Preliminary examination of progression of Alzheimer's disease in a rural Southern African American...

10
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414 Preliminary examination of progression of Alzheimer’s disease in a rural Southern African American cohort Mark T. Wagner a,, Joy H. Wymer b , Noelle E. Carlozzi a , David Bachman a,c , Aljoeson Walker a , Jacobo Mintzer b,c , Alzheimer Study Group 1 a Department of Neurosciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United States b Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United States c Ralph H. Johnson Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Charleston, SC, United States Abstract African Americans are at significantly increased risk for the development of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), yet are seriously underrep- resented in research trials. Preliminary experiences on a large scale, multi-site, 5-year longitudinal trial investigating the psychometric expression and progression of AD targeting an aging Southern rural cohort of African Americans are reported. Sixty-five participants, ranging from asymptomatic to severely demented, underwent extensive individual diagnostic and psychometric evaluation. Results indicated that cultural factors strongly influenced the data. Recruitment with asymptomatic volunteers were found to have greater educational attainment than other participant groups. Psychomotor measures showed greater impairment in African Americans compared to Caucasians suggesting increased cerebrovascular burden. African Americans’ performance on the Boston Naming Test and the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading tests were significantly different than performance of Caucasian groups. The findings demonstrated that a better understanding of sociocultural factors associated with AD in the African American population may facil- itate the development of primary and secondary preventions, especially when considering the role of cerebrovascular comorbidity which is a modifiable risk factor. © 2007 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: African American; Alzheimer disease; Dementia; Neuropsychological assessment; Cerebrovascular disease Corresponding author at: MUSC-CSB Room 307, 96 Jonathon Lucas Street, Charleston, SC 29425, United States. Tel.: +1 843 792 6941; fax: +1 843 792 9142. E-mail address: [email protected] (M.T. Wagner). 1 R. Joglekar, MD & C. Ogle, LPN, Hospital Medicine Consultants, 819 North Fant St., Anderson, SC, 29621; R. Rubey, MD, Ralph H. Johnson VA, 109 Bee St., Charleston, SC, 29401; M. Stuckey, RN & F. Davis, LPN, Alzheimer’s Research and Clinical Programs, Medical University of South Carolina, 5900 Core Rd., N. Charleston, SC, 29406; V. Hirth, MD, L. Sherriff, RN, CCRC, R. Angle, BS, & T. Tocharoen, BS, Senior Primary Care Practice, Palmetto Health Alliance, University of South Carolina, 9 Medical Park Dr., Columbia, SC, 29203; J. Evans, MD & F. Shupe, RN, ACNP, Comprehensive Neurological Services, 436 W. Palmetto St., Florence, SC, 295402; A. Memmon, MD, B. Whitney, MD, & P. Horne, RN, Regional Senior Health, 118 Dillon Dr., Spartanburg, SC, 29307; M. Ebeling, RA & T. Hulsey, MSPH, ScD, Department of Pediatrics, Division of Epidemiology, Medical University of South Carolina; and S. Cannon, Center on Aging, Medical University of South Carolina, 26 Bee St., Charleston, SC, 29425. 0887-6177/$ – see front matter © 2007 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.014

Transcript of Preliminary examination of progression of Alzheimer's disease in a rural Southern African American...

Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

Preliminary examination of progression ofAlzheimer’s disease in a rural

Southern African American cohort

Mark T. Wagner a,∗, Joy H. Wymer b, Noelle E. Carlozzi a,David Bachman a,c, Aljoeson Walker a, Jacobo Mintzer b,c,

Alzheimer Study Group1

a Department of Neurosciences, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United Statesb Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston, SC, United States

c Ralph H. Johnson Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Charleston, SC, United States

Abstract

African Americans are at significantly increased risk for the development of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), yet are seriously underrep-resented in research trials. Preliminary experiences on a large scale, multi-site, 5-year longitudinal trial investigating the psychometricexpression and progression of AD targeting an aging Southern rural cohort of African Americans are reported. Sixty-five participants,ranging from asymptomatic to severely demented, underwent extensive individual diagnostic and psychometric evaluation. Resultsindicated that cultural factors strongly influenced the data. Recruitment with asymptomatic volunteers were found to have greatereducational attainment than other participant groups. Psychomotor measures showed greater impairment in African Americanscompared to Caucasians suggesting increased cerebrovascular burden. African Americans’ performance on the Boston NamingTest and the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading tests were significantly different than performance of Caucasian groups. The findingsdemonstrated that a better understanding of sociocultural factors associated with AD in the African American population may facil-itate the development of primary and secondary preventions, especially when considering the role of cerebrovascular comorbiditywhich is a modifiable risk factor.© 2007 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: African American; Alzheimer disease; Dementia; Neuropsychological assessment; Cerebrovascular disease

∗ Corresponding author at: MUSC-CSB Room 307, 96 Jonathon Lucas Street, Charleston, SC 29425, United States. Tel.: +1 843 792 6941;fax: +1 843 792 9142.

E-mail address: [email protected] (M.T. Wagner).1 R. Joglekar, MD & C. Ogle, LPN, Hospital Medicine Consultants, 819 North Fant St., Anderson, SC, 29621; R. Rubey, MD, Ralph H. Johnson

VA, 109 Bee St., Charleston, SC, 29401; M. Stuckey, RN & F. Davis, LPN, Alzheimer’s Research and Clinical Programs, Medical Universityof South Carolina, 5900 Core Rd., N. Charleston, SC, 29406; V. Hirth, MD, L. Sherriff, RN, CCRC, R. Angle, BS, & T. Tocharoen, BS, SeniorPrimary Care Practice, Palmetto Health Alliance, University of South Carolina, 9 Medical Park Dr., Columbia, SC, 29203; J. Evans, MD & F.Shupe, RN, ACNP, Comprehensive Neurological Services, 436 W. Palmetto St., Florence, SC, 295402; A. Memmon, MD, B. Whitney, MD, & P.Horne, RN, Regional Senior Health, 118 Dillon Dr., Spartanburg, SC, 29307; M. Ebeling, RA & T. Hulsey, MSPH, ScD, Department of Pediatrics,Division of Epidemiology, Medical University of South Carolina; and S. Cannon, Center on Aging, Medical University of South Carolina, 26 BeeSt., Charleston, SC, 29425.

0887-6177/$ – see front matter © 2007 National Academy of Neuropsychology. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.014

406 M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

The Alzheimer’s Association has identified Alzheimer’s disease (AD) as an emerging public health crisis for AfricanAmericans with a prevalence ranging from 14% to 100% greater than Caucasians. While it is well known that AD isassociated with advanced age, there is mounting evidence that cerebrovascular disease may be a key mechanism intriggering the manifestation of AD (Breteler, Clauss, Grobbee, & Hofman, 1994; Hofman et al., 1997; Skoog et al.,1996). The African American population that resides in South Carolina has disproportional risk factors with a veryhigh incidence of cerebrovascular disease, and thus, represents a unique cohort to examine the potential confoundingrole of cerebrovascular disease in the development and progression of Alzheimer’s dementia. However, this group hasbeen seriously underrepresented in large trials. This is a problem because genetic differences and response to drugsvary significantly by race and ethnicity. For these and other reasons, a large scale, longitudinal, cohort study of theexpression of dementia in the elderly African American population has been initiated.

The coastal region of SC (often referred to as the “Low Country”) offers a remarkable and unique opportunity toconduct such a study. First, this region of the country has a population where there are a disproportional number ofaging African Americans. In addition, African Americans in the Low Country have maintained a remarkable degreeof genetic and cultural homogeneity. Charleston is in the heart of the Low Country and historically emerged as oneof North America’s major slave ports during the Transatlantic slave trade (Franklin & Moss, 1994). From this trade,many slaves from the western coast of Africa were brought to Charleston and then transported to the rest of the state.The result is a strong local African community that still exists today (Franklin & Moss, 1994). An example of thisculture is captured in the life and culture of the African American Gullah population that is over 300 years old (Dewig,1998). The language, music, art, spiritual practices, culinary and medicinal customs, and other practices of this cultureare still present today (Avery Research Center, 2003). For the state’s teaching hospital an important relationship existsbetween the medical community and the African American community with several longitudinal studies completed orunderway.

Numerous studies suggest that dietary patterns of African Americans, especially rural African Americans, differfrom those of Caucasians (Block, Rosenberger, & Patterson, 1988; Kayrooz, Moy, Yanek, & Becker, 1998; Madanet al., 2002; Vitolins, Quandt, Bell, Arcury, & Case, 2002). Although not consistently observed in every study, ingeneral, African Americans consume more high-fat foods (in particular, red meat) and fewer raw vegetables andfish than Caucasians (Block et al.; Gillum, Mussolino, & Madans, 1996; Madan et al.; Vitolins et al.). This dietarypattern of African Americans is associated with an increased risk for high cholesterol, increased insulin resistance, andcerebrovascular disease. Indeed, this region of the country is often referred to as the “stroke belt” and the Low Countryhas been referred to as being located on the buckle of the stroke belt (Lanska, 1993; Lanska & Kryscio, 1994). However,the relationship between ethnicity and diet is complicated by socioeconomic status and education (Lu, Samuels, &Huang, 2002), as well as by geographic proximity to good sources of nutrition (Morland, Wing, & Diez Roux, 2002).

Cerebrovascular disease is associated with cognitive impairment and dementia (Breteler et al., 1994; Hofman et al.,1997; Skoog et al., 1996). The high prevalence rate of cerebrovascular disease among African Americans highlightsthe importance of early detection and adequate cognitive screening. Even after controlling for the confounding factorsof education, cerebrovascular disease and diabetes, recent research has demonstrated that for African Americans thereis a higher incidence of Alzheimer’s disease (Green et al., 2002; Tang et al., 2001). Identifying of dementia is anotherproblem. There are concerns that neuropsychological tests are not culturally sensitive (Jones, 2003; Manly, Byrd,Touradji, & Stern, 2004) and do not take into account level of acculturation and assimilation (Manly, 2005; Miles,2002) and, therefore, may be inappropriate for use with African Americans if the tests do not contain appropriate norms.In particular, performance may be biased against African Americans with low educational attainment (Ginsberg, 2003;Manly, Jacobs, Touradji, Small, & Stern, 2002). Further, African Americans demonstrate less awareness about thefacts of AD and indicate less of a perceived threat about having the disorder (Roberts et al., 2003).

Further, recent studies have suggested that the treatment of vascular risk factors in African Americans may reducethe likelihood of age-associated cognitive impairment or dementia (Richards et al., 2000). Cardio/cerebrovascular riskfactors and high cholesterol are now recognized as playing a potentially pivotal role in the deposition of beta amyloidin the brain, and subsequently, an increased risk of AD. A variety of risk factors linked to cardiovascular disease,including blood pressure, homocysteine levels, APOE genotype, and elevated cholesterol levels, may be linked to therisk of AD (Breteler, 2000; Haan, Shemanaski, Jagust, Manolio, & Kuller, 1999; Seshadri et al., 2002). Evans et al.(2000) have reported a relationship among total cholesterol, APOE genotype, and risk of AD in African Americans.These results suggest that cerebrovascular risk factors, especially total cholesterol, may play an especially importantrole in the risk of cognitive decline and ultimately dementia in African Americans.

M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414 407

In the current sample, the unique advantages of SC’s African American population are utilized (i.e., relatively stablegenetic background, relatively homogeneous cultural background, and established increased risk for cerebrovasculardisease and probable AD) to study this cohort. These advantages should aid in better delineating the multi-factoriallinks between environmental, behavioral, and genetic factors that cause AD.

The issue of culturally appropriate norms and diagnostics is of paramount significance in the study of mild cognitiveimpairment (MCI). The term “mild cognitive impairment” has been given to patients that meet a specific clinical criteriathat falls somewhere in between asymptomatic and dementia (Petersen, 2001; Petersen et al., 1999; Petersen, Stevens,& Ganguli, 2001). The term “amnestic MCI” has been further refined to denote those at specific imminent risk for thedevelopment of dementia of the Alzheimer’s type.

One general problem with the Peterson criteria (Petersen, 2001; Petersen et al., 1999; Petersen et al., 2001) hasbeen that it may fail to identify other individuals at risk for dementia, particularly with comorbid cerebrovascular risk.Another general problem is that the diagnosis of MCI relies heavily on self-report of memory problems. Both issuesare particularly problematic for an African American population because of cultural issues and delays associated withseeking health care as racial and ethnic minorities in the US are less likely to seek healthcare than Caucasians (USDepartment of Health and Human Services, 2001). Further, racial and ethnic minorities are also prone to delay treatmentuntil they experience severe symptoms (US Department of Health and Human Services). There are a number of factorsthat likely contribute to this situation (Brawley & Tejeda, 1995; McAdoo, 1993; McNeilly et al., 2000; Sinclair et al.,2000). To help combat some of these problems, the Peterson criteria have also been refined using neuropsychologicaltests. However, the use of such measures in minority populations has not been reported and is poorly understood.

The objective of this study was to better understand the expression of dementia in this underserved cohort. Asa preliminary investigation, we examined several primary outcome variables. Specifically, we sought to investigatewhether cultural trends may influence study subject recruitment. In this unique cohort at extremely high risk forvascular disease, cerebrovascular burden was operationalized and studied using psychomotor measures as a marker ofcerebrovascular burden. We also examined whether educational achievement, literacy or dysphasia may play a role indetection of cognitive impairment; the specificity of a confrontation naming test (Boston Naming Test) and publishednorms was investigated in this rural African American cohort. Because the early detection of dementia requires a methodto compare current cognitive abilities with premorbid abilities, we examined whether the reading test (Wechsler Testof Adult Reading) can be used to detect early change.

1. Method

1.1. Participants

Asymptomatic and symptomatic older African Americans from a diverse racial and ethnic background who areat risk for dementia and/or dementia progression were identified and recruited for the longitudinal study. This is amulti-center, multi-disciplinary project. Six recruitment sites in South Carolina were chosen to represent the LowCountry. Both symptomatic and control participants were recruited by word of mouth, presentations to cultural leadersand churches, newspaper advertisements, and flyers. Potential participants were evaluated for the inclusion/exclusioncriteria. Inclusion criterion was age 60 or older; exclusion criteria were presence of significant developmental delay,alcohol/drug dependence, active major psychiatric illness, significant medical problem, or interfering sensory impair-ment. Site directors determined whether participants had the decision capacity to provide informed consent. Consentwas provided by the patient’s legal representative according to SC state law for participants deemed to be withoutdecision capacity.

To determine diagnoses, each study investigator presented a potential participant at a bimonthly video consensusconference to determine clinical diagnosis. A multidisciplinary team including neurologists and coordinators fromeach of the six sites, neuropsychologists, psychiatrists and nurses, attended this conference. Participants were clas-sified according to standardized clinical criteria determined at a consensus conference. The diagnostic criteria usedincluded: AD–NINCDS/ADRDA criteria (McKhann et al., 1984), vascular dementia—California criteria (Chui et al.,1992), mixed vascular/AD dementia—California criteria (Chui et al), Lewy body dementia—consensus DLB criteria(McKeith et al., 1996), frontotemporal dementia—consensus FTP dementia criteria (Neary et al., 1999), mild cognitiveimpairment—amended Petersen criteria (Petersen et al., 2001). The Hachinski Ischemia Scale (HIS) as modified byRosen (Rosen, Terry, Fuld, Katzman, & Peck, 1980) was used as a marker of cerebrovascular burden (Moroney et al.,

408 M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

1997). The Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR) was used on each participant to stage symptom severity (Cohen-Mansfield et al., 1996) with the score determined at the video consensus conference. The CDR is a well-established,reliable, and valid diagnostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer type (Morris, 1997; Morris et al.,1989). As participants were entered into the study, they were assigned to annual longitudinal one-year follow-up exams.

1.2. Materials

Extensive demographic, educational, dietary, medical history and neurological exam data were collected on eachparticipant. In addition, all participants had neuropsychological data collected, which is the focus of this paper. Themeasures used are listed below.

The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001) measure was used in an attempt to estimate pre-morbid intellectual functioning and is valid for individuals up to 89 years. Unlike many intellectual and memory abilitiesmeasures, the WTAR assesses reading recognition, which is relatively stable in the presence of the cognitive declinesassociated with normal aging or brain injury, providing our rationale for its use. Further, the WTAR was included toexamine the stability of the so-called “hold” function over time as a function of individual disease progression.

The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer Disease neuropsychological battery (CERAD; Morris et al.,1989) was chosen because of its national and international popularity (Morris et al.). In addition, the CERAD has beenvalidated as culturally fair with established reliability in multiple ethnic populations (Unverzagt et al., 1999; Unverzagtet al., 1996). In fact, education and age only affected test performance to a relatively small degree (less than 15% of thevariance) and gender accounted for less than 2% of the variance in community dwelling, elderly African Americans(Unverzagt, Hall, Hui, & Hendrie, 2004).

Other cognitive measures included Digit Span and Digit-Symbol from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III(WAIS-III; Wechsler, 1997a), Logical Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R; Wechsler, 1987),Orientation and Mental Control from the Wechsler Memory Scale-III (WMS-III; Wechsler, 1997b), Rey ComplexFigure Test and Recognition Trial (Meyers & Meyers, 1995), Controlled Oral Word Association Test (Spreen & Strauss,1998), Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System-Sorting subtest (Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer, 2001), Grip Strength Test,Grooved Pegboard Test, Finger Tapping Test, Trail Making Test-Parts A&B (Reitan & Wolfson, 1985), and the BostonNaming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, Weintraub, & Segal, 1983). The Alzheimer Disease Awareness Interview was usedto measure metacognition (Wagner, O’Connell, & Bachman, 1997; Wagner, Spangenberg, Bachman, O’Connell, 1998;Wagner, Rayls, & Bachman, 1999).

The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI; Cummings et al., 1994) is a caregiver rating scale that assesses 12 neuropsy-chiatric disturbances common in dementia (Cummings et al., 1994; Cummings, 1997). The Informant Questionnaire

Table 1Areas of cognition evaluated by SCADRD neuropsychological battery

Area of cognition Test

Attention/working memory WAIS-III Digit-Symbol, WAIS-III Digit Span, WMS-III Mental Control, Trails AProcessing speed Trails A & B (psychomotor speed), WAIS-III Digit-Symbol (psychomotor speed), Grooved Pegboard

(motor speed), Finger Tapping Test (motor speed), Grip strength (motor),WAIS III- Symbol Search(visuomotor speed)

Memory WMS-R Logical Memory (prose recall), CERAD Word Lists & Constructional PraxisRecall/Recognition, Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial

Language CERAD Animal Naming (Fluency), Controlled Oral Word Association-FAS & Fruits and Vegetables(Fluency), CERAD Boston Naming 15 item, Boston Naming Test, WASI Vocabulary, WTAR

Reasoning/Problem-solving WASI Matrix Reasoning (novel problem-solving), Delis–Kalpan Sorting Test, Trails B (mental flexibility)Spatial Reasoning/Construction WASI Matrix Reasoning (visuospatial), CERAD-Praxis (constructional praxis), Trails B (visuospatial

sequencing)Orientation MMSE-First 10 items, WMS-III Information and OrientationMetacognition Alzheimer Disease Unawareness Interview, Delis–Kaplan Sorting Test, NEOOther Report Neuropsychiatric Inventory, Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly

Note: WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition, WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale-Third Edition, WMS-R: Wechsler MemoryScale-Revised, CERAD: Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer Disease, WASI: Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, WTAR:Wechsler Test of Adult Reading, MMSE: Mini-Mental State Examination, NEO: NEO Personality Inventory.

M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414 409

on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly (IQCODE) was completed by the caregiver (Jorm, 2004). The Geriatric DepressionScale (GDS; Yesavage et al., 1983) is used to detect and rate depression severity. Lastly, for those capable of self-rating,the 60-item NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992) was used in an effort to understand personality traitsof those who might volunteer for such a study as well as a measure of self-reflection/metacognition.

In addition to standard norms that are available to interpret raw test scores, we also employed the newly RevisedComprehensive Norms for an Expanded Halstead-Reitan Battery: Demographically Adjusted NeuropsychologicalNorms for African American and Caucasian Adults Scoring Program (Heaton, Miller, Taylor, Grant, & PAR Staff,2004). However, these norms were not used to interpret the BNT results because they provided information on onlyone BNT index. Since four BNT indices were used and to maintain normative consistency, the Tombaugh and Hubley(1997) norms were used. Please see Table 1 for a summary of the different areas of cognition being assessed and thespecific measures being used to assess these areas.

2. Design and procedure

All participants completed a standard clinical assessment after enrolment. The basis for this standard clinicalassessment was the practice parameter: Diagnosis of Dementia (an evidence-based review), Report of the QualityStandards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology (Knopman et al., 2001). Within 60 days of consent,participants completed a neuropsychological evaluation. A postdoctoral fellow who traveled to each site administeredthe battery for this evaluation. Efforts were made to capture multiple cognitive domains, meta-cognition, motor andpsychomotor function, neuropsychiatric symptomatology, and personality traits.

All participants received annual follow-up evaluations and quarterly telephone calls with a special interest inconfirmation of diagnosis, characterization of disease progression, and monitoring asymptomatic cases at risk forconversion. The participants who do not convert also were intensively studied as “successful agers.” The yearlyassessments permit frequent follow up of participants and maintenance of meaningful contact, yet allow enough timeto elapse to permit the assessment of meaningful change.

3. Results

To date, 65 participants have been enrolled in this study. Participants were 78.5% African American (n = 51) and21.5% Caucasian (n = 14). Age ranged from 61 to 95 years (M = 74.7, SD = 8 years). Participants’ level of educationranged from 4 to 21 years (M = 11.9, SD = 3.8). African Americans and Caucasians did not significantly differ in termsof age and education. Breakdown of diagnoses was as follows: 33.8% controls (n = 22), 23.1% MCI (n = 15), 33.8%dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (n = 22), 4.6% vascular dementia (n = 3), and dementia due to mixed Alzheimer’sdisease and vascular etiology (n = 3). Dementia severity as categorized by CDR rating was CDR = 0 (32%), CDR = 0.5(10%), CDR = 1 (16%), CDR = 2 (16%), and CDR = 3 (26%).

To test the hypothesis that cultural trends may influence study entry behavior, we examined dementia severity as afunction of education. Participants with CDR = 0 (i.e., asymptomatic controls) had achieved a significantly higher levelof education (M = 13.8, SD = 4.0) than participants with CDR = 1, 2, or 3 (i.e., mild, moderate and severe dementia;M = 10.3, SD = 3.8), t = −2.24, p = .03.

To test the idea that African Americans might have greater cerebrovascular burden independent of a diagnosis ofvascular dementia, we used psychomotor measures as an indicator of burden. While there was a clear trend for theAfrican Americans to score higher on the Hachinski Ischemia Scale, there were too few participants to analyze thedata statistically. On psychomotor measures, however, African Americans did perform significantly more poorly thanCaucasians on dominant hand (DH) Grip Strength, t (46.9*) = −2.9, p = .005, nondominant hand (NDH) Grip Strength,t (29.1*) = −2.5, p = .02, and Finger Tapping (DH: t (54) = −2.8, p = .007; NDH: t (55) = −4.2, p = .001). There wasa trend for African American participants to perform more poorly than Caucasian participants on the DH GroovedPegboard Test, t (58) = −1.8, p = .08. There was not a significant difference in NDH group performances, t (58) = −1.1,p = .28 (see Table 2).

In terms of expressive language, African American controls scored significantly lower than the standardizationsample on the Boston Naming Test (BNT): Spontaneous (SR) + Stimulus Cued (SC) responses, t (18) = −4.8, p = .001;Raw Mean = 46.1, SD = 9.14. Compared to Caucasians, African American participants performed significantly morepoorly on this BNT measure, t (57) = −2.5, p = .01. To determine whether these unexpected discrepancies were due

410 M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

Table 2Mean psychomotor raw scores for African Americans and Caucasians

African Americans M (SD) Caucasians M (SD)

Grip Strength: Dominant Hand 26.2 (12.8) 28.4 (10.5)Grip Strength: Nondominant Hand 24.5 (12.9) 25.8 (10.9)Finger Tapping: Dominant Hand 28.7 (11.7) 39.9 (6.2)Finger Tapping: Nondominant Hand 25.0 (9.7) 35.8 (3.9)Grooved Pegboard: Dominant Hand 244.5 (236.4) 130.6 (127.6)Grooved Pegboard: Nondominant Hand 259.0 (282.6) 153.9 (173.3)

Table 3Mean Boston Naming Test raw scores for African Americans and Caucasians

African Americans M (SD) Caucasians M (SD)

SR + SC 32.1 (17.0) 45.9 (12.6)Multiple choice 44.6 (16.1) 56.8 (6.5)(SR + SC)/multiple choice 0.72 (0.18) 0.80 (0.17)

Note: SR: number of correct spontaneous responses, SC: number of correct SR + additional number correct with stimulus cue.

to dysphasia, the following analyses using the BNT Multiple Choice subtest were done. African participants scoredsignificantly lower than Caucasian participants on the BNT Multiple Choice domain, t (50*) = −4.2, p = .001. However,a better measure of naming using the BNT might be interpretation of SR plus SC responses in light of multiple choiceresponse score. Dividing the sum of spontaneous and stimulus cued responses by the sum of spontaneous, stimuluscued, phonemic cued, and multiple choice responses yielded ratios that were not significantly different for AfricanAmerican versus Caucasian groups, t (60) = 1.3, p = .19 (see Table 3) ((*) = d.f. were modified due to unequal variancesbetween groups).

To identify a decline from premorbid functioning, a reliable and valid measure of previous cognitive abilities isneeded. The Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) was assessed as a measure of premorbid functioning for the SouthCarolina African American participants. The WTAR significantly underestimated premorbid IQ for African Americancontrols (M = 89.2; SD = 8.0) when compared with Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) 2-subtest FullScale IQ scores (M = 95.2; SD = 13.6), t (18) = 2.5, p = .02. Additional participants are needed to determine if a similarpattern exists for Caucasian participants.

4. Discussion

This study highlights our initial efforts to investigate AD and related dementias in a genetic and culturally homoge-nous cohort of aging African American at high risk for comorbid cerebrovascular disease. In addition to recruitmentissues and comorbid cerebrovascular disease or risk, this study also highlights the particularly challenging neuropsy-chological issues related to the use of established norms potential associated with the confounding effects of culturaland/or educational variables.

The preliminary results suggest that it is highly likely that this cohort has significant co-morbid cerebrovasculardisease. One major challenge has been determining the degree of cerebrovascular burden in this cohort. Self-reportwas problematic in reliably obtaining Hachinski scores on all study participants. Additionally, because the cost ofindividual MRI scans is prohibitive, we attempted to address this issue by loading the neurocognitive battery withmeasures of psychomotor function. The rationale was that pure AD is associated with little motor or psychomotorinvolvement, especially in the early stages of the disease (Lezak, Howieson, & Loring, 2004). On the other hand, smallvessel angioplasty is associated with greater motoric involvement (Lezak et al.). The preliminary examination of thedata showed that African Americans tend to perform more poorly than Caucasians on tests of psychomotor functioningand cognitive functioning. Data was interpreted using the extended HRB norms. We had no reason to doubt that theextended norms from the HRB were colored by the cultural factors. However, in addition to the possibility that thisfinding represents cerebrovascular burden, peripheral neuropathy might be another possible explanations for thesefindings. If identified differences in psychomotor functioning are in fact a true difference, these results would argue

M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414 411

that there is increased cerebrovascular burden affecting the South Carolina African American participants. Differencesin diet, utilization of health care resources, and genetically transferred medical problems could are modifiable riskfactors and this has received a great deal of attention in the recent literature (for review see Skoog, Kalaria, & Breteler,1999) and might serve as a target for future prevention efforts.

Another finding that has become quite evident during the first year of this study had to do with the use of vocab-ulary as a predictor of premorbid intelligence. This is a critical issue in determining degree of cognitive loss andis vital in defining MCI. In the current data, it would appear that not only amount, but also quality of education isconfounding this cohort’s psychometric performance on both the BNT and WTAR. In terms of differential languageability, African American participants had greater difficulty than Caucasians with vocabulary. Level of acculturationand assimilation, are factors that Miles (2002) and Manly and Jacobs (2002) have reported are associated with testperformance bias in African Americans. It would seem that the best approach to this problem is to develop specific localnorms.

This was illustrated in our examination of the BNT. More specifically, the normative data for the BNT seemedto lack specificity for this sample. Interestingly, when naming was interpreted in terms of correct multiple-choiceresponses, a different pattern emerged—African American performance was not significantly different from Caucasianperformance. If group differences disappear when naming is interpreted as a function of general fund of vocabulary,original differences are more likely a product of different educational opportunities rather than evidence of cognitiveimpairment.

This issue has been discussed by Heaton et al. (2003) who found that demographic variables such as age, education,gender and ethnicity do influence the interpretation of standardized scores and therefore requires corrections in stan-dardized norms to compensate for these confounding influences. However, Manly and her collaborators have extendedthis idea arguing that among ethnic minorities, literacy level is a better predictor than was years of education (Manly,Schupf, Tang, & Stern, 2005; Manly et al., 2002; Manly et al., 1998; Manly et al., 2004). Literacy is often operationallydefined as reading proficiency level which is thought to be a better reflection of quality of education rather than yearsof education. Indeed, Manly (Manly, Miller, Walden, et al.) has shown that accounting for acculturation of AfricanAmericans does seem to improve the diagnostic accuracy of certain neuropsychological tests. Therefore, one approachto the problem of poor specificity of cognitive norms among ethnic minorities has been to develop separate ethnicgroup norms. Manly (2005) has discussed the pros and cons of this approach.

Curiously in this data, the validity of the norms associated the WTAR vocabulary test as a measure of premorbidintelligence in the southern African American population was questionable. The WTAR underestimated premorbid IQfor African American controls suggesting that the norms for this measure of language may not be the best predictor ofpremorbid ability.

Overall, the preliminary data analysis for this study clearly suggested that some of the normative data used tointerpret test results for this African American cohort needs to be re-examined. Further study of this group with largersample sizes and longitudinal follow-up will help unravel some of these issues. To this end, we are in the process ofdeveloping new normative data for this rural Southern geriatric African American cohort by using our asymptomaticcontrols as the reference group and verifying absence of disease by longitudinal confirmation of stable psychometricscores.

In this same vein, we are also most interested in what measures will predict conversion to dementia in the MCIgroup. MCI has been defined as subjective memory complaints, and ideally, also by psychometrically defined standarddeviation decline in episodic memory. However, even when using this criterion, the risk of conversion to AD dementiais often misdiagnosed. We fully anticipate this problem to be magnified in the current cohort and we are beginningto unravel the question with longitudinal data. Specifically, unless premorbid IQ can be more accurately definedindividually, it is our opinion that a simplistic definition of standard deviation decline in memory will fail to accuratelydetect those at risk for conversion because of excessive variance. Efforts are underway to develop better norms forestimating premorbid IQ and we are also investigating the notion of changes in metacognition as the first sign of MCIand have several case examples in the current data set that seem to support this notion. To our knowledge, these issueshave never been studied in this specific cohort.

In terms of recruitment, an interesting trend is beginning to emerge that highlights a national problem in conductingthis type of culturally fair research. It appears to be the case that well educated African Americans are volunteeringas asymptomatic controls. Conversely, it would appear that lesser educated African Americans are coming forth toparticipate, but only after dementia is fairly advanced. We have likewise noted this trend in the clinics.

412 M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

This trend is concerning for several reasons. First, the group of intense interest for therapeutic trials is the preclinicalAD population. Prevailing thought is that this is the stage where interventions will likely have the greatest impact.A challenge for any intervention trial is to recruit those at imminent risk of AD. Therefore, it is ideal if the cohortsample were skewed toward the less impaired. Secondly, it is likely that the better educated African Americans alsohave a lifestyle and diet that is associated with less cerebrovascular burden. Because of this possible factor, the MCIparticipants that convert to AD may not allow the degree of opportunity we envision to study comorbid cerebrovascularburden.

By systematic neuropsychological investigation of this unique cohort, we are beginning to understand the possiblechanging structure of the neurocognitive expression of AD as a function of premorbid education and cerebrovascularburden. We also are beginning to understand trends that we have observed in the clinic that parallel what we are seeingin study recruitment patterns. Further study of longitudinal data will likely offer greater insight in understanding thisnationally under-represented cohort.

References

Avery Research Center (2003). The Walter Pantovic Collection, http://www.cofc.edu/avery/pantovic/afua/gullah.html.Block, G., Rosenberger, W. F., & Patterson, B. H. (1988). Calories, fat and cholesterol: Intake patterns in the us population by race, sex and age.

American Journal of Public Health, 78, 1150–1155.Brawley, O. W., & Tejeda, H. (1995). Minority inclusion in clinical trials. Issues and potential strategies. Journal of the National Cancer Institute

Monograph, 17, 55–57.Breteler, M. M. (2000). Vascular risk factors for AD: An epidemiologic perspective. Neurobiology of Aging, 21, 153–160.Breteler, M. M., Claus, J. J., Grobbee, D. E., & Hofman, A. (1994). Cardiovascular disease and distribution of cognitive function in elderly people:

The Rotterdam Study. British Medicine Journal, 308, 1604–1608.Chui, H. C., Victoroff, J. I., Margolin, D., Jagust, W., Shankle, R., & Katzman, R. (1992). Criteria for the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia

proposed by the State of California AD Diagnostic and Treatment Centers. Neurology, 42, 473–480.Cohen-Mansfield, J., Reisberg, B., Bonnema, H. J., Berg, L., Dastoor, D. P., Pfeffer, R. I., et al. (1996). Staging methods for the assessment of

dementia: Perspectives. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 57, 190–198.Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc..Cummings, J. L. (1997). The Neuropsychiatric Inventory: Assessing psychopathology in dementia patients. Neurology, 48(5 Suppl. 6), S10–S16.Cummings, J. L., Mega, M., Gray, K., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., Carusi, D. A., & Gornbein, J. (1994). The Neuropsychiatric inventory: Compre-

hensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology, 44, 2308–2314.Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System: Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX.Dewig R. (1998). Preservationists from across the country get lesson in Gullah history. Savannah NOW: The Savannah Morning News Electronic

Edition, <http://www.savannahmorningnews.com/stories/102598/CMNgullah.html>.Evans, R. M., Emsley, C. L., Gao, S., Sahota, A., Hall, K. S., Farlow, M. R., et al. (2000). Serum cholesterol, APOE genotype, and the risk of

Alzheimer’s disease: A population-based study of African Americans. Neurology, 54, 240–242.Franklin, J., & Moss, A. (1994). From slavery to freedom. USA: McGraw-Hill, Inc..Gillum, R. F., Mussolino, M. E., & Madans, J. H. (1996). The relationship between fish consumption and stroke incidence. The NHANES I study

epidemiological follow-up study (National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey). Archives of Internal Medicine, 156, 537–542.Ginsberg, J. P. (2003). Wechsler test of adult reading. Applied Neuropsychology, 10, 182–184.Green, R. C., Cupples, L. A., Go, R., Benke, K. S., Edeki, T., Griffith, P. A., et al. (2002). Risk of dementia among White and African American

relatives of patients with Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 287, 329–336.Haan, M., Shemanski, L., Jagust, W. J., Manolio, T. A., & Kuller, L. (1999). The role of APOE e4 in modulating effects of other risk factors for

cognitive decline in elderly persons. Journal of the American Medical Association, 282, 40–46.Heaton, R. K., Miller, W., Taylor, M. J., Grant, I., & PAR staff. (2004). Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery:

Demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults. San Antonio, TX: Psychological AssessmentResources, Inc..

Heaton, R. K., Taylor, M. J., & Manly, J. J. (2003). Demographic effects and use of demographically corrected norms with the WAIS-III andWMS-III. In D. S. Tulsky, D. H. Saklofske, G. J. Chelune, R. K. Heaton, & R. J. Ivnik (Eds.), Clinical interpretation of the WAIS-III andWMS-III (pp. 181–210). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Hofman, A., Ott, A., Breteler, M. M., Bots, M. L., Slooter, A. J., van Harskamp, F., et al. (1997). Atherosclerosis, apolipoprotein E, and prevalenceof dementia and Alzheimer’s disease in the Rotterdam Study. Lancet, 349, 151–154.

Jones, R. N. (2003). Racial bias in the assessment of cognitive functioning of older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 7, 83–102.Jorm, A. F. (2004). The informant questionnaire on cognitive decline in the elderly (IQCODE): A review. International Psychogeriatrics, 16, 1–19.Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea Febiger.Kayrooz, K., Moy, T. F., Yanek, L. R., & Becker, D. M. (1998). Dietary fat patterns in urban AA women. Journal of Community Health, 23, 453–469.Knopman, D. S., DeKosky, S. T., Cummings, J. L., Chui, H., Corey-Bloom, J., Relkin, N., et al. (2001). Practice parameter: Diagnosis of dementia

(an evidence based review). Neurology, 56, 1143–1153.Lanska, D. J. (1993). Geographic distribution of stroke mortality in the United States: 1939–41 to 1979–81. Neurology, 43, 1839–1851.

M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414 413

Lanska, D. J., & Kryscio, R. (1994). The geographic distribution of hospital admissions, Case fatality, and mortality from stroke among Medicareenrollees. Neurology, 44, 1541–1550.

Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., & Loring, D. W. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (4th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Lu, N., Samuels, M. E., & Huang, K. C. (2002). Dietary behavior in relation to socioeconomic characteristics and self-perceived health status.

Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved, 13, 241–257.Madan, A. K., Barden, C. B., Beech, B., Fay, K., Sintich, M., & Beech, D. J. (2002). Self-reported differences in daily raw vegetable intake by

ethnicity in a breast screening program. Journal of the National Medical Association, 94, 894–900.Manly, J. J. (2005). Advantages and disadvantages of separate norms for African Americans. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 270–275.Manly, J. J., Byrd, D. A., Touradji, P., & Stern, Y. (2004). Acculturation, reading level, and neuropsychological test performance among African

American elders. Applied Neuropsychology, 11, 37–46.Manly, J. J., & Jacobs, D. (2002). Future directions in neuropsychological assessment with African Americans. In R. Ferrarro (Ed.), Minority and

cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment (pp. 79–96). Swets & Zeitlinger.Manly, J. J., Jacobs, D. M., Touradji, P., Small, S. A., & Stern, Y. (2002). Reading level attenuates differences in neuropsychological test performance

between African American and White elders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 341–348.Manly, J. J., Miller, S. W., Walden, S., Heaton, R. K., Byrd, D., Relly, J., Velasquez, R. J., Saccuzzo, D. P., & Grant, I. (1998). The effect of

African-American acculturation on neuropsychological test performance in normal and HIV-positive individuals. Journal of the InternationalNeuropsychological Society, 4, 291–302.

Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., Tang, M., & Stern, Y. (2005). Cognitive decline and literacy among ethnically diverse elders. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatryand Neurology, 18, 213–217.

McAdoo, J. (1993). Crime stress, self-esteem, and life satisfaction. In J. Jackson, L. Chatters, & R. J. Taylors (Eds.), Aging in Black America.Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

McKeith, I. G., Galasko, D., Kosaka, K., Perry, E. K., Dickson, D. W., Hansen, L. A., et al. (1996). Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologicdiagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB): Report of the consortium on DLB international workshop. Neurology, 47, 1113–1124.

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of AD: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDAWork Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on AD. Neurology, 34, 939–944.

McNeilly, M., Musick, M., Efland, J. R., Baughman, J. T., Toth, P. S., Saulter, T. D., et al. (2000). Minority populations and psychophysiologicresearch. Challenges in trust building and recruitment. Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 6, 91–112.

Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey complex figure and recognition trial. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Miles, G. T. (2002). Neuropsychological assessment of African Americans. In F. R. Ferraro (Ed.), Studies on neuropsychology, development, and

cognition (pp. 63–77). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers.Morland, K., Wing, S., & Diez Roux, A. (2002). The contextual effect of the local food environment on residents’ diets: The atherosclerosis risk in

communities study. American Journal of Public Health, 92, 1761–1767.Moroney, J. T., Bagiella, E., Desmond, D. W., Hachinski, V. C., Molsa, P. K., Gustafson, L., et al. (1997). Meta-analysis of the Hachinski Ischemia

Score in pathologically verified dementias. Neurology, 49, 1096–1105.Morris, J. C. (1997). Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): A reliable and valid diagnostic and staging measure for dementia of the Alzheimer type.

International Psychogeriatrics, 9, 173–176.Morris, J. C., Heyman, A., Mohs, R. C., Hughes, J. P., Van Belle, G., Fillenbaum, G., et al. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for AD

(CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of AD. Neurology, 39, 1159–1165.Neary, D., Snowden, J. S., Gustafson, L., Passant, U., Stuss, D., Black, S., et al. (1999). Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: A consensus on clinical

diagnostic criteria. Neurology, 51, 1546–1554.Petersen, R. C. (2001). Mild cognitive impairment. Transition from aging to AD. In K. Iqbal, S. S. Sisodia, & B. Winblad (Eds.), AD: advances in

etiology, pathogenesis, and therapeutics. W Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.Petersen, R. C., Smith, G. E., Waring, S. C., Ivnik, R. J., Tangalos, E. G., & Kokmen, E. (1999). Mild cognitive impairment: Clinical characterization

and outcome. Archives of Neurology, 56, 303–308.Petersen, R. C., Stevens, J. C., & Ganguli, M. (2001). Practice parameter: Early detection of dementia: Mild cognitive impairment (an evidence-

based review) Report of the Quality Standards Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. American Academy of Neurology, 56,1133–1142.

Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan neuropsychological test battery. Theory and clinical interpretation. Tuscan: Neuropsy-chology Press.

Richards, S. S., Emsley, C. L., Roberts, J., Murray, M. D., Hall, K., Gao, S., et al. (2000). The association between vascular risk factor-mediatingmedications and cognition and dementia diagnosis in a community-based sample of African-Americans. Journal of the American GeriatricSociety, 48, 1035–1041.

Roberts, J. S., Connell, C. M., Cisewski, D., Hipps, Y. G., Demissie, S., & Green, R. C. (2003). Differences between African Americans and Whitesin their perceptions of Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 17, 19–26.

Rosen, W. G., Terry, R. D., Fuld, P. A., Katzman, R., & Peck, A. (1980). Pathological verification of ischemia score in differentiation of dementias.Annals of Neurology, 7, 486–488.

Seshadri, S., Beiser, A., Selhub, J., Jaques, P. F., Rosenberg, I. H., D’Agostino, R. B., et al. (2002). Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementiaand AD. New England Journal of Medicine, 346, 476–483.

Sinclair, S., Hayes-Reams, P., Myers, H., Hector, F., Allen, W., Hawes-Dawson, J., et al. (2000). Recruiting AAs for health studies: Lessons fromthe Drew-Rand Center on Health and Aging. Journal of Mental Health and Aging, 6, 39–51.

Skoog, I., Lernfelt, B., Landahl, S., Palmertz, B., Andreasson, L. A., Nilsson, L., et al. (1996). 15 year longitudinal study of blood pressure anddementia. Lancet, 347, 1141–1145.

414 M.T. Wagner et al. / Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 22 (2007) 405–414

Skoog, I., Kalaria, R. N., & Breteler, M. (1999). Vascular factors and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Disease & Associated Disorders, 3(Suppl. 3),S106–S114.

Spreen, O., & Strauss, E. A. (1998). A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (2nd ed.). London: OxfordUniversity Press.

Tang, M. X., Cross, P., Andrews, H., Jacobs, D. M., Small, S., Bell, K., et al. (2001). Incidence of AD in African-Americans, Caribbean Hispanics,and Caucasians in northern Manhattan. Neurology, 56, 49–56.

Tombaugh, T. N., & Hubley, A. N. (1997). The 60-item Boston Naming Test: Norms for cognitively intact adults aged 25–88 years. Journal ofClinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 922–932.

Unverzagt, F. W., Hall, K. S., Hui, S. L., & Hendrie, H. C. (2004). Normative data on the CERAD neuropsychological battery in elderly AAsperformance in clinical practice. In F. R. Ferraro (Ed.), Minority and cross-cultural aspects of neuropsychological assessment. Linas BieliauskasSwets & Zeitlinger Publishers.

Unverzagt, F. W., Hall, K. S., Torke, A. M., Rediger, J. D., Mercado, N., Gureje, O., et al. (1996). Effects of age, education, and gender on CERADneuropsychological test performance in an African-American sample. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 10, 180–190.

Unverzagt, F. W., Morgan, O., Thesiger, C., Eldemire, D., Luseko, J., Pokuri, S., et al. (1999). Clinical utility of the CERAD neuropsychologicalbattery in elderly Jamaicans. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 5, 255–259.

US Department of Health and Human Services. (2001). Mental Health: Culture, Race, and Ethnicity-A Supplement to Mental Health: A Report of theSurgeon General. Rockville, MD: US Department of Health and Human Services, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration,Center for Mental Health Services.

Vitolins, M. Z., Quandt, S. A., Bell, R. A., Arcury, T. A., & Case, L. D. (2002). Quality of diets consumed by older rural adults. Journal of RuralHealth, 18, 49–56.

Wagner, M. T., O’Connell, P., & Bachman, D. L. (1997). Unawareness of cognitive deficit in AD and related dementias. AD and Associated Disorders,11, 125–131.

Wagner, M. T., Rayls, K. R., & Bachman, D. L. (1999). The measurement of unawareness of cognitive loss and its application in AD. Research andPractice in Alzheimer’s Disease, 2, 162–167.

Wagner, M. T., Spangenberg, K. B., Bachman, D. L., & O’Connell, P. (1998). Unawareness of cognitive deficit in Alzheimer disease. NeuropsychologyAbstracts, 7, 34.

Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler memory scale-revised. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Wechsler, D. (1997a). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Wechsler, D. (1997b). Wechsler memory scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler test of adult reading. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., et al. (1983). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale : A preliminary report.

Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17, 37–49.