Dissociative Identity Disorder: A Diagnostic Puzzle Literature Review

28
A Diagnostic Puzzle 1 Running head: DIAGNOSTIC PUZZLE Dissociative Identity Disorder: A Diagnostic Puzzle Literature Review Trish Klawe Trinity Western University Spring, 1996

Transcript of Dissociative Identity Disorder: A Diagnostic Puzzle Literature Review

A Diagnostic Puzzle 1

Running head: DIAGNOSTIC PUZZLE

Dissociative Identity Disorder:

A Diagnostic Puzzle

Literature Review

Trish Klawe

Trinity Western University

Spring, 1996

A Diagnostic Puzzle 2

Abstract

Over the last two decades, the diagnosis of dissociative identity disorder has evoked polarized contention

in both research and clinical arenas. The level of emotion elicited in the proponents and sceptics of the

diagnosis appears to transcend the content of the debate. When a concern commands such potent

reactions, perhaps there are underlying agendas which warrant further investigation. The present work

seeks to review historical evidence, the evolving nosological criteria, an alternative dissociation

construct, the etiological and epidemiological controversy, the forensic psychiatric ramifications, and the

supernatural element. In the process, the writer wishes a) to demonstrate the quality and quantity of

scientific investigation completed, to date, in validation of the diagnosis; b) to stress the need for a

paradigm shift in the official North American nosological schema; c) and to promote future dissociation

screening studies in the prison population.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 3

Historical Evidence

Dissociative identity disorder was first granted official North American diagnostic status under

the label multiple personality disorder (MPD) in the 1980 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, (American Psychiatric Association) (2nd ed.). However, that was not the historical inception

of MPD. Fragmentation of self is central to the Egyptian Osiris complex; the outcome of the myth is

perpetuated by way of persisting child abuse through future generations. A similar focus on

transformation of identity was prevalent in the North American Haida Indian tribe, years before the

arrival of the first white settlers. A third illustration rests in the dissociation and trance states of

circumpolar Shamanism (Ross, 1989). Fragmentation of self, transformation of identity, dissociation and

trance states are components of the MPD diagnosis. Historians have, in addition, underscored the

evolution from demon possession to MPD (Oesterreich, 1974; Rogo, 1987; Ross, 1989; Russell, 1981),

as applied in Adityanjee, Raju & Khandelwal's 1989 examination of possession syndrome and MPD

epidemiology in East India. The last century is especially prominent in the historical unfolding of MPD.

A strong team of dissociation clinical investigators, including Bernheim, Binet, Charcot, James,

Janet, Jung, Freud and Prince, recorded cases of MPD, hereafter termed dissociative identity disorder

(DID), between the late 19th and early 20th century (North, Ryall, Ricci & Wetzel, 1993; Ross, 1989).

Their clinical and therapeutic models of dissociation, in the opinion of contemporary dissociation

proponents, were flawed; Janet focused on concepts such as "mental degeneration" leading to an ultimate

diagnosis of schizophrenia (Rosenbaum, 1980; Ross, 1989), Prince supported the ascendancy of one

primary alter in therapy (Coons, 1980). The greatest credit for scientific theoretical constructs, clinical

observation and experimental evidence of DID was awarded to Alfred Binet (Ross, 1989).

Goff and Simms (1993) disagreed with the correlation between 19th and 20th century DID case

accounts. They contested that statistically significant differences exist in the mean number of

A Diagnostic Puzzle 4

personalities, age of onset, proportion of males, and in the prevalence of childhood abuse histories across

the two historically and culturally detached samples. This work was rebutted directly by Armstrong

(1993), and indirectly by an extensive study of the subjects (North et al., 1993). Out of the entire 20th

century collection of clinical DID accounts, the studies by Sigmund Freud generated the greatest respect.

In 1896, Freud presented a paper to the Society for Psychiatry and Neurology in Vienna (as cited

in Powell & Boer, 1994). The Aetiology of Hysteria depicted the "seduction theory" in which patients,

upon recalling previously forgotten events of childhood sexual abuse, suffered abreactions of traumatic

effect followed by alleviation of symptoms. In his clinical diaries, Freud recorded the phenomenon of

dissociation or double consciousness; he even noted an appearance of superior intelligence outside the

patients' consciousness (Ross, 1989). Later Freudian theory, however, negated the trauma-driven

etiology of dissociative disorders; his previous constructs were usurped by a set of ancillary

metapsychological interpretations for his patients' symptoms. Freud's repudiation of the seduction theory

was instrumental in the discreditation of DID (Coons, 1980; Gutowski, 1993; Kluft, 1987a; North et al.,

1993; Putnam, 1989; Ross, 1989).

Despite their reciprocal disdain, Freudian and biological psychiatry have, for much of this

century, enjoyed a mutually propitious homeostasis. The biologists of classical psychiatry inspired the

biologically-driven mental illness model; Axis I clinical disorders of the DSM-IV (1994) testify to their

success. Today, support of pure Freudian psychoanalysis is fading. Simultaneously, psychiatric and

psychological validation of DID, and its primary construct of dissociation, a strategy employed for

coping with severe trauma, is flourishing despite the raging dispute. Increasingly, the contribution of

dissociation to the psychopathology of other Axis I and II disorders is being recognized (Bernstein &

Putnam, 1986; DiTomasso & Routh, 1993). How, then, have the DSM editors rewarded the

polydiagnostic DID classification?

A Diagnostic Puzzle 5

Evolving Nosological Criteria

DID was unobtrusively introduced in the DSM-II (American Psychiatric Association, 1968, p.

40) under the division, "hysterical neurosis; dissociative type." By 1980, the disorder achieved status as

a dissociative disorder in the DSM-III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980). The third edition also

narrowed the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia, possibly enhancing recognition of DID (Rosenbaum,

1980; Ross, 1989). The criteria modifications in the DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association,

1987) rendered that distinct personalities or personality states (otherwise known as “alters”) with

separate social relationships are not necessarily present in all DID cases. Nor is there invariably a

distinct differentiation of which alter personality is in control at any given time.

The DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 1994) lists the following criteria:

A. The presence of two or more distinct identities or personality states (each with its own

relatively enduring pattern of perceiving, relating to, and thinking about the environment

and self).

B. At least two of these identities or personality states recurrently take control of the person's

behaviour.

C. Inability to recall important personal information that is too extensive to be unexplained by

ordinary forgetfulness.

D. The disturbance is not due to the direct psychological effects of a substance (e.g., blackouts

or chaotic behaviour during alcohol intoxication) or a general medical condition ( e.g.,

complex partial seizures). Note: In children, the symptoms are not attributable to

imaginary playmates or other fantasy play.

The key modification in the latest edition is the presence of amnesia. Clearly, differential diagnosis is

not a pure science.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 6

Upon receiving diagnostic status two decades ago, DID has been diagnosed by an increasing

number of North American mental health care workers (Amstutz, 1992; Coons, 1994; Cozolino, 1990;

Friesen, 1991, 1992; Gutowski; 1993; Kluft, 1984; Loewenstein, 1994; Putnam, 1989, 1992; Putnam &

Lowenstein, 1993; Rogo, 1987; Ryder, 1992; Wells, 1990). Sceptics of DID argue for false positive

diagnosis; proponents stress possible underdiagnosis. DID is as complex to assess as it is a diagnostic

disorder. Perhaps a new theoretical construct is warranted.

Trauma Disorders, A New Dissociative Construct

Unfortunately many DSM disorders, of which DID is only one, are not purely biomedical

illnesses; for many the natural history and prognosis remain uncertain, as do known effective treatments

(Maxmen & Ward, 1994; Ross, 1989). The emotional controversy is partly fuelled by the fact that DID

does not fit into the DSM-IV (1994) paradigm, nor into its hierarchical and parsimonious diagnostic

constructs. A DID patient often satisfies numerous DSM-IV diagnoses; a more discerning single

diagnosis which encompasses all of the symptomology is required.

Intense study of dissociation may demand a paradigm shift. Scientists and clinicians could be

forced to modify the established diagnostic scheme, their comprehension of phenomenology, and their

therapeutics. For example, recent work indicates that schneiderian first-rank symptoms are more

common in DID than schizophrenic patients (Kluft, 1987b; Ross, 1989; Ross, Heber, Norton &

Anderson, 1989; Ross, Miller, Reagor, Bjornson, Fraser & Anderson, 1990). Curiously, the schneiderian

symptoms are also trauma-driven and treatable with psychotherapy (Ross, 1989). Due to the prevailing

political leverage of biological psychiatry, it is imperative that DID be investigated with clinical

objectivity. Experiments and observations employing reliable and valid instruments must generate

scientific data. Otherwise, the debate is open to unsubstantiated contentions and highly temperamental

reasoning.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 7

Over the past decade, the mental health community has become increasing aware of the need to

screen for dissociative symptoms within a variety of clinical settings (Bliss, 1988; Gutowski, 1993; North

et al., 1993; Putnam, 1989; Ross, 1989, 1990; Ross, Heber et al., 1989; Saxe, van der Kolk, Berkowitz,

Chinman, Hall, Lieberg & Schwartz, 1993). Past investigations report relatively high frequencies of

dissociative experiences among a) psychiatric inpatients (Ross, Anderson, Fleisher, & Norton, 1991;

Saxe et al. 1993), b) Vietnam veterans diagnosed with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (Branscomb,

1991; Bremner & Southwick, 1992; Bremner & Steinberg, 1993; Brende & Benedict, 1980; Hyer &

Albrecht, 1993; Marmar & Weiss, 1994; Spiegel & Gardena, 1990, 1991; Van der Hart & Eliezer, 1993;

van der Kolk, 1994), and c) individuals with a history of childhood abuse (Coons, 1994; DiTomasso &

Routh; 1993; Kluft, 1985a; Ross, 1989; Sanders & Giolas, 1991).

The maturing link between dissociative symptoms and traumatic experiences led Ross (1989) to

propose an alternative diagnostic category. Under the inclusive category of trauma disorders, DID is

considered the most severe and therefore supraordinate in terms of treatment focus. Time of onset

further classifies disorders within this model. For example, chronic trauma with childhood onset leads to

DID, acute or chronic trauma wheras trauma with adult onset may lead to a diagnosis of PTSD. This

alternative is both impressive and encouraging. Authors of future DSM editions would, however, have to

agree with Ross' etiological model of DID.

Etiological & Epidemiological Controversy

Most proponents envision DID as a subtype of chronic trauma disorder; they contend that such a

classification is consistent with available laboratory and scientific evidence (Bliss, 1988; Coons, 1980,

1994; Friesen, 1991, 1992; Gutowski, 1993; Kluft, 1985a, North et al., 1993; Putnam, 1989; Ross, 1989,

1990, 1991; Saxe et al., 1993; Stolzenburg, 1990). The etiology helps to explain the comorbidity

quandary. For example, Putnam & Loewenstein (1993) suggest that the increase in diagnosis results

A Diagnostic Puzzle 8

from a reformulation of DID as a "chronic form of childhood-onset posttraumatic dissociative disorder."

Kemp, Gilbertson, & Torem (1988) examined the seriously abusive background shared by DID and

borderline personality disorder (BPD) clients, concluding that, where multiples employ dissociation to

cope, borderlines do not.

The disorder begins early in life; children as young as three have been treated for DID (Confer &

Ables, 1983; Coons, 1994; Gutowski, 1993; North et al., 1993; Schaffer & Cozolino, 1992). In 1992,

Kluft presented what is now commonly termed the four-factor theory of etiology. It entails a) biological

capacity for dissociation; b) a history of trauma or abuse; c) specific psychological structures or contents

which can be employed in the creation of alternate personalities; and d) lack of adequate nurturing or

opportunities to recover from abuse.

The sceptics counter with two leading, yet related, etiological constructs. Some suggest

iatrogenesis, unnaturally manufactured through misdiagnosis, suggestion and hypnosis, explains the

exponential diagnostic increase (Baker, 1992; Ganaway, 1989; Merskey, 1992; Ofshe & Waters, 1993;

Orne, Soskis, Dinges, Orne & Tonry, 1985; Spanos, Weekes & Bertrand, 1985; Toland, Hoffman &

Lofuts, 1991). Warnings to therapists with respect to possible countertransference needs, which

unwittingly contribute to the overdiagnosis of DID, have been published by at least three DID proponents

(Coons, 1991; Ross, 1991; Rosik, 1995). Others propose that wilful malingering or misguided patients

seeking out underwriting therapists are the culprits (Fahy, 1988; Piper, 1994a, 1994b; Sarbin, 1995;

Spanos et al., 1985; Spanos, 1994; Spiegel, 1988).

Regarding iatrogenesis, Ross (1990, p. 350) responds, "There is not a single reported case of

alleged, let alone confirmed, iatrogenic MPD reported in the world literature." Ross challenges the

scientific reasoning behind the "pseudo-databased" work of researchers such as Harriman, Kampman,

Levitt or Spanos who have claimed to artificially manufacture multiple personalities. This point elicits

A Diagnostic Puzzle 9

discussion of hypnosis and suggestion, and the often cited 1986 simulation study of Spanos and his

colleagues in which non-MPD college students displayed MPD-like symptomology following hypnotic

interview treatments (Spanos, Weekes, Menary & Bertrand, 1986).

One could dedicate an entire literature review to the issue of hypnosis and the retrieval of

memory (Kluft, 1985b), not to mention False Memory Syndrome. Ganaway (1989), Orne et al. (1985)

and Spanos et al. (1985) contend, and I happen to agree, that DID patients are easy candidates for

memory contamination at the hands of therapists. Memory contamination may be occurring on a limited

basis; but, it is an error to generalise the supposition to a substantial, and ever growing, portion of the

mental health community. The overgeneralised conjecture has been refuted; tests have shown that

artificially created alters differ significantly in symptomology from genuine DID alters (Coons &

Milstein, 1994; Kluft, 1987a; Ross, 1989, 1990). Over a century ago, Alfred Binet created alters

experimentally; he concluded that they could be called out only by the persons who created them.

Autohypnotic alters could, alternately, be called out by anyone (Ross, 1989). Memory retrieval is a

complex subject.

Van der Kolk and his colleagues, of the Trauma Clinic at Harvard Medical School/Massachusetts

General Hospital, have contributed a significant body of work in the field of traumatic memory recovery.

Van der Kolk contends that traumatic memory must be differentiated from normal or explicit memory.

Normal memories are organised by category in the hippocampus of the brain and later data may indeed

contaminate earlier memories. With severe trauma, however, particularly when it occurs early in life, the

stress of the event may overload the circuitry of the brain's limbic system, thus barring the information

from reaching the hippocampus. Rather, it remains engraved in the sensorimotor processes and it is

dissociated from the person's conscious awareness (Van der Hart & Witztum, 1993; van der Kolk, 1994;

A Diagnostic Puzzle 10

van der Kolk, Perry & Herman, 1991; van der Kolk, Roth & Pelcovitz, 1992; van der Kolk & Van der

Hart, 1991).

Other sceptics have appropriately challenged the validity of research designs, specifically self-

report procedures, and have demanded supporting corroborative evidence (Spanos et al., 1985; Tillman,

Nash, & Lerner, 1994; Wakefield & Underwager, 1992). This controversy was addressed by Coons

(1994) in which he contends, "this study provides documentation through collateral reports from

numerous sources that the abuse actually did occur." In addition to the Minnesota Multiphasic

Personality Inventory (MMPI), the last ten years has seen the design of a variety of dissociative

diagnostic scales and psychometric tests to facilitate scientific collaboration.

The leading tests, with sound psychometric properties, employed in programmatic research of

dissociative disorders include: a) Dissociative Experiences Scale (DES) (Bernstein & Putnam, 1986), b)

the Dissociative Disorders Interview Schedule (DDIS) (Ross, Heber, Norton & Anderson, 1989), and c)

the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R-Dissociative Disorders (SCID-D) (Steinberg,

Rounsavilee & Cichetti, 1990). Multicentred studies and reports have supported the validity and

reliability of these scales (Carlson, Putnam, Ross, Torem, Coons, Dill, Loewenstein & Braun, 1993;

Ellason & Ross, 1994; Frischholz, Braun, Sachs, Hopkins, Shaeffer, Lewis, Leavitt, Pasquotto &

Schwartz, 1990; Hall & Steinberg, 1994; Ross, Heber et al., 1989; Ross, Miller et al., 1990; Steinberg et

al., 1990, 1991).

With respect to children, Sanders & Giolas (1991) have conducted psychometric examination of

dissociation related to child abuse/trauma scales; Putnam, Helmers & Trickett (1993) have focused on the

development, reliability, and validity of a child dissociation scale. Psychological testing of dissociative

disorders has also invoked interest (Armstrong & Loewenstein, 1990; Wagner, Allison & Wagner, 1983;

Wagner & Heise, 1974).

A Diagnostic Puzzle 11

A further argument regarding accurate prevalence rate relates to the sociocultural bias of a

positive DID diagnosis. Aldridge-Morris of Britain (1989) alleges that DID is an almost exclusively

North American curiosity. Perhaps the significant rift in British and American treatment of DID relates

to Britain's hesitancy to adopt the DSM-III (1980) modifications of both DID and schizophrenia criteria.

Merskey (1992) warns that when the diagnostic occurrence of a psychological disorder, namely DID,

which is not an infectious disease, increases at such a rate; psychiatrists should consider that social

effects may be the cause. This contention can be rebutted by the works of epidemiological researchers in

India, New Zealand and Australia, the Netherlands, and Switzerland, respectively (Adityanjee et al.,

1989; Atchinson & McFarlane, 1994; Boon & Draijer, 1993; Modestin, 1992). Historical study also

refutes Merskey's caution.

Forensic Psychiatry, The DID Defence

Between 1973 and 1993, at least 18 cases employing DID as a defence have reached the public

eye: two were acquitted on an insanity plea, two were found incompetent to stand trial, two verdicts

were unconfirmed, and the remaining offenders were found guilty (North et al. 1993). While Bianchi,

the infamous Hillside Strangler, ultimately failed diagnostic DID tests conducted by Orne and seven

others, Ross (1989, p. 73) considered that "Perhaps both malingering and genuine MPD were coexistent

in Bianchi." Clearly, this diagnosis, and the possible associated secondary gain courtesy of the insanity

plea, is a judicial and political hot potato.

Realistically, no battery of tests can prove scientifically, beyond reasonable doubt, that a criminal

is or is not multiple (Appelbaum & Greer, 1994; Saks, 1995; Watkins, 1984). Coons (1991) presented an

excellent journal with guidelines to enable the clinician to discern between genuine and malingered DID

in the forensic setting. In 1994, Coons & Milstein designed a study to contrast genuine DID subjects

with DID simulators, and to test for the presence of symptoms characteristic of either malingering or

A Diagnostic Puzzle 12

factitious disorder. I argue that antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) individuals, a large majority of

whom are not present in the psychiatric populace due to incarceration, represent another group who

warrant dissociative screening. I propose three chief grounds.

The first driving rationale behind further investigation of dissociation in an ASPD population is

of an epidemiological nature. The sex ratio of DID is one male to approximately eight females (Ross,

Norton et al., 1989); it is proposed that the balance of seven men are likely serving sentences resulting

from their abusive tendencies (North et al., 1993; Gutowski, 1993; Kluft, 1987; Ross, 1989; van der

Kolk, 1994). Waxmen & Ward (1994) state, "In psychiatric populations, multiple personality is reputed

to affect four times as many women as men, but if prison populations were included, the sex differential

would narrow." Second, considerable overlap in DID and ASPD symptomology is well documented

(Fink, 1991; Kluft, 1987a; North et al., 1993; Ross, 1989). Third, literature indicates that a large

proportion of ASPD patients have childhood histories of neglect and abuse (Benjamin, 1993; Cline,

1995; Frick & Lahey, 1992; Hodge, 1992; Lahey & Rolf, 1995; Lewis & Bucholz, 1993; Luntz, &

Widom, 1994).

Specifically, Lahey & Piacentini (1988) conclude that conduct disorder, a precursor to ASPD, is

clearly linked to parental psychopathology. In their 1993 work, in which over 2,500 men and women

were surveyed, Lewis & Bucholz reported a strong relationship between alcoholism, antisocial behaviour

and negative family histories. Hodge (1992) proposed that ASPD has its origins in PTSD as a result of

childhood and sexual abuse. He conceptualised ASPD persons as suffering from an addiction to violence

resulting from developmentally mediated PTSD.

Dissociation screening of the prison populace would enhance understanding of the comorbidity

issue between ASPD and DID subjects. The ramifications of the DID diagnosis to forensic psychiatry

are beyond description and clearly of political import. If dissociative disorders are discovered in the

A Diagnostic Puzzle 13

prison population, alternative therapeutic applications would warrant research investigation

(Modlin,1992). Piper (1994b) and his colleagues advise that the treatment of DID is an unjustified

economical burden to society. What of the price paid by individuals and mankind in general when one

individual is jailed for life on a murder conviction? The writer's philosophical rhetoric leads to the last

aspect of the present work.

The Supernatural Element

Prasad (1985, p. 301) eloquently introduces the supernatural element, "The concept that more

than one person may exist within one body is so alien to common sense that it borders on the

supernatural." I wonder if one can equally tolerate the concept of genuine, pathological evil in today's

cultural milieu? Pure scientific inquiry, particularly in the field of social sciences, must acknowledge

historical constancy. It is illogical to "scientifically" deny the reality of mankind's history of cruelty.

Perhaps the most fascinating facet of DID is that it has, without intention, drawn together both

Christian and non-Christian scientists/therapists who are dedicated to working with, or on behalf of, DID

individuals. For those in this field, and I speak from experience, the discussion of ritualistic abuse is

often sadly unavoidable (Cozolino, 1990; Friesen, 1992; Gutowski, 1993; Hilgard, 1986; McCulley,

1995; Shaffer & Cozolino, 1992). This is very dangerous, for the arguments against satanic ritual abuse

seem to parallel those against the veracity of DID as a diagnosis (Mulhern, 1992; Gardner, 1992; Rogers,

1992; Spanos, 1994). A number of proponents have been accused of dualistic mind sets which advocate

belief in the diagnosis; evangelical Christians often fall within this category (Rosik, 1995; Ross, 1989).

As a caution to overenthusiastic Christian therapists, Rosik produced an excellent paper designed to aid

one in avoiding the traps which lead to this type of accusation.

Spanos (1994) boldly alleges that satanism is a myth. He described a book, Michelle

Remembers, (Smith & Pazder, 1980) as "propaganda used by the evangelical Christian movement that

A Diagnostic Puzzle 14

became increasingly prominent in many facets of American social and political life during the 1980s."

Dr. Pazder, the therapist and co-author, was not a Christian at the time of writing, and the victim was of

Roman Catholic heritage. Spanos further purports that law enforcement agencies have failed to

substantiate the reports of ritualistic abuse. This is clearly false (Crowley, 1974; Frattorola, 1986;

Johnson, 1989; LaVey, 1969; Marron, 1988; Morgan & Zedner, 1992; Ryder, 1992). A copious quantity

of evidence is available from agencies such as a) the Office of Criminal Justice Planning; b) the Los

Angeles Ritual Abuse Task Force, which annually updates a Ritual Abuse handbook; or c) Minister of

Supply and Services Canada, which also publishes Canada's law on child sex abuse: A handbook (Wells,

1990). Wells directly compares the signs of satanic ritualistic abuse with those of non-ritualistic sexual

or physical abuse observed in victimised children. Marron (1988) stages a compelling case for the

existence of ritualistic abuse in the context of Canada's most infamous trial on child abuse.

Generational cults play a major role in familial incidence of ritualistically abused DID survivors

(Bryant, Kessler & Shirar, 1992; Friesen, 1991; Gutowski, 1993; McCulley; 1995; Wells, 1990). The

family of origin confesses rigid, false, religious or mystical beliefs. Though the unit may present a united

front to the community, it is internally fraught with conflict. Often the family is isolated from the

community which creates strong resistance against intervention and/or assistance from external sources

(Frattorola, 1986; Gutowski, 1993; Johnson, 1989; Wells, 1990). Generally, at least one caretaker

demonstrates severe pathology and the child is subject to contradictory communications from his/her

significant others during childhood (Wells, 1990).

For those who present DID as a sociocognitive phenomenon (Spanos & Burgess, 1994), Ross

(1989), a non-Christian, retorts that the evolution of this disorder is not without historical explanation.

He outlines a four step progression stemming from the early 1800s to our modern world a) classical

demon possession, b) early transitional demon possession, c) late transitional demon possession, and d)

A Diagnostic Puzzle 15

postdemonic demon possession (see also Kenny, 1981). In today's postdemonic demon possession

civilization, Christianity no longer functions as the guiding authority of society. DID is therefore subject

to scientific scrutiny. Unfortunately, science, is not immune to political or economical influence.

With regard to memory recovery and ritualistic abuse, the writer agrees that a)memories are not

necessarily accurate and can be false, as components of the False Memory Syndrome camp would

propose; b) cases exist in which the therapist has influenced the patient in retrieval of questionable

memories of ritualistic abuse; and c) it is extremely difficult to forensically corroborate retrospective

traumatic events which may have occurred over two decades ago in the patient's life. It is for the

foregoing reasons that both proponents and sceptics of DID must remain rationally balanced, and be

exceedingly careful not to diagnose from a partisan perspective.

Conclusion

The writer has sought to present both angles of this dispute, though each appears immersed in

today's plethora of cultural doctrines. Whether debating historical, diagnostic, etiological,

epidemiological, forensic or supernatural facets of this controversy, each facet tends to rest in the

overarching philosophy of the debater.

As DID does not appear to be an infectious illness, it is the responsibility of the proponents to

satisfy the scientific requirements of current biomedical psychiatry. If they are successful, dissociation

could challenge the existing nosologies and metatheories of psychopathology. A true correlation,

scientifically demonstrated, between dissociation and other Axis I and II disorders, could force a major

change in the DSM-V or -VI (Kihlstrom, Glisky & Angiulo, 1994; Ross, 1989; Saxe et al., 1993). Even

then, if the proponents fail to meet the fashionable philosophical requirements of the day, DID may again

achieve obscurity.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 16

Having observed the destruction of self in clients dear to me, I personally hope that the scientific

proponents meet the task ahead, regardless of the opposition. Perhaps there are indeed men in jail whose

broken hearts and minds have been shattered by childhood abuse. If this were the case of even 1% of the

prison populace, imagine the benefit to society, not to speak of the misdiagnosed ASPD convicts. It is a

diagnostic puzzle worth solving.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 17

References

Adityanjee, G. S. P., Raju & Khandelwal, S. K. (1989). Current status of multiple personality

disorder in India. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146 (12), 1607-1610.

Aldridge-Morris, R. (1989). Multiple Personality: An exercise in deception.

London: Erlbaum.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Amstutz, W. (1991). Exposing and confronting Satan & associates. (Rev. 5th ed.). Rochester,

MN: National Counselling Resource Centre.

Appelbaum, P. S. & Greer, A. (1994). Who's on trial? Multiple personalities and the insanity

defence. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 45 (10), 965-966.

Armstrong, J. G. (1993). Commentary: Is anything consistent over time? The Journal of Nervous

and Mental Disease, 181 (10), 601-603).

Armstrong, J. G. & Loewenstein, R. J. (1990). Characteristics of patients with Multiple

personality and dissociative disorders on psychological testing. The Journal of Nervous and Mental

Disease, 178 (7), 448-454.

Atchinson, M. & McFarlane, A. C. (1994). A review of dissociation and dissociative disorders

[Abstract]. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 28 (4), 591-594.

Benjamin, L. S. (1993). Interpersonal diagnosis and treatment of Personality disorders. New

York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Berger, A. M., Knutson, J. F. Mehm, J. G. & Perkins, K. A. (1988). The self report of punitive

childhood experiences of young adults and adolescents. Child Abuse & Neglect, 12, 251-262.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 18

Bernstein, E. M. & Putnam, F. W. (1986). Development, reliability, and validity of a

Dissociation Scale. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174 (12), 727-735.

Bliss, E. L. (1988). Professional skepticism about multiple personality. Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease, 176, 533-534.

Blisson, J. I. (1993). Automatism and post-traumatic stress disorder. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 163, 830-832.

Boon, S. & Draijer, N. (1993). Multiple personality disorder in the Netherlands: A clinical

investigation of 71 patients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150,489-494.

Branscomb, L. P. (1991). Dissociation in combat-related post-traumatic stress disorder.

Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 4 (1), 13-20.

Bremner, J. D. & Southwick, S. (1992). Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder in

Vietnam combat veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149 (3), 328-332.

Bremner, J. D. & Steinberg, M. (1993). Use of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV

Dissociative Disorders for systematic assessment of dissociative symptoms in posttraumatic stress

disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150 (7), 1011-1014.

Brende, J. O., & Benedict, B. D. (1980). The Vietnam combat delayed stress response syndrome:

Hypnotherapy of dissociative symptoms [Abstract]. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 23, 34-40.

Bryant, D., Kessler, J. & Shirar, L. (1992). The family inside: Working with the multiple. New

York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Carlson, E. B., Putnam, F., Ross, C. A., Torem, M., Coons, P. M., Dill, D. L., Loewenstein, R. J.

& Braun, B. G. (1993). Validity of the Dissociative Experiences Scale in screening for multiple

personality disorder: A multicenter study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150 (7), 1030-1036.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 19

Cline, F. W. (1995). Conscienceless acts societal mayhem: Uncontrollable, unreachable youth

and today's desensitized world. Golden, Colorado: The Love and Logic Press, Inc.

Confer, W. N. & Ables, B. S. (1983). Multiple personality - Etiology, diagnosis and treatment.

New York, New York: Human Sciences Press.

Coons, P. M. (1980). Multiple personality: Diagnostic considerations. Journal of Clinical

Psychiatry, 41, 330-336.

Coons, P. M. (1994). Confirmation of childhood abuse in child and adolescent cases of Multiple

personality disorder and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified. The Journal of Nervous and

Mental Disease, 182 (8), 461-464.

Coons, P. M. & Milstein, V. (1994). Factitious or malingered multiple personality disorder:

Eleven cases. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 7 (2), 81-85.

Cozolino, L. J. (1990). Ritualistic child abuse, psychopathology, and evil. The Journal of

Psychology and Theology, 18 (3), 218-227.

Crowley, A. (1974). Magick in theory and practice. New York: Samuel Weiser.

DiTomasso, M. J. & Routh, D. K. (1993). Recall of abuse in childhood and three measures of

dissociation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 477-485.

Dubester, K. A. & Braun, B. G. (1995). Psychometric properties of the Dissociative Experiences

Scale. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 183 (4), 231-235.

Edwards, L. M. (1990). [Special issue]. Journal of Child and Youth Care, 67-70.

Ellason, J. W. & Ross, C. A. (1994). Convergent validity of the new form of the DES.

Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 7 (2), 101-103.

Fahy, T. A. (1988). The diagnosis of multiple personality disorder: A critical review. British

Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 597-606.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 20

Fink, D. (1991). The comorbidity of multiple personality disorder and DSM-III-R Axis II

disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 14, 547-566.

Frattorola, J. (1986). America's best-kept secret [Special issue]. Passport Magazine.

Friesen, J. G. (1991). Uncovering the mystery of MPD - its shocking origins... Its surprising cure.

San Bernardino, CA: Here's Life Publishers.

Friesen, J. G. (1992). Ego-dystonic or ego-alien: Alternate personality or evil spirit? Journal of

Psychology and Theology, 10 (3), 197-200.

Frick, P. J. & Lahey, B. B. (1992). Familial risk factors to oppositional defiant disorder and

conduct disorder: Parental psychopathology and maternal parenting. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 60 (1), 49-55.

Frischholz, E. J., Braun, B. G., Sachs, R. G., Hopkins, L., Shaeffer, D. M., Lewis, J., Leavitt, F.,

Pasquotto, M. A. & Schwartz, D. R. (1990). The Dissociative Experiences Scale: Further replication and

validation. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative Disorders, 3, 151-153.

Ganaway, G. K. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying the role of exogenous

trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants. Dissociation, 2 (4), 405-220.

Gardner, R. A. (1992). True and false accusations of child sex Abuse : A guide for legal and

mental health professionals. Cresskill, New Jersey: Creative Therapeutics.

Goff, C. C. & Simms, C. A. (1993). Has multiple personality disorder remained consistent over

time? A comparison of past and recent cases. The Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 181 (10), 595-

600.

Gutowski, W. (1993). [Interviews regarding treatment of psychiatric inpatient/outpatient MPD,

Chilliwack General Hospital]. Unpublished raw data.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 21

Hall, P. & Steinberg, M. (1994). Systematic assessment of dissociative symptoms and disorders

using the SCID-D in a clinical outpatient setting: Three cases. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative

Disorders, 7 (2), 112-116.

Hilgard, E. R. (1986). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action.

(Exp. ed.). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Hodge, J. E. (1992). Addiction to violence: A new model of psychotherapy [Special issue].

Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 2 (2), 212-223.

Hyer, L. A.. & Albrecht, J. W. (1993). Dissociative experiences of Vietnam veterans with

chronic posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Reports, 73 (2), 519-530.

Johnson, J. (1989). The edge of evil: The rise of Satanism in North America. Dallas, Texas:

Word Publishing.

Kemp, K., Gilbertson, A.D. & Torem, M. (1988, December). The differential diagnosis of

Multiple personality disorder from borderline personality disorder. Dissociation.

Kenny, M. G. (1981). Multiple personality and spirit possession. Psychiatry, 44, 337-357.

Kihlstrom, J. F., Glisky, M. L. & Angiulo, M. J. (1994). Dissociative tendencies and dissociative

disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 117-124.

Kluft, R. P. (1985a). Childhood antecedents of multiple personality disorder. Washington, DC:

American Psychiatric Press.

Kluft, R. P. (1985b). Using hypnotic inquiry protocols to monitor treatment progress and stability

in multiple personality disorder. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 28 (2), 63-75.

Kluft, R. P. (1986). High-functioning multiple personalitypatients: Three cases. The Journal of

Nervous and Mental Disease, 174 (12), 722-726.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 22

Kluft, R. P. (1987a). An update on multiple personality disorder. Hospital and Community

Psychiatry, 38, 363-373.

Kluft, R. P. (1987b). First-rank symptoms as a diagnostic clue to multiple personality disorder.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 144 (3), 293-298.

Kluft, R. P. (1992). Discussion: A specialist's perspective on multiple personality disorder.

Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 12 (1), 139-171.

Lahey, B. B. & Loeber, R. (1995). Four-year longitudinal study of conduct disorder in boys:

Patterns and predictors of persistence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104 (1), 83-93.

LaVey, A. S. (1969). The satanic bible. New York, NY: Avon Books.

Lewis, C. E. & Bucholz, K. K. (1993). Alcoholism, antisocial behaviour and family history:

Erratum. Addiction, 88 (11), 1587.

Loewenstein, R. J. (1994). Diagnosis, epidemiology, clinical course, treatment, and cost

effectiveness of treatment for dissociative disorders and MPD: Report submitted to the Clinton

Administrative Task Force on Health Care Financing Reform. Dissociation: Progress in the Dissociative

Disorders, 7 (1), 3-11.

Loewenstein, R. J., Hamilton, J., & Algona, S., Reid, N. & deVries, M. (1987). Experiential

sampling in the study of multiple personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144 (1), 19-24.

Luntz, B. K. & Widom, C. S. (1994). Antisocial Personality disorder in abused and neglected

children grown up. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151 (6), 670-674.

Marmar, C. R. & Weiss, D. S. (1994). Peritraumatic dossociation and posttraumatic stress in

male Vietnam theater veterans. American Journal of Psychiatry, 151 (6), 902-907.

Marron, K. (1988). Satanic cults. Ritual abuse - Canada's most infamous trial on child abuse (pp.

101-118). Canada: McLellan-Bantum Inc.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 23

Maxmen, J. S. & Ward, N. G. (1994). Essential psychopathology and its treatment (2nd ed.:

Rev. for DSM-IV). New York, New York: W. W. Norton & Company.

McCulley, D. (1995). Satanic ritual abuse: A question of memory. Journal of Psychology and

Theology, 22 (3), 167-172.

Merskey, H. (1992). The manufacture of personalities: The production of multiple personality

disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 327-340.

Modestin, J. (1992). multiple personality disorder in Switzerland. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 149 (1), 88-92.

Modlin, C. T. (1992). Conduct and antisocial personality disorder: II. Treatment [Abstract].

Medical Hypnoanalysis Journal, 7 (1), 20-30.

Morgan, J. & Zedner, L. (1992). Child victims: crime, impact and criminal justice. Oxford:

Clarendon Press.

Mulhern, S. A. (1992). Defining a syndrome versus defending a belief. Journal of Psychology

and Theology, 20 (3), 230-232.

North, C. S., Ryall, J. M., Ricci, D. A. & Wetzel, R. D. (1993). Multiple Personalities, Multiple

Disorders - Psychiatric classification and media influence. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

Orne, M.T., Soskis, D. A. Dinges, D. F., Orne, E. C. & Tonry, M. H. (1985). Hypnotically

refreshed testimony: Enhanced memory or tampering with evidence? Washington, DC: U.S. Dept. of

Justice, National Institute of Justice.

Oesterreich, T. K. (1974). Possession demoniacal and other. Secaucus: Citadel Press.

Office of Criminal Justice Planning. (1989-1990, Winter). Research Update: occult crime: law

enforcement primer. (Rep. no. 1: 6). Sacramento, CA.

Piper, A. (1994a). Multiple personality disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164 (5), 600-612.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 24

Piper, A. (1994b). Treatment for multiple personallity disorder: At what cost? American

Journal of Psychotherapy, 48, (3), 392-400.

Powell, R. A. & Boer, D. P. (1994). Did Freud mislead patients to confabulate memories and

abuse? Psychological Reports, 74, 1283-1298.

Prasad, A. (1985). Multiple personality syndrome. British Journal of Hospital Medicine, 34, 301-

303.

Putnam, F.W. (1989). Diagnosis and treatment of multiple personality disorder.

New York: The Guilford Press.

Putnam, F.W. (1992). Altered states - peeling away the layers of a multiple personality. The

Sciences, 32 (6), 30-37.

Putnam, F. W., Helmers, K. & Trickett, P. K. (1993). Development, reliability, and validity of a

child dissociation scale. Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 731-741.

Putnam, F. W. & Loewenstein, R.J. (1993). Treatment of multiple personality disorder: A

survey of current practices. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150 (7), 1048-1052.

Ritual Abuse Task Force. (September 15, 1989). Ritual abuse: definitions, glassory, the use of

mind control. Los Angeles, CA: County Commission for Women. [Hall of Administration, 500 W.

Temple St., Los Angeles, CA 90012.]

Rogers, M. L. (1992). A Call for discernment-natural and spiritual: An introductory editorial to

a special issue on SRA. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 20 (3), 175-176.

Rogo, D. S. (1987). The infinite boundary: A psychic look at spirit possession, madness, and

Multiple Personality. New York, NY: Dodd, Mead & Company.

Rosenbaum, M. (1980). The role of the term schizophrenia in the decline of diagnoses of

Multiple Personality. Archives of General Psychiatry, 37, 1383-1385.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 25

Rosik, C. H. (1995). The misdiagnosis of multiple Personality disorder by Christian counsellors:

Vulnerabilities and safeguards. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 23 (2), 75-88.

Ross, C. A. (1989). Multiple personality disorder: Diagnosis, clinical features, and treatment.

New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Ross, C. A. (1990). Twelve cognitive errors about multiple personality disorder. American

Journal of Psychotherapy,44 (3), 348-356.

Ross, C. A. (1991). Epidemiology of multiple personality disorder and dissociation. Psychiatric

Clinics of North America, 14, 503-517.

Ross, C. A., Anderson, G., Fleisher, W. P. & Norton, G. R. (1991). The frequency of multiple

personality disorder among psychiatric inpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry,148 (12), 1717-

1720.

Ross, C. A., Heber, S. Norton, G. R. & Anderson, B. A. (1989). Differences between multiple

personality disorder and other diagnostic groups on structured interview. Journal of Nervous and Mental

Disease, 179 (8), 487-491.

Ross, C. A., Miller, S. D., Reagor, P., Bjornson, M A., Fraser, G. A. & Anderson, G. (1990).

Structured interview data on 102 cases of multiple personality disorder from four centers. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 147 (5), 596-601.

Ross, C. A. & Norton, G. R. (1989). Suicide and parasuicide in multiple personality disorder.

Psychiatry, 52 (3), 365-371.

Ross, C. A., Norton, G R, & Wozney, K. (1989) Multiple personality disorder: An analysis of

236 cases. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 34, 413-418.

Russell, J. (1981). A history of witchcraft. London: Thames and Huson.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 26

Ryder, D. (1992). Breaking the circle of Satanic Ritual Abuse - Recognizing and recovering

from the hidden trauma. Minneapolis, MN: CompCare Publishers.

Saks, E. R. (1995). The criminal responsibility of people with multiple personality disorder

[Special Issue: Research in mental health and law]. Psychiatric Quarterly, 66 (2), 119-131.

Sanders, B. & Becker-Lausen, E. (1995). The measurement of psychological maltreatment:

Early data on the Child Abuse and Trauma Scale. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19 (3), 315-323.

Sanders, B. & Giolas, M. H. (1991). Dissociation and childhood trauma in psychologically

disturbed adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148 (1), 50-54.

Sarbin, T. R. (1995) On the belief that one body may be host to two or more personalities.

International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 43 (2), 163-183.

Saxe, G. N., van der Kolk, B. A., Berkowitz, R., Chinman, G., Hall, K., Lieberg, G. & Schwartz,

J. (1993). Dissociative disorders in psychiatric inpatients. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150 (7), 1037-

1042.

Shaffer, R. & Cozolino, L. J. (1992). Adults who report childhood ritualistic abuse. Journal of

Psychology and Theology, 20 (3), 188-193.

Spanos, N. P. (1994). Multiple identity enactments and multiple personality disorder: A

sociocognitive perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 143-165.

Spanos, N. P. & Burgess, C. (1994). Hypnosis and multiple personality disorder: A

sociocognitive perspective. In S. J. Lynn & J. W. Rhue (Eds.) Dissociation: Clinical and Theoretical

Perspectives (pp. 136-155). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Spanos, N. P., Weekes, E. M. & Bertrand, L. D. (1985). Multiple Personality: A social

psychological perspective. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 94, 362-376.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 27

Spanos, N. P., Weekes, E. M., Menary, E., & Bertrand, L. D. (1986). Hypnotic interview and age

regression in the elicitation of multiple personality symptoms: A simulation study. Psychiatry, 49, 298-

311.

Spiegel, D. (1988). The treatment accorded those who treat patients with multiple personality

disorder [Commentary]. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 176, 535-536.

Spiegel, D. & Cardena, E. (1990). New uses of hypnosis in the treatment of posttraumatic stress

disorder. Paper presented at the 143rd Annual Meeting of the American Psychiatric Association. New

York, NY.

Spiegel, D. & Cardena, E. (1991) Disintegrated experience: The dissociative disorders revisited.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 366-378.

Steinberg, M., Rounsaville, B. & Ciccheti, D.V. (1990). The Structured Clinical Interview for

DSM-III-R Dissociative Disorders: Preliminary report on a new diagnostic interview. American Journal

of Psychiatry, 147 (1), 76-82.

Steinberg, M., Rounsaville, B. & Ciccheti, D.V. (1991). Detection of Dissociative disorders in

psychiatric patients by a screening instrument and a structured diagnostic interview. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 148 (8), 1050-1054. .

Stolzenburg, W. (1990). Interviews unmask multiple personalities. Science News, 137 (20), 25-

28.

Toland, K., Hoffman, H., & Lofuts, E. (1991). How suggestion plays tricks with memory. In

J.F. Schumaker (Ed.) Human Suggestibility ( pp. 235-252). New York, NY: Routledge, Chapman &

Hall. van der Kolk, B. A. (1994). The body keeps the score: Memory and the evolving psychobiology

of post traumatic stress. Harvard Review of Psychiatry,1, 254.

A Diagnostic Puzzle 28

van der Kolk, B. A., Perry, J. C. & Herman, J. L. (1991). Childhood origins of self-destructive

behaviour. American Journal of Psychiatry, 148, 1665-1671.

van der Kolk, B. A., Roth, S. & Pelcovitz, D. (1992). Field trials for DSM-IV, post-traumatic

stress disorder II: Disorders of extreme stress. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

van der Kolk, B. A. & Van der Hart, O. (1991). The intrusive past: The flexibility of memory

and the engraving of trauma. American Imago, 48, 428-431.

Van der Hart, O. & Witztum, E. (1993). From hysterical psychosis to reactive dissociative

psychosis. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6 (1), 43-64.

Wagner, E.E., Allison, R.B. & Wagner, C.F. (1983). Diagnosing Multiple Personalities with the

Rorschach: A confirmation. Journal of Personality Assessment, 47 (2), 143-149.

Wagner. E.E. & Heise, M.R. (1974). A comparison of Rorschach records of three Multiple

Personalities. Journal of Personality Assessment, 38 (4), 308-331.

Wakefield, H. & Underwager, R. (1992). Recovered memories of alleged sexual abuse:

Lawsuits against parents. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 10, 483-507.

Watkins, J. G. (1984). The Bianchi (L.A. Hillside Strangler) case: Sociopath of multiple

personality? The International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 32 (2), 67-101.

Wells, Mary. (1990). Canada's law on child sex abuse: A handbook. Canada: Minister of Supply

and Services Canada.