The Knowledge and Misconceptions of Primary and Secondary School Teachers about the Brain and Their...

100
1 The Knowledge and Misconceptions of Primary and Secondary School Teachers about the Brain and Their Perceptions about Neuroscience in Education: A Mixed Methods Research to Analyse the Situation in Turkey in 2013 by Ozge Karakus A dissertation submitted to the University of Bristol in accordance with the requirements of the Degree of Master of Education in the Graduate School of Education September 2013 16.336

Transcript of The Knowledge and Misconceptions of Primary and Secondary School Teachers about the Brain and Their...

1

The Knowledge and Misconceptions of Primary and Secondary School Teachers about

the Brain and Their Perceptions about Neuroscience in Education: A Mixed Methods

Research to Analyse the Situation in Turkey in 2013

by

Ozge Karakus

A dissertation submitted to the University of Bristol

in accordance with the requirements of the Degree of Master of Education

in the Graduate School of Education

September 2013

16.336

2

Synopsis

This study firstly explores primary and secondary school teachers’ knowledge and their

misconceptions about the brain; secondly, the source of their misconceptions; and finally,

teachers’ perceptions about neuroscience in education in Turkey.

Since the birth of educational neuroscience, the gap between neuroscience and education has

tried to be bridged. Thus there have been some researches done in some countries about

teachers’ knowledge about the brain and their views on the usage of neuroscience in

education. However, the field is new and there is no study among teachers about neuroscience

in education in Turkey.

In this dissertation, in order to fill this gap, a mixed methods approach was taken by using a

questionnaire and interviews. The questionnaire was taken from Dekker et al.’s (2012) paper

and consisted of 32 statements. 15 of these were actually misconceptions about the brain. In

total, 278 primary and secondary school teachers completed the questionnaire; with 6 of them

being interviewed.

Findings from the questionnaires showed that the majority of primary and secondary school

teachers had limited knowledge about the brain, and they generally believed in neuromyths.

These findings were compared with Dekker et al.’s research results. Comparisons showed that

the prevalent neuromyths were mostly the same in all three (UK, Netherlands, Turkey)

countries; furthermore, the level of teachers’ believing in these neuromyths was not

significantly different either.

Findings from the interviews conducted revealed that teachers’ personal experiences were

more influential as being the source of their knowledge and misconceptions about the brain.

Finally, the majority of participants (from questionnaires and interviews) believed in

3

neuroscience's being a necessity in education; however, they highlighted the essentialness for

teachers to be trained in neuroscience.

To conclude, by taking into consideration the prevalence of neuromyths, it is suggested that

training is needed in order to prevent pseudo-scientific applications throughout classrooms in

Turkey.

4

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank those who have supported me throughout the research process.

First of all, I would like to acknowledge my research supervisor Dr. Tim Jay for his support

and guidance. He helped me to realise the missing parts of my dissertation by guiding me with

the right questions and advices.

I would also like to thank Dr. Paul Howard-Jones for introducing educational neuroscience to

me; it was a great opportunity to have been able to listen to the field from someone who had

such a wide knowledge-base.

Special thanks are also due to Sengul Apari, Cagil Kaderoglu and Nahide Murt Bakin for

checking the translations of the questionnaires and interviews throughout the entire research

process.

Most of all, I would like to thank my brother Hasan Can for being on my side, whatever the

time or situation. I will always need his support throughout my life, it means a lot to me.

Mum and dad: your standing behind my decisions makes me more strong and motivated.

Thank you both for your trust.

Finally, special thanks are due to my grand mum for her lovely smile and for her supporting

me at all times.

5

Declaration

I declare that the work in this dissertation was carried out in accordance with the regulations

of the University of Bristol. The work is original except where indicated by special reference

in the text, and no part of the dissertation has been submitted for any other degree.

Any views expressed in the dissertation are those of the author and in no way represent those

of the University of Bristol.

The dissertation has not been presented to any other University for examination either in the

United Kingdom or overseas.

Signed ______________________ Date: _____________

6

Content

Page no

Chapter 1 9

Introduction

1.1 Personal and Intellectual Background 9

1.2 Purpose and Significance of the Research 10

1.3 Research Setting 11

1.4 An Outline of the Structure and the Content of the Study 12

Chapter 2 14

Literature Review

2.1 The Historical Development and Roots of Educational Neuroscience 14

2.2 ‘A Bridge Too Far’ 16

2.3 New Insights 19

2.3.1 What is ‘Brain-Based’ Knowledge? 22

2.4 Neuromyths 22

2.4.1 Are They Really ‘Myths’? 22

2.4.2 How are neuromyths generated? 25

2.4.3 How have neuromyths spread so rapidly? 26

2.5 Significance of Teachers and Their Perceptions about Educational Neuroscience on the

Development of the Field 27

2.6 What is the Situation in Turkey? 31

Chapter 3 34

Methodology

3.1 Epistemological Perspective and Methodological Orientation 34

3.2 Research Design 37

3.3 Questionnaire Design (Participants and Procedure) 39

3.4 Interview Design (Participants and Procedure) 41

3.5 Ethical Considerations 42

3.6 Reliability, Validity and Triangulation 44

7

Chapter 4 47

Presentation of Findings and Their Analysis

4.1 Research Questions 47

4.2 Characteristics and The Context of Schools 48

4.3 Descriptive Statistics about Participants 49

4.4 Findings from Questionnaires 50

4.5 Findings from Interviews 54

Chapter 5 69

Summary and Conclusion

5.1 Summary 69

5.2 Summary of the Findings 70

5.3 Limitations of the Study 71

5.4 Conclusion 72

5.5 Future Research 73

References 74

APPENDIX-1 (Questionnaire) 83

APPENDIX-2 (Results of Pilot Study) 86

APPENDIX-3 (Interview Questions) 88

APPENDIX-4 (Consent Form for Questinnaire) 90

APPENDIX-5 (Consent Form for Interviews) 91

APPENDIX-6 (An Example Transcript) 92

APPENDIX-7 (Table 4.4) 97

APPENDIX-8 (Table 4.5) 98

APPENDIX-9 (Table 4.6) 99

8

Page no

List of Figures

2.1 Educational theories of human development and learning 15

before educational neuroscience

2.2.a Interdisciplinarity and Multidisciplinarity 18

2.2.b The Evolution of Transdisciplinarity 18

3.1 A Framework for Research Design 35

List of tables

4.1 Descriptive Statistics of Participants for Questionnaires 49

4.2 Participants Took Part in Interviews 50

4.3 Total Percentages - from 32 statements - of Correct (C), Incorrect (I) 51

and Do not know (DK) Answers’ of Primary and Secondary School

Teachers from Turkey

4.4 17 General Statements about Brain and Brain Functions with 97

Teacher Answers by Percentages (from Turkey)

4.5 15 Neuromyths about Brain and Brain Functions on Learning 98

with Teachers’ Responses by Percentages

4.6 15 Neuromyths about Brain and Brain Functions about Learning 99

with Teacher Responses from Turkey, UK and Netherlands by percentages

4.7 Teacher Responses about Brain-based Learning Approaches 54

4.8 Some Characteristics of Teachers 55

9

Chapter 1

Introduction

The research area of this study is in neuroscience and education in general. But, more

specifically:

primary and secondary school teachers’ knowledge and misconceptions about the

brain,

the potential source of teachers’ misconceptions,

and their perceptions and views about neuroscience in education

were all investigated among a sample in Turkey.

The aims and significance of the study, the research design, as well as an outline of the

structure and a brief presentation of the chapters will be given in this Introduction. However,

first of all, I shall briefly explain my personal and intellectual background.

1.1 Personal and Intellectual Background

Although I have worked as a counselling psychologist for 4.5 years and given seminars to

students, parents and teachers about effective teaching or learning, I had never considered the

functioning of the brain on behaviours or learning until the "Brain, Mind and Education" unit

which I took in the first semester of this year.

While I had been reading on educational neuroscience, I realised the influence of commercial,

unscientific or unproven applications in education. This reminded me of some baseless

allegations without references or pseudo-scientific explanations about ‘education’ in some

popular science magazines. Besides, when I was a counselling psychologist, I came across

10

many private certificate training courses about behaviour-modification and, according to my

experiences and observations, I can attest to the ineffectiveness of these courses both for

students and teachers. Furthermore, during counselling sessions with parents about their

children, I often had to correct their groundless ‘hearsay’ knowledge about children’s

education which they had taken from the internet or from mass media. Therefore, I decided to

investigate about the misconceptions about the brain and its functions on learning

(neuromyths). Since teachers have a significant role in spreading knowledge in educational

fields -mainly because they communicate with other teachers, parents, administrators and

students-, I decided to focus this study on teachers in particular.

1.2 Purpose and Significance of the Research

The field of educational neuroscience has been emerging rapidly since the 1990s. However,

there have been many discussions about the gap between neuroscience and education since

the birth of the field. Although, there are pessimistic views about the application of

neuroscientific findings to education, the field is gaining ground due to the improvement of

brain-imaging techniques and the efforts of many optimistic neuroscientists, psychologists

and educators.

Teachers’ understanding and perceptions of the field are significant because they are the ones

who use the scientific findings practically in their classrooms. Thus, teachers’ knowledge and

misconceptions about the brain and their views about educational neuroscience have been

investigated for more than a decade in some countries, such as in the UK, the US, Brazil, the

Netherlands, Greece, etc.

On the other hand, educational neuroscience is completely new in Turkey. There is no

research about teachers’ views and knowledge, or about their misconceptions about brain,

11

although baseless allegations are quite prevalent because of the improvement of mass media

in the world. Therefore, the primary aim of the research is to determine the knowledge of

primary and secondary school teachers with regards to the brain and, more specifically,

identifying their beliefs with regards to neuromyths; I shall focus on neuromyths because they

can waste one's time, effort and money. Secondly, specifying the source of neuromyths is

targeted in order to better understand the ways teachers receive their information in that

regard. Finally, as teachers play an important role in education, their perceptions and needs

could generate a basis for grounding the field in Turkey as well.

1.3 Research Setting

14 schools were visited in 2 cities in Turkey: viz., Istanbul and Mersin. In total, 320

questionnaires were distributed as hand-outs; a total of 281 teachers returned the

questionnaires. 3 of these questionnaires, however, were thrown out due to those participants

not answering all of the questions that they were asked. In the end, 278 participants (124

primary and 154 secondary school teachers) participated in this research.

Data from the questionnaires was analysed and the most prevalent neuromyths were identified

for the purpose of the interview questions; 6 of the teachers who filled out the questionnaire

were interviewed.

Detailed explanations about the questionnaire and interviews will be given in Chapter 3

(Methodology).

12

1.4 An Outline of the Structure and the Content of the Study

This dissertation is organised into five main chapters:

The Introduction identifies the area of investigation and particularly examines the ground of

the present study. Moreover, I tried to express my personal and intellectual background in

order to explain the motivation and relevance of the research from my own perspective. This

chapter is also important in that it allows one understand the purpose and significance of the

research is in general.

The literature review (Chapter 2) examines the literature with regards to the topic. It will

attempt to draw a background for educational neuroscience by explaining the historical

development process and the formation process of neuromyths. The gap between

neuroscience and education causes the propagation of neuromyths and, consequently,

neuromyths obstruct bridging "the gap". Therefore, I will particularly mention the generation

of neuromyths, their rapid spreading in many countries, and the justification of their being

called ‘myths’. Finally, the significant role of teachers on the improvement of the field and on

the prevention of neuromyths will be stated with the previous research findings in order to

better examine the situation in Turkey.

In the methodology chapter (Chapter 3), I will justify the epistemological perspective and

methodological orientation of the research. This study is based on a mixed methods research.

Since the field is completely new in Turkey, I used both quantitative and qualitative methods

in order to reach a broader understanding. Furthermore, this chapter includes ethical

considerations and reliability-validity-triangulation issues.

The presentation of the findings and analysis of this study (Chapter 4) involves the presenting

the research questions and connecting them with the research aims, which are presented in

this chapter at the beginning. The context of the schools that participated in this study and the

13

characteristics of the participants in terms of age, sex, branch and education level will be

given; then, the representations and analyses of the findings from the questionnaires and

interviews regarding the research questions will be given.

The Conclusion involves a summary of the literature review with its key concepts and a

summary of the research findings; furthermore, the limitations of the study will be presented,

before a brief conclusion about why educational neuroscience is important and why it should

be improved in Turkey and why teachers’ role is crucial during this improvement process.

14

Chapter 2

Literature Review

Educational neuroscience is a new and rapidly growing research field which brings biology,

cognitive science, developmental science and education together for the purpose of studying

how genetics and brain processes affect learning and teaching (Fischer, Goswami and Geake,

2010). Therefore, it is extremely challenging to summarise the literature of this field as it is

quite interdisciplinary and multifaceted. Nevertheless, this literature review will focus on the

relevant parts which are connected with the topic and aims of the present study.

In this chapter, first the historical development and roots of educational neuroscience will be

mentioned because it is important to generate an understanding about the aims and usefulness

of this field. After that, neuromyths will be defined and explained; moreover, the sources of

those neuromyths will be discussed since it shall be particularly relevant to the aims of this

research. The importance of “teachers” and their perceptions about educational neuroscience

and neuromyths will be explained according to previous researches about teachers’

knowledge and perceptions on educational neuroscience. Finally, the situation in Turkey will

be examined before moving on to the methodology chapter.

2.1 The Historical Development and Roots of Educational Neuroscience

The questions behind why children behave as they do or how children learn have been tried to

understood through an educational perspective for many years, and answers were given in

different ways with different theories, from psychology, sociology, biology to cognition

(Edgar, 2012). It was claimed that the theories from behaviourist and cognitive levels were

dominant before, according to the history and development of learning theories (Volckmar,

15

2011). There is a model below (Figure 2.1), which was built by Nina Volckmar (2011)

through Howard-Jones`s (2011) `level of actions` model, which explains the interactions of

educational research and learning theories in terms of brain-mind-behaviour perspective

before the birth of educational neuroscience.

Figure 2.1: Educational Theories of Human Development and Learning before Educational

Neuroscience

Individual Society

The decade between 1990 and 1999 was declared the Decade of the Brain in the US (Sousa,

2010). This provided rapid developments and researches about the mind, the brain and

education. By taking into consideration previous efforts and explanations on learning, it can

be claimed that the birth of educational neuroscience as a discipline has made a significant

contribution to the field since the 1990s. This can be seen as being propelled by a realisation

about the truth of the functioning of the brain on learning. Because the brain is counted as the

`organ of learning` and since learning is one of the most significant goals of education,

scientists made an inference that brain research might add valuable knowledge to promote

Individual

Constructivism

(Piaget) Socio-

Constructivism

(Vygotsky)

Contextual

Variables

Behaviour

(Scientifically

observable facts) Behaviouristic

Approaches

Mind

(Cognitive Models)

Brain

16

education and that education, in turn, could have something valuable to add to brain research

(Coch and Ansari, 2012).

An important leader in educational neuroscience was the Organization of Economic Co-

Operation and Development (OECD), during that time, which has organised three

international conferences in New York (2000), Granada, Spain (2001) and Tokyo, Japan

(2001) (Ferrari and McBride, 2011). The OECD also held a seven year project from 1999 to

2006 (Learning Science and Brain Research Project) about educational neuroscience in order

to highlight the importance of brain research in education.

Likewise, since the 1990s, there has been growing interest and research (Draganski et al.,

2004; Maguire et al., 2003; Stewart et al., 2003) regarding the role of the brain on learning.

Furthermore, neuroscientific findings have been informing the field of education on brain

development (Sowell et al., 1999). Some examples will be given later in this chapter.

There have been some concerns, however, regarding policymakers’ misinterpreting the

science; though, if that is the case, educational neuroscience could be doing more harm than

good (Hirsh-Pasek and Bruer, 2007).

2.2 ‘A Bridge Too Far’

Almost two decades ago, Bruer (1997), who was one of the strongest critics against

educational neuroscience, argued with regards to the relevance of neuroscience to education.

He concluded, however, that the potential bridge between neuroscience and education is “a

bridge too far”.

This pessimism still continues because transfer of knowledge from neuroscience to education

or from education to neuroscience -i.e. this is a two-way interaction- was not always clear or

17

obvious (Blakemore and Frith, 2005; Goswami, 2009; Coch and Ansari, 2012). The gap

between the two disciplines regarded their differences in vision, philosophy, aims,

methodology, research tools, etc., thereby hindering the improvement of the field. This issue

explains why the OECD report stated that:

‘A neuroscientific perspective on learning adds a new important dimension to the study of

learning in education, and educational knowledge could help direct neuroscience research

towards more relevant areas. Because both fields are well-developed, however, they have

deeply-rooted disciplinary cultures with field-specific methods and language which make it

extremely difficult for experts from one field to use the knowledge from the other (OECD,

2007: 132-133).’

As can be discerned from these explanations regarding bridge issues, educational

neuroscience involves more than one discipline; it combines the knowledge of a variety of

different fields. During efforts made about bridging the gap, several definitions with regards

this combination have been formed. For instance, Szucs and Goswami (2007: 114) define the

term as ‘the combination of cognitive neuroscience and behavioural methods to investigate

the development of mental representations’ (multidisciplinary); while Fischer et al. (2007: 1)

highlighted the term `integration`—‘the integration of diverse disciplines that investigate

human learning and development; to bring together education, biology and cognitive science

to form the new field of mind, brain and education’ (interdisciplinary)—in their definition.

On the other hand, transdisciplinarity—i.e. the creation of a new field—has been

recommended in order to connect the work on learning ‘across intellectual walls dividing

disciplines' (OECD, 2007: 133).

According to Campbell (2011), transdisciplinarity can provide new philosophical frameworks

and research methodologies, thereby bridging the gap between the two different fields.

Samuels’s (2009) proposal for a collaborative framework is also transdisciplinarity, he

believes that the knowledge yielded from educational neuroscience research would be bigger

18

than the overall knowledge from each field. Figure 2.2.a and Figure 2.2.b show three

perspectives separately:

Figure 2.2.a: Interdisciplinarity and Multidisciplinarity (OECD, 2007: 134)

Figure 2.2.b: The Evolution of Transdisciplinarity (Source: Tokuhama-Espinosa (2011) based

on Hideaki Koizumi (1999) and Boba Samuel’s (2009) concepts of transdisciplinary studies.)

DISCIPLINE 1

DISCIPLINE 3 DISCIPLINE 2

Multidisciplinarity

Inter-disciplinarity

19

Although a transdisciplinary perspective could help to overcome the bridge issue, the

complexity of the relations in education is another difficulty. This is because social and

cultural factors of learning cannot be excluded from the educational context (Schumacher,

2007), not to mention the fact that the science of learning could be prescriptive and

straightforward without taking into account any other factors such as society, culture,

environment or politics (Anderson and Della-Sala, 2012: 4). Relatedly, according to Howard-

Jones (2008), two individuals` interacting during the learning process makes the process more

complex; thus, the brain-mind-behaviour relationship should be taken into consideration with

social communication and interaction together in the educational context.

Despite all of these issues and challenges, from a scientific point-of-view, Campbell (2011)

believes that, under any circumstances, either the multidisciplinary, interdisciplinary or

transdisciplinary way of practising will support the future of education. This is because, if

new roads for experimental design and collaboration can be provided, the significant potential

of educational neuroscience would inform educational philosophy and theory; furthermore, it

has the power of understanding the human condition in order to make a contribution to

education. The "new insights" of educational neuroscience will thus be examined in the next

section.

2.3 New Insights

Educational neuroscience research findings have been already offered new insights related to

brain plasticity (brain plasticity continues throughout life), brain development (from

childhood to adulthood), the importance of memory in learning, the significant role of

working memory in learning new mental processes, etc. (Howard-Jones, 2010). There are

many findings which can be used in classrooms practically. For instance, some findings

20

compiled from Sousa (2010; 2011), Howard-Jones (2010) and OECD`s report (2007) about

neuroscience and education are given below:

'Before it was thought that only infant brains are plastic' (OECD, 2007: 42). However,

neuroscientific findings have shown that the brain retains its plasticity throughout its

lifespan. 'And because plasticity underlies learning, we can learn at any stage of life’

(OECD, 2007: 42).

Movement improves memory and learning (Sibley and Etnier, 2003; Winter et al.,

2006). Many studies have affirmed the positive relationship between physical activity

and school achievement; exercise promotes the growing of developing brains and

prevents the regression of older brains (Zervas et al., 1991; Jensen, 1998; Blakemore,

2003).

Sleep is important for memory because memories are consolidated during sleep

(Marshall and Born, 2007; Capellini et al., 2009; Maquet et al., 2000; Potkin and

Bunney, 2012). 'Regular and sufficient sleep is thus essential for the brain to learn

efficiently' (Howard Jones, 2007: 11).

The regular consumption of caffeinated-drinks or foods (especially cola and chocolate

consumption among children) affects cognitive function negatively. There are

researches which show the long-term effects of regular caffeine consumption, such as

slowing hippocampus-dependent learning and impairing long-term memory (Han et

al., 2007). Caffeine blocks adenosine action, which increases during wakefulness and

declines during sleep; that is why a lack of caffeine may cause fatigue and slowed

thinking if an individual starts to consume regularly (Rogers, 2007).

'When we observe others carrying out actions, some of the same cortical regions are

activated as if we were carrying the actions ourselves' (Howard Jones, 2009: 24).

These are defined as mirror neurons, and they enable us to understand, interpret and

21

learn from other people’s actions and behaviours; this is called imitation learning

(Rizzolatti and Craighero, 2004). This mechanism in humans can show the

significance of teacher behaviours on learning (Schober and Sabitzer, 2013).

Recent discoveries on the structural changes of the brain of a child or an adolescent

(Gogtay et al., 2004; Galvan et al., 2006) can inform teachers about their behaviours.

For instance, an unbalanced developmental process between the socio-emotional

system and the cognitive control system in the brain can explain the suboptimal

choices (risky behaviours) of adolescents (Eshel et al., 2007; Casey et al., 2008;

Steinberg, 2010). Such studies help one to understand a potentially risky and

sometimes even dangerous term of adolescents’ development (Howard Jones, 2010).

As evaluated by some examples, neuroscience helps to prove some previous knowledge about

education and offering new insights. However, according to Howard-Jones (2010), because of

the popularity of this emerging field, not only scientific-base knowledge, but also incorrect

`brain-based` knowledge (pseudo-science) was constituted through neuroscientific findings.

Public interest as well as the popularity of neuroscience has caused some to use unscientific

and uncorroborated `brain-based` knowledge as a marketing strategy, thereby causing the

occurrence of neuromyths in education. Ansari and Coch (2006: 147) mentioned with relation

to this issue that:

'Although there is growing body of peer-reviewed literature and websites that provides clear

and accurate summaries of progress in the cognitive neuroscience of learning, there are at

the same time questionable media reports and numerous other claims about "brain-based

learning" that, in our opinion, often oversimplify, misrepresent, and allow for neuromyths to

flourish.'

22

2.3.1 What is ‘Brain-Based’ Knowledge?

‘The term ‘brain-based’ has been associated with learning theories, principles and products

that posit a link to the brain’s structure or function. (Sylvan and Christodoulou, 2010: 1)’

According to the definition above, there is no harm done of brain-based knowledge or

products to education. Having regard to the uncontrolled global information-network and the

commercialization of education, however, ‘pseudo brain-based’ knowledge, products,

commercials, books, popular magazine articles or etc. become popular and widespread in the

world.

Because this is an integral element of this dissertation, however, the next two sections will

examine in-depth the literature with relation to brain-based knowledge and neuromyths.

2.4 Neuromyths

The OECD’s Brain and Learning project (1999-2006) drew international attention to the

destructive effects of misconceptions, misunderstandings and misusages of scientific facts and

findings of brain research in education (OECD, 2002: 69; Dekker et al., 2012). The

organisation defined "neuromyths" as 'a misconception generated by a misunderstanding, a

misreading or a misquoting of facts scientifically established (by brain research) to make case

for use of brain research in education and in other contexts' (OECD, 2002). Furthermore,

according to Beauchamp and Beauchamps` (2012) literature review, the first problem in

educational neuroscience is seen as being the occurrence of “neuromyths” by scientists in the

field.

2.4.1 Are They Really ‘Myths’?

Since the emergence of neuroscience in education during the early 1990s, commercial ‘brain-

based’ applications and `myths` have increased correspondingly (Howard Jones, 2007). They

23

often have developed without any neuroscientific study. There are few examples about so

called brain-based programmes and neuromyths which shall be examined below:

The first regards the "Brain gym". The'Brain gym promotes the idea that neural mechanism

can be influenced by specific physical exercises' (Howard Jones, 2007: 15). However, even

the terms used are pseudo-scientific, since they are not recognisable in neuroscience and there

is no scientific evidence to prove that concept (Howard Jones, 2007).

Another prevalent ‘brain-based’ programme is learning styles. learning styles [visual, audial

or kinaesthetic (VAK)], are claimed to be based on a valid scientific finding: thus, ‘visual,

auditory and kinaesthetic information is processed in different parts of the brain’; however,

there is some additional information such that ‘these separate structures in the brain are

highly interconnected and there is profound cross-modal activation and transfer of

information between sensory modalities’ (Gilmore et al., 2007). Moreover, researches have

shown that children do not understand more effectively when they are taught with their

preferred learning styles (Coeffield et al.’ 2004); thus, scientists have concluded that those

attempts are wasted-efforts (Kratzig and Arbuthnott, 2006). Therefore, more than one style

should be used for an effective teaching-learning process.

Multiple intelligences theory (Gardner, 1983) and practices have been prevalent and have

been used in classrooms by means of curriculums. However, there is no valid empirical

evidence to support this theory's claims (Allix, 2000; Sternberg and Grigorenko, 2004;

Waterhouse, 2006); besides, cognitive researches have asserted that 'the human brain is

unlikely to function via Gardner’s multiple intelligences because it is unlikely that that each

of Gardner’s intelligences could operate via a different set of neural mechanisms’

(Waterhouse, 2006: 213).

24

As mentioned before, there are prevalent neuromyths on education which have been identified

by the OECD:

That ‘[w]e use 10% of our brain’ is one of the most persistent neuromyths according

to OECD (2002). 'There is absolutely no scientific evidence to suggest that we only

use 10 or any other specific or limited percentage of our brains. On the

contrary, neuroscience findings show that the brain is 100% active' (OECD, 2007:

113).

‘There are critical periods when certain things must be taught and learnt’ is another

common myth. There is a common acceptance in the neuroscience community that

human learning depends on changes in the patterns of connectivity between neurons

(Howard-Jones, 2008: 363). For a long time it was believed that the number of

neurons declines with age. But, with the advent of new brain imaging techniques,

many studies have shown that new neurons occur at any time in a person’s life and

that the brain remains plastic throughout its lifespan (OECD, 2007). Therefore,

learning is possible at any age. It is true that there are some sensitive periods for

learning some certain things. For instance, grammar learning could be an example of

sensitive-period learning because, according to some researches, ‘grammar is learned

faster and easier up to approximately age 16' (OECD, 2007: 111); this does not mean,

however, that grammar cannot be learned after age 16.

The myth that ‘enriched environments [enhance] the brain’s capacity for learning’

occurred first from a scientific research done on rats by Falkenberg et al. (1992).

'Researchers found that neurons in rats, which were raised in an enriched

environment, had formed more connections, i.e. synapses and expressed more proteins

associated with the maintenance of synaptic contacts' (Goswami, 2004: 11); however,

25

according to Goswami (ibid), further research is needed in order to apply this

inference to human learning.

As can be seen from the above examples, the term "neuromyths" is completely valid seeing as

there exist scientific explanations which explain why they are called "myths". Although some

explanations were required in order to critically analyse the sources above, there is a need to

investigate and understand the source of these neuromyths so as to better prevent against their

promulgation as well, especially because neuromyths hamper the development of educational

neuroscience, not to mention the fact that they waste time, effort and money (OECD, 2002).

Indeed, one of the reasons behind why some scientists hesitate when it comes to using

neuroscientific findings about education is because of the existence of these neuromyths in

general (Varma et al., 2008). Thus, I shall next turn to how neuromyths themselves are

generated.

2.4.2 How are neuromyths generated?

Pasquinelli (2012) claims that neuromyths can be generated in three ways:

1. Distortions of scientific facts; e.g. the ‘individual differences can be explained by

hemispheric dominance’ myth is a product of research on hemispheric specialisation

and dominance (Geake, 2008).

2. Neuromyths can be the offspring of scientific hypotheses which have been accepted as

truth before being debunked. For instance, even though the so called Mozart Effect—

which related to Mozart music’s being able to increase a child's IQ scores (Rauscher,

Shaw and Ky, 1993)—has not been replicated, people currently still believe it.

3. Neuromyths can be developed from the misinterpretations of experimental results. For

instance, the research finding that ‘visual, auditory and kinaesthetic (VAK)

information [is] processed in different parts of the brain’ caused the generation of the

26

neuromyth with regards to learning styles which asserts that child learning can be

improved through children’s being classified into different learning styles and being

taught according to those different styles.

Next, we shall examine how neuromyths can spread so rapidly.

2.4.3 How have neuromyths spread so rapidly?

The reason behind the rapid spread of neuromyths is firstly because of neuroscience’s being

alluring to so many different sciences—such as economy, marketing, aesthetics, theology,

law, anthropology and politics—; there is a general optimism that neuroscience's scientific

manner could solve what has not yet been solved and what may be seen as being `chronic`

problems (Anderson and Della Salla, 2012). In other words, knowledge about the brain seems

much more `compelling`, real, scientific and `persuasive`. For instance, it was indicated that

the representation of brain images in an article is more persuasive than other data

representations for readers (Sylvan and Christodoulou, 2010).

Secondly, the gap which causes the existence of neuromyths has also caused the prevalence of

neuromyths at the same time. According to Goswami (2006), the problem about the gap arises

from the miscommunication of neuroscientists with teachers. He claimed that most teachers

expect to be told what works and prefer practical knowledge; conversely, neuroscientists

study neurons and brain functions through brain imaging techniques and make contributions

through what they find from the brain images; furthermore, they are not good at

communication with the society in general.

Because of this same reason—i.e. the different needs and perspectives of neuroscientists and

teachers—, some pseudo-sciences have been created faster than educational neuroscience

knowledge and applications. These pseudo-sciences do not consult experts from neuroscience

or education and do not conduct experiments and, unfortunately, are transformed into easily

27

understandable recipes for teachers to practise in their classrooms. This is why teachers`

perceptions and attitudes on educational neuroscience are so important in terms of the

development of the field—because teachers are the ones that will end up promulgating

knowledge to their students. Therefore, the sooner they are informed about true educational

neuroscience, the better.

2.5 Significance of Teachers and Their Perceptions about Educational Neuroscience on

the Development of the Field

Teaching is a serious and crucial action in terms of the development and maintenance of the

well-being of human beings (Fischer et al., 2010) and the change in brain structure. Sausa

(2011) nicknames teachers ‘brain changers’, which could explain the significance of brain

knowledge on the process of teaching-learning.

As the field of neuroscience in education evolves and brain-based educational products are

developed, it will become important for teachers to be critical consumers; this is because there

is a hunger for `brain-based` knowledge among teachers (Goswami, 2006). Hirsh-Paserk and

Bruer (2007) observed in the ‘Human Early Education and Brain Development Conference’

in Chile-Santiago that 'myths about brain-based pedagogy dominated participants’ (ministers,

educators, scientists) thinking'.

Moreover, it is possible to find a cornucopia of information on brain-based learning on

websites and in popular science magazines. Particularly, teachers` have shown an interest in

learning some practical techniques to use in classrooms and expect to get through some of

their educational problems (Alferink and Farmer-Dougan, 2010). According to Hirsh-Paserk

and Bruer (2007), educational literature is now stocked with books and articles on brain-based

learning practices and a market has grown for brain-based toys, not to mention brain-based

28

consultants visiting school districts. For instance, at a conference which was held by the

Centre for Neuroscience in Education at the University of Cambridge, teachers` reported that

they receive more than 70 e-mails in a year, encouraging them to join courses or training

programmes about brain-based learning (Goswami, 2006).

These courses claim that they offer some `practical` techniques, such as:

- Learning styles: this relates to distinguishing children in terms of their learning styles

(VAK), as well as suggested labelling for children for a successful teaching-learning

process;

- Education via identifying children`s hemispheric dominance suggested that teachers

should find out whether their children are left- or right-brained learners so as to

understand their children’s strengths and weaknesses in terms of their neural ability

(Goswami, 2006);

- In terms of the brain gym: courses assert that enhancing learning is possible through

some body movements; thus, they suggest making accessible all brain areas for better

learning.

These efforts claim to be based on scientific findings about the brain, as mentioned before. On

the contrary, they are based on conclusions that are not supported by existing data and there is

no evidence to show whether or not they are accurate (Alferink and Farmer-Dougan, 2010).

The influence of these neuromyths has been seen as problematic because they cause a waste

of money, time and effort (Dekker et al., 2012). For instance, after the pseudo-scientific

hypothesis regarding the ‘Mozart Effect’ was published, the governor of the State of Georgia

in the US asked for $105.000 for the production and distribution of classical music to new-

borns (Pasquinelli, 2012). That could be one reason why educators’ belief about brain-based

29

approaches is significant; though, as has been shown, those approaches can bring more harm

than benefit.

The commercialisation of these approaches causes these programmes to spread around the

world (Dekker et al., 2012). Goswami (2006) underlines the danger the spreading speed of

these programmes and courses; thus, he insists on the urgency of preventions against the

brain-based industry of almost a decade ago. He claims that informing teachers is important to

stop misapplications in practice. More currently, Paul A. Howard-Jones (as cited in Sparks,

2012) said `in the absence of legitimate neuroscience in education, a neuro-mythology has

arisen in schools`. This statement shows there is still a necessity for preventions.

According to Goswami (2006), progress towards prevention could be made possible by

collaborating with teachers; however, this collaboration should not only be about highlighting

the misuse of scientific findings, but also there is a need to inform teachers about true and

applicable neuroscientific findings, especially since teacher quality is quite significant for a

successful teaching-learning environment. Ansari and Coch (2006) also made a crucial

conclusion that collaboration between neuroscience and education needs to start with teacher

training.

There has been a significant effort to introduce the field to teachers in recent years. The

OECD’s Brain and Learning Project especially drew international attention to neuromyths in

2002 (Hook and Farah, 2012).

It could be a good starting point for the purpose of collaborating and developing the field to

ask teachers their thoughts about educational neuroscience. It is possible to see the

improvement of the field by looking at the researches done and the articles and books written.

There is a huge potential from the neuroscientific side. The problem with this, however, is

firstly that the deficiency in the knowledge that teachers have and their unfounded perceptions

30

about the field; and secondly, is that there still exists insufficient communication between

neuroscientists and educators.

For instance, researches have been started with regards to the perceptions of teachers on the

usage of neuroscientific findings in education in some counties, such as the UK (Pickering

and Jones, 2007; Howard-Jones, Franey, Mashmoushi and Liao, 2009), the Netherlands (a

comparative study with teachers in the UK) (Dekker et al., 2012), Brazil (Bartoszeck and

Bartoszeck, 2012) and the USA (Serpati and Loughan, 2012).

Briefly, the results from all of the above researches were similar. Teachers’ knowledge on

neuroscience was low, despite their interest in educational neuroscience and its findings. For

instance, the majority of participants in the UK were enthusiastic about the knowledge of

brain in educational activities (Pickering and Jones, 2007); a large majority of teachers (94%)

in the US agreed that the neurological underpinnings of learning, cognition and behaviour are

significant information to know in terms of their professions’ efficiency (Serpati and

Loughan, 2012); furthermore, more than 80% (n=163) of Brazilian teachers believed that

better knowledge about educational neuroscience could enhance their teaching abilities and

increase their students’ learning potentials (Bartoszeck and Bartoszeck, 2012).

However, besides this general aura of enthusiasm, the results clearly show that their

knowledge about the brain was quite insufficient. Findings from the comparative study

between the UK and the Netherlands show that teachers agreed with 49% of the neuromyths

which were identified (Dekker, Lee, Howard-Jones and Jolles, 2012) and there was no

significant difference between these countries. Moreover, it was determined in another study

in the UK that most participants (trainee teachers) already believed a range of neuromyths;

thus, the researchers concluded that the brain-based industry had been successful in UK

schools (Howard-Jones, Franey, Mashmoushi and Liao, 2009).

31

Considering these results, it is clear that there is a general enthusiasm for brain research

findings among teachers, which is one of the key issues for bridging the gap, even though

there is a lack of knowledge about brain at the same time.

As mentioned above, there are considerable efforts to develop the field in some countries. The

importance of the field and the hazards of the brain-based learning industry are beyond

argument. Thus, ‘How can the field be improved?’ and ‘How can the bridge between

neuroscience and education be built?’ could be the questions that one needs to answer. We

need to find out the ways to bridge this gap and to use neuroscience more effectively in

classrooms. Thus, the development of this field is needed in all countries, particularly where

the brain-based learning industry has had an effect on education, such as in Turkey. Thus, it

shall be to the situation in Turkey with regards to educational neuroscience that I will now

focus my attention.

2.6 What is the Situation in Turkey?

Unfortunately, there is great enthusiasm for brain-based learning practices (pseudo-science) in

Turkey. It is possible to find a considerable number of websites and articles related to brain-

based learning in the popular science area. Also, courses and training programmes can be

found for teachers which claim to improve one's knowledge about the brain and to teach some

practical and helpful techniques which can be used in classrooms.

On the other hand, there have been minimal efforts in educational neuroscience in academia.

These articles include: “Brain and learning" (Keles and Cepni: 2006) and “Research in

neuroscience and music education: towards knowledge transfer possibilities" (Akyildiz,

2010). However, there have not been any studies about teachers’ knowledge, perceptions or

training.

32

Teacher training is quite important for a successful teaching-learning environment. As Ansari

and Coch (2006) emphasise, teacher education is one of the most effective mechanisms for

improving neuroscience literacy in education. However, the last revision with regards to

teacher education in Turkey was made in 1997 (Cakiroglu and Cakiroglu, 2003; Yuksel,

2012). Thus, in considering the recent developments in Turkey when compared to the

educational research done around the globe and the new findings about education or changing

needs, cultures, behaviours, understandings; the absence of new insights with regards to

teacher training could be a problem in terms of Turkey's developing a successful education

system. For instance, neuromyths and brain-based literature are quite prevalent in the world

because, in recent years, due to new technologies and mass media, knowledge is available for

all, through network access. Nevertheless, there is no awareness about this subject in Turkey.

New developments were made in this regard with the membership negotiations with the

European Union (EU) that it has had since 2009 (Ozmusul, 2012); there has not been any

significant change, however, about teacher education at universities—something which is

pivotal for the standardisation of teacher education. Besides, there exists a concern and

criticism about the effectiveness of decisions and trainings since some academics have found

the developments with the EU membership negotiations unrealistic in terms of how different

the Turkish education system's culture, conditions and infrastructure is compared to European

standards. How realistic are the decisions made by international organizations and the

European Union with regards to education? To what extent can they fit the culture and

conditions of Turkey?

Briefly,

There is a great amount of pseudo-scientific information on the internet and in popular

science magazines; however, there do not exist any preventions regarding

33

neuroscience in education in terms of teachers’ knowledge about both the brain and

neuromyths.

The improvements made by the Turkish Ministry of National Education are connected

with EU policies; however, there are still concerns about the reality and effectiveness

of the improvements in terms of culture and conditions.

In line with these significant points, the progress of educational neuroscience in Turkey has

two stages in order to pass. Firstly, teacher education should be revised in terms of

neuroscience; and secondly, culture and conditions should be taken into consideration when

putting decisions into practice. The aim of the present research is to examine how to establish

a framework for educational neuroscience in Turkey. Moreover, the literature can be

expanded by adding new evidences and results about teachers’ believing neuromyths from

different countries.

34

Chapter 3

Methodology

As Wilfred Carr states:

'Research always conveys a commitment to philosophical beliefs even if this is unintended and

even though it remains implicit and unacknowledged. Researchers cannot evade the

responsibility for critically examining and justifying the philosophical ideas that their

enquiries incorporate. It follows that philosophical reflection and argumentation are central

features of the methods and procedures of educational research' (Carr, 1995: 1)

Briefly, it can be concluded that a study's research methodology reflects the philosophical

position of the researcher and that this philosophical position also determines which research

methodology will be used in the study. The present chapter shall:

Present an understanding about the epistemology of research methodology and a

rationale for the research methods used in this study;

Provide an outline of the research design and describe the methods of the research and

its analysis;

Examine and analyse the ethical issues of the research;

Discuss issues regarding reliability, validity and triangulation.

3.1 Epistemological Perspective and Methodological Orientation

According to Morrison (2007) research enquiry is full of uncertainties and challenges. Thus,

we need to make connections between epistemology and methodology to determine whether

our conclusion is “right” or not. There is a strong, steady and essential link between

epistemology, methodology and method in a research.

35

Epistemology is about the nature and scope of knowledge; moreover it is central for research.

It is important for a researcher to determine and indicate how he/she understands knowledge

about the world (Morrison, 2007: 18); hence, we can say that methodology is affected by

epistemology.

Scott and Morrison (2006:153) specified two questions to show the content of one's

methodology—i.e. they explain "how" the researcher obtains the knowledge as well as "why"

it was obtained. Moreover, they underlined the significance of the "why" question in that it

describes the rationale, or reasons, behind the researcher's using certain methods in order to

collect data. Put briefly, methodology represents a rationale to show the ways and reasons of

the research's techniques. In this respect, when considering the scope of methodology,

Morrison (2007: 19) underlined that 'methodology is much more than techniques or methods

or tools for a research'.

Figure-3.1: A Framework for Research Design (Cresswell, 2003: 5)

Figure 3.1 shows the elements and process of a research design. As can be seen from the

figure, there are three different approaches for research. I have used both quantitative and

qualitative methods (i.e. a mixed methods approach) to research.

Elements of Inquiry

Alternative Knowledge Claims

Approaches to Research Design Processes

Strategies of Inquiry Qualitative of Research

Quantitative Questions, theoretical

Methods Mixed Methods lens, data collection,

Conceptualised by data analysis, write-up

the researcher validation

36

Historically and scientifically, there is a distinct difference between qualitative and

quantitative methods in terms of their epistemology and methodology. Those differences

arose from the positivism-idealism debate of the late 19th

century. At a general level, the

fundamental distinction derives from the question “what is to count as knowledge?” (Smith,

1983). On the one hand, positivists claim that knowledge should be observable and

measurable and that the observer should be neutral to the subject; relativists, on the other

hand, point out that a researcher, as a human being, cannot stand outside the subject and its

world. We are socially bound up with one another; moreover, facts are human interpretations

of reality, which means that they could change according to culture or time (Walliman and

Buckler, 2008).

From the methodological perspective, quantitative research has been affected by positivism

and the application of the scientific method; thus, from this perspective, statistical techniques

could guarantee the quality of the research (Morrison, 2007: 33). Qualitative research, on the

other hand, has been affected by interpretivism in that qualitative researchers concentrate on

detailed observation by trying to make rich and deep descriptions; in other words, they pay

attention to words rather than to numbers (Morrison, 2007: 27).

There has been a debate—or a competition, as it were—between these two philosophical

perspectives. More recently, however, arguments about combining these two approaches have

been supported as being ‘a third methodological movement’ (Morrison, 2007: 28). It is called

the ‘mixed methods research’ approach, which is basically defined as 'the class of research

where the researcher mixes or combines quantitative and qualitative research techniques,

methods, approaches, concepts or language into a single study' (Johnson and Onwuegbuzie,

2004: 17). From this point-of-view, Johnson and Onwuegbuzie believe that, by combining

qualitative and quantitative methods, mixed methods research and its philosophy provide

complete comprehension of the study as well as a ‘workable solution’. The fundamental

37

principle of mixed research is that it considers the strengths and weaknesses of quantitative

and qualitative research, thereby providing a general evaluation (Johnson and Turner, 2003);

further, Tashakkori and Teddlie (1998) claim that the mixed methods approach produces the

most “effectiveness”. More briefly and conspicuously, Johnson and Onwuegbuzie (2004)

asserted that ‘mixed methods research is a research paradigm whose time has come’.

Mixed methods research is situated in the pragmatist paradigm. Pragmatists believe that

qualitative and quantitative researches both have their advantages and disadvantages; needs,

possibilities and contingencies should be evaluated in order to decide which method will be

more appropriate. Furthermore, they claim that the mixed methods approach is more practical

and offers the opportunity to combine methods; in this way, researchers can find applicable

answers for many of their research questions. They define the mixed methods approach as

being like a bridge between methodologies while also providing for a more pluralistic and

compatibilist approach (Cohen, Manion and Morrison, 2011).

In this study, while using the quantitative methods approach (in the form of a questionnaire)

provides a broad perspective about primary and secondary school teachers’ knowledge about

the brain, as well as about the situation regarding neuromyths in Turkey; the qualitative

methods approach (in the form of interviewing a select number of those who answered the

questionnaire) helps us to find out more in detail about the sources of neuromyths, as well as

teachers’ perspectives with regards the use of neuroscience in education.

3.2 Research Design

Vaus (2001: 8) uses an analogy to explain the significance of research design; 'when

constructing a building there is no point ordering materials or setting critical dates for

completion of project stages until we know what sort of building is being constructed'. He

38

continues, saying that, similarly, we cannot start a social research project until we have a

research plan with which to follow. There is an indisputable need for a research design to

obtain the knowledge that we need. A research design involves the issues and explanations

about sampling, method of data collection, data analysing, etc., and all of these points are

closely related with the research questions. By designing a research, a researcher determines

the way the research questions will be answered. In this way, one should try to be as

unambiguous as possible during the research process (Vaus, 2001: 16).

In the present study, the research design mostly depended on the previous researches done

about teachers’ knowledge and perceptions about educational neuroscience in different

countries. Educational neuroscience, however, is a completely new and undiscovered field in

Turkey. Although there are little efforts as mentioned (see Chapter 2, 2.6), there is no research

which examines the broad perspective about the situation of the field in terms of its general

situation or in regards to the neuromyths believed by Turkish teachers. Therefore, it was

firstly a necessity to review the literature with regards to a teachers’ role, knowledge or

perceptions about neuroscience in education in the world. Reviewing the literature helped me

to find the right way for reaching the knowledge more clearly at the beginning because

previous perspectives and experiences provided me with a broader view by noticing the

limitations and results, not to mention comparing those results would enable a standpoint

regarding the situation about neuromyths in practice among teachers in Turkey.

Having outlined the epistemological and methodological perspectives, the process of deciding

the research design of the study and the content of the research design will now be outlined in

two parts: viz. the questionnaire design and the interview design.

39

3.3 Questionnaire Design (Participants and Procedure)

As mentioned before, the mixed methods research was used. For the first part of the study a

questionnaire (see Appendix 1) was conducted. It was used in Dekker et al.’s (2012) paper

before and included 32 statements about the brain and its influence on learning. Among these

32 statements were 15 statements which were defined and accepted as educational

neuromyths by the OECD (2002) such as “we only use 10% of our brains”. The other 17

statements were general scientific information about the brain such as “learning occurs

through modification of the brains’ neural connections”. The participants could answer these

statements by choosing “correct”, “incorrect” or “do not know”.

The questionnaire was personally translated into Turkish. The translation was checked by two

English teachers from Turkey—who were experts in both languages—,as well as one PhD

student from the social sciences in Turkey—whose education language was English—and one

research assistant at Ankara University. Besides this, a pilot study was conducted online

among primary and secondary school teachers in order to see the feedbacks and to correct the

unclear parts of the questions or statements if there were any. 38 teachers participated in the

pilot study (for the results of pilot study see Appendix 2). Many of them responded that they

could understood the questionnaire easily and that they did not find any problematic points. 6

of them gave feedbacks about some ‘unclear’ parts in the statements. The questionnaire was

revised according to these feedbacks. Some parts were changed (whereas the meanings did

not change) in order to prepare more coherent statements and questions.

The participants of the questionnaire were also asked some additional questions—such as age,

sex, level of education (either undergraduate, Masters or PhD) as well as the branch (either

primary or secondary school teacher)—for background information. Moreover, there were 11

questions which were based on a likert scale. They ranked about whether they: read either

scientific or popular scientific articles; or attended conferences, in-service training courses or

40

private certificate courses, so as to be able to investigate the source of neuromyths. Also, they

ranked their interests and their confidences about neuroscience in education. Furthermore,

they indicated whether neuroscience in education is significant or not and whether they need

an in-service training course about the brain and its effect on learning or not. Finally, they

were asked if they heard about multiple intelligences, learning styles or brain gyms.

Data was collected between 2 and 20 April, 2013. Schools were visited individually and

questionnaires were distributed as hand-outs. In total, 320 questionnaires were distributed and

281 participants returned the questionnaires; however, 3 questionnaires were thrown out due

to invalid answering issues. Thus, in the end, 278 participants (124 primary and 154

secondary school teachers) participated in the research. Data was collected from 14 different

primary and secondary schools from two different cities—8 schools in Istanbul and 6 schools

in Mersin. Descriptive statistics about the participants will be given in Chapter 4 (Findings) in

detail.

The data was analysed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version

19.0 for Windows. For all analysis, a statistical threshold of α = 0.05 was used. Percentages

were calculated; independent t-tests were used to compare teachers’ (from Turkey) knowledge

in terms of sex, branch, age and educational level. Regression analysis was performed to

examine the potential predictors of neuromyths (i.e. age, sex or reading popular science

magazines). Results were compared with the results from the UK and the Netherlands in

Dekker et al.’s (2012) paper.

There are some advantages and disadvantages to using questionnaires. According to Cohen et

al. (2000), questionnaires are more economic in terms of time and it is possible to reach many

participants by using questionnaires; moreover, the anonymity provides more reliability due

to honesty issues. On the other hand, misunderstandings and misinterpretations cannot be

41

fixed, and some participants may fill inattentively and thus cannot be understood by the

researcher. Data from interviews provided more in-depth responses and prevented the

misunderstandings. The biggest benefit of mixed methodology research is that the two

different methods’ completing each other in terms of their advantages and disadvantages.

3.4 Interview Design (Participants and Procedure)

For the second part of the study, the participants—i.e. those who filled out the

questionnaire—consisted of 3 primary and 3 secondary school teachers (3 male, 3 female)

(n=6). Data was collected from two different cities (4 participants from Mersin, 2 participants

from Istanbul) as well. A semi-structured interview (for interview questions see Appendix 3)

was used.

The first part of the interview questions were created according to the questionnaire results. 7

statements regarding the neuromyths which were most believed by participants who attended

the first part of the study, were utilised in order to take in-depth responses so as to reach the

sources of the participants’' misconceptions. In the second part of interviews, questions about

their previous knowledge about educational neuroscience (did they hear about them before

and where?); their knowledge on some misconceptions, such as multiple intelligences,

learning styles and brain gyms; and their perception and interest with regards the use of

neuroscientific findings in education were asked.

After the data was collected from all interviews, they were first transcribed. The transcripts

were subsequently read. For each question, a table was organised in order to depict the

answers from the 6 participants all together; in other words, answers were collected separately

for each question. Then, the data was coded with respect to common and salient themes.

While analysing the data, firstly, the general context was explained for each question and then

42

different comments, examples or explanations from each participant were inserted into the

Findings chapter (i.e. Chapter 4).

3.5 Ethical Considerations

Ethical considerations can change according to the researcher’s disciplinary, ideological or

socio-cultural background (Busher and James, 2007); however, I considered the ethical codes

which were included in the ethical form of the University of Bristol which we had to have

filled in before beginning the research; moreover, I also adhered to the codes which have been

developed by The British Psychological Society (2009) throughout the entire research

process. Ethical considerations will be explained according to the items below:

1. Information given to participants / Debriefing / Informed consent

- Voluntariness

- Anonymity

- Confidentiality

- Participant's right of withdrawal

- Safety and well-being of participants/ researchers

2. Data analysis

3. Data Protection

The British Psychological Society (2009) states that researchers should inform participants

about everything which might affect their attendance. Thus, informed consent forms, which

had detailed information, were prepared for both the questionnaire (Appendix 4) and the

interview (Appendix 5). They were prepared in English and then translated into Turkish. All

43

essential information that should have been given to the participants was clearly indicated in

the consent forms.

In the consent forms, firstly, the content and the aim of the research were explained; also,

brief instructions about the structure of the questionnaire and interview were briefly provided.

Then, their rights as participants were stated. Voluntariness and their right to withdrawal were

highlighted; the anonymity and confidentiality of the data obtained were guaranteed.

Additionally, interviewees were informed about the recording process—i.e. that the

interviews were taped—as well as the transcription process—i.e. that two languages were

used during the data collection process and that, thus, the transcription of the interviews were

required in order to be checked by experts in the two languages for approval.

While distributing the questionnaires, person-to-person, voluntariness and their right to

withdraw were underlined. During the interviews, additional explanations were made about

the aim of the research and the information in consent form was repeated, such as

confidentiality and anonymity. Having a little conversation before starting provided to create

a mutual-trust environment and to make the participants more relaxed. By means of those

additional explanations, the safety and well-being of participants were tried to be assured.

Data analysis is another significant issue for ethical procedures. It was thought that, since

analysing the data from the interviews could be required, a translation check for approval by

an expert in both languages became necessary. Thus, transcriptions (all parts which were used

in the Findings section) were check by an English teacher from Turkey.

Finally, data storage and protection were also other important confidentiality issues; thus, all

data has been encrypted in my laptop and I have put all questionnaires in a safe in my room in

the UK.

Now I shall mention issues about reliability, validity and triangulation.

44

3.6 Reliability, Validity and Triangulation

Bush (2007: 102) highlighted that ‘there is no perfect truth’ according to Lincoln and

Denzin’s (1998) claim, which is that ‘research could always have been better grounded, the

subjects more representative, the researcher more knowledgeable, the research instruments

better formulated, and so on..’

However, a focus on reliability, validity and triangulation could satisfy both researchers and

readers (Bush, 2007). In other words, the authenticity of an educational research (quantitative

or qualitative) can be evaluated through procedures used which also address the reliability,

validity and triangulation issues.

With simple definitions: reliability shows the consistency of data, meaning that it analyses

whether the same results will be obtained if the same procedure is followed (Walliman and

Buckler, 2008); while validity addresses the accuracy of the results, illustrating the

representativeness of the data (Walliman & Buckler, 2008); and triangulation is the

application of several methods to compare many sources of evidence in order to check the

accuracy of the information (Gray, 2009).

Bush (2007) pointed out that reliability and validity are two fundamental issues to provide

authenticity, and triangulation is essentially important for the purpose of cross-checking data

in order to ensure validity.

However, these three terms are not interdependent. As Robson (2002: 101) indicates,

reliability can be provided without validity; further, Bush (2007: 102) notes that reliability

may be achieved by decreasing validity; thus, triangulation is significant in that it is certain

with regards to validity and authenticity in general.

45

The reliability of the quantitative data was checked through semi-structured interviewing.

This was done by utilising 7 statements from the questionnaire which were used to get

detailed explanations from participants about the origins in their believing in neuromyths.

Bush (2007: 94-96), on the other hand, claims that semi-structured interviews may limit the

extent for reliability while increasing validity. There are two types of validity: internal

validity and external validity (Bush, 2007: 98). According to Cohen and Manion (1994: 99),

even if participants do not fill the questionnaire accurately, validity can be checked by

interviewing. Internal validity was checked with methodological triangulation as well.

Briefly, triangulation provides the balance between reliability and validity. Mixed methods

research, similar to data triangulation (using more than one data) and methodological

triangulation (combining qualitative and quantitative research), were used in this research in

order to ensure the enhancement on reliability and validity (Gray, 2009).

Lastly, external validity refers to the generalisability of the research. According to Yin (as

cited in Briggs and Coleman, 2007), the generalisability problem can be overcome by

replicating the research in other, similar setting. The questionnaire used for the present

research was taken from Dekker et al.’s (2012) paper. They also conducted a similar research

in the UK and the Netherlands among teachers. Although there were differences—i.e. their

participants were teachers who were interested in educational neuroscience—the settings were

similar.

On the other hand, although survey results may contribute to greater confidence in the

generalisability of results from a positivist perspective (Jick, 1979); it is neither possible nor

necessary to generalise the results from an interpretivist perspective since they reject to

generalise the results as an aim (Schofield, 2002: 173) and claim that every topic has its own

meaning, structure and sense of order.

46

This study is based on the mixed methods research approach. The aim of this study involves

making contributions and interpretations about the knowledge and misconceptions of primary

and secondary school teachers in Turkey; however, no claims are made for generalisability.

47

Chapter 4

Presentation of Findings and Their Analysis

This chapter involves the representation and analysis of findings from this study's

questionnaires and interviews. All the data collected will be analysed according to the

research questions. This chapter will present:

Research questions;

Characteristics and the context of the schools;

Descriptive statistics about participants;

Research findings from quantitative data (questionnaires) and their analysis;

Research findings from qualitative data (interviews) and their analysis.

4.1 Research Questions

As highlighted before, the purpose of this research was appropriate for the mixed methods

research approach because neuroscience in education is a completely new field in Turkey;

thus, I tried to find both statistical answers and detailed, in-depth answers, as far as possible,

to my research questions. I tried to analyse the situation in terms of teachers’ general

knowledge about the brain and their beliefs regarding neuromyths, at the same time trying to

compare the results with the results from different countries, such as the UK and the

Netherlands. In light of this information, my research questions are:

1. What is the primary and secondary school teachers’ general knowledge about the brain

in Turkey? and What is the difference between the UK, the Netherlands and Turkey in

terms of teachers’ knowledge about the brain?

48

2. What is the situation of neuromyths in Turkey? And what is the difference between

the UK, the Netherlands and Turkey in terms of teachers’ believing in such

neuromyths?

3. What are primary and secondary school teachers’ sources of knowledge about

neuromyths?

4. What do teachers’ think about the usage of neuroscience in education? Do they find it

necessary to use neuroscientific findings in education?

4.2 Characteristics and The Context of Schools

Questionnaires were conducted at 14 different schools in 2 cities, viz. Mersin and Istanbul. 6

people who completed the first part (questionnaires) of the research were chosen from 2

schools (one in Istanbul, one in Mersin). As a part of research ethics, research participants’

confidentiality and anonymity were assured; thus, in order to ensure this, school names will

not be used.

There is no data about the representativeness of cities in terms of education, schools or

teachers; however, Istanbul is the most crowded and mixed city in terms of the structure of the

population. Although Mersin is much smaller and less crowded than Istanbul, the diversity of

its population is similar with that of Istanbul due to its migration-receiving potential (Kleff,

2007).

The features of these schools are also significant in terms of the representativeness of the

general population because the physical infrastructure of the schools—which is dependent on

the socio-economic status of the region and its students—determines the conditions of the

schools which affect the quality of education, in Turkey. Public education spending as a share

of gross domestic product (GDP) was 3.8% in 2010 in Turkey, which is averagely 5.8%

49

among OECD countries and which should be 6% in developing countries according to a

UNESCO report (www.unicef.org.tr). Moreover, the overpopulation of formal education

students in Turkey should be taken into account for a more realistic evaluation. 8 (4 of them

in Mersin, 5 of them in Istanbul) schools were located in low socio-economical regions while

5 (2 of them in Mersin, 3 of them in Istanbul) were located in middle and high socio-

economical regions. Both quantitative and qualitative data were collected from both types of

schools (i.e. with relation to their socio-economical region).

4.3 Descriptive Statistics about Participants

The research participants were primary and secondary school teachers. 278 teachers, whose

ages ranged between 23 and 64, participated in the study. The average age of the participants

was 36. 51.8% of the participants were female and 48.2% were male. Table 4.1 below shows

the descriptive statistics of the participants in percentages in terms of the branch of education

they work in (primary or secondary school teacher), their education level (university, Masters

or PhD), and their gender.

Table 4.1: Descriptive statistics of participants for questionnaire.

Female (Overall percentage: 51.8%) Male (Overall percentage: 48.2%)

Primary school Secondary school Primary school Secondary school

Uni. Masters PhD Uni. Masters PhD Uni. Masters PhD Uni. Masters PhD

98.2% 1.8% - 87.5% 22.5% - 100% - - 81% 16% 3%

For the second part of study, there were 6 interviewees whose experience level for teaching

changed between 1.5 to 21 years. Table 4.2 shows the features of participants.

50

Table 4.2: Participants who took part in interviews.

Female (n=3) Male (n=3)

Primary school Secondary school Primary school Secondary school

1 2 2 1

4.4 Findings from Questionnaires

The findings of research questions 1 and 2 will be presented in this section. Research

questions 1 and 2 will draw upon the survey, while the 3rd

and 4th

questions will be answered

through both the questionnaire results and interview findings.

Also it should be noted that, the aim of this research is not for the purpose of comparing

teachers from Turkey with teachers’ from the UK or the Netherlands; however, comparing

these results could give a more clear idea and provide a background with which to interpret

the findings. Therefore, the research's results will be compared with previous findings.

Research question 1: What is the primary and secondary school teachers’ general knowledge

about the brain in Turkey? And what is the difference between the UK, the Netherlands and

Turkey in terms of teachers’ general knowledge about the brain?

First of all, ‘correct’, ‘incorrect’ and ‘do not know’ responses were counted up for each

participant and their average scores. ‘Incorrect’ answers: M=11.29 SD=2.77; for ‘Do not

know’ answers: M=7.13, SD= 4.15 were calculated among the sample. On average,

participants' answer correctly 14.56 statements out of 32; they had 11.29 wrong answers and

7.13 did not know the responses. Results are shown by percentages for the sample in Table

4.3 below:

51

Table 4.3: Total Percentages - from 32 statements - of Correct (C), Incorrect (I) and Do not

know (DK) Answers of Primary and Secondary School Teachers from Turkey.

Correct responses Incorrect responses Do not know responses

Tota

l

Per

cen

tages

of

C,I

an

d D

K

an

swer

s’

By percentages

M=43.92%

SD= 9.7

By percentages

M=35.3%

SD=8.66

By percentages

M=22.8%

SD=12.96

The total percentages of answers of primary and secondary school teachers’ for 17 general

statements about general knowledge on the brain can be seen in Table 4.4 as Appendix-7

(see pp.97). Statements are given with correct answers in brackets; it is possible to compare

the correct answers and teachers’ average responses—by percentages—from the table. Thus,

each statement can be judged on its own merit.

Independent t-tests between groups (age, sex, branch, education level) showed that teacher

characteristics’ did not relate to their general knowledge. On the whole, teachers’ average

score from Turkey on general knowledge according to 17 general statements (M=56.9%

correct, SD=25.7) was lower than teachers’ average score from UK (M = 67% correct, SD =

13.5) and the Netherlands (M = 73% correct, SD = 12.7) (Dekker et al., 2012). This result

supports concerns (OECD, 2002; Goswami, 2004) about teachers’ having insufficient

knowledge about the brain in general, which, in turn, causes the prevalence of neuromyths at

the same time. Besides the last revision of teacher education was done in 1997 in Turkey, this

result could show a need to revise teacher education with respect to the developments in

educational research internationally; furthermore, it shows that education is affected by mass

media tools unwittingly.

52

Research Question 2: What is the situation of neuromyths in Turkey? And what is the

difference between the UK, the Netherlands and Turkey in terms of teachers believing in such

neuromyths?

In order to understand the general situation about neuromyths in Turkey, 15 neuromyths are

given with their correct answers in brackets and with the percentages of teachers’ responses in

Table 4.5 as Appendix-8 (see pp.98).

As can be seen from Table 4.5, the first nine assertions (neuromyths) received over 50 per

cent agreement. The results of these nine assertions can be termed as "misconceptions" and

which are widely believed and being used by primary and secondary school teachers in

Turkey currently, by considering the sample (n=278) of the present research.

According to the OECD’s report (2002), neuromyths are gaining wide currency and there are

concerns about the prevalence of neuromyths being spread to the rest of the world rapidly.

Thus, one of the purposes of this research is finding out the similarities and differences

between countries in terms of the prevalence of neuromyths which are believed and used by

"teachers" in education.

Teacher responses on neuromyths from three countries can be seen in Table 4.6 as Appendix

9 (see pp.99).

The analysis of the teacher responses for each neuromyth indicates many variations between

countries. For instance, the neuromyth about second language learning, viz. "Children must

acquire their native language before a second language is learned. If they do not do so

neither language will be fully acquired" was agreed upon by 58.3% of teachers in Turkey,

while it was believed by only 7% teachers of teachers in the UK and by 36% of teachers in the

Netherlands.

53

On the other hand, there were six assertions which can be seen with bold fonts in red in

Table 4.6 (pp.99) which are the most prevalent neuromyths (because they received over 50

per cent agreement from all three countries). This was especially prevalent with regards to the

neuromyths concerning learning styles (i.e. "Individuals learn better when they receive

information in their preferred learning style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinaesthetic)";

hemispheric dominance (i.e. "Differences in hemispheric dominance (left brain, right brain)

can help explain individual differences amongst learners"); and a possible integration of the

left and right hemispheres of the brain by means of exercises (i.e. "Short bouts of co-

ordination exercises can improve integration of left and right hemispheric brain function") all

received over 70 per cent agreement from all three countries. Overall, in Turkey, teachers

agreed with 53.02% (SD=27.80) of the neuromyths. This statistic is lower in the UK (49%)

and the Netherlands (49%) (Dekker et al., 2012). However, the similar level of teachers’

agreement on neuromyths in all three countries could show the prevalence of neuromyths in

general. In other words, although general knowledge was higher in the UK and the

Netherlands than in Turkey, the percentages of believing in neuromyths were similar. These

results could strongly validate the concerns about the prevalence of neuromyths across the

globe (OECD, 2002; Dekker et al., 2012).

Another research finding regards brain-based learning approaches (multiple intelligences,

learning styles and brain gyms), which are also related with neuromyths because they are

being used without valid evidences. Table 4.7 below shows the percentages of all of the three

countries with regards to teachers' encountering brain-based learning approaches.

54

Table 4.7: Teacher Responses about Brain-based Learning Approaches (Statistics about the

UK and the Netherland were taken from Dekker et al.’s (2012) research).

Turkey UK Netherlands

Yes (%) Yes (%) Yes (%)

Have you heard about multiple intelligences before? 77.7 71 67

Have you heard about learning styles (VAK) before? 80.6 98 64

Have you heard about brain gym before? 50 82 8

These differences between the encountering percentages of countries might be due to the

differences between countries regarding to the development of brain-based marketing

(Pasquinelli, 2012; Dekker et al., 2012). For instance, brain gym was not common in the

Netherlands and Turkey when compared to the UK. This situation links the different

marketing progress and process of brain-based knowledge in countries.

4.5 Findings from Interviews

Findings for the 3rd

and 4th

research questions will be presented in this section. As explained

before, the 3rd

and 4th

research questions will be answered through both questionnaire results

and interview findings.

Research Question 3: What are primary and secondary school teachers’ sources of

knowledge about neuromyths?

55

Table-4.8: Some Characteristics of Teachers

Always

%

Generally

%

Sometimes

%

Rarely

%

Never

%

Did

n

ot

rep

ly %

Do you read scientific articles? 3.6 21.9 41.4 23 5 5

Do you attend conferences? 2.5 9.7 41.4 33.5 6.1 6.8

Do you read popular science magazines? 17.6 43.9 25.2 10.1 1.4 1.8

Do you attend in-service training courses? 7.2 28.1 39.6 18 1.8 5.4

Do you attend private certificate courses? 2.2 11.5 38.1 27.7 11.5 9

Once a

week

Once a

month

Once in 3

months

Once

a year

Never

Did

not

rep

ly %

Do you read popular science magazines? 2.9 39.6 32.7 12.2 10.8 1.8

Table 4.8 shows some characteristics of teachers who participated in the study. There is a

difference between the percentages of reading ‘scientific articles’ and ‘popular science

magazines’; teachers indicated that they prefer to read popular science magazines

(always=17.6%, generally=43.9%) more than scientific articles (always=3.6%,

generally=21.9%). The majority of teachers sometimes (41.4%) attends conferences; also, the

majority sometimes (39.6%) attends in-service training courses; moreover, the majority

sometimes (38.1%) attends private certificate courses. There were not any significant

differences between genders, ages and branches in general; however, statistics showed that

females attend conferences (t=2.229,df=257,p=.027) and in-service training courses

(t=2.023,df=261,p=.044) more than males. Also primary school teachers read popular science

magazines more than secondary school teachers (t=2.125,df=271,p=.034); furthermore,

primary school teachers attend in-service training courses more than secondary school

teachers (t=-2.104,df=261,p= .036).

56

At the bottom of Table 4.8, there is another statistic about how often teachers read popular

science magazines; the majority indicated that they read them once a month (39.6%) or once

in every three months (32.7%). There was no significant difference between genders, ages or

branches for this question. Those variations do not show any significant differences between

groups (age, sex. branch, education level) according to the Independent-Samples T-test results

between groups regarding their general knowledge and misconceptions.

Regression analyses were performed for percentage of myths (dependent variable) with age,

sex, branch, education level, reading popular science, reading scientific journals, in-service

training, and percentage of correct answers on general assertions to examine which factors are

predictors of neuromyths. They show that belief in neuromyths was predicted as being

significant for those who had general knowledge about the brain (β=.138,t=2.31,p=.022),

which was similarly predicted in the UK and the Netherlands (β=.24,t=3.39,p=.001) (Dekker

et al., 2012). This result shows that the ones who have higher levels of general knowledge

about the brain tend to believe in neuromyths more. Moreover, according to these analyses,

attending in-service courses was an effective indicator for believing in neuromyths (β=-.13,t=-

2.114,p=.035). The other factors of age, sex, branch, education level; or reading popular

science magazines, reading scientific journals, attending private certificate courses or

conferences were not predicted as being of any significance for believing in neuromyths.

The above findings are some of the statistics which were obtained from the quantitative data

and which which shows the potential predictors of belief in neuromyths. These results could

give us a general outlook to understand the origins of neuromyths; however, there is a need to

have more detailed information so as to interpret the situation more accurately. One of the

purposes of interviews is to understand the sources of misconceptions, as mentioned before.

Therefore, the first 7 neuromyths (as seen in Table 4.4 at pp.97), which were believed by the

57

majority of participants, were chosen to be used in the interview part of the research. Instead

of using the 7th

and 8th

most prevalent neuromyths; the 9th

—"we only use 10% of our brain"—

was used by taking into consideration its prevalence according to the OECD (2002).

Therefore, 7 neuromyths were asked about to all interviewees in the first part of the

interviews (for interview questions see Appendix-3). These findings will be presented now

(for an example transcript, see Appendix 6).

Neuromyth 1: "Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred

learning style (VAK)".

All six of the interviewees strongly agreed with this statement. They were also asked where

they received this knowledge from:

Interviewees 1 and 2 underlined that their ‘self’ experiences as students from their school

years were important for their coming to that conclusion. For instance, interviewee 2 gave the

following example:

‘When I was at university, I had many observations I realised that some of my friends could

understand by writing while some of them could understand by listening. I could understand

by watching and imitating behaviours.’

Interviewee 3 indicated that he is an audial learner:

‘I believe that my audial memory is really strong. I am interested in this knowledge currently

and I learned the reaches of the Kizilirmak (Red River) and the Yesilirmak (Green River)

reaches (Kizilirmak and Yesilirmak are rivers in Turkey which have many reaches) by coding

them into a song. That is why I believe individuals learn in different ways in terms of their

learning styles.’

When he was asked about where he got this information, he highlighted that he did not read

anything about it but rather that he had just heard this information from some friends who

learned something by coding it to a song.

58

Interviewees 4, 5 and 6 told me that they concluded that this statement was true based on their

observations in their classrooms. They all underlined that all students have different learning

styles and it is possible to observe this in classrooms; that is why it is important to consider

students' learning styles.

Interviewee 6 stated clearly that she/he learned this information at university in the ‘Teaching

methods and styles’ class.

These responses show that interviewees in general—except interviewee 6—came to believe

neuromyth-1 by either coming to that conclusion from their personal own

experiences/observations or that they concluded that this information was true based solely

upon hearsay evidences.

Neuromyth 2: "Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-school

children".

All six of the participants strongly agreed with this statement.

Interviewees 1 and 4 evaluated the information in terms of a generation gap; they used their

previous knowledge, experiences and observations in order to deduce that this neuromyth was

true.

Interviewee 1:

‘I remember my childhood; we grew up in a poorer environment with fewer stimuli. I have 3

kids; and my parents also feel the same thing more strongly when they compare their

childhood and today's kids. Nowadays, there are lots of opportunities due to technological

improvements in comparison to the past. So they learn fast and they are more informed.’

59

Interviewee 4:

‘I agreed. When I compare my current students and my students from previous years I can

easily discern the difference between them in terms of their perception levels. My students

from previous years were learning more slowly and their vocabulary was poorer than my

present students. Nowadays, visual stimuli and early education are more effective than in past

times. So students start primary school more equipped.’

Interviewees 2 and 5 underlined the effect of socio-economic status differences when they

used their previous knowledge, experiences and observations to deduce the truth of the

neuromyth.

Interviewee 2:

‘From my experiences I can say that the environment which kids grow up in is important for

their mental development.’

Interviewee 5:

‘It is not the same for a child who attends a school from lower socio-economical environment

and a school from upper class environment in terms of the environment and stimulus they

have. That is why; I think socio-economical environment of a child is significant. According

to my observations, I can say environmental differences in terms of family education, school

conditioning and opportunities are effective for pre-school childdren.’

Interviewee 2 interprets this information through his/her neuroscientific knowledge from

university:

‘The mind works with its full capacity till 7 years old and then this capacity starts to

decrease; therefore, I think that the more they are stimulated, the more their brain can

improve.’

Interviewee 6 concluded according to a research result:

‘I think both nature and nurture is important; however, I read an article which says children

who go to nursery class are more successful than children who do not go to nursery class. So

I think that the environment and education they have are very important.’

60

Thus, except for interviewees 2 and 6, all participants based their knowledge on experiences

and observations. Interviewees 2 and 6 came to their conclusions through what they learned at

university or what they read.

Neuromyth 3: "Differences in hemispheric dominance can help to explain individual

differences among learners"

1 interviewee agreed with this statement, while 2 of them did not agree and 3 of them

indicated that they were not sure about the accuracy of the information.

Interviewees 1 and 5 thought that this was scientific information, although they did not

remember the source of their knowledge; however, while interviewee 1 agreed with the

statement, interviewee 5 was not sure and did criticise the statement.

Interviewee 1:

‘As I know this was proven scientifically and I think it makes sense. For example, whatever a

child takes as knowledge from family or from school, the capacity is limited to his/her

intelligence. As far as I know, verbal and mathematical intelligence depends on this

difference.’

Interviewee 5:

‘From a scientific perspective, it seems that hemispheric dominance explains individual

differences; but I never observe this, because it is impossible to observe. I can say that there

are differences between children in terms of their verbal and mathematical ability but I am

not sure whether it can be explained through hemispheric dominance. For example, some

children are successful in all fields, so how would we explain this through-hemispheric

dominance?’

Interviewees 2 and 3 stated that they heard but they were not sure about the accuracy and also

they could not remember the source

Interviewees 4 and 6 strongly disagreed. Interviewee 4 concluded through his/her own

experiences while interviewee 6 answered through a book that she/he had read.

61

Interviewee 4:

‘We cannot explain this by hemispheric dominance. There are lots of effects of individual

differences such as family, school, environment or socio-economic status.’

Interviewee 6:

‘I read a book which says that this statement is completely wrong. It says that our brain can

develop from every field and that we have that capacity. Also it said that the right and left

hemispheres always work together—that the brain works as a whole.’

Thus, their responses varied for this statement. In general, 4 of them did not remember their

source of knowledge; one of them answered through experiences and finally interviewee 6,

who gave the right response, answered through a book which he/she had read before.

Neuromyth 4: "It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid supplements (omega-3,

omega-6) have a positive effect on academic achievement."

Interviewees 1 and 4 accepted this statement, while 2 and 3 disagreed; interviewee 5 was not

sure and interviewee 6 had no idea about the information.

Interviewees 1 and 4 claimed that a healthy diet has a positive relationship both on brain

development and academic achievement from their own experiences and observations.

Interviewee 1:

‘When I was a teacher in Aladag (it is a county in northern Turkey), due to an iodine

deficiency, 80-90% of people had iodine-deficiency-related-goitre. And children’s perception

of knowledge and mental development were low; therefore, I can conclude through my own

observations that omega-3 or omega-6 could have a positive effect on academic

achievement.’

62

Interviewee 4:

‘I observed from my students that lacking a healthy diet causes difficulty in learning and that

they learn slower. I used omega-3 and omega-6 for my children as well. I read this

information from somewhere—I do not remember.’

In addition, both of them also emphasised that they saw many advertisements about the

effects of omega-3 and omega-6 on academic achievement.

Interviewees 2 and 3 looked at the subject from a different stand point, concluding from their

previous knowledge and experiences.

Interviewee 2:

‘So how will we explain the achievement of children who have low-income families, who are

living in the country-side? For example, in my village, there are many children who are

successful academically and yet do not take any supplements like omega-3 or omega-6.’

Interviewee 3:

‘So that means every children can be successful academically if they take omega-3 and

omega-6; but we know that it will not be like that; therefore it does not make any sense.’

Interviewee 5, on the other hand, mentioned a book which he/she read. According to that

book, fatty acid supplements have positive effects on achievement, but he/she was still not

sure about the accuracy of that information.

For this neuromyth, two of participants mentioned advertisements of omega-3 and omega-6

which shows the effect and prevalence of brain-based marketing in Turkey.

63

Neuromyth 5: "Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of left and

right hemispheric brain function"

All six of the interviewees were in agreement with this statement. Each of them gave some

examples:

Interviewee 3:

‘As I know, if we use our right hand, in general, the our left part of our brain works more or

vice versa; that is why we should support our students using their other hands’

Interviewee 4:

‘Sometimes I stop teaching during lessons and tell a story and I want them to draw that

story—whatever they imagine according to the story—on a piece of paper. It is a short

activity but I think it works and it strengthens the link between the two hemispheres; because,

according to my observations, their drawings improved over time after these kinds of

exercises.’

Interviewee 6:

‘Yes, I agree. I read that in a book. It says some activities, such as crosswords or walking

backward, can help to establish the link between the neurons again.’

For this neuromyth, I observed that the interviewees –except interviewee 6- in general were

not quite sure about the source from whence they got their knowledge, but they all tried

different types of exercises in their classrooms in order to improve the integration between the

left and right hemispheres in their students.

Neuromyth 6: "We only use 10% of our brains"

All of the interviewees have heard this statement before, but nobody, except interviewee 6,

was not sure where they received this information. Interviewees 1 and 5 were in agreement;

interviewee 6 strongly disagreed; and, while interviewee 2 was not sure, interviewee 3

64

thought that ‘we can use more than 10%’; contrariwise, interviewee 4 stated that ‘we are

using less than 10%’.

Interviewee 2:

‘I cannot show evidence if I disagree. But I heard something from a TV programme; it was

said that human beings have a high capacity but that we cannot use it all.’

Interviewee 3 thought that we can use more than 10%, claiming that the ‘brain is a kind of

muscle; if we exercise it, we can enhance it and use more of it. But I do not think we can use

more than 50%’. He was not sure, however, from which source he received this knowledge.

Interviewee 4 claimed that we use less than 10%, giving the following example to support his

claim:

‘I read from somewhere that in Russia, people can heal diseases with brain/mental power or

that they can move objects with brain power. But I am not sure where I read it. So, if we can

use our brain's full capacity, we can achieve lots of things, as with the Russian example’

Only interviewee 6 strongly disagreed with this statement:

‘No, this is definitely wrong information; as I read, our brains work as a whole. It has been

already proven scientifically. I read that in the book which I mentioned before.’

Neuromyth 7: "There are critical periods in childhood after which certain things can no

longer be learned."

5 of the interviewees agreed, with only one of them not accepting this information. The ones

who agreed also highlighted that certain things could be thought after critical periods but that

it takes time and that they cannot learn those behaviours completely after the period has

passed.

65

Interviewee 3 disagreed with this consensus, adding that, according to his/her experiences:

‘That would mean a child who could not learn how to write and read in the first grade,

cannot learn later; I do not think so. That child can learn but it would take that child more

time.’

Conversely, interviewee 4 gave the same example, though adding through his/her experiences

that:

‘There is a threshold for all behaviours. For example, I had a student who mingled letters ‘v’

and ‘u’ with each other in the first grade; I could not change the behaviour for more than 2

years. It is really hard to change the behaviour after critical periods have passed.’

The other interviewees (1, 2, 5 and 6) gave similar examples, such as toilet training, (they

stated that after a period, it is hard to change the behaviour).

Interviewee 6 gave an example from a scientific article that he/she had heard about at

university:

‘One of my teachers at university mentioned about a research which was about a child who

had not communicated with anybody and had been kept during critical periods from learning

certain things. Consequently, the child could not learn how to write and read and his/her

level did not reach the same as his/her age group.’

For this neuromyth, they used their experiences from classrooms or their personal lives to

respond to this statement, except for interviewee 6.

At this point, additional information should be given. Interviewee 6 had graduated 1.5 years

ago from university; thus, it can be claimed that that is why his/her responses were based on

books, articles and knowledge from university in general. Other participants, on the other

hand, mainly answered through their own experiences and observations.

Briefly, it can be concluded that for many statements, they came to the conclusion through

their experiences/observations; secondly they indicated that they did not remember the source

of knowledge; one may conclude that this means that, sometimes,

66

"hearsay" knowledge could have affected their practices and thoughts. This findings can

support Goswami’s (2006) and OECD’s (2007) concerns about the propagation of

neuromyths and moreover Sylvan and Christodoulou’s (2010) offer about teachers’ being

critical while using the knowledge about brain due to pseudo-scientific brain-based

applications.

However, it is not possible to evaluate the sources they gave as sources for their believing in

neuromyths in general. It was thought that the comments that they made gave some clues

about the reasons behind their misconceptions. In order to obtain clearer responses about what

the sources were for their knowledge, the questions below were asked:

Question: "How do you improve yourself with regards to your profession?" (N.B.

there is no response for this question from interviewee 1)

Mainly, the interviewees told me that they searched the internet and that they share

information and experiences with their colleagues. One of them mentioned attending in-

service training courses, two of them indicated that they read books, and only one of them

mentioned anything about reading scientific articles.

Research Question 4: What do teachers think about the usage of neuroscience in education?

Do they find it necessary to use neuroscientific findings in education?

First of all, the interviewees were asked whether they heard about neuroscience in education

before, or not.

Question: Have you heard about neuroscience in education at university?

67

5 of interviewees responded “no”, with only one of them (interviewee 5) mentioning that they

had had a class about ‘learning’ and that they had been given information briefly about the

role of the brain and its functions on learning during class.

Question: Have you heard about neuroscience and education in conferences, in-

service training courses or seminars after starting work?

All 6 of the interviewees stated that they were not informed about neuroscience after they

started work.

After talking about their previous experiences, it was obvious that they had not heard about

educational neuroscience, neither at university nor in their work-related experience. These

findings are related with the current situation of educational neuroscience in Turkey. It is a

completely new field and teachers generally did not have knowledge about this emerging field

until the 1990s.

The next question was about their perceptions about the field.

Question: What do you think? Is neuroscientific knowledge significant for teachers?

And do they need training for this?

Firstly, I will present the data from the questionnaire which was specifically about teachers’

perceptions (with regards the significance of field); confidence (about brain knowledge); and

need (for in-service training courses about neuroscience in education).

93.2% of the sample (n=259) agreed about the significance of neuroscience in education.

Furthermore, 90.3% stated that they are interested in receiving further knowledge about the

brain and its relationship to education. For more in-depth responses and data triangulation,

these same questions were asked during the interviews:

68

All of them definitely agreed about the significance of neuroscientific knowledge in

education. This result is consistent with previous results made by other researches; Pickering

and Howard-Jones (2007) reported in a similar setting that the majority of teachers rated the

role of the brain as being either very important or important. In another study conducted in the

US, Serpati and Loughan (2012) stated that 94% (n=221) of teachers agreed that it is

significant to know about the neurological underpinnings of learning, cognition and

behaviour.

As regards the second part of the question (do you need a training?), 83.2% (n=277) stated in

the questionnaires that they either “do not trust”, “trust little” or “averagely trust” their

knowledge about the brain; thus, 71.6% (n=275) indicated that they need an in-service

training course so as to learn more about the brain and its functions on learning. Similarly, all

of interviewees said that teachers’ knowledge on educational neuroscience is quite

inadequate; therefore, training is crucial for all teachers.

69

Chapter 5

Summary and Conclusion

This chapter concludes the literature review by bringing together potential key issues in

accordance with the findings of the research. It shall be divided into the following sections:

A brief summary of the literature review;

A summary of the findings;

The limitations of the study;

Conclusion;

Future research.

5.1 Summary

Neuroscience adds a new perspective to the field of education. However, it is challenging to

bridge the gap between those deeply-rooted fields in terms of the strong differences of their

respective methodologies and language in general. Besides, the gap generates certain

neuromyths and brain-based literature which are quite damaging to the bridging of this

inherent gap. Firstly, they waste time, effort and money; secondly, they hinder the

improvement of educational neuroscience as a discipline. More generally, they could block

scientific research in education. In order to prevent this situation, teachers should be

informed, especially since they are the end users of scientific research; what is more,

deleterious brain-based literature is gaining wide currency in education in many countries.

Although there are some efforts in some countries to inform teachers about educational

neuroscience, such as the UK, the Netherlands, Brazil and the US; there have been no studies

conducted about the field, particularly in relation to the perceptions and knowledge of

70

teachers’ in Turkey. Therefore, a framework is needed for a more science-based education so

as to protect the Turkish education system from neuromyths. As Goswami (2006) and Ansari

and Coch (2006) highlight, teachers have a significant and fundamental role in educational

neuroscience; thus, teacher training is crucial. By considering these purposes, a research

setting was created. 278 teachers completed the questionnaire and 6 were interviewed later for

more in-depth responses. Findings from the questionnaire and interviews were presented and

analysed in detail in Chapter 4.

5.2 Summary of the Findings

The questionnaire results showed that primary and secondary school teachers in Turkey

(based on a sample of n=278) believe (53.02%) in neuromyths in general. Especially, 9

neuromyths out of the 15 identified generated more than 50% agreement, with 5 of these 9

being believed by more than 70% of participants.

Although teachers’ general knowledge on the brain and its functioning are higher in the UK

and the Netherlands than in Turkey, the percentages were not significantly different regarding

people believing in neuromyths; this reflects the reality of concerns with regards to the

prevalence of neuromyths in many countries (OECD, 2002).

Those interviewed were not sure about the source from which they gained their knowledge

about the misconceptions about the brain, in general. They stated that, while responding to

neuromyths, their sources of knowledge were based on either personal

experiences/observations or teaching experiences, mainly.

93.2% of the questionnaire participants and all of the interviewees agreed about the

significance of neuroscience in education; furthermore, 71.6% of participants and all of the

interviewees stated that training is crucial for all teachers.

71

5.3 Limitations of the Study

Educational neuroscience is a new field in Turkey. In considering the newness of the field, a

mixed methods research approach (questionnaire and interviews) was chosen because it

provides a broader overview. However, both quantitative and qualitative researches have

certain limitations, and this study is no exception.

I handed out the questionnaires and visited the schools personally in order to give an

opportunity for teachers to clarify any points they did not understand; however,

because of the time issue (they tried to fill the questionnaires during their break times),

some participants might have not been able to ask me any questions.

The questionnaire sample (n=278) was quite wide; however, it was not large enough

when considering the total number of primary and secondary school teachers

(551.802) in Turkey (according to 2013 Turkish National Statistics).

During the interviews, I did not give information about neuromyths and did not say

that some of the statements given had pseudo-scientific information; thus, they might

have accepted the statements I gave as scientific and true. That is why they might have

tried to answer positively. However, this is a remote possibility because there are

examples that interviewees did not agree with all of the statements.

The interview sample size was small (n=6). So, although interview findings provided

in-depth explanations, it is crucial to remember that this study’s interview findings

cannot be generalised to a wider population.

This study has strengths, significances and limitations, as has been seen. I would now like to

conclude my research with some key points in accordance with the previous literature and

current findings.

72

5.4 Conclusion

As mentioned in the literature review, the first problem identified by the scientists in the field

of educational neuroscience is the promulgation of neuromyths, especially since neuromyths

hinder further development in the field. Furthermore, the OECD drew international attention

to the fact that neuromyths waste time, effort and money in the educational field. The present

research results proved the prevalence of neuromyths among primary and secondary school

teachers in Turkey, as is also the case for the UK, the Netherlands, and many other countries.

Although there are some scientists and educators who do not support the necessity of

neuroscience in education, it is obvious that unscientific applications, especially brain-based

marketing practices, could damage education in many aspects. To hinder neuroscientific

knowledge on education could cause pseudo-scientific applications and the occurrence of

baseless and ungrounded information. This is due to the fact that ‘knowledge and its

manifestations are no longer "out there", to be acquired from a centre, mastered and applied'

(Deakin Crick, 2007: 135). Due to new technologies and mass media, knowledge is available

for all through network access.

Therefore, awareness is crucial to prevent the prevalence of neuromyths and brain-based

marketing applications.

To summarise the key points of this research:

Teachers have not heard about neuroscience in education before, there have not been

any studies about educational neuroscience in academia, and there are only a few

articles which were based on knowledge from different researches from different

countries in Turkey;

Primary and secondary school teachers have limited knowledge about the brain and its

functions in general in Turkey;

73

However, brain-based marketing has been very effective in Turkey and neuromyths

are as prevalent in that country as in any other country;

Teachers believe more than half of neuromyths and they do not trust their knowledge

about the brain and its functions regarding the learning process; however, there are no

training courses or programmes or even concerns about its unscientific applications in

Turkey;

Nevertheless, teachers believe that they need training in order to better understand the

brain.

5.5 Future Research

The harm of neuromyths is obvious. This study showed, however, that primary and secondary

school teachers in Turkey (as well as elsewhere) lacked knowledge about the brain and its

functions in general and that most of them believed in many neuromyths. Even though they

have not heard about neuroscience in education, they mostly knew about the brain-based,

pseudo-scientific information. In considering the efficacy of brain-based marketing in

education due to the improvement of mass media, the field should be grounded better in order

to prevent baseless information from keeping education in its clutches.

As highlighted before, the significance of this research is about its being the first study among

teachers in Turkey about neuroscience in education, as well as their believing in neuromyths.

It is hoped that this study has demonstrated the importance of the field, as well as providing a

beginning for this field in Turkey.

74

References

Akyıldız, O. (2010). Research in Neuroscience and Music Education: Towards Knowledge

Transfer Possibilities. Paper in International Conference on New Trends in Education and

Their Implications, Antalya-Turkey. Retrieved from:

http://www.iconte.org/FileUpload/ks59689/File/176.pdf

Allix, N.M. (2000). The Theory of Multiple Intelligences: A Case of Missing Cognitive

Matter. Australian Journal of Education - Special Issue on Education and Cognition -

Naturalistic Explorations Into the Nature of Mind and Learning. 44 (3), 272-293.

Alferink, L.A. & Farmer-Dougan, V. (2010). Brain-(not) Based Education: Dangers of

Misunderstanding and Misapplication of Neuroscience Research. Exceptionallity, 18, 42-52.

Anderson, M. & Della-Sala, S. (2012). Neuroscience in Education: An (opinionated)

Intoduction. In S. Della-Salla, M. Anderson (Eds.), Neuroscience in Education: The Good,

The Bad and The Ugly (pp. 3-12). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Ansari, D. & Coch, D. (2006). Bridges over Troubled Waters: Education and Cognitive

Neuroscience. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(4), 146-151.

Bartoszeck, A.B & Bartoszeck, F.K. (2012). How In-Service Teachers Perceive Neuroscience

as Connected to Education: An Exploratory Study. European Journal of Educational

Research, 1(4), 301-319.

Beauchamp, M. & Beauchamp, C. (2012). Understanding the Neuroscience and Education

Connection: Themes Emerging From a Review of the Literature. In S. Della-Salla, M.

Anderson (Eds.), Neuroscience in Education: The Good, The Bad and The Ugly (pp. 13-30).

Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Blakemore, C.L. (2003). Movement is Essential to Learning. Journal of Physical Education,

Recreation & Dance, 74, 22-25.

Blakemore, S.J & Frith, U. (2005). The Learning Brain: Lessons for Education: a Précis.

Developmental Science. 8(5), 459-471.

75

Bruer, J. (1997). Education and the Brain: A Bridge too Far. Educational Researcher, 26(8),

4-16.

Bush, T. (2007). Authenticity in Research-Reliability, Validity and Triangulation. In A.R.J.

Briggs, M. Coleman (Eds.) Research Methods in Educational Leadership and Management

(2nd

Edition) (pp. 91-105). London: SAGE Publications.

Busher, H. & James, N. (2007). Ethics and Access in Research Education. In A. Briggs, M.

Coleman (Eds.) Research in Educational Management (pp. 106-122). London: Paul

Chapman.

Cakiroglu, E. & Cakiroglu, J. (2003). Reflections on Teacher Education in Turkey. European

Journal of Teacher Education, 26(2), 253-264.

Campbell, S.R. (2011). Educational Neuroscience: Motivations, Methodology and

Implications. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 43(1), 7-16.

Capellini, I., McNamara, P., Preston, B.T., Nunn, C.L. & Barton, R.A. (2009). Does Sleep

Play a Role in Memory Consolidation? A Comparative Test. PLoS ONE, 4(2).

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004609

Carr, W. (1995). Philosophy and Educational Research. Paper presented at the British

Educational Research Association/ECER Conference. Bath, UK, 5 – 7 September, 1995.

Casey, B.J. & Getz, S. & Galvan, A. (2008). The Adolescent Brain. Developmental Review,

28, 62-77.

Coch, D. & Ansari, D. (2012). Constructing Connection: The Evolving Field of Mind, Brain

and Education. In S. Della-Salla, M. Anderson (Eds.), Neuroscience in Education: The Good,

The Bad and The Ugly (pp. 33-46). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Coffield, F., Moseley, D., Hall, E. & Ecclestone, K. (2004). Learning Styles and Pedagogy in

Post-16 Learning. A Systematic and Critical Review. London: Learning and Skills Research

Centre.

Cohen, L. & Manion, L. (1994). Research Methods in Education (4th

Edition). London:

Routledge.

76

Cohen, L., Manion, L. & Morrison, K. (2011). Research Methods in Education (7th

Edition).

New York: Routledge.

Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research Design (2nd

Edition). California: SAGE Publications.

Deakin Crick, R. (2007). Learning How to Learn: The Dynamic Assessment of Learning

Power. Curriculum Journal, 18:2, 135-153.

Dekker, S., Lee, N.C., Howard-Jones, P. & Jolles, J. (2012). Neuromyths in Education:

Prevalence and Predictors of Misconceptions among Teachers. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1-

8.

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H.G., Winkler, J., Büchel, C. & May, A.

(2004). Temporal and Spatial Dynamics of Brain Structure Changes during Extensive

Learning. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(23), 6314-6317.

Edgar, D.W. (2012). Learning Theories and Historical Events Affecting Instructional Design

in Education: Recitation Literacy toward Extraction Literacy Practices. SAGE Open, 2(4), 1-9.

Eshel, N., Nelson, E.E., Blair, J., Pine, D.S. & Ernst, M. (2007). Neural Substrates of Choice

Selection in Adults and Adolescents: Development of the Ventrolateral Prefrontal and

Anterior Cingulate Cortices. National Institutes of Health, 45(6), 1270-1279.

Falkenberg, T., Mohammed, A.K., Henriksonn, B., Persson, H., Winblad, B. & Lindefors, N.

(1992). Increased Expression of Brain-Derieved Neurotrophic Factor mRNA in Rat

Hippocampus is Associated with Improved Spatial Memory and Enriched Environment.

Neuroscience Letters, 138(1), 153-156.

Ferrari, M. & McBride, H. (2011). Mind, Brain and Education: The Birth of a New Science.

Learning Landscapes, 5(1), 85-100.

Fischer, K.W., Daniel, D.B., Immordino-Yang, M.H., Stern, E., Battro, A. & Koizumi, H.

(2007). Why Mind, Brain and Education? Why now? Mind, Brain and Education, 1(1), 1-2.

Fisher, K.W., Goswami, U. & Geake, J. (2010). The Future of Educational Neuroscience.

Mind, Brain and Education, 4, 68-80.

77

Galvan, A., Hare, T.A., Parra, C.E., Penn, J., Voss, H., Glover, G. & Casey, B.J. (2006).

Earlier Development of the Accumbens Relative to Orbitofrontal Cortex might Underlie Risk-

Taking Behaviour in Adolescents. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(25), 6885-6892.

Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of Mind. New York: Basic Books.

Geake, J. (2008). Neuromythologies in Education. Educational Research, 50, 123-133.

Gilmore, C. K., McCarthy, S. E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). Symbolic Arithmetic Knowledge

without Instruction. Nature, 447, 589–592.

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., ...

Thompson, P.M. (2004). Dynamic Mapping of Human Cortical Development during

Childhood through Early Adulthood. The National Academy of Science, 101, 8174-8179.

Goswami, U. (2004). Annual review: Neuroscience and Education. British Journal of

Educational Psychology, 74, 1-14.

Goswami, U. (2006). Neuroscience and Education: From Research to Practice? Nature

Reviews Neuroscience, pp. 2-6. doi: 10.1038/nrn1907

Goswami, U. (2009). Mind, Brain, and Literacy: Biomarkers as Usable Knowledge for

Education. Mind, Brain and Education, 3(3), 176-184.

Gray, D.E. (2009). Doing Research in the Real World (2nd

Edition). London: SAGE

Publications.

Han, M., Park, K., Baek, S., Kim, B., Kim, J., Kim, H. & Oh, S. (2007). Inhibitory Effects of

Caffeine on Hippocampal Neurogenesis and Function. Biochemical and Biophysical Research

Communications, 356, 976–980.

Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Bruer, J. (2007). The Brain/Education Barrier. Science, 317, pp.1293.

Hook, C.J. & Farah, M.J. (2012). Neuroscience for Educators: What are They Seeking? And

What are They Finding? Neuroethics. doi: 10.1007/s12152-012-9159-3

Howard-Jones, P. (2007). Neuroscience and Education: Issues and Opportunities: A

Commentary by the Teaching and Learning Research Programme. London: ESCR.

78

Howard-Jones, P. (2008). Education and Neuroscience. Educational Research, 50(2), 119-

122.

Howard-Jones, P. (2008). Philosophical Challenges for Researchers at the Interface between

Neuroscience and Education. Journal of Philosophy of Education, 42(3-4), 361-380.

Howard-Jones, P. (2009). Neuroscience, Learning and Technology (14-19). Becta. Retrieved

from:http://www.education.gov.uk/complexneeds/modules/Module-4.3-Insights-from-

neuroscience/All/downloads/m15p630c/becta_2009_deeplearningneuroscience.litrev.pdf

Howard-Jones, P., Franey, L., Mashmoushi, R. & Liao, Yen-Chun (2009). The Neuroscience

Literacy of Trainee Teachers: Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association

Annual Conference, University of Manchester. Education-Line. Retrieved from:

http://70.33.241.170/~neuro647/wp-content/uploads/2012/03/Literacy.pdf

Howard-Jones, P. (2010). Introducing Neuroeducational Research. New York, NY:

Routledge.

Howard-Jones, P. (2011). A Multiperspective Approach to Neuroeducational Research.

Educational Philosophy and Theory, 43(1), 24-30.

Jensen, E. (1998). Teaching with the Brain in Mind. Alexandria, VA: Association of

Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Jick, T.D. (1979). Mixing Qualitative and Quantitative Methods: Triangulation in Action.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 24(4). Retrieved from:

http://www.jstor.org/discover/10.2307/2392366?uid=2&uid=4&sid=21102573453221

Johnson, R.B. & Onwuegbuzie, A.J. (2004). Mixed Methods Research: A Research Paradigm

Whose Time Has Come. Educational Researcher, 33, 14-26.

Johnson, L.B. & Turner, L.A. (2003). Data Collection Strategies in Mixed Methods Research.

In A. Tashakkori., C. Teddlie (Eds.). Handbook of Mixed methods in Social and Behavioural

Research (pp. 297-319). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Keles, E. & Cepni, S. (2006). Brain and Learning. Journal of Turkish Science Education, 3(2),

66-82.

79

Kleff, H.G. (2007). The Turkish Interior Migration. Retrieved from:

http://migrationeducation.de/27.1.html?&rid=91&cHash=e9359cf8a6f23fc45ddf877c0ae365f

8

Koizumi, H. (1999). A Practical Approach towards Trans-disciplinary Studies for the 21st

Century. Jornal of Seizon and Life Science, 9, 5–24.

Krätzig, G. P. & Arbuthnott, K. D. (2006). Perceptual Learning Style and Learning

Proficiency: A Test of the Hypothesis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 238–246.

Lagemann, E.C. (2000). An Elusive Science: The Troubling History of Education Research.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Lincoln, Y. & Denzin, N. (1998). The Fifth Moment. In N. Denzin, Y. Lincoln (Eds.) The

Landscape of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

Maguire E.A., Spiers H.J., Good C.D., Hartley T., Frackowiak R.S. & Burgess N. (2003)

Navigation Expertise and the Human Hippocampus: A Structural Brain Imaging Analysis.

Hippocampus, 13, 250 –259.

Marshall, L. & Born, J. (2007). The Contribution of Sleep to Hippocampus-dependent

Memory Consolidation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11(10), 442–450.

doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.001

Maquet, P., Laureys, S., Peigneux, P., Fuchs, S., Petiau, C., Phillips, ...Cleeremans, A. (2000)

Experience-Dependent Changes in Cerebral Activation During Human REM Sleep. Nature

Neuroscience, 3, 831–836.

Morrison, M. (2007). What do We Mean by Educational Research? In A.R.J. Briggs, M.

Coleman (Eds.) Research Methods in Educational Leadership and Management (2nd

Edition)

(pp. 13-36). London: SAGE Publications.

OECD (2002). Understanding the Brain: Towards a New Learning Science. Paris: OECD

Publications.

OECD (2007). Understanding the Brain: Birth of a New Learning Science. Paris: OECD.

Ozmusul, M. (2012). Developments in Turkish Education System towards International

Dimension. International Journal Social Science & Education, 2(3), 345-361.

80

Pasquinelli, E. (2012). Neuromyths: Why Do They Exist and Persist? Mind, Brain and

Education, 6(2), 89-96.

Pickering, S. J. & Howard-Jones, P. (2007). Educators’ Views on the Role of Neuroscience in

Education: Findings from a Study of UK and International Perspectives. Mind, Brain and

Education, 1, 109–113.

Potkin, K.T. & Bunney, W.E. (2012). Sleep Improves Memory: The Effect of Sleep on Long

Term Memory in Early Adolescence. PLoS ONE 7(8). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042191

Rauscher, F., Shaw, G. & Ky, K. (1993). Music and Spatial Task Performance. Nature, 365,

611.

Rizzolatti, G. & Craighero, L. (2004). The Mirror Neuron System. Annual Review of

Neuroscience, 27, 169-92.

Rogers, P.J. (2007). Caffeine, Mood and Mental Performance in Everyday Life. Nutrition

Bulletin, 32, 84-89.

Robson, C. (2002). Real World Research (2nd

Edition). West Sussex, UK: Wiley.

Samuels, B. M. (2009). Can differences between education and neuroscience be overcome by

Mind, Brain, and Education? Mind, Brain, and Education, 3(1), 45-53.

Schober, P. & Sabitzer, B. (2013). Mirror Neurons for Education. In IATED (Hrsg):

Proceedings CD 7th

International Technology, Education and Development Conference,

Valencia (Spain), 4th

-8th

of March 2013. Barcelona: International Association of Technology,

Education and Development (IATED), (pp. 40-45).

Schofield, J.W. (2002). Increasing the Generalizability of Qualitative Research. In M.

Huberman, M.B. Miles (Eds.), The Qualitative Researcher’s Companion (pp. 171-204).

Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications.

Schumacher, R. (2007). The Brain is not Enough: Potential and Limits in Integrating

Neuroscience and Pedagogy. Analyse & Kritik, 29(1), 38–46.

Scott, D. & Morrison, M. (2006). Key Ideas in Educational Research. London: Continuum.

Serpati, L. & Loughan, A.R. (2012). Teacher Perceptions of Neuroeducation: A Mixed

Methods Survey of Teachers in the United States. Mind, Brain and Education, 6(3), 174-176.

81

Sibley, B.A. & Etnier, J.L. (2003). The Relationship Between Physical Activity and

Cognition in Children: A Meta-Analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science, 15, 243-256.

Smith, J. K. (1983). Quantitative versus Qualitative Research: An Attempt to Clarify the

Issue. Educational Researcher, 12, 6–13.

Sousa, D. (2010). How Science Met Pedagogy. In D. Sousa (Ed.), Mind, Braind and

Education: Neuroscience Implications for the Classrooms (pp. 9-24). Bloomington, US:

Solution Tree Press.

Sousa, D. (2011). Mind, Brain, and Education: The Impact of Educational Neuroscience on

the Science of Teaching. Learning Landscapes, 5(1), 37-43.

Sowell, E.R., Thopmson, P.M., Tessner, K.D. & Toga, A.W. (1999). Mapping Continued

Brain Growth and Grey Matter Density Reduction in Dorsal Frontal Cortex: Inverse

Relationships During Postadolescent Brain Maturation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(22),

8819-8829.

Sparks, S. (2012, June). Teachers Need Lessons in Neuroscience, Experts Say. Education

Week, 31, 16-17. Retrieved from:

http://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2012/06/06/33teachers.h31.html

Steinberg, L. (2010). A Dual Systems Model of Adolescent Risk-Taking. Developmental

Psychobiology, 52, 216-224.

Sternberg, R.J. & Grigorenko, E.L. (2004). Successful Intelligence in the Classroom. Theory

into Practice, The Ohio State University, 43(3), 274-280.

Stewart, L., Henson, R., Kampe, K., Walsh, V., Turner, R. & Frith, U. (2003). Brain Changes

after Learning to Read and Play Music. Neuroimage, 20, 71-83.

Sylvan, L.J. & Christodoulou, A. (2010). Understanding the Role of Neuroscience in Brain

Based Products: A Guide for Educators and Consumers. Mind, Brain and Education, 4(1), 1-

7.

Szucs, D. & Goswami, U. (2007). Educational neuroscience: Defining a New Discipline for

the Study of Mental Representations. Mind, Brain and Education, 1(3), 114-124.

82

Tashakkori, A. & Teddlie, C. (1998). Mixed Methodology: Combining Qualitative and

Quantitative Approaches. Applied Social Research Method Series (Volume 46). Thousand

Oaks, California: Sage.

The British Psychological Society (2009). Codes of Ethics and Conduct. Retrieved from:

http://www.bps.org.uk/system/files/documents/code_of_ethics_and_conduct.pdf

Tokuhama-Espinosa, T. (2011). Why Mind, Brain, and Education Science is the "New"

Brain-Based Education. New Horizons for Learning, 9(1). Retrieved from

http://education.jhu.edu/PD/newhorizons/Journals/Winter2011/Tokuhama1

Turkish National Statistics about Teacher Numbers (March, 2013). Retrieved from:

http://kamudan.com/iste-turkiyedeki-ogretmen-ve-ogrenci-sayilari-9620.html

UNESCO Report. Children in Formal Education. Retrieved from:

http://www.unicef.org.tr/tr/content/detail/56/children-in-the-formal-education-system-2.html

Varma, S., McCandliss, B.D. & Schwartz, D.L. (2008). Scientific and Pragmatic Challenges

for Bridging Education and Neuroscience. Educational Researcher, 37(3), 140-152.

Vaus, D. (2001). Research Design in Social Research. London: SAGE Publications.

Volckmar, N. (2011). Educational Neuroscience: A Critical Discourse Analysis. Trondheim,

Norway: Norwegian University of Science and Technology.

Walliman, N. & Buckler, S. (2008). Your Dissertation in Education. London: SAGE

Publications.

Waterhouse, L. (2006). Multiple Intelligences, Mozart Effect and Emotional Intelligences: A

Critical Review. 41(4), 207-225.

Winter, B., Breitenstein, C., Mooren, F.C., Voelker, K., Fobker, M., Lechtermann, A., ...

Knecht, S. (2006). High Impact Running Improves Learning. Neurobiology of Learning and

Memory, 87, 597-609.

Yuksel, I. (2012). The Current Developments in Teacher Education in Turkey on the

Threshold of European Union. International Journal of Humanities and Social Science, 2(8),

49-56.

83

Zervas, Y., Apostolos, D. & Klissouras, V. (1991). Influence of Physical Exertion on Mental

Performance with Reference to Training. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 72, 1215–1221.

APPENDIX-1

QUESTIONNAIRE

PART 1

84

1-Year of birth :

2-Gender : Female / Male (Please circle)

3-Phase : Primary / Secondary (Please circle)

4-Educational Level : University / Postgraduate / PHD (Please circle)

5-Where do you get the information about your profession?

Always Mostly Occasionally Rarely Never

Academic journals

Conferences

Scientific web magazines

In-service training

(private) Certificate programs

(If there is) Other.....................

6-Do you read popular science magazines? (Please circle)

No Once a week Once a month Once in three months Once a year Never

7- Are in-service training courses useful?

Yes / Sometimes / No

8-Are you interested in to have scientific knowledge about brain and its influence on learning?

Yes / Not sure / No

9-Do you think, is it valuable to have scientific knowledge on brain, and its influence on learning for your teaching

practice? Yes / Not sure / No

10- How confident you are about your knowledge on brain?

Do not trust 1 2 3 4 5

11-Do you think that you need an in-service training course to understand about brain and its effect on learning?

Yes / Not sure / No

12-Which one is the most effective on a child’s academic achievement? Please put an order from the most effective (1)

to least effective (4)(write the numbers to boxes from 1 to 3 or 4, You do not have to state an “Other”).

School education Genes Home environment Other (if there is, please state)………………

13-Which (if any) of these approaches to learning have you come across in schools)? (Please tick)

Multiple Intelligences Learning Styles (e.g. VAK) Brain Gym

85

T F D

1 We use our brains 24 h a day

2 Children must acquire their native language before a second language is learned. If they do not do so neither language will be fully

acquired.

3 Boys have bigger brains than girls.

4 If pupils do not drink sufficient amounts of water (6–8 glasses a day) their brains shrink

5 It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid supplements (omega-3 and omega-6) have a positive effect on academic achievement.

6 When a brain region is damaged other parts of the brain can take up its function

7 We only use 10% of our brain.

8 The left and right hemispheres of the brain always work together.

9 Differences in hemispheric dominance (left brain, right brain) can help to explain individual differences amongst learners.

10 The brains of boys and girls develop at the same rate.

11 Brain development has finished by the time children reach secondary school.

12 There are critical periods in childhood after which certain things can no longer be learned.

13 Information is stored in the brain in a network of cells distributed throughout the brain.

14 Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the brain

15 Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinesthetic).

16 Learning occurs through modification of the brains’ neural connections.

17 Academic achievement can be affected by skipping breakfast.

18 Normal development of the human brain involves the birth and death of brain cells.

19 Mental capacity is hereditary and cannot be changed by the environment or experience.

20 Vigorous exercise can improve mental function.

21 Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-school children.

22 Children are less attentive after consuming sugary drinks and/or snacks.

23 Circadian rhythms (“body-clock”) shift during adolescence, causing pupils to be tired during the first lessons of the school day

24 Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks reduces alertness.

25 Exercises that rehearse co-ordination of motor-perception skills can improve literacy skills.

26 Extended rehearsal of some mental processes can change the shape and structure of some parts of the brain.

27 Individual learners show preferences for the mode in which they receive information (e.g., visual, auditory, kinesthetic)

28 Learning problems associated with developmental differences in brain function cannot be remediated by education.

29 Production of new connections in the brain can continue into old age.

30 Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of left and right hemispheric brain function

31 There are sensitive periods in childhood when it’s easier to learn things.

32 When we sleep, the brain shuts down.

86

APPENDIX-2

RESULTS OF PILOT STUDY

Descriptive Statistics (n=38):

Average Age : 32

Gender : 54% Female, 46% Male

Branch : 29% Secondary School Teacher, 71% Primary School Teacher

Education Level: 88% Undergraduate, 12% Masters

Part-1 (General Questions about Their Attitudes)

1- Do you read scientific journals? 3% Always, 77% Sometimes, 20% Never

2- Do you follow the latest developments about your profession? If so, do you try to

apply these knowledge to the classroom?

11% Always, 71% Sometimes, 17% Never

3- Do you attend in-service training courses?11%Always,47% Sometimes,14% Never

4- Do you think that in-service training courses are useful? 57% Yes, 43% No

5- Do you read popular science magazines?11% Always,69% Sometimes,20% Never

6- Do you think that the knowledge about brain and its functions is important for a

teacher? 100% Yes

7- Have you heard multiple intelligences before? (36% Yes)

8- Have you heard learning styles before (visual, auditory, kinaesthetic)? (42% yes)

9- Have you heard brain gym before? (22% yes)

87

T% F% D%

1 We use our brains 24 h a day 71 17 11

2 Children must acquire their native language before a second language is learned. If they do not do so neither language will be fully

acquired.

60 29 11

3 Boys have bigger brains than girls. 31 34 34

4 If pupils do not drink sufficient amounts of water (6–8 glasses a day) their brains shrink 26 23 51

5 It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid supplements (omega-3 and omega-6) have a positive effect on academic achievement. 83 0 17

6 When a brain region is damaged other parts of the brain can take up its function 9 51 40

7 We only use 10% of our brain. 46 29 26

8 The left and right hemispheres of the brain always work together. 29 46 26

9 Differences in hemispheric dominance (left brain, right brain) can help to explain individual differences amongst learners. 83 3 14

10 The brains of boys and girls develop at the same rate. 26 34 40

11 Brain development has finished by the time children reach secondary school. 6 71 23

12 There are critical periods in childhood after which certain things can no longer be learned. 65 24 12

13 Information is stored in the brain in a network of cells distributed throughout the brain. 54 6 40

14 Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the brain 40 31 29

15 Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinesthetic). 100 0 0

16 Learning occurs through modification of the brains’ neural connections. 54 6 40

17 Academic achievement can be affected by skipping breakfast. 91 3 6

18 Normal development of the human brain involves the birth and death of brain cells. 51 3 46

19 Mental capacity is hereditary and cannot be changed by the environment or experience. 14 77 9

20 Vigorous exercise can improve mental function. 37 43 20

21 Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-school children. 91 6 3

22 Children are less attentive after consuming sugary drinks and/or snacks. 31 20 49

23 Circadian rhythms (“body-clock”) shift during adolescence, causing pupils to be tired during the first lessons of the school day 46 11 43

24 Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks reduces alertness. 43 31 26

25 Exercises that rehearse co-ordination of motor-perception skills can improve literacy skills. 65 12 24

26 Extended rehearsal of some mental processes can change the shape and structure of some parts of the brain. 53 6 41

27 Individual learners show preferences for the mode in which they receive information (e.g., visual, auditory, kinesthetic) 97 0 3

28 Learning problems associated with developmental differences in brain function cannot be remediated by education. 20 60 20

29 Production of new connections in the brain can continue into old age. 49 6 46

30 Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of left and right hemispheric brain function 74 3 23

31 There are sensitive periods in childhood when it’s easier to learn things. 100 0 0

32 When we sleep, the brain shuts down. 3 89 9

Part-2 (Responses to the Statements in the Questionnaire by Percentages) For the Pilot Study

88

APPENDIX-3

INTERVIEW QUESTIONS

Part-1 (Questions about Neuromyths)

7 Prevalent Neuromyths from the Questionnaire results were asked in the first part of

interviews:

1- ‘Individuals learn better when they receive information in their preferred learning style

(visual, auditory, kinaesthetic).’

2- ‘Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-school children.’

3- Differences in hemispheric dominance can help to explain individual differences

amongst learners.’

4- ‘It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid supplements (omeg-3, omega-6) have

a positive effect on academic achievement.’

5- ‘Shourt bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of left and right

hemispheric brain function.’

6- ‘We only use 10% of our brains.’

7- ‘There are critical periods in childhood after which certain things can no longer be

learned.’

After each neuromyth one or two additional questions (depends on the responses) were asked

for in-depth responses:

Why you agree/disagree?

Why do you think like that?

Where did you get this information?

If they say ‘do not know’: Can I ask you to make a guess with your knowledge? Or

With ‘do not know answer you mean you have not come across before or you have but

you still do not know?

What did you know before about this statement to reach that conclusion?

Do you have any examples? Can you give an example?

Final Question about Neuromyths: How each statement affects the practice in the

classroom? Are they useful and do you apply these statements in practice in your classroom?

89

Part-2 (General Questions)

Have you ever come across neuroscience in education at university?

Have you ever come across neuroscience in education after you become a teacher at

in-service training courses or conferences?

Are you interested in learning for your profession more and how do you improve

yourself about your profession?

Do you think that it is important for teachers to know about the brain? Why/Why not?

Do you think, currently, is there enough training courses for teachers about this area?

Are you feel confident to your knowledge about brain and its functions on learning?

Have you heard about multiple intelligences before? If so, where?

Have you heard about learning styles before? If so, where?

Have you heard about brain gym before? If so, where?

90

APPENDIX-4

CONSENT FORM FOR QUESTIONNAIRE

Supervisor: Tim Jay Name of the School: Graduate School of Education

Researcher: Ozge Karakus Contact e mail:[email protected]

I am conducting a survey on Neuroscience in Education for my master dissertation. I want to define “The

knowledge of primary and secondary school teachers about brain and also their perceptions about the

application of neuroscientific research findings in educational field, in Turkey”. Your voluntary participation is

requested.

The questionnaire will take approximately 15 minutes. There are 2 parts; in the 1st part, you will be asked to

answer some questions about yourself (such as your year of birth, sex, and educational level) and also some

questions about your attitudes in terms of your profession. In the 2nd

part, there are 32 questions about brain, you

will be asked to mark either “T for True”, “F for False” or “D for Don’t Know” for each one.

Your name will not be wanted or recorded; your responses will be anonymous. The data will not be seen by

anybody except the researcher. Again, your participation is voluntary and you may choose not to answer all of

the questions on the questionnaire even after signing the consent; you have the right of withdrawal. However,

because of the anonymity of collecting data, you cannot withdrawal, once you pass the questionnaire in to the

researcher.

It is aimed to finish the report of the study until October-2013. If you want to be informed about the results or if

you have any questions pertaining to this study, please do not hesitate to contact with me by the e-mail address

above.

If you are willing to participate, please sign the “participant’s agreement” part below.

Thank you for your assistance.

Participant’s Agreement

I read the explanations about the study and my rights as a participant, above. I know that my participation is

voluntary and anonymous; I have the right of withdrawal, however once I fill and give back the questionnaire, I

cannot withdrawal because of the anonymity; the data will not be seen and used except the researcher and this

study; there is an e-mail address which I can use if I want to ask any questions and want to learn the results. I am

informed about the process and willing to participate through the information given.

Participant Signature____________________________

Date ___________________

91

APPENDIX-5 CONSENT FORM FOR INTERVIEWS

Supervisor: Tim Jay Name of the School: Graduate School of Education

Researcher: Ozge Karakus Contact e mail: [email protected]

I am conducting a study on Teachers’ Perception about Neuroscience in Education for my Masters

dissertation. I want to define “The knowledge of primary and secondary school teachers about brain

and also their perceptions about the application of neuroscientific research findings in educational

field, in Turkey”. Your voluntary participation is requested.

I would like to record the interview, if you are willing, and also use some parts as a source in the

dissertation report, after transcription of the record. Your responses will be anonymous. Please feel

free to say as much or as little as you want. You can decide not to answer any question, or to stop the

interview any time you want; moreover you have the right to withdraw data from the study before the

submission of the report.

The interview will be in Turkish, however as I will prepare the report in English, I will have to

translate your words from Turkish to English; thus I need to receive an approval from an expert on

both languages. That means an expert in both languages will also read some parts of your comments

and answers except me; but they will not see the names of participants. Besides this, all the

information will be kept confidential. I will keep the data in a secure place. The tapes and transcripts

will become the property of only this project.

This interview is designed to learn first-hand information about this topic. Upon completion of this

project and after the evaluation of the dissertation and the results will be announced, approximately by

the end of October, all data will be destroyed. If you would like, I can inform you about the results,

you can contact by e-mail address above.

Participant’s Agreement:

I am aware that my participation in this interview is voluntary. If, for any reason, at any time, I wish

to stop the interview, I may do so without having to give an explanation. I understand the intent and

purpose of this research.

I am aware the data will be used for a Masters dissertation. I have the right to review, comment on,

and withdraw information prior to the paper’s submission. The data gathered in this study are

confidential and anonymous with respect to my personal identity unless I indicate otherwise. I grant

permission for the use of this information for the master dissertation.

I have read the above form, and, with the understanding that I can withdraw at any time, and for

whatever reason, I consent to participate in today’s interview.

Participant’s signature Date

---------------------------- --------------------------------

92

APPENDIX-6

An Example Transcript

Interview 4

Interviewee: A primary school teacher with twenty-year experience

Interviewer: Thank you for participating this study. You read the consent form, would you

like to ask anything that you want me to explain?

Interviewee: No, thanks. I understand everything.

Interviewer: Well, as you read in the consent form, this study is about neuroscience in

education and this interview will last approximately 20 minutes. In the first part of interview I

will read and ask you 7 statements about brain and its functions, so plase answer either agree,

disagree or do not know. Then I will ask some additional questions about each statement to

understand the reasons behind your responses. Is it clear, can we start?

Interviewee: Yes.

Interviewer: The first one is: ‘Individuals learn better when they receive information in their

preferred learning style (visual, auditory, kinaesthetic).’ Did you hear before? And do you

agree/disagree?

Interviewee: Yes, I heard and I agree.

Interviewer: Why you agree?

Interviewee: Can I give an example?

Interviewer: Yes, sure. You can give examples or share your experiences/observations.

Interviewee: Last year, a new student who could not learned reading-writing in the first year

of his studies, came to my classroom. I was teaching to the first year students and I gave

special attention to him. However I could not teach reading and writing. My own students

who had just started the school, learned in two months. 3 Students out of 40 could not learn.

Then I realised, I just teach visually with visual tools, and if you teach with different

perspectives and tools (i.e. visual, auditory), students can learn through one way which suits

their learning style. However, it is hard to realise children’s learning style quickly, it takes

93

time, sometimes more than a month or two. You need to observe well. It is true, children can

learn better through their learning styles. I can also give example from myself, I can

understand better visually.

Interviewer: Ok, I see. So can we say your experiences and observations in the classroom

provide the knowledge to reach that conclusion?

Interviewee: Yes.

Interviewer: Second one is: ‘Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of

pre-school children.’ Do you agree/disagree?

Interviewee: Yes, agree. For example, when I compare my current students and my students

from previous years I can easily discern the difference between them in terms of their

perception levels. My students from previous years were learning more slowly and their

vocabulary was poorer than my present students. Nowadays, visual stimuli and early

education are more effective than in past times. So students start primary school more

equipped.

Interviewer: Yes this is an important observation, so again you answer through your

experiences, right?

Interviewee: Yes.

Interviewer: The third statement is: ‘Differences in hemispheric dominance can help to

explain individual differences amongst learners.’ Do you agree/disagree?

Interviewee: Yes I heard but I do not remember, what were the abilities of right and left

brain? What was the difference?

Interviewer: It is claimed that the left brain is more ‘logical’ and the right brain is more

‘creative’. Do you think that is true? And this can explain our individual differences?

Interviewee: No, this is not enough to explain our individual differences, I think; I do not

agree. We cannot explain just according to left-right brain differences.

Interviewer: So, what else are important?

94

Interviewee: Environment is quite important, such as school environment. It is not the same a

child’s studying in the city centre and country side. Also, there are different factors like

family, so I do not think so.

Interviewer: Ok, so you do not think that hemispheric dominance can explain individual

differences?

Interviewee: No, I do not agree, it cannot. Not alone, there are other factors which are

significant.

Interviewer: What do you think for the questions I asked so far? Did you answer through

your experiences or you read from somewhere or you learn at university, in general?

Interviewee: For the second statement, I can say that I went a training course about children

with mental retardation, they did mention about individual differences regarding right-left

brain. However, experiences are more important I think because you can compare and justify

your knowledge through your experiences.

Interviewer: I see. The next one is: ‘It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid

supplements (omeg-3, omega-6) have a positive effect on academic achievement.’ Do you

agree?

Interviewee: Yes, I agree.

Interviewer: What did you know before about this statement to reach that conclusion?

Interviewee: I observed from my students that lacking a healthy diet cause difficulty in

learning and that they learn slower. I used omega-3 and omega-6 for my children as well.

Interviewer: Where did you get this information? Did you read from somewhere?

Interviewee: Yes, I read this information from somewhere but I do not remember where.

However, it is easy and possible to come across that information in several websites on the

internet. There are advertisements about omega-3 and omega-6 as well.

Interviewer: Ok. The fifth statement is: ‘Shourt bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve

integration of left and right hemispheric brain function.’ Do you agree/disagree?

Interviewee: Yes, I think it is possible. For example, sometimes I stop teaching during

lessons and tell a story and I want them to draw that story—whatever they imagine according

95

to the story—on a piece of paper. It is a short activity but I think it works and it strengthens

the link between the two hemispheres; because, according to my observations, their drawings

improved over time after these kinds of exercises.

Interviewer: The next one is: ‘We only use 10% of our brains.’ Have you heard before? Do

you agree?

Interviewee: Frankly speaking, I think we use less than 10%. Because I read from

somewhere that in Russia, people can heal diseases with brain/mental power or that they can

move objects with brain power. But I am not sure where I read it. So, if we can use our brain's

full capacity, we can achieve lots of things, such as moving the objects by brain power as with

the Russian example.

Interviewer: The last one is: ‘There are critical periods in childhood after which certain

things can no longer be learned.’ Do you agree/disagree?

Interviewee: Yes, I agree. There is a threshold for all behaviours. If a child could not learn

how to read and write in the first grade, that child cannot learn that behaviour completely,

later. Shall I give an example?

Interviewer: Sure.

Interviewee: For example, I had a student who mingled letters ‘v’ and ‘u’ with each other in

the first grade; I could not change the behaviour for more than 2 years. It is really hard to

change the behaviour after critical periods have passed.or for example if you could not teach

how to they wash their hands and how often they should wash, in the first year, it becomes

much more difficult to teach in the second or third year.

Interviewer: Ok, thank you. Now there are some general questions in the second part of

interview. We talked about 7 statements and what do you think, how each statement affects

the practice in the classroom? Do you use that knowledge (statements) in practice?

Interviewee: Yes, I can say, I use all of them. Especially when I start to teach to first year

students; I try to apply this knowledge to the classroom.

Interviewer: Have you ever come across with neuroscience in education (i.e. brain, its

functions on behaviours and learning) at the university?

96

Interviewee: Partially, but did not get in-detail information. So, I can say, no I have not come

across.

Interviewer: So, have you ever come across with neuroscience in education after you became

a teacher (i.e. among in-service training courses or conferences)?

Interviewee: No.

Interviewer: Are you interested in learning for your profession more? And how do you do

that? How do you improve yourself regarding your profession?

Interviewee: Generally, I try to attend in-service training courses. I read a lot. I like to learn

more about my job. Moreover I talk with my colleagues, we always share our experiences.

Interviewer: Do you think that it is important for teachers to know about brain?

Interviewee: Yes, definitely.

Interviewer: Well, do you think teachers need to be trained about this field?

Interviewee: Yes, definitely we need. The brain is the most important organ with relation to

learning. This field can bring a new perspective to education.

Interviewer: Have you heard about multiple intelligences before? And where?

Interviewee: Yes, I heard in a seminar. It was kind of in-service training session.

Interviewer: Have you heard about learning styles before? And where?

Interviewee: Yes, I heard in the course which was about children with mental retardation.

Interviewer: Well lastly, have you heard about brain gym before?

Interviewee: No I have not.

Interviewer: Ok thank you very much. This is the end of the interview. Thank you for your

sparing time and participation.

97

APPENDIX-7

Table 4.4: 17 General Statements (13 Correct, 4 Incorrect) about Brain with Their Correct

Answers in Brackets and Teachers’ Answers (from Turkey) by Percentages (C = Correct, I =

Incorrect, D.K = Do not Know).

17 General Statements in the Questionnaire Teachers’ Responses

C % I % D.K%

1-We use our brains 24 h a day. (Correct) 79.5 13.7 6.8

2-Boys have bigger brains than girls. (Correct) 40.6 29.5 29.9

3-When a brain region is damaged other parts of the brain can take

up its function. (Correct)

11.2 69.1 19.8

4-The left and right hemispheres of the brain always work together.

(Correct)

23.4 53.2 23.4

5-The brains of boys and girls develop at the same rate. (Incorrect) 25.5 45.3 29.1

6-Brain development has finished by the time children reach

secondary school. (Incorrect)

8.6 74.8 16.5

7-Information is stored in the brain in a network of cells distributed

throughout the brain. (Correct)

60.8 7.2 3.2

8-Learning is not due to the addition of new cells to the brain.

(Correct)

25.9 36.3 37.8

9-Learning occurs through modification of the brains’ neural

connections. (Correct)

44.6 11.9 43.5

10-Academic achievement can be affected by skipping breakfast.

(Correct)

94.6 2.5 2.9

11-Normal development of the human brain involves the birth and

death of brain cells. (Correct)

57.2 13.7 29.1

12-Mental capacity is hereditary and cannot be changed by the

environment or experience. (Incorrect)

12.6 82 5.4

13-Vigorous exercise can improve mental function. (Correct) 52.2 26.3 21.6

14-Circadian rhythms (“body-clock”) shift during adolescence,

causing pupils to be tired during the first lessons of the school day.

(Correct)

50.7 11.2 38.1

15-Production of new connections in the brain can continue into old

age. (Correct)

39.6 16.2 44.2

16-There are sensitive periods in childhood when it’s easier to learn

things. (Correct)

97.1 0.7 2.2

17-When we sleep, the brain shuts down. (Incorrect) 4.3 88.1 7.6

98

APPENDIX-8

Table-4.5: 15 Neuromyths with Correct Answers in Brackets and Teachers’ Responses by

Percentages (sorted by prevalence–from the most common to the least) (C = Correct, I =

Incorrect, D.K = Do not Know).

15 Neuromyths in the Questionnaire Teachers’ Responses

C % I % D.K%

1 - Individuals learn better when they receive information in their

preferred learning style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinaesthetic). (Incorrect)

97.1 1.1 1.8

2 - Environments that are rich in stimulus improve the brains of pre-

school children. (Incorrect)

86.7 6.8 6.5

3 -It has been scientifically proven that fatty acid supplements (omega-

3 and omega-6) have a positive effect on academic achievement.

(Incorrect)

79.1 3.6 17.3

4 - Differences in hemispheric dominance (left brain, right brain) can

help explain individual differences amongst learners. (Incorrect)

78.8 5.4 15.8

5 - Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can improve integration of

left and right hemispheric brain function. (Incorrect)

72.3 2.9 24.8

6 - There are critical periods in childhood after which certain things

can no longer be learned. (Incorrect)

67.3 26.6 6.1

7 - Children must acquire their native language before a second

language is learned. If they do not do so neither language will be fully

acquired. (Incorrect)

58.3 32.4 9.4

8 - Exercises that rehearse co-ordination of motor-perception skills can

improve literacy skills. (Incorrect)

56.8 15.8 27.3

9 - We only use 10% of our brain. (Incorrect) 50.4 31.3 18.3

10 -Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks reduces alertness. (Correct) 45.7 34.9 19.4

11 -Children are less attentive after consuming sugary drinks and/or

snacks. (Incorrect)

43.9 22.3 33.8

12 -Extended rehearsal of some mental processes can change the shape

and structure of some parts of the brain. (Correct)

39.9 20.9 39.2

13 -If pupils do not drink sufficient amounts of water (=6–8 glasses a

day) their brains shrink. (Incorrect)

24.8 35.6 39.6

14 -Learning problems associated with developmental differences in

brain function cannot be remediated by education. (Incorrect)

21.6 64.7 13.7

15 -Individual learners show preferences for the mode in which they

receive information (e.g., visual, auditory, kinaesthetic). (Correct)

94.6 2.5 2.9

99

APPENDIX-9

Table 4.6: 15 Neuromyths about the Brain and Brain Functions about Learning with Correct Answers

in Brackets and Teachers’ Responses from Turkey, The UK and the Netherlands by Percentages (C =

Correct, I = Incorrect, D.K = Do not Know).

15 Neuromyths in the Questionnaire

Turkey UK Netherlands

C% I% D.K% C% I% D.K% C% I% D.K%

1-Individuals learn better when they receive

information in their preferred learning

style (e.g., auditory, visual, kinaesthetic).

(Incorrect)

97.1 1.1 1.8 93 4 3 96 3 1

2-Environments that are rich in stimulus

improve the brains of pre-school children.

(Incorrect)

86.7 6.8 6.5 95 1 4 56 29 15

3-It has been scientifically proven that fatty

acid supplements (omega-3 and omega-6) have

a positive effect on academic achievement.

(Incorrect)

79.1 3.6 17.3 69 12 20 54 16 30

4-Differences in hemispheric dominance (left

brain, right brain) can help explain individual

differences amongst learners. (Incorrect)

78.8 5.4 15.8 91 3 6 86 4 11

5-Short bouts of co-ordination exercises can

improve integration of left and right

hemispheric brain function. (Incorrect)

72.3 2.9 24.8 88 0 12 82 5 13

6-There are critical periods in childhood after

which certain things can no longer be learned.

(Incorrect)

67.3 26.6 6.1 33 53 14 52 38 10

7-Children must acquire their native language

before a second language is learned. If they do

not do so neither language will be fully

acquired. (Incorrect)

58.3 32.4 9.4 7 82 11 36 61 3

8-Exercises that rehearse co-ordination of

motor-perception skills can improve literacy

skills. (Incorrect)

56.8 15.8 27.3 78 3 19 63 11 27

9-We only use 10% of our brain. (Incorrect) 50.4 31.3 18.3 48 26 26 46 42 12

10-Regular drinking of caffeinated drinks

reduces alertness. (Correct)

45.7 34.9 19.4 39 26 35 41 36 23

11-Children are less attentive after consuming

sugary drinks and/or snacks. (Incorrect)

43.9 22.3 33.8 57 24 20 55 24 21

12-Extended rehearsal of some mental

processes can change the shape and structure

of some parts of the brain. (Correct)

39.9 20.9 39.2 69 6 26 58 14 28

13-If pupils do not drink sufficient amounts of

water (=6–8 glasses a day) their brains shrink.

(Incorrect)

24.8 35.6 39.6 29 46 26 16 49 35

14-Learning problems associated with

developmental differences in brain function

cannot be remediated by education. (Incorrect)

21.6 64.7 13.7 16 69 15 19 62 19

15-Individual learners show preferences for

the mode in which they receive

information(e.g., visual, auditory, kinaesthetic).

(Correct)

94.6 2.5 2.9 95 4 2 82 13 5

100