Tractography of the corticospinal tracts in infants with focal perinatal injury: comparison with...

336
The printing of this thesis was financially supported by: Abbott, Chiesi, Eurocept, Nestlé Nutrition, Nutricia Nederland, Novo Nordisk, the Research School of Behavioural and Cognitive Neurosciences, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen, Universitair Medisch Centrum Groningen. Functional development at school age of newborn infants at risk. © Copyright 2011, E. Roze, The Netherlands All rights reserved. No part of this thesis may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, without the written permission from the author or, when appropriate, from the publishers of the publications. ISBN: 978-90-367-5169-8 ISBN electronic version: 978-90-367-5168-1 Cover design and Layout: Stephan Eikens, Leeuwarden Printing: Drukkerij Ridderprint, Ridderkerk

Transcript of Tractography of the corticospinal tracts in infants with focal perinatal injury: comparison with...

The printing of this thesis was financially supported by: Abbott, Chiesi, Eurocept,

Nestlé Nutrition, Nutricia Nederland, Novo Nordisk, the Research School

of Behavioural and Cognitive Neurosciences, Rijksuniversiteit Groningen,

Universitair Medisch Centrum Groningen.

Functional development at school age of newborn infants at risk.

© Copyright 2011, E. Roze, The Netherlands

All rights reserved. No part of this thesis may be reproduced, stored in a

retrieval system, or transmitted in any form or by any means, without the written

permission from the author or, when appropriate, from the publishers of the

publications.

ISBN: 978-90-367-5169-8

ISBN electronic version: 978-90-367-5168-1

Cover design and Layout: Stephan Eikens, Leeuwarden

Printing: Drukkerij Ridderprint, Ridderkerk

RIJKSUNIVERSITEIT GRONINGEN

Functional Development at School Age of

Newborn Infants at Risk

Proefschrift

ter verkrijging van het doctoraat in de

Medische Wetenschappen

aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen

op gezag van de

Rector Magnificus, dr. E. Sterken,

in het openbaar te verdedigen op

woensdag 7 december 2011

om 12:45 uur

door

Elise Roze

geboren op 12 december 1985

te Kampen

Promotor(es): Prof. Dr. A.F. Bos (UMC Groningen)

Beoordelingscommissie: Prof. Dr. P.J.J. Sauer (UMC Groningen)

Prof. Dr. L.S. de Vries (UMC Utrecht)

Prof. Dr. F.J. Walther (UMC Leiden)

Paranimfen: Petra Hoen

Elise Verhagen

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Chapter 1 General introduction and outline of the thesis

Part 1 Functional outcome of infants exposed prenatally to

environmental pollutants

Chapter 2 Prenatal exposure to organohalogens, including

brominated flame retardants, influences motor,

cognitive, and behavioral performance at school age

Environmental Health Perspectives 2009;117(12):1953-1958

Part 2 Functional outcome of newborn infants with brain

injury

Chapter 3 Risk factors for adverse outcome in preterm infants with

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

Pediatrics 2008;122(1):e46-e52

Chapter 4 Functional outcome at school age of preterm infants

with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

Pediatrics 2009;123(6):1493-1500

Chapter 5 Long-term neurological outcome of term-born children

treated with two or more anti-epileptic drugs in the

neonatal period

Early Human Development 2011; DOI 10.1016/

j.earlhumdev.2011.06.012

Chapter 6 Tractography of the corticospinal tracts in infants with

focal perinatal injury: comparison with normal controls

and to motor development

Neuroradiology 2011; accepted

Part 3 Functional outcome of newborn infants with

systemic diseases

11

31

33

59

61

83

107

133

167

Table of contents

Chapter 7 Functional impairments at school age of children with

necrotizing enterocolitis or spontaneous intestinal

perforation

Pediatric Research 2011; DOI 10.1203/

PDR.0b013e31823279b1

Chapter 8 Functional impairments at school age of preterm born

children with late-onset sepsis

Early Human Development 2011; DOI 10.1016/j.

earlhumdev.2011.06.008

Chapter 9 Motor and cognitive outcome at school age of children

with surgically treated intestinal obstructions in the

neonatal period

Submitted

Part 4 Functional outcome: how various aspects of

neurodevelopment interrelate

Chapter 10 Developmental trajectories from birth to school age in

healthy term-born children

Pediatrics 2010;126(5):e1134-e1142

Chapter 11 Neuropsychological profiles at school age of very

preterm born children compared to term-born peers

Submitted

Chapter 12 General discussion and future perspectives

Chapter 13 Summary in English

Nederlandse samenvatting

Dankwoord

About the author

List of publications

Abbreviations

Authors and affiliations

169

195

217

235

237

263

287

308

314

322

326

328

332

336

Chapter 1

GENERAL INTRODUCTION

AND OUTLINE OF THE THESIS

ELISE ROZE

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

The main goal of this thesis is to establish the long-term, school age outcome of

newborn infants with perinatal risk factors of adverse outcome. Early in the life

of newborn infants major steps in brain development take place. These form the

basis of ongoing neuronal ripening and outgrowth that continues from childhood

well into adolescence.1 The organization of brain structures throughout this

period are important for the child’s motor, cognitive, and behavioral functioning

later in life. Several environmental aspects during prenatal and postnatal life may

interfere with developmental processes in a child’s brain. To various degrees these

may have an impact on long-term development. On the one hand, persistent

environmental pollutants and deficiencies of nutritional components such as

long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, to which the whole population is more or

less exposed, are known to have mild effects on the development of the central

nervous system. On the other hand, preterm birth and neonatal disease, both of

which occur less frequently, have a much greater impact on neurodevelopment.

Environmental Pollutants

Prenatal exposure to environmental pollutants may lead to less optimal conditions

for brain development in fetal life. Examples of environmental pollutants are

organohalogens, used extensively as flame retardants as well as in other

industrial applications. During pregnancy, these compounds are transferred

across the placenta to the fetus. During this critical period for fetal growth and

development, these compounds may interfere with brain development by

disrupting neurotransmitters and interfering with endocrine systems.2,3 Animal

studies indicated that prenatal exposure to different brominated flame retardants

may cause long-lasting behavioral alterations, particularly in motor activity and

cognitive behavior.4,5 To date, the long-term effects of prenatal exposure to

environmental pollutants in humans have not been investigated. We hypothesized

that brominated flame retardants may have subtle effects on motor, cognitive, and

behavioral outcome in children at school age.

13Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

Preterm Birth

Preterm birth, a relative common complication of pregnancy, has a considerable

impact on the neurodevelopment of newborn infants. Overall, 8% to 12% of all births

occur before 37 weeks of gestation. Very preterm births (<32 weeks of gestation)

occur in around 1% to 2% of pregnancies. Over the past decades, advances in

neonatal intensive care have led to a decrease in the mortality of infants born

preterm. As a consequence, more and more surviving children of very low birth

weight, i.e. below 1500 grams, now participate in everyday life, school, and reach

adulthood. Throughout their lives, however, the long-term neurodevelopmental

impairments following preterm birth remain a significant problem.

Perinatal and Neonatal Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome

Preterm infants are exposed to various extra-uterine characteristics that may

interfere with developmental processes that would normally take place in utero.

They may, for example, lead to altered oxygen delivery to body and brain tissue,

changes in cerebral blood flow and metabolism, and altered nutritional delivery.

Changes in these physiological processes may lead to less optimal conditions

for brain development and may contribute to the risk of brain damage. Indeed,

low gestational age and low birth weight are well known risk factors for adverse

neurodevelopmental outcome.6

In addition, several diseases of the neonatal period have been identified as risk

factors for neurodevelopmental impairments among preterm infants. These

diseases can be classified into overt types of brain injury that directly affect the

structure and organization of the brain, or systemic diseases of which one would

not in itself expect a direct influence on brain development, even though these

diseases are associated with adverse neurodevelopmental outcome.

Overt Brain Injury in the Newborn Period

Overt, focal types of brain injury typically found in preterm infants include germinal

matrix hemorrhages-intraventricular hemorrhages and lesions affecting white

matter such as periventricular leukomalacia and periventricular hemorrhagic

infarction (Figure 1).7 Perinatal ischemic stroke is another type of brain lesion that

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

is found mainly in fullterm infants. This lesion is characterized by focal disruption

of cerebral blood flow secondary to either arterial or venous thrombosis or

embolization (Figure 1). These different types of brain injury are associated with

the development of cerebral palsy.8-10

Figure 1. Cranial ultrasound scans of an infant with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction in (a) a coronal and (b) sagittal plane, and (c) Magnetic Resonance Images of an infant with perinatal ischemic stroke at the cortical level and (d) the level of the basal ganglia

15Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

Systemic Diseases in the Newborn Period

Systemic diseases in preterm infants that may arise in the neonatal period

include sepsis, necrotizing enterocolitis, and bronchopulmonary dysplasia

following prolonged ventilatory support. Although these infants do not often

exhibit abnormalities on cranial ultrasound scans, they still have adverse

neurodevelopmental outcome at 2 years of age.11-13 Various authors suggested that

inflammation may lead to more diffuse types of brain damage in these infants.14-

16 The effect of these diseases on long-term motor, cognitive, and behavioral

functioning is unknown.

Long-term Outcome

While the rates of major handicaps among preterm-born children have remained

relatively constant over the last decade, the prevalence of milder dysfunctions

seem to be increasing. Cognitive, behavioral, and mild motor problems without

major motor deficits are now the most dominant neurodevelopmental sequelae in

children born preterm.17,18

Motor Outcome

One of the most well-recognized motor impairments following preterm birth is

cerebral palsy (CP). This permanent, non-progressive neurological condition occurs

in around 5% to 10% of very low birth weight infants.19,20 It involves disorders of

movement, posture, and motor function and may lead to limitations in performing

daily activities. Gross motor functioning in children with CP and the impact on

everyday routines can be assessed with the Gross Motor Function Classification

System (GMFCS). This is a functional, five level classification system for CP based

on self-initiated movement with particular emphasis on sitting (truncal control) and

walking.21 Children with CP can present with additional neurosensory deficits such

as deafness, visual impairment, and epilepsy. Rates of CP are found to decrease

over time (Figure 2). In a recent study from the Netherlands the incidence of CP was

found to have decreased to 2.2% in preterm infants born <34 weeks of gestation

in 2005.22

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Error bars represent the standard error. Adapted from Platt MJ et al.23

Nevertheless, many preterm children who do not develop CP may still present

with impaired motor skills. A recent meta-analysis on the prevalence of motor-skill

impairment in preterm children without CP reported a pooled estimate of 40.5%

for mild to moderate impairment (defined as 5th to 15th percentile on standardized

tests) and 19.0% for moderate impairment (defined as < 5th percentile).24 The latter is

frequently found in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD).

Intellectual and Neuropsychological Outcome

Cognitive problems are increasingly recognized among preterm-born children without

major handicaps.25 Previous studies reported intelligence quotients that were 4 to 10

points lower in preterm children compared to fullterm controls. This equals a decline

of 0.3 to 0.6 standard deviations.25-27 In addition, higher percentages of preterm-born

children have borderline IQ scores (i.e. IQs between 70 and 85) with a prevalence of

15% to 37% compared to their term-born peers.28,29 In addition, in a meta-analysis

of 1556 cases, Bhutta et al. showed that there is an inverse relationship of both birth

weight and gestational age with intellectual development in preterm-born children at

school age.6 Figure 3 shows that especially in the lower gestational age ranges there

is a steep decline in intellectual functioning.

1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 Birth year

120

100

80

60

40

20

0

Gestational age 28-31 weeksGestational <28 weeks

Birt

h p

reva

lenc

e p

er 1

000

live

birt

hs

Figure 2 Gestational-age-specific birth prevalence of cerebral palsy (3-year moving average), from nine European centers, 1981–1995

17Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

The figure is based on a meta-analysis by H. Lagercrantz of studies by D. Wolke and N. Marlow.30

The cognitive impairments found in preterm children are not restricted to

a poorer intellectual development detected as lower intelligence. Specific

neuropsychological functions like attention, visuomotor integration, and executive

functioning can also be affected.27,31,32 These functional impairments are likely

to have an impact on daily life and academic performance.33-35 These so-called ‘high

prevalence, low severity’ impairments occur in more than 50% of children born preterm

with very low birth weights, and often do not occur in isolation.18 Their pathogenesis is

still largely unclear, although a lower gestational age is a prominent factor.

Behavioral Outcome

Behavioral problems that frequently occur in preterm-born children are attention deficit

hyperactivity problems, emotional problems, and autism spectrum disorders.17,36 It has

been estimated that between 13% to 30% of preterm-born children have behavioral

problems in childhood and adolescence.37-39 A meta-analysis by Bhutta et al. using

120

110

100

90

80

70

60

50

n=

24 27 39 9273 23 67144 103 397 917162 415414 289252 465348 100

Mean ± SEM

Bavarian Study 4.8y

EPICure Study 6y

adjusted for comparison GCA

22 24 26 28 30 32 34 36 38 40 42Gestational age (w)

Kau

fman

-AB

C M

PC

Figure 3 The relationship between intelligence quotient (IQ) and gestational age

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

formal diagnostic criteria (the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual), found a pooled

relative risk of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder of 2.64 for preterm-born children

compared to controls.6

Children’s competencies and behavioral and emotional problems can be assessed

by a parental questionnaire, the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL).40 A study by

Reijneveld et al. showed that around 13% of preterm-born children without overt

brain lesions have behavioral problems that can be detected with the CBCL.41 Both

internalizing and externalizing problems were found, with social and attentional

behavior most frequently affected. The exact pathophysiology of behavioral disorders

among preterm-born children is unclear, but an interaction between neurobiological

vulnerability and environmental aspects seems likely.42 The role of specific neonatal

morbidities in behavioral problems remain to be elucidated.

Functional Outcome at School Age

When performing follow-up studies to determine the effect of disease in early life on

long-term outcome, it is important to think about which tests to use and at what age

certain skills can best be assessed. We were particularly interested in the functional

outcome of children, i.e. their motor, cognitive, and behavioral functioning. The aim of

functional assessment is to determine a child’s ability to perform essential everyday

tasks and to fulfill the social roles expected of a physically and emotionally healthy

individual of the same age and culture.43

Regarding the child’s age at testing, school age is a more reliable age on which

to base a prediction on functioning in later life and adulthood than, for example,

toddler age. It is often not until school age that impairments in motor, cognitive, and

behavioral functioning come to light. The reason being that at school age functional

demands are higher than at younger ages. Also, from 6 years of age onwards, a wider

variety of tests are available and more precise assessment of attention and school

achievement is possible. Previously existing problems, not apparent earlier, become

evident at school age when functions involving these deficits are challenged.44 There

is growing evidence of an increased incidence of cognitive and functional disability

among preterm-born children at school age.17,45 Moreover, at school age academic

achievements can also be measured and later correlated to outcome in adulthood.

19Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

How Various Aspects of Neurodevelopment Interrelate

The motor, cognitive, and behavioral problems following preterm birth often do not

occur in isolation. We know, for example, that children with cerebral palsy are at

increased risk of cognitive and behavioral impairments, which may be associated

with the specific types of brain injury they suffered.46,47 In children with ‘high

prevalence, low severity’ impairments, however, these relationships are less clear.

Since most studies on neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm infants describe

group means on motor, cognitive, and behavioral tests, we know little about the

co-occurrence of impairments in functions among individual children. Regarding

neuropsychological functions, it is unclear how many preterm-born children suffer

from impairments in one or multiple domains and how neuropsychological functions

interrelate. Some studies reported that specifically preterm children with low

intelligence are at risk of developing problems in attention, memory, and executive

functions,48,49 while other studies suggested there may be preterm children with

specific neuropsychological dysfunctions in whom intellectual development is

preserved.50,51

Another issue is that of the stability of test scores obtained from birth until

school age. The majority of outcome studies of preterm-born children consider

neurodevelopmental outcome at two years of age assessed with the Bayley Scales

of Infant Development, as a measure for long-term development. We know that in

high-risk infants, such as extreme low birth weight infants, early neurological tests

scores have a strong predictive value for functioning later in life. Less is known,

however, about the stability of developmental trajectories in children with only a

mild risk of adverse outcome, or in healthy, term-born children.

Prediction of Outcome in the Early Period

In order to guide treatment and to counsel parents we need predictors of

neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm and ill fullterm infants for the neonatal

period. In addition, early identification of infants at risk of mild to moderate motor,

cognitive, and behavioral impairments could lead to early implementation of

individual intervention strategies in order to improve long-term outcome.

Knowledge of the effects of specific neonatal diseases on long-term outcome

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

may contribute to making reliable prognoses in the neonatal period. In addition,

neonatal assessment methods such as the Nursery Neurobiological Risk Score

(NBRS) and the Score for Neonatal Acute Physiology (SNAP), both of which aim

to summarize illness severity in newborn infants in their first days to weeks of

life, may also contribute to reliable estimates of neurodevelopmental outcome.

Although NBRS scores in relation to long-term outcome have never been studied,

previous studies did show that higher NBRS scores are associated with mental,

motor, and neurological impairments at 2 years of age.52

In addition, several techniques are available to assess the integrity of the central

nervous system of newborn infants shortly after birth. These include neuroimaging

techniques such as cranial ultrasound (CUS) and magnetic resonance imaging

(MRI), the observation and assessment of the quality of spontaneous general

movements, and the neonatal neurological examination.

The increased use of CUS and MRI in the neonatal period has led to an increased

understanding of common cerebral pathologies in newborn infants and global brain

development after preterm birth. Global white matter damage revealed by CUS and

MRI is quite common in children born preterm, and relations with motor function

have been found in the short-term.53,54 To date, clear associations of cognition

with pathological changes on neuroimaging have not been demonstrated beyond

doubt.55 This also holds for mild motor impairments in the long-term.

New imaging techniques may provide insight into microstructural changes in preterm

brain development. One such technique, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), studies

the diffusion properties of water among different regions of the brain. Quantitative

measures derived from DTI provide an objective and reproducible assessment of

white matter and provides insights into neonatal brain development and injury.56-59

These quantitative measures include the apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC), a

measure of the overall magnitude of water diffusion, and fractional anisotropy (FA)

(the fraction of diffusion that can be attributed to anisotropic diffusion). Additionally,

DTI allows us to visualize white matter tracts such as the corticospinal tract in-vivo.

In diffusion tractography it is assumed that the direction of greatest diffusion in an

imaging voxel is parallel to the underlying dominant fibre orientation. By following

this direction of greatest diffusion on a voxel by voxel basis, it is possible to

21Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

generate three dimensional reconstructions of white matter tracts. This technique

allows the quantitative assessment of white matter tracts in regions of relatively

low FA such as in unmyelinated white matter in the neonatal brain.60,61 As yet, the

prognostic value of diffusion characteristics of the corticospinal tract for motor

outcome in newborn infants with focal brain injury is unknown.

Aims of the thesis

Our main aim was to establish the motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome of

newborn infants with perinatal risk factors for adverse outcome at school age. Our

secondary aims were two-fold:

I. to investigate the interrelationship between motor, cognitive, and

neuropsychological development up to school age in preterm- born infants

compared to healthy, fullterm infants and

II. to relate perinatal and cerebral characteristics of newborn infants to long-

term neurodevelopmental outcome.

Part 1. Functional outcome of infants exposed prenatally to environmental

pollutants

In Part 1 we describe the outcome at school age of healthy Dutch children

who were exposed prenatally to environmental pollutants. In Chapter 2 we

describe the influence of prenatal exposure to organohalogen compounds,

including widely used brominated flame retardants, on motor, cognitive,

and behavioral outcome in healthy term-born children at school age.

Part 2. Functional outcome of newborn infants with brain injury

Part 2 of the thesis reviews the outcome of newborn infants with

different types of brain injury and assesses the relation between cerebral

characteristics and outcome. In Chapter 3 we determine risk factors

for adverse outcome (i.e. mortality and motor outcome at 18 months

of age) in preterm infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. In

Chapter 4 we establish the functional outcome at school age, i.e. the

motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome, of preterm-born children with

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. We also describe cerebral risk

factors for adverse outcome. In Chapter 5 we describe the neurological

outcome of fullterm infants with neonatal seizures who required two or

more anti-epileptic drugs. We also determine the prognostic value of

treatment efficacy and seizure etiology for outcome. In Chapter 6 we

describe a novel technique, DTI, to assess diffusion characteristics of the

corticospinal tracts in newborn infants with focal neonatal ischemic brain

lesions, and the relation between conventional MRI and tractography

findings with later motor function in these infants.

Part 3. Functional outcome of newborn infants with systemic diseases

Part 3 reviews the functional outcome of preterm and term-born infants

with systemic diseases in the neonatal period and the role of inflammation

on development at school age. In Chapters 7 and 8 we describe functional

impairments in motor, cognitive, and behavioral development at school

age of preterm-born children with necrotizing enterocolitis, spontaneous

intestinal perforation and sepsis in which inflammation played a key role.

In Chapter 9 we establish the outcome at school age of newborn infants

with intestinal obstructions treated surgically during their first days of life.

In this intestinal condition inflammation does not play a prominent role.

Part 4. Functional outcome: How various aspects of neurodevelopment

interrelate

The final part of the thesis focuses on developmental processes and the

association between various developmental domains in preterm-born and

term-born children. In Chapter 10 we describe developmental trajectories

of healthy term-born children until school age. More specifically, we

determine the stability of scores on motor development tests from birth

until school age, and we determine the added value of the scores on early

motor tests for complex cognitive functions at school age. In Chapter 11

we establish the neuropsychological profiles at school age of a cohort of

very preterm-born children compared to term-born controls. In addition, we

23Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

describe their relation with academic performance, and identify neonatal

characteristics related to the neuropsychological profiles of preterm-born

children at school age.

In Chapter 12 we provide a general discussion of our findings and some

future perspectives. In Chapter 13 we summarize the findings set out in

this thesis in English and Dutch.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Sowell ER, Trauner DA, Gamst A, Jernigan TL.

Development of cortical and subcortical brain

structures in childhood and adolescence: a structural

MRI study. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;44:4-16.

2. Solomon GM, Schettler T. Environment and health:

6. Endocrine disruption and potential human health

implications. CMAJ 2000;163:1471-1476.

3. Weisglas-Kuperus N. Neurodevelopmental,

immunological and endocrinological indices of

perinatal human exposure to PCBs and dioxins.

Chemosphere 1998;37:1845-1853.

4. Gee JR, Moser VC. Acute postnatal exposure to

brominated diphenylether 47 delays neuromotor

ontogeny and alters motor activity in mice.

Neurotoxicol Teratol 2008;30:79-87.

5. Viberg H, Fredriksson A, Eriksson P. Neonatal

exposure to polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE

153) disrupts spontaneous behaviour, impairs

learning and memory, and decreases hippocampal

cholinergic receptors in adult mice. Toxicol Appl

Pharmacol 2003;192:95-106.

6. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM,

Anand KJS. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of

school-aged children who were born preterm - A

meta-analysis. JAMA 2002;288:728-737.

7. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the Newborn. 5th ed.

Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier; 2008.

8. de Vries LS, Rademaker KJ, Groenendaal F, et al.

Correlation between neonatal cranial ultrasound,

MRI in infancy and neurodevelopmental outcome

in infants with a large intraventricular haemorrhage

with or without unilateral parenchymal involvement.

Neuropediatrics 1998;29:180-188.

9. Bass WT, Jones MA, White LE, Montgomery TR,

Aiello F, Karlowicz MG. Ultrasonographic differential

diagnosis and neurodevelopmental outcome of

cerebral white matter lesions in premature infants. J

Perinatol 1999;19:330-336.

10. Mercuri E, Rutherford M, Cowan F, et al. Early

prognostic indicators of outcome in infants

with neonatal cerebral infarction: a clinical,

electroencephalogram, and magnetic resonance

imaging study. Pediatrics 1999;103:39-46.

11. Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Adams-Chapman I, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and growth impairment among

extremely low-birth-weight infants with neonatal

infection. JAMA 2004;292:2357-2365.

12. Hintz SR, Kendrick DE, Stoll BJ, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of

extremely low birth weight infants after necrotizing

enterocolitis. Pediatrics 2005;115:696-703.

References

25Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

13. Schmidt B, Asztalos EV, Roberts RS, Robertson

CM, Sauve RS, Whitfield MF. Impact of

bronchopulmonary dysplasia, brain injury, and

severe retinopathy on the outcome of extremely

low-birth-weight infants at 18 months: results from

the trial of indomethacin prophylaxis in preterms.

JAMA 2003;289:1124-1129.

14. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by

white matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance

imaging at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

15. Glass HC, Bonifacio SL, Chau V, et al. Recurrent

postnatal infections are associated with progressive

white matter injury in premature infants. Pediatrics

2008;122:299-305.

16. Gagliardi L, Bellu R, Zanini R, Dammann O.

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia and brain white

matter damage in the preterm infant: a complex

relationship. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol

2009;23:582-590.

17. Aylward GP. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of

infants born prematurely. J Dev Behav Pediatr

2005;26:427-440.

18. Johnson S. Cognitive and behavioural outcomes

following very preterm birth. Sem Fetal Neonatal

Med 2007;12:363-373.

19. Hack M, Flannery DJ, Schluchter M, Cartar L,

Borawski E, Klein N. Outcomes in young adulthood

for very-low-birth-weight infants. N Engl J Med

2002;346:149-157.

20. Himmelmann K, Hagberg G, Uvebrant P. The

changing panorama of cerebral palsy in Sweden. X.

Prevalence and origin in the birth-year period 1999-

2002. Acta Paediatr 2010;99:1337-1343.

21. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D,

Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability

of a system to classify gross motor function in

children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

22. van Haastert IC, Groenendaal F, Uiterwaal CS, et al.

Decreasing incidence and severity of cerebral palsy

in prematurely born children. J Pediatr

2011;159:86-91.

23. Platt MJ, Cans C, Johnson A, et al. Trends in

cerebral palsy among infants of very low birthweight

(<1500 g) or born prematurely (<32 weeks) in

16 European centres: a database study. Lancet

2007;369:43-50.

24. Williams J, Lee KJ, Anderson PJ. Prevalence of

motor-skill impairment in preterm children who do

not develop cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev

Med Child Neurol 2010;52:232-237.

25. Aylward GP. Cognitive and neuropsychological

outcomes: more than IQ scores. Ment Retard Dev

Disabil Res Rev 2002;8:234-240.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

26. Taylor HG, Klein N, Minich NM, Hack M. Middle-

school-age outcomes in children with very low

birthweight. Child Dev 2000;71:1495-1511.

27. Van Braeckel KNJA, Butcher PR, Geuze RH,

van Duijn MA, Bos AF, Bouma A. Less efficient

elementary visuomotor processes in 7- to 10-year-

old preterm-born children without cerebral palsy:

an indication of impaired dorsal stream processes.

Neuropsychology 2008;22:755-764.

28. Whitfield MF, Grunau RV, Holsti L. Extremely

premature (< or = 800 g) schoolchildren: multiple

areas of hidden disability. Arch Dis Child Fetal

Neonatal Ed 1997;77:F85-F90.

29. Vohr BR, Wright LL, Dusick AM, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and functional outcomes of

extremely low birth weight infants in the National

Institute of Child Health and Human Development

Neonatal Research Network, 1993-1994. Pediatrics

2000;105:1216-1226.

30. Lagercrantz H. The hard problem. Acta Paediatr

2008;97:142-143.

31. Taylor HG. Children born preterm or with very low

birth weight can have both global and selective

cognitive deficits. J Dev Behav Pediatr

2006;27:485-486.

32. Mulder H, Pitchford NJ, Hagger MS, Marlow N.

Development of executive function and attention

in preterm children: a systematic review. Dev

Neuropsychol 2009;34:393-421.

33. Feder KP, Majnemer A, Bourbonnais D, Platt R,

Blayney M, Synnes A. Handwriting performance in

preterm children compared with term peers at age 6

to 7 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:163-170.

34. Saigal S, den Ouden L, Wolke D, et al. School-age

outcomes in children who were extremely low birth

weight from four international population-based

cohorts. Pediatrics 2003;112:943-950.

35. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Oosterlaan J, Duivenvoorden

HJ, van Goudoever JB, Weisglas-Kuperus N.

Development of preschool and academic skills in

children born very preterm. J Pediatr

2011;158:15-20.

36. Johnson S, Hollis C, Kochhar P, Hennessy E, Wolke

D, Marlow N. Psychiatric disorders in extremely

preterm children: longitudinal finding at age 11

years in the EPICure study. J Am Acad Child

Adolesc Psychiatry 2010;49:453-463.

37. Gray RF, Indurkhya A, McCormick MC. Prevalence,

stability, and predictors of clinically significant

behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3,

5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics 2004;114:736-743.

38. Botting N, Powls A, Cooke RW, Marlow N. Attention

deficit hyperactivity disorders and other psychiatric

outcomes in very low birthweight children at 12

years. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:931-941.

27Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

39. Elgen I, Sommerfelt K, Markestad T. Population

based, controlled study of behavioural problems

and psychiatric disorders in low birthweight children

at 11 years of age. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed

2002;87:F128-F132.

40. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the

Child Behavior Checklist: 4–18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: Univ. of Vermont, Department of

Psychiatry; 1991.

41. Reijneveld SA, de Kleine MJ, van Baar AL, et al.

Behavioural and emotional problems in very preterm

and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Arch

Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2006;91:F423-F428.

42. Bendersky M, Lewis M. Environmental risk,

biological risk, and developmental outcome.

Developmental Psychology 1994;30:484-494.

43. Vohr BR. Cognitive and functional outcomes of

children born preterm. In: Nosarti C, Murray RM,

Hack M, editors. Neurodevelopmental Outcomes

of Preterm Birth. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

University Press; 2010. p.141-163.

44. Aylward GP. Methodologcial considerations in

neurodevelopmental outcome sutdies of infants

born prematurely. In: Nosarti C, Murray RM,

Hack M, editors. Neurodevelopmental Outcomes

of Preterm Birth. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

University Press; 2010. p.164-175.

45. Hack M, Friedman H, Fanaroff AA. Outcomes

of extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics

1996;98:931-937.

46. Sigurdardottir S, Vik T. Speech, expressive

language, and verbal cognition of preschool children

with cerebral palsy in Iceland. Dev Med Child Neurol

2011;53:74-80.

47. Himmelmann K, Beckung E, Hagberg G, Uvebrant

P. Gross and fine motor function and accompanying

impairments in cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

2006;48:417-423.

48. Dewey D, Crawford SG, Creighton DE, Sauve RS.

Long-term neuropsychological outcomes in very

low birth weight children free of sensorineural

impairments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol

1999;21:851-865.

49. Herrgard E, Luoma L, Tuppurainen K, Karjalainen S,

Martikainen A. Neurodevelopmental profile at five

years of children born at < or = 32 weeks gestation.

Dev Med Child Neurol 1993;35:1083-1096.

50. Korkman M, Mikkola K, Ritari N, et al.

Neurocognitive test profiles of extremely low birth

weight five-year-old children differ according to

neuromotor status. Dev Neuropsychol

2008;33:637-655.

51. Grunau RE, Whitfield MF, Davis C. Pattern of

learning disabilities in children with extremely low

birth weight and broadly average intelligence. Arch

Pediatr Adolesc Med 2002;156:615-620.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

52. Brazy JE, Eckerman CO, Oehler JM, Goldstein RF,

O’Rand AM. Nursery Neurobiologic Risk Score:

Important factors in predicting outcome in very low

birth weight infants. J Pediatr 1991;118:783-792.

53. Spittle AJ, Brown NC, Doyle LW, et al. Quality

of general movements is related to white matter

pathology in very preterm infants. Pediatrics

2008;121:e1184-e1189.

54. Bos AF, Martijn A, Okken A, Prechtl HFR. Quality of

general movements in preterm infants with transient

periventricular echodensities. Acta Paediatr

1998;87:328-335.

55. Hart AR, Whitby EW, Griffiths PD, Smith MF.

Magnetic resonance imaging and developmental

outcome following preterm birth: review of current

evidence. Dev Med Child Neurol 2008;50:655-663.

56. Huppi PS, Maier SE, Peled S, et al. Microstructural

development of human newborn cerebral white

matter assessed in vivo by diffusion tensor magnetic

resonance imaging. Pediatr Res 1998;44:584-590.

57. Neil JJ, Shiran SI, McKinstry RC, et al. Normal

brain in human newborns: apparent diffusion

coefficient and diffusion anisotropy measured

by using diffusion tensor MR imaging. Radiology

1998;209:57-66.

58. Seghier ML, Lazeyras F, Zimine S, et al.

Combination of event-related fMRI and diffusion

tensor imaging in an infant with perinatal stroke.

Neuroimage 2004;21:463-472.

59. Ward P, Counsell S, Allsop J, et al. Reduced

fractional anisotropy on diffusion tensor magnetic

resonance imaging after hypoxic-ischemic

encephalopathy. Pediatrics 2006;117:e619-e630.

60. Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Woolrich MW, et

al. Non-invasive mapping of connections between

human thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging.

Nat Neurosci 2003;6:750-757.

61. Counsell SJ, Dyet LE, Larkman DJ, et al. Thalamo-

cortical connectivity in children born preterm

mapped using probabilistic magnetic resonance

tractography. Neuroimage 2007;34:896-904.

29Chapter 1 General Introduction and Outline of the Thesis

Chapter 2 Prenatal exposure to organohalogens, including brominated

flame retardants, influences motor, cognitive,

and behavioral performance at school age

PART 1Functional outcome of infants exposed prenatally

to environmental pollutants

Chapter 2

PRENATAL EXPOSURE TO ORGANOHALOGENS,

INCLUDING BROMINATED FLAME RETARDANTS,

INFLUENCES MOTOR, COGNITIVE, AND

BEHAVIORAL PERFORMANCE AT SCHOOL AGE

ELISE ROZE, LISETHE MEIJER, ATTIE BAKKER, KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL,

PIETER J. J. SAUER, AREND F. BOS

ENVIRONMENTAL HEALTH PERSPECTIVES 2009;117(12):1953-1958

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Background

Organohalogen compounds (OHCs) are known to have neurotoxic effects on the

developing brain.

Objective

We investigated the influence of prenatal exposure to OHCs, including brominated

flame retardants, on motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome in healthy children

of school age.

Methods

This study was part of the prospective Groningen infant COMPARE (Comparison

of Exposure-Effect Pathways to Improve the Assessment of Human Health

Risks of Complex Environmental Mixtures of Organohalogens) study. It included

62 children in whose mothers the following compounds had been determined

in the 35th week of pregnancy: 2,2´-bis-(4 chlorophenyl)-1,1´-dichloroethene,

pentachlorophenol (PCP), polychlorinated biphenyl congener 153 (PCB-153),

4-hydroxy-2,3,3´,4´,5-pentachlorobiphenyl (4OH-CB-107), 4OH-CB-146, 4OH-CB-

187, 2,2´,4,4´-tetrabromodiphenyl ether (BDE-47), BDE-99, BDE-100, BDE-153,

BDE-154, and hexabromocyclododecane. Thyroid hormones were determined

in umbilical cord blood. When the children were 5–6 years of age, we assessed

their neuropsychological functioning: motor performance (coordination, fine motor

skills), cognition (intelligence, visual perception, visuomotor integration, inhibitory

control, verbal memory, and attention), and behavior.

35Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

Results

Brominated flame retardants correlated with worse fine manipulative abilities,

worse attention, better coordination, better visual perception, and better behavior.

Chlorinated OHCs correlated with less choreiform dyskinesia. Hydroxylated

polychlorinated biphenyls correlated with worse fine manipulative abilities,

better attention, and better visual perception. The wood protective agent (PCP)

correlated with worse coordination, less sensory integrity, worse attention, and

worse visuomotor integration.

Conclusions

Our results demonstrate for the first time that transplacental transfer of

polybrominated flame retardants is associated with the development of children at

school age. Because of the widespread use of these compounds, especially in the

United States, where concentrations in the environment are four times higher than

in Europe, these results cause serious concern.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Introduction

Organohalogen compounds (OHCs) are toxic environmental pollutants used

extensively in pesticides, flame retardants, hydraulic fluids, and in other industrial

applications.1 They are ubiquitously present in the environment, both in neutral

and in phenolic form.2 OHCs are known to bioaccumulate because of their high

lipophilicity and resistance to degradation processes3 and have been detected

in human adipose tissue and blood.4 In pregnant women these compounds

are transferred across the placenta to the fetus.5,6 During this critical period of

fetal growth and development, there is a risk for damage of the central nervous

system because OHCs may interfere with developmental processes in the brain.

Some compounds have effects on neuronal and glial cell development and are

associated with disruption of neurotransmitters. Others interfere with endocrine

systems, such as thyroid and sex hormones.7,8 OHCs may also produce their toxic

effects through other pathways that are currently not well understood.

Previous studies in humans on the effect of prenatal OHC exposure on outcome

reported that polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) have adverse effects on neurologic

performance and cognitive development at 6–11 years of age.9-13 Knowledge of

the neurotoxicity of PCBs led to their abandonment in most Western countries in

the late 1970s. Despite this, metabolites of PCBs, the hydroxylated PCBs (OH-

PCBs), are still present in high concentrations in maternal serum.6,14 Previous

studies postulated that OH-PCBs are even more toxic to brain development

than are PCBs.15,16 The long-term effect of prenatal OH-PCB exposure on human

development is unknown.

Brominated flame retardants such as polybrominated biphenyls (PBBs) and

poly-brominated diphenyl ethers (PBDEs) were introduced as the new, allegedly

harmless, successors of PCBs. However, the effect of prenatal exposure to

brominated flame retar-dants on neurodevelopmental outcome at school age has

never been investigated.

37Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

The primary aim of this explorative study was to investigate the influence of

prenatal OHC exposure, including OH-PCBs and PBDEs, on motor, cognitive, and

behavioral outcomes in healthy Dutch children at 5–6 years of age.

OHCs are also known to influence fetal thyroid hormone levels.17 Because thyroid

hormones are involved in neurodevelopmental processes, our second aim was

to investigate whether thyroid hormone levels at birth were related to outcome in

these children.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Materials and Methods

Cohort Selection and Sampling

This prospective cohort study is part of the Groningen infant COMPARE

(Comparison of Exposure-Effect Pathways to Improve the Assessment of Human

Health Risks of Complex Environmental Mixtures of Organohalogens) (GIC) study

launched within the European COMPARE study. The cohort of the GIC study

consisted of 90 white, healthy pregnant women randomly selected from those

who had given birth to a healthy, full-term, singleton infant and lived in the northern

provinces of the Netherlands.6 All the women who had registered with midwives

between October 2001 and November 2002 in the province of Groningen were

invited to participate in the study.

To determine the concentrations of the neutral and phenolic OHCs, blood (30

mL) was taken from the women at the 35th week of pregnancy. The blood was

centrifuged at 3,600 rpm for 10 min, and the serum was collected and stored in

acetone-prewashed glass tubes at –20°C until analysis.

Chemical Analyses

Chlorinated OHCs [PCB-153 and 2,2´-bis-(4 chlorophenyl)-1,1´-dichloroethene

(4,4´-DDE)], OH-PCBs (4OH-CB-107, 4OH-CB-146, and 4OH-CB-187), and a

wood protective agent, pentachlorophenol (PCP), were analyzed in 90 serum

samples taken at the 35th week of pregnancy. Because of financial constraints,

brominated flame retardants [BDE-47, BDE-99, BDE-100, BDE-153, BDE-154,

and hexabromocyclododecane (HBCDD)] were analyzed in 69 randomly selected

serum samples taken at the 35th week of pregnancy. Mean levels of BDEs 47, 99,

and 100 measured in blank samples were subtracted from values measured in

study samples to correct for background exposures (4.8, 1.9, and 0.8 pg/g serum,

respectively). Samples that were below the limit of detection (LOD) for BDE-47

(n=2), BDE-99 (n=3), or BDE-100 (n=3) [0.08–0.16 pg/g serum]6 were assigned a

concentration of 0 for analyses. Chemical and lipid analyses were performed as

described elsewhere.6

39Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

Thyroid Hormone Analyses

Thyroxin (T4), free T4, reverse triiodothyronin (rT3), triiodothyronin (T3), thyroid-

stimulating hormone (TSH), and thyroid-binding globulin levels were determined in

the umbilical cord blood of the 90 women, provided that enough cord blood was

available to perform the analyses.

Follow-up

We intended to include the 69 children for whom all the neutral and phenolic OHC

concentrations had been determined. The children were invited prospectively to

participate in an extensive follow-up program that assessed motor performance,

cognition, and behavior at 5–6 years of age. Parents gave their informed consent

for themselves and their children to participate in the follow-up program before

the study. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the

University Medical Center Groningen and complied with all applicable international

regulations.

Motor Outcome

To determine the children’s motor outcomes, we administered the Movement

ABC, a standardized test of motor skills for children 4–12 years of age.18 This test,

which is widely used in practice and in research, yields a score for total movement

performance based on separate scores for manual dexterity (fine motor skills), ball

skills, and static and dynamic balance (coordination). Items on the Movement ABC

included, for example, posting coins in a bank box, drawing a line between two

existing lines of a figure, catching a bean bag, and jumping over a rope. The test

required 20–30 min to administer. The tasks that make up the Movement ABC are

representative of the motor skills that are required of children attending elementary

school and are adapted to the children’s ages.

Supplementary to the Movement ABC, we assessed qualitative aspects of

coordination and balance and fine manipulative abilities and the presence of

choreiform dyskinesia, associated movements, sensory integrity, and tremors

with Touwen’s age-specific neurologic examination.19 Approximately 20–30%

of children from the general population obtain nonoptimal scores on one or two

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

clusters of neurologic functions on Touwen’s neurologic examination. If a child’s

score is nonoptimal on a specific item of the examination, the total score can still

be within the normal range.20,21

Finally, we administered the Dutch version of the Developmental Coordination

Disorder Questionnaire (DCD-Q).22 This questionnaire, which is filled out by the

parents, was developed to identify motor problems in children ≥ 4 years of age.

It contains 17 items relating to motor coordination, which are classified into

three categories: control during movement, fine motor skills/writing, and general

coordination.

Cognitive Outcome

Total, Verbal, and Performance Intelligence levels were assessed using a short

form of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, revised (WPPSI-

R).23 Examples on items of the WPPSI-R are vocabulary, picture completion, and

reproduction of block designs.

In addition, we assessed several neuropsychological functions to investigate whether

these were impaired by prenatal OHC exposure. They were assessed by subtests

of the NEPSY-II (Neuropsychological Assessment, 2nd ed.), a neuropsychological

battery for children.24 Central visual perception was assessed using the “geometric

puzzles” subtest, in which the child is asked to match two shapes outside a grid with

shapes inside the grid. Visuomotor integration was assessed by the “design copying”

subtest, in which the child is asked to reproduce geometric forms of increasing

complexity. Visuomotor integration involves the integration of visual information

with finger–hand movements. Furthermore, we assessed inhibitory control with the

“inhibition” subtest, which assesses the inhibitory control of automated behavior.

In the first timed task, the child is asked to name a set of figures (i.e., squares and

circles); in the second timed task, the child is asked to name the opposite of what

is shown (i.e., squares instead of circles and circles instead of squares).

We assessed verbal memory using a standardized Dutch version of the Rey’s

Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).25 This test consists of five learning trials with

immediate recall of words (tested after each presentation), a delayed recall trial,

and a delayed recognition trial.25

41Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

We measured sustained attention and selective attention with the two subtests

“Score!” and “Sky Search” of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children.26

Sustained attention involves maintaining attention over an extended period of

time. Selective attention refers to the ability to select target information from an

array of distracters.27 For example, the children were asked to count tones in 10

items, varying from 9 to 15 tones per item.

The total duration of the follow-up was approximately 2.5 hr. Test scores obtained

when a child was too tired and uncooperative, as assessed by the experimenter,

were excluded.

Behavioral Outcome

To obtain information on the children’s competencies and their behavioral and

emotional problems, the parents completed the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)28

and the teachers filled out the Teacher’s Report Form.28 These questionnaires

consist of a total scale and two subscales: internalizing problems (emotionally

reactive, anxious/depressed scales, somatic complaints, withdrawn behavior) and

externalizing problems (attention problems and aggres-sive behavior).

In addition, the parents filled out an attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD) questionnaire that contains 18 items on inattention, hyperactivity, and

impulsivity.29

To gain insight in the socioeconomic status (SES) and home environmental factors

that may influence development, the highest level of maternal education and the

Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) questionnaire

were assessed during the first year after birth during an earlier stage of the GIC

study.6

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Statistical Analyses

Chemical values are presented as medians with range because of the skewed

distribution. Neutral compounds are expressed on lipid weight basis (nanograms

per gram lipid) and phenolic compounds on fresh weight basis (picograms per

gram serum). To compare the scores on the Movement ABC and cognitive tests

with the reference values, we classified the scores into “normal” (> 15th percentile),

“subclinical” (5th to 15th percentile), and “clinical” (≤ 5th percentile). We classified

the questionnaires according to the instructions in the manual that provides

the percentiles corresponding to the raw scores. The results on the neurologic

examination are reported as percentage of children with nonoptimal function. We

calculated intelligence quotient (IQ) scores by deriving the standard scores from

the mean of the scores on the verbal and performance subtests. Because no Dutch

norms are available for the NEPSY-II, we used the American norms to classify the

scores of the children into percentiles. For the AVLT, we used the Dutch norms for

children of 6 years of age. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to determine

which neutral and phenolic OHC concentrations and outcome measures were

distributed normally. We used the Pearson correlation for normally distributed

variables and the Spearman’s rank correlation for nonnormally distributed variables,

to relate the OHC concentrations to motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome.

The raw scores of the outcome variables were used for these calculations. Where

appropriate, the test scores were inversely transformed so that for all tests higher

scores indicated better outcomes. We used the Mann–Whitney U-test to relate the

neurologic outcome (normal or abnormal) to OHC concentrations.

We corrected cognition and behavior of the children for SES and HOME, because

these factors may exert an influence on the cognition and behavior of the children.30

We also investigated whether sex influenced the outcome measures in our study

group (Mann–Whitney U-test). If so, we corrected for sex on that outcome measure.

The corrections were performed by means of partial correlations controlling for

confounders.

43Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

When correlations between OHCs and outcome did not reach significance, we

explored their relationship by means of scatterplots, to determine whether some

other, nonlinear relationship existed.

In this article, negative correlations indicate that higher OHC concentrations were

related to worse outcome and positive correlations indicate that higher OHC

concentrations were related to better outcome. Throughout the analyses, p<.05

was considered to be statistically significant. SPSS 14.0 software for Windows

(SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL, USA) was used for all the analyses.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Of the 69 children invited, 62 (90%) participated in the follow-up program. Six sets

of parents declined the invitation to participate. One girl had to be excluded because

she suffered severe cognitive impairment of unknown origin and therefore could not

be tested. The OHC concentrations of the seven children not followed up were not

different from those who did participate.

Table 1 shows the concentrations of the neutral and phenolic OHCs measured at

the 35th week of pregnancy of the 62 mothers and the concentrations of the thyroid

hormones in the umbilical cord blood of 51 mothers.

The mean maternal age was 32 years (range, 24–42 years). The highest level of maternal

education was primary school for 4 mothers, secondary school for 30 mothers, and

tertiary school for 28 mothers. The mean score on the HOME questionnaire was 33

(range, 24–37).

Outcome at School Age

The cohort consisted of 38 boys and 24 girls. The mean age at follow-up was 5

years 10 months (range, 5 years 8 months to 6 years 2 months). Table 2 presents an

overview of the children’s motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes. We excluded

the test scores of two children on inhibition and sustained attention and scores of

one child on visual perception and verbal memory, because they were too tired and

uncooperative to attend the assessment. Their OHC concentrations were not different

from those who did participate.

The scores of the children were comparable to the reference values, except for selective

attention, verbal memory, and internalizing and externalizing behavioral problems, on

which the children obtained slightly worse scores compared with the reference values.

The mean (± SD) for total IQ of the children was 103 ± 9 (range, 82–125); mean verbal

IQ, 102 ± 9 (range, 83–130); and mean performance IQ, 103 ± 13 (range, 73–133).

According to the neurologic examination, we found that of the 62 children examined,

1 child (2%) had coordination problems, 2 children (3%) had mild tremors, 18 children

(29%) had nonoptimal fine manipulative abilities, and 21 children (34%) had nonoptimal

sensory integration.

45Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

OHCs in Relation to Outcome

Table 3 shows the OHCs that were significantly related to motor, cognitive, and

behavioral outcome, uncorrected for possible confounders. We found both

positive and negative correlations between OHCs and outcome. Brominated flame

retardants correlated with worse fine manipulative abilities, worse attention, better

coordination, better visual perception, and better behavior. Chlorinated OHCs

correlated with less choreiform dyskinesia. OH-PCBs cor-related with worse fine

manipulative abilities, better attention, and better visual perception. The wood

protective agent PCP correlated with worse coordination, less sensory integrity,

worse attention, and worse visuomotor integration.

We corrected the cognitive and behavioral outcome for SES and HOME, and

because boys and girls differed significantly for selective attention (p=.044), we

corrected selective attention for sex. After these corrections, we found additional

correlations between OHCs and outcome. Some correlations before the correction

were stronger after controlling for confounders, whereas others disappeared.

Table 4 presents these results and gives an overview of the number of analyses

performed, including the correlations that nearly reached significance (p<.10).

Scatterplots of the relations between OHCs and outcome that did not reach

significance revealed no further information about the existence of nonlinear

relationships between variables (data not shown).

Thyroid Hormone Analyses

Table 5 shows the thyroid hormones from the umbilical cord blood that were

related to outcome at 5–6 years of age. TSH correlated with worse motor skills and

worse attention. rT3 correlated with better fine manipulative abilities. T3 correlated

with better visuomotor integra-tion and better behavior. T4 correlated with better

sensory integrity and less ADHD.

We also found that OHC concentrations were related to thyroid hormones. PCP

correlated with lower concentrations of T3 (r=–.292, p=.037); BDE-47 correlated

with higher concentrations of T3 (r=.322, p=.021), as did BDE-99 (r=.311, p=.031)

and BDE-100 (r=.291, p=.038).

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 OHC concentrations and thyroid hormone levels

LOD, limit of detection: 0.08-0.16 pg/g serumData are given as median (range)1. On lipid weight basis (ng/g lipid) 2. On fresh weight basis (pg/g serum)3. In pmol/L4. In nmol/L5. In mg/L

Compound, medium

Organohalogen

4,4’-DDE1

CB-1531

BDE-471

BDE-991

BDE-1001

BDE-1531

BDE-1541

HBCDD1

PCP2

4OH-CB-1072

4OH-CB-1462

4OH-CB-1872

Thyroid hormone

FT43

T44

rT34

T34

TSH4

TBG5

Concentration

Maternal serum (n=62)

94.7 (17.5-323.8)

63.0 (34.0-162.2)

0.9 (<LOD-6.1)

0.2 (<LOD-2.1)

0.2 (<LOD-1.4)

1.6 (0.3-19.7)

0.5 (0.1-3.5)

0.8 (0.3-7.5)

1018 (297-8532)

26.0 (5.4-102.3)

103.3 (36.3-290.1)

79.3 (35.8-180.5)

Umbilical cord serum (n=51)

19.2 (12.0-25.1)

122 (76-157)

3.9 (1.8-6.8)

0.8 (0.5-1.8)

8.5 (3.5-23.5)

30.5 (20.1-43.4)

47Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

TABLE 2 Motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome

Outcome Normal1 Subclinical1 Clinical1

Motor outcome

Movement-ABC (n=62) 55 (89) 4 (6) 3 (5)

DCDQ (n=62) 59 (95) 3 (5)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=62) 60 (97) 2 (3)

Verbal intelligence (n=62) 60 (97) 2 (3)

Performance intelligence (n=62) 56 (90) 6 (10)

Visual perception (n=61) 60 (98) 1 (2)

Visuomotor integration (n=62) 60 (97) 2 (3)

Verbal memory (n=61) 44 (72) 14 (23) 3 (5)

Inhibition (n=60) 52 (87) 7 (12) 1 (2)

Attention, sustained (n=60) 54 (90) 6 (10)

Attention, selective (n=62) 44 (71) 11 (18) 7 (11)

Behavioral outcome

Total behavioral problems2 (n=62) 58 (94) 3 (5) 1 (2)

Internalizing problems2 (n=62) 56 (90) 1 (2) 5 (9)

Externalizing problems2 (n=62) 55 (89) 6 (10) 1 (2)

Total behavioral problems3 (n=57) 51 (89) 4 (7) 2 (4)

Internalizing problems3 (n=57) 51 (89) 4 (7) 2 (4)

Externalizing problems3 (n=57) 52 (91) 3 (5) 2 (4)

ADHD-questionnaire (n=62) 57 (92) 2 (3) 3 (5)

Data are given as numbers (percentage).1. Normal was defined as >15th percentile, subclinical as 5th-15th percentile and clinical as <5th

percentile, with regard to intelligence, normal was defined as IQ>85, subclinical as IQ 70-85 and clinical as IQ <70

2. Derived from the Child Behavior Checklist (parents)3. Derived from the Teacher’s Report Form

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 3 OHCs in relation to outcome

Organohalogen Function Correlation p-value coefficient (R)3

Brominated flame retardants

BDE-47 Attention, sustained 0.267 0.039

Internalizing behavior1 0.301 0.018

Total behavioral outcome1 0.288 0.024

Coordination2 0.255 0.045

BDE-99 Internalizing behavior1 0.323 0.013

Total behavioral outcome1 0.281 0.032

BDE-100 Coordination2 0.309 0.014

Internalizing behavior1 0.403 0.001

Externalizing behavior1 0.305 0.017

Total behavioral outcome1 0.389 0.002

BDE-153 Visual perception 0.289 0.026

BDE-154 Fine manipulative abilities2 0.300 0.018

HBCDD Coordination2 0.290 0.023

Chlorinated OHCs

PCB-153 Choreiform dyskinesia2 0.345 0.007

4OH-CB-107 Fine manipulative abilities2 0.311 0.016

Attention, selective 0.293 0.021

Visual perception 0.278 0.030

4OH-CB-187 Attention, selective 0.318 0.012

4,4’-DDE Choreiform dyskinesia2 0.308 0.016

Wood protective agent

PCP Coordination2 0.363 0.004

Sensory integrity2 0.0344

Visuomotor integration 0.287 0.024

Attention, selective 0.254 0.046

Behavioral outcome was corrected for environmental confounders (HOME) 1. Derived from the Child Behavior Checklist (parents)2. Derived from Touwen’s neurological examination3. Positive correlations indicate better outcome and negative correlations indicate worse outcome 4. Calculated by the Mann-Whitney U test

-

-

-

-

-

-

49Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

TAB

LE 4

Co

rrel

atio

n co

effic

ient

s fo

r O

HC

s in

rel

atio

n to

out

com

e, c

orr

ecte

d f

or

SE

S, H

OM

E, a

nd s

ex

BDE-

47BD

E-99

BDE-

100

BDE-

153

BDE-

154

HBC

DD

PCB-

153

4,4’

-DD

E4O

H-C

B-10

74O

H-C

B-14

64O

H-

CB-

187

PCP

Mov

emen

t-AB

CC

oord

inat

ion1

0.29

0**

0.24

4*0.

239*

-0.3

63**

*Fi

ne m

anip

ulat

ive

abili

ties1

-0.2

53*

-0.3

11**

Trem

ors1

Sen

sory

inte

grat

ion1

Cho

reifo

rm d

yski

nesi

a10.

345*

**0.

308*

*0.

228*

DC

D-Q

Tota

l int

ellig

ence

0.39

3**

Verb

al in

telli

genc

e0.

479*

**P

erfo

rmal

inte

llige

nce

0.31

5*-0

.337

**Vi

sual

per

cept

ion

-0.2

55*

Visu

omot

or in

tegr

atio

nVe

rbal

mem

ory

-0.7

23#

Inhi

bitio

n-0

.310

*-0

.355

**-0

.346

**A

ttent

ion,

sus

tain

ed-0

.264

**-0

.264

**-0

.261

*A

ttent

ion,

sel

ectiv

e0.

230*

0.43

9***

Tota

l beh

avio

ral o

utco

me2

0.27

6**

0.23

1*In

tern

aliz

ing

beha

vior

20.

237*

0.28

3**

0.25

3*0.

344*

*E

xter

naliz

ing

beha

vior

2-0

.278

*To

tal b

ehav

iora

l out

com

e3-0

.314

*-0

.411

**In

tern

aliz

ing

beha

vior

30.

265*

0.23

6*-0

.298

*E

xter

naliz

ing

beha

vior

3-0

.288

*-0

.328

**A

DH

D Q

uest

ionn

aire

Dat

a ar

e sh

own

as c

orre

latio

n co

effic

ient

s, *

p<

.10,

**

p<

.05,

# p

<.0

1 1.

Der

ived

from

Tou

wen

’s n

euro

logi

cal e

xam

inat

ion

2. D

eriv

ed fr

om t

he C

hild

Beh

avio

r C

heck

list

(par

ents

) 3.

Der

ived

from

the

Tea

cher

’s R

epor

t Fo

rm

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 5 Thyroid hormones in relation to outcome

Thyroid hormone Function Correlation p-value coefficient (r)3

TSH General motor skills1 0.430 0.002

Fine manipulative abilities 0.291 0.038

Attention, sustained 0.298 0.038

rT3 Fine manipulative abilities 0.279 0.047

T3 Visuomotor integration 0.308 0.028

Internalizing behavior2 0.319 0.031

T4 Sensory integrity 0.0074

Attention deficit/hyperactivity 0.380 0.009

1. Assessed with the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire2. Derived from the Child Behavior Checklist (parents)3. Positive correlations indicate better outcome and negative correlations indicate worse outcome 4. Calculated by the Mann-Whitney U test

-

-

-

51Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

Discussion

The present explorative study indicated that prenatal background exposure to

OHCs, including OH-PCBs and the more recently introduced PBDEs, correlated

both positively and negatively with neurodevelopmental outcome in healthy Dutch

children at 5–6 years of age. To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first

to investigate the influence of background exposure to these toxic environmental

pollutants on developmental outcome in healthy children at school age.

With regard to PBDEs, animal studies have indicated that prenatal exposure

to different PBDEs may cause long-lasting behavioral alterations, particularly

in motor activity and cogni-tive behavior.31,32 We found that brominated flame

retardants also correlated with motor function, cognition, and behavior in humans.

A study by Fischer et al. (2008) showed that BDE-99 has effects on behavior in

mice.33 They found that BDE-99 and methylmercury exposure leads to disrupted

spontaneous behavior. These results are in line with our findings in children that

BDE-99 correlated with behavior, as measured with the CBCL.

Human studies on the 1974 Michigan PBB incident showed that accidental

exposure to high levels of PBBs may lead to perceptual and perceptual–motor

problems and lower scores on subtests of the McCarthy Scales of Children’s

Abilities in children 4–6 years of age.34,35 During this incident, Michigan residents

unknowingly ingested PBBs through eggs, meat, and dairy products from animals

whose feed had been inadvertently contaminated through the substitution of a

fire retardant for a cattle feed supplement.35 Our study demonstrated that even

background levels of brominated flame retardants exert an influence on diverse

neurologic and neuropsychological functions in children at school age.

With regard to PCBs, exposure can lead to subtle cognitive deficits, motor delay,

and adverse effects on neurologic status in children at school age.9,13 These effects,

however, are often counteracted by the home environment.13 Furthermore, Lee et

al. (2007) described associations between chlorinated persistent organic pollutants

and attention deficit disorder in children 12–15 years of age.36 Less is known about

metabolites of PCBs. Recently, new techniques have become available to detect

these metabolites in human serum. We found that, after correction for SES and

the home environment, OH-PCBs correlated with multiple neuropsychological

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

functions at school age: fine manipulative abilities, choreiform dyskinesia, verbal

memory, inhibition, and behavior. Our results indicate that OH-PCBs might even

be more neurotoxic than PCBs, as postulated in animal studies.15,16

OHCs are known to exert their neurotoxic influence by affecting thyroid hormone

homeostasis. It is hypothesized that OHCs affect thyroid hormone homeostasis

by interfering with thyroid hormone signaling in the developing brain, by changing

intracellular thyroid hormone availability, and by interacting directly at the level

of the thyroid hormone receptors. On the one hand, OHCs have a high affinity

for thyroid hormone receptors and lead to a decrease in thyroid hormone levels,

whereas levels of TSH increase through hormonal feedback mechanisms. Previous

studies on pregnant women and their infants found that PCBs are associated with

higher levels of TSH and lower levels of T4.37 We found that PCP correlated with

lower levels of thyroid hormone but brominated flame retardants correlated with

higher levels of thyroid hormone. It is unknown whether the underlying mechanism

by which PCBs affect thyroid hormones is the same for these OHCs. Our study

disclosed consistent relations between thyroid hormones and outcome. We found

that TSH correlated with worse neuropsychological functions. Thyroid hormones

(T3 and T4), by contrast, correlated with better outcome. These findings, together

with the negative correlations between OHCs and development, seem to confirm

the hypothesis that thyroid hormone homeostasis may be involved.

Because the threshold levels of toxicity for the different OHCs are unknown, we

did not statistically test low versus high levels of OHCs in relation to outcome. The

toxic equivalents of most of the OHCs investigated are also unknown. Research

has shown that some compounds enhance each other, whereas others counteract

each other. No data are available for all the compounds tested in our study. As a

consequence, we explored relations between OHCs and outcome at school age

by means of correlations. Because this study is, to the best of our knowledge,

the first to investigate the association between background levels of OHCs and

outcome at school age, it was difficult to hypothesize on the expected effect on

the outcome measures. Our study might serve as a basis for power calculations

for future studies, because it demonstrates the variability of various parameters

and their mutual associations.

53Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

It was striking that we found both positive and negative correlations between

OHCs and outcome. It is difficult to determine the implications of these results for

functioning in later life. The multiple statistical analyses that were performed might

have played a role in this finding. Furthermore, it is difficult to determine how many

of these effects can reliably be assigned to the specific contaminants, because

some of them were likely to show some degree of collinearity. Other contaminants

that were not measured, such as methyl mercury, might also have played a role.

We did not compare postnatal OHC concentrations with outcome at 5–6 years

of age because previous studies have pointed out that the most serious effects

of neurotoxic OHCs are produced on developmental processes that occur

prenatally.13,38

Concentrations of PBDEs in the environment are much higher in the United States

than in Europe, Asia, or Australia.3,39 Levels of PBDEs in breast milk have been

increasing in the past 20–30 years, along with the serum levels in the general

population worldwide.4,39,40 The fact that even low background levels of OHCs, as

is the case in the Netherlands, may interfere with developmental processes, as

illustrated by the present study, indicates that there is an urgent need for further

research. We believe that future research should focus on longitudinal follow-up of

children exposed to OHCs and should investigate the effect and nature of specific

OHCs. Besides, the effect of combinations of OHCs (by means of composite

scores) on neurodevelopment in large cohorts should also be investigated.

Conclusions

Prenatal background exposure to OHCs not previously studied correlated with

neuropsychological functioning in children at school age. PBDEs, used extensively

worldwide, correlated with motor performance, attention, visual perception, and

behavior, and we found the same results for OH-PCBs. We believe that unrelenting

efforts should be made to find safe alternatives for these compounds.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Mariussen E, Fonnum F. Neurochemical targets and

behavioral effects of organohalogen compounds: an

update. Crit Rev Toxicol 2006;36:253–289.

2. Law RJ, Alaee M, Allchin CR, et al. Levels and

trends of polybrominated diphenylethers and other

brominated flame retardants in wildlife. Environ Int

2003;29:757–770.

3. Rahman F, Langford KH, Scrimshaw MD, Lester

JN. Polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) flame

retardants. Sci Total Environ 2001;275:1–17.

4. Jensen AA. Polychlorobiphenyls (PCBs),

polychlorodibenzo-p-dioxins (PCDDs) and

polychlorodibenzofurans (PCDFs) in human

milk, blood and adipose tissue. Sci Total Environ

1987;64:259–293.

5. Lanting CI, Huisman M, Muskiet FA, van der Paauw

CG, Essed CE, Boersma ER. Polychlorinated

biphenyls in adipose tissue, liver, and brain from nine

stillborns of varying gestational ages. Pediatr Res

1998;44:222–225.

6. Meijer L, Weiss J, van Velzen M, Brouwer A, Bergman

A, Sauer PJ. Serum concentrations of neutral and

phenolic organohalogens in pregnant women and

some of their infants in the Netherlands. Environ Sci

Technol 2008;42:3428–3433.

7. Solomon GM, Schettler T. Environment and health:

6. Endocrine disruption and potential human health

implications. CMAJ 2000;163:1471–1476.

8. Weisglas-Kuperus N. Neurodevelopmental,

immunological and endocrinological indices of

perinatal human exposure to PCBs and dioxins.

Chemosphere 1998;37:1845–1853.

9. Boersma ER, Lanting CI. Environmental exposure

to polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and dioxins.

Consequences for longterm neurological and

cognitive development of the child lactation. Adv Exp

Med Biol 2000;478:271–287.

10. Chen YC, Guo YL, Hsu CC, Rogan WJ. Cognitive

development of Yu-Cheng (“oil disease”) children

prenatally exposed to heat-degraded PCBs. JAMA

1992;268:3213–3218.

11. Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW. Intellectual impairment

in children exposed to polychlorinated biphenyls in

utero. N Engl J Med 1996;335:783–789.

12. Stewart PW, Lonky E, Reihman J, Pagano J, Gump

BB, Darvill T. The relationship between prenatal PCB

exposure and intelligence (IQ) in 9-year-old children.

Environ Health Perspect 2008;116:1416–1422.

13. Vreugdenhil HJ, Lanting CI, Mulder PG, Boersma

ER, Weisglas-Kuperus N. Effects of prenatal PCB

and dioxin background exposure on cognitive and

motor abilities in Dutch children at school age. J

Pediatr 2002;140:48–56.

References

55Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

14. Guvenius DM, Aronsson A, Ekman-Ordeberg G,

Bergman A, Norèn K. Human prenatal and postnatal

exposure to polybrominated diphenyl ethers,

polychlorinated biphenyls, polychlorobiphenylols,

and pentachlorophenol. Environ Health Perspect

2003;111:1235–1241.

15. Kimura-Kuroda J, Nagata I, Kuroda Y. Disrupting

effects of hydroxy-polychlorinated biphenyl

(PCB) congeners on neuronal development of

cerebellar Purkinje cells: a possible causal factor

for developmental brain disorders? Chemosphere

2007;67:S412–S420.

16. Kitamura S, Jinno N, Suzuki T, et al. Thyroid

hormone-like and estrogenic activity of hydroxylated

PCBs in cell culture. Toxicology 2005;208:377–387.

17. Zoeller RT. Environmental chemicals impacting

the thyroid: targets and consequences. Thyroid

2007;17:811–817.

18. Smits-Engelsman BCM. Movement Assessment

Battery for Children. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets

& Zeitlinger; 1998.

19. Touwen BCL. Examination of the Child with Minor

Neurological Dysfunction. London, UK: William

Heinemann; 1979.

20. Hadders-Algra M. Two distinct forms of minor

neurological dysfunction: perspectives emerging

from a review of data of the Groningen Perinatal

Project. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;44:561–571.

21. Peters LH, Maathuis KG, Kouw E, Hamming M,

Hadders-Algra M. Test-retest, inter-assessor and

intra-assessor reliability of the modified Touwen

examination. Eur J Paediatr Neurol

2008;12:328–333.

22. Schoemaker MM, Flapper B, Verheij NP, Wilson

BN, Reinders-Messelink HA, de Kloet A. Evaluation

of the Developmental Coordination Disorder

Questionnaire as a screening instrument. Dev Med

Child Neurol 2006;48:668–673.

23. van der Steene G, Bos A. Wechsler Preschool and

Primary Scale of Intelligence, Revised. Vlaams-

Nederlandse aanpassing, handleiding [in Dutch].

Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

24. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY II. Clinical

and Interpretative Manual. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation; 2007.

25. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545–559.

26. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample

and ADHD performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065–1081.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

27. Heaton SC, Reader SK, Preston AS, et al. The

Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch):

patterns of performance in children with ADHD and

clinical controls. Child Neuropsychol

2001;7:251–264.

28. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA

Preschool Forms and Profiles. Burlington, VT:

University of Vermont, Research Center for Children,

Youth and Families; 2000.

29. Scholte EM, van der Ploeg JD. ADHD-vragenlijst

(AVL) handleiding [in Dutch]. Houten, the

Netherlands: Bohn Stafleu Van Loghum; 2004.

30. Tong S, Baghurst P, Vimpani G, McMichael A.

Socioeconomic position, maternal IQ, home

environment, and cognitive development. J Pediatr

2007;151:284–288.

31. Gee JR, Moser VC. Acute postnatal exposure to

brominated diphenylether 47 delays neuromotor

ontogeny and alters motor activity in mice.

Neurotoxicol Teratol 2008;30:79–87.

32. Viberg H, Fredriksson A, Eriksson P. Neonatal

exposure to polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE

153) disrupts spontaneous behaviour, impairs

learning and memory, and decreases hippocampal

cholinergic receptors in adult mice. Toxicol Appl

Pharmacol 2003;192:95–106.

33. Fischer C, Fredriksson A, Eriksson P. Coexposure

of neonatal mice to a flame retardant PBDE 99

(2,2’,4,4’,5-pentabromodiphenyl ether) and methyl

mercury enhances developmental neurotoxic

defects. Toxicol Sci 2008;101:275–285.

34. Schwartz EM, Rae WA. Effect of polybrominated

biphenyls (PBB) on developmental abilities in young

children. Am J Public Health 1983;73:277–281.

35. Seagull EA. Developmental abilities of children

exposed to polybrominated biphenyls (PBB). Am J

Public Health 1983;73:281–285.

36. Lee DH, Jacobs DR, Porta M. Association of serum

concentrations of persistent organic pollutants with

the prevalence of learning disability and attention

deficit disorder. J Epidemiol Community Health

2007;61:591–596.

37. Koopman-Esseboom C, Morse DC, Weisglas-

Kuperus N, et al. Effects of dioxins and

polychlorinated biphenyls on thyroid hormone

status of pregnant women and their infants. Pediatr

Res 1994;36:468–473.

38. Jacobson JL, Jacobson SW, Humphrey HE. Effects

of in utero exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls

and related contaminants on cognitive functioning in

young children. J Pediatr 1990;116:38–45.

39. Costa LG, Giordano G. Developmental neurotoxicity

of polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) flame

retardants. Neurotoxicology 2007;28:1047–1067.

57Chapter 2 Prenatal Exposure to Organohalogens, including Brominated Flame Retardants, influences Motor, Cognitive, and Behavioral Performance at School Age

40. Sjodin A, Jones RS, Focant JF, et al. Retrospective

time-trend study of polybrominated diphenyl ether

and polybrominated and polychlorinated biphenyl

levels in human serum from the United States.

Environ Health Perspect 2004;112:654–658

Chapter 3 Risk factors for adverse outcome in preterm

infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

Chapter 4 Functional outcome at school age of preterm

infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

Chapter 5 Long-term neurological outcome of term-born

children treated with two or more

anti-epileptic drugs in the neonatal period

Chapter 6 Tractography of the corticospinal tracts in infants

with focal perinatal injury: comparison with

normal controls and to motor development

PART 2Functional outcome of newborn infants

with brain injury

Chapter 3

RISK FACTORS FOR ADVERSE OUTCOME IN

PRETERM INFANTS WITH PERIVENTRICULAR

HEMORRHAGIC INFARCTION

ELISE ROZE, JORIEN M. KERSTJENS, CAREL G.B. MAATHUIS, HENDRIK J. TER

HORST, AREND F. BOS

PEDIATRICS 2008;122(1):E46-E52

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Objective

To identify risk factors associated with mortality and adverse neurological outcome

at 18 months of age in preterm infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

(PHVI).

Study design

Retrospective cohort study, including all preterm infants <37 weeks with PVHI,

admitted between 1995 and 2006. Ultrasound scans were reviewed for grading

of germinal matrix hemorrhage (GMH), localization and extension of the infarction,

and other abnormalities. Several clinical factors were scored. Outcome measures

were: mortality, cerebral palsy and Gross Motor Function Classification System

(GMFCS) level. Odds ratios were calculated by univariate and multivariate logistic

regression analyses.

Results

Of 54 infants, 16 died (30%). Twenty-five of 38 survivors (66%) developed cerebral

palsy, 21 mild (GMFCS levels I and II), 4 moderate-to-severe (levels III and IV).

Several perinatal and neonatal risk factors were associated with mortality. After

multivariate logistic regression, only use of inotropics (OR 31.2, 95%CI 2.6-373,

p<.01) and maternal intrauterine infection (OR 12.2, 95%CI 1.2-127, p<.05) were

predictors for mortality. In survivors, only the most extended form of PVHI was

associated with the development of cerebral palsy (OR>4.7, p<.005), but not with

severity of cerebral palsy. Cystic periventricular leukomalacia and concurrent

grade III-GMH were associated with more severe cerebral palsy.

Conclusion

In preterm infants with PVHI, mortality occurred despite optimal treatment and was

associated with circulatory failure and maternal intrauterine infection. In survivors,

motor development was abnormal in 66%, but functional abilities were good in

the majority. Extension and localization of the PVHI were not related to functional

outcome.

63Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Introduction

Periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PVHI), formerly described as a grade

IV germinal matrix hemorrhage (GMH), is seen in approximately 10 to 15% of

preterm infants with GMH.1 It is presumably caused by pressure of the GMH on

the periventricular terminal vein that drains the cerebral hemisphere.2-4 This leads

to venous congestion in the periventricular white matter and subsequently to

ischemia and hemorrhage. PVHI, or venous infarction, is now considered as a

complication of the GMH instead of an extension of the GMH.4

Mortality in preterm infants with PVHI has been found to range between 38 and

60%.1,5-7 Surviving preterm infants with PVHI are at high risk for developing cerebral

palsy and for an abnormal neurological examination beyond 12 months’ adjusted

age.8-11 Several factors in the perinatal period are associated with PVHI. These

include lower gestational age, emergency caesarean section, low Apgar scores,

need for respiratory resuscitation, pneumothorax, pulmonary hemorrhage, patent

ductus arteriosus, acidosis, and neonatal seizures.1,6,7,12,13

It is unknown, within a group of preterm infants with PVHI, which perinatal and

neonatal characteristics are associated with mortality and adverse neurological

outcome. The aim of this study was therefore to identify perinatal and neonatal

risk factors for mortality and adverse neurological outcome at 18 months of age in

preterm infants with PVHI.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Methods

Patients

In a retrospective analysis, we studied infants who had been admitted to the

Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) of the University Medical Center Groningen

(UMCG) between 1995 and 2006. We intended to include preterm infants

(gestational age < 37 weeks) with PVHI diagnosed by cranial ultrasonography. To

this end we searched the patient database (which contains all diagnoses of every

infant admitted to the NICU) on infants diagnosed with PVHI, venous infarction,

GMH grade IV and parenchymal cerebral bleeding. We excluded patients with

major chromosomal and congenital abnormalities.

Cranial Ultrasonography Classification

According to the routine scanning protocol of our unit, the first ultrasound scan

was made within 72 hours after birth and then at a minimum of weekly intervals

until stabilization or the disappearance of any abnormality. The scans were

performed by means of real-time mechanical sector scanners equipped with a 7.5

MHz transducer.

All infants admitted to our NICU are subjected to the scanning protocol, if they match

one of the following criteria: gestational age < 35 weeks, birth weight < 1500 grams,

complicated delivery with a risk for brain injury, perinatal asphyxia, and suspected

congenital cerebral malformation.

Three experts (AFB, JK, HTH) reviewed the serial ultrasound scans of the infants

who were included after the patient database search, to determine whether PVHI

was present or not. The experts were blind as to the infants’ outcome. The final

study group was defined after this review.

We scored several characteristics of the PVHI. These included unilateral or bilateral

infarction, left or right-sided infarction, localization and extension of the infarction

(frontal, parietal, occipital or combinations) and residual abnormalities (e.g.

porencephalic cysts). When bilateral, the localization and extension were based on

the side that was affected most. Next we scored additional cerebral abnormalities.

These included grade of GMH (grades I, II or III),14 unilateral or bilateral GMH,

presence or emergence of posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation (PHVD) and cystic

65Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

periventricular leukomalacia (PVL). PHVD was defined as a lateral ventricle size of

more than 0.33 according to Evans’ index (the right and left lateral horn width

divided by the maximum internal skull width).15 Cystic periventricular leukomalacia

was classified as stage 2 or worse, according to de Vries et al.16

When the experts disagreed, the scans involved were re-evaluated by all experts,

together. In all cases a consensus was reached on a final score.

Perinatal Risk Factors

We reviewed the medical charts of the infants for perinatal and neonatal risk

factors. These included signs of maternal intrauterine infection, early onset sepsis,

asphyxia, intrauterine growth restriction, respiratory failure, circulatory failure

and neonatal seizures. Maternal intrauterine infection was based on such clinical

signs as fetal tachycardia and maternal fever (>38°C), often combined with the

use of maternal antibiotics. We also scored the presence of premature rupture

of membranes (>24h) and histological characteristics of the placenta for signs of

inflammation. Early onset sepsis was diagnosed by a positive blood culture and/

or clinical signs within the first 48 hours of life. Asphyxia was determined by Apgar

scores after 1 and 5 minutes (<5), resuscitation (external heart massage and/or

use of epinephrine) and umbilical cord pH (arterial pH <7.10). Intrauterine growth

restriction was scored if birth weight was below the 10th centile, according to

the Dutch intrauterine growth standards.17 Respiratory failure, defined as need for

ventilatory support, was scored by mode and duration of the support, idiopathic

respiratory distress syndrome, pneumothorax, and the presence of respiratory

acidosis. Circulatory failure was defined as hemodynamic instability and scored

by the need for fluid resuscitation and use of inotropics during the first week, often

combined with metabolic acidosis (pH<7.15).

Late onset morbidity was also recorded. This included retinopathy of prematurity

(ROP), late onset sepsis, necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC), and bronchopulmonary

dysplasia (BPD). Late onset sepsis was diagnosed by a positive blood culture

and/or clinical signs after the first 5 days of life. BPD was defined as oxygen

dependency beyond 36 weeks post-menstrual age.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Outcome

We determined the mortality rate and cause of death in the study group. Follow-

up of the survivors was part of the regular follow-up program of preterm infants

and consisted of a pediatric and neurological examination at 3, 6, 12 and 18 to 24

months’ corrected age, based on Touwen.18 The presence or absence of cerebral

palsy was determined when infants had reached the age of at least 18 months,

following Hagberg’s criteria.19

In case of cerebral palsy, gross motor functioning was scored by a physiatrist using

the Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS).20 This is a functional,

five level classification system for cerebral palsy based on self-initiated movement

with particular emphasis on sitting (truncal control) and walking. A higher level

correlates with more serious impairment of functional abilities. Level I represents

children who walk without restrictions but have limitations in more advanced

gross motor skills, e.g. co-ordination and balance. Level II represents those who

walk without assistive devices but have limitations in walking outdoors and in

the community, e.g. running and jumping. Level III represents children who walk

with assistive mobility devices and have limitations in walking outdoors and in the

community. Level IV represents those with limitations in self-mobility, these children

use power mobility outdoors and in the community. Level V represents those with

severely limited self-mobility even with the use of assistive technology.20

To investigate whether the outcome of preterm infants with PVHI at our NICU

changed over time we studied mortality rate, development of cerebral palsy, and

GMFCS throughout the study period.

67Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Statistical Analysis

We calculated odds ratios by univariate logistic regression to determine which

perinatal and neonatal factors and which cerebral characteristics were related to

mortality. We repeated these calculations in the survivors for the development of

cerebral palsy. We used chi2 for trend to relate the severity of cerebral palsy to the

risk factors and cerebral characteristics, and to calculate the changes in outcome

throughout the study period. Finally, we used backward multivariate logistic

regression analysis to determine which risk factors detected by the univariate

analyses (with p<.10) had independent prognostic value for outcome. Throughout

the analyses p<.05 was considered to be statistically significant. SPSS software

for Windows, version 14.0 (SPSS Inc. Chicago, Illinois) was used for all analyses.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Between 1995 and 2006, 4022 patients with a gestational age of < 37 weeks

were admitted to our NICU. After database search, 66 infants were selected with

(suspected) intraparenchymal hemorrhages. After reviewing the cranial ultrasound

scans, 54 infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarctions were included in the

study. This is 1.3% of all infants (gestational age < 37 weeks) admitted to the NICU

during the study period. Table 1 gives an overview of the patient demographics.

The PVHI was unilateral in 45 infants (83%): left-sided in 26 (48%), right-sided in

19 (35%). Nine infants (17%) had bilateral PVHI. The PVHI was localized to the

frontoparietal region in 14 infants (26%), to the parieto-occipital region in 25 infants

(46%) and extended throughout the entire periventricular region (fronto-parieto-

occipital) in 15 infants (28%). In 37 infants (69%) the PVHI was associated with a

GMH grades I or II; in 17 infants (31%) with a GMH grade III. Sixteen infants (30%)

developed PHVD. Seven of them did not need drainage of cerebrospinal fluid,

five needed a temporary ventricular drain and four required a permanent shunt.

Bilateral subtle echodensities, not related to the PVHI, developed into cystic PVL

in two infants (4%).

Mortality

Of the 54 infants, 16 died (30%). Fourteen infants died despite optimal treatment.

In six infants the cause of death was respiratory failure and five infants died

of combined respiratory and circulatory failure. One infant died because of a

necrotizing enterocolitis, one because of a late onset sepsis and one because of

extensive bilateral hemorrhage with a persistent flat trace EEG. Two infants died

after life support was withdrawn. In both cases the prognosis was considered to

be poor, based on the combination of severe respiratory and neurological signs.

Table 2 gives an overview of the perinatal and neonatal factors associated with

mortality. A lower gestational age, signs of circulatory failure (use of inotropics

and metabolic acidosis), maternal intrauterine infection and neonatal seizures

were associated with increased mortality. Table 3 gives an overview of the

characteristics of the PVHI and additional cerebral abnormalities in relation to

mortality. Localization and extension of the PVHI were not associated with death

69Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

or survival. Only three infants had PVHI limited to the frontal region and all three

survived. Of additional cerebral abnormalities, GMH grade III was present more

frequently in infants who died than in survivors. As a consequence, GMH grades I

and II were present more frequently in survivors.

Neurological Outcome

Of the 38 surviving children, 13 (34%) were normal and 25 (66%) had developed

cerebral palsy. Four infants (11%) had cerebral palsy with moderate to severe

functional limitations, GMFCS levels III (n=1) and IV (n=3). These four infants all

had bilateral cerebral palsy. Twenty one infants (55%) had mild, unilateral cerebral

palsy, GMFCS levels I (n=10) and II (n=11).

Table 4 gives an overview of the cerebral characteristics on ultrasound in relation

to the development of cerebral palsy and GMFCS levels. All children with extensive

PVHI (fronto-parieto-occipital, n=8) developed cerebral palsy, with GMFCS levels

I or II (OR > 4.7 p=.04). We could not identify any other localization of the PVHI

that correlated with the development and severity of cerebral palsy. Concerning

additional cerebral abnormalities, GMH grade III was associated with higher

GMFCS levels, thus with more severe cerebral palsy. Only four infants had GMFCS

levels III or IV. In two of them bilateral cystic PVL (and GMH grade III) was present,

in the other two we could not identify any reason why these infants in particular

developed a more severe cerebral palsy.

Perinatal and neonatal factors (including late onset morbidity) did not correlate with

the development and severity of cerebral palsy, only intrauterine growth restriction

nearly reached significance (p=.06). Of the survivors, two children were born after

intrauterine growth restriction; one had GMFCS level II and one level IV.

Trends in Outcome over Time

Mortality decreased from 1995 to 2006 (p=.03). In addition, the incidence of

cerebral palsy in surviving children also decreased (p=.01). The severity of cerebral

palsy, measured by the GMFCS level, did not change during the study period.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Multivariate Logistic Regression Analysis

Since the clinical and cerebral characteristics are likely to be interdependent, we

performed a multivariate logistic regression analysis to investigate which factors

contributed independently to mortality. Perinatal and neonatal factors and cerebral

characteristics which had shown associations with mortality at p<.10 were entered

as predictors: gestational age, maternal intrauterine infection, Apgar score at 1

minute, pneumothorax, signs of circulatory failure, neonatal seizures, GMH grade

III, bilateral GMH and fronto-parieto-occipital extension of the PVHI. Only maternal

intrauterine infection (OR 12.2; 95% CI 1.2 – 127, p=.04) and signs of circulatory

failure (use of inotropics during the first week, OR 31.2; 95% CI 2.6 - 373, p=.007)

remained in the model. We did not perform a multivariate logistic regression

analysis on survivors to determine independent predictors for the development

and severity of cerebral palsy, because only one association was found in the

univariate analyses for both development and severity of cerebral palsy.

71Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

TABLE 1 Patient demographics

Number n=54

Males/ females 32/ 22

Gestational age (weeks) 27.7 (25.4-35.0)

Birth weight (grams) 1050 (700-2430)

IUGR (<P10) n=4 (7)

Maternal intrauterine infection1 n=11 (20)

Early onset sepsis n=7 (13)

Asphyxia

Resuscitation2 n=2 (4)

Apgar at 1’ 4 (1-9)

Apgar at 5’ 8 (2-10)

Umbilical cord pH 7.21 (6.83-7.42)

Respiratory failure

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO) n=47 (87)

Pneumothorax n=9 (17)

Respiratory acidosis n=14 (26)

Circulatory failure

Inotropics n=27 (50)

Metabolic acidosis n=10 (19)

Neonatal seizures n=13 (24)

Mortality n=16 (30)

Age of death 5.5 days (1-120)

Late neonatal morbidity

ROP (grade I-II vs. grade III-V) 11 (20) vs. 2 (5)

Late onset sepsis 14 (26)

Necrotizing enterocolitis 3 (6)

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia 14 (26)

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or numbers (percentage).1. Maternal intrauterine infection was scored by clinical signs as fetal tachycardia and maternal fever

(>38°C), often combined with the use of maternal antibiotics2. Resuscitation was scored in case of external heart massage and/or use of epinephrine

Abbreviations: IUGR, intrauterine growth restriction; IPPV, intermittent positive pressure ventilation; HFO, high frequency oscillation; ROP, retinopathy of prematurity

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TAB

LE 2

Per

inat

al a

nd n

eona

tal r

isk

fact

ors

in r

elat

ion

to m

ort

ality

Infa

nts

who

die

d

S

urvi

vors

OR

(95%

CI)

p

-val

ue

Num

ber

n=

16

n=

38G

esta

tiona

l age

(wee

ks)

27

.1 (2

5.6-

28.6

)

28

.7 (2

5.4-

35.0

)

0.58

(0.3

7-0.

90)

0.

016

Birt

h w

eigh

t (gr

ams)

993

(780

-145

0)

1113

(700

-243

0)

nsIU

GR

(<P1

0)

n=

2 (1

3)

n=

2 (5

)

nsM

ater

nal in

traut

erin

e in

fect

ion1

n=

6 (3

8)

n=

4 (1

1)

5.1

(1.2

-21.

7)

0.

027

Early

ons

et s

epsi

s

n=2

(13)

n=5

(13)

nsAs

phyx

iaR

esus

itatio

n2

n=3

(19)

n=3

(8)

ns

Apga

r at 1

3 (1

-8)

5

(1-9

)

0.

79 (0

.61-

1.0)

0.07

5Ap

gar a

t 5’

7.

5 (2

-9)

8

(3-1

0)

ns

Um

bilic

al c

ord

pH

7.

13 (7

.04-

7.22

)

7.

22 (6

.83-

7.42

)

ns

Res

pira

tory

failu

re

Vent

ilato

ry s

uppo

rt (IP

PV o

r HFO

)

n=16

(100

)

n=

31 (8

2)

ns

Pneu

mot

hora

x

n=5

(31)

n=4

(11)

3.

86 (0

.88-

17.0

)

0.07

3R

espi

rato

ry A

cido

sis

n=

4 (2

5)

n=

10 (2

8)

nsC

ircul

ator

y fa

ilure

Inot

ropi

cs

n=

14 (8

8)

n=13

(34)

13.5

(2.6

-68.

4)

0.

002

Met

abol

ic a

cido

sis

n=

7 (4

4)

n=

3 (8

)

9.

1 (2

.0-4

2.2)

0.00

5N

eona

tal S

eizu

res

n=

8 (5

0)

n=

5 (1

3)

6.6

(1.7

-25.

7)

0.

006

Dat

a ar

e gi

ven

as m

edia

n (m

inim

um-m

axim

um) o

r nu

mb

ers

(per

cent

age)

.1.

Intr

aute

rine

infe

ctio

n w

as s

core

d b

y cl

inic

al s

igns

as

feta

l tac

hyca

rdia

and

mat

erna

l fev

er (>

38 d

egre

es),

ofte

n co

mb

ined

with

the

us

e of

mat

erna

l ant

ibio

tics

2. R

esus

cita

tion

was

sco

red

as

exte

rnal

hea

rt m

assa

ge a

nd/o

r us

e of

ep

inep

hrin

e

Ab

bre

viat

ions

: ns,

not

sig

nific

ant;

IUG

R, i

ntra

uter

ine

grow

th r

estr

ictio

n; IP

PV,

inte

rmitt

ent

pos

itive

p

ress

ure

vent

ilatio

n; H

FO, h

igh

freq

uenc

y os

cilla

tion

73Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

TAB

LE 2

Per

inat

al a

nd n

eona

tal r

isk

fact

ors

in r

elat

ion

to m

ort

ality

Infa

nts

who

die

d

S

urvi

vors

OR

(95%

CI)

p

-val

ue

Num

ber

n=

16

n=

38G

esta

tiona

l age

(wee

ks)

27

.1 (2

5.6-

28.6

)

28

.7 (2

5.4-

35.0

)

0.58

(0.3

7-0.

90)

0.

016

Birt

h w

eigh

t (gr

ams)

993

(780

-145

0)

1113

(700

-243

0)

nsIU

GR

(<P1

0)

n=

2 (1

3)

n=

2 (5

)

nsM

ater

nal in

traut

erin

e in

fect

ion1

n=

6 (3

8)

n=

4 (1

1)

5.1

(1.2

-21.

7)

0.

027

Early

ons

et s

epsi

s

n=2

(13)

n=5

(13)

nsAs

phyx

iaR

esus

itatio

n2

n=3

(19)

n=3

(8)

ns

Apga

r at 1

3 (1

-8)

5

(1-9

)

0.

79 (0

.61-

1.0)

0.07

5Ap

gar a

t 5’

7.

5 (2

-9)

8

(3-1

0)

ns

Um

bilic

al c

ord

pH

7.

13 (7

.04-

7.22

)

7.

22 (6

.83-

7.42

)

ns

Res

pira

tory

failu

re

Vent

ilato

ry s

uppo

rt (IP

PV o

r HFO

)

n=16

(100

)

n=

31 (8

2)

ns

Pneu

mot

hora

x

n=5

(31)

n=4

(11)

3.

86 (0

.88-

17.0

)

0.07

3R

espi

rato

ry A

cido

sis

n=

4 (2

5)

n=

10 (2

8)

nsC

ircul

ator

y fa

ilure

Inot

ropi

cs

n=

14 (8

8)

n=13

(34)

13.5

(2.6

-68.

4)

0.

002

Met

abol

ic a

cido

sis

n=

7 (4

4)

n=

3 (8

)

9.

1 (2

.0-4

2.2)

0.00

5N

eona

tal S

eizu

res

n=

8 (5

0)

n=

5 (1

3)

6.6

(1.7

-25.

7)

0.

006

Dat

a ar

e gi

ven

as m

edia

n (m

inim

um-m

axim

um) o

r nu

mb

ers

(per

cent

age)

.1.

Intr

aute

rine

infe

ctio

n w

as s

core

d b

y cl

inic

al s

igns

as

feta

l tac

hyca

rdia

and

mat

erna

l fev

er (>

38 d

egre

es),

ofte

n co

mb

ined

with

the

us

e of

mat

erna

l ant

ibio

tics

2. R

esus

cita

tion

was

sco

red

as

exte

rnal

hea

rt m

assa

ge a

nd/o

r us

e of

ep

inep

hrin

e

Ab

bre

viat

ions

: ns,

not

sig

nific

ant;

IUG

R, i

ntra

uter

ine

grow

th r

estr

ictio

n; IP

PV,

inte

rmitt

ent

pos

itive

p

ress

ure

vent

ilatio

n; H

FO, h

igh

freq

uenc

y os

cilla

tion

TAB

LE 3

Cha

ract

eris

tics

of

PV

HI a

nd a

dd

itio

nal c

ereb

ral a

bno

rmal

ities

in r

elat

ion

to m

ort

ality

Infa

nts

who

die

d

Sur

vivo

rs

OR

(95%

CI)

p-v

alue

Num

ber

n=

16

n=38

Loca

lizat

ion

and

exte

nsio

n PV

HI

Fron

topa

rieta

l1

n=3

(19)

n=

11 (2

9)

ns

Parie

to-o

ccip

ital2

n=

6 (3

8)

n=19

(50)

ns

Fron

to-p

arie

to-o

ccip

ital

n=

7 (4

4)

n=

8 (2

1)

2.

9 (0

.83-

10.3

) 0.

095

Uni

late

ral/b

ilate

ral P

VHI

Left

n=

8 (5

0)

n=

18 (4

7)

ns

Rig

ht

n=

4 (2

5)

n=

15 (3

9)

ns

Bila

tera

l

n=4

(25)

n=5

(13)

ns

Addi

tiona

l abn

orm

alitie

s

Gra

des

I-II G

MH

n=6

(38)

n=31

(82)

0.14

(0.0

4-0.

50)

0.00

3

Gra

de II

I GM

H

n=

10 (6

3)

n=7

(18)

7.4

(2.1

-27.

2)

0.00

3

Bila

tera

l GM

H

n=

11 (6

9)

n=16

(42)

3.0

(0.8

8-10

.4)

0.08

PHVD

n=4

(25)

n=

12 (3

2)

ns

cyst

ic P

VL

n=

0 (0

)

n=

2 (5

)

ns

1. In

clud

es fr

onta

l, p

arie

tal,

and

fron

top

arie

tal i

nfar

ctio

ns2.

Incl

udes

par

ieto

- oc

cip

ital a

nd o

ccip

ital i

nfar

ctio

ns

Ab

bre

viat

ions

: PV

HI,

per

iven

tric

ular

hem

orrh

agic

infa

rctio

n; G

MH

, ger

min

al m

atrix

hem

orrh

age;

PH

VD

, pos

them

orrh

agic

ven

tric

ular

dila

tatio

n; P

VL,

p

eriv

entr

icul

ar le

ukom

alac

ia; O

R, o

dd

s ra

tio; C

I, co

nfid

ence

inte

rval

; ns,

not

sig

nific

ant

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 4 Characteristics of PVHI and additional cerebral abnormalities

in relation to cerebral palsy and GMFCS in survivors (numbers in the

table refer to the numbers of children)

GMFCS level

No CP I II III-IV p-value

Number 13/ 38 10/ 38 11/ 38 4/ 38

Localization and extension PVHI

Frontoparietal1 5 2 2 2 ns

Parieto-occipital2 8 4 5 2 ns

Fronto-parieto-occipital 0 4 4 0 ns

Unilateral/bilateral PVHI

Left 7 5 4 2 ns

Right 4 5 5 1 ns

Bilateral 2 0 2 1 ns

Additional abnormalities

Grades I-II GMH 13 9 7 2 0.005

Grade III GMH 0 1 4 2 0.005

Bilateral GMH 5 5 4 2 ns

PHVD 1 5 4 2 ns

cystic PVL 0 0 0 2 0.011

P-value’s are derived from the chi2 test for trend.1. Includes frontal, parietal, and frontoparietal infarctions2. Includes parieto- occipital and occipital infarctions

Abbreviations: GMFCS, Gross Motor Function Classification Scale; CP, cerebral palsy; PVHI, periventricular hemorrhagic infarction; GMH, germinal matrix hemorrhage; PHVD, posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation; PVL, periventricular leukomalacia; ns, not significant

75Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Discussion

We identified several risk factors predictive for mortality and adverse neurological

outcome at 18 months of age in preterm infants with PVHI. A low gestational age,

neonatal seizures, signs of circulatory failure (use of inotropics and metabolic

acidosis), maternal intrauterine infection and an associated GMH grade III were

related to mortality. Independent predictors were circulatory failure and maternal

intrauterine infection. Sixty six percent of the survivors developed cerebral palsy.

Only the most extended PVHI (fronto-parieto-occipital) was associated with

the development of cerebral palsy. Characteristics of the PVHI (localization and

extension) were not related to the severity of cerebral palsy. However, additional

cerebral abnormalities such as GMH grade III and cystic PVL were associated with

more severe cerebral palsy.

We found a lower mortality (30%) compared to previous studies, which reported

percentages from 38 to 60%.1,5-7,21 It is striking that in our study the extension of

the PVHI was not related to mortality. This association was reported by Guzzetta

et al. in 19865 and recently by Bassan et al.22 An explanation for the difference

might be the characteristics of the study groups. Both studies reported a higher

percentage of infants with extended lesions (50-66%) than the present study

(28%). The percentage of infants with associated GMH grade III in these studies

was also much higher (80-88% versus 31%).5,22 This suggests that morbidity of the

infants in the present study might have been less severe than in these previous

studies. Advances in neonatal care, better imaging techniques and decisions

regarding withdrawal of life support might also have influenced the association.22

In the present study, most of the infants who died had suffered respiratory or

circulatory failure, unresponsive to treatment.

We found that mortality in infants with PVHI was associated with low gestational

age, maternal intrauterine infections, signs of circulatory failure, an associated

GMH grade III and neonatal seizures. After multivariate logistic regression analysis,

only circulatory failure (use of inotropics) and signs of maternal intrauterine

infection were independent predictors for mortality. Circulatory failure, leading

to disturbances of systemic and cerebral hemodynamics, is identified as a risk

factor for the emergence of PVHI in several studies.7,12,13 Apparently, this factor

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

is associated not only with the emergence of PVHI but also with a considerably

increased risk of dying (OR 31.2) in case of PVHI. The association of mortality

with maternal intrauterine infections in the present study was less strong and just

reached significance. Intrauterine (perinatal) infections and inflammation have been

associated in particular with periventricular leukomalacia23 and not with PVHI.13,24

However, other larger studies have demonstrated that intrauterine infections,

chorio-amnionitis, placental inflammation and early sepsis are associated with

GMH grade III and PVHI.25-27 Although the percentage of infants with signs of

maternal intrauterine infection was low in the present study, our data suggest that

its presence increases the risk of dying.

Neurological outcome at the age of 18 months was normal in 34% of the survivors.

The remaining infants developed cerebral palsy, which was mild in most cases.

The percentage of infants developing cerebral palsy after PVHI in the present

study was similar to previous studies, ranging from 60 to 90%.1,5,8,9,11,22 Of all

cerebral characteristics investigated, only the most extended localization (fronto-

parieto-occipital) was associated with the development of cerebral palsy. This was

also found by Bassan et al.22 There is controversy about anterior frontal lesions.

Rademaker et al. demonstrated in a small study of 20 infants that a localization

of PVHI in the occipital region was associated with the development of cerebral

palsy.8 A localization more frontally, anterior of the trigonum, was associated with a

normal outcome.8 Bassan et al. found the opposite, i.e. anterior frontal involvement

of the PVHI was associated with an abnormal neuromotor examination at the age

of 1 year.22 We could not confirm any of these results. In our study, we found that

some infants with frontal, frontoparietal and parieto-occipital lesions developed

cerebral palsy, but others did not. Concerning perinatal and neonatal risk factors,

no associations existed with the development of cerebral palsy.

Functional limitations in children developing cerebral palsy were mostly mild, with

GMFCS levels I and II. Only a few infants had cerebral palsy with more severe

functional limitations. This was associated with additional cerebral abnormalities,

such as cystic PVL and an associated GMH grade III. It is known that cystic PVL

causes more severe neurological sequelae than PVHI.1,11,28 To our surprise, GMFCS

levels were not related to localization and extension of the PVHI, nor to any other

77Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

perinatal or neonatal risk factor. Other studies in infants with PVHI and functional

outcomes are scarce and do not report about outcomes of the infants in terms of

functional abilities and limitations.

As a retrospective analysis, the present study is subject to potential limitations.

We took care to include all children with PVHI, but perhaps we still missed some.

Being a single-center study, caution should be taken to generalize our results to

other centers. Perinatal and neonatal care has changed considerably during the

long range of years covered by this study (1995-2006). During the last 20 years,

the incidence of severe intracranial hemorrhages has declined rapidly.26 Factors

that were risk factors in the past are not necessarily risk factors at present.

We did not confirm the PVHI on cranial ultrasound scans using magnetic resonance

imaging in all infants. We are confident however, that we could reliably make the

diagnosis of PVHI by cranial ultrasonography alone. Earlier studies comparing

magnetic resonance imaging with cranial ultrasonography in PVHI showed that

ultrasonography is a reliable method for diagnosing PVHI.1,10 The distinction

between bilateral PVHI and cystic PVL is sometimes difficult to make.1,10 We used

the criteria mentioned in De Vries et al.1 to differentiate between the two types

of brain lesions. In our opinion, we could reliably make this distinction because

ultrasound scans were performed at least at weekly intervals. Only two infants had

both PVHI and cystic PVL. This incidence is similar to other studies.1,10

All surviving children were examined at follow-up at a minimum of 18 months’

corrected age. We realize that 18 months is a bit young to determine the GMFCS

level reliably. It is known that the GMFCS is a rather stable measure from the age

of two onwards.29 In the present study, most children had already turned two. With

this in mind, the GMFCS levels in the majority of the infants who had developed

cerebral palsy would remain low, indicating rather good gross motor functioning.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Conclusion

We identified several risk factors predictive for mortality and adverse neurological

outcome at 18 months of age in preterm infants with PVHI. Thirty percent of the infants

died, mostly with respiratory and/or circulatory failure, unresponsive to treatment.

Circulatory failure and maternal intrauterine infection, but not characteristics of the

PVHI, were independently predictive for mortality. Sixty six percent of the survivors

developed cerebral palsy, which was mild in most cases. Only the most extended

PVHI (fronto-parieto-occipital) was associated with the development of cerebral

palsy. Characteristics of the PVHI (localization and extension) were not related to

the severity of cerebral palsy, but the presence of concurrent GMH grade III and

cystic PVL were associated with more severe cerebral palsy.

Acknowledgements

We greatly acknowledge the help of Dr. T. Brantsma - van Wulfften Palthe in

Utrecht, for correcting the English. This study was part of the research program

of the post-graduate school, Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences (BCN),

University of Groningen and supported by a Junior Scientific Masterclass grant of

the University of Groningen.

79Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

1. de Vries LS, Roelants-van Rijn AM, Rademaker KJ,

van Haastert I, Beek FJ, Groenendaal F. Unilateral

parenchymal haemorrhagic infarction in the preterm

infant. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2001;5:139-149.

2. Takashima S, Mito T, Ando Y. Pathogenesis of

periventricular white matter hemorrhages in preterm

infants. Brain Dev 1986;8:25-30.

3. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in the premature infant:

overview of clinical aspects, neuropathology, and

pathogenesis. Semin Pediatr Neurol

1998;5:135-151.

4. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the Newborn. 4th ed.

Philadelphia, PA: W.B.Saunders; 2001.

5. Guzzetta F, Shackelford GD, Volpe S, Perlman

JM, Volpe JJ. Periventricular intraparenchymal

echodensities in the premature newborn: critical

determinant of neurological outcome. Pediatrics

1986;78:995-1006.

6. Perlman JM, Rollins N, Burns D, Risser

R. Relationship between periventricular

intraparenchymal echodensities and germinal matrix-

intraventricular hemorrhage in the very low birth

weight neonate. Pediatrics 1993;91:474-480.

7. Bassan H, Feldman HA, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Periventricular hemorrhagic infarction: Risk factors

and neonatal outcome. Pediatr Neurol

2006;35:85-92.

8. Rademaker KJ, Groenendaal F, Jansen GH, Eken P,

de Vries LS. Unilateral haemorrhagic parenchymal

lesions in the preterm infant: shape, site and

prognosis. Acta Paediatr 1994;83:602-608.

9. de Vries LS, Rademaker KJ, Groenendaal F, et al.

Correlation between neonatal cranial ultrasound,

MRI in infancy and neurodevelopmental outcome

in infants with a large intraventricular haemorrhage

with or without unilateral parenchymal involvement.

Neuropediatrics 1998;29:180-188.

10. Bass WT, Jones MA, White LE, Montgomery TR,

Aiello F, Karlowicz MG. Ultrasonographic differential

diagnosis and neurodevelopmental outcome of

cerebral white matter lesions in premature infants. J

Perinatol 1999;19:330-336.

11. Cioni G, Bos AF, Einspieler C, et al. Early

neurological signs in preterm infants with unilateral

intraparenchymal echodensity. Neuropediatrics

2000;31:240-251.

12. Osborn DA, Evans N, Kluckow M. Hemodynamic

and antecedent risk factors of early and late

periventricular/intraventricular hemorrhage in

premature infants. Pediatrics 2003;112:33-39.

13. Bass WT, Schultz SJ, Burke BL, White LE, Khan

JH, Karlowicz MG. Indices of hemodynamic and

respiratory functions in premature infants at risk for

the development of cerebral white matter injury. J

Perinatol 2002;22:64-71.

14. Volpe JJ. Intraventricular hemorrhage in the

premature infant - current concepts. Part II. Ann

Neurol 1989;25:109-116.

References

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

15. Evans WA. An encephalographic ratio for estimating

ventricular enlargement and cerebral atrophy. Arch

Neurol Psychiatry 1942;47:931-937.

16. de Vries LS, Eken P, Dubowitz LMS. The spectrum

of leukomalacia using cranial ultrasound. Behav

Brain Res 1992;49:1-6.

17. Kloosterman GJ. On intrauterine growth. The

significance of prenatal care. Int J Gynaecol Obstet

1970;8:895-912.

18. Touwen BCL. Neurological Development in Infancy.

London, UK: William Heinemann Medical Books

Ltd.; 1976.

19. Hagberg B, Hagberg G, Olow I. The changing

panorama of cerebral palsy in Sweden 1954-1970.

I. Analysis of the general changes. Acta Paediatr

Scand 1975;64:187-192.

20. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D,

Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability

of a system to classify gross motor function in

children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

21. de Vries LS, van Haastert IC, Rademaker KJ,

Koopman-Esseboom C, Groenendaal F. Ultrasound

abnormalities preceding cerebral palsy in high-risk

infants. J Pediatr 2004;144:815-820.

22. Bassan H, Benson CB, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Ultrasonographic features and severity scoring

of periventricular hemorrhagic infarction in

relation to risk factors and outcome. Pediatrics

2006;117:2111-2118.

23. Dammann O, Leviton A. Infection remote from the

brain, neonatal white matter damage, and cerebral

palsy in the preterm infant. Semin Pediatr Neurol

1998;5:190-201.

24. Turner MA, Vause S, Howell L, et al. Isolated

parenchymal lesions on cranial ultrasound in very

preterm infants in the context of maternal infection.

Early Hum Dev 2007;83:63-68.

25. Hansen A, Leviton A. Labor and delivery

characteristics and risks of cranial ultrasonographic

abnormalities among very-low-birth-weight

infants. The Developmental Epidemiology Network

Investigators. Am J Obstet Gynecol

1999;181:997-1006.

26. Thorp JA, Jones PG, Clark RH, Knox E, Peabody

JL. Perinatal factors associated with severe

intracranial hemorrhage. Am J Obstet Gynecol

2001;185:859-862.

27. Linder N, Haskin O, Levit O, et al. Risk factors for

intraventricular hemorrhage in very low birth weight

premature infants: A retrospective case-control

study. Pediatrics 2003;111:e590-e595.

28. Pierrat V, Duquennoy C, van Haastert I, Ernst M,

Guilley N, de Vries LS. Ultrasound diagnosis and

neurodevelopmental outcome of localised and

extensive cystic periventricular leucomalacia. Arch

Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2001;84:F151-F156.

81Chapter 3 Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

29. Palisano RJ, Cameron D, Rosenbaum PL, Walter

SD, Russell D. Stability of the gross motor function

classification system. Dev Med Child Neurol

2006;48:424-428.

Chapter 4

FUNCTIONAL OUTCOME AT SCHOOL AGE OF

PRETERM INFANTS WITH PERIVENTRICULAR

HEMORRHAGIC INFARCTION

ELISE ROZE, KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL, CHRISTA N. VAN DER VEERE,

CAREL G. B. MAATHUIS, ALBERT MARTIJN, AREND F. BOS

PEDIATRICS 2009;123(6):1493-1500

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Objective

Our objective was to determine motor, cognitive and behavioral outcome at school

age in preterm children with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PVHI) and to

identify cerebral risk factors for adverse outcome.

Methods

This was a prospective cohort study of all preterm infants who were <37 weeks’

gestation, had periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PVHI), and were admitted

between 1995 and 2003. Ultrasound scans were reviewed for characteristics

of PVHI and other cerebral abnormalities. At 4 to 12 years motor outcome was

assessed by the Gross Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS) and the

Manual Ability Classification System (MACS), by a neurological examination

(Touwen), an intelligence test (WISC-III/WPPSI-R), and tests for visual-motor

integration (VMI), visual perception (TVPS) and verbal memory. Behavior was

assessed by the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), and Behavior Rating Inventory

of Executive Function (BRIEF).

Results

Of 38 infants, 15 (39%) died. Twenty-one of 23 survivors were included in the follow-

up. Four were neurologically normal, one had minor neurological dysfunction,

thirteen had unilateral spastic cerebral palsy (CP) and three bilateral CP.

Coordination, associated movements and fine manipulative abilities were affected

most according to the neurological examination. GMFCS: seven children level 1,

seven level 2, one level 3, two level 4. MACS: four children normal, eight level 1,

seven level 2, two level 3. The mean and median total intelligence quotient (IQ)

was 83 (range 55-103, SD 11). Visual perception was normal in 88%, visuomotor

integration was normal in 74% and verbal memory was normal in 50% of the

children. Behavior was normal in 53% and executive functions were normal in

65% and 29% (parents and teachers). Characteristics of the PVHI were not related

to functional motor outcome and intelligence.

85Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation (PHVD) was a risk factor for poorer total

and performance intelligence and abnormal fine manipulative abilities.

Conclusions

The majority of surviving preterm children with PVHI had CP with limited functional

impairment at school age. Intelligence was within one SD of the norm of preterm

children without lesions in 60% to 80% of the children. Verbal memory, in particular,

was affected. Behavioral and executive function problems occurred slightly more

than in preterm infants without lesions. The functional outcome at school age of

preterm children with PVHI is better than previously thought.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Introduction

Periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PVHI) is a complication of a germinal matrix

hemorrhage (GMH) in which extension to the periventricular region occurs. PVHI

is seen in 1% to 3% of preterm infants.1,2 Mortality in preterm infants with PVHI

ranges between 30% and 60%.2-4 Although mortality has decreased during the

last years, PVHI is still considered a disastrous lesion and is sometimes associated

with withdrawal of care.5-7

Outcome studies of 2 to 3-year-old children reported that 50% to 85% of children

with PVHI develop cerebral palsy (CP)2-5,8 and that cognition is impaired in 20%

to 79% of children.4,7,9 Functional outcome at school age in preterm infants with

PVHI, however, is still largely unknown. Therefore the first aim of this study was to

determine motor, cognitive and behavioral outcome in preterm children with PVHI

at school age.

Some studies suggested that particular subtypes of PVHI are related to adverse

outcome, while others do not demonstrate this relationship.2,4,8,10,11 Our second

aim was therefore to identify cerebral risk factors for adverse outcome.

87Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Methods

Patients

We selected preterm infants who had been admitted to the Neonatal Intensive Care

Unit (NICU) of the University Medical Center Groningen (UMCG) between 1995 and

2003, who were < 37 weeks’ gestational and who had PVHI diagnosed by cranial

ultrasonography. We found the infants by searching the patient database, which

contains all diagnoses of every infant admitted to the NICU, for infants diagnosed

with PVHI, venous infarction, GMH grade 4 and parenchymal cerebral bleeding.

We excluded patients with major chromosomal and congenital anomalies.

Cranial Ultrasonography Classification

In accordance with the routine scanning protocol of our unit the first ultrasound

scan was made within 72 hours after birth and subsequently at weekly intervals until

stabilization or disappearance of any abnormality. The scans were made by means

of real-time mechanical sector scanners equipped with a 7.5 MHz transducer. This

scanning protocol is observed for each infant admitted to our NICU with at least

one of the following criteria: gestational age < 35 weeks, birth weight < 1500 g, a

complicated delivery with risk for brain injury, perinatal asphyxia, and suspected

congenital cerebral malformation. Infants with a gestational age > 35 weeks were

scanned in case of clinical signs suggestive of intracranial pathology.

The cranial ultrasound scans of the infants selected for this study were reviewed by

two experts (AFB, AM) to determine whether PVHI was present or not. The experts

were blind as to the infants’ outcome. In case the experts disagreed with one

another they re-evaluated the scans in question, together. In all cases a consensus

was reached on the final score. The final study group was defined after this review

procedure.

We scored several characteristics of the PVHI, i.e. unilateral or bilateral infarction,

left or right-sided infarction, localization and extension of the infarction (frontal,

parietal, occipital or combinations) and residual abnormalities (e.g. porencephalic

cysts). In case of bilateral PVHI localization and extension were based on the

side that was affected most. Next, we scored additional cerebral abnormalities,

i.e. grade of GMH (grade 1, 2 or 3),12 unilateral or bilateral GMH, presence or

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

emergence of posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation (PHVD), and cystic

periventricular leukomalacia (PVL). PHVD was defined as a lateral ventricle size

of > 0.33 according to Evans’ index (the right and left lateral horn width divided

by the maximum internal skull width).13 Cystic periventricular leukomalacia was

classified as stage 2 or worse, according to De Vries et al.14

Follow-up

The children were invited prospectively to participate in an extension of the routine

follow-up program. It entailed the assessment of motor performance, cognition

and behavior at the age of 4 to 12 years. Parents gave their informed consent

to participate in the follow-up program. The study was approved by the Medical

Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen.

Motor Outcome

On the basis of the reports of the routine follow-up program we determined

the presence or absence of CP following Bax’ criteria.15 In case of CP, gross

motor functioning was scored by a physiatrist using the Gross Motor Function

Classification System (GMFCS). This is a functional, five level classification system

for CP based on self-initiated movement with particular emphasis on sitting (truncal

control) and walking.16 During the follow-up program at school age the presence or

absence of CP and GMFCS scores were re-evaluated.

To classify the manual abilities of the children, we used the Manual Ability

Classification System (MACS). The MACS is a functional, five level system for CP

in which levels are based on the children’s ability to handle objects and their need

for assistance or adaptations to perform manual tasks in daily life.17 The MACS

was scored by both the investigator (ER) and the parents. Higher levels of GMFCS

and MACS indicate serious impairment of functional abilities (Table 1).

To determine the children’s neurological status, we administered Touwen’s

neurological examination to children without CP and those with GMFCS levels 1

and 2.18 This age-specific neurological examination assesses posture and muscle

tone, reflexes, presence of choreiform dyskinesia, coordination and balance, fine

manipulative ability, cranial nerves, associated movements, and sensory integrity.19

89Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Cognitive Outcome

Total, verbal and performance intelligence were assessed using a short form of the

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third version (WISC-III) in children over 5

years of age.20 In younger children, we used the Wechsler Preschool and Primary

Scale of Intelligence, revised (WPPSI-R).21

In addition, we assessed visual perception, visuomotor integration and verbal

memory to investigate whether these neuropsychological functions were

specifically impaired in preterm infants with PVHI. Central visual perception was

assessed using two subtests (visual form-constancy and visual closure) of the Test

of Visual-Perceptual Skills, revised (TVPS-R).22 We used the Beery developmental

test of visual-motor integration (VMI, 4th Edition) to assess visuomotor integration.23

Visuomotor integration involves the integration of visual information with finger-

hand movements.

In children over 5 years of age, we assessed verbal memory using Rey’s Auditory

Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).24 This test consists of five learning trials with immediate

recall of words (tested after each presentation), a delayed recall trail and a delayed

recognition trial.24

Test scores obtained when a child was inattentive or too tired, as assessed by the

experimenter, were excluded.

Behavioral Outcome

In order to obtain information on children’s competencies and behavioral/emotional

problems the parents of children over 5 years of age completed the Child Behavior

Checklist (CBCL).25 The CBCL consists of one total scale and two subscales, i.e.

internalizing problems (withdrawn behavior, somatic complaints, and anxious/

depressed scales) and externalizing problems (delinquent and aggressive behavior

scales).

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

In addition, the parents and the teachers filled in the Behavior Rating Inventory of

Executive Function (BRIEF)26 for children over 4 years of age. The BRIEF assesses

executive functioning involved in well-organized, purposeful, goal-directed,

and problem-solving behavior. Examples of executive functioning are the ability

to inhibit competing actions of attractive stimuli, the flexibility to shift problem-

solving strategies if necessary, and the ability to monitor and evaluate one’s own

behavior.

Statistical Analyses

IQ scores were calculated by the median of scores on the verbal and performance

subtests. We used the percentiles on the standardization samples of each cognitive

test to classify the scores into ‘normal (>P15)’, ‘subclinical (P5-P15)’ and ‘clinical

(<P5)’. For the CBCL and the BRIEF we used a similar classification following the

criteria in the manual. We corrected the scores on visuomotor integration, visual

perception and verbal memory for mental age, which was calculated by using

the IQ scores. We used the Chi2, the Mann-Whitney U test and Spearman’s rank

correlation where appropriate to relate motor, cognitive and behavioral outcomes

to cerebral characteristics and late onset morbidity. Throughout the analyses p<.05

was considered to be statistically significant. SPSS 14.0 software for Windows,

(SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) was used for all the analyses.

91Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Results

Between 1995 and 2003, 3034 patients with a gestational age of < 37 weeks were

admitted to our NICU. After database search and review of the cranial ultrasound

scans, 38 infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarctions were included in the

study. Of these 38 infants, 15 (39%) died in the neonatal period. Twenty one of the

23 survivors participated in the follow-up program – two sets of parents declined

the invitation to participate.

Patient Characteristics

Table 2 shows an overview of the patient demographics and the cerebral

characteristics of the 21 surviving infants. The demographics of the two children

who did not participate were slightly better than the children included in our study

(median gestational age 31.8 weeks and median birth weight 1178 g).

Eight infants developed PHVD in addition to the PVHI. One of them did not require

drainage of cerebrospinal fluid, three infants required a temporary ventricular

drain and four were fitted with a permanent shunt. In one infant bilateral subtle

echodensities in the periventricular region, not related to the PVHI, developed into

cystic PVL.

The mean age at follow-up was 8.7 years (range 4.4 to 12.5 years). Three children

were 4 to 5 years old. In these three children some cognitive tests were not

administered because norms were only available for children of 5 years and older.

None of the children were blind.

Motor Outcome

Of 21 children, five (24%) were neurologically normal and sixteen (76%) had CP.

Thirteen children had unilateral CP with limited functional impairment, GMFCS

levels 1 (n=6) and 2 (n=7). One child with GMFCS level 1 did not have CP but

showed limited functional disabilities and minor neurological dysfunction. Three

children had GMFCS levels 3 (n=1) and 4 (n=2). These three infants all had bilateral

spastic CP. The functional manual abilities were normal in four children, eight

children had MACS level 1, seven had level 2, and two had level 3.

According to the neurological examination (assessed in eighteen children),

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

coordination and balance (n=11), associated movements (n=10) and fine

manipulative abilities (n=9) were affected most.

Cognitive Outcome

The results on intelligence are shown in Table 3. Total IQ was distributed normally

in our study group. Mean and median total IQ was 83 (range 55-103, SD 11).

Median verbal IQ was 88 (range 55-115), median performance IQ was 80 (range

40-100). Forty percent of the infants had a total IQ of 85 or more, thus falling

within the normal range. Of 21 children, twelve attended normal education and

nine attended special education. Of these nine, two went to special schools for

children with physical impairment and seven attended special schools for children

with learning problems.

Visual perception (TVPS, n=16) was normal in ten children, subclinical in three and

clinical in another three children. Visuomotor integration (VMI, n=19) was normal

in eight children, subclinical in four and clinical in seven children. Verbal memory

(AVLT, n=13) was normal in five children, subclinical in five and clinical in three

children. We corrected these scores for mental age to investigate whether a poor

performance on the neuropsychological tests was due to intelligence. These data

are shown in Table 3. One child was excluded from the calculations on the AVLT

because her mental age, based on the WISC-III, was below 6 years.

Behavioral Outcome

The results of the CBCL and the BRIEF are shown in Table 3. According to the

CBCL 35% of the children fell in the clinical range. These children had various

behavioral problems that were not particular for one scale.

The Global Executive Composite score showed that 18% of the children fell in the

clinical range according to the parents and 29% fell in this range according to the

teachers. The executive functioning scales most affected according to the parents

were monitor (24%), working memory (22%) and inhibit (22%). The scales most

affected according to the teachers were working memory (67%), initiate (42%),

monitor (41%), and shift (39%).

93Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Cranial Ultrasound Findings

Of the three children with bilateral spastic CP, one had bilateral PVHI and one had

cystic PVL.

The characteristics of the PVHI (i.e. localization and extension) were not related

to the functional motor outcome, intelligence, visual perception, visuomotor

integration and most items on behavior. In this respect verbal memory formed

an exception. We found that two of the five children with the most extended

PVHI (fronto-parieto-occipital) we tested had abnormal scores on verbal memory,

compared to one out of ten with non-extended PVHI. With regards to behavior,

parieto-occipital PVHI was related to abnormal scores on the Global Executive

Composite score (Chi2, p=.019) and abnormal behavior monitoring (Chi2, p=.012)

according to the teachers.

PHVD was associated with several functional limitations. Children with PHVD were

affected in their fine manipulative abilities more so than children without PHVD

(Chi2, p=.016). Furthermore, they had poorer cognitive functions (Table 4). Cystic

PVL was the only risk factor for a higher degree of functional motor disabilities.

Performance IQ was extremely low in the one child with cystic PVL (40) and this

child’s verbal IQ and other cognitive functions could not be assessed.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TAB

LE 1

Des

crip

tions

of

GM

FC

S a

nd M

AC

S le

vels

Leve

l G

MFC

S

MA

CS

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

ob

ject

s ea

sily

and

suc

cess

fully

. At

mos

t, t

hey

exp

erie

nce

limita

tions

in t

he e

ase

of p

erfo

rmin

g m

anua

l tas

ks r

equi

ring

spee

d a

nd a

ccur

acy.

No

rest

rictio

ns in

the

ir in

dep

end

ence

to

per

form

dai

ly a

ctiv

ities

.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

mos

t ob

ject

s b

ut w

ith r

educ

ed q

ualit

y an

d/o

r sp

eed

of a

chie

vem

ent.

Cer

tain

act

iviti

es m

ay b

e av

oid

ed o

r ar

e ac

hiev

ed w

ith

som

e d

ifficu

lty; a

ltern

ativ

e w

ays

of p

erfo

rmin

g ac

tiviti

es m

ay b

e us

ed.

Usu

ally

no

rest

rictio

ns in

the

ir in

dep

end

ence

to

per

form

dai

ly a

ctiv

ities

.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

ob

ject

s w

ith d

ifficu

lty; n

eed

hel

p t

o p

rep

are

and

/

or m

odify

act

iviti

es. P

erfo

rman

ce is

slo

w a

nd a

chie

ved

with

lim

ited

succ

ess

rega

rdin

g q

ualit

y an

d q

uant

ity. A

ctiv

ities

are

per

form

ed

ind

epen

den

tly if

the

y ha

ve b

een

set

up o

r ad

apte

d.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

a li

mite

d s

elec

tion

of e

asily

man

aged

ob

ject

s in

adap

ted

situ

atio

ns. P

arts

of a

ctiv

ities

are

per

form

ed w

ith e

ffort

and

with

limite

d s

ucce

ss. R

equi

re c

ontin

uous

sup

por

t an

d a

ssis

tanc

e an

d/o

r

adap

ted

eq

uip

men

t, e

ven

for

par

tial a

chie

vem

ent

of t

he a

ctiv

ity.

Chi

ldre

n ar

e un

able

to

hand

le o

bje

cts

and

are

sev

erel

y lim

ited

in t

heir

abili

ty t

o p

erfo

rm e

ven

sim

ple

act

ions

. Req

uire

tot

al a

ssis

tanc

e.

1 C

hild

ren

can

wal

k w

ithou

t re

stric

tions

but

hav

e

limita

tions

in m

ore

adva

nced

gro

ss m

otor

ski

lls,

e.g.

coo

rdin

atio

n an

d b

alan

ce.

2 C

hild

ren

can

wal

k w

ithou

t as

sist

ive

dev

ices

but

have

lim

itatio

ns in

wal

king

out

doo

rs a

nd in

the

com

mun

ity, e

.g. r

unni

ng a

nd ju

mp

ing.

3 C

hild

ren

can

wal

k w

ith a

ssis

tive

mob

ility

dev

ices

and

hav

e lim

itatio

ns in

wal

king

out

doo

rs a

nd in

the

com

mun

ity.

4 C

hild

ren

with

lim

itatio

ns in

sel

f-m

obili

ty, t

hese

child

ren

use

pow

er m

obili

ty o

utd

oors

and

in t

he

com

mun

ity.

5 C

hild

ren

have

sev

erel

y lim

ited

sel

f-m

obili

ty, e

ven

with

the

use

of a

ssis

tive

tech

nolo

gy.

95Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

TAB

LE 1

Des

crip

tions

of

GM

FC

S a

nd M

AC

S le

vels

Leve

l G

MFC

S

MA

CS

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

ob

ject

s ea

sily

and

suc

cess

fully

. At

mos

t, t

hey

exp

erie

nce

limita

tions

in t

he e

ase

of p

erfo

rmin

g m

anua

l tas

ks r

equi

ring

spee

d a

nd a

ccur

acy.

No

rest

rictio

ns in

the

ir in

dep

end

ence

to

per

form

dai

ly a

ctiv

ities

.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

mos

t ob

ject

s b

ut w

ith r

educ

ed q

ualit

y an

d/o

r sp

eed

of a

chie

vem

ent.

Cer

tain

act

iviti

es m

ay b

e av

oid

ed o

r ar

e ac

hiev

ed w

ith

som

e d

ifficu

lty; a

ltern

ativ

e w

ays

of p

erfo

rmin

g ac

tiviti

es m

ay b

e us

ed.

Usu

ally

no

rest

rictio

ns in

the

ir in

dep

end

ence

to

per

form

dai

ly a

ctiv

ities

.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

ob

ject

s w

ith d

ifficu

lty; n

eed

hel

p t

o p

rep

are

and

/

or m

odify

act

iviti

es. P

erfo

rman

ce is

slo

w a

nd a

chie

ved

with

lim

ited

succ

ess

rega

rdin

g q

ualit

y an

d q

uant

ity. A

ctiv

ities

are

per

form

ed

ind

epen

den

tly if

the

y ha

ve b

een

set

up o

r ad

apte

d.

Chi

ldre

n ha

ndle

a li

mite

d s

elec

tion

of e

asily

man

aged

ob

ject

s in

adap

ted

situ

atio

ns. P

arts

of a

ctiv

ities

are

per

form

ed w

ith e

ffort

and

with

limite

d s

ucce

ss. R

equi

re c

ontin

uous

sup

por

t an

d a

ssis

tanc

e an

d/o

r

adap

ted

eq

uip

men

t, e

ven

for

par

tial a

chie

vem

ent

of t

he a

ctiv

ity.

Chi

ldre

n ar

e un

able

to

hand

le o

bje

cts

and

are

sev

erel

y lim

ited

in t

heir

abili

ty t

o p

erfo

rm e

ven

sim

ple

act

ions

. Req

uire

tot

al a

ssis

tanc

e.

TABLE 2 Patient demographics

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or as numbers (percentage).1. Resuscitation was scored in case of external heart massage and/or use of epinephrine2. Includes frontal, parietal, and frontoparietal infarctions3. Includes parieto-occipital and occipital infarctionsAbbreviations: IUGR- intrauterine growth restriction; IPPV- intermittent positive pressure ventilation; HFO- high frequency oscillation; PVHI- periventricular hemorrhagic infarction; GMH- germinal matrix hemorrhage; PHVD- posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation

Number n=21Males/females 12/ 9 Gestational age (weeks) 27.6 (25.4-35.0)Birth weight (grams) 1045 (700-2430)IUGR (<P10) n=1 (5)

AsphyxiaResuscitation1 n=3 (14)Apgar at 5 minutes (n=21) 8 (3-10) Umbilical cord pH (n=17) 7.21(6.83-7.42)

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO) n=19 (90)Neonatal seizures (clinical) n=3 (14)Late onset morbidity

Retinopathy of prematurity (grade 1-2 vs. 3 and worse) n=7 (33) vs. n=2 (10)Necrotizing enterocolitis n=1 (5)Late onset sepsis n=4 (19)Bronchopulmonary dysplasia n=12 (57)

Socioeconomic status Below average n=6 (30)Average n=6 (30)Above average n=8 (40)

Characteristics of PVHIFrontoparietal2 n=5 (24)Parieto-occipital3 n=11 (52)Fronto-parieto-occipital n=5 (24)Left / Right n=10 (48) vs. n=9 (43)Bilateral n=2 (9)Porencephalic cysts n=12 (57)

Additional cerebral abnormalitiesGrade 1-2 GMH n=17 (33)Grade 3 GMH n=4 (19)Unilateral GMH n=12 (57)Bilateral GMH n=9 (43)PHVD n=8 (38)

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 3 Cognitive and behavioral outcome in preterm infants with PVHI

Normal2 Subclinical2 Clinical2

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=19) 8 (42) 9 (48) 2 (11)

Verbal intelligence (n=19) 14 (73) 3 (16) 2 (11)

Performance intelligence (n=21) 7 (33) 10 (48) 4 (19)

Visual perception (n=16)1 14 (88) 2 (12) 0 (0)

Form-constancy (n=15) 12 (80) 2 (13) 1 (7)

Visual closure (n=14) 9 (65) 3 (21) 2 (14)

Visuomotor integration (n=19)1 14 (74) 3 (16) 2 (10)

Verbal memory (n=12)1 6 (50) 3 (25) 3 (25)

Delayed recall 6 (50) 3 (25) 3 (25)

Recognition 10 (77) 2 (23)

Behavioral outcome

Total behavioral Problems (n=17) 9 (53) 2 (12) 6 (35)

Internalizing problems (n=17) 12 (70) 2 (12) 3 (18)

Externalizing problems (n=17) 14 (82) 2 (12) 1 (6)

Executive functioning (parents) (n=17) 11 (65) 3 (18) 3 (18)

Executive functioning (teachers) (n=17) 5 (29) 7 (41) 5 (29)

Data are given as numbers (percentage).1. After correcting for mental age2. Normal was defined as >P15, subclinical as P5-P15 and clinical <P5, with regard to intelligence,

normal was defined as IQ 85-115, subclinical as IQ 70-85 and clinical as IQ <70

97Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

TAB

LE 4

Co

gni

tive

and

beh

avio

ral o

utco

me

acco

rdin

g t

o c

rani

al u

ltras

oun

d fi

ndin

gs

Dat

a ar

e gi

ven

as n

umb

ers

(per

cent

age)

. ns-

not

sig

nific

ant

* A

fter

cor

rect

ing

for

men

tal a

ge1.

Nor

mal

was

defi

ned

as

>P

15, s

ubcl

inic

al a

s P

5-P

15 a

nd c

linic

al <

P5,

with

reg

ard

to

inte

llige

nce,

nor

mal

was

defi

ned

as

IQ 8

5-11

5, s

ubcl

inic

al a

s

IQ 7

0-85

and

clin

ical

as

IQ <

70

Cog

nitiv

e ou

tcom

e

Tota

l int

ellig

ence

(n=

19)

Verb

al in

telli

genc

e (n

=19

)

Per

form

ance

inte

llige

nce

(n=

21)

Vis

ual p

erce

ptio

n* (n

=16

)

Vis

uom

otor

inte

grat

ion*

(n=

19)

Verb

al m

emor

y* (n

=12

)

Beh

avio

ral o

utco

me

Tota

l beh

avio

ral P

rob

lem

s (n

=17

)

Inte

rnal

izin

g p

rob

lem

s (n

=17

)

Ext

erna

lizin

g p

rob

lem

s (n

=17

)

Exe

cutiv

e fu

nctio

ning

(par

ents

) (n=

17)

Exe

cutiv

e fu

nctio

ning

(tea

cher

s) (n

=17

)

8 (6

2)

11 (8

5)

7 (5

4)

9 (9

0)

11 (8

5)

5 (7

1)

5 (5

0)

8 (8

0)

7 (7

0)

6 (6

0)

4 (4

0)

4 (3

1)

1 (8

)

4 (3

1)

1 (1

0)

1 (8

)

2 (2

9)

1 (1

0)

1 (1

0)

2 (2

0)

2 (2

0)

3 (3

0)

1 (8

)

1 (8

)

2 (1

5)

1 (8

)

4 (4

0)

1 (1

0)

1 (1

0)

2 (2

0)

3 (3

0)

3 (5

0)

5 (8

3)

3 (5

0)

1 (2

0)

4 (5

7)

4 (5

7)

7 (1

00)

5 (7

1)

1 (1

4)

5 (8

3)

2 (3

3)

6 (7

5)

1 (1

7)

2 (3

3)

1 (2

0)

1 (1

4)

1 (1

4)

1 (1

4)

4 (5

7)

1 (1

7)

1 (1

7)

2 (2

5)

1 (1

7)

3 (6

0)

2 (2

9)

2 (2

9)

1 (1

4)

2 (2

9)

0.02

8

ns

0.00

8

ns ns

0.01

8

ns ns ns ns ns

Nor

mal

1

PV

HI (

n=13

)

Sub

clin

ical

1C

linic

al1

PV

HI

Nor

mal

1

with

PH

VD

Sub

clin

ical

1

(n=

8) Clin

ical

1

p-v

alue

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Discussion

The present study indicated that the majority of children who had PVHI as preterm

infants developed CP at school age (mean: 8 years), but mostly with limited

functional impairment. On average, their intelligence was 17 IQ points lower than

the average of the norm populations, but a majority (57%) still attended normal

education. Of the other cognitive functions investigated verbal memory was

particularly affected. Behavioral functioning was abnormal in 35% of the children

and executive functioning was abnormal in 18% and 29% (parents and teachers).

Hardly any associations between the characteristics of the PVHI and functional

outcome were found.

The strength of this study is that we investigated outcome at school age (4 to 12

years), which is more reliable than studying outcome at 2 to 3 years of age, as

was the case in previous studies. We investigated motor, cognitive and behavioral

functioning extensively and could therefore reliably describe the overall functional

outcome of preterm children with PVHI.

In the present study 76% of the surviving children developed CP, which is similar

to previous studies, with prevalences ranging from 50% to 85%.2-5,8 Functionally,

motor outcome was only mildly restricted in the majority of the children. This result

was also found by Brouwer et al. They found that 73% of children with PVHI, who

developed CP, have GMFCS levels 1 and 2.7 In our study, children’s coordination

and balance, associated movements and fine manipulative abilities were affected

most. This is in agreement with the main characteristics of CP.15 However, abnormal

reflexes and postural problems, which are also characteristics of CP, were found

in only a minority of our children with CP. The presence of associated movements

indicates neurological dysfunction and may be part of the hemisyndrome, in which

case the neurologically impaired side shows more such movements.18

Previous studies showed no consensus on cognitive outcome in children with

PVHI. Percentages of children with normal cognition range from only 20% up to

79%.4,7,9 In the present study, cognitive tests revealed that intelligence was within

1 SD of the mean of the norm group in 40% of the children and total IQ was

approximately 17 points lower than that of the norm population. At first sight it

seems that only a small proportion of children with PVHI is not affected cognitively.

99Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

However, preterm infants with similar gestational age but without lesions are already

known to have lower IQs. A recent review of Aylward revealed that the IQ scores

of preterm children are approximately 4 to 9 points lower when compared to term

controls.27 A study of Taylor et al., from the same era as our own study, showed

that preterm children had IQ scores that were 10 points lower than controls.28

In this study, mean gestational age, birth weight and age of testing were similar

to those of our study. From our own data it is known that, at 8 years of age, the

mean IQ of preterm children without lesions is 5 points lower than that of the norm

population.29 Therefore, if we take these children as the norm and compare our

group to them, then 60% to 80% of the children would score within one SD of the

mean.

After correcting for mental age, visual perception and visuomotor integration were

normal in the majority of children (88% and 74% respectively). In a previous study,

visuomotor integration in children with intraparenchymal echodensities leading

to porencephaly was impaired in comparison to a control group.30 Vollmer et

al., however, could not demonstrate differences in visuomotor integration after

parenchymal compared to non-parenchymal lesions.31 Our results suggested that

the children’s poorer visual perception and visuomotor integration were related to

a lower intellectual level rather than to a specific visual-perceptual or visuomotor

impairment, since the majority of children scored within the normal range after

correcting for mental age.

By contrast, verbal memory might be a function that is affected in children with

PVHI since, after correcting for mental age, only 50% of the children fell within

the normal range. Especially recall of memorized words was impaired, and not

recognition. Our results on memory are in line with previous studies, which showed

that preterm infants are at risk for deficits in working memory and in recognition

memory.32-34 This is the case especially in infants who sustained white matter

injury.33

Behavioral problems occurred in 35% of the children with PVHI. Reijneveld et al.

reported in 2006 that 13% of 402 preterm children without lesions had behavioral

problems.35 The children had a similar gestational age, birth weight and SES

compared to those in our study. Gray et al. reported in 2004 that 20% of 869

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

preterm children with higher gestational age (mean 32 weeks) and birth weight

(mean 1799 grams), had behavioral problems.36 In the present study, executive

functions were abnormal in 18% and 29% (parents and teachers), compared to

13% in preterm infants without lesions.37 We also found that more children were

in the subclinical group compared to a norm population, while the percentage of

children in the clinical group was comparable.

It is striking that we hardly found associations between particular subtypes of

the PVHI and outcome, since the common understanding is that larger lesions

are prognostic for adverse outcome.8,10 The inability of ultrasound scans to

predict outcome may be due to, for example, small sample size, different medical

interventions or SES. In a previous study involving 36 infants, we did find that the

most extended localization of PVHI (fronto-parieto-occipital) is associated with the

development of CP, but not with the severity at 18 months.2 Two previous studies

reported that more severe PVHI is associated with poorer cognitive outcome.4,9

Sherlock et al. found that children with intraparenchymal echodensities and

porencephaly have lower IQ scores.30 We found no such associations.

Regarding additional cerebral abnormalities, we found that PHVD was a risk

factor for abnormal manipulative abilities and for poorer total and performance

intelligence. This is in line with earlier studies, which also found that PHVD is a risk

factor for lower intelligence. Ment et al. found that ventriculomegaly in 4-year-olds

is an independent predictor of a very low total IQ (<70) compared to intraventricular

hemorrhage with parenchym involvement and other risk factors in age mates.38

Brouwer et al. showed that infants with intraparenchymal lesions and PHVD score

significantly lower on developmental outcome than infants with intraparenchymal

lesions without PHVD at 2 years of age.7

The results on outcome at school age in children with PVHI, as demonstrated by

the present study, were better than was previously thought. Studies on risk factors

for adverse outcome in preterm infants often classify infants with parenchymal

lesions at great risk for adverse outcomes,31,39,40 but when focussing specifically on

preterm infants with PVHI, the present study showed that the majority of children

had only limited functional impairment. It appeared that the characteristics of the

PVHI in themselves were not risk factors for adverse outcome. An explanation for

101Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

this finding might be that the influence of PVHI on short-term outcome decreases

later on in life. Development and learning may have contributed to a better outcome

than previously reported. Little understood processes involving plasticity of the

brain may have contributed to this phenomenon.

This study is subject to potential limitations such as it being a single center study

and the small number of children. It stands to question whether our results are

applicable to other nurseries. We tried to minimize this problem by the use of

standardized testing. Another limitation is that we did not include neuroimaging in

our follow-up program. We therefore may have missed small but relevant additional

lesions.

Hence, our results should be interpreted with caution. Further research with larger

groups is needed to investigate outcome in adulthood after PVHI at preterm age.

Conclusions

The majority of surviving preterm infants with PVHI had CP with limited functional

impairment at school age. In 60% to 80% of the children intelligence was within

1 SD of the mean of preterm children without lesions. Verbal memory in particular

may be affected by the PVHI. Behavioral functioning was abnormal in 35% of

the children and executive functioning was abnormal in 18% and 29% (parents

and teachers). Hardly any associations existed between the characteristics of the

PVHI and functional outcome. PHVD was an independent risk factor for lower

intelligence (performance and total) and fine manipulative abilities. Functional

outcome at school age of preterm children with PVHI is better than previously

reported.

Acknowledgements

We greatly acknowledge the help of Dr Titia Brantsma - van Wulfften Palthe in

Utrecht, for correcting the English. Further, we acknowledge the help of Drs Jorien

M. Kerstjens and Henk J. ter Horst for reviewing the ultrasound scans, and Dr. Mijna

Hadders-Algra for her suggestions regarding a previous version of the manuscript.

This study was part of the research program of the postgraduate school for

Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences (BCN), University of Groningen.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Hamrick SEG, Miller SP, Leonard C, et al. Trends in

severe brain injury and neurodevelopmental outcome

in premature newborn infants: The role of cystic

periventricular leukomalacia. J Pediatr

2004;145:593-599.

2. Roze E, Kerstjens JM, Ter Horst HJ, Maathuis CGB,

Bos AF. Risk factors for adverse outcome in preterm

infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction.

Pediatrics 2008;122:e46-e52.

3. de Vries LS, Roelants-van Rijn AM, Rademaker KJ,

van Haastert I, Beek FJ, Groenendaal F. Unilateral

parenchymal haemorrhagic infarction in the preterm

infant. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2001;5:139-149.

4. Guzzetta F, Shackelford GD, Volpe S, Perlman

JM, Volpe JJ. Periventricular intraparenchymal

echodensisties in the premature newborn: critical

determinant of neurological outcome. Pediatrics

1986;78:995-1006.

5. Bassan H, Feldman HA, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Periventricular hemorrhagic infarction: Risk factors

and neonatal outcome. Pediatr Neurol

2006;35:85-92.

6. Larroque B, Marret S, Ancel PY, et al. White matter

damage and intraventricular hemorrhage in very

preterm infants: the EPIPAGE study. J Pediatr

2003;143:477-483.

7. Brouwer A, Groenendaal F, van Haastert

IL, Rademaker K, Hanlo P, de Vries L.

Neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm infants

with severe intraventricular hemorrhage and therapy

for post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation. J Pediatr

2008;152:648-654.

8. Rademaker KJ, Groenendaal F, Jansen GH, Eken P,

de Vries LS. Unilateral haemorrhagic parenchymal

lesions in the preterm infant: shape, site and

prognosis. Acta Paediatr 1994;83:602-608.

9. Bassan H, Limperopoulos C, Visconti K, et al.

Neurodevelopmental outcome in survivors of

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics

2007;120:785-792.

10. Dudink J, Lequin M, Weisglas-Kuperus N,

Conneman N, Goudoever JV, Govaert P. Venous

subtypes of preterm periventricular haemorrhagic

infarction. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed

2008;93:F201-F206.

11. Bassan H, Benson CB, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Ultrasonographic features and severity scoring

of periventricular hemorrhagic infarction in

relation to risk factors and outcome. Pediatrics

2006;117:2111-2118.

12. Volpe JJ. Intraventricular hemorrhage in the

premature infant - current concepts. Part II. Ann

Neurol 1989;25:109-116.

13. Evans WA. An encephalographic ratio for estimating

ventricular enlargement and cerebral atrophy. Arch

Neurol Psychiatry 1942;47:931-937.

References

103Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

14. de Vries LS, Eken P, Dubowitz LMS. The spectrum

of leukomalacia using cranial ultrasound. Behav

Brain Res 1992;49:1-6.

15. Bax M, Goldstein M, Rosenbaum P, et al. Proposed

definition and classification of cerebral palsy, April

2005. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:571-576.

16. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D,

Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability

of a system to classify gross motor function in

children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

17. Morris C, Kurinczuk JJ, Fitzpatrick R, Rosenbaum

PL. Reliability of the manual ability classification

system for children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med

Child Neurol 2006;48:950-953.

18. Touwen BCL. Examination of the Child with Minor

Neurological Dysfunction. 2nd ed. London, UK:

William Heinemann Medical Books Ltd.; 1979.

19. Hadders-Algra M. Two distinct forms of minor

neurological dysfunction: perspectives emerging

from a review of data of the Groningen Perinatal

Project. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002;44:561-571.

20. Kort W, Compaan EL, Bleichrodt N, et al. WISC-III

NL Handleiding. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: NIP

dienstencentrum; 2002.

21. van der Steene G, Bos A. Wechsler Preschool and

Primary Scale of Intelligence, revised. Vlaams-

Nederlandse aanpassing, handleiding. Lisse, the

Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

22. Gardner MF. Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills (Non-

Motor) – Revised. Hydesville, CA: Psychological and

Educational Publications Inc; 1996.

23. Beery KE BN. Developmental Test of Visual-Motor

Integration, 4th edition, revised ed. Parsippany, NJ:

Modern Curriculum Press; 1997.

24. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545-559.

25. Verhulst FC, van der Ende J, Koot HM. Manual

for the Child Behavior Checklist: 4–18 (In Dutch).

Rotterdam: Department of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, Erasmus University; 1996.

26. Gioia GA ,Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L.

BRIEF—Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive

Function: professional manual. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

27. Aylward GP. Cognitive and neuropsychological

outcomes: more than IQ scores. Ment Retard Dev

Disabil Res Rev 2002;8:234-240.

28. Taylor HG, Klein N, Minich NM, Hack M. Middle-

school-age outcomes in children with very low

birthweight. Child Dev 2000;71:1495-1511.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

29. van Braeckel KNJA, Butcher PR, Geuze RH, van

Duijn MAJ, Bos AF, Bouma JM. Less efficient

elementary visuomotor processes in 7- to 10-year

old preterm born children without cerebral palsy:

an indication of impaired dorsal stream processes.

Neuropsychology 2008;in press.

30. Sherlock RL, Synnes AR, Eckstein GR, et al. Long

term outcome after neonatal intraparenchymal

echodensities with porencephaly. Arch Dis Child

Fetal Neonatal Ed 2008;93:F127-31

31. Vollmer B, Roth S, Riley K, et al. Long-term

neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm children

with unilateral cerebral lesions diagnosed by

neonatal ultrasound. Early Hum Dev

2006;82:655-661.

32. de Haan M, Bauer PJ, Georgieff MK, Nelson CA.

Explicit memory in low-risk infants aged 19 months

born between 27 and 42 weeks of gestation. Dev

Med Child Neurol

2000;42:304-312.

33. Woodward LJ, Edgin JO, Thompson D, Inder TE.

Object working memory deficits predicted by early

brain injury and development in the preterm infant.

Brain 2005;128:2578-2587.

34. Vicari S, Caravale B, Carlesimo GA, Casadei AM,

Allemand F. Spatial working memory deficits in

children at ages 3-4 who were low birth weight,

preterm infants. Neuropsychology

2004;18:673-678.

35. Reijneveld SA, de Kleine MJ, van Baar AL, et al.

Behavioural and emotional problems in very preterm

and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Arch

Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2006;91:F423-F428.33.

36. Gray RF, Indurkhya A, McCormick MC. Prevalence,

stability, and predictors of clinically significant

behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3,

5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics

2004;114:736-743.

37. Anderson PJ, Doyle LW. Executive functioning in

school-aged children who were born very preterm

or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s.

Pediatrics 2004;114:50-57.

38. Ment LR, Vohr B, Allan W et al. The etiology and

outcome of cerebral ventriculomegaly at term in

very low birth weight preterm infants. Pediatrics

1999;104:243-248.

39. Sherlock RL, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW.

Neurodevelopmental sequelae of intraventricular

haemorrhage at 8 years of age in a regional cohort

of ELBW/very preterm infants. Early Hum Dev

2005;81:909-916.

40. Hoekstra RE, Ferrara TB, Couser RJ, Payne

NR, Connett JE. Survival and long-term

neurodevelopmental outcome of extremely

premature infants born at 23-26 weeks’ gestational

age at a tertiary center. Pediatric 2004;113:e1-e6.

105Chapter 4 Functional Outcome at School Age of Preterm Infants with Periventricular Hemorrhagic Infarction

Chapter 5

LONG-TERM NEUROLOGICAL OUTCOME OF

TERM-BORN CHILDREN TREATED WITH TWO

OR MORE ANTI-EPILEPTIC DRUGS IN THE

NEONATAL PERIOD

MARISKA J. VAN DER HEIDE, ELISE ROZE, CHRISTA N. VAN DER VEERE,

HENDRIK J. TER HORST, OEBELE F. BROUWER, AREND F. BOS

EARLY HUMAN DEVELOMENT 2011; DOI 10.1016/J.EARLHUMDEV.2011.06.012

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Background

Neonatal seizures may persist despite treatment with multiple anti-epileptic drugs

(AEDs).

Objective

To determine in term-born infants with seizures that required two or more

AEDs, whether treatment efficacy and/or the underlying disorder were related to

neurological outcome.

Design/Methods

We included 82 children (born 1998-2006) treated for neonatal seizures. We recorded

mortality, aetiology of seizures, the number of AEDs required, achievement of seizure

control, and amplitude-integrated-EEG (aEEG) background patterns. Follow-up

consisted of an age-adequate neurological examination. Surviving children were

classified as normal, having mild neurological abnormalities, or cerebral palsy

(CP).

Results

Forty-seven infants (57%) had status epilepticus. The number of AEDs was not

related to neurological outcome. Treatment with three or four AEDs as opposed

to two showed a trend towards an increased risk of a poor outcome, i.e. death or

CP, odds ratio (OR) 2.74; 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.98-7.69; p=.055. Failure to

achieve seizure control increased the risk of poor outcome, OR 6.77; 95%-CI 1.42-

32.82, p=.016. Persistently severely abnormal aEEG background patterns also

increased this risk, OR 3.19; 95%-CI 1.90-5.36; p<.001. In a multivariate model

including abnormal aEEG background patterns, failure to achieve seizure control

nearly reached significance towards an increased risk of poor outcome, OR 5.72,

95%-CI 0.99–32.97, p=.051. We found no association between seizure aetiology

and outcome.

109Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

Conclusions

In term-born infants with seizures that required two or more AEDs outcome was

poorer if seizure control failed. The number of AEDs required to reach seizure

control and seizure aetiology had limited prognostic value.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Introduction

The incidence of seizures is higher in the neonatal period than in any other period

of life and is estimated at approximately 3 per 1000 live births.1,2 In term infants, the

most common cause of seizures is perinatal asphyxia. Other causes are cerebral

infarction, cerebral haemorrhage, and infection.1,3-5 Neonatal seizures are associated

with the development of neurological impairments in later life, such as cerebral palsy

(CP), microcephaly, epilepsy, and psychomotor retardation.3,4

Several anti-epileptic drugs (AEDs) are used for the treatment of neonatal seizures.

Throughout the world, in accordance to international guidelines, phenobarbitone is

the first choice of treatment. Failing this, a number of other AEDs including midazolam,

lidocaine, phenytoin, diazepam, and clonazepam are available as second-choice

drugs. Seizures may, however, persist in some infants despite treatment with multiple

AEDs. This may either be a reflection of the severity of the underlying brain disorder

that leads to the seizures, or the damaging effect of the recurrent and ongoing seizures

themselves, or a combination of both. Ronen et al. suggest that the necessity to treat

seizures with multiple drugs and the success of anticonvulsive treatment to achieve

seizure control may have prognostic value for neurological outcome.1

To date, the long-term neurodevelopmental outcome of children with recurrent

seizures during the neonatal period is largely unknown. Particularly, the association

between long-term outcome and the number of AEDs required and whether seizure

control had been achieved, is unknown. Our primary aim, therefore, was to determine

whether treatment efficacy, i.e. the number of AEDs required and seizure control,

had prognostic value for outcome in term-born infants treated with two or more

AEDs. Our second aim was to determine whether the underlying disorder causing

the seizures had prognostic value for outcome. Our hypotheses were that a higher

number of AEDs and the seizures persisting despite therapy were associated with

poor outcome, irrespective of the underlying disorder.

111Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

Materials and Methods

Subjects

We selected all the term-born infants that had been admitted to the Neonatal

Intensive Care Unit (NICU) of the University Medical Center Groningen between 1

January 1998 and 31 December 2006 and that had been diagnosed with seizures.

The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of University Medical

Center Groningen. We examined the infants’ medical charts and included those

infants that had been treated with two or more AEDs. We excluded infants with

major chromosomal and congenital anomalies.

Perinatal and Neonatal Clinical Data

From the medical charts we obtained data on gestational age, birth weight, Apgar

scores, and mode of delivery and we noted the age of onset of the seizures in

hours after birth. Asphyxia was diagnosed by the presence of at least two of the

following signs: foetal distress (abnormal cardiotocogram and/or meconium-

stained amniotic fluid), Apgar scores of <5 after 5 minutes, need for artificial

ventilation for >5 minutes, resuscitation (external heart massage and/or the

use of epinephrine), and umbilical cord pH (arterial pH <7.10). We identified the

causes of seizures, based on the combination of findings regarding the history,

physical and neurological examination, laboratory results, e.g. blood glucose, and

neuro-imaging, all obtained during the first weeks after birth. Neuroimaging was

performed by cranial ultrasonography, in many cases complemented by Magnetic

Resonance Imaging.

Amplitude Integrated EEG (aEEG) Patterns

We started recording of infants admitted with seizures or suspected seizures

immediately following admission. The aEEGs were recorded by either one of

two cerebral function monitors (CFM): the analogue Lectromed® Multitrace 2

or the digital Olympic® CFM 6000 (from September 2003 onwards). When the

former was used, it was calibrated every 24 hours. The nursing staff recorded

all nursing and medical procedures including the occurrence of clinical seizures

and the timing of the administration of all medication. The aEEGs were analyzed

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

by pattern recognition based on Toet et al.6 For this study, we assessed the

patterns on the presence of epileptic discharges and status epilepticus (epileptic

state).7 We also analyzed background patterns such as continuous normal voltage

(CNV), discontinuous normal voltage (DNV), continuous low voltage (CLV), burst

suppression (BS), and flat trace (FT).

Treatment of Seizures

We obtained information on treatment with AEDs from the medical and nursery

charts. According to the protocol at our NICU, the drug of first choice was

phenobarbitone at a loading dose of 20 mg/kg, administered intravenously.

Approximately 30 to 60 minutes were allowed to establish, both clinically and

electrographically, whether the seizures had stopped. If they persisted or recurred,

we administered an additional 10 mg/kg phenobarbitone. Our second-choice drug

was lidocaine (2 mg/kg intravenous loading dose, 6 mg/kg/h maintenance therapy)

if the seizures persisted. Midazolam (0.05 mg/kg intravenous loading dose, 0.15

mg/kg/h maintenance therapy) was our third-choice drug. In infants born from

2001 onwards, our second-choice drug for treating seizures was midazolam and

lidocaine our third-choice. We used the same dosing schedules as mentioned

above. During the entire study period clonazepam was our fourth-choice drug (0.1

mg/kg administered intravenously and repeated after 10 minutes, if necessary).

We evaluated the effect of treatment both clinically, as recorded in the medical

charts, and according to the aEEG recordings. We considered treatment with AEDs

to have been successful, i.e. seizure control was achieved, if the seizures stopped

(no more seizures) and did not recur, neither clinically nor electrographically.

Outcome

We determined the mortality rate and cause of death. Follow-up of the survivors

was part of the regular follow-up programme of children that had been admitted

to the NICU, and consisted of paediatric and neurological examinations. Parents

gave their informed consent to participate in the follow-up programme.

The follow-up programme consisted of Hempel’s age-specific neurological

examination for children aged one-and-a-half to four years.8 This examination

113Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

was developed to detect subtle neurological abnormalities by observing the

child during play. It assesses seven subcategories: posture, coordination of trunk

and extremities, fluency and adequacy of movements, sensorimotor functions,

cranial nerves and development.8 Children with abnormalities on two or more

subcategories were diagnosed as having minor neurological dysfunction (MND).9

For children older than four years we used Touwen’s age-specific neurological

examination.10 Eight subcategories were assessed: posture and muscle tone,

reflexes, choreiform dyskinesia, coordination and balance, fine manipulative ability,

cranial nerves, sensory integrity, and rarely occurring dysfunctions including an

excess of associated movements. The children were diagnosed as having MND in

the presence of dysfunction in three or more subcategories.9,10

Following Bax’ criteria, the presence or absence of cerebral palsy (CP) was

determined when infants had reached the age of at least 18 months.11 In case of CP,

gross motor functioning was scored using the Gross Motor Function Classification

System (GMFCS).12 This is a functional, five level classification system for CP based

on self-initiated movement with particular emphasis on sitting (truncal control)

and walking. The higher the level the more serious the impairment of functional

abilities.

Epilepsy at Follow-up

In addition to categorising outcome as normal, mild, or major neurological

abnormalities, we determined the presence of epilepsy during follow-up. Epilepsy

was defined as at least two unprovoked seizures a year during follow-up. In these

cases we also recorded whether anti-epileptic treatment was started.

Statistical Analyses

For all the statistical analyses we used SPSS version 16.0. We first established

whether the infant had survived the neonatal period. Next we classified outcome

into any of four categories: normal, mild neurological abnormalities, major

neurological abnormalities, and death. Children classified as normal showed no

neurological signs or the neurological dysfunction was below the cut-off for MND.

Children classified as having mild neurological abnormalities had MND but no CP.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Children with CP were classified as having major neurologic abnormalities.

We used the X2 test for trend and Fisher’s exact test to evaluate the relations between

seizure aetiology, the number of AEDs, seizure control, and outcome results. To do

logistic regression analyses, we divided the above mentioned four categories into

two groups: ‘good outcome’ (normal and mild neurological abnormalities) and ‘poor

outcome’ (major neurological abnormalities and death). In addition, we assessed the

relation between the presence of epilepsy at follow-up and seizure-related factors

during the neonatal period. Results were expressed in odds ratios (OR) including

95% confidence intervals (CI). Finally, we performed backward multiple logistic

regression analyses to determine which factors were independently associated with

outcome. Throughout the analyses p<.05 was considered statistically significant.

115Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

Results

Between 1998 and 2006, 147 term infants with neonatal seizures were admitted to

our NICU. Fifty-two infants were treated with one drug only and were not included

in the study for that reason. Ninety-five infants were treated with two or more AEDs.

After excluding 13 infants with major congenital or chromosomal abnormalities, 82

children remained.

Patient Characteristics

In Table 1 we present an overview of the patient characteristics and the underlying

causes of the seizures. In all cases the onset of the seizures was from half an hour to

more than five days after birth. Asphyxia was the most common cause of the seizures

(n=55) followed by subdural haemorrhage (n=8), arterial cerebral infarction (n=7), and

infection (n=6). Ten children had two potential causes for the seizures. All ten had

intracranial haemorrhages or cerebral infarctions, combined with asphyxia in five

children, bacterial infection in one child, a combination of subdural/subarachnoidal

and intraparenchymal haemorrhage in two children, and a combination of subdural

haemorrhage and infarction in two children.

aEEG Patterns

The aEEG recordings of the infants showed status epilepticus at admission in 26

infants (32%) and during the clinical course in another 21 infants (26%). In 29 infants

(35%), background activity of the aEEG at admission was scored as DNV or CNV,

in 14 (17%) as BS and in 13 infants (16%) as FT. During admission, aEEG patterns

of 48 infants (59%) remained normal or normalised into CNV or DNV. In 34 infants

(41%) the patterns remained or became severely abnormal, i.e. FT and BS. During

the clinical course, the most severely abnormal aEEG patterns were FT in 21 infants

(26%), BS in 27 (33%), and status epilepticus (without FT or BS) in 16 infants (20%).

Eighteen infants (22%) had consistently normal (CNV or DNV) aEEG patterns.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Number of AEDs and Seizure Control

The number of AEDs required to treat seizures and the achievement of seizure control

are presented in Figure 1. Twenty-one infants (26%) were treated with two AEDs, 36

(44%) with three AEDs, and 25 (30%) with four AEDs. In 21 of the infants (26%)

seizure control was achieved after administering a second AED, in another 27 (33%)

after administering a third drug, and finally in another 17 (21%) after we administered

a fourth drug. In 17 infants (21%) the seizures persisted despite therapy, 8 of these

infants (10%) received four AEDs. Thus, 9 infants received three AEDs although

seizure control was not achieved. Our reasons for not administering a fourth AED

were various: short and/or unrecognised (electrographic) seizures and normal to

high plasma levels of midazolam with the notion that clonazepam belonged to the

same category of AED. Sometimes it remained unresolved. Of the 17 infants in

whom seizures persisted despite therapy, 12 had asphyxia, 2 had arterial cerebral

infarctions, 2 had meningitis, and in 1 infant the cause remained unresolved, but

was presumably based on an unknown inborn error of metabolism. The distribution

of aetiologies and neuroimaging findings of the infants in whom seizure control was

achieved did not differ from that of the infants in whom seizure control failed.

Outcome

Of the 82 infants, 31 (38%) children died. In 22 children intensive care treatment

was withdrawn because of very poor prognosis based on the combined results of

clinical findings, neuroimaging, background patterns of aEEG, and conventional

EEG. Failure to achieve seizure control, whether clinically or on aEEG, was not a

criterion to withdraw intensive care treatment. Nine children died due to combined

respiratory and circulatory failure despite maximum treatment.

Forty-five of the 51 survivors (88%) participated in the follow-up programme. Three

sets of parents declined to participate while another three sets were lost to follow-up.

Thus, outcome was known of 76 children (93%) of the total study group. Seventeen

(38%) of the 45 children examined at follow-up were normal. Thirteen children (29%)

had minor neurological abnormalities. These children all had coordination problems

and most children also had deviations of development, abnormalities in the fluency

and/or adequacy of movements, and/or in fine motor skills. Fifteen children (33%)

117Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

had major neurological abnormalities (14 had spastic CP and one dyskinetic CP)

with GMFCS ranging from levels I to V (median II).

The Number of AEDs in Relation to Outcome

We found no relation between the number of AEDs and outcome (Figure 2A).

Nevertheless, treatment with three or four AEDs as opposed to two AEDs showed

a trend towards an increased risk of neonatal death (OR 2.60; 95% CI 0.85 - 7.97;

p=.095); and of poor outcome (OR 2.74; 95% CI 0.98 - 7.69; p=.055). Treatment with

four AEDs, as opposed to treatment with two or three AEDs, did not increase the risk

of neonatal death or poor outcome. In addition, the number of AEDs was not related

to the cause of the seizures.

Seizure control in relation to outcome

The outcome of children in whom seizure control had not been achieved was worse

than that of children in whom seizure control had been achieved (Figure 2B). Of the

17 children in whom seizure control had not been achieved, 13 (76%) died and of

the 65 in whom seizure control had been achieved, 18 (28%) died. Failure to achieve

seizure control increased the risk of neonatal death considerably (OR 8.49; 95% CI

2.44 - 29.48; p=.001). It was also related to outcome categorised into four categories

(p=.004). The risk of poor outcome, i.e. death and CP, was significantly increased if

seizure control had not been achieved (OR 6.77; 95% CI 1.42 – 32.82; p=.016). Of

the four surviving infants in whom seizure control had not been achieved two were

classified as normal and two had CP with GMFCS levels IV and V (not significant).

The Cause of Seizures and Outcome

The most common cause of seizures was asphyxia. It reached a nearly significant

relation with death in the neonatal period. In the neonatal period, 24 of the 55 children

(44%) with asphyxia died compared to seven out of 27 children (26%) with other

causes underlying the seizures (Fisher’s exact test, p=.094). There were no other

relations between the several causes of the seizures and outcome (Table 2). Taken

together, intracranial haemorrhage (subdural, subarachnoidal, and intraparenchymal

haemorrhage), arterial cerebral infarction, and cerebral venous sinus thrombosis,

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

were not related with outcome either. Finally, the outcome of the ten children with

more than one potential cause for the seizures did not differ from the children with a

single aetiology for seizures.

aEEG Patterns and Outcome

The outcome of the children was significantly related to the course of the aEEG

patterns. Out of the 33 children in whom aEEG patterns became or remained

severely abnormal (FT and BS), 31 had a poor outcome, i.e. death and CP. Still, of

the 43 children in whom background patterns became or remained normal, 15 had

a poor outcome (Fisher’s exact, p<.001). There was a significant relation between an

abnormal background pattern on the last aEEG registration and poor outcome (OR

3.19; 95% CI 1.90-5.36; p< .001).

Status epilepticus was observed on aEEG in 44 of the 76 children of whom

outcome data were available. Twenty of them died, 12 developed CP, 4 had mild

neurological abnormalities and 8 were classified normal. Of the 32 children in whom

status epilepticus was not observed on aEEG, 11 died, 3 developed CP, 9 had

mild abnormalities and 9 were classified normal. This distribution nearly reached

significance (X2 test for trend, p=.074).

Epilepsy at Follow-up

Epilepsy was diagnosed during follow-up in 13 of the 45 surviving children (29%):

ten during the first year, two the second year, and one child during the fourth year.

Epilepsy was more frequent in children with major neurological abnormalities, i.e. CP

(X2 test for trend, p=.004). There was no relation between the development of epilepsy

and the number of AEDs, causes of the seizures, specific neuroimaging findings,

presence of status epilepticus on aEEG, and achievement of seizure control.

Multivariate Analysis

We performed a multivariate logistic regression analysis to investigate which

seizure-related factors contributed independently to death and poor outcome.

Factors which had shown associations with poor outcome at p<.10 were entered

as predictors: seizure control, and treatment with three or four AEDs, as opposed

119Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

to two AEDs. Regarding neonatal death, failure to achieve seizure control remained

in the model (OR 8.49; 95% CI 2.44 - 29.48; p=.001). It explained 20.7% of the

variance. Regarding good and poor outcome, again only failure to achieve seizure

control remained in the model (OR 6.77; 95% CI 1.42 – 32.28, p=.016) explaining

13.5% of the variance. We did not perform a multivariate logistic regression analysis

among survivors to determine independent predictors of neurological development.

The reason for this was that in some groups there were so few survivors so as to

render statistical analyses inappropriate.

We repeated the analyses entering the course of aEEG patterns in the model,

together with seizure control and treatment with three or four AEDs, as opposed to

two AEDs. Regarding good and poor outcome, the course of aEEG patterns (OR

3.08, 95% CI 1.82 – 5.24, p<.0001) and failure to achieve seizure control (OR 5.72,

95% CI 0.99 – 32.97, p=.051) remained independently in the model, of which failure

to achieve seizure control nearly reached significance. The model explained 47.7%

of the variance.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Patient characteristics and aetiology of seizures

Patient characteristics (n=82)

Male / Female 52/30

Gestational age (weeks) 40.0 (36.4 – 42.4)

Birth weight (grams) 3405 (2000 – 5475)

Onset of seizures (hours post partum) 4.0 (0.5- 135)

Age at follow-up (years) 4.0 (1.5 – 9)

Cause of seizures1

Asphyxia2 n=55 (67%)

Subdural haemorrhage n=8 (10%)

Arterial cerebral infarction n=7 (9%)

Infection / meningitis n=6 (7%)

Intraparenchymal haemorrhage n=4 (5%)

Hypoglycaemia3 n=3 (4%)

Miscellaneous causes4 n=2 (2%)

Cerebral venous sinus thrombosis n=2 (2%)

Subarachnoïdal haemorrhage n=1 (1%)

Unknown n=4 (5%)

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or as numbers (percentage).1. Given as percentage of the total group. Since 10 infants had more than one cause for the seizures

the total is > 100%2. Asphyxia was defined as two or more of the following criteria present: Signs of foetal distress

(abnormal cardiotocogram and/or meconium-stained amniotic fluid); Apgar score of <5 after 5 minutes; need for artificial ventilation for more than 5 minutes; resuscitation (external heart massage and/or use of epinephrine); umbilical cord pH <7.10

3. Hypoglycaemia was defined as: blood glucose < 2.0 mmol/L or symptomatic hypoglycaemia with blood glucose <2.6 mmol/L

4. Kernicterus (n=1) and urea cycle disorder (n=1)

121Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

TAB

LE 2

The

mo

st c

om

mo

n ae

tiolo

gie

s o

f re

pea

ted

sei

zure

s in

rel

atio

n to

out

com

e

The

num

ber

s in

the

tab

le r

efer

to

num

ber

s of

chi

ldre

n. P

-val

ues

are

bas

ed o

n th

e χ2

-for

-tre

nd t

est.

1. I

nclu

din

g ar

teria

l inf

arct

ion,

sub

dur

al h

aem

orrh

age,

sub

arac

hnoi

dal

hae

mor

rhag

e,

Intr

apar

ench

ymal

hae

mor

rhag

e, s

inus

thr

omb

osis

and

com

bin

atio

ns 2

. Inc

lud

ing

hyp

ogly

caem

ia

Asp

hyxi

a

Intr

acra

nial

hae

mor

rhag

e,

or in

farc

tion1

Mis

cella

neou

s ca

uses

2

Infe

ctio

n /

men

ingi

tis

Nor

mal

12 4 - 1

Mild

ab

norm

aliti

es

6 3 2 -

Maj

or

abno

rmal

ities

10 5 1 2

Die

d

24 6 - 3

Unk

own

3 - 2 -

p-v

alue

0.31

0.79

0.73

0.42

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Persistent seizures

3rd drug (n=61)

Persistent seizures

4th drug (n=25)

Seizure control

n = 27 (33%)

Seizure control

n = 17 (21%)

Persisting seizures, no other drugs

n = 9 (11%)

Persisting seizures, no other drugs

n = 8 (10%)

Persistent seizures despite first choice

treatment

2nd drug

Seizure control

n = 21 (26%)

Figure 1 Distribution of children that received two, three, and fouranti-epileptic drugs in relation to seizure control

123Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

In six of the children the outcome was unknown. Of these, one child received two drugs, three children received three drugs, and two children received four drugs.

100%

90%

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10%

100%

90%

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10%

Per

cent

age

of

child

ren

Per

cent

age

of

child

ren

n=20

2

n=59

yes

Number of AEDs required

Seizure control

n=33

3

n=17

no

n=23

4

Normal

Mild neurological

abnormalities

Cerebral palsy

Died

Normal

Mild neurological

abnormalities

Cerebral palsy

Died

Figure 2A Achievement of seizure control in relation to outcome

Figure 2B Number of anti-epileptic drugs required in relation to outcome

n=4n=4

n=9

n=7

n=5

n=1

n=4

n=13

n=13

n=15

n=2

n=2

n=7

n=4

n=5

n=18

n=13

n=17

n=9

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Discussion

With this study we demonstrated that seizures persisting despite anticonvulsant

treatment in children receiving two or more AEDs were highly prognostic for poor

outcome. Especially the risk of neonatal death was significantly increased. Moreover,

we found that an increasing number of AEDs required to control seizures had some

prognostic value. Treatment with three or four AEDs, as opposed to two AEDs,

increased the risk of poor outcome: either death or CP. Seizure aetiology was not

associated with outcome.

Our most striking finding was that failure to achieve seizure control was strongly

associated with poor outcome rather than the number of AEDs required. Not

surprisingly, persistently severely abnormal aEEG background patterns were most

predictive of a poor outcome. Previously, the predictive value of aEEG background

patterns have been demonstrated beyond doubt in asphyxiated infants.5-7 In our

study, asphyxia was by far the most common cause of therapy-resistant seizures.

It was found in two thirds of our children. Still, failure to achieve seizure control

remained independently in the multivariate model, although its predictive value for

adverse outcome now just failed to reach statistical significance. Previously, Castro

et al. also reported that seizures persisting despite adequate therapy are prognostic

for adverse outcome.13 They found that outcome at one year was better in children

with seizure control with two or more AEDs than in children with persistent seizures

with single or two-drug treatment.13 Their study group, however, was very small with

only 13 children receiving midazolam as second-choice drug. Ronen et al., in their

study on risk factors for adverse outcome following neonatal seizures, suggested

that the need for multiple AEDs is a sign for poor outcome.1 Unlike us, they also

included children that had been treated with only one AED. It is not clear from their

study whether treatment with two or more AEDs, as opposed to one drug only,

increased the risk of poor outcome. Possibly this fact was responsible for their

findings. Our study on a large, high-risk group of children since they all required two

or more AEDs, makes us believe that in these children achieving seizure control is

more important than the number of AEDs required in the management of neonatal

seizures.

125Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

We did not find a relation between seizure aetiology and outcome. Others have

reported that prognosis is strongly related to the aetiology of the seizures.4 In particular,

asphyxia and cerebral haemorrhage are related to poor outcome.1,4,11 We think that

the discrepancy can be explained by the differences in study populations. The

seizures in the children of our study were difficult to treat because two or more AEDs

were considered clinically necessary. Perhaps the severity of the underlying brain

damage, irrespective of its cause, led to the seizures being impervious to treatment.

We found that asphyxia was a relatively common cause for the seizures among

children that died, but the association just failed to reach statistical significance.

In the present study the age at follow-up ranged from one-and-a half to nine years.

We only investigated neurological and motor functioning; cognitive functions were

not tested in our follow-up programme. Of the surviving children the outcome in

38% was normal, in 29% it was mildly abnormal, and in 33% outcome was severely

abnormal. Other studies on neonatal seizures, without considering the number of

AEDs, report comparable percentages of surviving children with normal outcomes

(35-54%), mildly abnormal outcomes (37%), and severely abnormal outcomes

(28%).1,4,14 This suggests that the requirement of multiple AEDs is not a poor

prognostic sign per se. This is supported by our finding that outcome did not differ

between children that required three versus four AEDs.

We found that 29% of the surviving children developed epilepsy three months or

more after discharge from our NICU. The development of epilepsy was strongly

related to CP. Other studies in term-born infants that do not distinguish between the

number of AEDs, reported percentages ranging from 9.4 to 29%.1,4,14-16 As was the

case in our study, they found a strong association between development of epilepsy

and poor neurological outcome.15 We were unable to identify neonatal risk factors

that were independently associated with the development of epilepsy.

It may well be that cerebral injury is the keystone of a poor response to AEDs. It

explains the adverse outcome in children with seizures persisting despite therapy.

Whether the seizures themselves are harmful for the brain remains unresolved. Several

studies indicated that seizures may disrupt brain development in infants.2,17,18 This

is in line with our findings of a better neurological outcome in the children in whom

seizure control had been achieved. Other studies found the number or duration of

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

seizures to be predictive of poor outcomes which suggests that seizures cause

additional damage.3,14,15,19 Finally, it has been suggested that AEDs interfere with

brain development or even initiate apoptotic neurodegeneration.20,21 Especially the

combination of several different AEDs would cause these changes. This is, however,

highly speculative and could be neither confirmed nor denied by our data.

Strengths of our study are that we took both clinical and subclinical seizures

into consideration and that we included a large number of children with a high

percentage remaining for follow-up. We also recognise some limitations. We only

included children treated with two or more AEDs; we did not include a control group

of children, e.g. children treated with only one drug. Since the latter group is usually

not referred to a NICU, including these children would have biased our results.

Another limitation was the fact that ours was a single-centre study. Even though

referring hospitals follow the same guidelines for treatment, caution should be taken

to generalise our results to other centres. A third limitation was that we do not have

data on cognitive outcome. Finally, our study was retrospective.

Our study may have implications. On the basis of our findings it seems to be more

important to achieve seizure control than to restrict the number of AEDs, irrespective

of the aetiology of the seizures. It might be that newer AEDs such as levetiracetam

and topiramate are more successful in stopping neonatal seizures than the AEDs

currently used. Nevertheless, further research is needed to assess in detail the use

of these drugs in newborns and to find optimal dosing regimens.22

Conclusion

Our study demonstrated that persistent seizures in term-born newborns, despite

multiple AEDs, are prognostic for poor outcome. We also found that treatment with

three or four AEDs, as opposed to two, had some prognostic value. Persistently

severely abnormal aEEG background patterns were most predictive of a poor

outcome. It may well be that cerebral damage is the common underlying factor,

related both to the difficulty in achieving seizure control and to poor neurological

outcome. Additionally, ongoing seizures and AEDs may possibly exacerbate already

existing brain damage. Our findings may have implications for the treatment policy

of neonatal seizures.

127Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

Acknowledgements

This study was part of the research programme of the postgraduate School for

Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Groningen. Elise Roze was

supported by a grant from the Junior Scientific Master Class of the University of

Groningen. We greatly acknowledge the help of Dr. T. Brantsma–van Wulfften Palthe

for correcting the English manuscript.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Ronen GM, Buckley D, Penney S, Streiner DL. Long-

term prognosis in children with neonatal seizures: a

population-based study. Neurology

2007;69:1816-1822.

2. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the Newborn, 5th ed.

Philadelphia, PA: W.B.Saunders; 2008.

3. McBride MC, Laroia N, Guillet R. Electrographic

seizures in neonates correlate with poor

neurodevelopmental outcome. Neurology

2000;55:506-513.

4. Tekgul H, Gauvreau K, Soul J, et al. The current

etiologic profile and neurodevelopmental outcome

of seizures in term newborn infants. Pediatrics

2006;117:1270-1280.

5. Van Rooij LGM, Toet MC, Osredkar D, van Huffelen

AC, Groenendaal F, de Vries LS. Recovery of

amplitude integrated electroencephalographic

background patterns within 24 hours of perinatal

asphyxia. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed

2005;90:F245-F251.

6. Toet MC, Hellstrom-Westas L, Groenendaal F, Eken

P, de Vries LS. Amplitude integrated EEG 3 and 6

hours after birth in full term neonates with hypoxic-

ischaemic encephalopathy. Arch Dis Child Fetal

Neonatal Ed 1999;81:F19-F23.

7. Ter Horst HJ, Sommer C, Bergman KA, Fock JM, van

Weerden TW, Bos AF. The prognostic significance

of aEEG patterns during the first 72 hours after

birth in severely asphyxiated neonates. Pediatr Res

2004;55:1026-1033.

8. Hempel MS. Neurological development during

toddling age in normal children and children at risk of

developmental disorders. Early Hum Dev

1993;34:47-57.

9. Hadders-Algra M. Developmental coordination

disorder: is clumsy motor behavior caused by

a lesion of the brain at early age? Neural Plast

2003;10:39-50.

10. Touwen BCL. Examination of the Child with Minor

Neurological Dysfunction, 2nd ed. London, UK:

William Heinemann Medical Books Ltd.; 1979.

11. Bax M, Goldstein M, Rosenbaum P, et al. Proposed

definition and classification of cerebral palsy, April

2005. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:571-576.

12. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D,

Wood E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability

of a system to classify gross motor function in

children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

13. Castro Conde JR, Hernandez Borges AA,

Domenech Martinez E, Gonzalez Campo C, Perera

Soler R. Midazolam in neonatal seizures with no

response to phenobarbital. Neurology 2005;64:876-

879.

14. Toet MC, Groenendaal F, Osredkar D, van Huffelen

AC, de Vries LS. Postneonatal epilepsy following

amplitude-integrated EEG-detected neonatal

seizures. Pediatr Neurol 2005;32:241-247.

References

129Chapter 5 Long-Term Neurological Outcome of Term-Born Children Treated with Two or More Anti-Epileptic Drugs in the Neonatal Period

15. Pisani F, Cerminara C, Fusco C, Sisti L. Neonatal

status epilepticus vs recurrent neonatal seizures

- Clinical findings and outcome. Neurology

2007;69:2177-185.

16. Kwong KL, Wong SN, So KT. Epilepsy in children

with cerebral palsy. Pediatr Neurol 1998;19:31-36.

17. Rennie JM, Boylan GB. Neonatal seizures and their

treatment. Curr Opin Neurol 2003;16:177-181.

18. Thibeault-Eybalin MP, Lortie A, Carmant L. Neonatal

Seizures: Do They Damage the Brain? Pediatr

Neurol 2009;40:175-180.

19. Klinger G, Chin CN, Beyene J, Perlman M.

Predicting the outcome of neonatal bacterial

meningitis. Pediatrics 2000;106:477-482.

20. Kaindl AM, Sifiringer M, Koppelstaetter A, et al.

Erythropoietin Protects the Developing Brain from

Hyperoxia-Induced Cell Death and Proteome

Changes. Ann Neurol 2008;64:523-534.

21. Bittigau P, Sifringer M, Genz K, et al. Antiepileptic

drugs and apoptotic neurodegeneration in the

developing brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A

2002;99:15089-15094.

22. Furwentsches A, Bussmann C, Ramantani G, et al.

Levetiracetam in the treatment of neonatal seizures:

a pilot study. Seizure 2010;19:185-189.

Chapter 6

TRACTOGRAPHY OF THE CORTICOSPINAL

TRACTS IN INFANTS WITH FOCAL PERINATAL

INJURY: COMPARISON WITH NORMAL

CONTROLS AND TO MOTOR DEVELOPMENT

ELISE ROZE, POLLY A. HARRIS, GARETH BALL, LEIRE Z. ELORZA, RODRIGO M.

BRAGA, JOANNA M. ALLSOP, NAZAKAT MERCHANT, EMMA PORTER, TOMOKI

ARICHI, A. DAVID EDWARDS, MARY A. RUTHERFORD, FRANCES M. COWAN,

SERENA J. COUNSELL

NEURORADIOLOGY 2011; ACCEPTED

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Purpose

Our aims were 1. to assess the corticospinal tracts (CSTs) in infants with focal injury

and healthy term controls using probabilistic tractography and 2. to correlate the

conventional MRI and tractography findings in infants with focal injury with their later

motor function.

Methods

We studied 20 infants with focal lesions and 23 controls using magnetic resonance

imaging (MRI) and diffusion tensor imaging. Tract volume, fractional anisotropy (FA),

apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC) values, axial (AD) and radial diffusivity (RD) of

the CSTs were determined. Asymmetry indices (AIs) were calculated by comparing

ipsilateral to contralateral CSTs. Motor outcome was assessed using a standardized

neurological examination.

Results

Conventional MRI was able to predict normal motor development (n=9) or hemiplegia

(n=6). In children who developed a mild motor asymmetry (n=5), conventional MRI

predicted a hemiplegia in 2 and normal motor development in 3 infants. The AIs for

tract volume, FA, ADC and RD showed a significant difference between controls and

infants who developed a hemiplegia and RD also showed a significant difference in

AI between controls and infants who developed a mild asymmetry.

Conclusion

Conventional MRI was able to predict subsequent normal motor development or

hemiplegia following focal injury in newborn infants. Measures of RD obtained from

diffusion tractography may offer additional information for predicting a subsequent

asymmetry in motor function.

133Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Introduction

Neonatal arterial ischemic stroke (AIS) and hemorrhagic parenchymal infarction (HPI)

can be reliably identified on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). AIS most commonly

affects the territory of the left middle cerebral artery (MCA)1 whilst HPI occurs mainly

in preterm infants affecting tissue adjacent to the body of the lateral ventricles.

Motor impairment and hemiplegia frequently follow focal lesions in the region of the

corticospinal tracts (CST).2-5 Conventional MRI studies have demonstrated that injury

to the basal ganglia, posterior limb of the internal capsule (PLIC), and hemispheric

tissue following AIS is associated with hemiplegia, whereas injury to only one or two

of these sites is associated with a good motor outcome.2 In preterm infants with HPI

asymmetry in the signal intensity in the PLIC on MRI at term equivalent age (TEA) is

associated with the development of a hemiplegia.5

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is an MRI technique that characterizes the diffusion

properties of water molecules in tissue. In cerebral white matter (WM) water diffuses

preferentially along the direction of axons (axial diffusion, AD) and is relatively

restricted perpendicular to axons (radial diffusion, RD), this directional dependence

is termed anisotropy. Quantitative measures derived from DTI provide objective and

reproducible assessment of WM and have provided insights into neonatal brain

development and injury.6-9 Additionally, DTI allows visualization of WM tracts in-vivo.

In diffusion tractography it is assumed that the direction of greatest diffusion in a voxel

is parallel to the underlying dominant fibre orientation. By following this direction of

greatest diffusion on a voxel by voxel basis, 3 dimensional reconstructions of WM

tracts are possible. Probabilistic tractography allows the quantitative assessment of

WM tracts in regions of relatively low FA such as in unmyelinated WM.10,11

Diffusion imaging in acute AIS shows the region of infarction as high signal intensity

(SI) on DTI and low SI on the ADC map, representing restricted diffusion of water

molecules, before the infarction is clearly visualised on conventional imaging.12,13

With AIS DTI changes in the PLIC, cerebral peduncle and brainstem remote from the

stroke in the acute period are associated with the developmental of hemiplegia.14,15

There is little data on DTI in preterm infants with HPI in the acute period, however,

tractography is able to demonstrate disruption of the affected CSTs on later imaging.11

To our knowledge, there have been no studies assessing the CSTs in infants with

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

focal lesions using tractography in the neonatal period. Furthermore, there have

been few tractography studies assessing the healthy infant brain.16,17

The primary aim of this study was to assess, in infants with a focal lesion of perinatal

onset, the CSTs from the cerebral peduncle to the cortex using probabilistic

tractography and to correlate the conventional MRI and tractography findings at

term with later motor function. We hypothesized that asymmetries in CST diffusion

properties would predict impaired motor function. In order to achieve our primary

aim we needed to establish normative values for the diffusion properties of the CSTs

in healthy infants.

135Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Materials and Methods

Approval for MRI was granted by the local Research Ethics Committee and written

parental consent was obtained prior to scanning.

Subjects

Infants born between October 2005 and October 2009 who underwent MRI and DTI,

had an AIS or HPI and who had a neurodevelopmental assessment at ≥12 months of

age were included. We also studied 23 healthy term-born control infants.

Imaging

MRI was performed on a Philips 3 Tesla system using a phased array head coil. Case

infants were sedated with 30-50 mg/kg oral chloral hydrate. Control infants were

imaged during postprandial sleep. Throughout the examination, pulse oximetry and

electrocardiography were monitored and a pediatrician trained in MRI procedures

was in attendance.

3D MPRAGE and dual echo weighted imaging was obtained prior to DTI. Single shot

echo planar DTI was acquired in either 15 or 32 non-collinear directions using the

following parameters: TR 7230-9765 ms, TE 49 ms, slice thickness 2mm, voxel size

1.75 x1.75 x 2mm3, b value 750 s/mm2. The data were acquired with a SENSE factor

of 2. All MR images were reviewed by an experienced perinatal neuroradiologist

(M.A.R.) unaware of outcome.

Probabilistic Tractography

Image processing was performed using FSL18 and scalar maps of ADC, FA, λ1, λ2

and λ3 were generated. Seed and waypoint masks were generated on color coded

FA maps and tractography was performed as described previously.19

Assessment of Motor Function

Neurological status was assessed using the Hammersmith Infant Neurological

Examination20 and the presence of hemiplegia was determined. Asymmetries of limb

function and hand preference were looked for and the independence and quality

of finger movements noted. Children with minor asymmetries of tone, posture or

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

reflexes or a hand preference but who had normal symmetrical independent finger

movements and good bimanual function were classified as being asymmetrical but

not to have a hemiplegia.

Statistical Analyses

Volume, FA, ADC, AD (λ1) and RD ([λ2 + λ3]/2) of the CSTs were determined. From

the control infant data an asymmetry index (AI) for these parameters was calculated

using [2 x (mean left tract–mean right tract))/(mean left tract+ mean right tract)].21

For the infants with a focal lesion an AI for these parameters was determined using

[2 x (mean contralateral tract-mean ipsilateral tract))/ (mean contralateral+ mean

ipsilateral tract)]. The AI for tract volume, FA, ADC, AD and RD in the case infants

were assessed to see if they were outside the AI range obtained in the healthy

controls. Next, the data were tested for normality and found compatible with a

normal distribution. One way ANOVA was performed to assess between groups

differences in AIs of the diffusion measures (controls and cases which were divided

into 3 groups based on their motor performance; those who had no evidence of

asymmetry, those who had an asymmetry and those who developed a hemiplegia).

Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using a Bonferroni test. Where

a significant difference was observed, post-hoc analyses were performed using

Tukey’s HSD and Dunnett’s test.

Tractography was repeated in 5 healthy controls. The coefficient of variation was

<3% for tract volume and FA.

137Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Results

Subjects

Twenty-four infants were found to have a unilateral focal lesion underwent MRI and

DTI between October 2005 and October 2009. DTI data was corrupt due to motion

in 1, could not be retrieved from archive in 1 and follow-up data was not available

for 2 cases. Our study group consisted of 20 infants who had MRI and DTI data

available for analysis and who had early neurodevelopmental assessment. The

infants were imaged at a median age of 24 (range 3 – 97) days after birth, at a median

post-menstrual age of 41 (range 39 – 46+5) weeks. Their clinical characteristics are

shown in Table S1. We also studied 23 healthy term control infants, with a median

gestational age of 40 weeks (range 38 - 41+5) and median birth-weight of 3657 grams

(range 2790-4304).

MRI Findings

MRI findings for the infants are described in Table 1. Based on the criteria of 3 site

involvement (basal ganglia, PLIC, hemisphere) in MCA infarction2 or asymmetrical

PLIC at TEA following HPI,5 the imaging findings predicted a hemiplegia in 8 infants

and normal motor development in 12. No lesions or PLIC asymmetries were found in

the control infants. Conventional MRI, diffusion images and ADC maps for an infant

with subsequent normal motor function and an infant who developed a hemiplegia

are shown in Figures 1 and 2 respectively.

Tractography

Infants with lesions: DTI was obtained in 15 non-collinear directions in 9 infants and

in 32 noncollinear directions in 11 infants. In 3 infants who had extensive brain injury

we were not able to generate a connectivity distribution in the CST in the hemisphere

ipsilateral to the injury. Figure 3 shows the CSTs in an infant with subsequent normal

motor function (3a) and in an infant who developed a hemiplegia (3b and c).

Healthy control infants: In the 23 control infants, DTI was obtained in 15 non-

collinear directions in 8 infants and in 32 non-collinear directions in 15. There were

no significant differences in volume (p=.23), FA (p=.32), ADC (p=.83), AD (p=.73) or

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

RD (p=.91) between left and right CSTs in the controls. The values for volume, FA,

ADC, AD, and RD for the CSTs in all infants are shown in Table S2. The AIs of the

tract parameters from the infants with lesions and the controls are shown in Figure

S1.

Motor Outcome (Case Infants Only)

The median age at follow-up was 22.5 (range 12-48) months. Six infants developed

a hemiplegia contralateral to the lesion, 5 developed a mild motor asymmetry and 9

had no evidence of asymmetry in motor function.

Correlation between Conventional MRI Findings and Motor Performance

Conventional MRI findings predicted normal motor outcome in all 9 infants who had

no evidence of motor asymmetry and predicted a hemiplegia in all 6 infants who

developed a hemiplegia. In the 5 infants with a mild motor asymmetry, conventional

MRI was predictive of a hemiplegia in 2 and of normal motor development in 3

infants.

Combining the results for infants who developed a hemiplegia or asymmetry,

conventional MRI predicted an abnormal motor outcome in 8/11 children. The

sensitivity of conventional MRI to predict an abnormal motor outcome was 73%

and the specificity for prediction of a normal motor outcome was 100%.

Correlation of Tractography Findings and Motor Performance

Tract volume, FA, ADC, AD and RD were within the normal range for AI in all children

who had no evidence of motor asymmetry.

The 3 infants in whom tractography was unsuccessful due to extensive brain injury

developed a hemiplegia. In the remaining 3 infants who developed a hemiplegia, the

AI was outside the normal range for FA, volume and RD in all 3 and for ADC in 2.

In the 5 infants who developed a mild motor asymmetry, the AI was outside the

normal range for at least one diffusion measure in 4 infants (reduced volume in 2,

reduced FA value in 1 and increased RD in 3).

Combining the results for the infants who developed a hemiplegia or asymmetry, an

AI outside the normal range in ≥1 DTI measurement predicted an abnormal motor

139Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

outcome in 10/11 children. The sensitivity to predict an abnormal motor outcome

was 91% and the specificity for prediction of a normal motor outcome was 100%.

Results of Between Groups Differences Analyses of AIs

One way ANOVA demonstrated a significant difference in AI between the 4

groups (controls and cases who were divided into 3 groups based on their motor

performance) in tract volume (p=.05), FA (p<.0001), ADC (p=.015) and RD (p<.0001).

There was no significant between groups difference in AD (p=.95).

Results of Post-Hoc Analyses

Table 3 shows the results of the post-hoc analyses comparing AIs for tract volume,

FA, ADC and RD between healthy controls and the case infants.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Conventional MRI and DTI findings and motor outcome (1/3)

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

1 4 41+1 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. Small areas of abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 48 Mild asymmetry Lt hand preference but good independent finger movements bilaterally

2 20 42+2 Venous infarction. Abnormal SI in anterior WM (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

3 83 40+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal and temporal lobes. Ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and PLIC with atrophy of the Lt thalamus, brainstem and mesencephalon. Abnormal high SI Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

4 16 40+1 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform, and cystic lesion in Lt caudate head. Thalamus, mesencephalon and brainstem were smaller on the Lt. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC. Punctate white matter lesions on the Rt and Rt ventricular dilatation consistent with previous PHI, although the PLIC appeared normal on the Rt. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

5 5 42+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. Small foci of abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and Lt ventricle minimally dilated. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

6 8 42 Lt MCA infarct in frontal parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in the Lt BG and thalamus. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC and mesencephalon on conventional imaging and DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

24 Rt hemiplegia

7 34 44+5 Lt MCA infarct in posterior parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus, asymmetrical PLIC and moderate Lt ventricular dilatation. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

15 Rt hemiplegia

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

1 4 41+1 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. Small areas of abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 48 Mild asymmetry Lt hand preference but good independent finger movements bilaterally

2 20 42+3 Venous infarction. Abnormal SI in anterior WM (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

3 83 40+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal and temporal lobes. Ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and PLIC with atrophy of the Lt thalamus, brainstem and mesencephalon. Abnormal high SI Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

4 16 40+1 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform, and cystic lesion in Lt caudate head. Thalamus, mesencephalon and brainstem were smaller on the Lt. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC. Punctate white matter lesions on the Rt and Rt ventricular dilatation consistent with previous PHI, although the PLIC appeared normal on the Rt. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

5 5 42+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. Small foci of abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and Lt ventricle minimally dilated. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

6 8 42 Lt MCA infarct in frontal parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in the Lt BG and thalamus. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC and mesencephalon on conventional imaging and DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

24 Rt hemiplegia

7 34 44+5 Lt MCA infarct in posterior parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus, asymmetrical PLIC and moderate Lt ventricular dilatation. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

15 Rt hemiplegia

141Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

1 4 41+1 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. Small areas of abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 48 Mild asymmetry Lt hand preference but good independent finger movements bilaterally

2 20 42+2 Venous infarction. Abnormal SI in anterior WM (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

3 83 40+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal and temporal lobes. Ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and PLIC with atrophy of the Lt thalamus, brainstem and mesencephalon. Abnormal high SI Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

4 16 40+1 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform, and cystic lesion in Lt caudate head. Thalamus, mesencephalon and brainstem were smaller on the Lt. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC. Punctate white matter lesions on the Rt and Rt ventricular dilatation consistent with previous PHI, although the PLIC appeared normal on the Rt. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

5 5 42+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. Small foci of abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and Lt ventricle minimally dilated. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

6 8 42 Lt MCA infarct in frontal parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in the Lt BG and thalamus. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC and mesencephalon on conventional imaging and DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

24 Rt hemiplegia

7 34 44+5 Lt MCA infarct in posterior parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus, asymmetrical PLIC and moderate Lt ventricular dilatation. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

15 Rt hemiplegia

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

1 4 41+1 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. Small areas of abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 48 Mild asymmetry Lt hand preference but good independent finger movements bilaterally

2 20 42+3 Venous infarction. Abnormal SI in anterior WM (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and thalamus. The PLICs were symmetrical on conventional MRI and no abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

3 83 40+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal and temporal lobes. Ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt). Abnormal SI in the Lt lentiform and PLIC with atrophy of the Lt thalamus, brainstem and mesencephalon. Abnormal high SI Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

4 16 40+1 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform, and cystic lesion in Lt caudate head. Thalamus, mesencephalon and brainstem were smaller on the Lt. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC. Punctate white matter lesions on the Rt and Rt ventricular dilatation consistent with previous PHI, although the PLIC appeared normal on the Rt. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA and RD

36 Rt hemiplegia

5 5 42+5 Lt MCA infarct in parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. Small foci of abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and Lt ventricle minimally dilated. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

6 8 42 Lt MCA infarct in frontal parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in the Lt BG and thalamus. Abnormal SI in Lt PLIC and mesencephalon on conventional imaging and DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

24 Rt hemiplegia

7 34 44+5 Lt MCA infarct in posterior parietal and temporal lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus, asymmetrical PLIC and moderate Lt ventricular dilatation. Abnormal high SI in the Lt PLIC on the ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia Volume, FA, ADC and RD

15 Rt hemiplegia

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Conventional MRI and DTI findings and motor outcome (2/3)

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

8 4 40+6 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

9 4 39 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry RD 15 Mild asymmetry Preference for Lt hand. Good independent finger movements bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt.

10 22 44+4 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 12 No asymmetry

11 10 43+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia FA and RD 16 Good independent finger move-ments bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt. Mild asymmetry in tone and increased flexibility Lt leg

12 3 40+3 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

15 Rt hemiplegia

13 32 45+5 Lt MCA infarct in frontoparietal lobe and Lt medial occipital region (either in the territory of the Lt posterior cerebral artery of water-shed area of the MCA). PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

14 70 40 Lt MCA infarct affecting posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

15 97 40+3 Lt HPI. Mild asymmetrical ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt) and porencephalic cyst on Lt. Minimal atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC and mesencephalon appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 25 No asymmetry

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

8 4 40+6 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

9 4 39 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry RD 15 Mild asymmetry Preference for Lt hand. Good independent finger movements bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt.

10 22 44+4 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 12 No asymmetry

11 10 43+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia FA and RD 16 Good independent finger move-ments bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt. Mild asymmetry in tone and increased flexibility Lt leg

12 3 40+3 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

15 Rt hemiplegia

13 32 45+5 Lt MCA infarct in frontoparietal lobe and Lt medial occipital region (either in the territory of the Lt posterior cerebral artery of water-shed area of the MCA). PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

14 70 40 Lt MCA infarct affecting posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

15 97 40+3 Lt HPI. Mild asymmetrical ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt) and porencephalic cyst on Lt. Minimal atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC and mesencephalon appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 25 No asymmetry

143Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

8 4 40+6 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

9 4 39 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry RD 15 Mild asymmetry Preference for Lt hand. Good independent finger movements bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt.

10 22 44+4 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 12 No asymmetry

11 10 43+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia FA and RD 16 Good independent finger move-ments bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt. Mild asymmetry in tone and increased flexibility Lt leg

12 3 40+3 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

15 Rt hemiplegia

13 32 45+5 Lt MCA infarct in frontoparietal lobe and Lt medial occipital region (either in the territory of the Lt posterior cerebral artery of water-shed area of the MCA). PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

14 70 40 Lt MCA infarct affecting posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

15 97 40+3 Lt HPI. Mild asymmetrical ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt) and porencephalic cyst on Lt. Minimal atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC and mesencephalon appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 25 No asymmetry

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

8 4 40+6 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

9 4 39 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry RD 15 Mild asymmetry Preference for Lt hand. Good independent finger movements bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt.

10 22 44+4 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 12 No asymmetry

11 10 43+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

Rt hemiplegia FA and RD 16 Good independent finger move-ments bilaterally but pincer grip better on Lt. Mild asymmetry in tone and increased flexibility Lt leg

12 3 40+3 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Rt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

15 Rt hemiplegia

13 32 45+5 Lt MCA infarct in frontoparietal lobe and Lt medial occipital region (either in the territory of the Lt posterior cerebral artery of water-shed area of the MCA). PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 13 No asymmetry

14 70 40 Lt MCA infarct affecting posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 24 No asymmetry

15 97 40+3 Lt HPI. Mild asymmetrical ventricular dilatation (Lt>Rt) and porencephalic cyst on Lt. Minimal atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC and mesencephalon appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 25 No asymmetry

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Conventional MRI and DTI findings and motor outcome (3/3)

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

16 93 39 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Volume 24 Slight tightness in Lt ankle. Symmetrical upper limb function.

17 30 46+5 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry Volume and RD

21 Mild asymmetry with increased tone and less spontaneous movement on Rt. Symmetrical hand function.

18 26 44+5 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 21 No asymmetry

19 29 45+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

No asymmetry No 18 No asymmetry

20 71 40+2 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

27 Lt hemiplegia

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

16 93 39 Rt HPI. Mild ventricular dilatation with porencephalic cyst on the Rt. Abnormal SI in superior portion of Rt PLIC and lentiform on MRI and DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Volume 24 Slight tightness in Lt ankle. Symmetrical upper limb function.

17 30 46+5 Lt MCA infarct affecting fronto-parietal lobe. Abnormal SI in the Lt ALIC. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry Volume and RD

21 Mild asymmetry with increased tone and less spontaneous movement on Rt. Symmetrical hand function.

18 26 44+5 Infarct in Rt posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 21 No asymmetry

19 29 45+2 Lt MCA infarct. Abnormal SI in Lt caudate head and atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC appeared normal and symmetrical on MRI and DTI.

No asymmetry No 18 No asymmetry

20 71 40+2 Large Rt MCA infarct with involvement of the BG, thalamus and PLIC. Atrophy of the Rt mesencephalon and brain stem. Ventricular dilatation on Rt. Abnormal high SI in R PLIC on ADC map.

Lt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

27 Lt hemiplegia

145Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

16 93 39 Rt MCA (posterior branch) infarct in superior parietal lobe. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Volume 24 Slight tightness in Lt ankle. Symmetrical upper limb function.

17 30 46+5 Lt MCA infarct in superior parietal and temporal lobes. Symmetrical BG and PLIC. Small region of abnormal SI in Lt lentiform nucleus. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry Volume and RD

21 Mild asymmetry with increased tone and less spontaneous movement on Rt. Symmetrical hand function.

18 26 44+5 Infarction in frontal lobes bilaterally, and Lt occipital and Lt temporal lobes. BG and PLIC were symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 21 No asymmetry

19 29 45+2 Lt hemorrhagic infarction in frontal, parietal and occipital lobes. Abnormal SI in Lt thalamus and hemorrhage in Lt lentiform and PLIC. Abnormal high SI in Lt PLIC on ADC map.

No asymmetry No 18 No asymmetry

20 71 40+2 Lt MCA infarct in frontal, parietal, occipital and temporal lobes. Areas of abnormal SI in Lt BG, thalamus and PLIC on conventional MRI. Abnormal SI in the L PLIC and cerebral peduncle on DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

27 Lt hemiplegia

Case Age at

scan

(days)

PMA at

scan

(weeks)

MRI findings Prediction of motor

performance from

conventional MRI

AI outside

control range

Age at

assessment

(months)

Motor performance

16 93 39 Rt HPI. Mild ventricular dilatation with porencephalic cyst on the Rt. Abnormal SI in superior portion of Rt PLIC and lentiform on MRI and DTI.

Lt hemiplegia Volume 24 Slight tightness in Lt ankle. Symmetrical upper limb function.

17 30 46+5 Lt MCA infarct affecting fronto-parietal lobe. Abnormal SI in the Lt ALIC. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry Volume and RD

21 Mild asymmetry with increased tone and less spontaneous movement on Rt. Symmetrical hand function.

18 26 44+5 Infarct in Rt posterior parietal lobe. PLIC, BG and thalami appeared normal and symmetrical. No abnormality seen in the PLIC or cerebral peduncle on DTI.

No asymmetry No 21 No asymmetry

19 29 45+2 Lt MCA infarct. Abnormal SI in Lt caudate head and atrophy of Lt thalamus. PLIC appeared normal and symmetrical on MRI and DTI.

No asymmetry No 18 No asymmetry

20 71 40+2 Large Rt MCA infarct with involvement of the BG, thalamus and PLIC. Atrophy of the Rt mesencephalon and brain stem. Ventricular dilatation on Rt. Abnormal high SI in R PLIC on ADC map.

Lt hemiplegia Tractography not possible on affected side

27 Lt hemiplegia

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 2 Summary of MRI and DTI findings and correlation with motor

performance

MRI/ DTI findings Motor outcome

Conventional MRI suggestive of OOOOOOOOO λ λ λ

normal motor outcome

Conventional MRI suggestive of hemiplegia XXXXXX λ λ

DTI findings all within normal range for AI OOOOOOOOO λ

At least 1 DTI measure outside normal range XXXXXX λ λ λ λ

Key: O = normal motor outcome, X = hemiplegia, λ= asymmetry

TABLE 3 Post-hoc analyses comparing AIs in cases to controls

Hemiplegia (n=6) Mild asymmetry (n=5) Normal (n=9)

Volume p<.0001, 95% p=.079, 95% p=.592, 95%

CI 0.096-0.356 CI -0.004-0.103 CI -0.024-0.062

FA p<.001, 95% p=.079, 95% p=.592, 95%

CI 0.161-0.295 CI -0.004-0.103 CI 0.0235-0.0623

ADC p=.002, 95% p=.281, 95% p=.861, 95%

CI 0.0176-0.0789 CI -0.007-0.042 CI -0.014-0.025

RD p<.0001, 95% p=.001, 95% p>.99, 95%

CI 0.0718-0.124 CI 0.0143-0.0563 CI -0.0158-0.0177

Results of post-hoc analyses comparing AIs for tract volume, FA, ADC and RD in the 3 groups of cases (those who developed a hemiplegia, those who developed a mild asymmetry and those who had no evidence of asymmetry) to healthy control infants.Abbreviations: ADC = apparent diffusion coefficient, CI = confidence interval, FA =fractional anisotropy, RD = radial diffusivity

147Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE S1 Patient characteristics (1/2)

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

1 M 40+4 3506 35.5 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress.

7.34 9/10 - 72 hours

2 F 39+4 2504 35.2 Normal antenatal history SVD not known

9/10 - Day 13

3 M 28+6 1370 27.3 Oligohydramnios at 28+3 weeks. IUGR (<10th centile)

Em CS for cord prolapse, preterm

not known

2/4 Ventilated 48 hours, CPAP 8 days

None

4 F 37+6 1850 31.5 IUGR (<10th centile) Em CS for sub-optimal CTG.

7.38 9/9 - Day 1

5 M 42 4000 35.0 Normal antenatal history Forceps following failed ventouse, PROM Meconium

7.25 4/9 O2 14 hours

6 M 40+6 3500 35.3 Irregular fetal heart rate at 26 weeks GA. Normal fetal echo Variable decelerations on CTG for 2 hrs before delivery

Em CS following failed ventouse for failure to progress. PROM 48 hrs

7.3 5/10 O2 19 hours

7 F 39+6 3250 33.4 Normal antenatal history SVD. not known

8/9 - 38 hours

8 M 40+2 3300 34.4 Maternal depression Maternal fever in labor and decelerations on CTG

Em CS. Therapeutic hypothermia for suspected HIE

7.1 1/6 O2 5 hours

9 M 38+3 3440 37.2 Maternal diabetes and Vitamin D deficiency Em CS for failure to progress. Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 23 hours

10 F 41+3 3000 35.4 Normal antenatal history Em CS Thick meconium 7.04 2/5 Ventilated till day 3, CPAP till day 17. Nasal prong O2 till day 21

8 hours

11 M 41+6 3890 35.0 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress

7.1 8/9 - 9 hours

Case Sex GA

(wk)

BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

1 M 40+4 3506 35.5 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress.

7.34 9/10 - 72 hours

2 F 39+4 2504 35.2 Normal antenatal history SVD not known

9/10 - Day 13

3 M 28+6 1370 27.3 Oligohydramnios at 28+3 weeks. IUGR (<10th centile)

Em CS for cord prolapse, preterm

not known

2/4 Ventilated 48 hours, CPAP 8 days

None

4 F 37+6 1850 31.5 IUGR (<10th centile) Em CS for sub-optimal CTG.

7.38 9/9 - Day 1

5 M 42 4000 35.0 Normal antenatal history Forceps following failed ventouse, PROM Meconium

7.25 4/9 O2 14 hours

6 M 40+6 3500 35.3 Irregular fetal heart rate at 26 weeks GA. Normal fetal echo Variable decelerations on CTG for 2 hrs before delivery

Em CS following failed ventouse for failure to progress. PROM 48 hrs

7.3 5/10 O2 19 hours

7 F 39+6 3250 33.4 Normal antenatal history SVD. not known

8/9 - 38 hours

8 M 40+2 3300 34.4 Maternal depression Maternal fever in labor and decelerations on CTG

Em CS. Therapeutic hypothermia for suspected HIE

7.1 1/6 O2 5 hours

9 M 38+3 3440 37.2 Maternal diabetes and Vitamin D deficiency Em CS for failure to progress. Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 23 hours

10 F 41+3 3000 35.4 Normal antenatal history Em CS Thick meconium 7.04 2/5 Ventilated till day 3, CPAP till day 17. Nasal prong O2 till day 21

8 hours

11 M 41+6 3890 35.0 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress

7.1 8/9 - 9 hours

149Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

1 M 40+4 3506 35.5 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress.

7.34 9/10 - 72 hours

2 F 39+4 2504 35.2 Normal antenatal history SVD not known

9/10 - Day 13

3 M 28+6 1370 27.3 Oligohydramnios at 28+3 weeks. IUGR (<10th centile)

Em CS for cord prolapse, preterm

not known

2/4 Ventilated 48 hours, CPAP 8 days

None

4 F 37+6 1850 31.5 IUGR (<10th centile) Em CS for sub-optimal CTG.

7.38 9/9 - Day 1

5 M 42 4000 35.0 Normal antenatal history Forceps following failed ventouse, PROM Meconium

7.25 4/9 O2 14 hours

6 M 40+6 3500 35.3 Irregular fetal heart rate at 26 weeks GA. Normal fetal echo Variable decelerations on CTG for 2 hrs before delivery

Em CS following failed ventouse for failure to progress. PROM 48 hrs

7.3 5/10 O2 19 hours

7 F 39+6 3250 33.4 Normal antenatal history SVD. not known

8/9 - 38 hours

8 M 40+2 3300 34.4 Maternal depression Maternal fever in labor and decelerations on CTG

Em CS. Therapeutic hypothermia for suspected HIE

7.1 1/6 O2 5 hours

9 M 38+3 3440 37.2 Maternal diabetes and Vitamin D deficiency Em CS for failure to progress. Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 23 hours

10 F 41+3 3000 35.4 Normal antenatal history Em CS Thick meconium 7.04 2/5 Ventilated till day 3, CPAP till day 17. Nasal prong O2 till day 21

8 hours

11 M 41+6 3890 35.0 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress

7.1 8/9 - 9 hours

Case Sex GA

(wk)

BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

1 M 40+4 3506 35.5 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress.

7.34 9/10 - 72 hours

2 F 39+4 2504 35.2 Normal antenatal history SVD not known

9/10 - Day 13

3 M 28+6 1370 27.3 Oligohydramnios at 28+3 weeks. IUGR (<10th centile)

Em CS for cord prolapse, preterm

not known

2/4 Ventilated 48 hours, CPAP 8 days

None

4 F 37+6 1850 31.5 IUGR (<10th centile) Em CS for sub-optimal CTG.

7.38 9/9 - Day 1

5 M 42 4000 35.0 Normal antenatal history Forceps following failed ventouse, PROM Meconium

7.25 4/9 O2 14 hours

6 M 40+6 3500 35.3 Irregular fetal heart rate at 26 weeks GA. Normal fetal echo Variable decelerations on CTG for 2 hrs before delivery

Em CS following failed ventouse for failure to progress. PROM 48 hrs

7.3 5/10 O2 19 hours

7 F 39+6 3250 33.4 Normal antenatal history SVD. not known

8/9 - 38 hours

8 M 40+2 3300 34.4 Maternal depression Maternal fever in labor and decelerations on CTG

Em CS. Therapeutic hypothermia for suspected HIE

7.1 1/6 O2 5 hours

9 M 38+3 3440 37.2 Maternal diabetes and Vitamin D deficiency Em CS for failure to progress. Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 23 hours

10 F 41+3 3000 35.4 Normal antenatal history Em CS Thick meconium 7.04 2/5 Ventilated till day 3, CPAP till day 17. Nasal prong O2 till day 21

8 hours

11 M 41+6 3890 35.0 Normal antenatal history Em CS for failure to progress

7.1 8/9 - 9 hours

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE S1 Patient characteristics (2/2)

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

12 M 40 3630 35.3 High maternal BMI SVD Cord around neck. Floppy at birth, initial hypoglycemia

7.17 5/9 O2 Day 1

13 M 41+1 3112 33.3 Maternal gestational diabetes Decreased fetal movements 24 hours prior to delivery

Em CS for fetal bradycardia Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 13 hours

14 M 30 1315 28 IVF pregnancy “Elective” CS for high risk pregnancy

not known

9/10 n-CPAP for 6 hours None

15 F 26+5 930 23.7 IVF pregnancy. Prolonged rupture of membranes (19 weeks GA)

Emergency CS 7.2 8/9 Ventilated for 5 hours. N-CPAP till day 18. Nasal prong O2 till day 41

None

16 F 25+5 850 23.5 Normal antenatal history. Presented with 24-36 hours of abdominal pain at 25+5 with blood PV. No rupture of membranes until two minutes prior to delivery.

Spontaneous vaginal delivery

not known

6/9 Ventilated till day 4. CPAP till day 58. Nasal prong O2 till day 73

None

17 M 42+5 4330 36.7 Normal antenatal history Vaginal ventouse- transient fetal bradycardia

7.23 9/9 - 24 hours

18 M 41 3.34 33.5 Normal antenatal history Spontaneous vaginal delivery Thick meconium

7.15 6/9 5 inflation breaths at delivery

12 hours

19 M 41+1 3840 35.7 Normal antenatal history Em CS for fetal bradycardia.Thick meconium.

not known

7/9 - 12 hours

20 F 29+1 1380 26.5 Normal antenatal history Emergency CS for fetal bradycardia

not known

8/9 Ventilated for 1 day, CPAP for 10 day and nasal prong O2 for 6 days

None

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

12 M 40 3630 35.3 High maternal BMI SVD Cord around neck. Floppy at birth, initial hypoglycemia

7.17 5/9 O2 Day 1

13 M 41+1 3112 33.3 Maternal gestational diabetes Decreased fetal movements 24 hours prior to delivery

Em CS for fetal bradycardia Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 13 hours

14 M 30 1315 28 IVF pregnancy “Elective” CS for high risk pregnancy

not known

9/10 n-CPAP for 6 hours None

15 F 26+5 930 23.7 IVF pregnancy. Prolonged rupture of membranes (19 weeks GA)

Emergency CS 7.2 8/9 Ventilated for 5 hours. N-CPAP till day 18. Nasal prong O2 till day 41

None

16 F 25+5 850 23.5 Normal antenatal history. Presented with 24-36 hours of abdominal pain at 25+5 with blood PV. No rupture of membranes until two minutes prior to delivery.

Spontaneous vaginal delivery

not known

6/9 Ventilated till day 4. CPAP till day 58. Nasal prong O2 till day 73

None

17 M 42+5 4330 36.7 Normal antenatal history Vaginal ventouse- transient fetal bradycardia

7.23 9/9 - 24 hours

18 M 41 3.34 33.5 Normal antenatal history Spontaneous vaginal delivery Thick meconium

7.15 6/9 5 inflation breaths at delivery

12 hours

19 M 41+1 3840 35.7 Normal antenatal history Em CS for fetal bradycardia.Thick meconium.

not known

7/9 - 12 hours

20 F 29+1 1380 26.5 Normal antenatal history Emergency CS for fetal bradycardia

not known

8/9 Ventilated for 1 day, CPAP for 10 day and nasal prong O2 for 6 days

None

151Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

12 M 40 3630 35.3 High maternal BMI SVD Cord around neck. Floppy at birth, initial hypoglycemia

7.17 5/9 O2 Day 1

13 M 41+1 3112 33.3 Maternal gestational diabetes Decreased fetal movements 24 hours prior to delivery

Em CS for fetal bradycardia Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 13 hours

14 M 30 1315 28 IVF pregnancy “Elective” CS for high risk pregnancy

not known

9/10 n-CPAP for 6 hours None

15 F 26+5 930 23.7 IVF pregnancy. Prolonged rupture of membranes (19 weeks GA)

Emergency CS 7.2 8/9 Ventilated for 5 hours. N-CPAP till day 18. Nasal prong O2 till day 41

None

16 F 25+5 850 23.5 Normal antenatal history. Presented with 24-36 hours of abdominal pain at 25+5 with blood PV. No rupture of membranes until two minutes prior to delivery.

Spontaneous vaginal delivery

not known

6/9 Ventilated till day 4. CPAP till day 58. Nasal prong O2 till day 73

None

17 M 42+5 4330 36.7 Normal antenatal history Vaginal ventouse- transient fetal bradycardia

7.23 9/9 - 24 hours

18 M 41 3.34 33.5 Normal antenatal history Spontaneous vaginal delivery Thick meconium

7.15 6/9 5 inflation breaths at delivery

12 hours

19 M 41+1 3840 35.7 Normal antenatal history Em CS for fetal bradycardia.Thick meconium.

not known

7/9 - 12 hours

20 F 29+1 1380 26.5 Normal antenatal history Emergency CS for fetal bradycardia

not known

8/9 Ventilated for 1 day, CPAP for 10 day and nasal prong O2 for 6 days

None

Abbreviations: EmCS = emergency cesarean section, SVD = spontaneous vaginal delivery, PROM = prolonged rupture of membranes, IUGR = intra-uterine growth restriction, HIE = hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy, GA = gestational age, M = male, F = female, BMI = body mass index

Case Sex GA BW (g) Birth HC

(cm)

Antenatal Delivery Cord

pH

Apgar

(1’ /5’)

Respiratory support Onset of

Clinical seizures

12 M 40 3630 35.3 High maternal BMI SVD Cord around neck. Floppy at birth, initial hypoglycemia

7.17 5/9 O2 Day 1

13 M 41+1 3112 33.3 Maternal gestational diabetes Decreased fetal movements 24 hours prior to delivery

Em CS for fetal bradycardia Thick meconium

7.2 8/9 O2 13 hours

14 M 30 1315 28 IVF pregnancy “Elective” CS for high risk pregnancy

not known

9/10 n-CPAP for 6 hours None

15 F 26+5 930 23.7 IVF pregnancy. Prolonged rupture of membranes (19 weeks GA)

Emergency CS 7.2 8/9 Ventilated for 5 hours. N-CPAP till day 18. Nasal prong O2 till day 41

None

16 F 25+5 850 23.5 Normal antenatal history. Presented with 24-36 hours of abdominal pain at 25+5 with blood PV. No rupture of membranes until two minutes prior to delivery.

Spontaneous vaginal delivery

not known

6/9 Ventilated till day 4. CPAP till day 58. Nasal prong O2 till day 73

None

17 M 42+5 4330 36.7 Normal antenatal history Vaginal ventouse- transient fetal bradycardia

7.23 9/9 - 24 hours

18 M 41 3.34 33.5 Normal antenatal history Spontaneous vaginal delivery Thick meconium

7.15 6/9 5 inflation breaths at delivery

12 hours

19 M 41+1 3840 35.7 Normal antenatal history Em CS for fetal bradycardia.Thick meconium.

not known

7/9 - 12 hours

20 F 29+1 1380 26.5 Normal antenatal history Emergency CS for fetal bradycardia

not known

8/9 Ventilated for 1 day, CPAP for 10 day and nasal prong O2 for 6 days

None

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE S2 Diffusion characteristics of the corticospinal tracts in infants

with lesions and controls

Case1 No of

directions

FA Volume3 Axial diffusivity4 ADC value Radial diffusivity4

Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral

1 15 0.28 ± 0.09 0.30 ± 0.12 2517 2505 1.54 ± 0.32 1.62 ± 0.26 1.12 ± 0.25 1.22 ± 0.20 1.01 ± 0.20 1.02 ± 0.20

2 15 0.25 ± 0.13 0.27 ± 0.13 3687 3491 1.66 ± 0.30 1.69 ± 0.28 1.31 ± 0.27 1.32 ± 0.27 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

3 15 0.18 ± 0.08 0.26 ± 0.11 2995 4128 1.56 ± 0.44 1.55 ± 0.26 1.32 ± 0.39 1.21 ± 0.20 1.20± 0.40 1.04 ± 0.20

4 15 0.19 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.11 2137 3467 1.66 ± 0.29 1.64 ± 0.30 1.38 ± 0.28 1.33 ± 0.24 1.25 ± 0.30 1.17 ± 0.30

5 15 0.25 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.11 4140 3687 1.49 ± 0.15 1.55 ± 0.15 1.19 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.25 1.03 ± 0.30 1.04 ± 0.30

6 15 # 0.24 ± 0.12 # 3620 # 1.56 ± 0.24 # 1.24 ± 0.18 # 1.08 ± 0.20

7 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.27 ± 0.13 3950 4969 1.50 ± 0.39 1.49 ± 0.35 1.25 ± 0.38 1.16 ± 0.33 1.13 ± 0.40 1.00 ± 0.30

8 32 0.25 ± 0.11 0.26 ± 0.11 3197 4104 1.56 ± 0.27 1.51 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.21 1.19 ± 0.21 1.04 ± 0.20 1.03 ± 0.20

9 32 0.23 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.10 3871 4435 1.37 ± 0.22 1.48 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.18 1.20 ± 0.21 0.97 ± 0.20 1.07 ± 0.20

10 32 0.26 ± 0.12 0.27 ± 0.11 6623 6347 1.43 ± 0.26 1.41 ± 0.28 1.13 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.23 0.97 ± 0.30 0.95 ± 0.20

11 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.25 ± 0.13 3975 3993 1.46 ± 0.28 1.49 ± 0.34 1.20 ± 0.25 1.18 ± 0.32 1.07 ± 0.30 1.02 ± 0.30

12 32 # 0.23 ± 0.11 # 4275 # 1.60 ± 0.43 # 1.30 ± 0.29 # 1.14 ± 0.30

13 32 0.24 ± 0.09 0.26 ± 0.09 3078 3583 1.34 ± 0.22 1.34 ± 0.20 1.06 ± 0.18 1.05 ± 0.18 0.92 ± 0.20 0.91 ± 0.20

14 32 0.24± 0.09 0.26± 0.10 3706 3785 1.11 ± 0.19 1.07± 0.23 0.90 ± 0.19 0.88 ± 0.20 0.80 ± 0.23 0.79 ± 0.20

15 15 0.23 ± 0.11 0.26± 0.11 2064 2269 1.69 ± 0.23 1.60 ± 0.23 1.32 ± 0.25 1.29 ± 0.23 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

16 15 0.24 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.13 2542 3099 1.63 ± 0.33 1.59 ± 0.22 1.30 ± 0.32 1.27 ± 0.23 1.13 ± 0.34 1.11 ± 0.26

17 15 0.31 ± 0.12 0.33 ± 0.13 4716 5886 1.50 ± 0.19 1.58 ± 0.24 1.15 ± 0.12 1.12 ± 0.16 0.98 ± 0.25 0.93 ± 0.20

18 32 0.29 ± 0.13 0.29 ± 0.11 7235 7832 1.59 ± 0.33 1.46 ± 0.25 1.17 ± 0.24 1.10 ± 0.19 0.96 ± 0.38 0.93 ± 0.20

19 32 0.28 ± 0.11 0.29 ± 0.11 4235 5152 1.45 ± 0.22 1.46 ± 0.22 1.10 ± 0.17 1.12 ± 0.20 0.95 ± 0.20 0.93 ± 0.21

20 32 # 0.24 ± 0.11 # 4777 # 1.64 ± 0.24 # 1.31 ± 0.23 # 1.14 ±0.27

Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right

Controls2 (n=8) 15 0.25 ± 0.02 0.25 ± 0.02 3589 ± 462 3433 ± 436 1.23 ± 0.14 1.25 ± 0.14 0.97 ± 0.10 0.99 ± 0.10 0.84 ± 0.09 0.85 ± 0.09

Controls2 (n=15) 32 0.26 ± 0.02 0.26 ± 0.02 4398 ± 518 4097 ± 613 1.49 ± 0.08 1.50 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.06 1.01 ± 0.07 1.01 ± 0.07

Case1 No of

directions

FA Volume3 Axial diffusivity4 ADC value Radial diffusivity4

Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral

1 15 0.28 ± 0.09 0.30 ± 0.12 2517 2505 1.54 ± 0.32 1.62 ± 0.26 1.12 ± 0.25 1.22 ± 0.20 1.01 ± 0.20 1.02 ± 0.20

2 15 0.25 ± 0.13 0.27 ± 0.13 3687 3491 1.66 ± 0.30 1.69 ± 0.28 1.31 ± 0.27 1.32 ± 0.27 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

3 15 0.18 ± 0.08 0.26 ± 0.11 2995 4128 1.56 ± 0.44 1.55 ± 0.26 1.32 ± 0.39 1.21 ± 0.20 1.20± 0.40 1.04 ± 0.20

4 15 0.19 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.11 2137 3467 1.66 ± 0.29 1.64 ± 0.30 1.38 ± 0.28 1.33 ± 0.24 1.25 ± 0.30 1.17 ± 0.30

5 15 0.25 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.11 4140 3687 1.49 ± 0.15 1.55 ± 0.15 1.19 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.25 1.03 ± 0.30 1.04 ± 0.30

6 15 # 0.24 ± 0.12 # 3620 # 1.56 ± 0.24 # 1.24 ± 0.18 # 1.08 ± 0.20

7 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.27 ± 0.13 3950 4969 1.50 ± 0.39 1.49 ± 0.35 1.25 ± 0.38 1.16 ± 0.33 1.13 ± 0.40 1.00 ± 0.30

8 32 0.25 ± 0.11 0.26 ± 0.11 3197 4104 1.56 ± 0.27 1.51 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.21 1.19 ± 0.21 1.04 ± 0.20 1.03 ± 0.20

9 32 0.23 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.10 3871 4435 1.37 ± 0.22 1.48 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.18 1.20 ± 0.21 0.97 ± 0.20 1.07 ± 0.20

10 32 0.26 ± 0.12 0.27 ± 0.11 6623 6347 1.43 ± 0.26 1.41 ± 0.28 1.13 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.23 0.97 ± 0.30 0.95 ± 0.20

11 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.25 ± 0.13 3975 3993 1.46 ± 0.28 1.49 ± 0.34 1.20 ± 0.25 1.18 ± 0.32 1.07 ± 0.30 1.02 ± 0.30

12 32 # 0.23 ± 0.11 # 4275 # 1.60 ± 0.43 # 1.30 ± 0.29 # 1.14 ± 0.30

13 32 0.24 ± 0.09 0.26 ± 0.09 3078 3583 1.34 ± 0.22 1.34 ± 0.20 1.06 ± 0.18 1.05 ± 0.18 0.92 ± 0.20 0.91 ± 0.20

14 32 0.24± 0.09 0.26± 0.10 3706 3785 1.11 ± 0.19 1.07± 0.23 0.90 ± 0.19 0.88 ± 0.20 0.80 ± 0.23 0.79 ± 0.20

15 15 0.23 ± 0.11 0.26± 0.11 2064 2269 1.69 ± 0.23 1.60 ± 0.23 1.32 ± 0.25 1.29 ± 0.23 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

16 15 0.24 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.13 2542 3099 1.63 ± 0.33 1.59 ± 0.22 1.30 ± 0.32 1.27 ± 0.23 1.13 ± 0.34 1.11 ± 0.26

17 15 0.31 ± 0.12 0.33 ± 0.13 4716 5886 1.50 ± 0.19 1.58 ± 0.24 1.15 ± 0.12 1.12 ± 0.16 0.98 ± 0.25 0.93 ± 0.20

18 32 0.29 ± 0.13 0.29 ± 0.11 7235 7832 1.59 ± 0.33 1.46 ± 0.25 1.17 ± 0.24 1.10 ± 0.19 0.96 ± 0.38 0.93 ± 0.20

19 32 0.28 ± 0.11 0.29 ± 0.11 4235 5152 1.45 ± 0.22 1.46 ± 0.22 1.10 ± 0.17 1.12 ± 0.20 0.95 ± 0.20 0.93 ± 0.21

20 32 # 0.24 ± 0.11 # 4777 # 1.64 ± 0.24 # 1.31 ± 0.23 # 1.14 ±0.27

Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right

Controls2 (n=8) 15 0.25 ± 0.02 0.25 ± 0.02 3589 ± 462 3433 ± 436 1.23 ± 0.14 1.25 ± 0.14 0.97 ± 0.10 0.99 ± 0.10 0.84 ± 0.09 0.85 ± 0.09

Controls2 (n=15) 32 0.26 ± 0.02 0.26 ± 0.02 4398 ± 518 4097 ± 613 1.49 ± 0.08 1.50 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.06 1.01 ± 0.07 1.01 ± 0.07

153Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Case1 No of

directions

FA Volume3 Axial diffusivity4 ADC value Radial diffusivity4

Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral

1 15 0.28 ± 0.09 0.30 ± 0.12 2517 2505 1.54 ± 0.32 1.62 ± 0.26 1.12 ± 0.25 1.22 ± 0.20 1.01 ± 0.20 1.02 ± 0.20

2 15 0.25 ± 0.13 0.27 ± 0.13 3687 3491 1.66 ± 0.30 1.69 ± 0.28 1.31 ± 0.27 1.32 ± 0.27 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

3 15 0.18 ± 0.08 0.26 ± 0.11 2995 4128 1.56 ± 0.44 1.55 ± 0.26 1.32 ± 0.39 1.21 ± 0.20 1.20± 0.40 1.04 ± 0.20

4 15 0.19 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.11 2137 3467 1.66 ± 0.29 1.64 ± 0.30 1.38 ± 0.28 1.33 ± 0.24 1.25 ± 0.30 1.17 ± 0.30

5 15 0.25 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.11 4140 3687 1.49 ± 0.15 1.55 ± 0.15 1.19 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.25 1.03 ± 0.30 1.04 ± 0.30

6 15 # 0.24 ± 0.12 # 3620 # 1.56 ± 0.24 # 1.24 ± 0.18 # 1.08 ± 0.20

7 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.27 ± 0.13 3950 4969 1.50 ± 0.39 1.49 ± 0.35 1.25 ± 0.38 1.16 ± 0.33 1.13 ± 0.40 1.00 ± 0.30

8 32 0.25 ± 0.11 0.26 ± 0.11 3197 4104 1.56 ± 0.27 1.51 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.21 1.19 ± 0.21 1.04 ± 0.20 1.03 ± 0.20

9 32 0.23 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.10 3871 4435 1.37 ± 0.22 1.48 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.18 1.20 ± 0.21 0.97 ± 0.20 1.07 ± 0.20

10 32 0.26 ± 0.12 0.27 ± 0.11 6623 6347 1.43 ± 0.26 1.41 ± 0.28 1.13 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.23 0.97 ± 0.30 0.95 ± 0.20

11 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.25 ± 0.13 3975 3993 1.46 ± 0.28 1.49 ± 0.34 1.20 ± 0.25 1.18 ± 0.32 1.07 ± 0.30 1.02 ± 0.30

12 32 # 0.23 ± 0.11 # 4275 # 1.60 ± 0.43 # 1.30 ± 0.29 # 1.14 ± 0.30

13 32 0.24 ± 0.09 0.26 ± 0.09 3078 3583 1.34 ± 0.22 1.34 ± 0.20 1.06 ± 0.18 1.05 ± 0.18 0.92 ± 0.20 0.91 ± 0.20

14 32 0.24± 0.09 0.26± 0.10 3706 3785 1.11 ± 0.19 1.07± 0.23 0.90 ± 0.19 0.88 ± 0.20 0.80 ± 0.23 0.79 ± 0.20

15 15 0.23 ± 0.11 0.26± 0.11 2064 2269 1.69 ± 0.23 1.60 ± 0.23 1.32 ± 0.25 1.29 ± 0.23 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

16 15 0.24 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.13 2542 3099 1.63 ± 0.33 1.59 ± 0.22 1.30 ± 0.32 1.27 ± 0.23 1.13 ± 0.34 1.11 ± 0.26

17 15 0.31 ± 0.12 0.33 ± 0.13 4716 5886 1.50 ± 0.19 1.58 ± 0.24 1.15 ± 0.12 1.12 ± 0.16 0.98 ± 0.25 0.93 ± 0.20

18 32 0.29 ± 0.13 0.29 ± 0.11 7235 7832 1.59 ± 0.33 1.46 ± 0.25 1.17 ± 0.24 1.10 ± 0.19 0.96 ± 0.38 0.93 ± 0.20

19 32 0.28 ± 0.11 0.29 ± 0.11 4235 5152 1.45 ± 0.22 1.46 ± 0.22 1.10 ± 0.17 1.12 ± 0.20 0.95 ± 0.20 0.93 ± 0.21

20 32 # 0.24 ± 0.11 # 4777 # 1.64 ± 0.24 # 1.31 ± 0.23 # 1.14 ±0.27

Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right

Controls2 (n=8) 15 0.25 ± 0.02 0.25 ± 0.02 3589 ± 462 3433 ± 436 1.23 ± 0.14 1.25 ± 0.14 0.97 ± 0.10 0.99 ± 0.10 0.84 ± 0.09 0.85 ± 0.09

Controls2 (n=15) 32 0.26 ± 0.02 0.26 ± 0.02 4398 ± 518 4097 ± 613 1.49 ± 0.08 1.50 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.06 1.01 ± 0.07 1.01 ± 0.07

1. Data are given as mean ± standard deviation of the individual connectivity distributions 2. Data are given as mean ± standard deviation of the whole group 3. In mm3 4. Value * 10-3

# Indicates that connectivity distributions could not be generated.

Case1 No of

directions

FA Volume3 Axial diffusivity4 ADC value Radial diffusivity4

Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral Ipsilateral Contra-lateral

1 15 0.28 ± 0.09 0.30 ± 0.12 2517 2505 1.54 ± 0.32 1.62 ± 0.26 1.12 ± 0.25 1.22 ± 0.20 1.01 ± 0.20 1.02 ± 0.20

2 15 0.25 ± 0.13 0.27 ± 0.13 3687 3491 1.66 ± 0.30 1.69 ± 0.28 1.31 ± 0.27 1.32 ± 0.27 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

3 15 0.18 ± 0.08 0.26 ± 0.11 2995 4128 1.56 ± 0.44 1.55 ± 0.26 1.32 ± 0.39 1.21 ± 0.20 1.20± 0.40 1.04 ± 0.20

4 15 0.19 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.11 2137 3467 1.66 ± 0.29 1.64 ± 0.30 1.38 ± 0.28 1.33 ± 0.24 1.25 ± 0.30 1.17 ± 0.30

5 15 0.25 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.11 4140 3687 1.49 ± 0.15 1.55 ± 0.15 1.19 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.25 1.03 ± 0.30 1.04 ± 0.30

6 15 # 0.24 ± 0.12 # 3620 # 1.56 ± 0.24 # 1.24 ± 0.18 # 1.08 ± 0.20

7 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.27 ± 0.13 3950 4969 1.50 ± 0.39 1.49 ± 0.35 1.25 ± 0.38 1.16 ± 0.33 1.13 ± 0.40 1.00 ± 0.30

8 32 0.25 ± 0.11 0.26 ± 0.11 3197 4104 1.56 ± 0.27 1.51 ± 0.26 1.21 ± 0.21 1.19 ± 0.21 1.04 ± 0.20 1.03 ± 0.20

9 32 0.23 ± 0.09 0.22 ± 0.10 3871 4435 1.37 ± 0.22 1.48 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.18 1.20 ± 0.21 0.97 ± 0.20 1.07 ± 0.20

10 32 0.26 ± 0.12 0.27 ± 0.11 6623 6347 1.43 ± 0.26 1.41 ± 0.28 1.13 ± 0.23 1.10 ± 0.23 0.97 ± 0.30 0.95 ± 0.20

11 32 0.20 ± 0.09 0.25 ± 0.13 3975 3993 1.46 ± 0.28 1.49 ± 0.34 1.20 ± 0.25 1.18 ± 0.32 1.07 ± 0.30 1.02 ± 0.30

12 32 # 0.23 ± 0.11 # 4275 # 1.60 ± 0.43 # 1.30 ± 0.29 # 1.14 ± 0.30

13 32 0.24 ± 0.09 0.26 ± 0.09 3078 3583 1.34 ± 0.22 1.34 ± 0.20 1.06 ± 0.18 1.05 ± 0.18 0.92 ± 0.20 0.91 ± 0.20

14 32 0.24± 0.09 0.26± 0.10 3706 3785 1.11 ± 0.19 1.07± 0.23 0.90 ± 0.19 0.88 ± 0.20 0.80 ± 0.23 0.79 ± 0.20

15 15 0.23 ± 0.11 0.26± 0.11 2064 2269 1.69 ± 0.23 1.60 ± 0.23 1.32 ± 0.25 1.29 ± 0.23 1.14 ± 0.30 1.13 ± 0.30

16 15 0.24 ± 0.11 0.25 ± 0.13 2542 3099 1.63 ± 0.33 1.59 ± 0.22 1.30 ± 0.32 1.27 ± 0.23 1.13 ± 0.34 1.11 ± 0.26

17 15 0.31 ± 0.12 0.33 ± 0.13 4716 5886 1.50 ± 0.19 1.58 ± 0.24 1.15 ± 0.12 1.12 ± 0.16 0.98 ± 0.25 0.93 ± 0.20

18 32 0.29 ± 0.13 0.29 ± 0.11 7235 7832 1.59 ± 0.33 1.46 ± 0.25 1.17 ± 0.24 1.10 ± 0.19 0.96 ± 0.38 0.93 ± 0.20

19 32 0.28 ± 0.11 0.29 ± 0.11 4235 5152 1.45 ± 0.22 1.46 ± 0.22 1.10 ± 0.17 1.12 ± 0.20 0.95 ± 0.20 0.93 ± 0.21

20 32 # 0.24 ± 0.11 # 4777 # 1.64 ± 0.24 # 1.31 ± 0.23 # 1.14 ±0.27

Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right Left Right

Controls2 (n=8) 15 0.25 ± 0.02 0.25 ± 0.02 3589 ± 462 3433 ± 436 1.23 ± 0.14 1.25 ± 0.14 0.97 ± 0.10 0.99 ± 0.10 0.84 ± 0.09 0.85 ± 0.09

Controls2 (n=15) 32 0.26 ± 0.02 0.26 ± 0.02 4398 ± 518 4097 ± 613 1.49 ± 0.08 1.50 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.07 1.17 ± 0.06 1.01 ± 0.07 1.01 ± 0.07

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Figure 1. Infant 5. a. T1 and b. T2 weighted imaging at the level of the PLIC demonstrating a left MCA infarct in the left posterior temporal lobe (arrow) and a slightly dilated left ventricle. The basal ganglia and PLIC appear symmetrical. The diffusion image (c) and ADC map (d) show areas of high and low SI respectively in this region

155Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Figure 2. Infant 6. a. T1 b. T2, c. diffusion images and d. ADC map demonstrating a left MCA at the level of the PLIC (i) and mesencephalon (ii). Abnormal SI is present in the left PLIC, basal ganglia and thalamus (ai and bi) and in the left cerebral peduncle (arrow aii) on conventional MRI. The diffusion image (c) and ADC map (d) show abnormal SI in these regions.

Figure 3. The corticospinal tracts in 2 infants with ischemic lesions; a. case 8 and b and c. case 3. The volume of the left, ipsilateral CST is diminished in case 3. This is more clearly seen in the sagittal projection (c).

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

0.6

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

AI 0

-0.1

-0.2

-0.3

-0.4

-0.5

-0.6

0.400

0.300

0.200

0.100

AI 0

-0.100

-0.200

Age at scan (weeks)

Age at scan (weeks)

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48

Figure S1A The asymmetry index (AI) for tract volume

Figure S1B The asymmetry index (AI) for fractional anisotropy

157Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

0.25

0.20

0.15

0.10

0.05

AI 0

-0.05

-0.10

-0.15

-0.20

-0.25

Age at scan (weeks)

Figure S1D The asymmetry index (AI) for axial diffusivity

0.12

0.10

0.08

0.06

0.04

0.02

AI 0

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06

-0.08

-0.10

Age at scan (weeks)

Figure S1C The asymmetry index (AI) for the apparent diffusion coefficient

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

The asymmetry index for tract volume, fractional anisotropy, apparent diffusion coefficient values, axial diffusivity, and radial diffusivity of the corticospinal tract in newborn infants with ischemic lesions and controls.

Key = normal controls = cases with normal motor outcome = cases who developed an asymmetry = cases who developed a hemiplegia

0.15

0.10

0.05

AI 0

-0.05

-0.10

-0.15

-0.20

Age at scan (weeks)

Figure S1E The asymmetry index (AI) for radial diffusivity

38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48

159Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Discussion

In this study we examined a group of healthy control infants and a group with

unilateral perinatally acquired focal lesions using diffusion tractography of the

CSTs in the neonatal period. Our analyses revealed that, whilst the AI for AD did

not show any differences between cases and controls, the AI for all the other

diffusion measures we assessed showed a significant difference between controls

and infants who developed a hemiplegia and RD also showed a significant

difference in AI between controls and infants who developed a mild asymmetry.

We established a normal range for asymmetry of diffusion measures from the

control infants, which when within the normal range for all diffusion characteristics

in case infants were associated with normal motor development. All infants who

developed a hemiplegia either had at least 3 diffusion measures outside the

normal range, or tractography could not be performed. In the five children who

developed a mild motor asymmetry, at least 1 of the DTI measures was outside

the normal range in 4. Of interest, the functional asymmetry was very mild in the

child whose DTI findings were within the normal range and this child had good

independent finger movements bilaterally at 4 years of age and a normal gait.

The conventional MRI findings were similar; no asymmetry in motor function was

predicted in all 9 children who had no evidence of asymmetry, and hemiplegia was

predicted in all 6 children who subsequently developed a hemiplegia. However,

for children who developed a mild motor asymmetry conventional MRI predicted

a hemiplegia in 2 and normal motor development in 3 infants. The 2 cases where

tractography correctly predicted an abnormal outcome and conventional MRI did

not were imaged 3 days and 29 days after the onset of clinical seizures. The ability

of conventional MRI or tractography to predict abnormal motor development

does not, therefore, appear to be related to the time of imaging after injury. The

specificity for prediction of good motor performance was 100% for both MRI and

tractography, and the sensitivity for prediction of abnormal motor performance

was 73% for MRI and 91% for tractography.

Our study group included all the term born infants with AIS who were imaged

neonatally and had good quality DTI over a 4 year period. The incidence of

hemiplegia in the term infants with AIS in our study (6 of 14) is similar to that reported

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

in other MRI studies of neonatal infarction.2 We included 3 infants who were born

preterm who had an MCA territory infarction, 2 preterm infants who had HPI and a

term born infant with venous HPI in addition as the motivation for this study was to

determine whether tractography can provide additional information compared to

conventional MRI alone in infants who reflect the common unilateral focal lesions

presenting to the clinical neonatologist and radiologist. Our results suggest that

tractography offers little extra prognostic information compared to that which can

be obtained from conventional MRI in infants with normal motor outcome or who

develop a hemiplegia. However, tractography may be useful in identifying those

infants who subsequently develop a mild motor asymmetry. In this study, both

cases where MRI was predictive of normal motor development and tractography

findings were asymmetrical developed an asymmetry. We accept that our study

is limited by a relatively small subject group and the early age at assessment for

some infants. In addition, it remains to be seen whether the children with mild

motor problems will develop a mild hemiplegia in later childhood, although that

appears unlikely in the child who was assessed at 4 years. In the study by Mercuri

et al only one child who was asymmetrical at 2 years had a mild hemiplegia at 5.5

years.22

Studies have shown that an increased SI at the level of the PLIC and the cerebral

peduncle on diffusion weighted images (DWIs), indicating areas of restricted

diffusion, is followed by the development of a hemiplegia in infants with AIS.15

Kirton et al. examined DWIs from 14 neonates with AIS and found a difference in

relative affected volume of the CST using slice-by-slice analysis, when comparing

children with good and poor motor outcome.14 Similar findings have been reported

in the adult brain following stroke, with marked reductions in anisotropy following

ischemia being associated with the structural breakdown of WM fibers.24,25 This

decrease in anisotropy is presumably caused by Wallerian degeneration (WD), that

is the anterograde degeneration of axons and myelin sheaths after proximal axonal

or cell body injury, which is observed as subtle changes in SI on conventional

imaging.5,26 The asymmetries in tract volume, FA and RD observed in infants who

developed motor impairments in this present study may be early markers of the

progressive disintegration of fibers due to WD.

161Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Using an AI to assess differences between the injured and contra-lateral CSTs

allowed us to combine data acquired using 2 different diffusion schemes, and

offers the possibility of performing larger studies comparing data from different

imaging centers in future studies. Another advantage of this approach is that we

were able to assess images obtained over a wide age range following birth, and so

imaging in the acute period following the injury was not essential.

Our secondary aim was to establish normative values for diffusion characteristics

of the CSTs in healthy control infants at term which, to our knowledge, have

not been reported in such a large group previously. Aeby et al. assessed CST

development using probabilistic tractography and, in 6 healthy term infants, they

reported a mean FA of around 0.23, close to our finding of 0.26.17 Other studies

using a deterministic approach reported slightly higher FA values at TEA in infants

who were born preterm.27 By using a probabilistic approach, we could delineate

tracts not only in areas with high FA, but also areas with lower FA values, such as

the peripheral portion of the tract close to the motor cortex, which may explain

this difference. Previous studies have demonstrated a trend towards differences

in tract volume and FA between hemispheres in preterm infants at TEA,28 however,

we did not demonstrate inter-hemispheric differences in either volume or FA in our

healthy term-born controls.

In summary, both conventional MRI and tractography of the CSTs performed in

the neonatal period were able to predict subsequent normal motor development

or hemiplegia following ischemic injury. Diffusion tractography appears to offer

additional information over conventional MRI in predicting a subsequent mild

motor asymmetry.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Benders MJ, Groenendaal F, Uiterwaal CS, et al.

Maternal and infant characteristics associated with

perinatal arterial stroke in the preterm infant. Stroke

2007;38:1759-1765.

2. Mercuri E, Rutherford M, Cowan F, et al. Early

prognostic indicators of outcome in infants

with neonatal cerebral infarction: a clinical,

electroencephalogram, and magnetic resonance

imaging study. Pediatrics 1999;103:39-46.

3. deVeber GA, MacGregor D, Curtis R, Mayank S.

Neurologic outcome in survivors of childhood arterial

ischemic stroke and sinovenous thrombosis. J Child

Neurol 2000;15:316-324.

4. Sreenan C, Bhargava R, Robertson CM. Cerebral

infarction in the term newborn: clinical

presentation and long-term outcome. J Pediatr

2000;137:351-355.

5. de Vries LS, Groenendaal F, van Haastert I, Eken P,

Rademaker KJ, Meiners LC. Asymmetrical myelination

of the posterior limb of the internal capsule in infants

with periventricular haemorrhagic infarction: An early

predictor of hemiplegia. Neuropediatrics

1999;30:314-319.

6. Huppi PS, Maier SE, Peled S, et al. Microstructural

development of human newborn cerebral white

matter assessed in vivo by diffusion tensor magnetic

resonance imaging. Pediatr Res 1998;44:584-590.

7. Neil JJ, Shiran SI, McKinstry RC, et al. Normal brain in

human newborns: apparent diffusion coefficient and

diffusion anisotropy measured by using diffusion tensor

MR imaging. Radiology 1998;209:57-66.

8. Seghier ML, Lazeyras F, Zimine S, et al. Combination of

event-related fMRI and diffusion tensor imaging in an

infant with perinatal stroke. Neuroimage

2004;21:463-472.

9. Ward P, Counsell S, Allsop J, et al. Reduced fractional

anisotropy on diffusion tensor magnetic resonance

imaging after hypoxicischemic encephalopathy.

Pediatrics 2006;117:e619-e630.

10. Behrens TE, Johansen-Berg H, Woolrich MW, et al.

Non-invasive mapping of connections between human

thalamus and cortex using diffusion imaging. Nat

Neurosci 2003;6:750-757.

11. Counsell SJ, Dyet LE, Larkman DJ, et al. Thalamo-

cortical connectivity in children born preterm mapped

using probabilistic magnetic resonance tractography.

Neuroimage 2007;34:896-904.

12. Dudink J, Mercuri E, Al-Nakib L, et al. Evolution

of unilateral perinatal arterial ischemic stroke on

conventional and diffusion weighted MR imaging. Am

J Neuroradiol 2009;30:998-1004.

13. Krishnamoorthy KS, Soman TB, Takeoka M, Schaefer

PW. Diffusion-weighted imaging in neonatal cerebral

infarction: clinical utility and follow-up. J Child Neurol

2000;15:592-602.

References

163Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

14. Kirton A, Shroff M, Visvanathan T, deVeber G.

Quantified corticospinal tract diffusion restriction

predicts neonatal stroke outcome. Stroke

2007;38:974-980.

15. de Vries LS, Van der Grond J, van Haastert IC,

Groenendaal F. Prediction of outcome in new-

born infants with arterial ischaemic stroke using

diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging.

Neuropediatrics 2005;36:12-20.

16. Gilmore JH, Lin W, Corouge I, et al. Early postnatal

development of corpus callosum and corticospinal

white matter assessed with quantitative tractography.

Am J Neuroradiol 2007;28:1789-1795.

17. Aeby A, Liu Y, De Tiege, X, et al. Maturation of

thalamic radiations between 34 and 41 weeks’

gestation: a combined voxel-based study and

probabilistic tractography with diffusion tensor

imaging. Am J Neuroradiol 2009;30:1780-1786.

18. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, et al.

Advances in functional and structural MR image

analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage

2004;23:S208-S219.

19. Bassi L, Chew A, Merchant N, et al. Diffusion tensor

imaging in preterm infants with punctate white matter

lesions. Pediatr Res 2011; 69:561-566.

20. Haataja L, Mercuri E, Regev R, et al. Optimality score

for the neurologic examination of the infant at 12 and

18 months of age. J Pediatr 1999;135:153-161.

21. Choi CH, Lee JM, Koo BB, et al. Sex differences in the

temporal lobe white matter and the corpus callosum:

a diffusion tensor tractography study. Neuroreport

2010;21:73-77.

22. Mercuri E, Barnett A, Rutherford M, et al. Neonatal

cerebral infarction and neuromotor outcome at school

age. Pediatrics 2004;113: 95-100.

23. Puig J, Pedraza S, Blasco G, et al. Wallerian

degeneration in the corticospinal tract evaluated by

diffusion tensor imaging correlates with motor deficit

30 days after middle cerebral artery ischemic stroke.

Am J Neuroradiol 2010;31:1324-1330.

24. Pierpaoli C, Barnett A, Pajevic S, et al. Water

diffusion changes in Wallerian degeneration and their

dependence on white matter architecture. Neuroimage

2001;13:1174-1185.

25. Werring DJ, Toosy AT, Clark CA, et al. Diffusion

tensor imaging can detect and quantify corticospinal

tract degeneration after stroke. J Neurol Neurosurg

Psychiatry 2000;69:269-272.

26. Bouza H, Dubowitz LM, Rutherford M, Pennock JM.

Prediction of outcome in children with congenital

hemiplegia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.

Neuropediatrics 1994;25:60-66.

27. Adams E, Chau V, Poskitt KJ, Grunau RE, Synnes

A, Miller SP. Tractographybased quantitation of

corticospinal tract development in premature

newborns. J Pediatr 2010;156:882-888.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

28. Liu Y, Baleriaux D, Kavec M, et al. Structural

asymmetries in motor and language networks

in a population of healthy preterm neonates at

term equivalent age: a diffusion tensor imaging

and probabilistic tractography study. Neuroimage

2010;51:783-788.

165Chapter 6 Tractography of the Corticospinal Tracts in Infants with Focal Perinatal Injury: Comparison with Normal Controls and to Motor Development

Chapter 7 Functional impairments at school age of children

with necrotizing enterocolitis or

spontaneous intestinal perforation

Chapter 8 Functional impairments at school age of

preterm born children with late-onset sepsis

Chapter 9 Motor and cognitive outcome at school age

of children surgically treated for intestinal obstructions

in the first days of life

PART 3Functional outcome of newborn infants with

systemic diseases

Chapter 7

FUNCTIONAL IMPAIRMENTS AT SCHOOL

AGE OF CHILDREN WITH NECROTIZING

ENTEROCOLITIS OR SPONTANEOUS

INTESTINAL PERFORATION

ELISE ROZE, BASTIAAN D.P. TA, MEIKE H. VAN DER REE, JOZIEN C. TANIS,

KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL, JAN B.F. HULSCHER, AREND F. BOS

PEDIATRIC RESEARCH 2011; DOI 10.1203/PDR.0B013E31823279B1

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

We aimed to determine motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome at school age of

children that had either necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) or spontaneous intestinal

perforation (SIP). This case-control study included infants with NEC Bell’s stage-

IIA onwards, infants with SIP and matched controls (1996-2002). At school age

we assessed motor skills, intelligence, visual perception, visuomotor integration,

verbal memory, attention, behavior, and executive functions. Of 93 infants with

NEC or SIP, 28 (30%) died. We included 52 of 65 survivors for follow-up. At mean

age 9 years we found that 68% of the children had borderline or abnormal scores

on the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (versus 45% of controls). Their

mean total intelligence quotient (IQ) was 86±14 compared to 97±9 in the controls.

Additionally, attention and visual perception were affected (p<.01 and p=.02). In

comparison to controls, surgically treated children were at highest risk for adverse

outcome. In conclusion, at school age the motor functions and intelligence of many

children with NEC or SIP were borderline or abnormal and, specifically, attention

and visual perception were impaired. Children with NEC or SIP form a specific risk-

group for functional impairments at school age even though the majority does not

have overt brain pathology.

171Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

Introduction

Necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC) is a common, acute gastrointestinal disease in newborn,

mostly preterm infants. The clinical manifestation and course of NEC can vary from

non-specific symptoms requiring conservative treatment to a fulminant disease with

major abdominal and systemic symptoms requiring surgery.

Spontaneous intestinal perforation (SIP) is a less common gastrointestinal disorder. Its

incidence has increased over time with the increasing rate of survival of very low birth

weight infants. Infants with SIP have clinical and radiographic features that are often

less pronounced than infants with NEC, but they always require surgical treatment.1

Mortality in infants with NEC or SIP is found to range between 15 to 30%.1,2

Newborn infants with NEC or SIP often require prolonged ventilatory support and

they are prone to develop sepsis, which both can lead to white matter injury.3 In

addition, they often have difficulty tolerating enteral feeding that subjects them to

inadequate nutrition and growth impairment. All these factors contribute to the risk

of neurodevelopmental disabilities. Earlier studies reported that ~20% of the infants

with NEC develop cerebral palsy (CP) while even more children have cognitive

impairments.4,5 Most of these studies, however, only followed the children up to the

age of 2 years. Furthermore, infants with SIP are frequently excluded from outcome

studies since SIP is a different disease than NEC, while they have similar presenting

signs. Consequently, the neurodevelopmental outcome at school age of children with

NEC or SIP is unknown. Since functional demands at school age are higher than at

younger ages, more specific motor, cognitive, and behavioral deficits that were not

detected previously may now become apparent.

The first aim of our study was to determine the motor, cognitive, and behavioral

outcome at school age of children with the gastrointestinal diseases NEC or SIP

compared to control children of similar gestational age. Our second aim was to identify

disease-related risk factors for adverse outcome, such as type of treatment, Bell’s

stage, and presence of late onset sepsis. We hypothesized that children with surgically

treated NEC (SurgNEC) would be at highest risk for neurodevelopmental impairments

compared to children with SIP and medically treated NEC (MedNEC), since they had

often been severely ill for a significant period of time and their illness was frequently

complicated with perforation.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Methods

Patients

We selected all newborn infants that were admitted to the Neonatal Intensive Care

Unit (NICU) of the University Medical Center Groningen between 1996-2002 and

diagnosed with either NEC, from Bell’s stage-IIA onwards, or SIP. We found the

infants by searching the patient database on the diagnoses NEC, and intestinal and

gastric perforations. We also included control infants from our NICU that were born in

the same period (1996-2002) and matched for gender and gestational age. We aimed

at including 1.5-2 times as many control infants as cases in the subgroups of infants

with gastrointestinal diseases (i.e. MedNEC (n=15), SurgNEC (n=17), SIP (n=20)).

We therefore included 31 controls. We excluded patients with major chromosomal

anomalies. The infants in the control group were comparable to our study group in

all respects except that they did not have gastrointestinal diseases (Table 1).6 The

diagnosis of either NEC or SIP was made in a multi-disciplinary setting, involving

neonatologists, pediatric surgeons and radiologists based on i. clinical signs,

including abdominal distention and discoloration, bloody stools, and circulatory

and respiratory instability; II. radiological signs consistent with either pneumatosis

intestinalis, pneumoperitoneum or both; and III. the extent of the affected bowel during

surgery. All of the infants with SIP and SurgNEC underwent laparotomy in the acute

phase of the illness. We reviewed the medical charts for neonatal and disease-related

characteristics. Disease severity in the infants with NEC was classified according to

Bell’s stages by using the clinical, radiographic, and laparotomy findings.7 We also

recorded the number of disease-related reoperations in the first year of life.

Follow-up

The children were invited prospectively to participate in an extension of the routine

follow-up program which was supervised by a child neuropsychologist (K.N.J.A.B.).

The program entailed the assessment of motor performance, cognition, and

behavior at the age of 6 to 13 years. The follow-up took approximately 2.5 hours

to complete including breaks. Parents gave their written informed consent to

participate in the follow-up program. The study was approved by the Medical

Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen.

173Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

Motor Outcome

We determined the presence or absence of CP following Bax’ criteria.8 In case

of CP, gross motor functioning was scored with the Gross Motor Function

Classification System (GMFCS), a functional, five level classification system for

CP.9 Higher GMFCS levels indicate more severe functional impairments.

To assess the children’s motor outcome we administered the Movement

Assessment Battery for Children (Movement-ABC), a standardized test of motor

skills for children.10 This test yields a total motor performance score that is based on

subscores for manual dexterity (fine motor skills), ball skills, and static and dynamic

balance (coordination). The higher the score, the poorer the performance.

We also measured height and weight of the children which are expressed as

z-scores.

Cognitive Outcome

Total, verbal, and performance intelligence were assessed using a shortened

version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition, Dutch version

(WISC-III-NL).11 In addition, we assessed central visual perception and visuomotor

integration with the subtests Geometric Puzzles and Design Copying of the NEPSY-

II, a neuropsychological test battery for children.12 Visuomotor integration involves

the integration of visual information with finger-hand movements.

We assessed verbal memory with a standardized Dutch version of Rey’s Auditory

Verbal Learning Test.13

We measured selective attention and attentional control with the subtests Map

Mission and Opposite Worlds of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children.14

Selective attention refers to a child’s ability to select target information from an

array of distractors. Attentional control refers to the ability to shift attention flexibly

and adaptively.

Behavioral Outcome

To obtain information on the children’s behavioral and emotional competencies

and problems we asked the parents to complete the Child Behavior Checklist.15

It consists of two subscales, one for internalizing problems (withdrawn behavior,

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

somatic complaints, and anxious/depressed scales) and one for externalizing

problems (delinquent and aggressive behavior scales), and a composite total

scale.

The parents also filled out the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function16 to

assess executive functioning involved in well-organized, purposeful, goal-directed,

and problem-solving behavior.

Test scores obtained when a child was too tired and/or uncooperative (as

assessed by the trained experimenter) as well as incomplete questionnaires, were

excluded.

The experimenter was blinded to the presence or absence of gastrointestinal

diseases in the children.

Statistical Analyses

We classified the intelligence quotients (IQs) as normal (IQ≥85), borderline (mildly

abnormal, IQ 70-85), moderately abnormal (IQ 69-55), and severely abnormal

(IQ<55). We used the percentiles on the standardization samples of the Movement-

ABC and cognitive tests to classify raw scores into normal (>P15), borderline (P5-

P15), and abnormal (<P5). For the Child Behavior Checklist and Behavior Rating

Inventory of Executive Function we used a similar classification following the

criteria in the manual. Visual inspection of the histograms and Q-Q plots were used

to determine which outcome measures were normally distributed. We then used

the Student’s t, Mann-Whitney U, and Chi2 test where appropriate to compare the

outcome measures of the study group with the control group and to relate disease

characteristics to outcome. We used backward logistic regression analysis to

calculate the odds ratios (ORs) for worse outcome when comparing the children

with gastrointestinal diseases to the controls, and the children with SurgNEC

to the groups of children with both MedNEC and SIP. We repeated the logistic

regression analyses, adjusting for severe cerebral pathology. Cerebral pathology

was detected by serial cranial ultrasound scans, and defined as severe in case

of grade III germinal matrix hemorrhage, post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation,

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction and cystic periventricular leukomalacia.

Next, to identify additional disease-related risk factors for adverse outcome,

175Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

we performed a univariate analysis to relate age at development of NEC, Bell’s

stage, localization of SIP (gastric or intestinal), presence of late onset sepsis, age

at surgery, and multiple surgeries to outcome within the group of children with

gastrointestinal diseases. Throughout the analyses p<.05 was considered to be

statistically significant. SPSS 16.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL)

was used for the analyses. The analyses were performed by E.R. and A.F.B. with

support from a statistician.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Between 1996-2002 a total of 3,947 patients were admitted to our NICU. After

database search 59 infants with NEC and 34 infants with SIP were included.

Twenty (34%) of the infants with NEC and 8 (24%) of the infants with SIP died in

the neonatal period. A total of 65 survivors with gastrointestinal diseases remained

of whom 52 (80%) participated in the follow-up program – 8 sets of parents

declined the invitation to participate, 4 could not be traced, and 1 child could not

be assessed as a result of being deaf. We included 31 control infants from our

NICU for follow-up.

Patient Characteristics

Table 1 gives an overview of the patient demographics of the children with

gastrointestinal diseases and the controls. The demographics of the 13 children

who did not participate were comparable to the children included in our study

(n=7 with NEC, n=6 with SIP, median gestational age 28.9 weeks, birth weight

1,100 grams). The child that could not be assessed as a result of being deaf had

MedNEC.

Of the 15 infants with MedNEC, 13 had Bell’s stage-IIA and 2 stage-IIB. One of

these infants was surgically treated for an intestinal stenosis several weeks after

recovering from NEC. Of the 17 infants with SurgNEC, 1 had Bell’s stage-IIA, 1

stage-IIB, 3 stage-IIIA, and 12 stage-IIIB.

Two of the 20 infants with SIP had a stomach perforation and 18 had an intestinal

perforation. All infants with SIP and SurgNEC underwent laparotomy in the acute

phase of their gastrointestinal disease. One child with SIP received postnatal

steroids from day 11 onwards because of respiratory problems, prior to the

development of SIP.

During childhood, 3 children required pediatric intensive care treatment for

respiratory support, which was not related to their gastrointestinal disease from

the neonatal period. One child with MedNEC had subglottic laryngitis, one child

with SIP required a cardiac intervention for pulmonary artery stenosis and one

control child had a respiratory syncytial virus infection.

177Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

Follow-up

The mean age at follow-up was 9.3 years (range 6.2-13.3 years). At school age we

found that 1 child had visual problems requiring prescription glasses, 4 children

had hearing problems requiring hearing aids, and 4 children had epilepsy. These

disabilities were not found in the control group. Regarding growth, we found that

children with gastrointestinal diseases had a mean height z-score of -0.37 (SD,

1.17), weight of 0.09 (SD 1.34), and head circumference of -0.40 (SD 1.09). In the

control children this was -0.40 (SD 0.95) for height, 0.11 (SD 1.57) for weight, and

-0.43 (SD 1.45) for head circumference.

Motor Outcome

Of the 52 children with gastrointestinal diseases, 3 developed unilateral CP (6%)

and 5 bilateral CP (9%). Their functional impairments were limited to GMFCS level-I

in 3 and level-II in 4 children. One child with severe functional impairments had

GMFCS level-IV. In the control group only 1 child developed bilateral CP (5%) with

GMFCS level-II. The increased incidence of CP in the children with gastrointestinal

diseases was almost significant (p=.08).

The median scores on the Movement-ABC are shown in Table 2. The child with

severe CP (GMFCS-IV) was not assessed with the Movement-ABC. The children

with gastrointestinal diseases scored significantly worse than the controls on the

total Movement-ABC score and on the subtests fine motor skills and coordination.

In Table 3 we classified the outcome into the categories normal, borderline, and

abnormal, and present the ORs for worse outcome, after correction for cerebral

pathology. Sixty-eight percent of the children with gastrointestinal diseases

obtained borderline or abnormal scores on the Movement-ABC with an OR of

2.27 compared to controls. Before correction for cerebral pathology, ORs for

borderline /abnormal outcome on the total score of the Movement-ABC and

abnormal outcome on coordination were slightly higher. (OR 2.66, 95% CI 1.06-

6.68, p=.04, and OR 3.64, 95% CI 1.20-11.02, p=.02 respectively). All other ORs

were the same.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Cognitive and Behavioral Outcome

Of the 52 children with gastrointestinal diseases 15 (28%) attended special

education classes and 14 (27%) had to repeat classes. In the control group (n=31) 1

child (3%) attended a special education class and 8 (26%) had to repeat classes.

Table 2 shows the mean and median scores, where appropriate, on the cognitive

and behavioral measures. For 3 children with gastrointestinal diseases the

neuropsychological tests were too difficult because of very low IQ scores. In

comparison to the controls the children with gastrointestinal diseases scored

significantly lower on intelligence (total, verbal, and performance), visual perception,

and attention. There was a trend towards lower scores on visuomotor integration.

On the delayed recall of verbal memory they scored slightly better than the controls.

The incidence of behavioral problems in the groups was comparable.

In Table 3 we classified the scores into the categories normal, borderline, and

abnormal including the ORs for worse outcome after correction for cerebral

pathology. The children whose neuropsychological functions could not be

assessed were included in the category abnormal. Regarding IQ, we found that

n=3 children had moderately abnormal total IQs, whilst n=2 had severely abnormal

total IQs; for verbal IQ this was n=5 and n=2 and for performance IQ n=5 and n=2

respectively. The ORs confirmed the analyses of the mean scores except for visual

perception in which case the OR was not significant. The better scores on verbal

memory were not confirmed by the ORs. Analyses without correction for cerebral

pathology revealed similar results with slightly different ORs, but no differences in

level of significance (data not shown). Only ORs for borderline /abnormal verbal IQ

and visuomotor integration were higher prior to correction (OR 3.28, 95% CI 0.99-

10.88, p=.05 and OR 4.15, 95% CI 1.10-15.66, p=.03, respectively).

Disease Characteristics in relation to Outcome

Subsequently, we determined whether the disease characteristics of the children

with gastrointestinal diseases were related to outcome at school age. Between

the subgroups of children with gastrointestinal diseases (MedNEC, SurgNEC, and

SIP) no differences in outcome were found. Next, we analyzed the outcomes of the

subgroups in comparison to the controls. In Figure 1 we provide the Movement-

179Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

ABC and IQ scores. The children with SIP had the highest Movement-ABC scores,

which indicates adverse outcome. The IQ scores of the children with MedNEC,

SurgNEC, and SIP were lower than those of the controls. We found the biggest

differences for children with SurgNEC or SIP.

In Table 4 we present the ORs for borderline /abnormal outcome of the children

with MedNEC, SurgNEC, or SIP in comparison to the control group. The children

with SIP had a significant increased risk for worse motor outcome compared to

the controls. The ORs for lower intelligence reached significance in the children

with SurgNEC and SIP, and showed a trend towards significance in children with

MedNEC. The children with SurgNEC were at risk for worse visuomotor integration.

Attention was worse in all three groups, although only the ORs of the children with

SurgNEC and SIP reached significance. When calculating the ORs for abnormal

outcome only, the domains in which the children had increased ORs for worse

outcome were similar, apart from some empty fields (data not shown).

Analyses without correction for cerebral pathology showed that visual perception

tended to be worse in children with SIP compared to controls (OR 4.93, 95% CI

0.87-27.88, p=.07). All other ORs were the same before and after correction.

None of the additional disease-related risk factors (the age at development of

NEC, Bell’s stage, localization of SIP (gastric or intestinal), presence of late onset

sepsis, age at surgery, and multiple surgeries) were related to adverse

outcome at school age.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Patient demographics

Data are given as median (25th-75th percentile) or as numbers (percentage). *Indicates p<.05 when comparing MedNEC to SurgNEC, **Indicates p<.05 when comparing SurgNEC to SIP1. Mild cerebral pathology was defined as grade I and II germinal matrix-intraventricular hemorrhage

(GMH-IVH) 2. Severe cerebral pathology was defined as grade III GMH-IVH, post-hemorrhagic ventricular

dilatation (PHVD), periventricular hemorrhagic infarction, and cystic periventricular leukomalacia. PHVD was defined as a lateral ventricle size of <0.33 according to Evans’ index (the right and left lateral horn width divided by the maximum internal skull width)6

3. Retinopathy of prematurity grade III and worse 4. Calculated by the Chi2 and Mann-Whitney U test comparing the children with gastrointestinal

diseases (MedNEC, SurgNEC, and SIP taken together) to the controls Abbreviations: HFO- high frequency oscillation; GMH-IVH- germinal matrix hemorrhage-intraventricular hemorrhage; IPPV- intermittent positive pressure ventilation; IUGR- intrauterine growth restriction; ns- not significant; PHVD- post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation.

MedNEC SurgNEC SIP Controls p-value4

Number n=15 n=17 n=20 n=31

Males/females 9/6 11/6 14/6 17/14 ns

Gestational age (weeks) 29.8 (27.5-34.6)

31.2 (29.0-32.8)

29.5 (27.5-31.2)

30.0 (27.7-32.0)

ns

Birth weight (grams) 1100 (1020-1775)

1415 (1103-1770)

1173 (850-1494)

1220 (1060-1840)

ns

IUGR (<P10) n=2 (13) n=4 (24) n=1 (5) n=2 (6) ns

Apgar at 5 minutes 9 (7-10) 8 (6-10) 9 (4-10) 9 (5-10) ns

Asphyxia n=1 (7) n=1 (6) n=2 (10) n=0 (0) ns

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO)

n=13 (87) n=17 (100) n=20 (100) n=20 (65) <0.001

Inotropics n=4 (27) n=10 (59) n=8 (40) n=3 (10) 0.001

Cerebral pathology

None n=8 (53) n=11 (65) n=11 (55) n=24 (77) ns

Mild1 n=5 (33) n=5 (29) n=6 (30) n=7 (23) ns

Severe2 n=2 (13) n=1 (6) n=3 (15) n=0 (0) ns

Neonatal seizures n=0 (0) n=0 (0) n=3 (15) n=0 (0) ns

Late onset sepsis n=3 (20)* n=11 (65)** n=4 (20) n=3 (10) 0.009

Gram-positive bacteria n=2 (13)* n=9 (53)** n=3 (15) n=3 (10) ns

Gram-negative bacteria n=1 (7) n=3 (18) n=4 (20) n=0 (0) ns

Late onset morbidity

Retinopathy of prematurity3 n=1 (7) n=0 (0) n=1 (5) n=0 (0) ns

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia

n=5 (33) n=4 (24) n=4 (20) n=6 (19) ns

Postnatal steroids n=2 (13) n=5 (29) n=5 (25) n=6 (19) ns

Reoperations within first year of life

- 2 (1-4) 1 (0-4) -

181Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

MedNEC SurgNEC SIP Controls p-value4

Number n=15 n=17 n=20 n=31

Males/females 9/6 11/6 14/6 17/14 ns

Gestational age (weeks) 29.8 (27.5-34.6)

31.2 (29.0-32.8)

29.5 (27.5-31.2)

30.0 (27.7-32.0)

ns

Birth weight (grams) 1100 (1020-1775)

1415 (1103-1770)

1173 (850-1494)

1220 (1060-1840)

ns

IUGR (<P10) n=2 (13) n=4 (24) n=1 (5) n=2 (6) ns

Apgar at 5 minutes 9 (7-10) 8 (6-10) 9 (4-10) 9 (5-10) ns

Asphyxia n=1 (7) n=1 (6) n=2 (10) n=0 (0) ns

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO)

n=13 (87) n=17 (100) n=20 (100) n=20 (65) <0.001

Inotropics n=4 (27) n=10 (59) n=8 (40) n=3 (10) 0.001

Cerebral pathology

None n=8 (53) n=11 (65) n=11 (55) n=24 (77) ns

Mild1 n=5 (33) n=5 (29) n=6 (30) n=7 (23) ns

Severe2 n=2 (13) n=1 (6) n=3 (15) n=0 (0) ns

Neonatal seizures n=0 (0) n=0 (0) n=3 (15) n=0 (0) ns

Late onset sepsis n=3 (20)* n=11 (65)** n=4 (20) n=3 (10) 0.009

Gram-positive bacteria n=2 (13)* n=9 (53)** n=3 (15) n=3 (10) ns

Gram-negative bacteria n=1 (7) n=3 (18) n=4 (20) n=0 (0) ns

Late onset morbidity

Retinopathy of prematurity3 n=1 (7) n=0 (0) n=1 (5) n=0 (0) ns

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia

n=5 (33) n=4 (24) n=4 (20) n=6 (19) ns

Postnatal steroids n=2 (13) n=5 (29) n=5 (25) n=6 (19) ns

Reoperations within first year of life

- 2 (1-4) 1 (0-4) -

TABLE 2 Motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome in children with

gastrointestinal diseases (NEC or SIP) versus controls

Data are given as mean (standard deviation, range) for normally distributed variables or median (25th-75th percentile) for non normally distributed variables. NS; not significant# p-values derived from Student t-test or Mann-Whitney u test1. Raw-scores2. Intelligence quotients3. Percentiles4. T-scores

Children with gastrointestinal diseases (n=52)

Controls (n=31) p-values#

Age at follow-up 9.6 (2.2, 6.2-13.2) 8.9 (1.6, 6.8-13.3) ns

Motor outcome (n=82)1

Movement-ABC total 10 (7-20) 8 (5-13) 0.037

Fine motor skills 6 (3-9) 3 (1-6) 0.008

Ball skills 2 (0-5) 3 (1-5) ns

Coordination 4 (2-8) 2 (0-4) 0.006

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence2 86 (14, 45-118) 97 (9, 79-119) <0.001

Verbal intelligence2 87 (15, 50-125) 98 (12, 78-128) 0.001

Performance intelligence2 84 (15, 40-110) 95 (10, 78-118) 0.001

Visual perception (n=72)3 43 (29, 0.1-100) 61 (29, 5-98) 0.02

Visuomotor integration (n=80)3 47 (35, 0-100) 61 (30, 9-100) 0.08

Verbal memory (n=80)3 48 (39, 0-100) 48 (29, 1-91) ns

Delayed recall (n=74)3 54 (33, 0.1-99) 38 (30, 1-95) 0.04

Recognition (n=75)1 29 (2, 21-30) 29 (2, 22-30) ns

Selective attention (n=79)3 16 (6-50) 50 (25-63) 0.01

Attentional control (n=80)3 16 (3-50) 50 (16-75) 0.005

Behavioral outcome (n=81)

Total behavioral problems4 55 (11, 29-88) 55 (11, 34-81) ns

Internalizing problems4 53 (11, 33-77) 54 (13, 33-73) ns

Externalizing problems4 53 (11, 33-83) 52 (12, 33-82) ns

Executive functioning3 50 (27-81) 51 (30-66) ns

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 3 Outcome of children with gastrointestinal diseases (NEC or

SIP) classified into normal, borderline, and abnormal versus controls

(the presented ORs are adjusted for severe cerebral pathology)

Children with gastrointestinal diseases (n=52) Controls (n=31) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=82)

Movement-ABC total 16 (31) 14 (27) 21 (41) 17 (55) 6 (19) 8 (26) 2.27* (0.90-5.78) 1.69 (0.62-4.59)

Fine motor skills 20 (39) 16 (31) 15 (29) 21 (68) 7 (23) 3 (10) 3.23** (1.27-8.33) 3.89** (1.02-14.77)

Ball skills 32 (63) 8 (16) 11 (22) 18 (58) 9 (29) 4 (13) 0.61 (0.23-1.57) 1.86 (0.54-6.44)

Coordination 24 (47) 6 (12) 21(41) 22 (71) 4 (13) 5 (16) 2.75** (1.06-7.12) 3.05* (0.99-9.43)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence 30 (58) 17 (33) 5 (10) 29 (94) 2 (6) 10.63‡ (2.29-49.35) ||

Verbal intelligence 35 (67) 10 (19) 7 (13) 27 (87) 4 (13) 2.66 (0.77-9.10) ||

Performance intelligence 31 (60) 14 (27) 7 (13) 25 (81) 6 (19) 2.82* (0.99-8.06) ||

Visual perception (n=75) 41 (82) 7 (14) 2 (4) 23 (92) 2 (8) 3.63 (0.75-17.70) ||

Visuomotor integration 39 (75) 6 (12) 7 (13) 28 (90) 3 (10) 3.29* (0.85-12.84) ||

Verbal memory 43 (83) 4 (8) 5 (10) 26 (84) 1 (3) 4 (13)

Delayed recall (n=77) 41 (84) 2 (4) 6 (12) 20 (71) 6 (21) 2 (7) 0.53 (0.17-1.61) 1.95 (0.37-10.41)

Recognition (n=78) 42 (86) 7 (14) 0 (0) 24 (83) 2 (7) 3 (10)

Selective attention (n=82) 35 (69) 9 (18) 7 (14) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6) 4.01** (1.22-13.22) 3.54 (0.72-17.36)

Attentional control 33 (63) 6 (12) 13 (25) 26 (84) 2 (6) 3 (10) 3.81** (1.26-11.50) 4.15** (1.10-15.66)

Behavioral outcome (n=81)

Total behavioral problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 20 (65) 5 (16) 6 (19)

Internalizing problems 36 (72) 5 (10) 9 (18) 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29)

Externalizing problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 25 (81) 2 (6) 4 (13)

Executive functioning 42 (84) 6 (12) 2 (4) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6)

Children with gastrointestinal diseases (n=52) Controls (n=31) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=82)

Movement-ABC total 16 (31) 14 (27) 21 (41) 17 (55) 6 (19) 8 (26) 2.27* (0.90-5.78) 1.69 (0.62-4.59)

Fine motor skills 20 (39) 16 (31) 15 (29) 21 (68) 7 (23) 3 (10) 3.23** (1.27-8.33) 3.89** (1.02-14.77)

Ball skills 32 (63) 8 (16) 11 (22) 18 (58) 9 (29) 4 (13) 0.61 (0.23-1.57) 1.86 (0.54-6.44)

Coordination 24 (47) 6 (12) 21(41) 22 (71) 4 (13) 5 (16) 2.75** (1.06-7.12) 3.05* (0.99-9.43)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence 30 (58) 17 (33) 5 (10) 29 (94) 2 (6) 10.63‡ (2.29-49.35) ||

Verbal intelligence 35 (67) 10 (19) 7 (13) 27 (87) 4 (13) 2.66 (0.77-9.10) ||

Performance intelligence 31 (60) 14 (27) 7 (13) 25 (81) 6 (19) 2.82* (0.99-8.06) ||

Visual perception (n=75) 41 (82) 7 (14) 2 (4) 23 (92) 2 (8) 3.63 (0.75-17.70) ||

Visuomotor integration 39 (75) 6 (12) 7 (13) 28 (90) 3 (10) 3.29* (0.85-12.84) ||

Verbal memory 43 (83) 4 (8) 5 (10) 26 (84) 1 (3) 4 (13)

Delayed recall (n=77) 41 (84) 2 (4) 6 (12) 20 (71) 6 (21) 2 (7) 0.53 (0.17-1.61) 1.95 (0.37-10.41)

Recognition (n=78) 42 (86) 7 (14) 0 (0) 24 (83) 2 (7) 3 (10)

Selective attention (n=82) 35 (69) 9 (18) 7 (14) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6) 4.01** (1.22-13.22) 3.54 (0.72-17.36)

Attentional control 33 (63) 6 (12) 13 (25) 26 (84) 2 (6) 3 (10) 3.81** (1.26-11.50) 4.15** (1.10-15.66)

Behavioral outcome (n=81)

Total behavioral problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 20 (65) 5 (16) 6 (19)

Internalizing problems 36 (72) 5 (10) 9 (18) 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29)

Externalizing problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 25 (81) 2 (6) 4 (13)

Executive functioning 42 (84) 6 (12) 2 (4) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6)

183Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

Children with gastrointestinal diseases (n=52) Controls (n=31) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=82)

Movement-ABC total 16 (31) 14 (27) 21 (41) 17 (55) 6 (19) 8 (26) 2.27* (0.90-5.78) 1.69 (0.62-4.59)

Fine motor skills 20 (39) 16 (31) 15 (29) 21 (68) 7 (23) 3 (10) 3.23** (1.27-8.33) 3.89** (1.02-14.77)

Ball skills 32 (63) 8 (16) 11 (22) 18 (58) 9 (29) 4 (13) 0.61 (0.23-1.57) 1.86 (0.54-6.44)

Coordination 24 (47) 6 (12) 21(41) 22 (71) 4 (13) 5 (16) 2.75** (1.06-7.12) 3.05* (0.99-9.43)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence 30 (58) 17 (33) 5 (10) 29 (94) 2 (6) 10.63‡ (2.29-49.35) ||

Verbal intelligence 35 (67) 10 (19) 7 (13) 27 (87) 4 (13) 2.66 (0.77-9.10) ||

Performance intelligence 31 (60) 14 (27) 7 (13) 25 (81) 6 (19) 2.82* (0.99-8.06) ||

Visual perception (n=75) 41 (82) 7 (14) 2 (4) 23 (92) 2 (8) 3.63 (0.75-17.70) ||

Visuomotor integration 39 (75) 6 (12) 7 (13) 28 (90) 3 (10) 3.29* (0.85-12.84) ||

Verbal memory 43 (83) 4 (8) 5 (10) 26 (84) 1 (3) 4 (13)

Delayed recall (n=77) 41 (84) 2 (4) 6 (12) 20 (71) 6 (21) 2 (7) 0.53 (0.17-1.61) 1.95 (0.37-10.41)

Recognition (n=78) 42 (86) 7 (14) 0 (0) 24 (83) 2 (7) 3 (10)

Selective attention (n=82) 35 (69) 9 (18) 7 (14) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6) 4.01** (1.22-13.22) 3.54 (0.72-17.36)

Attentional control 33 (63) 6 (12) 13 (25) 26 (84) 2 (6) 3 (10) 3.81** (1.26-11.50) 4.15** (1.10-15.66)

Behavioral outcome (n=81)

Total behavioral problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 20 (65) 5 (16) 6 (19)

Internalizing problems 36 (72) 5 (10) 9 (18) 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29)

Externalizing problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 25 (81) 2 (6) 4 (13)

Executive functioning 42 (84) 6 (12) 2 (4) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6)

Data are given as number (percentage). Normal was defined as >P15, borderline as P5-P15 and abnormal <P5, with regard to intelligence, normal was defined as IQ >85, borderline as IQ 70-85 and abnormal as IQ <70. Data are given as odds ratio (95% confidence interval) derived from logistic regression analyses, corrected for severe cerebral pathology. *p<.10, **p<.05, ‡p<.01 || Could not be determined due to absence of abnormal controls. Empty fields in OR columns indicate that ORs were not calculated due to lack of significance after comparing the mean scores.1. Odds ratios for borderline and abnormal outcome2 .Odds ratios for abnormal outcome

Children with gastrointestinal diseases (n=52) Controls (n=31) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=82)

Movement-ABC total 16 (31) 14 (27) 21 (41) 17 (55) 6 (19) 8 (26) 2.27* (0.90-5.78) 1.69 (0.62-4.59)

Fine motor skills 20 (39) 16 (31) 15 (29) 21 (68) 7 (23) 3 (10) 3.23** (1.27-8.33) 3.89** (1.02-14.77)

Ball skills 32 (63) 8 (16) 11 (22) 18 (58) 9 (29) 4 (13) 0.61 (0.23-1.57) 1.86 (0.54-6.44)

Coordination 24 (47) 6 (12) 21(41) 22 (71) 4 (13) 5 (16) 2.75** (1.06-7.12) 3.05* (0.99-9.43)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence 30 (58) 17 (33) 5 (10) 29 (94) 2 (6) 10.63‡ (2.29-49.35) ||

Verbal intelligence 35 (67) 10 (19) 7 (13) 27 (87) 4 (13) 2.66 (0.77-9.10) ||

Performance intelligence 31 (60) 14 (27) 7 (13) 25 (81) 6 (19) 2.82* (0.99-8.06) ||

Visual perception (n=75) 41 (82) 7 (14) 2 (4) 23 (92) 2 (8) 3.63 (0.75-17.70) ||

Visuomotor integration 39 (75) 6 (12) 7 (13) 28 (90) 3 (10) 3.29* (0.85-12.84) ||

Verbal memory 43 (83) 4 (8) 5 (10) 26 (84) 1 (3) 4 (13)

Delayed recall (n=77) 41 (84) 2 (4) 6 (12) 20 (71) 6 (21) 2 (7) 0.53 (0.17-1.61) 1.95 (0.37-10.41)

Recognition (n=78) 42 (86) 7 (14) 0 (0) 24 (83) 2 (7) 3 (10)

Selective attention (n=82) 35 (69) 9 (18) 7 (14) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6) 4.01** (1.22-13.22) 3.54 (0.72-17.36)

Attentional control 33 (63) 6 (12) 13 (25) 26 (84) 2 (6) 3 (10) 3.81** (1.26-11.50) 4.15** (1.10-15.66)

Behavioral outcome (n=81)

Total behavioral problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 20 (65) 5 (16) 6 (19)

Internalizing problems 36 (72) 5 (10) 9 (18) 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29)

Externalizing problems 35 (70) 4 (8) 11 (22) 25 (81) 2 (6) 4 (13)

Executive functioning 42 (84) 6 (12) 2 (4) 27 (87) 2 (6) 2 (6)

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 4 Odds ratios for borderline and abnormal outcome in children

with medically and surgically treated NEC and SIP, adjusted for severe

cerebral pathology

MedNEC SurgNEC SIP

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

Motor outcome (n=51)

Movement-ABC total 10.32‡ (2.02-52.52)

Fine motor skills 7.88‡ (2.07-29.94)

Ball skills

Coordination 4.19** (1.25-14.09)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence 5.27* (0.84-32.99) 16.31‡ (2.92-91.15) 11.86‡ (2.21-63.79)

Verbal intelligence 4.73** (1.13-19.68) 3.64* (0.90-14.67)

Performance intelligence 4.69** (1.27-17.27)

Visual perception (n=50)

Visuomotor integration 6.53** (1.4-30.27)

Verbal memory

Delayed recall

Recognition

Selective attention (n=51) 4.73** (1.13-19.68) 3.94* (0.97-16.03)

Attentional control 3.47* (0.85-14.17) 4.62** (1.20-17.83) 3.47* (0.94-12.85)

Behavioral outcome (n=50)

Total behavioral problems

Internalizing problems 0.277* (0.066-1.16)

Externalizing problems

Executive functioning

Data are given as odds ratio (95% confidence interval) for borderline and abnormal outcome derived from logistic regression analyses, corrected for severe cerebral pathology. *p<.10, **p<.05, ‡p<.01Empty fields indicate results with p>.10.

185Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

40

30

20

10

0

14

12

10

8

6

4

2

0

M-A

BC

tot

al s

core

M-A

BC

sub

test

sco

re

Figure 1A Movement-ABC scores in children with gastrointestinal diseases

compared to controls

Total score Fine motor skills Ball skills Coordination

*

**

The Movement-ABC total score is shown on the left y-axis, scores on the subtests are shown on the right y-axis. *Indicates p<.05 in comparison to controls. MedNEC SurgNEC SIP Controls

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Figure 1B IQs in children with gastrointestinal disease compared to controls

140

120

100

80

60

40

IQ

Total IQ Verbal IQ Performance IQ

IQ scores are shown on the y-axis. *Indicates p<.05 in comparison to controls. MedNEC SurgNEC SIP Controls

*

*

**

* *

187Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

Discussion

This study showed that at school age 68% of newborn infants with gastrointestinal

diseases (NEC or SIP) had borderline or abnormal motor outcomes. On average

their intelligence was 11 IQ points lower than matched control children without

gastrointestinal diseases. In addition, their attention and visual perception were

impaired. Visuomotor integration was also affected, albeit to a lesser extent. Behavioral

problems and executive functions were comparable to the control group. Surgically

treated children were at highest risk for abnormal outcome at school age when

compared to the controls. These findings remained present after correction for severe

cerebral pathology of the children.

We found that at a mean age of 9 years 15% of the children with NEC or SIP had

developed CP and as many as 40% had abnormal motor skills. These results were

similar to the findings of previous studies on the neurodevelopmental outcome of

survivors with NEC at 2 to 3 years of age. These studies reported that around 20%

of children develop CP and that 31% of children with NEC have an abnormal (<70)

Psychomotor Developmental Index.4,5

With regard to cognitive outcome we found that the mean IQ of children with NEC or

SIP was 86, which was considerably lower than that of the control children. Although

we found that at school age only 10% of the children had an abnormal total IQ (<70),

43% had IQ scores below 85, which is the approximate cut-off point for being able

to attend regular education in the Netherlands. Hintz et al. found that at 2 years of

age children with NEC have a Mental Developmental Index that is 6 points lower than

controls and that 41% of the children obtain an abnormal score (<70).5 Thus, although

the percentage of children with abnormal intelligence at school age seems smaller in

our study, the difference in mean scores with matched control children seems to be

higher in our population as compared to Hintz’ study. At school age cognition can be

determined more reliably since behavioral aspects fit the testing situation better and

test validity is higher.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

In addition to lower intelligence, we also found poorer selective attention, attentional

control, visual perception, and visuomotor integration in children with NEC or SIP.

These specific cognitive deficits, which may only come to light when the children reach

school age, may hamper school performance even further. A case in point is poor

attention that may affect learning.

With regards to the incidence of behavioral problems and executive functions the study

group was comparable to the controls, even though in both groups the incidence of

behavioral problems was increased compared to the reference population. It is known

that ~20% of preterm infants develop behavioral problems, which is comparable to our

findings.17,18 Apparently, the presence of gastrointestinal disease does not increase this

risk still further.

It is likely that the neurodevelopmental impairments we found in the survivors with NEC

or SIP are the result of a combination of factors the infants were subjected to during

the neonatal period. NEC and associated tissue injury initially leads to an inflammatory

response. The subsequent release of pro-inflammatory cytokines by the activation

of microglial cells in the brain, leads to injury to the pre-oligodendrocytes and axons

in the white matter. These glial cells, which play an important role in myelination of

the immature brain, are highly vulnerable to damage.19 The increased permeability

of the blood-brain barrier during inflammation further increases the susceptibility of

the brain to this inflammatory response.20 Both NEC and SIP can be complicated by

sepsis, which is associated with an increased risk for overt, but also more subtle and

diffuse, white matter abnormalities.3,21,22 Furthermore, infants with gastrointestinal

diseases are at risk for respiratory and circulatory insufficiency which contribute to

cerebral hypoxemia. Indeed, in this study we found that the children with NEC or SIP

had been ventilated significantly more frequently and had required more inotropes as

compared to the controls. Finally, nutritional difficulties and the presence of short bowel

syndrome after surgery are other factors that could contribute to impaired growth and

neurodevelopment.23,24

Our second aim was to identify risk factors for adverse outcome in children with

gastrointestinal diseases. Since infants with SurgNEC had often been severely ill for a

significant length of time - an illness that was frequently complicated with perforation -

we hypothesized that their outcome would be worse than infants with SIP and MedNEC.

189Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

In this light it was striking that we did not find any significant differences in outcome

at school age within the group of children with gastrointestinal diseases, although we

did find a 10% higher mortality rate in children with overall NEC compared to SIP.

Perhaps the pathophysiological mechanisms mainly responsible for worse outcome

were similar among these children irrespective of mode of treatment and additional

disease related factors. The size of the study group may also have limited our ability to

detect significant differences in development between groups.

When compared to controls, however, we did find that surgically treated children

(both NEC and SIP) were at higher risk for worse outcome. Previous studies reported

that two-year-old children with SurgNEC were at higher risk for abnormal motor and

cognitive outcome than children with MedNEC compared to controls.5,25,26 The few

studies that compared the outcome of children with SurgNEC to SIP found worse

outcomes for children with NEC.1,27 In contrast, we found particularly high ORs for

worse motor outcome in children with SIP. We were puzzled by this finding. One could

suggest that the slightly higher rate of severe cerebral pathology in the children with

SIP of our cohort played a role, however, after correction for cerebral pathology our

results did not change. Perhaps it was a chance finding due to the low number of

children in the subgroups.

To the best of our knowledge this is the first study that determines the functional outcome

at school age of children with NEC or SIP. The strength of this study is that we examined

in great detail a broad range of motor and cognitive skills and behavioral aspects that

might limit functional abilities at school age. Moreover, we included control children

that were matched for gender, gestational age, and birth year. A possible limitation is

that this was a single-center study. Moreover, 1 child could not be assessed as a result

of being deaf. Outcome of the children with gastrointestinal diseases may thus have

been slightly worse than we reported.

Our study showed that survivors of either NEC or SIP form a specific high-risk group

for functional impairments at school age, in the majority in the absence of overt brain

pathology as can be detected on cranial ultrasonography. Not only their motor outcome,

but also their cognitive outcome was significantly impaired. Their intelligence, for

example, was barely better compared to preterm children with severe brain lesions.28,29

In our opinion, therefore, these children deserve to be followed-up up to school age

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

since it is likely that they are coping with impairments that require intervention. This

study also touches on the question of the pathophysiological mechanisms responsible

for these long-term functional impairments. We believe that MRI in the neonatal period

may help to clarify these pathophysiological mechanisms, and should be part of the

neurodevelopmental assessment in these children.

Acknowledgements

We greatly acknowledge the help of Dr. T. Brantsma – van Wulfften Palthe for correcting

the English manuscript and Dr. V. Fidler for support in the statistical analysis.

191Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

1. Adesanya OA, O’Shea TM, Turner CS, Amoroso

RM, Morgan TM, Aschner JL. Intestinal perforation

in very low birth weight infants: growth and

neurodevelopment at 1 year of age. J Perinatol

2005;25:583-589.

2. Lin PW, Stoll BJ. Necrotising enterocolitis. Lancet

2006;368:1271-1283.

3. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by

white matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance

imaging at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

4. Soraisham AS, Amin HJ, Al Hindi MY, et al.

Does necrotising enterocolitis impact the

neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes in

preterm infants with birthweight < or =1250 g? J

Paediatr Child Health 2006;42:499-504.

5. Hintz SR, Kendrick DE, Stoll BJ, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of

extremely low birth weight infants after necrotizing

enterocolitis. Pediatrics 2005;115:696-703.

6. Evans WA. An encephalographic ratio for estimating

ventricular enlargement and cerebral atrophy. Arch

Neurol Psychiatry 1942;47:931-937.

7. Bell MJ, Ternberg JL, Feigin RD, et al. Neonatal

necrotizing enterocolitis. Therapeutic decisions

based upon clinical staging. Ann Surg 1978;187:1-7.

8. Bax M, Goldstein M, Rosenbaum P, et al. Proposed

definition and classification of cerebral palsy. Dev

Med Child Neurol 2005;47:571-576.

9. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D, Wood

E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability of a system

to classify gross motor function in children with

cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

10. Smits-Engelsman BCM. Movement Assessment

Battery for Children. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets

& Zeitlinger; 1998.

11. Kort W, Compaan EL, Bleichrodt N, et al. WISC-

III NL Manual. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: NIP

dienstencentrum; 2002.

12. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY II. Clinical

and Interpretative Manual. San Antonio, TX;

PsychCorp. A Brand of Harcourt Assessments:

2007.

13. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545-559.

14. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample

and ADHD performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065-1081.

15. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the

Child Behavior Checklist: 4–18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: Univ. of Vermont, Department of

Psychiatry; 1991.

References

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

16. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L.

BRIEF--Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive

Function: professional manual. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

17. Reijneveld SA, de Kleine MJ, van Baar AL, et al.

Behavioural and emotional problems in very preterm

and very low birthweight infants at age 5 years. Arch

Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2006;91:F423-F428.

18. Gray RF, Indurkhya A, McCormick MC. Prevalence,

stability, and predictors of clinically significant

behavior problems in low birth weight children at 3,

5, and 8 years of age. Pediatrics 2004;114:736-743.

19. Volpe JJ. Postnatal sepsis, necrotizing entercolitis,

and the critical role of systemic inflammation in

white matter injury in premature infants. J Pediatr

2008;153:160-163.

20. Abbott NJ. Inflammatory mediators and modulation

of blood-brain barrier permeability. Cell Mol

Neurobiol 2000;20:131-147.

21. Volpe JJ. Neurobiology of periventricular

leukomalacia in the premature infant. Pediatr Res

2001;50:553-562.

22. Glass HC, Bonifacio SL, Chau V, et al. Recurrent

postnatal infections are associated with progressive

white matter injury in premature infants. Pediatrics

2008;122:299-305.

23. Cole CR, Hansen NI, Higgins RD, Ziegler TR, Stoll

BJ. Very low birth weight preterm infants with

surgical short bowel syndrome: incidence, morbidity

and mortality, and growth outcomes at 18 to 22

months. Pediatrics 2008;122:e573-e582.

24. Lucas A, Morley R, Cole TJ, Gore SM. A

randomised multicentre study of human milk versus

formula and later development in preterm infants.

Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed

1994;70:F141-F146.

25. Tobiansky R, Lui K, Roberts S, Veddovi M.

Neurodevelopmental outcome in very low

birthweight infants with necrotizing enterocolitis

requiring surgery. J Paediatr Child Health

1995;31:233-236.

26. Martin CR, Dammann O, Allred EN, et al.

Neurodevelopment of extremely preterm infants

who had necrotizing enterocolitis with or without

late bacteremia. J Pediatr 2010;157:751-756.

27. Blakely ML, Tyson JE, Lally KP, et al. Laparotomy

versus peritoneal drainage for necrotizing

enterocolitis or isolated intestinal perforation in

extremely low birth weight infants: outcomes

through 18 months adjusted age. Pediatrics

2006;117:e680-e687.

28. Roze E, Van Braeckel KN, van der Veere CN,

Maathuis CG, Martijn A, Bos AF. Functional

outcome at school age of preterm infants with

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics

2009;123:1493-1500.

193Chapter 7 Functional Impairments at School Age of Children with Necrotizing Enterocolitis or Spontaneous Intestinal Perforation

29. Downie AL, Frisk V, Jakobson LS. The impact of

periventricular brain injury on reading and spelling

abilities in the late elementary and adolescent years.

Child Neuropsychol 2005;11:479-495.

Chapter 8

FUNCTIONAL IMPAIRMENTS AT

SCHOOL AGE OF PRETERM BORN

CHILDREN WITH LATE-ONSET SEPSIS

MEIKE H. VAN DER REE, JOZIEN C. TANIS,

KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL, AREND F. BOS, ELISE ROZE

EARLY HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 2011; DOI 10.1016/J.EARLHUMDEV.2011.06.008

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Background

Late-onset sepsis is a relatively common complication particularly of preterm birth that

affects approximately a quarter of very low birth weight infants.

Aim

We aimed to determine the motor, cognitive, and behavioural outcome at school age of

preterm children with late-onset sepsis compared to matched controls.

Study design and subjects

A prospective case-control study that included preterm infants (gestational age<32

weeks and/or birth weight<1500 grams) admitted to our Neonatal Intensive Care

Unit in 2000- 2001 with a culture-proven late-onset sepsis, and controls matched for

gestational age.

Outcome measures

At school age we assessed motor skills, intelligence, visual perception, visuomotor

integration, verbal memory, attention, executive functioning, and behaviour.

Results

At 6-9 years, 21 of 32 children with late-onset sepsis (68%) had borderline or abnormal

motor outcome with most problems in fine motor skills. Their total IQ was 89 compared

to 98 in controls. In addition, verbal memory and attention were affected compared to

controls (0.61 standard deviations (SD), 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.04-1.17, p=.033

and 0.94 SD, 95% CI 0.32-1.62, p=.011, respectively). Multiple episodes of sepsis and

gram-negative sepsis were risk factors for worse cognitive outcome.

Conclusions

At school age, a majority of preterm children with late-onset sepsis had motor problems.

Their IQ was considerably lower than matched controls, and memory and attention

were specifically impaired. Outcome at school age of preterm children with late-onset

sepsis was worse than previously thought.

197Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Introduction

Late-onset sepsis is still a common complication in preterm infants admitted to

the neonatal intensive care unit despite a variety of strategies to prevent infection.

Among very low birth weight infants (<1500 grams), who are highly susceptible

to infection, around 25% develop one or more episodes of late-onset sepsis.1

Gram-positive bacteria, particularly coagulase-negative staphylococci (CoNS), are

the most common pathogens leading to late-onset sepsis.1 Mortality in preterm

infants with late-onset sepsis is about 20%.1

During the early neonatal period the brain and its white matter are vulnerable to

inflammation and changes in cerebral blood flow that can follow from late-onset

sepsis. Previous studies have indeed shown a relation between sepsis and white

matter abnormalities in preterm infants.2,3 These abnormalities contribute to the

risk of neurodevelopmental impairments among preterm infants with late-onset

sepsis. An earlier study reported that approximately 30% of children with neonatal

sepsis have motor impairments at 2 years of age, while even more children may

develop cognitive impairments.4 It is unknown whether these impairments are

persistent throughout school age, and whether specific cognitive deficits that may

further hamper school performance, are present.5

The first aim of our study was to determine the motor, cognitive, and behavioural

outcome at school age of children with late-onset sepsis compared to control

children of similar gestational age. Our second aim was to identify sepsis-related

risk factors for adverse outcome.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Methods

Patients

We retrospectively included preterm infants (gestational age <32 weeks and/or

birth weight <1500 grams) from the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU) of the

University Medical Center Groningen, who had been admitted between November

2000 and December 2001 and were diagnosed with late-onset. Late-onset sepsis

was defined as a positive blood culture occurring 96 hours or more after birth. We

also included control infants from our NICU. For every 2 infants with late-onset

sepsis we selected 1 control infant. These control infants were born in the same

period and matched for gestational age. We did not include infants, as either cases

or controls, in whom the diagnosis of late-onset sepsis was suspected, but not

confirmed by positive blood cultures. Infants with major congenital malformations

or syndromes were also not included.

During the study period, a total of 249 infants with gestational age <32 weeks and/

or birth weight <1500 grams were admitted to our NICU. After database search, 51

infants with late-onset sepsis (20%) were included in the study. Of 51 infants with

late-onset sepsis, 10 (20%) died in the neonatal period. A total of 41 survivors with

late-onset sepsis remained. Of these, 3 children were excluded since they were

diagnosed with neurofibromatosis I, Bartter syndrome and Fallot’s tetralogy. Two

sets of parents could not be traced. We then included 18 control infants born in

2000 and 2001 from our NICU for follow-up, since we aimed to include 1 control

for every 2 infants with late-onset sepsis. After inviting the parents and children for

follow-up, it appeared that 4 sets of parents declined the invitation to participate.

The final number of included children is thus 32 children with late-onset sepsis

(78%) and 18 control children.

199Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Perinatal and neonatal risk factors

We reviewed the medical charts of the patients for neonatal and sepsis-related

characteristics. We used the Score for Neonatal Acute Physiology Index, second

version (SNAP-II), to compare newborn illness severity in the late-onset sepsis

group with the control group. The SNAP-II is validated to predict risk of in-hospital

morbidity. This physiology-based score uses 6 routinely available vital signs and

laboratory results from the first 24 hours after birth.6 The higher the total score, the

more severe the infant’s illness.

Follow-up

The parents of the eligible patients were asked to bring their children to an extension

of the routine follow-up program for the research study. It entailed the assessment

of motor performance, cognition, and behaviour at the age of 6 to 9 years. Parents

gave their written informed consent to participate in the follow-up program and to

publication of the results. The total duration of the examination was approximately

2.5 hours including breaks. Incomplete assessments and test scores obtained

when a child was too tired or uncooperative, as assessed by the experimenter,

were excluded. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the

University Medical Center Groningen.

Motor Outcome

On the basis of the reports of the routine follow-up program we determined the

presence or absence of cerebral palsy (CP) following Bax’ criteria.7 In case of CP,

gross motor functioning was scored using the Gross Motor Function Classification

System (GMFCS). This is a functional, five level classification system for CP based

on self-initiated movement with particular emphasis on sitting (truncal control) and

walking.8 Higher GMFCS levels indicate more functional impairments.

To assess the children’s motor outcome we administered the Movement Assessment

Battery for Children (Movement ABC), a standardised test of motor skills for children.9

This test yields a score for total movement performance based on separate subscores

for manual dexterity (fine motor skills), ball skills, and static and dynamic balance

(coordination). The higher the score, the poorer the performance.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Cognitive Outcome

Total, verbal, and performance intelligence were assessed using a short form of

the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition, Dutch version (WISC-

III-NL).10,11

In addition, we assessed visual perception and visuomotor integration with

the subtests ‘geometric puzzles’ and ‘design copying’ of the NEPSY-II

(Neuropsychological Assessment, second edition), a neuropsychological test

battery for children.12 In geometric puzzles, the child is asked to match two shapes

outside a grid with shapes on the inside. In design copying, the child is asked to

reproduce geometric drawings of increasing complexity. Visuomotor integration

involves the integration of visual information with finger-hand movements.

We assessed verbal memory using a standardised Dutch version of the Rey’s

Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).13 This test consists of five learning trials with

immediate recall of fifteen words (tested after each presentation) and a delayed

recall trial followed by a recognition trial.

We measured selective attention and attentional control with the subtests ‘map

mission’ and ‘opposite worlds’ of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-

Ch).14 Selective attention refers to a child’s ability to select target information from

an array of distracters. Attentional control refers to the ability to shift attention

flexibly and adaptively.

To obtain information on attentional functioning in daily life, the parents filled out

an Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) questionnaire containing 18

items on inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity.15

Finally, the parents filled out the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function

(BRIEF) to assess executive functioning involved in well-organised, purposeful,

goal-directed, and problem-solving behavior.16 Examples of executive functioning

are the ability to inhibit competing actions towards attractive stimuli, the flexibility

to shift problem-solving strategies if necessary, and the ability to monitor and

evaluate one’s own behaviour.

201Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Behavioural Outcome

To obtain information on the children’s behavioural and/or emotional competencies

and problems, the parents completed the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL).17 The

CBCL consists of one total scale and two subscales, i.e. internalizing problems

(withdrawn behaviour, somatic complaints, and anxious and/or depressed scales)

and externalizing problems (delinquent and aggressive behaviour scales).

Statistical Analysis

We classified the intelligence quotients (IQs) into ‘normal’ (IQ ≥ 85), ‘borderline’

(IQ 70-85) and ‘abnormal’ (IQ<70). We used the percentiles on the standardization

samples of the Movement ABC and cognitive tests to classify raw scores into ‘normal’

(>P15), ‘borderline’ (P5-P15) and ‘abnormal’ (<P5). For the ADHD questionnaire,

BRIEF and CBCL we used a similar classification following the criteria in the manual.

Visual inspection of the histograms and quantile-quantile (Q-Q) plots were used to

determine which outcome measures were normally distributed. We then used the

Student’s t, Mann-Whitney U, and Chi2 tests where appropriate, to compare the

outcome measures of the study group with the control group and to relate disease

characteristics to outcome. We used backward logistic regression analyses to

calculate the odds ratios (OR) for worse outcome when comparing the children

with late-onset sepsis to the controls. Patient demographics that were different

in the sepsis group compared to the controls (p<.10) were entered as potential

confounders in the logistic regression model. Throughout the analyses p<.05 was

considered to be statistically significant. SPSS 16.0 software for Windows, (SPSS

Inc, Chicago, IL) was used for all the analyses.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Table 1 shows an overview of the patient demographics of the 32 infants with late-

onset sepsis and the 18 controls. There were no significant or nearly significant

differences in demographics between the groups, therefore none of these

characteristics were entered as potential confounders in the logistic regression

model for the prediction of outcome.

Eight children (25%) had more than one episode of sepsis. Thirty children had

a gram-positive blood culture (n=27 coagulase-negative Staphylococci and n=4

Staphylococcus aureus). Four children had a gram-negative blood culture caused

by Enterobacter cloacae in 2 infants, by Klebsiella pneumoniae in 1, and by

Klebsiella oxytoca in 1. Thus two children had both a gram-positive and a gram-

negative blood culture. None of the children had a fungal sepsis.

Motor Outcome

The mean age at follow up was 8.4 years (range 6.8 to 9.1 years). Of 32 children

with late-onset sepsis, 5 developed CP (16%). Their functional impairments were

limited to GMFCS level I in 1 child and level II in 2 children. Two children had more

severe functional impairments with GMFCS level III and IV. In the control group

none of the children developed CP.

Mean scores on the Movement ABC are shown in Table 2. The two children with

severe CP could not be assessed by the Movement ABC because the tasks were

too difficult for them. Children with late-onset sepsis showed significantly worse

fine motor skills, and a trend towards a worse total Movement ABC score.

Table 3 shows the motor outcome classified into three different groups: normal

(P>15), borderline (P5- P15) and abnormal (<P15). This is graphically shown in Figure

1. The two children with severe CP were included in the category ‘abnormal’.

As it is apparent from Table 3, 68% of the children with late-onset sepsis obtained

a borderline or an abnormal total score on the Movement ABC, with an OR for

the borderline and abnormal outcome of 3.30. In accordance with analyses of the

mean scores, most problems were found in fine motor skills (OR 5.46).

203Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Cognitive and Behavioural Outcome

Eight children (25%) with late-onset sepsis required special education compared to

none of the children in the control group (p=.026). Table 2 shows the mean scores on

the cognitive and behavioural measures. For two children with late-onset sepsis, the

neuropsychological tests were too difficult because of very low intellectual development.

The children with late-onset sepsis had significantly lower total and verbal IQs than

controls. In addition, verbal memory was worse in children with sepsis than in the controls

(0.61 SD, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.04-1.17). This was also the case for attentional

control (0.94 SD, 95% CI 0.32-1.62, Table 2). The incidence of behavioural problems

was comparable between the groups. Table 3 shows the cognitive and behavioural

outcome classified in normal, borderline, and abnormal. The children of whom the

neuropsychological functions could not be assessed were included in the category

‘abnormal’. Total IQ was borderline or abnormal in 44% of the children with late-onset

sepsis compared to 6% in the controls with an OR of 13.22. In addition, we found that of

the 18 children with normal total IQs, still 9 had problems in attention or memory.

The ORs confirmed the analyses of the mean scores, apart from attentional control for

which the OR was not significant.

Disease Characteristics in relation to Outcome

Subsequently, we determined whether certain disease characteristics of children

with late-onset sepsis were related to their outcome at school age. The disease

characteristics concerned were multiple episodes of sepsis, whether gram-negative or

gram-positive pathogens caused late-onset sepsis, and the children’s postmenstrual

age at development of late-onset sepsis.

Children who had more than one episode of sepsis had significantly lower IQs

than children with a single episode (79 versus 92, p=.02). They also showed worse

attentional control (p=.03) and visual perception (p=.02). The mean verbal IQ in children

with gram-negative pathogens was 16 points lower compared to children who only had

gram-positive pathogens (77 versus 93, p=.042) and showed a trend towards worse

total IQ (78 versus 91, p=.085). The scores on attentional control were also lower in

children with gram-negative pathogens (p=.037). There was no association between

the postmenstrual age at development of late-onset sepsis and outcome.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Patient demographics

Late-onset sepsis

(n=32)

Controls (n=18) p-value+

Males/females 22/10 9/9 0.16

Gestational age (weeks) 28.9 (25.7-33.4) 28.9 (25.7-33.6) 0.74

Birth weight (grams) 1010 (600-1690) 1122 (640-1455) 0.81

Very Low Birth Weight n=29 (91) n=18 (100) 0.25

IUGR (<P10) n=12 (38) n=5 (28) 0.35

Apgar at 5 minutes 8 (5-10) 9 (1-10) 0.67

SNAP-II score 14 (5-40) 19 (5-31) 0.26

Asphyxia none none #

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO) n=29 (91) n=14 (78) 0.20

Inotropics n=5 (16) n= 4 (22) 0.41

Cerebral pathology

Mild GMH-IVH1 n=9 (29) n=4 (22) 0.48

Severe GMH-IVH1 n=1 (3) None 0.66

Cystic PVL None None #

Noncystic PVL3 n=18 (56) n=8 (44) 0.38

Late-onset morbidity

Retinopathy of prematurity2 n=1 (3) None 0.64

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia n=9 (28) n=4 (22) 0.46

Meningitis n=2 (6) none 0.41

Necrotizing enterocolitis n=2 (6) n=2 (11) 0.46

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or as numbers (percentage). None of the patient demographics were significantly different between the groups. # Could not be determined due to absence of the demographic in both groups. + p-values derived from chi2 and Mann-Whitney U tests1. Mild GMH-IVH was defined as grade I and II, severe GMH-IVH as grade III and periventricular hemorrhagic infarction 2. Retinopathy of prematurity grade III and worse 3. Defined as periventricular echodensities present for more than 1 weekAbbreviations: IUGR- intrauterine growth restriction; IPPV- intermittent positive pressure ventilation; HFO- high frequency oscillation; GMH-IVH- germinal matrix haemorrhage-intraventricular haemorrhage; PVL- periventricular leukomalacia

205Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

TABLE 2 Motor, cognitive, and behavioural outcome in preterm born

children with late-onset sepsis versus controls

Data are given as mean (standard deviation, range). ns; not significant (p>.10)+ p-values derived from Student’s t and Mann-Whitney U tests, *p<.10, **p<.051. Two children with late-onset sepsis had severe functional impairments and could not be tested as a result2. Raw scores3. Intelligence quotients4. Percentiles5. T-scores

Late-onset sepsis (n=30)1

Controls (n=18) p-value+

Motor outcome (n=47)2

Movement ABC Total 13 (10, 1.5-39) 8 (6, 5-18) .073*

Fine motor skills 6 (4, 0-15) 3 (3, 0-8) .039**

Ball skills 3 (3, 0-10) 3 (3, 0-8.5) ns

Coordination 4 (5, 0-10) 2 (2, 0-6.5) ns

Cognitive outcome (n=48)

Total intelligence3 89 (14, 55-118) 98 (8, 82-110) .012**

Verbal intelligence3 91 (15, 55-128) 102 (14, 81-128) .015**

Performance intelligence3 87 (15, 55-118) 95 (10, 80-113) .060*

Visual perception (n=37)4 57 (25, 0.1-98) 61 (27, 5-95) ns

Visuomotor integration (n=47)4 56 (37, 2-100) 66 (30, 5-100) ns

Verbal memory (n=47)4 32 (26, 0.8-86) 50 (28, 2-88) .033**

Delayed recall (n=46)4 29 (24, 0.1-90) 49 (33, 6-98) .048**

Recognition (n=47)2 29 (1, 27-30) 29 (2, 21-30) ns

Selective attention (n=47)4 33 (27, 0.1-84) 47 (31, 2-98) ns

Attentional control (n=47)4 20 (20, 0.1-63) 45 (33, 1-84) .011**

Behavioural outcome

Total behavioural problems (n=48)5 53 (12, 25-75) 56 (9, 38-71) ns

Internalizing problems (n=48)5 52 (13, 33-78) 57 (11, 39-70) ns

Externalizing problems (n=48)5 49 (11, 33-70) 53 (9, 34-60) ns

ADHD symptoms (n=40)2 15 (14, 0-52) 18 (17, 0-53) ns

Executive functioning (n=46)4 46 (31, 4-95) 50 (22, 3-97) ns

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 3 Outcome of preterm born children with late-onset sepsis

classified into normal, borderline, and abnormal versus controls

Children with late-onset sepsis (n=32)

Controls (n=18) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=49)

Movement ABC Total 10 (32) 9 (29) 12 (39) 11 (61) 2 (11) 5 (28) 3.30 (0.98-11.07)* 1.64 (0.47-5.79)

Fine motor skills 10 (32) 10 (32) 11 (36) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 5.46 (1.52-19.58)*** 4.40 (0.85-22.77)*

Ball skills 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29) 12 (67) 3 (17) 3 (17) 1.44 (0.43-4.85) 2.05 (0.47-8.83)

Coordination 15 (48) 6 (19) 10 (32) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 3.81 (0.73-19.87)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=50) 18 (56) 9 (28) 5 (16) 17 (94) 1 (6) 13.22 (1.57-111.74)** #

Verbal intelligence 17 (53) 11 (34) 4 (13) 15 (83) 3 (17) 4.41 (1.07-18.27)** #

Performance intelligence 16 (50) 11 (35) 5 (16) 14 (78) 4 (22) 3.50 (0.95-12.97)* #

Visual perception (n=39) 22 (88) 3 (12) 13 (93) 1 (7) 1.77 (0.17-18.86) #

Visuomotor integration (n=49) 21 (68) 5 (16) 5 (16) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 3.27 (0.35-30.48)

Verbal memory (n=49) 18 (58) 6 (19) 7 (23) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 5.78 (1.13-29.61)** 4.96 (0.56-44.10)

Delayed recall (n=48) 19 (63) 4 (13) 7 (23) 13 (72) 5 (28) 1.51 (0.42-5.37) #

Recognition (n=49) 26 (84) 3 (10) 2 (7) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 0.50 (0.12-2.04) 0.35 (0.05-2.29)

Selective attention (n=49) 21 (68) 4 (13) 6 (19) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 4.08 (0.45-37.00)

Attentional control (n=49) 15 (48) 5 (16) 11 (36) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 2.75 (0.65-11.62)

Behavioural outcome (n=48)

Total behavioural problems 21 (70) 3 (10) 6 (20) 12 (67) 2 (11) 4 (22) 0.86 (0.25-3.00) 0.88 (0.21-3.64)

Internalizing problems 20 (67) 3 (10) 7 (23) 9 (50) 3 (17) 6 (33) 0.50 (0.15-1.63) 0.61 (1.67-2.22)

Externalizing problems 23 (77) 3 (10) 4 (13) 14 (78) 2 (11) 2 (11) 1.07 (0.26-4.31) 1.23 (0.21-7.51)

ADHD symptoms (n=40) 23 (92) 2 (8) 13 (87) 2 (13) # 0.57 (0.07-4.50)

Executive functioning (n=46) 24 (83) 4 (14) 1 (3) 15 (88) 1 (6) 1 (6) 1.56 (0.27-9.10) 0.57 (0.03-9.7)

Children with late-onset sepsis (n=32)

Controls (n=18) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=49)

Movement ABC Total 10 (32) 9 (29) 12 (39) 11 (61) 2 (11) 5 (28) 3.30 (0.98-11.07)* 1.64 (0.47-5.79)

Fine motor skills 10 (32) 10 (32) 11 (36) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 5.46 (1.52-19.58)*** 4.40 (0.85-22.77)*

Ball skills 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29) 12 (67) 3 (17) 3 (17) 1.44 (0.43-4.85) 2.05 (0.47-8.83)

Coordination 15 (48) 6 (19) 10 (32) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 3.81 (0.73-19.87)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=50) 18 (56) 9 (28) 5 (16) 17 (94) 1 (6) 13.22 (1.57-111.74)** #

Verbal intelligence 17 (53) 11 (34) 4 (13) 15 (83) 3 (17) 4.41 (1.07-18.27)** #

Performance intelligence 16 (50) 11 (35) 5 (16) 14 (78) 4 (22) 3.50 (0.95-12.97)* #

Visual perception (n=39) 22 (88) 3 (12) 13 (93) 1 (7) 1.77 (0.17-18.86) #

Visuomotor integration (n=49) 21 (68) 5 (16) 5 (16) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 3.27 (0.35-30.48)

Verbal memory (n=49) 18 (58) 6 (19) 7 (23) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 5.78 (1.13-29.61)** 4.96 (0.56-44.10)

Delayed recall (n=48) 19 (63) 4 (13) 7 (23) 13 (72) 5 (28) 1.51 (0.42-5.37) #

Recognition (n=49) 26 (84) 3 (10) 2 (7) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 0.50 (0.12-2.04) 0.35 (0.05-2.29)

Selective attention (n=49) 21 (68) 4 (13) 6 (19) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 4.08 (0.45-37.00)

Attentional control (n=49) 15 (48) 5 (16) 11 (36) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 2.75 (0.65-11.62)

Behavioural outcome (n=48)

Total behavioural problems 21 (70) 3 (10) 6 (20) 12 (67) 2 (11) 4 (22) 0.86 (0.25-3.00) 0.88 (0.21-3.64)

Internalizing problems 20 (67) 3 (10) 7 (23) 9 (50) 3 (17) 6 (33) 0.50 (0.15-1.63) 0.61 (1.67-2.22)

Externalizing problems 23 (77) 3 (10) 4 (13) 14 (78) 2 (11) 2 (11) 1.07 (0.26-4.31) 1.23 (0.21-7.51)

ADHD symptoms (n=40) 23 (92) 2 (8) 13 (87) 2 (13) # 0.57 (0.07-4.50)

Executive functioning (n=46) 24 (83) 4 (14) 1 (3) 15 (88) 1 (6) 1 (6) 1.56 (0.27-9.10) 0.57 (0.03-9.7)

207Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Children with late-onset sepsis (n=32)

Controls (n=18) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=49)

Movement ABC Total 10 (32) 9 (29) 12 (39) 11 (61) 2 (11) 5 (28) 3.30 (0.98-11.07)* 1.64 (0.47-5.79)

Fine motor skills 10 (32) 10 (32) 11 (36) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 5.46 (1.52-19.58)*** 4.40 (0.85-22.77)*

Ball skills 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29) 12 (67) 3 (17) 3 (17) 1.44 (0.43-4.85) 2.05 (0.47-8.83)

Coordination 15 (48) 6 (19) 10 (32) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 3.81 (0.73-19.87)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=50) 18 (56) 9 (28) 5 (16) 17 (94) 1 (6) 13.22 (1.57-111.74)** #

Verbal intelligence 17 (53) 11 (34) 4 (13) 15 (83) 3 (17) 4.41 (1.07-18.27)** #

Performance intelligence 16 (50) 11 (35) 5 (16) 14 (78) 4 (22) 3.50 (0.95-12.97)* #

Visual perception (n=39) 22 (88) 3 (12) 13 (93) 1 (7) 1.77 (0.17-18.86) #

Visuomotor integration (n=49) 21 (68) 5 (16) 5 (16) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 3.27 (0.35-30.48)

Verbal memory (n=49) 18 (58) 6 (19) 7 (23) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 5.78 (1.13-29.61)** 4.96 (0.56-44.10)

Delayed recall (n=48) 19 (63) 4 (13) 7 (23) 13 (72) 5 (28) 1.51 (0.42-5.37) #

Recognition (n=49) 26 (84) 3 (10) 2 (7) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 0.50 (0.12-2.04) 0.35 (0.05-2.29)

Selective attention (n=49) 21 (68) 4 (13) 6 (19) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 4.08 (0.45-37.00)

Attentional control (n=49) 15 (48) 5 (16) 11 (36) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 2.75 (0.65-11.62)

Behavioural outcome (n=48)

Total behavioural problems 21 (70) 3 (10) 6 (20) 12 (67) 2 (11) 4 (22) 0.86 (0.25-3.00) 0.88 (0.21-3.64)

Internalizing problems 20 (67) 3 (10) 7 (23) 9 (50) 3 (17) 6 (33) 0.50 (0.15-1.63) 0.61 (1.67-2.22)

Externalizing problems 23 (77) 3 (10) 4 (13) 14 (78) 2 (11) 2 (11) 1.07 (0.26-4.31) 1.23 (0.21-7.51)

ADHD symptoms (n=40) 23 (92) 2 (8) 13 (87) 2 (13) # 0.57 (0.07-4.50)

Executive functioning (n=46) 24 (83) 4 (14) 1 (3) 15 (88) 1 (6) 1 (6) 1.56 (0.27-9.10) 0.57 (0.03-9.7)

Data are given as number (percentage). Normal was defined as <P15, borderline as P5-P15 and abnormal<P5, with regard to intelligence, normal was defined as IQ>85, borderline as IQ 70-85 and abnormal as IQ<70. OR- odds ratio; CI- confidence interval, *p<.10, **p<.05, ***p<.01, # Could not be determined due to absence of borderline or abnormal controls.1. ORs for borderline and abnormal outcome 2. ORs for abnormal outcome

Children with late-onset sepsis (n=32)

Controls (n=18) OR (95% CI)1 OR (95% CI)2

Normal Borderline Abnormal Normal Borderline Abnormal

Motor outcome (n=49)

Movement ABC Total 10 (32) 9 (29) 12 (39) 11 (61) 2 (11) 5 (28) 3.30 (0.98-11.07)* 1.64 (0.47-5.79)

Fine motor skills 10 (32) 10 (32) 11 (36) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 5.46 (1.52-19.58)*** 4.40 (0.85-22.77)*

Ball skills 18 (58) 4 (13) 9 (29) 12 (67) 3 (17) 3 (17) 1.44 (0.43-4.85) 2.05 (0.47-8.83)

Coordination 15 (48) 6 (19) 10 (32) 13 (72) 3 (17) 2 (11) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 3.81 (0.73-19.87)

Cognitive outcome

Total intelligence (n=50) 18 (56) 9 (28) 5 (16) 17 (94) 1 (6) 13.22 (1.57-111.74)** #

Verbal intelligence 17 (53) 11 (34) 4 (13) 15 (83) 3 (17) 4.41 (1.07-18.27)** #

Performance intelligence 16 (50) 11 (35) 5 (16) 14 (78) 4 (22) 3.50 (0.95-12.97)* #

Visual perception (n=39) 22 (88) 3 (12) 13 (93) 1 (7) 1.77 (0.17-18.86) #

Visuomotor integration (n=49) 21 (68) 5 (16) 5 (16) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 3.27 (0.35-30.48)

Verbal memory (n=49) 18 (58) 6 (19) 7 (23) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 5.78 (1.13-29.61)** 4.96 (0.56-44.10)

Delayed recall (n=48) 19 (63) 4 (13) 7 (23) 13 (72) 5 (28) 1.51 (0.42-5.37) #

Recognition (n=49) 26 (84) 3 (10) 2 (7) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 0.50 (0.12-2.04) 0.35 (0.05-2.29)

Selective attention (n=49) 21 (68) 4 (13) 6 (19) 16 (89) 1 (6) 1 (6) 3.81 (0.73-19.87) 4.08 (0.45-37.00)

Attentional control (n=49) 15 (48) 5 (16) 11 (36) 13 (72) 2 (11) 3 (17) 2.77 (0.79-9.67) 2.75 (0.65-11.62)

Behavioural outcome (n=48)

Total behavioural problems 21 (70) 3 (10) 6 (20) 12 (67) 2 (11) 4 (22) 0.86 (0.25-3.00) 0.88 (0.21-3.64)

Internalizing problems 20 (67) 3 (10) 7 (23) 9 (50) 3 (17) 6 (33) 0.50 (0.15-1.63) 0.61 (1.67-2.22)

Externalizing problems 23 (77) 3 (10) 4 (13) 14 (78) 2 (11) 2 (11) 1.07 (0.26-4.31) 1.23 (0.21-7.51)

ADHD symptoms (n=40) 23 (92) 2 (8) 13 (87) 2 (13) # 0.57 (0.07-4.50)

Executive functioning (n=46) 24 (83) 4 (14) 1 (3) 15 (88) 1 (6) 1 (6) 1.56 (0.27-9.10) 0.57 (0.03-9.7)

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abnormal

Borderline

Normal

LOS- late-onset sepsis. *p<.10, **p<.05

LOS controls

Total scoreLOS controls

Fine motor skillsLOS controls

Ball skillsLOS controls

Coordination

100%

80%

60%

40%

20%

0%

* **

Figure 1 Motor outcome according to the Movement ABC in preterm born

children with late-onset sepsis compared to controls

209Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Discussion

This study demonstrated that motor outcome at school age of a majority

of preterm infants who survived late-onset sepsis was either borderline or

abnormal. On average, their intelligence was 9 points lower than matched control

children without late-onset sepsis. In addition, attention and verbal memory

were specifically impaired, while visual perception, visuomotor integration, and

executive functioning were not affected. The incidence of behavioural problems

was comparable between the groups. Within the group of children with late-onset

sepsis, more than one episode of sepsis and sepsis caused by gram-negative

bacteria were risk factors for worse outcome.

Late-onset sepsis remains a significant neonatal complication that affects up to

16,000 VLBW infants annually in the United States alone.1,18 To the best of our

knowledge this is the first study that established the functional outcome at school

age of children with late-onset sepsis. Our study showed that at a mean of 8.4

years of age, 39% of children with late-onset sepsis had abnormal Movement

ABC scores. This is higher than the previous findings of Stoll et al. in a large cohort

study (n=6314) on early outcome in extremely low birth weight infants (<1000g)

with either early or late-onset sepsis. They found that at 2 years of age, 27% of

children with sepsis had an abnormal (<70) Psychomotor Developmental Index

score of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development.4

Regarding cognition, we found that the IQs of our study group were considerably

lower than in the controls. Moreover, at school age 44% had IQ scores below 85,

which is an approximate cut-off point for being able to attend regular education

in the Netherlands. In the controls this was only 6%. Stoll et al. found that 37%

of children with sepsis had an abnormal (<70) Mental Developmental Index (MDI)

score of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development at 2 years of age, compared to

22% in the controls.4

Our study included infants of <32 weeks of gestational age and late-onset sepsis,

while the study by Stoll et al. included only extremely low birth weight infants of

lower gestational ages than our study. It thus appears that the motor and cognitive

impairments as found in preterm infants with late-onset sepsis at school age

are worse than previously reported. At school age, outcome can be determined

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

more reliably due to higher test validity and behaviour of the children that fits the

testing situation better. Moreover, at school age more subtle cognitive deficits may

come to light since school is more demanding which could make compensation

strategies more difficult to use.

In addition to lower intelligence, we also found poorer attentional control and

verbal memory. These specific cognitive deficits, which have not been identified

previously, can further hamper school performance. Poor attention and verbal

memory, for example, may both affect learning. One could speculate that these

impairments are attributable to lower IQs, especially verbal IQ, which may have had

an influence on verbal memory. We also found children with normal IQs, however,

who had problems with attention and verbal memory. It thus appears that sepsis-

induced brain disruptions in particular have a negative impact on the development

of networks in the brain involved in these functions.

It is likely that the functional impairments as found in our study are the result of a

multifactorial process leading to brain injury in which inflammatory mediators play

an essential role. The preterm brain and its white matter are highly vulnerable to

damage by inflammation and ischemia.19 During infectious episodes, the brain

is exposed to pro-inflammatory cytokines released by microglial cells. These

cytokines inhibit proliferation of neuronal precursor cells and contribute to damage

to the pre-oligodendrocytes, which play an important role in myelination of the

brain.20,21 Ischemia of brain areas can result from hypotension which is associated

with sepsis. Recently, it was postulated that the presence of systemic cytokines

may be related to a disturbance in cerebrovascular autoregulation and diminished

cerebral blood flow.21,22 In addition to the role of pro-inflammatory cytokines,

the injurious effects of microglial activation also relate to the release of reactive

oxygen and nitrogen species in infants with sepsis.23 Finally, sepsis and the

subsequent increase in cytokines are related to the development of germinal matrix

haemorrhage-intraventricular haemorrhage (GMH-IVH), particularly in infants with

early-onset sepsis.24 In our series GMH-IVH was already present before the late-

onset sepsis occurred. However the slight increased number of GMH-IVH among

infants with late-onset sepsis versus controls, although not significant, may have

worsened neurodevelopmental outcome. It has been postulated that brain injury

211Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

in the setting of systemic inflammation not only leads to destruction of tissue but

may also lead to altered brain development, even though the exact mechanisms

have yet to be uncovered.20

Our secondary aim was to identify sepsis-related risk factors for adverse outcome.

In our study, multiple episodes of sepsis were related to worse cognitive outcome.

Progressive white matter injury which follows from recurrent infection may be

responsible for this finding.25 Additionally, a gram-negative sepsis was related to

worse cognitive outcome even though it was found only in four children. Previous

studies already showed that infants with a gram-negative sepsis are often more

severely ill, have a higher mortality rate, and may have a different immune response

than infants with gram-positive infections.26,27

A limitation of this study was the relatively small study and control group. Since this

was a single centre study, generalizability to other centres needs to be established.

Moreover, not all the tests were completed by all the children due to unwillingness

to cooperate because the tasks were too difficult. This was particularly the case in

the sepsis group. Outcome in the sepsis group may thus have been even slightly

worse than we reported. We included fewer control children than children with

late-onset sepsis in the study since we were particularly interested in the outcome

of children with late-onset sepsis and therefore used standardised tests. We

included the control children to take into account the degree of prematurity. We

aimed at including control children that were as similar as possible in neonatal

characteristics compared to the study group, which is also apparent from the

clinical characteristics and SNAP scores of the children, to rule out the potential

role of other risk factors for adverse outcome. The strengths of this study are

that we examined in great detail a broad range of motor and cognitive skills and

behavioural aspects that might limit functional abilities at school age compared to

matched control children.

The functional impairments at school age as identified in the present study were

worse than one would expect based on previous outcome studies at 2 years of

age. We believe that in children with late-onset sepsis the focus of attention should

be on early identification of children at risk for functional impairments so as not

to miss opportunities for intervention. In addition, preventive measures beyond

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

antibiotics should be considered to prevent brain pathology. Previous quality

improvement studies showed that it may be possible to reduce infection rates

and to improve the outcome of neonatal care by implementation of preventive

measures.28,29 More research is needed to determine the exact pathophysiological

mechanisms responsible for the neurodevelopmental impairments in children

with late-onset sepsis. In particular, the question why certain children do and

others do not exhibit significant functional impairments at school age. Differences

in neuroprotective capacities of the brain, but also differences in environmental

aspects during childhood, may play a role.

Acknowledgements

This study was part of the research program of the Research School for Behavioural

and Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Groningen, the Netherlands. We greatly

acknowledge the help of Dr. T. Brantsma – van Wulfften Palthe for correcting the

English manuscript. Elise Roze was financially supported by a Junior Scientific

Master Class grant of the Research School for Behavioural and Cognitive

Neurosciences, University of Groningen, the Netherlands. No study sponsors were

involved.

213Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

1. Stoll BJ, Hansen N, Fanaroff AA, et al. Late-

onset sepsis in very low birth weight neonates:

The experience of the NICHD Neonatal Research

Network. Pediatrics 2002;110:285-291.

2. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by

white matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance

imaging at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

3. Graham EM, Holcroft CI, Rai KK, Donohue PK, Allen

MC. Neonatal cerebral white matter injury in preterm

infants is associated with culture positive infections

and only rarely with metabolic acidosis. Am J Obstet

Gynecol 2004;191:1305-10.

4. Stoll BJ, Hansen NI, Adams-Chapman I, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and growth impairment among

extremely low-birth-weight infants with neonatal

infection. JAMA 2004;292(19):2357-2365.

5. Hack M, Taylor HG, Drotar D, et al. Poor predictive

validity of the Bayley Scales of Infant Development

for cognitive function of extremely low birth weight

children at school age. Pediatrics 2005;116:333-341.

6. Richardson DK, Corcoran JD, Escobar GJ, Lee

SK. SNAP-II and SNAPPE-II: Simplified newborn

illness severity and mortality risk scores. J Pediatr

2001;138:92-100.

7. Bax M, Goldstein M, Rosenbaum P, et al. Proposed

definition and classification of cerebral palsy, April

2005. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:571-576.

8. Palisano R, Rosenbaum P, Walter S, Russell D, Wood

E, Galuppi B. Development and reliability of a system

to classify gross motor function in children with

cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol

1997;39:214-233.

9. Smits-Engelsman BCM. Movement Assessment

Battery for Children. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets &

Zeitlinger; 1998.

10. Kort W, Compaan EL, Bleichrodt N, et al. WISC-III

NL Handleiding. Amsterdam: NIP dienstencentrum;

2002.

11. Gregoire J. Comparison of three short forms

of the Wechsler intelligence scale for children

- third edition (WISC-III). Eur Rev Appl Psychol

2000;50:437-441.

12. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY II. Clinical

and Interpretative Manual. San Antonio, TX:

PsychCorp. A Brand of Harcourt Assessments;

2007.

13. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545-559.

14. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample

and ADHD performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065-1081.

References

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

15. Scholte EM, van der Ploeg JD. ADHD-vragenlijst

(AVL) handleiding. Houten, the Netherlands: Bohn

Stafleu Van Loghum; 2004.

16. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L.

BRIEF—Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive

Function: professional manual. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

17. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C. Manual for the

Child Behavior Checklist: 4–18 and 1991 profile.

Burlington, VT: Univ. of Vermont, Department of

Psychiatry; 1991.

18. Hamilton BE, Martin JA, Ventura SJ. Births:

Preliminary Data for 2007. Hyattsville, ML: National

Center for Health Statistics; 2009.

19. Adams-Chapman I, Stoll BJ. Neonatal infection

and long-term neurodevelopmental outcome in the

preterm infant. Curr Opin Infect Dis

2006;19:290-297.

20. Malaeb S, Dammann O. Fetal inflammatory

response and brain injury in the preterm newborn. J

Child Neurol 2009;24:1119-1126.

21. Volpe JJ. Postnatal sepsis, necrotizing entercolitis,

and the critical role of systemic inflammation in

white matter injury in premature infants. J Pediatr

2008;153:160-163.

22. Yanowitz TD, Potter DM, Bowen A, Baker RW,

Roberts JM. Variability in cerebral oxygen delivery

is reduced in premature neonates exposed to

chorioamnionitis. Pediatr Res 2006;59:299-304.

23. Khwaja O, Volpe JJ. Pathogenesis of cerebral white

matter injury of prematurity. Arch Dis Child Fetal

Neonatal Ed 2008;93:F153-F161.

24. Heep A, Behrendt D, Nitsch P, Fimmers R, Bartmann

P, Dembinski J. Increased serum levels of interleukin

6 are associated with severe intraventricular

haemorrhage in extremely premature infants. Arch

Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2003;88:F501-F504.

25. Glass HC, Bonifacio SL, Chau V, et al. Recurrent

postnatal infections are associated with progressive

white matter injury in premature infants. Pediatrics

2008;122:299-305.

26. Stoll BJ, Gordon T, Korones SB, et al. Late-onset

sepsis in very low birth weight neonates: a report

from the National Institute of Child Health and

Human Development Neonatal Research Network. J

Pediatr 1996;129:63-71.

27. Blanco A, Solis G, Arranz E, Coto GD, Ramos A,

Telleria J. Serum levels of CD14 in neonatal sepsis

by Gram-positive and Gram-negative bacteria. Acta

Paediatr 1996;85:728-732.

28. Horbar JD, Rogowski J, Plsek PE, et al.

Collaborative quality improvement for neonatal

intensive care. NIC/Q Project Investigators of the

Vermont Oxford Network. Pediatrics

2001;107:14-22.

29. Kilbride HW, Wirtschafter DD, Powers RJ, Sheehan

MB. Implementation of evidence-based potentially

better practices to decrease nosocomial infections.

Pediatrics 2003;111:e519-e533.

215Chapter 8 Functional Impairments at School Age of Preterm Born Children with Late-Onset Sepsis

Chapter 9

MOTOR AND COGNITIVE OUTCOME AT

SCHOOL AGE OF CHILDREN WITH

SURGICALLY TREATED INTESTINAL

OBSTRUCTIONS IN THE NEONATAL PERIOD

RACHEL M. ELSINGA, ELISE ROZE, KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL,

JAN B. F. HULSCHER, AREND F. BOS

SUBMITTED

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Objective

To determine motor and cognitive outcome at school age in children with surgically

treated intestinal obstructions as newborns, and to identify clinical risk factors for

adverse outcome.

Study design

Cohort study of infants born during 1995-2002 with atresia, stenosis, or intestinal

malrotation. At 6 to13 years we assessed their motor functioning, intelligence, attention,

visual perception, visuomotor integration, and verbal memory.

Results

Of 44 children three (7%) died. Twenty-seven survivors (66%) were included for follow-

up (median gestational age 36.7 weeks, birth weight 3000 g). Motor outcome was

abnormal (<5th percentile) in 22% of the children. This was worse than the reference

population (p<.01). Scores on selective attention were abnormal in 15% of the children

(p<.01). Other cognitive functions were not affected. Lower birth weight and intestinal

perforation were risk factors for poorer motor outcome (r2=53.0) while intrauterine growth

restriction was a risk factor for poorer selective attention (r2=36.6).

Conclusions

Children treated surgically for intestinal obstructions in the neonatal period had an

increased risk for poor motor functioning and selective attention at school age. Low

birth weight, IUGR, and intestinal perforation were risk factors for adverse outcomes. We

recommend to closely follow the motor and attentional development of these children.

219Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

Introduction

An intestinal obstruction is a gastrointestinal complication that leads to impaired

bowel passage. It occurs in approximately 9 out of every 10,000 newborn infants.1,2

It can be caused by congenital malformations of the intestine such as an intestinal

atresia or stenosis, or by a malrotation of the intestine. Infants who present with

such an obstruction often require surgery in the neonatal period to uplift the

obstruction, promote normal bowel function and allow oral feeding.

In this early period of life, infants with obstructions are submitted to several

potentially harmful factors, like physiological stress during surgery, that may lead

to adverse neurodevelopmental outcome.3 Furthermore, they undergo general

anesthesia at a very young age. Studies in rodents showed that the use of

anesthetics during the cerebral growth spurt has an adverse influence on long-

term neurological development.4,5

Although these factors are recognized risk factors for adverse neurodevelopmental

outcome, the motor and cognitive outcome at school age of newborn infants with

surgically treated intestinal obstructions is unknown. Our main aim was, therefore,

to determine the outcome of such infants by assessing their motor and cognitive

performance at school age. Motor and cognitive outcomes measured at school age

are known to be more robust and predictive for later life than outcomes measured

at pre-school age. Our secondary aim was to explore which clinical factors were

associated with adverse outcome.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Methods

Patients

We selected all infants who had been admitted to the NICU of the University Medical

Center Groningen between 1995 and 2002, and who were diagnosed by laparotomy

and found to have duodenal, jejunal, ileal, or colon atresia or ileal stenosis, or

malrotation of the intestine. We excluded patients with major chromosomal and

congenital anomalies other than single atresias or stenoses.

Follow-up

The children were invited prospectively to participate in this follow-up study which

was an extension of the regular follow-up program. It entailed the assessment of

motor performance and cognition at the age of 6 to 13 years. Parents gave their

informed consent for their children to participate in the follow-up program. The

study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the University Medical

Center Groningen.

Motor Outcome

To determine the children’s motor outcome, we administered the Movement

Assessment Battery for Children (Movement ABC), a standardized test of motor

skills for children aged 4 to 12 years.6 This test yields a score for total movement

performance based on subscales for fine motor skills, ball skills, and balance. The

tasks composing the Movement ABC are representative of the motor skills that

are required of children attending elementary school in the Netherlands and are

adapted to the individual child’s age.

Cognitive Outcome

We assessed total, verbal, and performance intelligence using a shortened version

of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition, Dutch version (WISC-

III-NL).7,8

In addition, we assessed several neuropsychological functions. To assess attention

we administered two subtests of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-

Ch).9 For selective attention we used the subtest “map mission”. Selective attention

221Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

refers to a child’s ability to select target information from an array of distracters. We

measured attentional control with the subtest “opposite worlds”. Attentional control

refers to the child’s ability to change attentional focus flexibly and adaptively.

We assessed visual perception with the subtest “geometric puzzles” and visuomotor

integration with the subtest “design copying” of the NEPSY-II.10 Visual perception

refers to the child’s ability to perform mental rotations and visuospatial analyses.

Visuomotor integration involves integrating visual information with finger-hand

movements.

We assessed verbal memory with a standardized Dutch version of Rey’s Auditory

Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).11 This test consists of five learning trials of fifteen words

each, followed by an immediate recall trial, a delayed recall trial, and a delayed

recognition trial, both after a delay of approximately 20 minutes.

Statistical Analysis

We classified the scores of the Movement ABC, TEA-Ch, and NEPSY-II into

normal (≥ 15th percentile), borderline (5th to 15th percentile), and abnormal (< 5th

percentile), in accordance with the manual based on a Dutch reference population.

To classify the scores of the WISC-III-NL and AVLT we used the norm scores,

means, and standard deviations of the reference population. Normal was defined

as ≥ -1 standard deviation (SD), borderline as -1 to -2 SDs, abnormal as < -2 SDs.

In addition, we calculated the z scores based on the norm scores and percentiles

given in the test manuals.

In order to compare the outcome of the study group with the norm scores of the

reference population, we used the one-sample Student t test in case of normality

and the Wilcoxon signed rank test in case of non-normality.

To determine whether clinical characteristics were related to adverse outcome, we

calculated odds ratios (ORs) by univariate logistic regression. Adverse outcome was

defined as an abnormal score (< 5th percentile). We subsequently used backward

multivariate logistic regression to determine which risk factors detected by the

univariate analysis (p<.10) had independent prognostic value for outcome.

Throughout the analyses, p<.05 was considered statistically significant. We used

SPSS 16.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) for all the analyses.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Patient characteristics

A database search resulted in 44 children who met our inclusion criteria, three (7%)

of whom died in the neonatal period. Twenty-seven (66%) out of the 41 survivors

were included in the follow-up study. The parents of seven children declined the

invitation to participate and seven other children could not be traced. Table 1

shows the patient demographics of the infants who were included in the follow-up.

Two infants were born preterm (<32 weeks) and had an extremely low birth weight

(<1000 g). The gestational age of the surviving infants who did not participate in

this study, was comparable to that of the study group (median 37.4 weeks, range

30.7-42.1). The birth weight of the survivors who did not participate was slightly

lower (median 2298 g, range 1120-3700) compared to the median of 3000 g in the

study group. The number of infants with intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR) was

comparable (n=3, 21%).

The most common type of obstruction was atresia (n=16). One child had an ileal

stenosis. In eight infants the obstruction was caused by a malrotation, in four of

whom the malrotation was complicated by a volvulus, while two infants suffered

from a volvulus without a malrotation. The median age at surgery was 3 days (range

1-22). Six infants required at least one more operation under general anesthesia

during their first year, while four required more than one subsequent operation

(median 2).

Follow-up

The mean age at follow-up was 9.5 years (6.6 – 13.2). The total score on the

Movement ABC, expressed as a z score, was significantly lower than the reference

population (p<.01; Figure 1). All motor domains were affected, with most problems

encountered on the balance test on which 33% of the children obtained abnormal

scores (Figure 2A).

We present the cognitive outcomes of the children in Figure 1 (z scores). Their

mean total IQ was 99 (range 63-120), their mean verbal IQ was 102 (range 65-123),

and their mean performance IQ was 96 (range 55-123). These scores did not differ

significantly from those of the reference population.

223Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

Regarding the other cognitive functions, only selective attention was worse in the

children with surgically treated obstructions compared to the reference population

(p<.01). Thirty percent of the children with intestinal obstructions had borderline

or abnormal scores on selective attention (Figure 2B). The scores on attentional

control, visuomotor integration, and verbal memory did not differ from the reference

population. The scores on visual perception were slightly better than the reference

population (p<.01; Figure 1). We were unable to test the visual perception of two

children because the test proved too difficult for them. In Figure 2B they are

classified as abnormal.

Clinical Factors in relation to Outcome

Subsequently, we investigated whether clinical characteristics of the neonatal

period were related to abnormal outcomes at school age. We entered the following

clinical characteristics in a univariate logistic analysis: gestational age, birth

weight, IUGR, presence of volvulus, presence of perforation, intestinal resection,

and late onset sepsis. In Table 2 we present the factors that were significantly

related to worse outcome after univariate analysis. After multivariate analyses, only

perforation and birth weight were related to abnormal total motor functioning. The

model including these variables explained 53.0% of the variance (r2). For selective

attention, IUGR remained in the model with a proportion of declared variance (r2)

of 36.6%.

To determine whether these poorer outcomes at school age were solely determined

by prematurity and perforation (motor outcome), and IUGR (attentional outcome),

we repeated the analyses after excluding the children with these risk factors. This

had no effect on any of the results.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Patient demographics

Number n=27Males/females 13/14 Gestational age (weeks) 36.7 (28.9-42.0)

Birth weight (grams) 3000 (710-5065)

IUGR (<P10) n=6 (22)

Asphyxia n=1 (4)

Apgar at 1 minute (n=21) 8 (2-10)

Apgar at 5 minutes (n=21) 9 (3-10) Late onset morbidity

Late onset sepsis n=7 (26)

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia1 n=1 (4)

Characteristics of intestinal obstructionAtresia n=16 (59)

Duodenal atresia n=3 (11)

Jejunal atresia n=4 (15)

Ileal atresia n=6 (22)

Colonal atresia n=3 (11)

Ileal stenosis n=1 (4)

Malrotation without volvulus n=4 (15)

Malrotation plus volvulus n=4 (15)

Isolated volvulus n=2 (22)

ComplicationsPerforation n=5 (19)

Short bowel syndrome n=1 (4) Surgical characteristics

Intestinal resection n=16 (59)

Jejunal resection n=5 (19)

Ileal resection n=5 (19)

Colonal resection n=1 (4)

Jejunal plus ileal resection excluding ileocoecal valve n=1 (4)

Ileal plus colonal resection including ileocoecal valve n=4 (15)

Length of resected intestine 13.5 (1-50)

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or as numbers (percentage). 1. Bronchopulmonary dysplasia was defined as oxygen dependence beyond 36 weeks postmenstrual ageAbbreviation: IUGR - intrauterine growth restriction

225Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

TABLE 2 Clinical factors in relation to outcome

Movement-ABC

Univariate Multivariate

p-value p-value OR 95 % CI

Gestational age1 0.039

Birth weight2 0.024 0.068 1.20 0.99 - 1.47

IUGR3 0.081

Late onset sepsis4 0.020

Perforation5 0.041 0.11 17.51 0.53 – 579.52

Selective attention

Univariate Multivariate

p-value p-value OR 95 % CI

IUGR3 0.022 0.022 20 1.53 – 260.79

Late onset sepsis4 0.038

1. Gestational age in weeks2. Birth weight in 100 g3. IUGR yes or no4. Late onset sepsis yes or no5. Perforation yes or no

Abbreviation: IUGR - intrauterine growthrestriction

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Motor and cognitive outcomes (x-axis), expressed as z-scores (y-axis).

2

0

-2

-4

MovementABC

Z-sore

VerbalIQ

PerformanceIQ

Selectiveattention

Attentionalcontrol

Visualperception

Visuomotorintegration

Verbalmemory

Figure 1 Motor and cognitive outcomes

227Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

Figure 2A Motor outcome of children with surgically treated intestinal

obstructions

Figure 2B Cognitive outcome of children with surgically treated

intestinal obstructions

Data are expressed as number of children with normal, borderline, and abnormal scores.

100%

90%

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10%

0%

100%

90%

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10%

0%

Norm

n=19 n=20n=18

n=22n=23n=23 n=23n=23n=21n=19

n=1n=1n=1 n=2n=2n=5n=4

n=4n=3n=3 n=2n=2

n=1n=4

n=18

n=9

n=4n=2

n=2

n=5n=5n=6

Norm

Total motor

Intelligence

Fine skills

Selectiveattention

Visuomotor integration

Ball skills

Attentionalcontrol

Verbal memory

immediate

Verbal memory delayed

Balance

Visual perception

Normal

Normal

Borderline

Borderline

Abnormal

Abnormal

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Discussion

This study showed that at school age more than twenty percent of children with

surgically treated intestinal obstructions in the neonatal period showed abnormal

motor functioning and fifteen percent showed abnormal selective attention. The

scores obtained by our study group on these functions were significantly lower

than those of the reference population. Outcomes were not affected on intelligence,

visual perception, visuomotor integration, and verbal memory. Low birth weight,

IUGR, and the presence of an intestinal perforation were risk factors for adverse

outcomes in infants with an intestinal obstruction that was treated surgically.

Since to our knowledge ours was the first study to determine the motor and

cognitive outcome at school age of mainly term-born children treated surgically

for intestinal obstructions in the neonatal period we are unable to compare our

results to other outcome studies. Most studies deal with preterm born children

who require surgery during the first weeks after birth. This makes it difficult to

review our results in the light of existing literature.

A previous study on the outcome of preterm infants with necrotizing enterocolitis

(NEC) found that surgically treated children had poorer neurodevelopmental

outcomes at 18 to 22 months than medically managed children.12 Partly, this could

be explained by a higher baseline risk for adverse outcome in the surgical group,

since these children were more severely ill. Nevertheless, this higher baseline risk

did not completely explain the variance. Possibly other factors associated with

surgical treatment like physiological stress and anesthesia, also played a role.

A study in term-born children with surgically treated congenital heart diseases

in the neonatal period showed that these children too were at risk for

adverse neurodevelopmental outcome at school age.13 This group showed lower

intelligence and fine motor skills as well as more specific neuropsychological

deficits in memory and attention. In our study group we found that motor function

and attention were also affected. Although both study groups required surgery for

a congenital anomaly in the neonatal period, the children with congenital heart

disease presumably had periods of reduced cerebral blood flow and oxygenation,

which may have further increased their risk for neurodevelopmental impairments.

This was not the case in our group.

229Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

In our study, low birth weight, IUGR, and intestinal perforation were related to poorer

outcome at school age. Low birth weight and IUGR are well-known risk factors

for neurodevelopmental impairments up to school age.14,15 In preterm infants,

perforation as a complication of the intestinal disease NEC was also a risk factor

for adverse outcome at school age.16 Perforation is frequently associated with

inflammation and infection. Particularly in preterm infants, systemic inflammation

is associated with cerebral white matter abnormalities.17,18 Brain development of

term-born infants is in a much more mature phase in terms of neuronal migration

and cortical folding than that of preterms. Nevertheless, to a large extent

myelination and dendritic and axonal arborization still have to take place.19,20 Our

finding that poorer motor and attentional outcomes were still present after we had

excluded children with the above mentioned risk factors, led us to believe that

additional, presumably surgery and disease-related factors, had also played a role

in our study. These included, for example, physiological stress during surgery, and

anesthesia. Rodent studies showed that the use of anesthesia during the cerebral

growth spurt can lead to neuroapoptosis and has an adverse influence on long-

term behavioral development.4,5 It may well be that maturational processes which

normally occur from birth until years postnatally, are also susceptible to injury from

anesthetic factors. Furthermore, infants with intestinal obstructions receive parenteral

nutrition for a considerable period of time, and approximately twelve percent of the

infants with surgically treated intestinal atresia suffer from short bowel syndrome, a

condition which subjects them to ongoing nutritional problems.2 Parenteral nutrition

and prolonged nutritional deficiencies are described as having an adverse influence

on cognition and learning at pre-school age.21 Although parenteral nutrition might

have played a role, ongoing nutritional deficiencies most likely did not, since in our

study only one child suffered from short bowel syndrome.

Our study was subject to potential limitations. It was a single-center study therefore

its generalizability to other centers needs to be established. We tried to minimize

this problem by using standardized tests. Another potential limitation was that

we compared the outcomes of our study group to a reference population rather

than to a control group. Despite this we did not expect an overestimation of the

differences between our study group and reference scores, since these reference

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

scores were obtained within the last ten years. Potentially, an underestimation of

the described effect of intestinal obstructions and surgery on outcome is more

likely since cognitive outcomes from the reference population may only have

increased over time, the so-called Flynn effect.22

To our knowledge, ours was the first study to investigate the motor and cognitive

outcome at school age of children who had been treated surgically for intestinal

obstructions during the first weeks of life. The strength of this study was that we

examined a broad range of motor and cognitive functions at school age. Motor

and cognitive outcomes measured at school age are known to be more robust

and predictive for later life than when measured at pre-school age. Since intestinal

obstructions are associated with few neonatal comorbidities, we believe that the

adverse motor and attentional outcomes at school age as found in the present

study could be ascribed mainly to the intestinal obstruction and its surgical

treatment. Since a significant proportion of children with intestinal obstructions

showed motor and attentional problems at school age, we recommend that these

children be followed-up and screened for motor and attentional deficits. Future

studies on larger cohorts of children who underwent surgical treatment in the

neonatal period could reveal perioperative factors which contribute to adverse

outcome at school age.

231Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

1. Berseth CL. Disorders of the intestines and pancreas.

In: Taeush WH BR, editor. Avery’s Disease of the

Newborn. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1998. p.918.

2. la Vecchia LK, Grosfeld JL, West KW, Rescorla FJ,

Scherer LR, Engum SA. Intestinal atresia and stenosis:

a 25-year experience with 277 cases. Arch Surg

1998;133:490-496.

3. Anand KJ, Hansen DD, Hickey PR. Hormonal-metabolic

stress responses in neonates undergoing cardiac

surgery. Anesthesiology 1990;73:661-670.

4. Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Hartman RE, Izumi Y, et al.

Early exposure to common anesthetic agents causes

widespread neurodegeneration in the developing

rat brain and persistent learning deficits. J Neurosci

2003;23:876-882.

5. Mellon RD, Simone AF, Rappaport BA. Use of

anesthetic agents in neonates and young children.

Anesth Analg 2007;104:509-520.

6. Smits-Engelsman BCM. Movement Assessment Battery

for Children. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger;

1998.

7. Kort W, Compaan EL, Bleichrodt N, et al. WISC-III

NL Handleiding. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: NIP

dienstencentrum; 2002.

8. Grégoire J. Comparison of three short forms of the

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - third edition

(WISC-III). Eur Rev Appl Psychol 2000;50:437-441.

9. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday Attention

for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD

performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065-1081.

10. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY II. Clinical and

Interpretative Manual. San Antonio, TX: PsychCorp. A

Brand of Harcourt Assessments; 2007.

11. Van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225 Dutch

school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test

(AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities with an interval of

3 months and normative data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol

1999; 14:545-559.

12. Hintz SR, Kendrick DE, Stoll BJ, et al.

Neurodevelopmental and growth outcomes of

extremely low birth weight infants after necrotizing

enterocolitis. Pediatrics 2005;115:696-703.

13. Miatton M, De Wolf D, Francois K, Thiery E,

Vingerhoets G. Neuropsychological performance

in school-aged children with surgically corrected

congenital heart disease. J Pediatr 2007;151:73-78.

14. Allen MC. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm

infants. Curr Opin Neurol 2008;21:123-128.

15. Kiechl-Kohlendorfer U, Ralser E, Pupp Peglow U,

Reiter G, Trawoger R. Adverse neurodevelopmental

outcome in preterm infants: risk factor profiles for

different gestational ages. Acta Paediatr

2009; 98:792-796.

References

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

16. Neubauer AP, Voss W, Kattner E. Outcome of

extremely low birth weight survivors at school

age: the influence of perinatal parameters on

neurodevelopment. Eur J Pediatr 2008;167:87-95.

17. Volpe JJ. Postnatal sepsis, necrotizing entercolitis,

and the critical role of systemic inflammation in white

matter injury in premature infants. J Pediatr

2008; 153:160-163.

18. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by white

matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging

at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

19. Brody BA, Kinney HC, Kloman AS, Gilles FH.

Sequence of central nervous system myelination in

human infancy. I. An autopsy study of myelination. J

Neuropathol Exp Neurol 1987;46:283-301.

20. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the Newborn. 5th ed.

Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier; 2008.

21. Yehuda S, Rabinovitz S, Mostofsky DI. Nutritional

deficiencies in learning and cognition. J Pediatr

Gastroenterol Nutr 2006;43:S22-S25.

22. Flynn JR. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What

IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin

1987;101:171-191.

233Chapter 9 Motor and Cognitive Outcome at School Age of Children with Surgically Treated Intestinal Obstructions in the Neonatal Period

Chapter 10 Developmental trajectories from birth to

school age in healthy term-born children

Chapter 11 Neuropsychological profiles at school age of very

preterm born children compared to term-born peers

PART 4Functional outcome: how various aspects of

neurodevelopment interrelate

Chapter 10

DEVELOPMENTAL TRAJECTORIES FROM BIRTH

TO SCHOOL AGE IN HEALTHY TERM-BORN

CHILDREN

ELISE ROZE, LISETHE MEIJER, KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL, SELMA A. J.

RUITER, JANNEKE L. M. BRUGGINK, AREND F. BOS

PEDIATRICS 2010;126(5):E1134-E1142

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Objective

To determine the stability of the scores obtained on tests of motor development

from birth until school age in healthy, full-term, singletons and to determine whether

early motor scores were associated with more complex cognitive functions such

as attention and memory, at school age.

Patients and Methods

This longitudinal prospective cohort study included 77 infants. During the neonatal

period their motor development was determined with Prechtl’s neurological

examination, in early infancy with Touwen’s neurological examination and general

movement assessment, at toddler age with Hempel’s neurological examination

and the psychomotor developmental index from the Bayley Scales of Infant

Development, Second Edition, Dutch Version (BSID-II-NL), and at school age with

the Movement ABC. Cognition was determined at toddler age with the mental

developmental index from the BSID-II-NL and at school age with an intelligence

test and attention and memory tests.

Results

The mean absolute difference in standardized motor scores over all time points

was 1.01 SD (95% confidence interval: 0.91-1.11). Only the explained proportions

of variance of maternal socioeconomic status (SES) and verbal intelligence were

significant for sustained attention and verbal memory (r2=0.104, p=.030 and

r2=0.074, p=.027, respectively). The children’s scores on early motor tests added

little value for their motor and cognitive development at school age.

Conclusions

In healthy children the stability of motor development from birth until school age is

low. Maternal SES and verbal intelligence rather than the infants’ scores on early

motor tests signified added value for complex cognitive functions at school age.

239Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

Introduction

Over the past decades there has been a growing interest in the prediction of outcome

from early infancy to performance at school age, particularly in infants at high risk

of developmental deficits. Gestational age, for example, is a strong predictor of

later motor and cognitive deficits.1,2 In high-risk infants, such as extreme low birth

weight infants, early neurological tests scores have a strong predictive value for

functioning later in life. In other words, children with early developmental delays,

such as motor impairments, have relatively stable developmental trajectories until

school age. This indicates that early alterations in brain development have an

important impact on the integrity and maturation of the central nervous system

during childhood.

Less is known about the stability of developmental trajectories in healthy, term-

born children. Previous studies suggested that in a norm population the age of

reaching developmental milestones predicts development later in life.3-5 Others

questioned whether a child’s developmental trajectory can be readily predicted

since it is influenced by a number of biological and environmental factors, such

as socio-economic status (SES), as well as medical, social, and developmental

interventions.6-8

To gain insight into the developmental trajectories of healthy children, our first aim

was to determine the stability of scores on motor development tests from birth

until school age in healthy, full-term, singletons.

There is growing evidence that early motor impairment may be related to later

cognitive problems. Therefore our second aim was to determine whether the

scores on tests of motor development in a normal population were associated

with performance at school age, particularly complex cognitive functions such as

attention and verbal memory.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Patients and Methods

Participants

The cohort of this study consisted of 90, white, healthy, and randomly selected

pregnant women who met the following inclusion criteria: giving birth to a healthy,

full-term, singleton infant, and who lived in one of the three northern provinces of

the Netherlands. All women registered with midwifes and gynecologists between

October 2001 and November 2002 in the province of Groningen were invited to

participate in the study. This cohort was originally established for a study on the

influence of prenatal exposure to environmental compounds on the development

of healthy children.9

Follow-up

The children were invited prospectively to participate in an extensive longitudinal

follow-up program. It entailed the assessment of motor performance and cognition

from birth until school age at five to seven years. Prior to the study parents gave

their informed consent for themselves and their children to participate in the

follow-up program. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of

the University Medical Center Groningen.

Neonatal Period

When the infants were ten days old we administered Prechtl’s neonatal neurological

examination.10 It contains sixty items for which an optimal range is defined. For

example, this test assesses excitability (e.g. apathy), motility, tonus (e.g. hypotonia,

dystonia), asymmetries, and visuomotor integration. We noted the presence of

possible neonatal neurological abnormalities and made a clinical interpretation of

the infant’s neurological condition. This led to the infant being classified as normal,

suspect, or abnormal. We also gave each infant a total neurological optimality

score (Prechtl’s optimality score, range 0-60).

Early Infancy

When the infants reached the age of twelve weeks, we performed Touwen’s

neurological examination to evaluate the infants’ neurological condition.11 This test

241Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

assesses neurological items similar to the neonatal neurological examination in an

age-specific context. Again, we obtained a total neurological optimality score for

each infant (Touwen’s optimality score, range 0-60) and classified the infants as

being normal, suspect, or abnormal.

At this age we also assessed the infants’ quality of spontaneous general movements

according to Prechtl et al.12-14 This is the age at which fidgety movements emerge;

movements characterized by small amplitude, moderate speed, and variable

acceleration. We assessed the quality of fidgety movements and scored them

as normal, abnormal (i.e. amplitude, speed, and jerkiness were exaggerated) or

absent. In addition, we calculated the infants’ motor optimality score (range 5-28

points) by assessing the quality of their fidgety movements, the quality and age-

adequacy of their concurrent motor repertoire, and the presence and normality of

their motor patterns.14,15

Toddler Age

When the children reached the age of eighteen months, we assessed their motor

and cognitive development with the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second

Edition, Dutch Version (BSID-II-NL).16 The BSID-II-NL contains items on the mental

and psychomotor development of children, and generates a mental developmental

index (MDI) and psychomotor developmental index (PDI). Furthermore, we

administered Hempel’s age-specific neurological examination to evaluate the

children’s neurological condition. This neurological examination contains items

on posture, coordination of trunk and extremities, gross and fine motor function,

visuomotor integration and sensory function.17 For each infant we obtained a total

neurological optimality score (Hempel’s optimality score, range 0-57) and we

classified them as normal, suspect, or abnormal.

During this period we measured the mothers’ verbal intelligence with the Wechsler

Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS).18

School Age

When the children reached the age of five to seven years, we assessed their

motor and cognitive development. We determined their motor outcome with the

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Movement ABC, a standardized test of motor skills for children aged four to twelve

years.19 This test, which is widely used in both practice and research, yields a score

on total movement performance based on separate scores for manual dexterity

(fine motor skills), ball skills, and static and dynamic balance (coordination). For

example, items on the Movement ABC include posting coins in a bank box, drawing

a line in between two existing lines on a figure, catching a bean bag, and jumping

over a rope. The tasks on the Movement ABC are representative of the motor skills

that are required of children attending elementary school and are adapted to the

children’s ages.

The children’s cognitive development was assessed by a shortened form of the

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, revised (WPPSI-R).20 We

used the subtests vocabulary, picture completion, and reproduction of block

designs to measure total, verbal, and performance intelligence.

In addition, we measured the children’s attention and verbal memory. Sustained

attention and selective attention were measured with the two subtests ‘Score!’

and ‘Sky search’ of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch).21

Sustained attention involves maintaining attention over an extended period of time.

Selective attention refers to the ability to select target information from an array

of distractors.22 We assessed verbal memory with a standardized Dutch version

of Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT).23 This test consists of five learning

trials with immediate recall of words (tested after each presentation), a delayed

recall trial, and a delayed recognition trial.

To gain insight in maternal SES, we determined the mother’s highest level of

education during the first year after birth.

A summary of the different tests that were used, including time period, abbreviations

and range of scores, is provided in Table 1.

Statistical Analyses

Intelligence quotient (IQ) scores were calculated by the mean of scores on the verbal

and performance subtests that were assessed. We classified scores on the BSID-

II-NL, Movement ABC and cognitive tests into ‘normal’ (>15th percentile), ‘suspect’

(5th> to ≤15th percentile), and ‘abnormal’ (≤5th percentile). For the neurological

243Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

examination the same classification was made, based on the criteria from the

manuals. We explored the relationship between the scores on the developmental

tests with Pearson’s and Spearman’s correlation coefficients, where appropriate.

For the BSID-II-NL, Movement ABC, and intelligence scores we used the norm

scores from the manuals, whereas for Prechtl’s, Touwen’s, Hempel’s, and the

motor optimality score we used the raw scores, since norm scores are unavailable.

The correlations were calculated both before and after correction for SES, as SES

may act as a confounding factor on both motor and cognitive development.24,25

To investigate the stability of the scores obtained on motor development tests, we

first calculated the z-scores for each test to be able to compare the different tests.

Subsequently, we calculated the differences in z-scores between the different time

points (i.e. between Prechtl’s optimality score and the motor optimality score, the

motor optimality score and Touwen’s optimality score, Touwen’s optimality score

and the PDI, the PDI and Hempel’s optimality score, and Hempel’s optimality score

and the Movement ABC). We then determined the mean absolute difference over

all time points.

We calculated the proportion of variance (r2) in development at school age

explained by the scores on the early tests and maternal characteristics, in order to

compare the added value of the latter. For normally distributed variables we used

a linear regression model. For non-normal variables we used a logistic regression

model in which we compared the normal children with the suspect and abnormal

children at school age. Throughout the analyses p<.05 was considered statistically

significant, which is uncorrected for multiple comparisons.

We used SPSS 14.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL) for all the

analyses.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Results

Of the 90 children invited, 77 (86%) participated in the follow-up program at school

age. Eight sets of parents declined the invitation to take part in the follow-up and

4 sets of parents could not be traced. One girl had to be excluded because she

suffered severe cognitive impairment of unknown origin and could not be tested as

a result. The demographics of the children are shown in Table 2.

Follow-up

The mean age at follow-up in the neonatal period was ten days (range eight to

twelve days). The mean for Prechtl’s optimality score derived from the neurological

examination (n=75) was 50 (range 39-59).

During early infancy the mean age at follow-up was twelve weeks (range eleven to

thirteen weeks). The mean for Touwen’s optimality score (n=77) was 45 (range 35-

51). Regarding the quality of spontaneous general movements (n=72), 69 children

showed normal fidgety movements, 2 showed abnormal fidgety movements, and

in 1 child fidgety movements were absent. The mean motor optimality score was

25 (range 10-28).

At toddler age, the mean age at follow-up was eighteen months (range seventeen

months and three weeks to eighteen months and one week). The mean Hempel’s

optimality score (n=75) was 43 (range 31-54). According to the BSID-II-NL, the

mean for the PDI (n=75) was 93 (range 69-118) and for the MDI (n=75) the mean

was 96 (range 66-125).

The mean age at follow-up at school age was six years and one month (range five

years and eight months to seven years and six months). The mean total IQ of the

children was 102 (range 82-125, SD 9), the mean verbal IQ was 100 (range 78-130,

SD 10), and the mean performance IQ was 103 (range 73-133, SD 12). In Table 3 we

present an overview of the longitudinal test results classified as normal, suspect and

abnormal. On the whole, the test scores were comparable to a reference population.

The neurological outcome in early infancy and toddler age, mothers’ intelligence,

and intelligence of the children at school age were slightly better than one would

expect in the total population. Furthermore, selective attention was slightly better

than sustained attention, although comparable to a reference population.

245Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

Stability of the Developmental Scores over Time

Figure 1 shows the correlations between the developmental scores at the various

ages and the total score on the Movement ABC. Hempel’s optimality score at

toddler age correlated significantly with motor outcome at school age both before

and after correction for SES (r=0.244, p=.035 and r=0.273, p=.018, respectively).

The MDI at toddler age correlated significantly with motor outcome at school age

after correction for SES (r=0.272, p=.019). Further analysis revealed that the MDI

and Hempel’s optimality score correlated significantly, in particular with the manual

dexterity subtest (both r=0.276, p=.019) of the Movement-ABC.

Figure 2 shows the correlations between the developmental test scores and

total intelligence at school age. None of these correlations reached significance.

Nevertheless, we found a trend towards correlation between the MDI at toddler

age and intelligence at school age before correction for SES (r=0.194, p=.098).

We also correlated the test scores in the neonatal period, early infancy, and toddler

age with one another. After correcting for SES, we found that the motor optimality

score in early infancy correlated with the PDI at toddler age (r=0.262, p= 0.030). At

toddler age we found a correlation between the PDI and Hempel’s optimality score

(r=0.284, p=.015) and the MDI and Hempel’s optimality score (r= 0.299, p=.011).

The correlation between the MDI and PDI was also significant (r=0.417, p<.001). At

school age, we found that the children’s motor development correlated significantly

with intelligence and sustained attention (r=0.267, p=.021 and r=0.337, p=.004).

Intelligence correlated significantly with selective attention and verbal memory

(r=0.253, p=.034 and r=0.268, p=.024).

After transforming the test scores to z-scores and calculating the mean absolute

difference in z-scores between the time points, the largest difference in the test

scores was observed between Prechtl’s optimality score (neonatal period) and the

motor optimality score (early infancy), and Touwen’s optimality score (early infancy)

and the PDI (toddler age), with mean absolute differences of 1.03 SD (95% confidence

interval (CI): 0.81-1.24) and 1.15 SD (95% CI: 0.96-1.35), respectively. The mean

absolute difference over all the time points was 1.01 SD (95% CI: 0.91-1.11).

Figure 3 shows the distribution of the neurological test scores at the different time

points, divided into three categories (<33rd percentile, 33rd> to ≤66th percentile,

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

>66th percentile) of motor performance at school age. As is apparent from Figure

3, there is little consistency in the motor trajectories of the children over time.

One can see in the second column that in early infancy, the distribution of motor

performance was approximately equivalent across all three neurological test score

groups. With increasing age, however, the tests do enable us to better identify

those children that will obtain motor scores below the 33rd percentile at school

age.

Early Predictors of Motor and Cognitive Development at School Age

The Movement ABC percentiles, IQ, and verbal memory were normally distributed,

and were therefore included in a linear regression model. Attention was not normally

distributed and was therefore included in a logistic regression model.

Table 4 gives the proportion of variance in motor and cognitive development at

school age explained by early tests scores and maternal SES and verbal intelligence.

Furthermore, we present the r2 for the complete model. Table 4 shows that the r2s

for the complete model on all outcome variables are low. We found significant

but low r2s for the MDI on motor development at school age (r2=0.074, p=.018),

SES on sustained attention (r2=0.104, p=.030), and mothers’ verbal intelligence on

verbal memory of the children at school age (r2=0.074, p=.027).

247Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

TABLE 1 Longitudinal follow-up assessments

Score Range

Neonatal period

Prechtl’s neonatal neurological

examination

Prechtl’s optimality score 0-60

Early infancy

Touwen’s neurological examination Touwen’s optimality score 0-60

General Movement assessment Motor optimality score 5-28

Toddler age

Hempel’s neurological examination Hempel’s optimality score 0-57

Bayley Scales of Infant

Development, Second Edition,

Dutch Version (BSID-II-NL)

Psychomotor Developmental Index

(PDI), Mental Developmental Index (MDI)

50-150

School age

Movement ABC Movement ABC total score 0-1001

Wechsler Preschool and Primary

Scale of Intelligence, Revised

(WPPSI-R)

Total intelligence, verbal intelligence,

performance intelligence

50-150

Test of Everyday Attention for

Children (TEA-Ch)

Sustained attention, selective attention 0-1001

Rey’s Auditory Verbal Learning

Test (AVLT)

Verbal memory 0-1001

1. Range is given in percentiles

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 2 Participant Demographics

Number n=77

Males/females 46/31

Gestational age (weeks) (n=75) 40 (38-42.5)

Apgar at 5 minutes (n=69) 10 (7-10)

Breast-feeding (n=75) n=62 (83)

Maternal characteristics (n=75)

Maternal age at labor 32 years (24-42)

Smoking during pregnancy

< 5 cigarettes/day 7 (9)

> 5 cigarettes/day 2 (3)

Alcohol during pregnancy

Occasionally (< one unit/ week) 21 (27)

Frequently (> one unit/ week) 1 (1)

Socioeconomic status (n=77)

Below average n=4 (5)

Average n=27 (35)

Above average n=46 (60)

Data are given as median (minimum-maximum) or as numbers (percentage).

249Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

TABLE 3 Longitudinal test results

Normal Suspect Abnormal

Mothers’ Verbal Intelligence (n=68) 65 2 1

Neonatal period

Prechtl’s optimality score (n=75)1 69 6 0

Early infancy

Touwen’s optimality score (n=77)1 76 1 0

Motor optimality score (n=71)2 36 32 3

Toddler age

Hempel’s optimality score (n=75)1 70 5 0

Mental Developmental Index (n=75)3 63 11 1

Psychomotor Developmental Index (n=75)3 56 17 2

School age

Movement ABC, total score (n=77)3 65 7 5

Total intelligence (n=76)3 73 3 0

Verbal intelligence (n=76)3 73 3 0

Performance intelligence (n=76)3 70 6 0

Sustained attention (n=74)3 59 11 4

Selective attention (n=75)3 67 4 4

Verbal memory (n=75)3 60 8 7

Data are given as numbers (percentage).1. Classification into normal, suspect and abnormal was based on presence of neurological abnormalities2. Normal was defined as a motor optimality score of 28, or 26 with reduced age adequacy of movements, suspect

as a motor optimality score of 20-26, abnormal was the absence of fidgety movements or presence of abnormal fidgety movements

3. Normal was defined as >15th percentile, suspect as 5th-15th percentile and abnormal as <5th percentile

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TAB

LE 4

Exp

lain

ed p

rop

ort

ions

of

vari

ance

on

mo

tor

and

co

gni

tive

dev

elo

pm

ent

at s

cho

ol a

ge

Mot

or d

evel

opm

ent

Inte

llige

nce

Atte

ntio

nVe

rbal

mem

ory

(r2 )

1(r

2 )1

Sus

tain

ed (r

2 )2S

elec

tive

(r2 )2(r

2 )2

Com

ple

te m

odel

30.

184

0.14

90.

278

0.39

40.

176

Soc

ioec

onom

ic s

tatu

s0.

000

0.00

80.

104*

0.00

00.

009

Mot

hers

’ Ver

bal

inte

llige

nce

0.00

30.

048

0.01

20.

078

0.07

4*

Neo

nata

l per

iod

Pre

chtl’

s op

timal

ity s

core

(n=

75)

0.00

40.

018

0.04

90.

021

0.00

7

Ear

ly in

fanc

y

Touw

en’s

op

timal

ity s

core

(n=

77)

0.00

10.

001

0.00

60.

076

0.00

4

Mot

or o

ptim

ality

sco

re (n

=71

)0.

039

0.00

40.

003

0.08

30.

005

Tod

dle

r ag

e

Hem

pel’s

opt

imal

ity s

core

(n=7

5)0.

069

0.00

00.

082

0.01

50.

014

Men

tal D

evel

opm

enta

l Ind

ex (n

=75)

0.07

4*0.

038

0.00

00.

058

0.02

7

Psy

chom

otor

Dev

elop

men

tal I

ndex

(n=7

5)0.

009

0.01

10.

000

0.02

30.

000

*p<

.05

1. r

2 d

eter

min

ed w

ith li

near

reg

ress

ion

mod

el

2. r

2 d

eter

min

ed w

ith lo

gist

ic r

egre

ssio

n m

odel

3.

r2

of t

he c

omp

lete

mod

el, i

nclu

din

g al

l the

dev

elop

men

tal t

est

scor

es, s

ocio

econ

omic

sta

tus,

and

mot

hers

’ ver

bal

inte

llige

nce

251Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

Figure 1 The correlation between developmental tests at various ages

and motor outcome

The correlation coefficient (r) between developmental tests at various ages and motoroutcome (Movement ABC) at school age (*p<.05)

1,0

0,9

0,8

0,7

0,6

0,5

0,4

0,3

0,2

0,1

0

Prechtl’s

optimality score

Neonatal period Early infancy Toddler age

Touwen’s

optimality score

Motor

optimality score

Psychomotor

Developmental

Index

Mental

Developmental

Index

Hempel’s

optimality score

Before correction for SES

After correction for SES

TIME

R

* **

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Figure 2 The correlation between developmental tests at various

ages and intelligence at school age

1,0

0,9

0,8

0,7

0,6

0,5

0,4

0,3

0,2

0,1

0

- 0,1

- 0,2

- 0,3

Prechtl’s

optimality score

Neonatal period Early infancy Toddler age

Touwen’s

optimality score

Motor

optimality score

Psychomotor

Developmental

Index

Mental

Developmental

Index

Hempel’s

optimality score

Before correction for SES

After correction for SES

TIME

R

253Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

33rd

100rd

66th

Percentile

Neonatal period(Prechtl’s optimality

score)

Early infancy(Touwen’s optimality

score)

Toddler age(Hempel’s optimality

score)

School age(Movement ABC)

Time

Figure 3 The distribution of neurological test scores at

different time points

School age

> 66th percentile

33rd - 66th percentile

< 33rd percentile

The distribution of neurological test scores at different time points, divided into three categories (<33rd percentile, 33rd to ≤66th percentile, >66th percentile) according to motor performance at school age. At each time point, the children were classified into three equal groups according to the centiles of their neurological test scores. The slices of the pies represent the number of children of the groups that scored <33rd percentile (black), 33rd to ≤66th percentile (gray), >66th percentile (white) at school age.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Discussion

The results of our study demonstrated that the stability of motor development was

limited in healthy, term-born, singletons from birth until school age. Moreover, in

healthy children the added value of developmental tests at early ages to predict

intelligence and more complex cognitive functions at school age was low. Only

the contribution of maternal SES and verbal intelligence was significant, though

limited, to sustained attention and verbal memory at school age.

Previous studies showed contrasting results on the stability of motor development

in healthy children. A study of Ridgway et al. found that the age at which certain

milestones (i.e. walking and unaided standing) are reached has prognostic value for

physical educational grades at fourteen years of age.26 Another study showed that

early fine and gross motor development at four to forty-eight months, as measured

with a parental questionnaire, is not related to fine and gross motor development

at six to twelve years of age.25 Our study showed that in healthy children even

the standardized assessment of motor and neurological performance during the

neonatal period, early infancy, and toddler age had limited predictive value for

motor development at school age. This means that results on outcome studies

based on a single measurement at an early age should be interpreted with utmost

caution.

Possible explanations for these conflicting results may be that different tests at

various ages may or may not measure the same function. Also, previous studies

have used many different measures at different ages. In addition the nature of the

investigated relationships may vary. Certain studies may have searched for small

effects that may not be relevant in everyday clinical practice, but may give a clue

about neurodevelopment.

The aim of several theories on motor development is to describe the mechanisms

involved in the development of motor functions. These theories shifted from the

idea that motor development is a gradual unfolding of predetermined patterns

in the central nervous system towards the notion that environmental factors

and sensory information play an important role in motor development.27-29 The

Neuronal Group Selection Theory states that cortical and subcortical systems in

the brain are dynamically organized into variable neuronal networks, the structure

255Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

and function of which are selected by development and behavior. The selection of

neuronal networks on the basis of afferent information plays a significant role in this

process. According to this theory, variation in developmental parameters, such as

motor performance, developmental sequence, or the duration of developmental

stages is the key to normal development.29 Our findings that the stability in

motor development until school age is limited, might be a reflection of this large

variability.

In this study, however, we did find some correlations between test scores obtained

at consecutive ages. We found correlations between the motor optimality score at

early infancy and the PDI at toddler age, and between Hempel’s optimality score

at toddler age and the Movement ABC at school age. This might be explained by

the fact that these measurements were obtained at subsequent ages. It is striking

to note in this respect that the MDI at toddler age, being considered a cognitive

measure, correlated with motor development rather than cognitive development

at school age. Indeed, the MDI contains items that require the use of fine motor

skills. This could explain our finding that the MDI correlated with manual dexterity

on the Movement ABC.

Various studies reported that in preterm born children the trajectory of motor

development is more stable. For example, Erikson et al. found that a majority

of preterm children show a stable motor development that could be predicted

by low birth weight and the presence of cerebral damage such as periventricular

leukomalacia.30 Other studies also found that early neurological assessments

are predictive of later neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants.31,32 This

seems to indicate that certain risk factors have such a strong impact on neurologic

development that it cannot be counteracted by, for example, SES, experience and

training, and other environmental factors. The development of healthy children, by

contrast, is more susceptible to variations in these factors.

With regard to the relationship between motor and cognitive development, we did

not find any early motor test score that contributed significantly to intelligence,

attention, and verbal memory at school age. This is in contrast to the finding of

Piek et al. who stated that gross motor functioning, measured with a parental

questionnaire, accounted for a significant proportion of the variance for cognitive

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

performance.25 Other studies suggested that specific items on motor tests, such

as aspects of the quality of spontaneous movements, or the age of reaching

milestones, might have predictive value for later cognitive development.5,33 Our

study showed that with regard to overall motor performance of children in early

life, there was no clear relation with cognitive development at school age. The only

factors that contributed significantly to the variance in complex cognitive functions

were maternal SES and verbal intelligence. This finding is supported by previous

studies that found that mother-infant interactions and environmental quality play

an important role in intellectual development at pre-school age.34,35 In our study,

this observation counts for complex cognitive functions, i.e. attention and memory,

in particular.

Several considerations should be taken into account when interpreting the present

results. Firstly, whether the different tests at the various ages measured the same

functions. The neurological examinations measure the integrity of the central nervous

system and identify infants at risk for minor and major neurological dysfunction,

while the Movement ABC at school age is a rather quantitative test of daily motor

skills. Infants identified as having minor neurological dysfunction, however, are at

risk of developing clumsy motor behavior and obtaining abnormal Movement ABC

scores.36 Furthermore, we found that at toddler age, for example, the results of the

neurological examination correlated highly with the children’s PDI and MDI. This

may reflect that these tests measure similar functions or aspects of development

that are interrelated. It is a fact that the BSID-II-NL and the neurological examination

both include a number of similar items. Secondly, the children’s behavioral state

might have had an impact on the test results. Especially during the first year of life,

children’s behavioral state might have led to an increased variability in test results

that may have made it difficult to find a relationship with development at school

age.

We aimed to report our results transparent, and therefore we did not adjust our

threshold of significance (p=.05) for multiple comparisons. This explorative study

assessed motor function at 4 time points, and cognition at 2 time points. A multiple

comparison correction would thus set our significance level around p=.01-.02.

Since the number of significant findings in our study was already limited, this

257Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

adjustment would not have changed the main findings of our study. Finally, we

observed that the SES of our study population was relatively high and our sample

size was thus biased to the upper classes. This limitation may also have influenced

our findings.

As a suggestion for future studies, it would be interesting to see how developmental

trajectories would evolve beyond school age, since little is known about motor

stability into adulthood.

Conclusion

Our study showed that in a cohort of healthy children motor developmental

trajectories varied considerably. Moreover, the added value of early assessments

of motor development for later cognitive function is limited. This leads us to believe

that a single abnormal test result at a certain age in an individual child at risk for

developmental delay should be interpreted cautiously. The effect of the risk factor

on development must be quite large to come to light at every assessment until

school age. If a certain risk factor only has a moderate impact on development, it

may soon be counteracted by factors that are known to have an important impact

on development. Cases in point are maternal SES and verbal intelligence and

possibly other factors that have yet to be discovered.

Acknowledgements

This study was part of the research program of the postgraduate school for

Behavioral and Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Groningen. We greatly

acknowledge the help of Dr. V. Fidler for statistical advice, Dr. T. Brantsma van

Wulfften Palthe for correcting the English manuscript, and E. Drent for help with

the Figures.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Goyen TA, Lui K. Longitudinal motor development of

“apparently normal” high-risk infants at 18 months, 3

and 5 years. Early Hum Dev 2002;70:103-115.

2. Larroque B, Ancel PY, Marret S, et al.

Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of

5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation

(the EPIPAGE study): a longitudinal cohort study.

Lancet 2008;371:813-820.

3. Biringen Z, Emde RN, Campos JJ, Appelbaum MI.

Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother,

and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its

timing. Child Dev 1995;66:499-514.

4. Taanila A, Murray GK, Jokelainen J, Isohanni M,

Rantakallio P. Infant developmental milestones: a 31-

year follow-up. Dev Med Child Neurol

2005;47:581-586.

5. Murray GK, Jones PB, Kuh D, Richards M. Infant

developmental milestones and subsequent cognitive

function. Ann Neurol 2007;62:128-136.

6. Goodman JF. Infant intelligence: Do we, can we,

should we assess it? In: Reynolds CR, Kamphaus

RW, eds. Handbook of Psychological and

Educational Assessment of Children: Intelligence and

Achievement. New York, NY: Guilford Press;

2002. p.183-208.

7. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ.

Socioeconomic gradients predict individual

differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev Sci

2007;10:464-480.

8. Aylward GP. Developmental screening and

assessment: what are we thinking? J Dev Behav

Pediatr 2009;30:169-173.

9. Roze E, Meijer L, Bakker A, Van Braeckel

KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF. Prenatal exposure

to organohalogens, including brominated flame

retardants, influences motor, cognitive, and

behavioral performance at school age. Environ Health

Perspect 2009;117:1953-1958.

10. Prechtl HFR. The Neurological Examination of the

Full-term Newborn Infant. 2nd ed. London, UK:

William Heineman medical books Ltd.; 1977.

11. Touwen BCL. Neurological Development in Infancy.

London, UK: William Heinemann Medical Books

Ltd.; 1976.

12. Prechtl HFR. Qualitative changes of spontaneous

movements in fetus and preterm infant are a marker

of neurological dysfunction (Editorial). Early Hum

Dev 1990;23:151-158.

13. Einspieler C, Bos AF, Ferrari F, Cioni G, Prechtl HFR.

Prechtl’s Method on the Qualitative Assessment of

General Movements in Preterm, Term and Young

Infants. London, UK: McKeith Press; 2004.

14. Bruggink JL, Einspieler C, Butcher PR, Van Braeckel

KN, Prechtl HF, Bos AF. The quality of the early

motor repertoire in preterm infants predicts minor

neurologic dysfunction at school age. J Pediatr

2008;153:32-39.

References

259Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

15. Bruggink JL, Einspieler C, Butcher PR, Stremmelaar

EF, Prechtl HF, Bos AF. Quantitative aspects of the

early motor repertoire in preterm infants: do they

predict minor neurological dysfunction at school

age? Early Hum Dev 2009;85:25-36.

16. Van der Meulen BF, Ruiter SAJ, Lutje Spelberg HC,

Smrkovsky M. Bayley Scales of Infant Development-

II, Dutch Version. Amsterdam, the Netherlands:

Harcourt Assessment; 1983.

17. Hempel MS. Neurological development during

toddling age in normal children and children at

risk of developmental disorders. Early Hum Dev

1993;34:47-57.

18. Stinissen J, Willems PJ, Coetsier P, Hulsman WLL.

Manual for the Dutch Translated and Adapted

Version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

(WAIS). Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger;

1970.

19. Smits-Engelsman BCM. Movement Assessment

Battery for Children. Handleiding. Lisse, the

Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1998.

20. van der Steene G, Bos A. Wechsler Preschool and

Primary Scale of Intelligence, revised. Vlaams-

Nederlandse aanpassing, handleiding. Lisse, the

Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

21. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample

and ADHD performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065-1081.

22. Heaton SC, Reader SK, Preston AS, et al. The

Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch):

patterns of performance in children with ADHD and

clinical controls. Child Neuropsychol

2001;7:251-264.

23. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545-559.

24. Tong S, Baghurst P, Vimpani G, McMichael A.

Socioeconomic position, maternal IQ, home

environment, and cognitive development. J Pediatr

2007;151:284-288.

25. Piek JP, Dawson L, Smith LM, Gasson N. The

role of early fine and gross motor development

on later motor and cognitive ability. Hum Mov Sci

2008;27:668-681.

26. Ridgway CL, Ong KK, Tammelin TH, Sharp S,

Ekelund U, Jarvelin MR. Infant motor development

predicts sports participation at age 14 years:

northern Finland birth cohort of 1966. PLoS One

2009;4:e6837.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

27. Illingworth RS. The Development of the Infant and

Young Child: Normal and Abnormal Development.

3rd ed. Edinburgh, UK: Churchill Livingstone; 1966.

28. Thelen E, Corbetta D, Kamm K, Spencer JP,

Schneider K, Zernicke RF. The transition to

reaching: mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics.

Child Dev 1993;64:1058-1098.

29. Hadders-Algra M. The Neuronal Group Selection

Theory: a framework to explain variation in normal

motor development. Dev Med Child Neurol

2000;42:566-572.

30. Erikson C, Allert C, Carlberg EB, Katz-Salamon M.

Stability of longitudinal motor development in very

low birthweight infants from 5 months to 5.5 years.

Acta Paediatr 2003;92:197-203.

31. Picciolini O, Gianni ML, Vegni C, Fumagalli M,

Mosca F. Usefulness of an early neurofunctional

assessment in predicting neurodevelopmental

outcome in very low birthweight infants. Arch Dis

Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2006;91:F111-F117.

32. Frisone MF, Mercuri E, Laroche S, et al. Prognostic

value of the neurologic optimality score at 9 and 18

months in preterm infants born before 31 weeks’

gestation. J Pediatr 2002;140:57-60.

33. Butcher PR, van Braeckel K, Bouma A, Einspieler

C, Stremmelaar EF, Bos AF. The quality of preterm

infants’ spontaneous movements: an early indicator

of intelligence and behaviour at school age. J Child

Psychol Psychiatry 2009;50:920-930.

34. Bee HL, Barnard KE, Eyres SJ, et al. Prediction of

IQ and language skill from perinatal status, child

performance, family characteristics, and mother-

infant interaction. Child Dev 1982;53:1134-1156.

35. Johnson W, McGue M, Iacono WG. Socioeconomic

Status and School Grades: Placing their Association

in Broader Context in a Sample of Biological and

Adoptive Families. Intelligence 2007;35:526-541.

36. Jongmans M, Mercuri E, de Vries LS, Dubowitz

LMS, Henderson SE. Minor neurological signs

and perceptual-motor difficulties in prematurely

born children. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed

1997;76:F9-14.

261Chapter 10 Developmental Trajectories from Birth to School Age in Healthy Term-Born Children

Chapter 11

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILES AT

SCHOOL AGE OF VERY PRETERM BORN

CHILDREN COMPARED TO TERM-BORN PEERS

ELISE ROZE, LISETHE MEIJER, KOENRAAD N. J. A. VAN BRAECKEL, SELMA A. J.

RUITER, JANNEKE L. M. BRUGGINK, AREND F. BOS

SUBMITTED

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Abstract

Objective

To establish neuropsychological profiles at school age in very preterm-born

children compared to the profiles of term-born children.

Study design

Comparative study including 60 very preterm-born children (gestational age ≤32

weeks) and 120 term-born controls. At school age, we assessed intelligence,

visuomotor integration, verbal memory, attention, and executive functioning.

Subsequently, we investigated co-occurrence of abnormal (<5th percentile) and

suspect-abnormal (<15th percentile) neuropsychological outcomes in preterm

children compared to controls.

Results

At mean 8.8 years, 55% of preterm children had suspect-abnormal

neuropsychological outcomes in multiple (≥2) domains, versus 25% of the controls

(OR 3.67, 95%-confidence interval 1.90-7.06). In preterm children with multiple

impairments, verbal memory, attention, and performance IQ were mostly affected.

No typical pattern of co-occurrence of neuropsychological impairments was

specific to preterm children, except for those that included low performance IQ

(OR 5.43, 95%-confidence interval 1.75-16.81). The higher the number of suspect-

abnormal neuropsychological domains, the worse the academic achievement

(p<.01).

Conclusions

A majority of preterm children had multiple neuropsychological impairments. Low

performance IQ was characteristic of profiles including multiple neuropsychological

impairments in preterm children but not in controls. Differences in neuropsychological

profiles between preterms and controls may reflect an altered organization of brain

structures in preterm children.

265Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

Introduction

Preterm birth is a common complication of pregnancy. Very preterm birth (<32

weeks gestation) occurs in around 1-2% of pregnancies. In the early neonatal

period, various factors in the extra-uterine environment contribute to the risk of

mild, to occasionally more severe, brain lesions in preterm infants. Early disruptions

and alterations in brain development can lead to functional impairments in motor,

cognitive, and behavioral development that may persist into childhood and even

adulthood.1-3 These functional impairments are likely to have an impact on daily life

and academic achievement.4,5

The cognitive impairments found in preterm children are not restricted to a poorer

intellectual development detected as lower intelligence. Specific neuropsychological

functions like attention, visuomotor integration, and executive functioning can also

be affected.6,7 Previous studies reported that specifically preterm children with low

intelligence are at risk for problems in these domains,8,9 while others suggested

there may be preterm children with specific neuropsychological dysfunctions in

whom intellectual development is preserved.10,11 As yet, very few studies described

the co-occurrence of impairments in neuropsychological functions in preterm

children.

Insight into the neuropsychological profiles, including intelligence, of preterm

children could reveal whether (I) preterm children develop a pattern of co-

occurring impairments in specific neuropsychological domains, or (II) multiple

neuropsychological domains are impaired without a pattern emerging, or (iii) in

most children a single neuropsychological domain is impaired. Identification of the

nature of these profiles could guide diagnostics and intervention. Our primary aim

was to establish the neuropsychological profiles at school age in a Dutch cohort of

very preterm-born children (gestational age ≤ 32 weeks), and to compare them to

the profiles of term-born children. Our second aim was to determine whether these

neuropsychological profiles relate to academic achievement. Finally, we aimed

to identify neonatal characteristics related to the neuropsychological profiles of

preterm-born children at school age.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Methods

Patients

This comparative study included 60 randomly selected preterm infants (gestational age

≤32 weeks) who had been admitted to the Neonatal Intensive Care Unit of the University

Medical Center Groningen (1996-2002). Most of these children had participated in

previous cohort studies on the outcome of preterm children with specific neonatal risk

factors, either as a case or control. We excluded children with major chromosomal and

congenital anomalies. The response rate at school age was 85%. For every preterm

child, we retrospectively included two randomly selected fullterm-born control children

born in 2002 and 2003, who lived in the northern provinces of the Netherlands. The

control children were initially recruited for the Lollypop study as described elsewhere.12

Of the fullterm born controls the response rate at school age was 73%.

Measures and Procedure

We reviewed the preterm-born children’s medical charts and recorded neonatal

characteristics and morbidities. These were gender, gestational age, birth weight, Apgar

score at 5 minutes, the presence of intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR), asphyxia,

ventilatory support, bronchopulmonary dysplasia (BPD), early and late onset sepsis,

necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC), and cerebral pathologies such as germinal matrix

hemorrhage-intraventricular hemorrhage (GMH-IVH), periventricular hemorrhagic

infarction (PVHI), post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation (PHVD), cystic periventricular

leukomalacia (PVL), and neonatal seizures.

Socioeconomic status was determined by classifying the highest occupation of both

parents into below average, average, and above average according to the Dutch

standard classification of occupations.

The preterm-born children were invited prospectively to participate in an extension

of our routine follow-up program. The control children did the same follow-up

program. It entailed the assessment of motor performance, neuropsychological

functions including intelligence, and behavior at the age of 6 to 12 years. Including

breaks the program took approximately 2.5 hours to complete. We report here on the

children’s neuropsychological functions including intelligence, as we were particularly

interested in the cognitive development of the children. We did not include the

267Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

children’s performances on motor tests. Parents gave their written, informed consent

to participate in the follow-up program and the study was approved by the Medical

Ethical Committee of the University Medical Center Groningen.

Neuropsychological Outcome

Total, verbal, and performance intelligence were assessed using a shortened version

of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition, Dutch version. We used

the subtests Vocabulary and Similarities to assess verbal intelligence and Picture

Arrangement and Block Design to assess performance intelligence.13

In addition, we assessed visuomotor integration with the subtest Design Copying of

the NEPSY-II, a neuropsychological test battery for children.14 In this subtest the child

is asked to copy geometric shapes of increasing complexity. Visuomotor integration

involves the integration of visual information with finger-hand movements.

We assessed verbal memory with a standardized Dutch version of Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test.15 This test consists of five learning trials of fifteen words with immediate

recall after each presentation and a delayed recall trial followed by a recognition trial.

We measured selective attention and attentional control with the subtests Map Mission

and Opposite Worlds of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children, Dutch version.16 In

Map Mission the child is asked to encircle as many symbols of a restaurant as possible

in one minute among distracting symbols on a map. In Opposite Worlds the child is

asked to read a string of digits 1 and 2 as 2 and 1 respectively as quickly as possible

(inhibition of automated reading). Selective attention refers to a child’s ability to select

target information from an array of distractors. Attentional control refers to the child’s

ability to shift attention flexibly and adaptively. Finally, the parents filled out the Behavior

Rating Inventory of Executive Function to assess executive functioning involved in well-

organized, purposeful, goal-directed, and problem-solving behavior as observed by the

parents in daily life.17 Examples of executive functioning are the ability to inhibit competing

actions towards attractive stimuli, the flexibility to shift problem-solving strategies when

necessary, and the ability to monitor and evaluate one’s own behavior. If children were

too tired and/or uncooperative (as assessed by the trained experimenter), we excluded

their test scores. Incomplete questionnaires were also excluded. We recorded the type

of education the children received and whether they had repeated classes.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Statistical Analyses

We used age-specific norm scores as provided in the manuals for all the

neuropsychological tests. We classified the intelligence quotients (IQs) into normal

(IQ ≥ 85), suspect (IQ 70 to 85) and abnormal (IQ<70). We used the percentiles on

the standardization samples of the neuropsychological tests to classify raw scores

into normal (≥ 15th percentile), suspect (5th to 15th percentile), and abnormal (< 5th

percentile). Verbal memory and attention have two different parameters that each

measure specific aspects of these functions. For each child we classified verbal

memory and attention as suspect or abnormal if either one of the two parameters

was suspect or abnormal.

First, to compare the neuropsychological test scores of preterm children and

controls we used the Student’s t-test for normally distributed variables and

the Mann-Whitney U test for non-normally distributed variables. Second, we

explored the neuropsychological profiles by graphically displaying the co-

occurrence of abnormal (< 5th percentile) and suspect-abnormal (< 15th percentile)

neuropsychological outcomes of each child. We then calculated odds ratios for

obtaining an abnormal outcome on at least one neuropsychological domain or

multiple (≥2) abnormal neuropsychological domains in preterms versus controls

by logistic regression analyses. These analyses were repeated after correction for

gender and socioeconomic status. Since we used age-specific norm scores, we did

not correct for age at testing. Third, to gain insight into combinations of impairments,

we tested differences in co-occurrence of neuropsychological impairments between

preterms and controls using the Chi2 test. Finally, we used the Student’s t-test and

Pearson correlation for normally distributed variables and the Mann-Whitney U test

and Spearman’s rank correlation for non-normally distributed variables to relate the

neonatal characteristics to the neuropsychological profiles in the preterm children.

The characteristics were gender, gestational age, birth weight, IUGR, ventilatory

support, BPD, early and late onset sepsis, NEC, mild cerebral pathology (i.e. GMH-

IVH grade I or II), and severe cerebral pathologies (i.e. either GMH-IVH grade III,

PVHI, PHVD or cystic PVL). We used SPSS 16.0 software for Windows (SPSS Inc,

Chicago, IL) for all the analyses and p<.05 was considered statistically significant

throughout.

269Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

Results

Table 1 provides an overview of the patient demographics of the 60 preterm and

120 control infants. There was a preponderance of males in the preterm group

compared to the controls (p<.01) and a trend towards lower socioeconomic status

(p=.052).

The mean age at follow-up was 8.8 years (range 6.8-12.8 years) in the preterm

group and 6.9 years (range 6.4-7.1 years) in the control group (p<.01).

Neuropsychological Outcome

The mean total IQ in the preterm group was 92 (standard deviation 13, range

55-118), mean verbal IQ was 95 (15, 55-128), and mean performance IQ was 90

(14, 55-118). In the control group the mean total IQ was 104 (11, 76-132), mean

verbal IQ was 106 (13, 75-143), and mean performance IQ was 103 (12, 70-128).

The box-plots in Figure 1 show the standardized test scores (z-scores) from the

neuropsychological tests of the preterm children compared to the controls. Preterm

children performed significantly worse on all tests except the test for visuomotor

integration, which showed a ceiling effect in its distribution. The distributions of the

neuropsychological scores in the preterm group were fairly similar to those in the

control group except that the means were lower in the former. Table 2 shows the

test results classified into normal, suspect, and abnormal.

Co-occurrence Analyses

Out of 60 preterm children, 33 (55%) did not have any abnormal results on the

neuropsychological assessments, whilst 27 children (45%) had an abnormal

outcome in at least one neuropsychological domain. In the control group, only 23

out of 120 (19%) had an abnormal outcome on at least one neuropsychological

domain. The odds ratio (OR) for having an abnormal score on at least one domain

in preterm children versus controls was 3.45, 95% confidence interval (CI) 1.74-

6.83, p<.01. After correction for gender and SES, the OR was 2.82, 95% CI 1.39-

5.37, p<.01. Figure 2 gives the percentage of children per number of abnormal (A)

or suspect-abnormal (B) neuropsychological domains. The following six domains

were included: verbal IQ, performance IQ, visuomotor integration, verbal memory,

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

attention, and executive functioning. Like the controls, the majority of preterm

children with an abnormal neuropsychological outcome had functional limitations

in only one domain (Figure 2A). The preterm children, however, had multiple

neuropsychological impairments more frequently, i.e. in 2 or more domains, than

the control children (9/60 (15%) versus 4/116 (3%), OR 5.12, 95% CI 1.51-17.39,

p<.01). After correction, the OR was 4.65, 95% CI 1.33-16.35, p=.02. Regarding

suspect-abnormal outcomes (Figure 2B), a majority of preterm children had

multiple neuropsychological impairments, which again was more frequent than

in the controls (33/60 (55%) versus 30/120 (25%), OR 3.67, 95% CI 1.90-7.06,

p<.01). After correction, the OR was 3.02, 95% CI 1.49-6.12, p<.01. An abnormal

IQ (<70) always co-occurred with multiple neuropsychological impairments. None

of the control children had abnormal IQs.

Of the preterm children with only one abnormal domain (n=18), most children had

problems with verbal memory (n=9), followed by visuomotor integration (n=4),

attention (n=4), and executive functioning (n=1). In the controls (n=19), although less

frequent, the distribution of problems in neuropsychological outcomes was similar.

Inspection of the neuropsychological profiles of the preterm children (n=9) and

controls (n=4) with two or more abnormal neuropsychological domains revealed

no clear pattern of combinations of abnormal outcomes (data not shown). Table

3 shows the frequency of suspect-abnormal outcomes in neuropsychological

domains in children with one versus multiple neuropsychological impairments

(≥ 2 suspect-abnormal domains). Within the group of preterm children, attention,

verbal memory, and performance IQ were abnormal in a majority of preterm

children with ≥ 2 suspect-abnormal outcomes. In comparison to the controls, a

suspect-abnormal performance IQ was more frequently accompanied by additional

neuropsychological impairments in preterm children than in control children (OR

5.43, 95% CI 1.75-16.81).

271Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

Neuropsychological Profile in relation to Academic Achievement

Table 4A gives the academic achievement (normal education, normal education

but repeated classes, special education) of the preterm and control children. It also

provides the median number of suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains per

type of academic achievement (normal education, normal education but repeated

classes, special education). Out of the six preterm children that received special

education, five attended special schools for children with learning problems and

one attended a school for children with behavioral problems. In the control group,

one child attended a school for children with learning problems and one attended

a school for children with language and hearing problems. The median number

of suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains was significantly higher for

children repeating classes and attending special education, both in the preterm

(p< .01) and control group (p< .01). Table 4B gives the type of neuropsychological

impairments in relation to academic achievement in preterm children. Verbal IQ,

performance IQ, visuomotor integration and attention were significantly more

frequently affected in children repeating classes and children attending special

education. This was not found for verbal memory and executive functions.

Neonatal Characteristics and Neuropsychological Profiles

The preterm children’s birth weight correlated with the number of abnormal and

suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains (r=-0.267, p=.04 and r=-0.353,

p<.01, respectively). This was not found for gestational age. We also found that

preterm children with IUGR had significantly more suspect-abnormal outcomes

than preterm children without IUGR (median 3 versus 2 suspect-abnormal

outcomes, p=.017). This was also found for late-onset sepsis (median 2 versus 1,

p=.019) and severe cerebral pathology (median 4 versus 1, p=.016).

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 1 Patient demographics

Preterm infants

(GA<32 weeks) n=60

Control infants n=120

Males/females 41/ 19 57/63

Gestational age (weeks) 29.4 (27.3-30.8) 40.0 (39.0-40.0)

Birth weight (grams) 1138 (941-1410) 3590 (3250-3945)

Socioeconomic status1

Below average 14 (23%) 24 (20%)

Average 33 (55%) 47 (39%)

Above average 13 (22%) 49 (41%)

Neonatal complications

IUGR (<P10) 11 (18%)

Apgar at 5 minutes 9 (4-10)

Asphyxia 1 (2%)

Ventilatory support (IPPV or HFO) 50 (83%)

Early onset sepsis 3 (5%)

Cerebral pathology

GMH-IVH grade I or II 12 (20%)

GMH-IVH grade III or PVHI 1 (2%)

Cystic PVL 0 (0%)

PHVD 2 (3%)

Neonatal seizures 0 (0%)

Late onset morbidity

Late onset sepsis 24 (40%)

Necrotizing enterocolitis 8 (13%)

Bronchopulmonary dysplasia 20 (33%)

Data are given as median (25th-75th percentile) or as numbers (percentage).1. According to the standard Dutch occupational classificationAbbreviations: GMH-IVH- germinal matrix hemorrhage-intraventricular hemorrhage; HFO- high frequency oscillation; IPPV- intermittent positive pressure ventilation; IUGR- intrauterine growth restriction; PHVD- post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation; PVHI- periventricular hemorrhagic infarction; PVL- periventricular leukomalacia.

273Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

TABLE 2 Neuropsychological outcome at school age of preterm-born

children and controls classified into normal, suspect, and abnormal

Preterms (n=60) Controls (n=120)

Normal Suspect Abnormal Normal Suspect Abnormal

Verbal IQ (n=179) 47 (78%) 10 (17%) 3 (5%) 114 (96%) 5 (4%) 0 (0%)

Performance IQ 41 (68%) 14 (23%) 5 (8%) 114 (95%) 6 (5%) 0 (0%)

Visuomotor integration

46 (77%) 6 (10%) 8 (13%) 97 (81%) 15 (13%) 8 (7%)

Verbal memory 36 (60%) 10 (17%) 14 (23%) 83 (69%) 26 (22%) 11 (9%)

Attention 33 (55%) 15 (25%) 12 (20%) 90 (75%) 23 (19%) 7 (6%)

Executive functioning 49 (82%) 7 (12%) 4 (7%) 115 (96%) 3 (3%) 2 (2%)

Data are given as number (percentage). Scores are classified into normal (≥ 15th percentile), suspect (5th to 15th percentile), and abnormal (< 5th percentile). Children were classified based on their worst score on one of the two parameters.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

TABLE 3 Type of neuropsychological impairment in children with one

versus multiple neuropsychological impairments (≥ 2 suspect-abnormal

domains)

1 suspect-abnormal

domain

≥2 suspect-abnormal

domains

Preterms

(n=11)

Controls

(n=36)

Preterms

(n=33)

Controls

(n=30)

Verbal IQ 2 (18%) 0 (0%) 11 (33%) 5 (17%)

Performance IQ 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 19 (58%)** 6 (20%)

Visuomotor integration 0 (0%) 7 (19%) 14 (42%) 16 (53%)

Verbal memory 4 (36%) 14 (39%) 21 (63%) 23 (77%)

Attention 5 (45%) 15 (42%) 21 (63%) 15 (50%)

Executive functioning 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 11 (33%) 5 (17%)

Data are given as number of children with suspect-abnormal outcomes (percentage of the group concerned). ** p<.01 when comparing preterms with ≥2 suspect-abnormal domains to controls with ≥2 suspect-abnormal domains (Chi2)

275Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

TABLE 4A Number of impairments in neuropsychological domains in relation

to academic achievement

Academic achievement Preterm children (n=60) Control children (n=120)

Normal education n=31 (52%) 1 (0-2) n=112 (93%) 1 (0-1)

Normal education, repeated classes

n=23 (38%) 3 (2-4) n=6 (5%) 2.5 (1.5-4)

Special educational classes

n=6 (10%) 4.5 (1.5-5) n=2 (2%) 3.5 (2-5)

Data are given as number of children (percentage) followed by median number of neuropsychological domains with suspect-abnormal scores (25th-75th percentile).

TABLE 4B Type of impairments in neuropsychological domains in relation to

academic achievement in preterm children

Normal education (n=31)

Normal education, repeated classes (n=23)

Special educational classes (n=6)

Verbal IQ** 1 (3%) 8 (35%) 4 (67%)

Performance IQ** 2 (6%) 13 (57%) 4 (67%)

Visuomotor integration** 2 (6%) 7 (30%) 5 (83%)

Verbal memory 10 (32%) 11 (48%) 3 (50%)

Attention** 8 (26%) 15 (65%) 4 (67%)

Executive functioning 4 (13%) 6 (26%) 1 (17%)

Data are given as number of children with suspect-abnormal outcomes (percentage of the group concerned). ** p<.01 (Chi2)

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Figure 1 Standardized test scores of neuropsychological tests at school age in the preterm group compared to the control group

Standardized test scores of neuropsychological tests at school age in the preterm group (n=60) compared to the control group (n=120). Box-plots represent z-score distribution (median, 25th and 75th percentile and range).**p<.01

3,00

2,00

1,00

0,00

-1,00

-2,00

-3,00

-4,00

95th centile

5th centile

Z-score

Performance IQ

Visuomotorintegration

Verbal memory

Attention Executivefunctioning

Verbal IQ

Controls (n=120)

Preterms (n=60)

**

****

**

**

277Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

Fig

ure

2B P

erce

ntag

es o

f ch

ildre

n b

y nu

mb

er o

f

susp

ect-

abno

rmal

neu

rop

sych

olo

gic

al o

utco

mes

Fig

ure

2A P

erce

ntag

es o

f ch

ildre

n b

y nu

mb

er

of

abno

rmal

neu

rop

sych

olo

gic

al o

utco

mes

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10% 0%

Num

ber

of n

euro

psy

chol

ogic

al d

omai

ns w

ith s

usp

ect-

abno

rmal

sco

res

03

14

25

Percentage

Con

trol

s (n

=12

0)

Con

trol

s w

ith a

bno

rmal

IQs

(n=

0/12

0)

Pre

term

s (n

=60

)

Pre

term

s w

ith a

bno

rmal

IQs

(n=

6/60

)

n=54

n=16

n=36

n=11

n=24

n=17

n=3

n=6

n=2

n=1

n=5

n=5

n=54

80%

70%

60%

50%

40%

30%

20%

10% 0%

Num

ber

of n

euro

psy

chol

ogic

al d

omai

ns w

ith a

bno

rmal

sco

res

03

14

25

Percentage

Con

trol

s (n

=12

0)

Con

trol

s w

ith a

bno

rmal

IQs

(n=

0/12

0)

Pre

term

s (n

=60

)

Pre

term

s w

ith a

bno

rmal

IQs

(n=

6/60

)

n=97

n=33

n=18

n=4

n=1

n=3

n=3

n=1

n=19

Six

Neu

rop

sych

olog

ical

dom

ains

wer

e in

clud

ed: v

erb

al IQ

, per

form

ance

IQ, v

isuo

mot

or in

tegr

atio

n, v

erb

al m

emor

y, a

tten

tion,

and

exe

cutiv

e fu

nctio

ning

. Sus

pec

t-ab

norm

al IQ

s (<

85) o

r ab

norm

al IQ

s (<

70) c

over

eith

er v

erb

al o

r p

erfo

rman

ce IQ

.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Discussion

This study showed that a majority (55%) of very preterm-born children had

suspect-abnormal outcomes in multiple neuropsychological domains, which was

more than in controls (25%). We found no typical neuropsychological profile or co-

occurrence of impairments in specific neuropsychological domains among preterm

children compared to controls, but in children with multiple suspect-abnormal

domains, verbal memory, attention, and performance IQ were mostly affected. Low

performance IQ was characteristic of profiles including multiple neuropsychological

impairments in preterm children but not in controls. These neuropsychological

impairments were related to academic achievement in the sense that more

neuropsychological domains were impaired in children who repeated classes and

attended special schools. The neonatal characteristics related to multiple suspect-

abnormal neuropsychological outcomes at school age were birth weight, IUGR,

late-onset sepsis, and severe cerebral pathology. Many preterm-born children had

thus been exposed to factors that, presumably through altered brain development,

led to impairments in combinations of neuropsychological domains that seemed

to hamper academic achievement.

The novelty of this study is that to provide insight into the effect of prematurity

on later neuropsychological outcome, we investigated the co-occurrence

of neuropsychological impairments in preterm children by examining

neuropsychological profiles, rather than reporting impairments across preterm

children. Comparing these profiles to the profiles of a large cohort of term-born

children enabled us to determine whether a typical neuropsychological profile was

attributable to very preterm birth.

We could not confirm a neuropsychological profile typical for preterm children,

since most preterm children were impaired in multiple neuropsychological domains

without a pattern emerging. Nevertheless, impaired attention, verbal memory, and

performance IQ were the neuropsychological domains most frequently involved in

preterm children with suspect-abnormal outcomes in multiple neuropsychological

domains. Previously, these functions have been identified as being affected by

prematurity,18,19 but they now also appear to be affected in combination with

impairments in other neuropsychological domains. Previous studies also reported

279Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

that neuropsychological impairments are found in preterm children with poor

intellectual development.9,20 Our study confirmed these findings by showing that

most preterm children with low IQs had multiple neuropsychological impairments.

Interestingly, when comparing the preterm profile to the profile of controls,

specifically low performance IQ was more frequently accompanied by additional

neuropsychological impairments in the preterm group, but not for instance attention

or verbal memory. Perhaps preterm-born children cannot compensate sufficiently

for other multiple impaired neuropsychological domains to achieve a normal

performance IQ while full-term born children can. If only one domain is impaired

then both preterm and control children can compensate sufficiently to achieve a

normal performance IQ. A substantial group of preterm children remained who

had suspect-abnormal outcomes in one or more domains in the absence of low

IQs. Others also stated that very preterm children can be at risk for neurocognitive

impairment despite average intelligence.10,11 Therefore, our study suggests that

including only general intellectual development in a follow-up program for preterm

children may wrongfully classify a preterm child as developing normally thereby

missing possible specific neuropsychological problems.

In our study the number of suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains was

clearly related to academic achievement. We know that up to half of former very

low birth weight infants require special assistance in school.21 Learning difficulties

typical for preterm children, e.g. in the field of reading, spelling, mathematics, or

writing can be independent of IQ scores.5,22 It may well be that combinations of

problems in neuropsychological functions as found here, such as attention and

visuomotor integration underlie such learning difficulties.

Regarding neonatal characteristics, we found that birth weight was related to

the neuropsychological profile of preterm children. There is an inverse relation

between birth weight and intellectual development in preterm-born children.18

Moreover, increased rates of neurocognitive impairments such as problems in

attention, executive functioning, and a decline in school achievement occur with

decreasing birth weight.5,21,23 Our study adds that birth weight also affects the

number of neuropsychological domains affected at school age.

Several characteristics of preterm brain development may be involved in the

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

underpinnings of these neuropsychological profiles at school age. First, disruption

of processes of brain connectivity such as synaptogenesis and corticogenesis that

normally occur in the intra-uterine environment, can lead to structural changes

in brain organization in preterm children that persist into young adulthood.24-26 In

addition to alterations in brain development, perinatal brain injury also affects brain

maturation processes.27 Particularly the white matter of preterm infants is vulnerable

to damage, for example from inflammatory responses in case of sepsis.28 These

disruptions and alterations in brain development may have several functional

implications. For example, diminished cortical volumes have been related to

lower intellectual development,29 altered pathways of brain activation to attention

allocation,30 and disturbed connectivity between posterior brain regions and the

prefrontal cortex to worse executive functions in preterm children.31 Nevertheless,

the direct functional implications of combinations of these processes, and how

they lead to specific neuropsychological profiles at a later age, remain poorly

understood.

This study was limited by a relatively small sample size. To increase the power

of our study, we included twice as many controls as preterm children. Preterm

and control children were not prospectively matched, which led to an imbalance

in gender and a trend towards lower socioeconomic status in the preterm group.

Low socioeconomic status is known to be involved in the etiology of prematurity.

We cannot elucidate the potential role of lower socioeconomic status in the

development and persistence of neuropsychological impairments in the preterm

children of our study although after correction for socioeconomic status our

results did not change. In our study, verbal memory and attention had two different

parameters that each measured specific aspects of these functions. Children were

classified suspect or abnormal if either one of the two was suspect or abnormal,

which may have led to an inflation of the number of children being classified as

such compared to other domains that were based on a single measure. Finally,

this was an exploratory study in which we chose to test those neuropsychological

functions that have been reported to be poorer in preterm born children. Due

to limited attention span of the children, we were unable to test all possible

neuropsychological functions, leaving out e.g. language functions.

281Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

In conclusion, a majority of preterm children had suspect-abnormal outcomes on

multiple neuropsychological domains. There was no typical pattern of impairment

in specific neuropsychological domains that distinguished preterm children from

controls, though low performance IQ was characteristic of profiles including

multiple neuropsychological impairments in preterm children but not in controls.

The number of suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains was related

to worse academic achievement. This study points to the necessity of early

identification of children at risk for complex neuropsychological profiles and raises

the question of how to implement intervention strategies that could contribute to

improving the outcome of these children. We recommend screening for a wide

range of neuropsychological functions including verbal memory, attention and

performance IQ, when performing neuropsychological diagnostics.

Acknowledgements

This study was part of the research program of the Research School for Behavioral

and Cognitive Neurosciences, University of Groningen, the Netherlands. It was

financially supported by the Dutch Brain Foundation and a research grant awarded

by FrieslandCampina and Friso Infant Nutrition. We greatly acknowledge the help

of Dr. T. Brantsma – van Wulfften Palthe for correcting the English manuscript.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

1. Marlow N, Hennessy EM, Bracewell MA, Wolke D.

Motor and executive function at 6 years of age after

extremely preterm birth. Pediatrics

2007;120:793-804.

2. Doyle LW, Anderson PJ. Adult outcome of extremely

preterm infants. Pediatrics 2010;126:342-351.

3. Dall’oglio AM, Rossiello B, Coletti MF, et al. Do

healthy preterm children need neuropsychological

follow-up? Preschool outcomes compared with term

peers. Dev Med Child Neurol 2010;52:955-961.

4. Feder KP, Majnemer A, Bourbonnais D, Platt R,

Blayney M, Synnes A. Handwriting performance in

preterm children compared with term peers at age 6

to 7 years. Dev Med Child Neurol 2005;47:163-170.

5. Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van

Goudoever JB, Oosterlaan J. Meta-analysis of

neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/

or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics

2009;124:717-728.

6. Taylor HG, Klein N, Drotar D, Schluchter M, Hack M.

Consequences and risks of <1000-g birth weight for

neuropsychological skills, achievement, and adaptive

functioning. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2006;27:459-469.

7. Van Braeckel KNJA, Butcher PR, Geuze RH,

van Duijn MA, Bos AF, Bouma A. Less efficient

elementary visuomotor processes in 7- to 10-year-

old preterm-born children without cerebral palsy:

an indication of impaired dorsal stream processes.

Neuropsychology 2008;22:755-764.

8. Dewey D, Crawford SG, Creighton DE, Sauve RS.

Long-term neuropsychological outcomes in very

low birth weight children free of sensorineural

impairments. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol

1999;21:851-865.

9. Herrgard E, Luoma L, Tuppurainen K, Karjalainen S,

Martikainen A. Neurodevelopmental profile at five

years of children born at < or=32 weeks gestation.

Dev Med Child Neurol 1993; 35:1083-1096.

10. Korkman M, Mikkola K, Ritari N, et al.

Neurocognitive test profiles of extremely low birth

weight five-year-old children differ according to

neuromotor status. Dev Neuropsychol

2008;33:637-655.

11. Grunau RE, Whitfield MF, Davis C. Pattern of

learning disabilities in children with extremely low

birth weight and broadly average intelligence. Arch

Pediatr Adolesc Med 2002;156:615-620.

12. Kerstjens JM, Bos AF, ten Vergert EM, de Meer G,

Butcher PR, Reijneveld SA. Support for the global

feasibility of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire

as developmental screener. Early Hum Dev

2009;85:443-447.

13. Gregoire J. Comparison of three short forms

of the Wechsler intelligence scale for children

- third edition (WISC-III). Eur Rev Appl Psychol

2000;50:437-41.

References

283Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

14. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY II. Clinical

and Interpretative Manual. San Antonio (TX):

PsychCorp. A Brand of Harcourt Assessments;

2007.

15. van den Burg W, Kingma A. Performance of 225

Dutch school children on Rey’s Auditory Verbal

Learning Test (AVLT): parallel test-retest reliabilities

with an interval of 3 months and normative data.

Arch Clin Neuropsychol 1999;14:545-559.

16. Manly T, Anderson V, Nimmo-Smith I, Turner A,

Watson P, Robertson IH. The differential assessment

of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday

Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample

and ADHD performance. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:1065-1081.

17. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L.

BRIEF—Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive

Function: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources; 2000.

18. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM,

Anand KJS. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of

school-aged children who were born preterm - A

meta-analysis. JAMA 2002;288:728-737.

19. Briscoe J, Gathercole SE, Marlow N. Everyday

memory and cognitive ability in children born

very prematurely. J Child Psychol Psychiatry

2001;42:749-754.

20. Pasman JW, Rotteveel JJ, Maassen B.

Neurodevelopmental profile in low-risk preterm

infants at 5 years of age. Eur J Paediatr Neurol

1998;2:7-17.

21. Taylor HG, Klein N, Minich NM, Hack M. Middle-

school-age outcomes in children with very low

birthweight. Child Dev 2000;71:1495-511.

22. Anderson P, Doyle LW. Neurobehavioral outcomes

of school-age children born extremely low birth

weight or very preterm in the 1990s. JAMA

2003;289:3264-3272.

23. Klebanov PK, Brooks-Gunn J, McCormick MC.

School achievement and failure in very low birth

weight children. J Dev Behav Pediatr

1994;15:248-256.

24. Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional

differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral

cortex. J Comp Neurol 1997;387:167-178.

25. Allin M, Walshe M, Fern A, et al. Cognitive

maturation in preterm and term born adolescents.

J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:381-386.

26. Counsell SJ, Edwards AD, Chew AT, et al. Specific

relations between neurodevelopmental abilities

and white matter microstructure in children born

preterm. Brain 2008;131:3201-3208.

27. Ferriero DM. Medical progress - Neonatal brain

injury. New Engl J Med 2004;351:1985-1995.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

28. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by

white matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance

imaging at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

29. Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, et al. Regional

brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive

outcome in preterm infants. JAMA

2000;284:1939-1947.

30. Lawrence EJ, Rubia K, Murray RM, et al. The neural

basis of response inhibition and attention allocation

as mediated by gestational age. Hum Brain Mapp

2009;30:1038-1050.

31. Skranes J, Lohaugen GC, Martinussen M, et al.

White matter abnormalities and executive function

in children with very low birth weight. Neuroreport

2009;20:263-266.

285Chapter 11 Neuropsychological Profiles at School Age of Very Preterm Born Children Compared to Term-Born Peers

Chapter 12

GENERAL DISCUSSION AND FUTURE

PERSPECTIVES

ELISE ROZE

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

The main aim of this thesis was to establish the motor, cognitive, and behavioral

outcome at school age of newborn infants with perinatal risk factors for adverse

outcome. The secondary aims were i. to investigate the interrelationship between

motor, cognitive, and neuropsychological development until school age in preterm

born infants compared to healthy, term-born infants and ii. to relate perinatal and

cerebral characteristics of newborn infants to long-term neurodevelopmental

outcome. To this end, we determined the impact of several perinatal risk factors

on long-term outcome. These ranged from persistent environmental pollutants, to

which the whole population is exposed and that showed a mild effect on long-term

neurodevelopmental outcome, to perinatal brain injury and systemic disease, to

which only a minority of children is exposed, but that had a much greater impact

on outcome at school age.

With regard to environmental pollutants we found that prenatal background

exposure to organohalogen compounds (OHCs) correlated with neuropsychological

functioning at 5 to 6 years of age in healthy Dutch children (Part 1, Chapter 2).1

In particular, polybrominated diphenyl ethers (PBDEs) that are used extensively

worldwide, correlated with motor performance, attention, visual perception, and

behavior. In this explorative study, the first of its kind to examine this relationship,

we found both negative and positive correlations between compounds and

outcome.

A more recent study by Herbstman et al. from the United States of America also

found an association between PBDEs and outcome in children. They found that

PBDE congeners were inversely related to neurodevelopmental indices such as

the psychomotor and mental developmental index of the Bayley Scales of Infant

Development and intelligence quotients in children at 1 to 6 years of age.2 They

found a difference in mean intellectual developmental scores of 5 to 8 points in

children who were in the highest 20% of prenatal exposure to PBDEs compared to

those in the lower 80% of exposure.

We did not find a relation between compounds and intellectual development. This

may be explained by the fact that in the Unites States, concentrations of OHCs are

at least 4 times higher than in Europe.3,4 In line with our study, a study by Gascon

et al. from Spain found that exposure to PBDEs was related to reduced attention

289Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

in children.5 In contrast to our study, however, they found that exposure during

childhood was related to outcome rather than prenatal exposure.

One of the potential mechanisms through which OHCs have an adverse effect on

neurodevelopment is through interference with thyroid hormone signaling in the

developing brain. Thyroid hormone plays a role in synaptogenesis and myelination

in the developing brain during the prenatal and postnatal period.6 In addition, two

other studies described an effect of organohalogens on neurotransmitter systems.7,8

Finally, an in vitro study recently showed that primary fetal human neural progenitor

cells exposed to the PBDEs BDE-47 and BDE-99 had decreased migration distance

and reduced differentiation into neurons and oligodendrocytes.9

In future studies we should examine which of the correlations we found in our

study can be confirmed in larger samples. It would also be interesting to perform

a longitudinal follow-up study of children exposed to OHCs in order to investigate

whether potential effects on neurodevelopment last throughout childhood

and continue into adulthood. Finally, the isolated effects of specific OHCs and

combinations of OHCs should be further investigated, as was done in the study

by Gascon et al.5

Subsequently, in Part 2 of the thesis, we determined the short-term and long-term

outcome of newborn infants who suffered from overt types of brain injury in the

perinatal and neonatal period.

Firstly, we found that in preterm infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

(PVHI), mortality occurred in 30% of the infants despite optimal treatment (Chapter

3).10 We also found that circulatory failure and maternal intrauterine infection in

these infants were associated with an increased risk of mortality. Previously, these

factors were described as risk factors for the emergence of PVHI11-13 but they also

appear to increase the risk of mortality. In the survivors, we found that at 18 months

of age motor development was abnormal in 66%, but functional abilities were

good in the majority of surviving children. In this study the percentage of children

who developed CP after PVHI was similar to that found in previous studies, i.e.

ranging from 60% to 90%.14-17

Secondly, we performed a follow-up study at 4 to 12 years of age of preterm-

born children with PVHI in order to determine to what extent PVHI had an impact

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

on outcome at school age (Chapter 4).18 Again, we found that the majority of the

surviving children who had suffered PVHI as infants had cerebral palsy with limited

functional impairment at school age. Intelligence was within 1 SD of the norm of

preterm-born children without lesions in 60% to 80% of the children. In particular

verbal memory was affected. Behavioral problems and executive function

problems occurred only slightly more compared to preterm infants without lesions.

Previous studies showed no consensus on cognitive outcome in children with

PVHI. The percentages of children with normal cognition range from only 20% up

to 79%.15,19,20 In many studies on the long-term outcome of preterm-born children,

children with severe brain injury, including PVHI, are often described as being on

the worst end of the outcome spectrum and they have very poor prognoses.21-23

In our cohort study of children with PVHI, however, we found that the functional

abilities in the majority of children were rather good and that a majority of children

was able to attend normal education. Our conclusion from these two studies is,

therefore, that the functional outcome at school age of preterm-born children with

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction is better than was previously thought.

To our surprise, in neither of our two studies were we able to relate the extent

and localization of PVHI to functional outcome (Chapters 3 and 4). We only found

that the most extended PVHIs (fronto-parieto-occipital) were associated with the

development of CP at 18 months, but not with the severity of CP. Previous studies

reported that more severe PVHIs and the presence of porencephaly are associated

with poorer cognitive outcome.15,20,24 It is difficult to determine why we were unable

to demonstrate such a relation. Small sample size, different medical interventions,

or socioeconomic status may have been involved, while potential interindividual

differences in the ability to recover from such a lesion may also have played a

role.

In Chapter 5 we studied the outcome of term-born infants with seizures who

required multiple anti-epileptic drugs (AEDs). We found a mortality rate of 38%.

In the survivors, motor development was normal in around a third of the children,

a third had minor neurological abnormalities and a third had major neurological

abnormalities. In addition, we found that outcome was poorer if seizure control

failed, whilst the etiology of the seizures and the number of AEDs required to reach

291Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

seizure control had limited prognostic value. We speculate that the severity of

cerebral injury is the keystone of a poor response to AEDs. Additionally, it may also

be that the seizures themselves are harmful to the brain. Previous studies stated

that neonatal seizure as such may have a disruptive effect on brain development

in infants.25-27 Others found the number and duration of seizures to be predictive of

poor outcomes.28-31 This phenomenon possibly explains why we found that failing

to achieve seizure control was associated with worse outcome.

The studies in Part 2 showed to what extent brain lesions in newborn infants can

affect motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome at school age and how treatment-

related factors are involved. Already in the neonatal period the question arises to

what extent brain lesions will influence later neurodevelopment of children.

In the final chapter of Part 2, our aim was to determine the prognostic value of an

advanced magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) technique for motor development

in newborns with focal perinatal brain injury (Chapter 6). We also aimed to

determine the added value for clinical practice of tractography to conventional

MRI in predicting motor outcome. We found that the diffusion characteristics

of corticospinal tracts of infants with focal perinatal injury (i.e. either arterial

ischemic stroke or hemorrhagic parenchymal infarction, equivalent to PVHI) were

prognostic for subsequent normal motor development, mild motor asymmetry or

the development of a hemiplegia. Particularly, the added value of tractography

to conventional MRI is that it identifies children who developed mild motor

asymmetries. We speculated that the asymmetries that we found in volume,

fractional anisotropy, and radial diffusivity of corticospinal tracts observed in

infants who developed motor impairments may be early markers of the progressive

disintegration of fibers due to Wallerian degeneration. Wallerian degeneration is

the anterograde degeneration of axons and myelin sheaths after proximal axonal

or cell body injury.32,33 It thus seems that advanced MRI techniques that study the

diffusion properties of water in different brain regions may have the potential of

providing early prognostic information for functional outcome in newborn infants

with brain lesions, including mild impairments.

In Part 3 of the thesis we studied the functional outcome at school age of preterm

and term-born infants who suffered systemic diseases in the neonatal period.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

These diseases included necrotizing enterocolitis (NEC), spontaneous intestinal

perforations (SIP), sepsis, and surgically treated intestinal obstructions. Off-hand

one would perhaps not expect these diseases to have a direct effect on motor,

cognitive, and behavioral functioning in later life. Nevertheless, we found that they

led to functional impairments at school age.

In the first study we found that 68% of the newborn infants with NEC or SIP

had borderline or abnormal motor outcomes as measured with the Movement

Assessment Battery for Children. On average, their intelligence was 11 intelligence

quotient (IQ) points lower than matched control children without gastrointestinal

diseases. In addition, their attention and visual perception was impaired (Chapter

7). In the second study on preterm-born infants with late-onset sepsis we also

found that 68% of the children had borderline or abnormal motor outcomes. On

average, their intelligence was 9 IQ points lower than matched control children

without late-onset sepsis and their attention and verbal memory was affected

(Chapter 8). The final cohort we investigated in Part 3 consisted mostly of term-

born infants treated surgically for intestinal obstructions in the first days of life

(Chapter 9). In these children we also found functional impairments at school age

albeit to a lesser extent. Of the included children, 33% had borderline or abnormal

motor outcomes, compared to 15% in a reference population. In addition,

selective attention was affected, whilst global cognitive development and other

neuropsychological functions were preserved.

The potential mechanism in these systemic diseases that is responsible for the

functional impairments we found at school age, is multifactorial with a key role for

neuroinflammation. In systemic diseases leading to inflammation there is cross-

talk between the central nervous system and the periphery.34 The emergence

of, for example NEC, SIP, and sepsis initially lead to a systemic inflammatory

response. Subsequently, pro-inflammatory cytokines are released by the activation

of microglial cells in the brain. These cytokines inhibit proliferation of neuronal

precursor cells and contribute to damage to the pre-oligodendrocytes that play

an important role in the myelination of the brain.35-37 The increased permeability

of the blood-brain barrier during inflammation further increases the susceptibility

of the brain to this inflammatory response.38 In addition, these ill infants are at

293Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

risk of respiratory and circulatory insufficiency that can lead to hypotension

and subsequent hypoxemia and ischemia of brain areas. It was suggested

recently that systemic cytokines are related to a disturbance in cerebrovascular

autoregulation and diminished cerebral blood flow.36,39 These events may lead to

a cascade of reactions in the brain, including the release of reactive oxygen and

nitrogen species.40 Indeed, previous studies showed that NEC, SIP, and sepsis are

associated with an increased risk of overt, but also more subtle and diffuse, white

matter abnormalities.41,42

In the children with surgically treated intestinal obstructions in the neonatal

period, who were mostly term-born, inflammation presumably also played a role

albeit to a lesser extent. We suggest that physiological stress during surgery, and

anesthesia-related factors may lie at the basis of their impairments. In an animal

study it was found that the use of anesthesia during the cerebral growth spurt

can lead to neuro-apoptosis and an adverse long-term behavioral outcome.43 In

addition, there are maturational processes of neuronal riping and outgrowth which

normally occur from birth until years postnatally that are potentially susceptible

to injury from anesthetic agents. In term-born infants suffering from brain injury, a

different pattern is observed compared to preterm-born infants. At this stage of

brain development it is the central grey matter regions, especially the basal ganglia

and thalami, which are specifically vulnerable to hypoxic–ischaemic injury. To date,

it is unknown if these structures are also affected in term-born infants that undergo

surgery.

Interestingly, in children with NEC and SIP we also found that surgical treatment

was a risk factor for adverse outcome. Whether the harmfull effects of anesthetics

on the brain and physiological stress during surgery are the result of the same

pathophysiological processes in preterm and term-born infants is an important

topic of future studies.

Parenteral nutrition and prolonged nutritional deficiencies have previously also

been described as risk factors for adverse outcome, particularly for cognition and

learning at pre-school age.44 Nutritional problems may have played a role in all

three studies.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

From the studies described in Parts 2 and 3 it is apparent that both overt brain

lesions and more diffuse, inflammation driven types of brain injury can contribute to

functional impairments at school age. Table 1 summarizes the main findings of the

different studies. It shows that most motor problems were found in children with

PVHI. In addition to the data shown in the table, in this cohort 76% of the children

developed CP compared to only 33% in the children with convulsions, 15% in the

children with NEC and SIP, 16% in the children with late-onset sepsis, and none

of the children with intestinal obstructions. With regard to cognitive outcomes,

however, the differences between the different cohorts were less pronounced.

Particularly when comparing children with PVHI to children with NEC, SIP, and

late-onset sepsis the differences in IQs were only 3 and 6 points, respectively (see

Table 1). The impairments in neuropsychological functions, although in different

domains, also were of fairly similar magnitude.

295Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

TAB

LE 1

Fun

ctio

nal o

utco

me

at s

cho

ol a

ge

of

new

bo

rns

with

ove

rt b

rain

lesi

ons

and

sys

tem

ic d

isea

ses

New

bor

ns w

ith o

vert

bra

in le

sion

sN

ewb

orns

with

sys

tem

ic d

isea

ses

New

bor

ns w

ith

PV

HI

New

bor

ns w

ith

seiz

ures

New

bor

ns w

ith

NE

C/S

IP

New

bor

ns w

ith

late

-ons

et s

epsi

s

New

bor

ns in

test

inal

obst

ruct

ions

Num

ber

n=21

n=45

n=52

n=32

n=27

Ges

tatio

nal a

ge (w

eeks

)27

.6 (2

5.4–

35.0

)40

.0 (3

6.9

– 42

.1)

30.5

(25.

2-40

.0)

28.9

(25.

7-33

.4)

36.7

(28.

9-42

.0)

Birt

h w

eigh

t (g

ram

s)10

45 (7

00–2

430)

3525

(200

0 –

5475

)12

53 (7

10-3

200)

1010

(600

-169

0)30

00 (7

10-5

065)

Age

at

asse

ssm

ent (

year

s) 8

.74.

0 9

.3 8

.4 9

.5

Mot

or o

utco

me

(nor

mal

/bor

der

line/

abno

rmal

)1

24%

/29%

/47%

38%

/36%

/27%

31%

/27%

/41%

32%

/29%

/39%

70%

/7%

/22%

Inte

llect

ual o

utco

me

(mea

n IQ

)83

unkn

own

8689

99

Neu

rop

sych

olog

ical

out

com

eV

isuo

mot

or

inte

grat

ion,

mem

ory

affe

cted

unkn

own

Att

entio

n, v

isua

l

per

cep

tion

affe

cted

Att

entio

n, m

emor

y

affe

cted

Att

entio

n af

fect

ed

Beh

avio

ral o

utco

me

(nor

mal

/bor

der

line/

abno

rmal

)2

53%

/12%

/35%

unkn

own

70%

/8%

/22%

70%

/10%

/20%

unkn

own

1. N

orm

al w

as d

efine

d a

s >

P15

, bor

der

line

as P

5-P

15 a

nd a

bno

rmal

as

<P

5 fo

r ch

ildre

n w

ith N

EC

/SIP

, lat

e-on

set

sep

sis,

and

inte

stin

al o

bst

ruct

ions

. For

chi

ldre

n w

ith P

VH

I and

se

izur

es, n

orm

al w

as d

efine

d a

s no

neu

rolo

gica

l ab

norm

aliti

es, b

ord

erlin

e as

chi

ldre

n w

ith m

inor

neu

rolo

gica

l dys

func

tions

or

a gr

oss

mot

or fu

nctio

n cl

assi

ficat

ion

syst

em

(GM

FCS

) lev

el I,

and

ab

norm

al a

s ≥

GM

FCS

leve

l II

2. A

sses

sed

with

the

Chi

ld B

ehav

ior

Che

cklis

t. N

orm

al, b

ord

erlin

e, a

nd a

bno

rmal

wer

e d

efine

d in

acc

ord

ance

with

the

man

ual

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

It is difficult to determine why motor functioning is affected most in children with

PVHI, whilst cognitive functioning is affected to a similar extent in both preterms

with systemic disease and overt brain lesions. In PVHI there is a germinal matrix

hemorrhage that causes pressure on the periventricular terminal vein that drains

the cerebral hemisphere.25 This leads to venous congestion in the periventricular

white matter and subsequently to ischemia and hemorrhage resulting in local

destruction of brain tissue of variable magnitude. In the local destruction of tissue

caused by PVHI it may be that the corticospinal tract, which plays a central role in

motor functioning, is involved more often whilst this is the case less often in preterm

infants with systemic disease. How perinatal brain injury can lead to impairments in

cognitive functioning is less well understood. What we do know is that disruptions

in white matter involving myelination and dendritic connections have an impact

on cortical circuits connecting different brain regions and this impact has been

related to cognitive functions. For example, disruptions of the circuitry connecting

frontal, striatal, and thalamic regions have been associated with dysregulation of

attention.45 In addition, potentially as a result of decreased connectivity, reduced

gray matter volumes have also been related to cognitive functions. For example,

intellectual development has been correlated particularly with gray matter volumes

of the sensorimotor cortex, midtemporal cortex, and corpus callosum.46,47 Finally,

deep gray matter structures are related to neuropsychological functions such as

reduced hippocampal volume that is associated with memory deficits.48

Global, diffuse white matter injury, which is the most common type of brain injury

in the preterm infant, lies at the basis of these changes in brain connectivity and

regional volumes and is responsible for the deficits in functional outcome.49 Among

a number of previous studies, a recent paper by Volpe et al. pointed out that pre-

oligodendrocytes are the principal cellular target in cerebral white matter injury,

the most important form of brain injury and subsequently of neurological disability

in preterm infants.50 In their paper, Volpe et al. state that primary injury to pre-

oligodendrocytes relates to a confluence of maturation-dependent characteristics

that render these cells vulnerable to two principal upstream mechanisms, hypoxia-

ischemia and systemic infection or inflammation. These upstream mechanisms

converge on three interacting downstream mechanisms, i.e. microglial activation,

297Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

excitotoxicity, and ultimately, free radical attack that contribute to neuronal

damage.50 We speculate that hypoxic-ischemic injury and inflammation played a

role in both the preterm-born children with PVHI and systemic disease. Some

previous studies also speculated on a potential association between germinal-

matrix hemorrhage and PVHI and diffuse white matter injury.51,52 We present

a schematic overview of a proposed mechanism contributing to functional

impairments in these children in Figure 1. Exact mechanisms and how these

processes lead to functional impairments should be the focus of future studies.

Figure 1 Proposed mechanism of brain injury leading to functional impairments in

preterm infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction and systemic diseases

In Part 4, we studied the interrelation between various aspects of neurodevelopment.

In the first study (Chapter 10) we determined the stability of scores obtained on tests

of motor development from birth until school age in healthy, term-born singletons.

The results showed that in a cohort of healthy children motor developmental

trajectories varied considerably. Moreover, the added value of early assessments

of motor development for later cognitive function was limited. These results led us

to believe that a single abnormal test result at a certain age in an individual child at

risk of developmental delay, such as a preterm-born child, should be interpreted

Systemic inflammation Ischemia/reperfusion

Neuroinflammation IVH/PVHI

Global diffuse white matter injury Focal brain injury

Cognitive impairments Motor impairments

Blood-brain barriercrosstalk

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

cautiously. The effect of a risk factor on development must be quite large to come

to light at every assessment until school age. If a certain risk factor only has a

moderate impact on development, it may soon be counteracted by factors that

are known to have an important impact on development. Cases in point identified

in our study were maternal SES and verbal intelligence, but possibly other factors

as well that have yet to be discovered.

In the second study (Chapter 11) we established the neuropsychological

profiles at school age in very preterm-born children compared to the profiles of

term-born children. We found that a majority of preterm children had multiple

neuropsychological impairments. We could not confirm a neuropsychological

profile typical for preterm children, although we did find that performance IQ

was characteristic of profiles including multiple neuropsychological impairments

in preterm children but not in controls. Moreover, we found an inverse relation

between birth weight and the number of neuropsychological domains that were

affected. This phenomenon has not been described before. We speculate that

differences in neuropsychological profiles between preterms and controls may

reflect an altered organization of brain structures in preterm children, presumably

mediated by white matter injury.

The studies described in this thesis were subject to potential limitations. First,

they were single-center studies. Their generalizability to other centers needs to

be established. In addition, due to the limited attention span of the children, we

were unable to test all possible neuropsychological functions that may be affected

by perinatal risk factors leaving out language functions, for example. We chose a

school age test battery that included most of the functions that may be affected by

preterm birth and perinatal brain injury. Finally, in most of the studies, neuroimaging

consisted of cranial ultrasonography in the neonatal period only. Although cranial

ultrasonography is a reliable method for diagnosing most overt brain lesions in the

neonatal period, including PVHI,14,53 we may have missed additional, more subtle

lesions that may also have had an impact on neurodevelopmental outcome.

299Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

Conclusion and Future Perspectives

This thesis provides insight into the question to what extent certain risk factors

encountered during the perinatal period have an impact on functional outcome at

school age. The proposed pathophysiological mechanisms were different for the

various risk factors and they were often complex and multifactorial. Environmental

pollutants mainly interfere with normal brain development by affecting hormone

systems involved in synaptogenesis and myelination of neurons. They led to a mild

effect on long-term outcome within the normal range. In preterm-born children

with systemic disease, neuroinflammation lies at the basis of disrupting normal

development, whilst in preterm-born children with overt types of brain lesions

both local destruction of tissue and diffuse white matter injury may have played

a role in the functional impairments we found at school age. This thesis touches

on the question of the exact pathophysiological mechanisms responsible for the

long-term functional impairments, a question that should be the focus of future

studies.

One question remains: why did some children in our study have more favorable

outcomes at school age than others who had similar clinical diseases in the

neonatal period? We hinted at environmental factors that play a role in functional

development, such as maternal intelligence and socioeconomic status, possibly

contributing to cognitive development in children (Chapter 10). But also other, less

well understood factors such as genetic factors for susceptibility and recovery of

brain disruptions, may be involved.54,55

A better understanding of factors that play a role in the development and recovery

from brain injury could guide future intervention attempts aimed at improving the

long-term outcome of newborn infants suffering from perinatal brain injury. More

recently, potential interventions to improve outcome have become a focus of

research. These include intervention strategies providing neuroprotection, neural

recovery or neurogenesis by means of cerebral plasticity. They can be deployed at

a neurobiological level such as administrating anti-inflammatory and anti-apoptotic

drugs, growth factors, and hypothermia in the early neonatal period following

perinatal brain injury.56-58 Additionally, numerous studies developed a variety of

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

intervention strategies at a functional level. Several aspects of early development

such as motor, cognitive, and behavioral development, as well as mother–infant

interaction, are emphasized depending on the outcomes targeted.59 To date,

early developmental interventions have been unable to show a lasting effect on

motor outcome in preterm-born children. Regarding cognitive development, they

have reported positive short-term effects but these benefits were not persistent

throughout school age.59 Future research should investigate which newborn infants

at risk for adverse outcome will benefit most from intervention strategies, which

aspects of the different programs are most useful, and how these programs can

be implemented cost-effectively.

Based on the findings of this thesis, we advocate that close follow-up of newborn

infants with a complicated neonatal history is all-important. Follow-up should

not only include children who suffered from perinatal brain injury but should also

include children who suffered systemic diseases in the neonatal period. Moreover,

children should be assessed at various ages until school age, since developmental

impairments may not come to light at every assessment (Chapter 10), or may

only come to light by the time children start going to school. The assessment

battery should include several aspects of neurocognitive functioning, for example,

attention and visuomotor skills. Limiting the follow-up program for preterm children

to include only the assessment of general intellectual development may wrongfully

classify a preterm-born child as developing normally. Specific neuropsychological

problems may be missed in so doing (Chapter 11). We therefore recommend that

measures of motor, intellectual, neuropsychological, and behavioral functioning be

included in the follow-up program. Finally, MRI not only during the neonatal period,

but also during follow-up may help to visualize pathophysiological mechanisms

responsible for functional impairments. MRI should, therefore, be incorporated in

neurodevelopmental assessment whenever possible.

Adequate follow-up of newborn infants leads to the timely identification of

functional impairments and in so doing creates the earliest possible opportunity

for intervention.

301Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

1. Roze E, Meijer L, Bakker A, Van Braeckel

KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF. Prenatal exposure

to organohalogens, including brominated flame

retardants, influences motor, cognitive, and

behavioral performance at school age. Environ Health

Perspect 2009;117:1953-1958.

2. Herbstman JB, Sjodin A, Kurzon M, et al. Prenatal

exposure to PBDEs and neurodevelopment. Environ

Health Perspect 2010;118:712-719.

3. Costa LG, Giordano G. Developmental neurotoxicity

of polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) flame

retardants. Neurotoxicology 2007;28:1047-1067.

4. Rahman F, Langford KH, Scrimshaw MD, Lester

JN. Polybrominated diphenyl ether (PBDE) flame

retardants. Sci Total Environ 2001;275:1-17.

5. Gascon M, Vrijheid M, Martinez D, et al. Effects

of pre and postnatal exposure to low levels of

polybromodiphenyl ethers on neurodevelopment and

thyroid hormone levels at 4 years of age. Environ Int

2011;37:605-611.

6. Bernal J. Thyroid hormone receptors in brain

development and function. Nat Clin Pract Endocrinol

Metab 2007;3:249-259.

7. Weisglas-Kuperus N. Neurodevelopmental,

immunological and endocrinological indices of

perinatal human exposure to PCBs and dioxins.

Chemosphere 1998;37:1845-1853.

8. Solomon GM, Schettler T. Environment and health:

6. Endocrine disruption and potential human health

implications. CMAJ 2000;163:1471-1476.

9. Schreiber T, Gassmann K, Gotz C, et al.

Polybrominated diphenyl ethers induce

developmental neurotoxicity in a human in vitro

model: evidence for endocrine disruption. Environ

Health Perspect 2010;118:572-578.

10. Roze E, Kerstjens JM, Maathuis CG, Ter Horst

HJ, Bos AF. Risk Factors for Adverse Outcome in

Preterm Infants With Periventricular Hemorrhagic

Infarction. Pediatrics 2008;122:e46-e52.

11. Bassan H, Feldman HA, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Periventricular hemorrhagic infarction: Risk factors

and neonatal outcome. Pediatr Neurol

2006;35:85-92.

12. Osborn DA, Evans N, Kluckow M. Hemodynamic

and antecedent risk factors of early and late

periventricular/intraventricular hemorrhage in

premature infants. Pediatrics 2003;112:33-39.

13. Thorp JA, Jones PG, Clark RH, Knox E, Peabody

JL. Perinatal factors associated with severe

intracranial hemorrhage. Am J Obstet Gynecol

200;185:859-862.

14. de Vries LS, Roelants-van Rijn AM, Rademaker KJ,

van Haastert I, Beek FJ, Groenendaal F. Unilateral

parenchymal haemorrhagic infarction in the preterm

infant. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2001;5:139-149.

15. Guzzetta F, Shackelford GD, Volpe S, Perlman

JM, Volpe JJ. Periventricular intraparenchymal

echodensisties in the premature newborn: critical

determinant of neurological outcome. Pediatrics

1986;78:995-1006.

References

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

16. Cioni G, Bos AF, Einspieler C, et al. Early

neurological signs in preterm infants with unilateral

intraparenchymal echodensity. Neuropediatrics

2000;31:240-251.

17. Bassan H, Benson CB, Limperopoulos C, et al.

Ultrasonographic features and severity scoring

of periventricular hemorrhagic infarction in

relation to risk factors and outcome. Pediatrics

2006;117:2111-2118.

18. Roze E, Van Braeckel KN, van der Veere CN,

Maathuis CG, Martijn A, Bos AF. Functional

outcome at school age of preterm infants with

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics

2009;123:1493-1500.

19. Brouwer A, Groenendaal F, van Haastert

IL, Rademaker K, Hanlo P, de Vries L.

Neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm infants

with severe intraventricular hemorrhage and therapy

for post-hemorrhagic ventricular dilatation. J Pediatr

2008;152:648-654.

20. Bassan H, Limperopoulos C, Visconti K, et al.

Neurodevelopmental outcome in survivors of

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics

2007;120:785-792.

21. Vollmer B, Roth S, Riley K, et al. Long-term

neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm children

with unilateral cerebral lesions diagnosed by

neonatal ultrasound. Early Hum Dev

2006;82:655-661.

22. Sherlock RL, Anderson PJ, Doyle LW.

Neurodevelopmental sequelae of intraventricular

haemorrhage at 8 years of age in a regional cohort

of ELBW/very preterm infants. Early Hum Dev

2005;81:909-916.

23. Hoekstra RE, Ferrara TB, Couser RJ, Payne

NR, Connett JE. Survival and long-term

neurodevelopmental outcome of extremely

premature infants born at 23-26 weeks’ gestational

age at a tertiary center. Pediatrics 2004;113:e1-e6.

24. Sherlock RL, Synnes AR, Eckstein GR, et al. Long

term outcome after neonatal intraparenchymal

echodensities with porencephaly. Arch Dis Child

Fetal Neonatal Ed 2008;93:F127-F131.

25. Volpe JJ. Neurology of the Newborn. 5th ed.

Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier; 2008.

26. Rennie JM, Boylan GB. Neonatal seizures and their

treatment. Curr Opin Neurol 2003;16:177-181.

27. Thibeault-Eybalin MP, Lortie A, Carmant L. Neonatal

seizures: do they damage the brain? Pediatr Neurol

2009;40:175-180.

28. McBride MC, Laroia N, Guillet R. Electrographic

seizures in neonates correlate with poor

neurodevelopmental outcome. Neurology

2000;55:506-513.

29. Pisani F, Cerminara C, Fusco C, Sisti L. Neonatal

status epilepticus vs recurrent neonatal seizures:

clinical findings and outcome. Neurology

2007;69:2177-2185.

303Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

30. Klinger G, Chin CN, Beyene J, Perlman M.

Predicting the outcome of neonatal bacterial

meningitis. Pediatrics 2000;106:477-482.

31. Toet MC, Groenendaal F, Osredkar D, van Huffelen

AC, de Vries LS. Postneonatal epilepsy following

amplitude-integrated EEG-detected neonatal

seizures. Pediatr Neurol 2005;32:241-247.

32. de Vries LS, Groenendaal F, van Haastert I, Eken

P, Rademaker KJ, Meiners LC. Asymmetrical

myelination of the posterior limb of the internal

capsule in infants with periventricular haemorrhagic

infarction: An early predictor of hemiplegia.

Neuropediatrics 1999;30:314-319.

33. Puig J, Pedraza S, Blasco G, et al. Wallerian

degeneration in the corticospinal tract evaluated

by diffusion tensor imaging correlates with motor

deficit 30 days after middle cerebral artery ischemic

stroke. Am J Neuroradiol 2010;31:1324-1330.

34. Degos V, Favrais G, Kaindl AM, et al. Inflammation

processes in perinatal brain damage. J Neural

Transm 2010;117:1009-1017.

35. Malaeb S, Dammann O. Fetal inflammatory

response and brain injury in the preterm newborn. J

Child Neurol 2009;24:1119-1126.

36. Volpe JJ. Postnatal sepsis, necrotizing entercolitis,

and the critical role of systemic inflammation in

white matter injury in premature infants. J Pediatr

2008;153:160-163.

37. Adams-Chapman I, Stoll BJ. Neonatal infection

and long-term neurodevelopmental outcome in the

preterm infant. Curr Opin Infect Dis

2006;19:290-297.

38. Abbott NJ. Inflammatory mediators and modulation

of blood-brain barrier permeability. Cell Mol

Neurobiol 2000;20:131-147.

39. Yanowitz TD, Potter DM, Bowen A, Baker RW,

Roberts JM. Variability in cerebral oxygen delivery

is reduced in premature neonates exposed to

chorioamnionitis. Pediatr Res 2006;59:299-304.

40. Khwaja O, Volpe JJ. Pathogenesis of cerebral white

matter injury of prematurity. Arch Dis Child Fetal

Neonatal Ed 2008;93:F153-F161.

41. Volpe JJ. Neurobiology of periventricular

leukomalacia in the premature infant. Pediatr Res

2001;50:553-562.

42. Shah DK, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, et al. Adverse

neurodevelopment in preterm infants with postnatal

sepsis or necrotizing enterocolitis is mediated by

white matter abnormalities on magnetic resonance

imaging at term. J Pediatr 2008;153:170-175.

43. Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Hartman RE, Izumi Y, et

al. Early exposure to common anesthetic agents

causes widespread neurodegeneration in the

developing rat brain and persistent learning deficits.

J Neurosci 2003;23:876-882.

44. Yehuda S, Rabinovitz S, Mostofsky DI. Nutritional

deficiencies in learning and cognition. J Pediatr

Gastroenterol Nutr 2006;43:S22-S25.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

45. Aylward GP. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of

infants born prematurely. J Dev Behav Pediatr

2005;26:427-440.

46. Peterson BS, Vohr B, Staib LH, et al. Regional

brain volume abnormalities and long-term cognitive

outcome in preterm infants. JAMA

2000;284:1939-1947.

47. Nosarti C, Al-Asady MH, Frangou S, Stewart AL,

Rifkin L, Murray RM. Adolescents who were born

very preterm have decreased brain volumes. Brain

2002;125:1616-1623.

48. Isaacs EB, Lucas A, Chong WK, et al. Hippocampal

volume and everyday memory in children of very

low birth weight. Pediatr Res 2000;47:713-720.

49. Volpe JJ. Brain injury in premature infants: a

complex amalgam of destructive and developmental

disturbances. Lancet Neurol 2009;8:110-124.

50. Volpe JJ, Kinney HC, Jensen FE, Rosenberg PA.

The developing oligodendrocyte: key cellular target

in brain injury in the premature infant. Int J Dev

Neurosci 2011;29:423-440.

51. Folkerth RD. Neuropathologic substrate of cerebral

palsy. J Child Neurol 2005;20:940-949.

52. Kuban K, Sanocka U, Leviton A, et al. White

matter disorders of prematurity: association with

intraventricular hemorrhage and ventriculomegaly.

The Developmental Epidemiology Network. J

Pediatr 1999;134:539-546.

53. Bass WT, Jones MA, White LE, Montgomery TR,

Aiello F, Karlowicz MG. Ultrasonographic differential

diagnosis and neurodevelopmental outcome of

cerebral white matter lesions in premature infants. J

Perinatol 1999;19:330-336.

54. Harding DR, Dhamrait S, Whitelaw A, Humphries

SE, Marlow N, Montgomery HE. Does interleukin-6

genotype influence cerebral injury or developmental

progress after preterm birth? Pediatrics

2004;114:941-947.

55. Bi B, Salmaso N, Komitova M, et al. Cortical glial

fibrillary acidic protein-positive cells generate

neurons after perinatal hypoxic injury. J Neurosci

2011;31:9205-9221.

56. Johnston MV, Fatemi A, Wilson MA, Northington F.

Treatment advances in neonatal neuroprotection

and neurointensive care. Lancet Neurol

2011;10:372-382.

57. Ma D, Hossain M, Chow A, et al. Xenon and

hypothermia combine to provide neuroprotection

from neonatal asphyxia. Ann Neurol

2005;58:182-193.

58. Carty ML, Wixey JA, Reinebrant HE, Gobe

G, Colditz PB, Buller KM. Ibuprofen inhibits

neuroinflammation and attenuates white matter

damage following hypoxia-ischemia in the immature

rodent brain. Brain Res 2011;1402:9-19.

305Chapter 12 General Discussion and Future Perspectives

59. Orton J, Spittle A, Doyle L, Anderson P, Boyd R. Do

early intervention programmes improve cognitive

and motor outcomes for preterm infants after

discharge? A systematic review. Dev Med Child

Neurol 2009;51:851-859.

Chapter 13

SUMMARY IN ENGLISH

NEDERLANDSE SAMENVATTING

DANKWOORD

ABOUT THE AUTHOR

LIST OF PUBLICATIONS

ABBREVIATIONS

AUTHORS AND AFFILIATIONS

ELISE ROZE

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Summary in English

The main goal of this thesis was to establish the functional outcome of newborn

infants with perinatal risk factors of adverse outcome. The functional outcome

indicates the motor, cognitive, and behavioral skills and abilities that are relevant

for functioning in daily life. To this end, we studied the impact of risk factors ranging

from prenatal exposure to environmental pollutions to neonatal systemic diseases

and overt brain lesions on long-term, school age outcome.

In Part 1 we studied the functional outcome of infants exposed prenatally to

environmental pollutants. We found that transplacental transfer of polybrominated

flame retardants was associated with the development of children at school

age. Brominated flame retardants correlated with worse fine manipulative

abilities, worse attention, better coordination, better visual perception, and better

behavior. Chlorinated organohalogens correlated with less choreiform dyskinesia.

Hydroxylated polychlorinated biphenyls correlated with worse fine manipulative

abilities, better attention, and better visual perception. The wood protective agent

correlated with worse coordination, less sensory integrity, worse attention, and

worse visuomotor integration. Because of the widespread use of these compounds

our findings cause serious concern (Chapter 2).

In Part 2 we determined the functional outcome of newborn infants with brain

injury. We found that in 54 preterm infants born <37 weeks of gestation with

periventricular hemorrhagic infarction (PVHI), mortality occurred in 30%. Of the

survivors, 66% developed cerebral palsy, which was mild in 21 children (Gross

Motor Function Classification System (GMFCS) levels I and II) and moderate to

severe in 4 (levels III and IV). Use of inotropics and maternal intrauterine infection

were independent predictors for mortality (odds ratio (OR) 31.2, 95% confidence

interval (CI) 2.6-373, p<.01 and OR 12.2, 95% CI 1.2-127, p<.05 respectively).

In survivors, only the most extended form of PVHI was associated with the

development of cerebral palsy (OR>4.7, p<.005), but not with severity of cerebral

palsy. Cystic periventricular leukomalacia and concurrent grade III germinal matrix

hemorrhage were associated with more severe cerebral palsy. We concluded from

309Chapter 13

this study that in preterm infants with PVHI, mortality occurred despite optimal

treatment and was associated with circulatory failure and maternal intrauterine

infection. In survivors, motor development was abnormal in 66%, but functional

abilities were good in the majority. Extension and localization of the PVHI were not

clearly related to functional outcome (Chapter 3).

We then studied the functional outcome at school age of preterm infants with

PVHI. Of 38 infants, 15 (39%) died. Twenty-one of 23 survivors were included

in the follow-up. Four were neurologically normal, 1 had minor neurological

dysfunction, 13 had unilateral spastic cerebral palsy (CP), and 3 bilateral CP.

Coordination, associated movements and fine manipulative abilities were affected

most according to the neurological examination. The GMFCS was level I in 7

children, level II in 7 children, level III in one child, and level IV in 2 children. The

Manual Ability Classification System score was normal in 4 children, level I in 8

children, level II in 7 children, and level III in 2 children. The mean and median total

intelligence quotient (IQ) was 83 (range 55-103, standard deviation (SD) 11). Visual

perception was normal in 88%, visuomotor integration was normal in 74% and

verbal memory was normal in 50% of the children. Behavior was normal in 53%

and executive functions were normal in 65% and 29% (parents and teachers).

Characteristics of the PVHI were not related to functional motor outcome and

intelligence. Posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation was a risk factor for poorer

total and performance intelligence and abnormal fine manipulative abilities. We

concluded from this study that the majority of surviving preterm children with

PVHI had CP with limited functional impairment at school age. Intelligence was

within 1 SD of the norm of preterm children without lesions in 60% to 80% of

the children. Verbal memory, in particular, was affected. Behavioral and executive

function problems occurred slightly more than in preterm infants without lesions.

The functional outcome at school age of preterm children with PVHI was better

than previously thought (Chapter 4).

We also studied the outcome of 82 term-born infants with seizures that required

two or more anti-epileptic drugs (AEDs) and determined whether treatment efficacy

and/or the underlying disorder were related to neurological outcome. We found that

47 infants (57%) had status epilepticus. The number of AEDs was not related to

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

neurological outcome. Treatment with three or four AEDs as opposed to two showed

a trend towards an increased risk of a poor outcome, i.e. death or CP, (OR 2.74; 95%

CI 0.98-7.69, p=.055). Failure to achieve seizure control increased the risk of poor

outcome (OR 6.77; 95% CI 1.42-32.82, p=.016). Persistently severely abnormal aEEG

background patterns also increased this risk (OR 3.19; 95% CI 1.90-5.36, p<.001).

In a multivariate model including abnormal aEEG background patterns, failure to

achieve seizure control nearly reached significance towards an increased risk of poor

outcome, (OR 5.72, 95% CI 0.99–32.97, p=.051). We found no association between

seizure aetiology and outcome. We concluded from this study that in term-born

infants with seizures that required two or more AEDs outcome was poorer if seizure

control failed. The number of AEDs required to reach seizure control and seizure

aetiology had limited prognostic value (Chapter 5).

We then describe an advanced magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) technique,

diffusion tensor imaging, to assess diffusion characteristics of the corticospinal

tracts in 20 newborn infants with focal neonatal ischemic brain lesions compared

to 21 healthy term controls. Conventional MRI was able to predict normal motor

development (n=9) or a unilateral spastic cerebral palsy (n=6). In children who

developed a mild motor asymmetry (n=5), conventional MRI predicted a unilateral

spastic cerebral palsy in 2 and normal motor development in 3 infants. The asymmetry

indices for tract volume, fractional anisotropy, apparent diffusion coefficient and

radial diffusivity showed a significant difference between controls and infants who

developed a unilateral spastic cerebral palsy and radial diffusivity also showed

a significant difference in asymmetry index between controls and infants who

developed a mild asymmetry. We concluded from this study that conventional MRI

was able to predict subsequent normal motor development or unilateral spastic

cerebral palsy following focal injury in newborn infants. Measures of radial diffusivity

obtained from diffusion tractography may offer additional information for predicting

a subsequent asymmetry in motor function (Chapter 6).

In Part 3 we determined the functional outcome of preterm and term-born

infants with systemic diseases in the neonatal period and we studied the role of

inflammation on development at school age. First, we determined motor, cognitive,

311Chapter 13

and behavioral outcome at school age of children that had either necrotizing

enterocolitis (NEC) or spontaneous intestinal perforation (SIP). We included 52

of 65 survivors for follow-up. At mean age 9 years we found that 68% of the

children had borderline or abnormal scores on the Movement Assessment Battery

for Children (versus 45% of controls). Their mean total intelligence quotient (IQ)

was 86±14 compared to 97±9 in the controls. Additionally, attention and visual

perception were affected (p<.01 and p=.02). In comparison to controls, surgically

treated children were at highest risk for adverse outcome. In conclusion, we found

that at school age the motor functions and intelligence of many children with

NEC or SIP were borderline or abnormal and, specifically, attention and visual

perception were impaired. We stated that children with NEC or SIP form a specific

risk-group for functional impairments at school age even though the majority did

not have overt brain pathology (Chapter 7).

We then determined the motor, cognitive, and behavioral outcome at school age of

preterm children with late-onset sepsis compared to matched controls. At 6-9 years,

21 of 32 children with late-onset sepsis (68%) had borderline or abnormal motor

outcome with most problems in fine motor skills. Their total IQ was 89 compared

to 98 in controls. In addition, verbal memory and attention were affected more

often compared to controls (0.61 SD, 95% CI 0.04-1.17, p=.033 and 0.94 SD, 95%

CI 0.32-1.62, p=.011, respectively). Multiple episodes of sepsis and gram-negative

sepsis were risk factors for worse cognitive outcome. We concluded that at school

age, a majority of preterm children with late-onset sepsis had motor problems.

Their IQ was considerably lower than matched controls, and memory and attention

were specifically impaired. Outcome at school age of preterm children with late-

onset sepsis was worse than previously thought (Chapter 8).

Finally, we established the outcome at school age of newborn infants with intestinal

obstructions treated surgically during their first days of life. Of 44 children, three (7%)

died. Twenty-seven survivors (66%) were included for follow-up (median gestational

age 36.7 weeks, birth weight 3000 grams). Motor outcome was abnormal (<5th

percentile) in 22% of the children. This was worse than the reference population

(p<.01). Scores on selective attention were abnormal in 15% of the children (p<.01).

Other cognitive functions were not affected. Lower birth weight and intestinal

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

perforation were risk factors for poorer motor outcome (r2=0.53) while intrauterine

growth restriction was a risk factor for poorer selective attention (r2=0.366). We

concluded that children treated surgically for intestinal obstructions in the neonatal

period had an increased risk for poor motor functioning and selective attention at

school age. Low birth weight, IUGR, and intestinal perforation were risk factors for

adverse outcomes. We recommended to closely follow the motor and attentional

development of these children (Chapter 9).

The final part of the thesis focused on developmental processes and the

association between various developmental domains in preterm-born and term-

born children (Part 4). First, we described developmental trajectories of 77 healthy

term-born children until school age. We found that the mean absolute difference

in standardized motor scores over all time points was 1.01 SD. Only the explained

proportions of variance of maternal socioeconomic status (SES) and verbal

intelligence were significant for sustained attention and verbal memory (r2=0.104,

p=.030 and r2=0.074, p=.027 respectively). The children’s scores on early motor

tests added little value for their motor and cognitive development at school age.

We concluded that in healthy children the stability of motor development from

birth until school age is low. Maternal SES and verbal intelligence rather than the

infants’ scores on early motor tests signified added value for complex cognitive

functions at school age (Chapter 10).

In the final chapter we established the neuropsychological profiles at school age

of a cohort of very preterm-born children compared to term-born controls. We

found that at mean 8.8 years, 55% of preterm children had suspect-abnormal

neuropsychological outcomes in multiple (≥2) domains, versus 25% of the controls

(OR 3.67, 95% CI 1.90-7.06). In preterm children with multiple impairments, verbal

memory, attention, and performance IQ were mostly affected. No typical pattern

of co-occurrence of neuropsychological impairments was specific to preterm

children, except for those that included low performance IQ (OR 5.43, 95% CI 1.75-

16.81). The higher the number of suspect-abnormal neuropsychological domains,

the worse the academic achievement (p<.01). We concluded from this study that

a majority of preterm children had multiple neuropsychological impairments. Low

313Chapter 13

performance IQ was characteristic of profiles including multiple neuropsychological

impairments in preterm children but not in controls. We speculated that differences

in neuropsychological profiles between preterms and controls may reflect an

altered organization of brain structures in preterm children (Chapter 11).

The studies as reported in this thesis provided insight in to what extent certain

risk factors from the perinatal period have an impact on functional outcome at

school age. The proposed pathophysiological mechanisms were different for the

various risk factors and they were often complex and multifactorial. Environmental

pollutants mainly interfere with normal brain development by affecting hormone

systems involved in synaptogenesis and myelination of neurons. They led to a mild

effect on long-term outcome within the normal range. In preterm-born children

with systemic disease, neuroinflammation is at the basis of disruption of normal

development whilst in preterm-born children with overt types of brain lesions

both local destruction of tissue and diffuse white matter injury may have played a

role in the functional impairments we found at school age. Based on the findings

of this thesis, we advocate that newborn infants with a complicated neonatal

period should be followed up closely until school age. We recommend to include

measures of motor, intellectual, neuropsychological and behavioral functioning in

a follow-up program. Adequate follow-up of newborn infants could lead to the

early identification of functional impairments so as to identify opportunities for

early intervention.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Nederlandse samenvatting

Het hoofddoel van dit proefschrift was het vaststellen van de functionele

ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd van pasgeboren kinderen met perinatale

risicofactoren voor een gestoorde ontwikkeling. Hierbij gaat het om de motorische,

cognitieve en gedragsmatige ontwikkeling die het functioneren beïnvloeden. Om

dit te onderzoeken hebben we de invloed van verschillende risicofactoren op de

ontwikkeling bepaald, variërend van prenatale blootstelling aan neurotoxische

milieuverontreinigende stoffen tot systemische ziekten en verschillende cerebrale

laesies die op kunnen treden in de neonatale periode.

In Deel 1 van het proefschrift hebben we de functionele uitkomst bestudeerd

van kinderen die prenataal blootgesteld zijn aan vlamvertragers. We vonden dat

de transplacentaire overdracht van gebromeerde vlamvertragers geassocieerd

is met de ontwikkeling van kinderen op schoolleeftijd. Gebromeerde

vlamvertragers correleerden met een slechtere fijne motoriek, een slechtere

aandacht, een betere coördinatie, een betere visuele perceptie en beter gedrag.

Gechloreerde organohalogenen correleerden met minder choreiforme dyskinesie.

Gehydroxyleerde, gechloreerde bifenylen correleerden met een slechtere

fijne motoriek, een betere aandacht en een betere visuele perceptie. De hout

beschermende stof PCP correleerde met een slechtere coördinatie, vermindering

van sensore integriteit, een slechtere aandacht en een slechtere visuomotore

integratie. Deze stoffen worden wereldwijd op grote schaal toegepast. We

concluderen daarom dat deze bevindingen leiden tot zorg (Hoofdstuk 2).

In Deel 2 van het proefschrift hebben we de functionele ontwikkeling van

pasgeboren kinderen met cerebrale laesies onderzocht. Allereerst vonden we

een sterfte van 30% bij prematuur geboren kinderen (zwangerschapsduur <37

weken) met een cerebraal veneus infarct (n=54). Van de overlevers ontwikkelde

66% een cerebrale parese. Deze was mild bij 21 kinderen (Gross Motor Function

Classification System (GMFCS) niveau I en II) en matig tot ernstig bij 4 kinderen

(niveau III en IV). We vonden dat het gebruik van inotropica en de aanwezigheid

van maternale intra-uteriene infectie onafhankelijke voorspellers zijn voor sterfte

315Chapter 13

in deze groep (odds ratio (OR) 31.2, 95% betrouwbaarheidsinterval (BHI) 2.6-373,

p<.01 en OR 12.2, 95% BHI 1.2-127, p<.05 respectievelijk). Bij de overlevers was

alleen de meest uitgebreide vorm van het veneus infarct geassocieerd met de

ontwikkeling van cerebrale parese (OR>4.7, p<.005), maar niet met de ernst ervan.

Cystische periventriculaire leukomalacie en het optreden van een graad III lamina

germinalis bloeding waren wel geassocieerd met een ernstigere vorm van cerebrale

parese. We concluderen naar aanleiding van deze studie dat sterfte bij prematuur

geboren kinderen met een veneus infarct optrad ondanks optimale behandeling

en geassocieerd was met circulatoir falen en maternale intra-uteriene infectie.

Van de overlevers was de motorische ontwikkeling afwijkend bij 66%, hoewel

de functionele mogelijkheden in de meerderheid goed waren. De uitgebreidheid

en lokalisatie van het infarct waren niet duidelijk gerelateerd aan de functionele

ontwikkeling van de kinderen (Hoofdstuk 3).

Vervolgens hebben we de functionele ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd onderzocht

van prematuur geboren kinderen met een veneus infarct. Van 38 kinderen

stierven er 15 (39%). Eenentwintig van de 23 overlevende kinderen werden

geïncludeerd in de follow-up. Vier waren neurologisch normaal, 1 had minor

neurological dysfunction, 13 hadden een unilaterale spastische cerebrale parese

en 3 een bilaterale spastische cerebrale parese. De coördinatie, geassocieerde

bewegingen en fijne motoriek van de kinderen was het meest aangedaan. De

GMFCS score was bij 7 kinderen niveau I, bij 7 kinderen niveau II, bij 1 kind niveau

III en bij 2 kinderen niveau IV. De Manual Ability Classification System score was

bij 4 kinderen normaal, bij 8 kinderen niveau I, bij 7 kinderen niveau II en bij 2

kinderen niveau III. Het gemiddelde en mediane intelligentie quotiënt (IQ) was 83

(range 55-103, standaarddeviatie (SD) 11). De visuele perceptie was normaal bij

88%, de visuomotore integratie was normaal bij 74% en het verbaal geheugen

was normaal bij 50% van de kinderen. Het gedrag was normaal bij 53% en de

executieve functies waren normaal bij 65% en 29% van de kinderen (ouders en

leerkrachten). Karakteristieken van het veneus infarct waren niet gerelateerd aan

de functionele motorische ontwikkeling en de intelligentie. Posthemorrhagische

ventrikel dilatatie was een risicofactor voor een slechter totaal en performaal IQ

en een abnormale fijne motoriek. We concludeerden uit dit onderzoek dat de

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

meerderheid van de overlevende kinderen met een veneus infarct een cerebrale

parese ontwikkelt, hoewel de functionele beperkingen op schoolleeftijd gering zijn.

Bij 60% tot 80% van de kinderen was de intelligentie binnen 1 SD van het IQ

van prematuur geboren kinderen zonder deze cerebrale laesie. Vooral het verbaal

geheugen was aangedaan bij deze groep. Het gedrag en de executieve functies

waren iets vaker aangedaan dan bij prematuren zonder een veneus infarct. De

functionele ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd van prematuur geboren kinderen met

een veneus infarct was beter dan wat voorheen gedacht werd (Hoofdstuk 4).

Daarna hebben we de uitkomst onderzocht van 82 à terme geboren kinderen met

convulsies, die daarvoor 2 of meer anti-epileptica (AEDs) nodig hadden. We hebben

ook onderzocht of de effectiviteit van de behandeling en/of de onderliggende

stoornis gerelateerd zijn aan de neurologische ontwikkeling. Zevenenveertig

kinderen (57%) hadden een status epilepticus. Het aantal verschillende AEDs

dat gebruikt is voor de behandeling van de convulsies was niet gerelateerd aan

de neurologische ontwikkeling. We vonden wel een trend richting een slechtere

uitkomst (sterfte of cerebrale parese) bij kinderen die met 3 of 4 middelen waren

behandeld in vergelijking met kinderen die 2 middelen kregen (OR 2.74; 95% BHI

0.98-7.69, p=.055). Het niet onder controle krijgen van de convulsies gaf ook een

verhoogd risico op een slechte uitkomst (OR 6.77; 95% BHI 1.42-32.82, p=.016).

Persisterende ernstig afwijkende aEEG achtergrond patronen droegen vooral bij

aan een verhoogd risico (OR 3.19; 95% BHI 1.90-5.36, p<.001). In een multivariaat

model waarin afwijkende aEEG achtergrond patronen werden meegenomen, kwam

het niet onder controle krijgen van de convulsies eruit als trend voor een slechtere

uitkomst (OR 5.72, 95% BHI 0.99–32.97, p=.051). Er bestond geen relatie tussen

de oorzaak van de convulsies en de neurologische ontwikkeling. We concluderen

uit dit onderzoek dat de uitkomst van à terme geboren kinderen met convulsies

die daarvoor 2 of meer middelen kregen slechter was als de convulsies niet onder

controle te krijgen waren. Het aantal AEDs die nodig waren om de convulsies onder

controle te krijgen en de oorzaak van de convulsies hadden maar zeer beperkte

prognostische waarde (Hoofdstuk 5).

Tenslotte hebben we een geavanceerde MRI techniek gebruikt (diffusion tensor

imaging) om de diffusie eigenschappen van de corticospinale baan te bestuderen

317Chapter 13

bij 20 pasgeboren kinderen met focale, ischemische neonatale hersenlaesies. We

vergeleken de eigenschappen met die van 21 gezonde, à terme geboren controle

kinderen. Conventionele MRI bleek in staat tot het voorspellen van een normale

motorische ontwikkeling (n=9) of een unilaterale spastische cerebrale parese

(n=6). Van de kinderen die een milde motorische asymmetrie ontwikkelden (n=5),

voorspelde de conventionele MRI een unilaterale spastische cerebrale parese bij

2 kinderen en een normale ontwikkeling bij 3 kinderen. De DTI asymmetrie indices

voor corticospinale baan volume, fractionele anisotropie, gemiddelde diffusie

coëfficiënt en radiale diffusiviteit lieten een significant verschil zien tussen controle

kinderen en kinderen die een unilaterale spastische cerebrale parese hadden

ontwikkeld. Daarnaast liet radiale diffusiviteit ook een significant verschil zien in

asymmetrie index tussen controle kinderen en kinderen met een milde motorische

asymmetrie. We concluderen daarom dat met behulp van conventionele MRI

een goede voorspelling van normale motorische ontwikkeling of een unilaterale

spastische cerebrale parese gegeven kan worden bij pasgeboren kinderen met

focale hersenschade. Door middel van metingen van radiale diffusiviteit verkregen

met DTI kan gedifferentieerd worden tussen kinderen die zich normaal ontwikkelen,

een milde motorische asymmetrie ontwikkelen of een unilaterale spastische

cerebrale parese ontwikkelen (Hoofdstuk 6).

In Deel 3 van het proefschrift hebben we de functionele uitkomst onderzocht van

prematuur en à terme geboren kinderen met systemische ziekten in de neonatale

periode. Eerst hebben we de motorische, cognitieve en gedragsmatige ontwikkeling

op schoolleeftijd onderzocht van kinderen met necrotiserende enterocolitis (NEC)

of een spontane geïsoleerde darmperforatie (SIP). We hebben 52 van de 65

overlevers geïncludeerd voor follow-up. Op een gemiddelde leeftijd van 9 jaar

vonden we dat 68% van de kinderen mild afwijkende of afwijkende scores had

op de Movement Assessment Battery for Children (vergeleken met 45% van de

controles). De gemiddelde intelligentie quotient (IQ) score was 86±14 vergeleken

met 97±9 bij de controles. Daarnaast waren aandacht en visuele perceptie vaker

aangedaan (p<.01 en p=.02). In vergelijking met de controles hadden geopereerde

kinderen het hoogste risico op een gestoorde ontwikkeling. Concluderend vonden

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

we dat de motorische functies en intelligentie van veel kinderen met NEC en SIP

aangedaan waren op schoolleeftijd. Kinderen met NEC en SIP vormen dus een

specifieke risico groep voor functionele beperkingen op schoolleeftijd terwijl de

meerderheid bij cerebrale beeldvorming geen grote cerebrale pathologie had

(Hoofdstuk 7).

Vervolgens hebben we de motorische, cognitieve en gedragsmatige ontwikkeling

op schoolleeftijd onderzocht van kinderen met een late-onset sepsis vergeleken

met gematchte controles. Op een leeftijd van 6-9 jaar hadden 21 van de 32

kinderen met late-onset sepsis (68%) een mild afwijkende of afwijkende motorische

ontwikkeling, met vooral problemen in de fijne motoriek. Het totale IQ was 89

vergeleken met 98 bij de controles. Daarnaast waren het verbaal geheugen en

aandacht ook aangedaan vergeleken met controles (0.61 SD, 95% BHI 0.04-1.17,

p=.033 en 0.94 SD, 95% BHI 0.32-1.62, p=.011 respectievelijk). Meerdere episodes

van sepsis en een gram-negatieve sepsis waren risicofactoren voor een slechtere

cognitieve uitkomst. We concluderen daarom dat op schoolleeftijd de meerderheid

van prematuur geboren kinderen met een late-onset sepsis motorische problemen

had. Het IQ was aanzienlijk lager dan bij gematchte controles en het geheugen en

de aandacht waren specifiek aangedaan. De ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd van

deze kinderen was slechter dan tot nu toe gedacht (Hoofdstuk 8).

Tenslotte hebben we de ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd vastgesteld van

pasgeboren kinderen met darm obstructies die daarvoor geopereerd zijn in de

eerste levensdagen. Van 44 kinderen stierven er 3 (7%). Zevenentwintig overlevers

(66%) werden geïncludeerd in de follow-up (mediane gestatieduur 36.7 weken,

geboortegewicht 3000 gram). De motorische ontwikkeling was afwijkend (<5e

percentiel) bij 22% van de kinderen. Dit was slechter dan in een referentie

populatie (p<.01). De scores op selectieve aandacht waren afwijkend bij 15% van

de kinderen (p<.01). Er waren geen andere cognitieve functies aangedaan. Een

lager geboortegewicht en darm perforatie waren risicofactoren voor een slechtere

motorische uitkomst (r2=0.53) terwijl intra-uteriene groeivertraging een risicofactor

was voor een slechtere selectieve aandacht (r2=0.366). We concluderen dat

kinderen die in de neonatale periode geopereerd zijn aan een darmobstructie

een verhoogd risico hebben op problemen van de motorische ontwikkeling en de

319Chapter 13

aandacht op schoolleeftijd. We bevelen daarom aan deze functies nauwgezet te

vervolgen bij deze kinderen (Hoofdstuk 9).

Het laatste deel van het proefschrift richt zich op ontwikkelingsprocessen en de

associatie tussen verschillende ontwikkelingsdomeinen bij prematuur en à terme

geboren kinderen (Deel 4). Allereerst hebben we de ontwikkelingstrajecten tot op

schoolleeftijd beschreven van 77 gezonde à terme geboren kinderen. We vonden dat

het gemiddelde absolute verschil in gestandaardiseerde motorische scores tussen

alle meetmomenten 1.01 SD bedroeg. Alleen de proportie verklarende variantie

van maternale sociaal economische status (SES) en maternaal verbale intelligentie

droegen significant bij aan volgehouden aandacht en het verbaal geheugen van de

kinderen (respectievelijk r2=0.104, p=.030 and r2=0.074, p=.027). De motorische

scores van de kinderen op eerdere motorische testen droegen weinig bij aan de

voorspelling van hun motorische en cognitieve ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd. We

concluderen dat de stabiliteit in motorische ontwikkeling vanaf de geboorte tot

aan schoolleeftijd bij gezonde kinderen laag is. SES en de verbale intelligentie van

moeder zijn belangrijker dan de scores op motorische en neurologische tests voor

het latere cognitieve functioneren van de kinderen (Hoofdstuk 10).

In het laatste hoofdstuk hebben we de neuropsychologische profielen op

schoolleeftijd onderzocht bij een cohort prematuur geboren kinderen (gestatieduur

≤32 weken), in vergelijking met à terme geboren controles. Op een leeftijd van

8.8 jaar had 55% van de prematuur geboren kinderen een mild afwijkende of

afwijkende neuropsychologische ontwikkeling op meerdere (≥2) domeinen. Dit

was slechts 25% bij de controles (OR 3.67, 95% BHI 1.90-7.06). Bij prematuur

geboren kinderen met meerdere aangedane domeinen, waren vooral het

verbaal geheugen, de aandacht en het performaal IQ aangedaan. We konden bij

prematuren geen typisch patroon aantonen, wat betreft het gelijktijdig voorkomen

van neuropsychologische beperkingen, hoewel het performaal IQ wel vaker in

combinatie met andere domeinen was aangedaan (OR 5.43, 95% BHI 1.75-

16.81). Hoe meer domeinen er mild afwijkend of afwijkend waren, hoe slechter

de schoolprestaties (p<.01). We concluderen daarom dat de meerderheid van

prematuur geboren kinderen beperkingen had in meerdere neuropsychologische

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

domeinen. Een laag performaal IQ was karakteristiek voor profielen van prematuur

geboren kinderen die op meerdere domeinen beperkingen hadden. Dit gold niet

voor controles. We speculeren dat verschillen in neuropsychologische profielen

tussen prematuren en controles verklaard kunnen worden door een veranderde

organisatie van hersenstructuren bij prematuur geboren kinderen (Hoofdstuk 11).

De verschillende studies die in dit proefschrift zijn beschreven geven inzicht in

de mate waarin bepaalde risicofactoren uit de perinatale periode de functionele

ontwikkeling op schoolleeftijd kunnen beïnvloeden. De pathofysiologische

mechanismen zijn verschillend voor de verschillende factoren en zijn vaak

complex en multifactorieel. Milieuverontreinigende stoffen kunnen de normale

hersenontwikkeling beïnvloeden door te interfereren met hormoonsystemen die

een rol spelen bij de synaptogenese en myelinisatie van neuronen. Ze hadden een

mild effect op de lange termijn uitkomst welke binnen de normale range viel. Bij

prematuur geboren kinderen met systemische ziekten ligt neuroinflammatie aan

de basis van verstoring van de normale hersenontwikkeling, terwijl bij prematuur

geboren kinderen met ernstige hersenpathologie zowel lokale destructie van weefsel

als diffuse witte stofschade een rol kan spelen bij de functionele beperkingen op

schoolleeftijd.

Op basis van de bevindingen van deze studies pleiten wij ervoor dat pasgeboren

kinderen met een gecompliceerde periode vervolgd worden in hun ontwikkeling

tot op schoolleeftijd. Zowel de motorische, intellectuele, neuropsychologische als

gedragsmatige ontwikkeling zou vervolgd moeten worden. Adequate follow-up

van pasgeboren kinderen kan leiden tot de vroege identificatie van functionele

beperkingen die mogelijk interventie vereisen.

321Chapter 13

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Dankwoord

Dit proefschrift is het resultaat van een MD/PhD traject binnen de onderzoekslijn

Neonatale Neurologie van de afdeling Neonatologie, Beatrix Kinderziekenhuis,

Universitair Medisch Centrum Groningen. Onderzoek doen doe je niet alleen, dat

leerde ik al snel. Daarom wil ik graag een aantal mensen bedanken die mij geholpen

hebben bij het onderzoek en bijgedragen hebben aan het plezier en enthousiasme

dat ik daarbij had. Mocht ik daarbij iemand vergeten dan is dit niet bewust en niet

persoonlijk bedoeld.

Allereerst wil ik mijn promotor Prof. Dr. A.F. Bos bedanken. Beste Arie, jij was

gedurende mijn promotietraject mijn steun en toeverlaat. Jij valt overduidelijk onder

het kopje ‘de professionele begeleider’, uit het boekje ‘Promoveren, een wegwijzer

voor de beginnend wetenschapper’ dat je me gaf aan het begin van mijn promotie

traject. “De professional heeft altijd tijd voor je, leest stukken snel, voorziet ze

van adequaat commentaar, motiveert je, en combineert de rollen van coach en

beoordelaar op bewonderenswaardige wijze.” Ik heb veel bewondering voor jou als

wetenschapper. Met jouw kritische en wetenschappelijke blik wist je mijn veldwerk

naar een hoger niveau te tillen. Daarnaast heb je mij de ruimte gegeven om eigen

ideeën te ontplooien en heb je mij als jonge onderzoeker geprikkeld tot nadenken.

We hadden vaak op een prettige manier discussies waarbij je mij de ruimte gaf om

mijn (soms kritische) mening te geven. Naast het onderzoek hebben we ook in onze

vrije tijd verschillende dingen ondernomen. Je hebt jouw passie voor klassieke

muziek en opera op me overgedragen doordat je me op congressen meenam

naar opera’s van onder andere Mozart en Verdi. Wij als onderzoekers namen jou

daarop mee naar een opera in het marionetten theater. Een iets andere entourage

maar minstens net zo onderhoudend. Dit waren bijzondere ervaringen voor mij.

Ook in jouw gezin heb ik een warm welkom gekregen. Regelmatig kwam ik samen

met Ewold of Elise pizza’s bij jullie bakken en deden we spelletjes met Judith,

Fionneke en Jasper. Arie, met jou als voorbeeld ben ik gepassioneerd geraakt

voor de zorg voor zieke pasgeboren kinderen. Ik had mij geen betere Doktervater

kunnen wensen.

323Chapter 13

Lieve Elise, jij bent naast een goede vriendin ook een intelligente en gezellige

collega om mee samen te werken en een trouwe paranimf. Wat hebben wij veel

mooie momenten beleefd de afgelopen jaren. We vlogen de hele wereld over om

anderen te vertellen over ons onderzoek en geïnspireerd te raken door de verhalen

van anderen. We deelden een werkkamer en hielden elkaar (en ook de andere

kamergenoten) ieder moment van de dag op de hoogte van de voortgang van ons

onderzoek. Wij werden vaak gekscherend ´de Elises´ genoemd. Soms vonden we

dat vervelend, maar eigenlijk was ik er altijd heel trots op. Jij was altijd bereid een

kritische blik te werpen op een abstract of paper van mijn hand. Samen hielpen

we elkaar waar mogelijk om ons onderzoek te verbeteren en onze persoonlijke

ambities waar te maken. Zo´n vriendschap en collegialiteit is in mijn ogen goud

waard.

Lieve Petra, naast een goede vriendin ben je een fantastische paranimf geweest.

Heel erg bedankt voor al je meedenken en enthousiaste helpen bij de laatste

loodjes rondom het proefschrift en de verdediging. Ook heb ik fijne herinneringen

aan de gezellige lunches en borrels die we samen met collega Kingma en Verhagen

hadden. Dank daarvoor!

Daarnaast heb ik heel plezierig met andere jonge en meer ervaren onderzoekers

samengewerkt. Koen, jij leerde mij de basis van de neuropsychologie en het

testen van diverse neurocognitieve functies. Met jouw Belgische humor en

kritische wetenschappelijke toon wist je mijn onderzoek van nuttig commentaar te

voorzien. Christa van der Veere, jij leerde me het Touwen neurologisch onderzoek

en de brede variabiliteit van de normale ontwikkeling herkennen. Carel Maathuis,

jij leerde me veel over de klinische kenmerken en behandeling van kinderen met

een cerebrale parese. Janneke Bruggink, Henk ter Horst, Jorien Kerstjens en

Lisethe Meijer, de prettige samenwerking die we hadden heeft geleid tot mooie

onderzoeksresultaten. Mijn dank hiervoor.

Prof. Dr. Mijna Hadders-Algra wil ik bedanken voor haar kritische blik op de opzet

en uitwerking van enkele van de onderzoeksprojecten.

Er zijn diverse studenten waar ik mee samen heb gewerkt en waarvan er velen

tevens co-auteur zijn op één van de papers. Attie, Bastiaan, Janyte, Jozien,

Mariska, Meike, en Rachel bedankt voor de prettige samenwerking. Daarnaast

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

gaat mijn dank uit naar de andere neonatologen en fellows voor hun kritische blik

en repliek op mijn onderzoek bij het bespreken van onderzoeksprojecten en het

oefenen van voordrachten tijdens de researchbesprekingen op dinsdagochtend.

Dan waren er verschillende ganggenoten en onderzoekers van de Neonatologie die

gezelligheid brachten in de koffiepauzes: Annemiek, Annemieke, Bertine, Carianne,

Chris Peter, Deirdre, Eryn, Hiltje, Ingrid, José, Karin, Marrit, Martijn, Menno,

Michelle, Nicole, Marieke, Paul, Tina en Tjitske. Ook wil ik enkele secretaresses

en beleidsmedewerkers van het Beatrix Kinderziekenhuis bedanken voor advies,

ondersteuning en interesse in mijn onderzoek: onder andere Janette Tienkamp,

Jannie Tjassing en Aad van Mourik.

During my PhD I went abroad for 4 months to the Hammersmith Hospital in London,

England. I want to thank Dr. S.J.J. Counsell and Dr. F.M. Cowan for giving me the

opportunity to work under their supervision on a challenging and very interesting

research project. Dear Serena, dear Frances, you taught me a lot about MRI and

DTI techniques and made me feel very welcome in your research group. Also I

want to thank Prof. Dr. M.A. Rutherford for the collaboration.

Dear Amy, Gareth, and Polly, it was great sharing an office with you, but particularly

the out of office drinks that we had in the evenings and weekends made my stay

in London very pleasant. Isotta, as my GM sister you were a great colleague and

friend. Finishing my project in London while staying in your penthouse in Notting

Hill was very pleasant. Ash, Guiliana, Jasenka, Joanna, Rodrigo, Vanessa, Veena,

and many others also contributed to the great time that I had in London.

De Junior Scientific Masterclass wil ik bedanken voor de mogelijkheid die ik heb

gekregen om dit onderzoek in de vorm van een MD/PhD traject uit te voeren.

Dr. Titia Brantsma – van Wulfften Palthe wil ik bedanken voor de beoordeling van

mijn artikelen op het Engels en Stephan Eikens voor het ontwerpen van de cover

en de lay-out van het proefschrift.

De leden van de leescommissie, Prof. Dr. P.J.J. Sauer, Prof. Dr. L.S. de Vries en Prof.

Dr. F.J. Walther wil ik bedanken voor hun kundige beoordeling van het proefschrift.

Daarnaast wil ik Prof. Dr. P.J.J. Sauer bedanken voor de prettige samenwerking

die we hebben gehad.

Lieve familie en vrienden, ook al waren jullie niet inhoudelijk betrokken bij mijn

325Chapter 13

onderzoek toch wil ik jullie bedanken. Papa en mama voor het (zelf)vertrouwen

dat jullie me mee hebben gegeven, het relativeringsvermogen en de interesse die

jullie altijd hadden in mijn onderzoek. Joline en Loes, mijn lieve zussen waarmee

ik in mijn vrije tijd veel heb ondernomen om zo even los te komen van studie en

onderzoek.

Lieve Ewold, jouw onvoorwaardelijk liefde en luisterend oor zijn van onschatbare

waarde voor mij geweest. Wij zijn maatjes en stimuleren elkaar om het beste uit

onszelf te halen, zowel in het werk als in ons privéleven. Op de juiste momenten

bracht jij mij een kopje koffie of bracht je me met de auto naar het ziekenhuis als

het weer eens regende. Dat jij mij dan ook nog altijd hielp met het maken van

figuren in de artikelen is dan bijzaak, maar ik ben je er zeer dankbaar voor!

Tenslotte wil ik de ouders en kinderen bedanken die geparticipeerd hebben in het

onderzoek. Zonder hen hadden we dit onderzoek niet kunnen uitvoeren.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

About the author

Elise Roze was born on 12 December 1985 in Kampen, the Netherlands. In 2003 she

graduated from secondary school at the Ichthus College in Kampen. Subsequently,

she successfully completed the first year of a degree program (propedeuse) in

Psychology at the University of Groningen. In 2004 she started studying Medicine

at the University of Groningen. She developed a special interest in Pediatrics and

participated in a research project at the Division of Neonatology of the Beatrix

Children’s Hospital under the supervision of Prof. dr. Arend F. Bos. This project

resulted in an MD/PhD trajectory in 2008, the results of which are presented in this

thesis. An MD/PhD trajectory is a challenging program enabling medical students

to combine studying for a master’s in Medicine with doing research for a PhD thesis

in two years’ extra time. In 2010 Elise went to England for four months to work on

one of her PhD projects under the supervision of Dr. Serena J.J. Counsell and Dr.

Frances M. Cowan of the MRC Clinical Sciences Centre at Hammersmith Hospital

in London. She further developed her skills as a researcher by presenting her work

at national and international conferences, supervising trainees, and participating in

the PhD curriculum of TULIPS (Training Upcoming Leaders in Pediatric Science).

Currently, she is doing her final clinical rotation at the department of Pediatrics of

the Martini Hospital in Groningen. In 2012 she will receive her master’s degree in

Medicine. It is her ambition to become an academic pediatrician and she hopes to

dedicate future research towards improving the neurodevelopmental outcome of

newborn infants at risk and towards understanding the underlying mechanisms of

perinatal brain injury.

327Chapter 13

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Articles

Harris PA, Ball G, Roze E, Merchant N, Arichi T, Porter E, Allsop JM, Edwards AD,

Rutherford MA, Counsell SJ. A 3D diffusion tractography atlas of the healthy infant

brain. 2011; submitted

Tanis JC, van der Ree MH, Roze E, Huis in ’t Veld AEH, van den Berg PP, Van

Braeckel KNJA, Bos AF. Functional outcome at school age of children born preterm

and small for gestational age. 2011; submitted

Holwerda JC, Van Braeckel KNJA, Roze E, Hoving EW, Maathuis CGB, Brouwer

OF, Martijn A, Bos AF. Cognitive, motor and behavioral outcome at school age of

children with posthemorrhagic hydrocephalus. 2011; submitted

Roze E, Bos AF. Neurological and cognitive development of preterm infants up to

school age. 2011; submitted

Roze E, Van Braeckel KNJA, Kerstjens JM, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Neuro-

psychological profiles at school age of very preterm-born children compared to

term-born peers. 2011; submitted

Kaandorp JJ, van Bel F, Veen S, Derks JB, Groenendaal JB, Rijken M, Roze E,

Uniken Venema MMA, Rademaker CMA, Bos AF, Benders MJNL. Long-term

neuroprotective effects of allopurinol after moderate perinatal asphyxia. Follow-up

of two randomised controlled trials. Arch Dis Child 2011; accepted

Roze E, Harris PA, Ball G, Elorza LZ, Braga RM, Allsop JM, Merchant N, Porter

E, Arichi T, Edwards AD, Rutherford MA, Cowan FM, Counsell SJ. Tractography

of the corticospinal tracts in infants with focal perinatal injury: comparison with

normal controls and to motor development. Neuroradiology 2011; accepted

List of publications

329Chapter 13

Bos AF, Roze E. Neurodevelopmental outcome in preterm infants. Dev Med Child

Neurol 2011;53(suppl.4):35-39

Roze E, Ta BDP, van der Ree MH, Tanis JC, Van Braeckel KNJA, Bos AF. Functional

impairments at school age of children with necrotizing enterocolitis or spontaneous

intestinal perforation. Pediatr Res 2011; DOI 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31823279b1

Van der Heide M, Roze E, van der Veere CN, ter Horst HJ, Brouwer OF, Bos AF.

Long-term neurological outcome of term-born children treated with two or more

anti-epileptic drugs in the neonatal period. Early Hum Dev 2011; DOI 10.1016/j.

earlhumdev.2011.06.012

Van der Ree MH, Tanis JC, Van Braeckel KNJA, Bos AF, Roze E. Functional

impairments at school age of preterm born children with late-onset sepsis. Early

Hum Dev 2011; DOI 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.06.008

Ta BPD, Roze E, Bos AF, Rassouli-Kirchmeier R, Hulscher JBF, Long-term

neurodevelopmental impairment in neonates surgically treated for necrotising

enterocolitis: the enterostomy is associated with worse outcome. Eur J Pediatr

Surg 2011;21:58-64.

Roze E, Meijer L, Van Braeckel KNJA, Ruiter SAJ, Bruggink JLM, Bos AF.

Developmental trajectories from birth to school age in healthy term-born children.

Pediatrics 2010;126:e1134-1142.

Roze E, Meijer L, Bakker A, Van Braeckel KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF. Prenatal

exposure to organohalogens, including brominated flame retardants, influences

motor, cognitive, and behavioral performance at school age. Environm Health

Persp 2009;117:1953-1958.

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

Roze E, Van Braeckel KNJA, van der Veere C, Maathuis CGB, Martijn A, Bos

AF. Functional outcome at school age of preterm infants with periventricular

hemorrhagic infarction. Pediatrics 2009;123:1493-1500

Roze E, Kerstjens JM, Maathuis CGB, ter Horst HJ, Bos AF. Risk factors for

adverse outcome in preterm infants with periventricular hemorrhagic infarction.

Pediatrics 2008;122:e46-e52

Other

Eggermont LHP, Roze E. Chapter 3.3. The importance of exercise- Brain and

Cognition. In: The importance of exercise, Cahier of the Foundation for Life

Sciences and Society. 2012; in press

Roze E, Bos AF. Cerebrale Parese. JA! 21st edition, 2010 (Jeugdgezondheidszorg

actueel, tijdschrift voor artsen Jeugdgezondheidszorg)

331Chapter 13

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

ADC apparentdiffusioncoefficient

ADHD AttentionDeficitHyperactivityDisorder

AEDs anti-epileptic drugs

aEEG amplitudeintegratedelectro-encephalography

AI asymmetryindex

AIS arterial ischemic stroke

AVLT AuditoryVerbalLearningTest

BG basalganglia

BPD bronchopulmonarydysplasia

BRIEF BehaviorRatingInventoryofExecutiveFunction

BS burstsuppression

BSID-II-NL BayleyScalesofInfantDevelopment,secondedition,Dutchversion

CBCL ChildBehaviorChecklist

CFM CerebralFunctionMonitor

CI confidenceinterval

CLV continuouslowvoltage

CNV continuousnormalvoltage

CoNS coagulase-negativestaphylococci

CP cerebralpalsy

CST corticospinal tract

DCDQ DevelopmentalCoordinationDisorderQuestionnaire

DNV discontinuousnormalvoltage

DTI diffusion tensor imaging

DWI diffusion weighted images

FA fractionalanisotropy

FT flattrace

FT4 freethyroxin

GA gestational age

GIC Groningen-infant-COMPARE

GMFCS GrossMotorFunctionClassificationSystem

Abbreviations

333Chapter 13

GMH germinal matrix hemorrhage

GMH-IVH germinal matrix haemorrhage-intraventricular haemorrhage

HOME Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment

HPI hemorrhagic parenchymal infarction

IQ intelligence quotient

IUGR intrauterine growth restriction

MACS ManualAbilityClassificationSystem

MCA middle cerebral artery

MDI mental developmental index

MedNEC medically treated necrotizing enterocolitis

Movement-ABC Movement Assessment Battery for Children

MND minor neurological dysfunction

MRI magnetic resonance imaging

NEC necrotizing enterocolitis

NEPSY Neuropsychological Assessment

NICU Neonatal Intensive Care Unit

OH-PCBs hydroxylated polychlorinated biphenyls

OHCs organohalogen compounds

OR odds ratio

PBBs polybrominated biphenyls

PBDEs polybrominated diphenyl ethers

PCBs polychlorinated biphenyls

PCP pentachlorophenol

PDI psychomotor developmental index

PHVD posthemorrhagic ventricular dilatation

PLIC posterior limb of the internal capsule

PVHI periventricular hemorrhagic infarction

PVL periventricular leukomalacia

Q-Q plot quantile-quantile plot

RD radial diffusion

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

ROP retinopathy of prematurity

rT3 reverse triiodothyronin

SD standard deviation

SES socioeconomic status

SI signal intensity

SIP spontaneous intestinal perforation

SNAP Score for Neonatal Acute Physiology

SurgNEC surgically treated necrotizing enterocolitis

T3 triiodothyronin

T4 thyroxin

TBG thyroid binding globulin

TEA term equivalent age

TEA-Ch Test of Everyday Attention for Children

TRF Teacher’s Report Form

TSH thyroid stimulating hormone

TVPS Test of Visual-Perceptual Skills

VMI test of Visual-Motor Integration

WAIS Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale

WISC Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children

WM white matter

WPPSI Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence

WPPSI-R Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, revised

335Chapter 13

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

From the division of Neonatology, Beatrix Children’s Hospital, University

Medical Center Groningen, University of Groningen, the Netherlands

AREND F. BOS, ATTIE BAKKER, KOENRAAD N.J.A. VAN BRAECKEL, JANNEKE

L. M. BRUGGINK, RACHEL M. ELSINGA, MARISKA J. VAN DER HEIDE, HENDRIK

J. TER HORST, JORIEN M. KERSTJENS, MEIKE H. VAN DER REE, ELISE ROZE,

JOZIEN C. TANIS, CHRISTA N. VAN DER VEERE

From the Beatrix Children’s Hospital, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, the Netherlands

LISETHE MEIJER, PIETER J.J. SAUER

From the department of Surgery, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

BASTIAAN D.P. TA, JAN B.F. HULSCHER

From the department of Neurology, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

OEBELE F. BROUWER

From the center for Rehabilitation, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

CAREL G.B. MAATHUIS

From the department of Radiology, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

ALBERT MARTIJN

From the department of Health Sciences, University Medical Center Groningen,

University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands

SIJMEN A. REIJNEVELD

AUTHORS & AFFILIATIONS

337Chapter 13

From the department of Special Needs, Education and Child Care, Faculty

of Behavioral and Social Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, the

Netherlands

SELMA A.J. RUITER

From the centre for the Developing Brain, Imperial College, Hammersmith

Campus, and the Medical Research Council, Clinical Sciences Centre,

Hammersmith Hospital, London, United Kingdom

JOANNA M. ALLSOP, GARETH BALL, RODRIGO M. BRAGA, SERENA J.

COUNSELL, POLLY A. HARRIS

From the division of Neonatology, Imperial College Healthcare, National

Health Service Trust, London, United Kingdom

TOMOKI ARICHI, FRANCES M. COWAN, A. DAVID EDWARDS, NAZAKAT

MERCHANT, EMMA PORTER, MARY A. RUTHERFORD

From the department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, Faculty of

Medicine, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

LEIRE ZUBIAURRE ELORZA

Functional Development at School Age of Newborn Infants at Risk

339Chapter 13