Nile grass rats did

not show preference to the dark compartment of the light/

dark box (Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al. 2009). It is possible

that the elevated plus-maze, which is assumed to induce

anxiety because of the increased exposure of the animal in

the open compared to the closed arms, is a better test for

anxiety-like behavior in diurnal rodents. However, we were

unable to employ the elevated plus-maze test in the degu,

as in preliminary trials we found that the degu jumps off

the elevated maze easily and does so shortly after being

placed on it. Because degus live in rocky outcroppings and

steep bushy hills, it is not surprising that they are used to

jumping from heights.

In contrast with the clear effects of SP in diurnal rodents,

the effects of photoperiod manipulations on depression- and

anxiety-like responses in nocturnal animals is more com-

plicated and appears to depend on a number of factors. A

few studies report similar results to the ones presented in

Sink 1 Sink 20

50

100

150 NPSP

Sink

Tim

e to

sin

k (s

ec)

Fig. 4 The time to reach first and second sink in the FST of degus

acclimated to natural photoperiod. (NP, light gray bars n = 6) or

short photoperiod. (SP, dark gray bars, n = 5). Repeated-measures

AVOVA across tests: photoperiod effect: F(1, 9) = 3.15, p = 0.11,

test effect : F(1, 18) = 14.9, p = 0.004, test X photoperiod interac-

tion: F(1, 18) = 0.3, p = 0.59. Separate analysis for sink 1 and sink

2: sink 1: t(9) = 1.33, p = 0.2; sink 2 t(9) = 2.21, p = 0.054

Short photoperiod in diurnal degu

123

diurnal rodents: in Siberian hamsters, short photoperiod

(SP: 8L/16D) acclimation resulted in depressive-like

behavior in the sucrose preference test and the forced swim

(Pyter and Nelson 2006; Prendergast and Nelson 2005). The

same effects of SP were shown in Wistar rats acclimated to

an even milder SP (10L/14D) (Prendergast and Kay 2008),

but others found no such effect in more extreme SP (5-h:19-

h LD) (Molina hernandez and Tellez-Alcantara 2000), and

short photoperiod in rats was recently demonstrated to

increases sucrose consumption (Sinitskaya et al. 2008).

Similarly, in a study comparing the effects of long, short or

neutral photoperiods in two strains of mice (C57Bl/6 and

CD-1 mice), none of the manipulations resulted in a clear

depression- or anxiety-like behavioral phenotype (Flaisher-

Grinberg et al. 2011). The effects of different photoperiod

conditions in nocturnal animals may suggest that the

behavioral changes observed are not directly related to

specific photoperiod manipulations, but to a more general

effect possibly related to stress or to different evolutionary

background (Flaisher-Grinberg et al. 2011).

The changes that occur in the degu when maintained

under short photoperiod may be relevant to the underlying

mechanisms of depression. Whatever is responsible for the

inability of the degu to effectively adapt to the short pho-

toperiod may be part of the mechanism underlying the

development of depressive-like behavior under these con-

ditions. Altered circadian motor activity is an integral part

of the clinical symptoms of a depressive state (Todder et al.

2006). Changes in activity patterns were found in depres-

sed patients including reduced wake time activity levels

(Raoux et al. 1994), higher motor activity during sleep

(Volkers et al. 2003) and a more complex and less struc-

tured activity patterns (Berle et al. 2010; Hauge et al.

2011). Moreover, in SAD patients, the daily rhythms of

locomotor activity were found to be unstable, peaking at

different times across days (Teicher et al. 1997); they had

abnormal daily rhythms of temperature and melatonin

levels (Dahl et al. 1993; Avery et al. 1997).

Further support for the common mechanisms underlying

the response of diurnal rodents to short photoperiod and

depression is that similar to the effects in humans (Wehr

et al. 1986; Golden et al. 2005), bright light treatment

improved depression- and anxiety-like behaviors induced

by a short photoperiod regimen, while having no effects on

animals maintained in neutral photoperiods. Moreover,

similar to the effects in humans, application of morning

bright light was more effective than application of evening

bright light (Lewy et al. 1998; Ashkenazy et al. 2009a;

Krivisky et al. 2012). These results support the benefit of

bright light in the treatment of seasonal affective disorder

and offer new tool to study the underlying biological

mechanisms of this effect.

Taken together, the present study further supports the

utilization of diurnal rodents to model circadian rhythms-

related affective change. Beyond the possible diversity in

the mechanisms underlying diurnality in different animals,

there is now evidence that in three different diurnal species,

the fat sand rat, the grass Nile rat and the degu, shortening

of photoperiod results in the appearance of anxiety- and

depression-like behaviors. It is now possible to explore

common mechanisms of these changes and possibly gain

additional knowledge on the complex interactions between

circadian rhythms and affect.

References

Abılio VC, Freitas FM, Dolnikoff MS, Castrucci AM, Frussa-Filho R

(1999) Effects of continuous exposure to light on behavioral

dopaminergic supersensitivity. Biol Psychiatry 45:1622–1629

Abılio VC, Vera JAR, Ferreira LSM, Duarte CRM, Martins CR,

Torres-Leite D, Ribeiro Rde A, Frussa-Filho R (2003) Effects of

melatonin on behavioral dopaminergic supersensitivity. Life Sci

72:3003–3015

Albrecht U (2010) Circadian clocks in mood-related behaviors. Ann

Med 42:241–251

Arble DM, Bass J, Laposky AD, Vitaterna MH, Turek FW (2009)

Circadian timing of food intake contributes to weight gain.

Obesity. (Silver Spring) 17:2100–2102

Ashkenazy T, Einat H, Kronfeld-Schor N (2009a) Effects of bright

light treatment on depression- and anxiety-like behaviors of

diurnal rodents maintained on a short daylight schedule. Behav

Brain Res 201:343–346

Ashkenazy T, Einat H, Kronfeld-Schor N (2009b) We are in the dark

here: induction of depression- and anxiety-like behaviours in the

diurnal fat sand rat, by short daylight or melatonin injections. Int

J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:83–93

Ashkenazy-Frolinger T, Kronfeld-Schor N, Juetten J, Einat H (2009)

It is darkness and not light: depression-like behaviors of diurnal

unstriped Nile grass rats maintained under a short photoperiod

schedule. J Neurosci Methods 186:165–170

Avery DH, Dahl K, Savage MV, Brengelmann GL, Larsen LH,

Kenny MA, Eder DN, Vitiello MV, Prinz PN (1997) Circadian

temperature and cortisol rhythms during a constant routine are

phase-delayed in hypersomnic winter depression. Biol Psychi-

atry 41:1109–1123

Bacigalupe LD, Rezende EL (2003) Activity and space use by degus:

a trade-off between thermal conditions and food availability?

J Mammal 84:311–318

Barak O, Kronfeld-Schor N (2013) Activity rhythms and Masking

response in the diurnal Fat Sand Rat under laboratory conditions.

Chronobiol Int 30(9):1123–1134

Benedetti F, Barbini B, Campori E, Fulgosi MC, Pontiggia A,

Colombo C (2001) Sleep phase advance and lithium to sustain

the antidepressant effect of total sleep deprivation in bipolar

depression: new findings supporting the internal coincidence

model? J Psychiatr Res 35:323–329

Benedetti F, Serretti A, Colombo C, Barbini B, Lorenzi C, Campori

E, Smeraldi E (2003) Influence of CLOCK gene polymorphism

on circadian mood fluctuation and illness recurrence in bipolar

depression. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet

123B:23–26

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

123

Berle JO, Hauge ER, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB (2010)

Actigraphic registration of motor activity reveals a more

structured behavioural pattern in schizophrenia than in major

depression. BMC Res Notes 3:149

Bilu C, Kronfeld-Schor N (2013) Effects of circadian phase and

melatonin injection on anxiety-like behavior in nocturnal and

diurnal rodents. Chronobiol Int 30:828–836

Bolker J (2012) Model organisms: there’s more to life than rats and

flies. Nature 491:31–33

Bunney WE, Bunney BG (2000) Molecular clock genes in man and

lower animals: possible implications for circadian abnormalities

in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 22:335–345

Challet E (2007) Entrainment of the suprachiasmatic clockwork in

diurnal and nocturnal mammals. Endocrinology 148:5648–5655

Cohen R, Kronfeld-Schor N (2006) Individual variability and photic

entrainment of circadian rhythms in golden spiny mice. Physiol

Behav 87:563–574

Cohen R, Smale L, Kronfeld-Schor N (2009) Plasticity of circadian

activity and body temperature rhythms in golden spiny mice.

Chronobiol Int 26:430–446

Cohen R, Kronfeld-Schor N, Ramanathan C, Baumgras A, Smale L

(2010a) The substructure of the suprachiasmatic nucleus:

similarities between nocturnal and diurnal spiny mice. Brain

Behav Evol 75:9–22

Cohen R, Smale L, Kronfeld-Schor N (2010b) Masking and temporal

niche switches in spiny mice. J Biol Rhythms 25:47–52

Colonnello V, Iacobucci P, Anderson MP, Panksepp J (2011a) Brief

periods of positive peer interactions mitigate the effects of total

social isolation in young Octodon degus. Dev Psychobiol

53:280–290

Colonnello V, Iacobucci P, Fuchs T, Newberry R, Panksepp J (2011b)

Octodon degus. A useful animal model for social-affective

neuroscience research: basic description of separation distress,

social attachment and play. Neurosci Behav Rev 35:1854–1863

Crawley JN (1981) Neuropharmacologic specificity of a simple

animal model for the behavioral actions of benzodiazepines.

Pharmacol Biochem Behav 15:695–699

Crawley JN (2007) Behavioral neuroscience. In: Crawley JN, McKay

J (eds) Current protocols in neuroscience. Wiley Interscience,

New York

Cryan JF, Holmes A (2005) The ascent of the mouse: advances in

modeling human depression and anxiety. Nat Rev Drug Discov

4:77–790

Cuesta M, Mendoza J, Clesse D, Pevet P, Challet E (2008)

Serotonergic activation potentiates light resetting of the main

circadian clock and alters clock gene expression in a diurnal

rodent. Exp Neurol 210:501–513

Dahl K, Avery DH, Lewy AJ, Savage MV, Brengelmann GL, Larsen

LH, Vitiello MV, Prinz PN (1993) Dim light melatonin onset and

circadian temperature during a constant routine in hypersomnic

winter depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 88:60–66

Dibner C, Schibler U, Albrecht U (2010) The mammalian circadian

timing system: organization and coordination of central and

peripheral clocks. Annu Rev Physiol 72:517–549

Dietzel M, Saletu B, Lesch OM, Sieghart W, Schjerve M (1986) Light

treatment in depressive illness. Polysomnographic, psychometric

and neuroendocrinological findings. Eur Neurol 25(Suppl

2):93–103

Eilam D, Dayan T, Ben-Elyahu S, Schulman II, Shefer G, Hendrie

CA (1999) Differential behavioural and hormonal responses of

voles and spiny mice to owl calls. Anim Behav 58:1085–1093

Einat H (2010) Strategies for the development of animal models for

bipolar disorder: new opportunities and new challenges. In:

Zarate CA, Manji HK (eds) Behavioral neurobiology of bipolar

disorder and its treatment. Current topics in behavioral neuro-

science, vol 5. Springer, Berlin, pp 69–87

Einat H, Kronfeld-Schor N (2009) Utilizing diurnal model animals in

the study of depression. Front Neurosci 3:242–243

Einat H, Kronfeld-Schor N, Eilam D (2006) Sand rats see the light:

short photoperiod induces a depression-like response in a diurnal

rodent. Behav Brain Res 173:153–157

Flaisher-Grinberg S, Overgaard S, Einat H (2009) Attenuation of high

sweet solution preference by mood stabilizers: a possible mouse

model for the increased reward-seeking domain of mania.

J Neurosci Methods 177:44–50

Flaisher-Grinberg S, Gampetro DR, Kronfeld-Schor N, Einat H

(2011) Inconsistent effects of photoperiod manipulations in tests

for affective-like changes in mice: implications for the selection

of appropriate model animals. Behav Pharmacol 22:23–30

Genaro G, Schmidek WR (2000) Exploratory activity of rats in three

different environments. Ethology 106:849–859

Giedke H, Schwarzler F (2002) Therapeutic use of sleep deprivation

in depression. Sleep Med Rev 6:361–377

Goel N, Lee TM (1995) Sex differences and effects of social cues on

daily rhythms following phase advances in Octodon degus.

Physiol Behav 58:205–213

Golden RN, Gaynes BN, Ekstrom RD, Hamer RM, Jacobsen FM et al

(2005) The efficacy of light therapy in the treatment of mood

disorders: a review and meta-analysis of the evidence. Am J

Psychiatry 162(4):656–662

Gordijn MC, Beersma DG, Bouhuys AL, Reinink E, Van den

Hoofdakker RH (1994) A longitudinal study of diurnal mood

variation in depression; characteristics and significance. J Affect

Disord 31:261–273

Gutman R, Dayan T, Levy O, Schubert I, Kronfeld-Schor N (2011)

The Effect of the lunar cycle on fecal cortisol metabolite levels

and foraging ecology of nocturnally and diurnally active Spiny

mice. PLoS One 6:e23446

Hagenauer MH, Lee TM (2008) Circadian organization of the

diurnal Caviomorph rodent, Octodon degus. Biol Rhythm Res

39:269–289

Hauge ER, Berle JØ, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB (2011)

Nonlinear analysis of motor activity shows differences between

schizophrenia and depression: a study using Fourier analysis and

sample entropy. PLoS One 6:e16291

Hendrie CA, Weiss SM, Eilam D (1996) Exploration and predation

models of anxiety: evidence from laboratory and wild species.

Pharmacol Biochem Behav 54:13–20

Hendrie CA, Weiss SM, Eilam D (1998) Behavioural response of

wild rodents to the calls of an owl: a comparative study. J Zool

245:439–446

Jawed S, Kim B, Ottenhof T, Brown GM, Werstiuk ES, Niles LP

(2007) Human melatonin MT1 receptor induction by valproic

acid and its effects in combination with melatonin on MCF-7

breast cancer cell proliferation. Eur J Pharmacol 560:17–22

Johansson C, Willeit M, Smedh C, Ekholm J, Paunio T, Kieseppa T,

Lichtermann D, Praschak-Rieder N, Neumeister A, Nilsson L-G,

Kasper S, Peltonen L, Adolfsson R, Schalling M, Partonen T

(2003) Circadian clock-related polymorphisms in seasonal

affective disorder and their relevance to diurnal preference.

Neuropsychopharmacology 28:734–739

Kara NZ, Einat H (2013) Rodent models for mania: practical

considerations. Cell, tissue research. (Special issue, rodent models

of psychiatric disorders: practical considerations), advance online

publication. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23504091

Karatsoreos IN, Bhagat S, Bloss EB, Morrison JH, McEwen BS

(2011) Disruption of circadian clocks has ramifications for

metabolism, brain, and behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

108:1657–1662

Kas MJ, Edgar DM (1998) Crepuscular rhythms of EEG sleep–wake

in a hystricomorph rodent, Octodon degus. J Biol Rhythms

13:9–17

Short photoperiod in diurnal degu

123

Kas MJ, Edgar DM (1999) A nonphotic stimulus inverts the diurnal–

nocturnal phase preference in Octodon degus. J Neurosci

19:328–333

Kenagy GJ, Vasquez RA (2004) Microstructure of summer activity

bouts of degus in a thermally heterogeneous habitat. J Mammal

85:260–267

Kenagy GJ, Nespolo RF, Vasquez RA, Bozinovic F (2002a) Daily

and seasonal limits of time and temperature to activity of degus.

Revista Chilena Hist Nat 75:567–581

Kenagy GJ, Vasquez RA, Nespolo RF, Bozinovic F (2002b) A time–

energy analysis of daytime surface activity in degus, Octodon

degus. Revista Chilena Hist Nat 75:149–156

Kripke DF (1998) Light treatment for nonseasonal depression: speed,

efficacy, and combined treatment. J Affect Disord 49:109–117

Krivisky K, Ashkenazy T, Kronfeld-Schor N, Einat H (2011)

Antidepressants reverse short-photoperiod-induced, forced swim

test depression-like behavior in the diurnal fat sand rat: further

support for the utilization of diurnal rodents for modeling

affective disorders. Neuropsychobiology 63:191–196

Krivisky K, Einat H, Kronfeld-Schor N (2012) Effects of morning

compared with evening bright light administration to ameliorate

short-photoperiod induced depression- and anxiety-like behav-

iors in a diurnal rodent model. J Neural Transm 119:1241–

1248

Kronfeld-Schor N, Einat H (2012) Circadian rhythms and depression:

human psychopathology and animal models. Neuropharmacol-

ogy 62:101–114

Kronfeld-Schor N, Haim A, Dayan T, Zisapel N, Klingenspor M,

Heldmaier G (2000) Seasonal thermogenic acclimation of

diurnally and nocturnally active desert spiny mice. Physiol

Biochem Zool 73:37–44

Kronfeld-Schor N, Dayan T, Elvert R, Haim A, Zisapel N, Heldmaier

G (2001) On the use of the time axis for ecological separation:

diel rhythms as an evolutionary constraint. Am Nat 158:451–457

Lam RW, Levitan RD (2000) Pathophysiology of seasonal affective

disorder: a review. J Psychiatry Neurosci 25:469–480

Leach G, Adidharma W, Yan L (2013a) Depression-like responses

induced by daytime light deficiency in the diurnal grass rat

(Arvicanthis niloticus). PLoS One 8:e57115

Leach G, Ramanathan C, Langel J, Yan L (2013b) Responses of brain

and behavior to changing day-length in the diurnal grass rat

(Arvicanthis niloticus). Neuroscience 234:31–39

Lee TM (2004) Octodon degus: a diurnal, social, and long-lived

rodent. ILAR J 45:14–24

Le-Niculescu H, McFarland MJ, Ogden CA, Balaraman Y, Patel S,

Tan J, Rodd ZA, Paulus M, Geyer MA, Edenberg HJ, Glatt SJ,

Faraone SV, Nurnberger JI, Kuczenski R, Tsuang MT, Niculescu

AB (2008) Phenomic, convergent functional genomic, and

biomarker studies in a stress-reactive genetic animal model of

bipolar disorder and co-morbid alcoholism. Am J Med Genet B

Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B:134–166

Lenox RH, Gould TD, Manji HK (2002) Endophenotypes in bipolar

disorder. Am J Med Genet 114:391–406

Lewy AJ, Bauer VK, Cutler NL, Sack RL, Ahmed S et al (1998)

Morning vs evening light treatment of patients with winter

depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 55(10):890–896

Li JZ, Bunney BG, Meng F, Hagenauer MH, Walsh DM, Vawter MP,

Evans SJ, Choudary PV, Cartagena P, Barchas JD, Schatzberg

AF, Jones EG, Myers RM, Watson SJ Jr, Akil H, Bunney WE

(2013) Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brain

and disruption in major depressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci

USA 110:9950–9955

Mandelik YT, Dayan (2000) Foraging activity of Acomys cahirinus

under different illumination levels: comparing giving-up densi-

ties to direct behavioral observations. Isr J Zool 46:167–168

Martijena ID, Tapia M, Molina VA (1996) Altered behavioral and

neurochemical response to stress in benzodiazepine-withdrawn

rats. Brain Res 712:239–244

McClung CA (2007) Circadian genes, rhythms and the biology of

mood disorders. Pharmacol Ther 114:222–232

Molina hernandez M, Tellez-Alcantara P (2000) Long photoperiod

regimen may produce antidepressant actions in the male rat. Prog

Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 24:105–116

Moscovici L, Kotler M (2009) A multistage chronobiologic inter-

vention for the treatment of depression: a pilot study. J Affect

Disord 116:201–207

Nestler EJ, Barrot M, DiLeone RJ, Eisch AJ, Gold SJ, Monteggia LM

(2002) Neurobiology of depression. Neuron 34:13–25

Nowak JZ, Zawilska JB (1998) Melatonin and its physiological and

therapeutic properties. Pharm World Sci 20:18–27

Papp M, Willner P, Muscat R (1991) An animal model of anhedonia:

attenuation of sucrose consumption and place preference condi-

tioning by chronic unpredictable mild stress. Psychopharmacol-

ogy (Berl) 104:255–259

Partonen T et al (2007) Three circadian clock genes Per2, Arntl, and

Npas2 contribute to winter depression. Ann Med 39:229–238

Porsolt RD, Bertin A, Jalfre M (1978) ‘‘Behavioural despair’’ in rats

and mice: strain differences and the effects of imipramine. Eur J

Pharmacol 51:291–294

Prendergast BJ, Kay LM (2008) Affective and adrenocorticotrophic

responses to photoperiod in Wistar rats. J Neuroendocrinol

20:261–267

Prendergast BJ, Nelson RJ (2005) Affective responses to changes in

day length in Siberian hamsters. (Phodopus sungorus). Psycho-

neuroendocrinology 30:438–452

Prut L, Belzung C (2003) The open field as a paradigm to measure the

effects of drugs on anxiety-like behaviors: a review. Eur J

Pharmacol 463:3–33

Pyter LM, Nelson RJ (2006) Enduring effects of photoperiod on

affective behaviors in Siberian hamsters. (Phodopus sungorus).

Behav Neurosci 120:125–134

Raoux N, Benoit O, Dantchev N, Denise P, Franc B, Allilaire JF,

Widlocher D (1994) Circadian pattern of motor activity in major

depressed patients undergoing antidepressant therapy: relation-

ship between actigraphic measures and clinical course. Psychi-

atry Res 52:85–98

Refinetti R (1996) Rhythms of body temperature and temperature

selection are out of phase in a diurnal rodent, Octodon degus.

Physiol Behav 60:959–961

Refinetti R (2004) Daily activity patterns of a nocturnal and a diurnal

rodent in a seminatural environment. Physiol Behav 82:285–294

Roll U, Dayan T, Kronfeld-Schor N (2006) On the role of phylogeny

in determining activity patterns of rodents. Evol Ecol

20:479–490

Rotics S, Dayan T, Levy O, Kronfeld-Schor N (2011) Light

masking in the field: an experiment with nocturnal and diurnal

spiny mice under semi-natural field conditions. Chronobiol Int

28:70–75

Roybal K, Theobold D, Graham A, DiNieri JA, Russo SJ, Krishnan

V, Chakravarty S, Peevey J, Oehrlein N, Birnbaum S, Vitaterna

MH, Orsulak P, Takahashi JS, Nestler EJ, Carlezon WA,

McClung CA (2007) Mania-like behavior induced by disruption

of CLOCK. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104:6406–6411

Sadock VA, Kaplan HI (2007) Kaplan and Sadock’s synopsis of

psychiatry. Lippincott Williams, Wilkins

Scheer FAJL, Hilton MF, Mantzoros CS, Shea SA (2009) Adverse

metabolic and cardiovascular consequences of circadian mis-

alignment. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:4453–4458

Shuboni DD, Cramm S, Yan L, Nunez AA, Smale L (2012) Acute

behavioral responses to light and darkness in nocturnal Mus

T. Ashkenazy-Frolinger et al.

123

musculus and diurnal Arvicanthis niloticus. J Biol Rhythms

27:299–307

Sinitskaya N, Schuster-Klein C, Guardiola-Lemaitre B, Gourmelen S,

Pevet P, Challet E (2008) Short day-length increases sucrose

consumption and adiposity in rats fed a high-fat diet. Psycho-

neuroendocrinology 33:1269–1278

Smale L, Heideman PD, French JA (2005) Behavioral neuroendocri-

nology in nontraditional species of mammals: things the

‘‘knockout’’ mouse CAN’T tell us. Horm Behav 48:474–483

Smale L, Nunez AA, Schwartz MD (2008) Rhythms in a diurnal

brain. Biol Rhythm Res 39:305–318

Sonntag A, Rothe B, Guldner J, Yassouridis A, Holsboer F, Steiger A

(1996) Trimipramine and imipramine exert different effects on

the sleep EEG and on nocturnal hormone secretion during

treatment of major depression. Depression 4:1–13

Tallman JF (1999) Neuropsychopharmacology at the new millen-

nium: new industry directions. Neuropsychopharmacology

20:99–105

Tan Z-L, Bao A-M, Tao M, Liu Y-J, Zhou J-N (2007) Circadian

rhythm of salivary serotonin in patients with major depressive

disorder. Neuro Endocrinol Lett 28:395–400

Tecott LH, Nestler EJ (2004) Neurobehavioral assessment in the

information age. Nat Neurosci 7:462–466

Teicher MH, Glod CA, Magnus E, Harper D, Benson GE, Krueger K,

McGreenery CE (1997) Circadian rest–activity disturbances in

seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:124–130

Todder D, Caliskan S, Baune BT (2006) Night locomotor activity and

quality of sleep in quetiapine-treated patients with depression.

J Clin Psychopharmacol 26:638–642

Volkers AC, Tulen JHM, van den Broek WW, Bruijn JA, Passchier J,

Pepplinkhuizen L (2003) Motor activity and autonomic cardiac

functioning in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord

76:23–30

Wallace-Boone TL, Newton AE, Wright RN, Lodge NJ, McElroy JF

(2008) Behavioral and pharmacological validation of the gerbil

forced-swim test: effects of neurokinin-1 receptor antagonists.

Neuropsychopharmacology 33:1919–1928

Walsh RN, Cummins RA (1976) The open-field test: a critical review.

Psychol Bull 83:482–504

Wehr TA, Wirz-Justice A, Goodwin FK, Duncan W, Gillin JC (1979)

Phase advance of the circadian sleep–wake cycle as an

antidepressant. Science 206:710–713

Wehr TA, Jacobsen FM, Sack DA, Arendt J, Tamarkin L et al (1986)

Phototherapy of seasonal affective disorder. Time of day and

suppression of melatonin are not critical for antidepressant

effects. Arch Gen Psychiatry 43(9):870–875

Wirz-Justice A, Van den Hoofdakker RH (1999) Sleep deprivation in

depression: what do we know, where do we go? Biol Psychiatry

46:445–453

Wirz-Justice A, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Kafka MS, Naber D,

Marangos PJ, Campbell IC (1980) Antidepressant drugs slow

circadian rhythms in behavior and brain neurotransmitter

receptors (proceedings). Psychopharmacol Bull 16:45–47

Woods CA, Boraker DK (1975) Octodon degus. American Society of

Mammalogists. Mammalian Species 67:1-5

Wu JC, Kelsoe JR, Schachat C, Bunney BG, DeModena A, Golshan

S, Gillin JC, Potkin SG, Bunney WE (2009) Rapid and sustained

antidepressant response with sleep deprivation and chronother-

apy in bipolar disorder. Biol Psychiatry 66:298–301

Yamada N, Martin-Iverson MT, Daimon K, Tsujimoto T, Takahashi S

(1995) Clinical and chronobiological effects of light therapy on

nonseasonal affective disorders. Biol Psychiatry 37:866–873

Zawilska JB, Derbiszewska T, Nowak JZ (1997) Prolonged exposure

of chicks to light or darkness differentially affects the quinpirole-

evoked suppression of serotonin N-acetyltransferase activity in

chick retina: an impact on dopamine D4-like receptor. J Pineal

Res 22:59–64

Zubidat AE, Ben-Shlomo R, Haim A (2007) Thermoregulatory and

endocrine responses to light pulses in short-day acclimated

social voles. (Microtus socialis). Chronobiol Int 24:269–288

Short photoperiod in diurnal degu

123