Peacebuilding Needs

34
Mari Fitzduff NEUROSCIENCE FOR THE PEACEBUILDER An introduction to

Transcript of Peacebuilding Needs

Mari Fitzdu�

NEUROSCIENCE FOR THEPEACEBUILDER

An introduction to

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  1  

 

         Section  1:     Introduction:  Why  Neuroscience?           Pg.    2      Section2:     The  Elephant  and  the  Rider:  Who  is  in  charge?         Pg.    7    Section3:     Vive  La  Difference:  How  Brains  differ.         Pg.    9      Section  4:     Us  and  Them:  Who  is  my  neighbor  in  today’s  world?     Pg.    12    Section  5:       How  we  fool  others  and  ourselves:  The  Believing  Brain.    Pg.  17    Section  6:   Questions  for  the  Peacebuilder              Pg.  21        Bibliography  and  Resources                  Pg.  22    

 

 

 

 

                   

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  2  

Section  1:    Introduction:    Why  Neuroscience  ?    ‘For   the  mind  of  man   is   far   from   the  nature   of   a   clear  and   equal   glass,  wherein   the  beams  of  things  should  reflect  according  to  their  true  incidence:  nay  it  is  rather  like  an  enchanted  glass,  full  of  superstition  and  importure,  if  it  be  not  delivered  and  reduced.’  Francis  bacon  Novum  Organum  (1620)  

 ‘We  all  want  peace  –  don’t  we?  ‘    (Prof.  Gareth  Fitzgerald,  Prime  Minister  of  Republic  of  Ireland,  1981-­‐1987    

This   paper   began  many   years   ago   in   the  midst   of   the   conflict   in   Northern  Ireland.  I  was  interested  in  how  one  could  effect  change,  particularly  in  ending  the  use   of   violence   to   achieve   social   and   political   ends.   I   knew   that   there  were   some  people  who  had  once  used  bombs  or  bullets   to   try   to  effect  change,  but  who  were  now  instead  engaged  in  dialogue  work,  rather  than  violence,  to  effect  their  desired  changes.   I  wondered  what  had  brought   about   the   change   in   their   behavior,   and   if  there  were  lessons  to  be  learned  from  them  for  those  of  us  who  were  interested  in  peacebuilding.  I  therefore  I  chose  this  focus  for  my  doctoral  work.  Somewhat  to  my  surprise,  I  discovered  that  there  were  very  few  people  who  had  been  reasoned  out  of   their   support   for   violence   –   by   far   the   majority   were   those   whose   minds   and  actions   had   been   changed   through   experiences   that   had   created   emotional  dissonance   for   them  and   their   lives.   I  was  also  struck  by   the   fact   that  most  of  our  existing   change   programs   were   focused   on   a   belief   in   the   use   of   argument   and  reason,  both  through  public  debate,  and  through  a  belief  in  the  processes  of  contact  to  undertake  such  discussions.    

Subsequently,   again  and  again,   through  my  work  as   an  active  policy  maker  and  funder  of  conflict  resolution  work  in  the  1990’s,  I  came  to  understand  how  little  part   reason   plays   in   social   conflicts.   I  met   Loyalist/Protestant   paramilitaries  who  believed  that  the  Battle  of  the  Boyne,  an  icon  of  Protestant  victory  that  took  place  in  1690,   was   enshrined   in   the   Christian   Bible.   Their   ignorance   of   actual   facts   was  palpable   –   just   like   that   of   the  many   foreign   young  men   now   fighting   against   the  government  in  Syria,  some  of  whom  do  not  even  appear  to  know  where  Syria  is,  or  which  side  they  are  on,  let  alone  know  or  understand  the  issues  or  the  actors  in  the  conflict.    

For  decades  I  have  continued  to  wonder  at  most  groups  capacity  to  believe  that  their  violence  is  justified  –  and  the  other  sides  violence  is  not.    At  their  ability  to  inevitably,   often   somewhat   tortuously,   find   ways   to   blame   others   for   what   had  happened   –   even   when   it   was   their   bombs   that   had   killed   and   maimed   women,  children  and  bystanders.    I  have  been  particularly  disconcerted  and  dismayed  at  the  short  -­‐  term  approaches  adopted  for  political  gain  by  so  many  of  our  politicians  and  governments  around  the  world,  despite  the  fact  that  many  of  their  strategies  almost  directly   and   inevitably   lead   to   communal   violence.   I   have   also   wondered   at   the  power   of   fundamentalist   ideologies   of   either   a   political   or   religious   nature   that  possess  the  minds  and  hearts  of  people  and  their  groups,  often  beyond  their  own  life,  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  3  

as  well  as  that  of  many  others.  And  I  have  frequently  caught  myself  cynical  about  the  belief  that  ‘of  course  everyone  wants  peace,  don’t  they?”    as  was  once  said  to  me  by  a  gentle  and  intelligent  professor  prime  minister  from  the  Republic  of  Ireland.    

Through  pondering  such  challenges,  I  have  come  to  believe  that  the  tenets  we  hold  about  how  to  bring  an  end  to  conflict  often  do  not  add  up,  nor  explain  the  many  seeming   irrationalities   and   inconsistencies   that   are   part   and   parcel   of   our   wars.    Through  my  work  as  a  social  psychologist,  I  have  long  been  aware  of  e.g.  the  effect  of  horizontal   inequalities  on  a   community’s   grievances,   and   the   importance  of   group  belonging,   chosen   traumas   and   intolerance   in   shaping   our   conflict   histories.   But  even   such   frameworks   left  many   puzzles   for  me   in  my   understandings   about   the  ubiquity  of  so  many  societal  conflicts.    

It  has  been  through  a  relatively  new  science  -­‐  or  parts  of  other  sciences  -­‐  that  I  came  to  see  more  and  more  of  the  confusions  I  had  faced  in  a  different  light,  and  a  light  that  has  somewhat  helped  explain  for  me  so  much  that  had  been  a  puzzle  to  me  in  my  previous  work.  These  were  the  processes  associated  with  emerging  ideas  that  question  whether  or   the  not   the  way   in  which  our  minds  had  been  physically  and  hormonally   shaped   by   the   exigencies   of   our   development   through   evolution   have  left  us  with   some   legacies  of  which  many  of  us   are  unaware,  but  which  appear   to  help   foment   violence.   Many   of   these   processes   are   currently   being   studied   by  businesses,   governments   and   others   for   their   possible   use   in   shaping   human  behavior.   These   relatively   new   fields   are   called   variously   e.g.   neuroscience,  neuropolitics,   neurobiology,   neuropsychology,   genopolitics,   political   physiology,  political   psychology,   behavioral   genetics,   and   cognitive   neuroscience.   The  understandings   they   suggest   about   the   human   brain   have   been   in   use   for   over   a  decade  now  by  economists,   and  by  businesses   such  as  Google  who  are   (rightly  or  wrongly)   using   neuroeconomics   and   neuromarketing   as   part   of   their   business  strategies.      

Such   utilization   is   spreading   through   other   fields.   There   is   now   e.g.  neurotheology,   utilized   by   those   studying   the   power   of   theism,   and   neurolaw,   by  those  wishing  to  understand  the  effective  possibilities  of  legal  processes.  These  new  fields  are  about  understanding  what  there  may  be  in  the  hardware,  or  software,  in  the   genes   or   the   biology   of   the  mind   and   the  body   that   affects   how  we   think   and  behave,  and  how  to  use  such  understanding  to  e.g.  sell  goods  more  effectively,  or  to  ‘nudge’  people  into  better  public  behavior.    One  interesting  example  of  the  latter  is  the   UK   cabinet   office’s   behavioral   insights   team,   usually   called   the   ‘Nudge’   unit,  which  was  set  up   in  2010   to   find  better  ways   to   look  at  policy   tweaks   that  would  help  the  quirks  of  our  neural  frameworks  work  better  for  us  -­‐  and  that  would  also  save   public   money.     While,   understandably,   some   may   find   the   use   of   such  techniques   somewhat   ethically   dubious,   they   have   been   used   very   effectively   to    increase   body   organ   donations   by   an   extra   100,000   in   the  UK,   by   establishing   an  opt-­‐out   scheme,   rather   than   an   opt-­‐in   scheme,   thus   demonstrating   how   such  understandings  can  bring  useful  benefits  to  what  is  a  public  good.  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  4  

As  yet,  in  the  field  of  peacebuilding,  we  have  not,  in  any  organized  way,  begun  to  try  and  investigate  whether  or  not  some  of  these  new  understandings  may  help  us   to   better   address   some   of   the   neural   forces   that   we   face   as   human   beings   in  trying  to  better  manage  conflicts  and  wars.  These  would  involve  investigating  issues  of  genes  and  biology,  and   the  study  of  bio-­‐physical  correlates  of  political  attitudes  and  behavior,  that  may  have  effected  road  blocks  and  blind  spots  for  many  of  us,  and  that  appear  to  make  it  more  difficult  for  us  to  end  our  wars.  Perhaps,  just  perhaps,  there  may  be  useful   grist   for  our   learning  as  peacebuilders,   and  our   evaluation  of  such   work,   in   such   investigations,   and   this   paper   is   an   attempt   to   begin   that  conversation.  

I  am  aware   that   in  beginning   it,   I  am  facing  at   least   three  major  challenges.  The  first  is  that  in  focusing  on  such  understandings  we  may  turn  the  spotlight  away  from   structural   and   societal   issues   that   are   unfair,   that   exclude,   and   that   oppress  certain  groups,  and  which  make  their  use  of  violence  understandable,  if  not  justified.  Any  study  of  my  policy  work,  and  my  publications  throughout  the  years  in  which  I  have   been   involved   in   this   work   will   show   that   my   very   first   concern   is   that  organizations,   nations,   regions   etc.  who  oppress   or   exclude   certain   groups   should  urgently  address   such   structural   issues,   as   they  are  often   the  abiding  backdrop   to  the  causes  of  many,   if  not  most,  of  our  wars  today.     Issues  of  psycho/sociocultural  understandings,  and  any  programs   that  are  based  upon  an  understanding  of   such,  will  be   ineffective   in   the   long  term  if  structural   issues  are  not  addressed.   I   remain  committed   to   that   belief,   and   the   research   that   follows   in   this   paper   will   in   fact  underline  the  reality  that  structural  contexts,  for  better  or  worse,  often  define  how  we  behave  to  each  other  as  groups  of  human  beings.    

The   second   is   the   fact   that   the   legacy   of   scientific   racism   i.e.   the   use   of  scientific   techniques   and   hypotheses   to   support   or   justify   presumptions   of   racial  inferiority,  or  racial  superiority,  is  still  with  us.  Many  within  our  various  fields  were,  and  still  are,  rightly  horrified  about  the  abuse  of  genetic  and  social  data  that  was  the  legacy  of   the  19th   and  20th   centuries,   and   to  a   certain  extent   is   still  with  us   today,  although  rarely  openly   justified.   I   am   from  a  group   that  has  suffered   for  centuries  from  being  portrayed  as  apes  in  the  popular  European  and  UK  press  of  the  19th  and  20th  centuries.  A  cartoon  published  in  Life  Magazine  on  May  11th,  1893  depicts  the  apes  in  the  zoo  who  are  sad  that  they  get  called  by  Irish  names  -­‐  which  they  feel  is  to  their  detriment!    However,  none  of  the  research  that  follows  will  give  any  sway  or  ballast   to   anyone   interested   in   eliciting   such   differences   for   reasons   of  discrimination.  Alas,  the  contrary  is  true  –  we  are  all  of  us  indicted  for  the  instincts  and  the  confusion  that  we  often  bring  to  bear  upon  the  situations  in  which  we  work,  and  strive  to  build  peace.    

The   third  challenge   is  one   that   I   concede   is  very  valid   indeed,  and   it   is  one  that   almost   prevented   me   from   writing   this   paper.   The   fact   is   that   much   of   the  research   that   I   write   about   in   this   paper   is   very   tentative.   The   advantages   of  neuroscience  have  been  much   touted   for   its   ability   to   look  under   the  hood  of   our  brains,  and  perhaps   to  help  us   to  see  some  of   the  mechanisms  and  processes   that  can  help  to  create  and  perpetuate  conflict.  However  the  reality   is  that  many  of  the  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  5  

mechanisms   used   to   measure   such   thoughts   and   behavior,   such   as   fMRI  neuroimaging,   are   still   in   their   infancy.   There   are   many   who   will   contest   the  explanatory  power  of  processes  like  brain  scans,  and  hormonal  measuring,  and  who  suggest   that   what   we   can   learn   from   current   research   in   neuroscience   has   been  vastly  overestimated.   Some   researchers  are   concerned   that   the  general  public  has  been   seduced   into   believing   that   any   study,   research   article,   or   news   report,  accompanied  by  a  brain  image  or  two,  is  reliable  and  scientific,  not  withstanding  the  tentative  nature  of  the  research.  In  fairness,  I  have  often  found  that  the  researchers  themselves  often  acknowledge  the  tentative  nature  of  their  findings.    

Whole  articles,  and  even  books,  have  been  written  about  many  of  the  issues  mentioned   in   the   paper.   I   have   included  many   of   these   sources   at   the   end   of   the  paper  so  that  people  can  follow  up  and  make  their  own  judgments  about  what  I,  no  doubt  coming   from  my  own  biases,  have  chosen   to  pinpoint  as  possibly  useful   for  our  work.  And  so  new  is  the  field  that  much  of  what  is  contained  within  the  paper  will   inevitably   change   as   the   research   continues   to  multiply.    As  Wikipedia  would  say  ‘This  article  has  multiple  issues  -­‐please  help  improve  it  !’  

We  also  need  to  remember  that  most  of   the  research  outlined   in  this  paper  has  been  conducted  by  and  on  what  are  called  WEIRD  people  i.e.  Western,  Educated,  Industrialized,  Rich  and  Democratic,  although  some  researchers  have  ventured  into  comparative   waters.   Such   limitation   in   terms   of   constituencies   studied   brings   its  own  biases.    

Despite  such  reservations,  I  believe  that  while  there  may  be  little  consensus  as  yet  on  many  of  the  details  of  the  research,  what  is  not  contentious  is  that  there  is  an   increasing  recognition  of  an   interdependence  between  the  genetic  and  physical  aspects   of   our   social   and   political   behavior   that   should   perhaps   be   taken   into  account  alongside  the  social  and  environmental  factors  that  influence  us  throughout  the  course  of  our  lives.    

A  few  caveats.  I  have  tried  to  bring  to  bear  a  non-­‐judgmental  approach  to  the  differences   that   appear   to  be   evident  between  human  beings   and  e.g.   their  neural  architectures.    As   the   research   shows,  people   and  groups  are   different   in   the  way  they  think,  process  information,  the  kinds  of  emotions  they  bring  to  bear  upon    decisions,  the  control  they  have  over  their  emotions,  the  needs  they  have  to  belong  to  a  group,  what  they  prioritize  as  values,  the  importance  of  ideology  in  their  lives,  the   way   they   identify   enemies,   the   way   they   see   facts,   what   they   remember   and  forget,   their   fear   and   suspicions   of   out   groups,   their   need   for   leaders,   etc.   These  differences   are   neither   good,   nor   bad.     They   just   are   –   and   they   exist   possibly  because  somewhere  along  the  line  such  differences,  and  a  mix  of  such  differences,    have  been  important  for  individual  and  group  survival.    What  is  important  I  believe  is  that  we  understand  these  differences  and  take  them  into  account  in  our  work.    

As  noted  before,  in  attempting  to  understand  these  various  neurological  and  biological  differences,  and  their  effects  on   individual  and  group  behavior,   I  am  not  suggesting   that  our   task   should  be   to   end  wars  by   focusing  on  e.g.   drugs   that   can  change   the   dispositions   of   groups   (although   I   confess   I   have   sometimes   been  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  6  

tempted   to   suggest   such  –  valium   in   some  water   systems  has   come   into  my  mind  occasionally!)   or   the   development   of   physical   processes   that   can   manipulate   our  brains   in   some   fashion.   What   is   important   to   remember   is   that   wars   start   not  because   of   such   differences,   but   because   such   differences   come   into   play   through  manipulation   and   violence   within   a   situation   where   people   feel   unequal   and  excluded.  Most  of  our  recent  and  current  wars  have  come  about  because  of  issues  of  inequality  or  exclusion  e.g.  Syria,  Iraq,  Sri  Lanka,  Philippines,  Vietnam,  Ukraine,    Kenya,  etc.    If  such  contexts  had  been  handled  differently,  the  individual  and  group  differences  noted  in  the  research  might  just  be  part  of  the  normal  grist  of  a  society  that  is  blessed  with  different  characters  and  approaches  to  the  social  issues  that  are  the  rule  in  most  peaceful  societies.    

Finally,   and   most   importantly,   there   is   nothing   determinist   about   what   is  revealed   by   the   research   in   this   paper.   Brains   can   be   relatively   plastic   in   their  nature.  They  can  change  both  their  hardware  and  software,  even  though  this  can  be  difficult   and   take   much   time,   depending   on   what   has   already   been   established  biologically  and  environmentally  in  terms  of  what  are  often  unconscious  tendencies.  The  memory  parts  of   the  brains  of  both   taxi  drivers,   and  of  waitresses  have  been  shown  to  grow  bigger  because  of  the  increased  activity  necessitated  by  their  work  focus.   Buddhist   monks  who   do  compassion   meditation  have   been   shown   to  modulate  their  amygdala,  the  part  of  the  brain  that  is  involved  with  the  experiencing  of  emotions,  during  their  practice.  It  has  also  been  suggested  that  increased  activity  in  the  amygdala  following  compassion-­‐oriented  meditation  may  contribute  to  social  connectedness.  And  evidence  of  increased  empathy  in  our  mirror  neurons  has  been  shown  to  be  possible  to  achieve  by  dialogue  processes  between  opposing  sides  in  a  conflict.      

 What   such  plasticity   suggests   is   that  while  we  are,  both  as   individuals  and  groups,   often   predisposed   to   respond   to   conflicts   in   certain   ways   that   are   more  sympathetic   to   our   genetic   and   biological   make-­‐up,   as   well   as   our   social   and  environmental   history,   we   are   not   trapped   by   such   characteristics.   Predisposition  does  not  mean  predetermination  –  there  is  no  individual  or  group  that  cannot  change    its   behavior   towards   another   individual   or   group.   But   there   are   many   situations  where  more  effective  strategies  for  peacebuilding  will  need  theories  of  change  based  on  a  more  thorough  understanding  of  the  study  of  bio-­‐physical  correlates  of  social  and  political  attitudes,  and  the  attendant  behavior  involved  in  such  correlates.  How   can,   and   how   should  we   use   such   insights   in   helping   us   to   better   effect   our  work  ?    Our  attention  to  such  may  help  us  achieve  more  thoughtful  approaches  than  hitherto   to   our   work   as   peacebuilders,   given   the   understandings   that   are   being  revealed  to  us  by  the  nascent  field  of  neuroscience.                

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  7  

Section  2:  The  Rider  and  the  Elephant:  Who  is  in  charge  in  our  minds?    In  our  heads  we  have  a   rational   charioteer  who  has   to   rein   in  an  unruly  horse   that  "barely  yields  to  horsewhip  and  goad  combined."  Plato.    

Who  is  in  charge  of  our  minds?    ‘Us’  of  course  we  will  say  –  but  which  us?  Our  brains  have  many  activity  centers  which  have  different  functions  and  are  connected  by   chemical   impulses.   These   impulses   transfer   information   through   the   cells   in  these  brain  regions,  sometimes  at  a  speed  of  250  miles  per  second.  There  appear  to  be  two  regions  of  the  brain  that  are  important  for  peacebuilders  to  know  about,  and  how   they   function.   These   are   the   parts   that   process   our   automatic/intuitive  impulses  and  the  part  that  deals  with  our  conscious/reasoned  processes.

The   first   is   the   size   of   an   almond   and   called   the   amygdala   the   part   of   our  brain  that  deals  with  our  senses,  with  our  memories,  and  with  our  emotions  such  as  pleasure  or  fear.  Its  processes  are  automatic,  sometimes  called  intuitive  or  implicit  processes,   and   are   impulsive   or   instinctive   in   nature    which  means   that  we   have  little   choice   about   feeling   them.     This   part   of   our   brain   is   often   called   the   old,   or  “lizard”  brain.  It  is  the  deepest  part  of  the  brain  and  is  reckoned  to  be  at  least  60-­‐200  million  years  old.  It  records  memories  of  behaviors,  which  have  produced  feelings  of  good   or   bad   experiences   in   our   lifetime,   and   in   those   of   our   ancestors,   and   is  responsible  for  our  emotions.  It  exerts  a  very  strong  influence  on  our  choices,  often  at   an   unconscious   level.   Those   of   us   who   have   bad   tempers,   or   difficulty   in  restraining  our  eating,  or  other  appetites,  can  understand  its  power.  

Another   important   section   of   the   brain,   is   the   prefrontal   cortex   which    concerns  itself  with  conscious/rational  processes,  and  is  the  part  that  contributes  to  more  analytic,  logical  and  thoughtful  responses  to  a  situation.  It  is  considered  to  be  the  ‘new’  brain  and  is  deemed  responsible  for  the  development  of  human  language,  for  abstract  thought,  for  our  imaginations,  for  reasoning,  and  our  consciousness.  The  amygdala  conveys  our   feelings   to   the   frontal  cortex,   the  brain  region   that  controls  thinking  and  planning,  and  such  feelings  then  affect  our  rational  processes.  

 These  ‘emotional’  and  ‘reasoning’  minds  coexist  uneasily.  This  challenge  has  

been  well  captured  by  Haidt  in  his  analogy  of  the  Elephant  and  the  Rider,  where  our  emotional  side  is  the  Elephant  and  our  rational,  reasonable  side  is  the  Rider.    The  Rider  is  perched  atop  the  Elephant,  and  seems  to  be  the  leader,  but  her/his  control  is  precarious  because  the  Rider  is  so  small  and  so  new  relative  to  the  much  older    and   deeper   Elephant.     Anytime   the   Elephant   and   the   Rider   disagree   about   which  direction  to  go,  the  amygdala/Elephant  part  of  our  brain  is  likely  to  win,  unless  we  are   able   to   bring   our   Prefrontal   Cortex/Reasoning   to   bear   strongly   upon   our  decisions.    

 The  amygdala  is  particularly  likely  to  come  to  the  fore  if  we  are  under  stress  

which  then  makes  it  more  likely  for  our  emotions  to  bypass  our  thinking  cortex  and  to   return   to   thinking   and   acting   according   to   the   “older”   brain   drives.   The  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  8  

predominance   of   intuitive/emotive   thinking   versus   cognitive   reasoning,   or   ‘fast  versus   slow   thinking’   as   it   has   been   termed   recently,   is   particularly   evident   and  automatic   in   situations   where   fear   is   a   factor,   which   is   an   emotion   that   is   very  present   in   many   of   the   contexts   we   deal   with   as   peacebuilders.   Such   amygdala  feelings   come   not   only   from   current   situations,   but   in   many   cases   have   been  generated   and   fostered   by  many  decades   of   conflict,   and   are   often   the   product   of  many  generations  histories  of  emotions.    

 We  are  all  of  us,  no  matter  how  well  educated,  subject  to  the  strength  of  such  

emotions.  Those  of  us  who  have  ever  been  in  riot  situations  have  noted  how  quickly  such  emotions  come  to  the   fore,  and  they  are  palpably   in  evidence   in  situations  of  genocide.     Following   such  violence,  people  often  wonder  at   themselves  and  at   the  atrocities  they  may  have  committed,  and  are  confused  because  they  did  not  believe  that  such  behavior  was  part  of  their  character.  Following  the  Summer  2011  riots  in  Britain,   it   was   evident   in   the   court   appearances   that   followed   that   many   young,  middle  class  teenagers,  who  would  normally  eschew  such  riotous  behaviors,  found  themselves   actively   caught   in   perpetrating   street   violence.   Many   of   them   looking  back   say   they   saw   the   riots   as   ‘some   form  of  midsummer  madness,   a   kind  of   bad  dream’,  and  completely  out  of  their  ‘real’  character.    

 Unfortunately,   many   of   us   fail   to   understand   how   precarious   is   the  

conscious/rational   side   of   our   brains,   until   we   find   ourselves   in   a   context   that  unleashes   the   forces   of   our   amygdala.     In   situations   of   conflict   and   violence,   and  often  preceding   such,  where   our   families,   communities   and  our  people   feel   under  stress,   fear  and  group  processes  often  overtake  our  emotions.  These  emotions  can  be  extremely  hard  to  control,  particularly  at  group  level.     It  has  been  suggested  by  Greene   that   the   human   brain   is   like   a   dual   mode   camera   with   both   automatic  settings   and   a  manual  mode,  which   is   the  brains   trade  off   between   efficiency   and  flexibility  i.e.  the  automatic  settings  for  emotions  are  efficient  but  inflexible,  and  the  manual  mode   is   reasoning,   which   can   be   shaped   by   emotion,   but   not   necessarily  dictated  to  by  it  unless  we  can  bring  sufficient  force  to  bear  upon  it  by  our  conscious  rational  selves.      

The  upshot  of  such  precariousness  between  fear,  emotion,  and  reasoning   is  that  for  change  to  happen,  we  need  to  be  both  emotionally  and  rationally  engaged.  It  is  not  enough  for  people  to   intellectually  understand  that  they  should  take  actions  that   can   end   a   conflict.   Many   of   us   have   filing   cases   full   of   possible   ‘rational’  approaches  to  all  of  the  conflicts  in  which  we  are  working.    However,  for  people  to  actually  approach  that  filing  cabinet,  and  its  possible  solutions,  they  also  need  to  be  emotionally  motivated  to  do  so,  which  is  often  a  harder  process  to  manufacture  than  any  reasoning  in  favor  of  particular  solutions.    

 In  basing  our  work  on  rational/logical   thinking,  we  often  fail   to  understand  

how   little   actual   sway   such   thinking   has   on   the   actors   concerned,   and   on   their  constituencies  in  the  field.  Time  and  again  we  see  groups  continue  fighting  beyond  much  apparent  reasonable  gain  for  all  involved.  Peace  agreements  fall  apart  because,  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  9  

although  the  cognitive  skills  of  those  involved  have  crafted  clever  political  and  social  compromises,  the  necessary  attendant  emotions  often  destroy  the  agreements  when  they   return   to   their   land   or   their   people.   And   when   crises   arise   in   the  implementation  of  such  agreements,  the  emotions  attendant  upon  such  crises  often  return   leaders   and   their   constituencies   back   to   the   emotions   that   have   helped   to  kindle  the  violence  in  the  first  place.  Hence  the  number  of  peace  agreements  that  fall  apart.    

 An  attendant  problem  is  that  most  of  the  research  on  conflict  is  undertaken  

by   academics   many   of   whom   who   often   fail   to   appreciate   the   strength   of   the  amygdala   factor.     Many   of   them   still   function   within   a   traditional   international  ‘realist’  politics  framework,  which  deals  with  so  called  ‘rational’  actors,  and  ‘rational’  states,  and  they  fail  to  appreciate,  or  have  the  vocabulary,  to  deal  with  the  emotional  factors,  which  is  probably  the  dominant  factor  in  what  is  happening  in  most  of  our  conflicts  today.        

Section  :  3  Vive  La  Difference:  How  Brains  differ    

 “I   often   think   it's   comical   /  How  Nature   always   does   contrive   /That   every   boy   and  every  gal  /  That's  born  into  the  world  alive  /  Is  either  a  little  Liberal  /  Or  else  a  little  Conservative!”  (W.S.  Gilbert  ditty  1882)  

People   have   genetically   varying   brain   structures.    Some   people   show   an  increased   gray   matter   volume   in   the   amygdala,  the   structure   in   the  temporal  lobes  that   performs   a   primary   role   in   the   processing   and   memory  of  emotions.  Others  show  an  increased  gray  matter  volume  in  the  anterior  cingulate  cortex,   the  structure   of   the   brain   associated   with   rational/logical   thinking,   with   monitoring  uncertainty   and   with   handling   conflicting   information.   Functional   magnetic  resonance   imaging  or  functional   MRI(fMRI)   is   a       technology   that   measures   brain  activity  by  detecting  associated  changes  in  blood  flow.  fMRI  scans  have  shown  that  these  differences   in  biology,  and   in  genetics,   influence  differences   in  attitudes  and  beliefs.     At   one   end   of   the   spectrum,   people,   usually   called   traditionalists   or  conservatives  in  the  literature,  more  often  use  their  right  amygdala,  the  part  of  the  brain  associated  with  the  body’s  fight-­‐or-­‐flight  system,  when  making  decisions.  They  appear   to   have   genetically   greater   sensitivity   to   fear   and   uncertainty,   and   these  differences  can  be  observed  from  birth.  They  respond  much  more  quickly  to  sudden  noises  and  threatening  visual  images.      

The   degree   to   which   individuals   are   physiologically   responsive   to   such  threats  appears  to  indicate  the  degree  to  which  they  advocate  policies  that  protect  the   existing   social   structure   from   both   external   out   groups   and   internal,   norm-­‐violator   threats.  Fearful  people   tend  to  be  more  conservative.  They  have  a  greater  need  for  order,  structure,  and  certainty  in  their  lives,  resist  change  more  often,  and  are   less   open   to   risk   taking.   Researchers   have   shown   that   they   are   usually  more  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  10  

supportive   of   policies   that   provide   them   with   a   sense   of   security:     hence   their  greater   backing   for   e.g.   military   spending,   capital   punishment,   patriotism,   and  tougher  laws  on  immigration.    

Such   people   also   report   greater   physiological   levels   of  disgust   when   faced  with   bugs,   or   blood,   and   in   relation   to   such   issues   as   gay  marriage   and   abortion.  They  value   ‘purity’   and  order   and  are  known   to  devote  high   levels  of   attention   to  norm  violations.  An  example  of  this  might  be  the  suggestion  by  some  historians  that  the  development  of  the  Taliban  originally  emerged  out  of  the  disgust  felt  by  former  pious  mujahedeen   living   in   Pakistan   at   the   lives   of   their   fellow   colleagues  whom  they   saw   as   socially   and   morally   corrupt   and   impure.   Thus   was   born   the  commitment  of  the  Taliban,  and  subsequently  the  ISIS/Islamic  State,  to  establishing  a   ‘pure’   Islamic   state,  with  no  compromises,   through   the  use  of  Sharia  Law.  While  the  predisposition  to  experience  greater  fear  has  a  genetic  component,  what  exactly  people  will  fear,  or  towards  which  out  group  they  will  harbor  suspicions,  will  differ  depending  on  their  environment,  their  culture,  and  their  social  contexts.    

On   the   other   end   of   a   continuum,   there   appear   to   be   people   who   are  genetically   more   open   to   new   things,   and   to   new   experiences   -­‐   these   are   often  termed   ‘liberals’.   fMRI  scans  have  shown  that   they  can  better   tolerate  uncertainty,  and  cognitive  complexity,    take  risks  more  often,  and  have  wider  and  more  diverse  friendships  They  tend  to  show  greater  activity  in  the  left  insula  of  their  brain,  which  is  an  area  that  appears  to  be  associated  with  self  and  social  awareness.  They  often  exhibit   stronger   preferences   for   social   change   and   equality   when   compared  with  traditionalists.   When   faced   with   a   conflict,   liberals   are   more   likely   than  conservatives   to   be  more   flexible   and   to   alter   their   habitual   response  when   cues  indicate  it   is  necessary.  Researchers  have  also  identified  a  variant  called  DRD4-­‐7R,  which  affects  the  neurotransmitter  called  dopamine,  and  individuals  carrying  it  are  more  likely  to  turn  out  to  be  liberals.  

There   is   speculation   in   the   literature   that   evolutionary   wise   it   may   have  proved  useful   to  have  such  varied  types  of   individuals   in  a  society  so  as   to  ensure  the   best   survival   responses   to   different   sets   of   societal   and   group   challenges.  Interestingly,  people  whose  amygdalae  have  been  completely  destroyed  appear   to  feel  that  everyone  is    ‘my  kind’,  and  they  feel  no  threats  from  out-­‐groups.  Such  lack  of   feelings   would   obviously,   throughout   history,   leave   them   open   to   possible  manipulation,   abuse   and   extinction.   It   has   also   been   suggested   that   conservatism  has  been   the  majority  norm   throughout  history,   and   that   the  growth   in   liberalism  may  be  viewed  as  an  evolutionary   luxury   that   is   increasingly  afforded  only  by   the  fact  that  many  societies  are  becoming  less  deadly  today.  

 For   some   researchers,   this   division   of   differences   into   traditionalist/liberal  

dimensions   in   not  multi-­‐dimensional   enough,   and   in   their   fMRI   studies   they   have  identified  three  different  patterns  of  brain  activation  that  they  believe  correlate  with  individualism,   conservatism,   and   radicalism.   They   have   used   multidimensional  scaling   and   parametric   functional   magnetic   resonance   imaging   to   identify   which  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  11  

criteria/dimensions   people   use   to   structure   complex   political   beliefs   and   which  brain   regions   are   concurrently   activated.   They   found   that   three   independent  dimensions   explained   the   variability   of   a   set   of   statements   expressing  political/social  beliefs  and  that  each  dimension  was  reflected  in  a  distinctive  pattern  of   neural   activation:   individualism   in   the   medial   prefrontal   cortex   and   in   the  temporoparietal   junction   of   the   brain,   conservatism   in   the   dorsolateral   prefrontal  and  radicalism  in  the  ventral  striatum  and  posterior  cingulate  areas.    The  structures  they  identified  are  also  known  to  be  important  in   ‘self-­‐other’  processing  and  social  decision-­‐making  in  ambivalent  situations.  

Twin   studies   seem   to   bear   out   the   contention   that   social   and   political  perspectives  are  genetically  shaped.  A  review  of  over  89  peer-­‐reviewed  papers  on  the   effects   of   genes   and   environment   (both   family   upbringing   and   their   wider  context   and   circumstances)   on   twins’   attitudes   to   racial,   sexual   and   religious  questions,   their   views   on  defense   and   foreign  policy,   and   their   identification  with  particular  political  parties  showed  a  direct  correlation  between  genetics  and  social  views,  although  not  a  correlation  with  a  political  affiliation.  A  2005  U.S.  twin  study  examined  the  attitudes  of  identical  twins  regarding  28  different  social  and  political  issues   such   as   capitalism,   trade   unions,   X-­‐rated   movies,   abortion,   school   prayer,  divorce,  property  taxes,  and  the  draft.  They  compared  them  to  non-­‐identical  twins,  and  estimated  that  genetic   factors  accounted  for  53%  of  the  variance  of  an  overall  score,  and  on  all  counts,  identical  twins  were  found  to  be  more  alike  in  their  views  than  fraternal  twins.    

The   findings   of   differing   kinds   of   neural   architecture   would   appear   to   be  comparative.   Systematic   differences   have   been   noted   across   populations.  Tolerances   for   group  differences   vary:   those   in   Scandinavia   and  Northern  Europe  demonstrated   the  most   tolerance   for   out-­‐groups,   and   this  may   help   explain   their  (current)  more  liberal  social  policies  towards  immigration.    

 However,   it   is   important   to   remember,   as   noted   before,   that   people   are  

predisposed  but  not  predestined   to  become   ‘conservatives’  or   ‘liberals.’  While   there  are  no  actual  genes  for  prejudice  or  tolerance,  a  person’s  genes  can  predispose  them  to   adopt   a   more   cautious,   or   a   more   liberal   response.     How   much   our   genetics  influence   our   attitudes   and   behavior   is   still   a   major   research   question.   The  suggestions  range  from  40%  to  53%.  While  it  is  suggested  that  predispositions  can  be   changed,   and   altered,   effecting   such   change   has   been   likened   to   turning   a  supertanker  –  such  change  takes  considerable  effort  and  time.  

Critical  also  to  note   is   the   fact   that  certain  environments  such  as  post  9/11  may  be  so  powerful  that  they  elicit  a  common  response  in  humans  that  leaves  little  room   for   genetic   differences   to   manifest.   A   study   of   survivors   who   were   highly  impacted   by   9/11   attack   on   the   New   York  World   Trade   center   found   that   three  times  as  many  survivors  reported  becoming  more  politically  conservative  in  the  18  months   following   the   attack,   regardless   of   partisan   identification   and  prior   voting  patterns.  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  12  

 Some   researchers    assert   that  determining   social/political     tendencies   from  

fMRI  data  is  overreaching  its  capacity.  There  are  also  others  who  suggest  that  such  categorizations   have   a   long   history   that   extends   way   before   the   invention   of  magnetic   resonance   imaging   and   hormonal   testing.   They   point   out   that   Thomas  Jefferson  can  be  noted  as  one  of  the  first  ‘neuro-­‐politicians’,  and  indeed  one  who  was  open  about  his  own  preferred  bias.    The  president  was  convinced  that  the  hardened  Federalists   he   opposed  were   suffering   from  damaged  brains,   and  diseased  minds,  which  rendered  them  incapable  of  supporting  healthy  social  growth.  He  suggested    that   they  were  “nervous  persons,  whose   languid  [nerve]   fibres  have  more  analogy  with  a  passive  than  active  state  of  things.’    

 Jefferson   of   course   did   not   have   the   advantage   of   fMRI   scans   capable   of  

confirming  or  otherwise  his  viewpoint.  But  the  fact  that  there  are  now  fMRI    scanners   that   can   test,   describe   and   predict   how  we  will   think   and   feel   about   an  issue  or  an  out-­‐group  must  give  us  pause  for  our  peacebuilding  work,  and  how    we  can    make  our  work  more  effective.      

 Section  4:     Us    and  Them:  Who  is  my  Neighbor  in  today’s  world?    ‘Biologically   humans   have   evolved   for   cooperation   -­‐   but   only   with   some   people’  (Greene,  2014)  

The  change   in  oxytocin  after  a   social   interaction  as  measured   in  a  person’s  blood  reflects  changes  in  oxytocin  in  the  brain.    If  it  is  present  in  abundance,  it  can  make  you  demonstrably  more  generous,  trusting  and  compassionate  towards  your  neighbors,   or   otherwise.     Experiments   have   shown   that   bonding  within   groups   is  assisted  by  a  hormone  called  oxytocin,  a  rise  in  the  level  of  which  appears  to  provide  a   ‘glue’   between   people.   This   hormone   increases   a   sense   of   belonging,   and   of  connectedness   to   a   group,   and   positively   rewards   cooperation   e.g.   spraying  Oxytocin  into  people’s  noses  makes  people  more  likely  to  cooperate  in  a  version  of  the  conflict  resolution  game  called  the  Prisoners  Dilemma,    

Oxytocin  reduces  the   fear  of  social  betrayal   in  humans  and   is   important   for  the  inhibition  of  the  brain  regions  such  as  the  amygdala,  which  as  we  have  seen  are  associated  with  stress,   fear,  and  anxiety.  The  genetic  basis   for  oxytocin  production  and  receptivity  has  been   identified,  and  there  are   indications  that   their   levels  may  be   inherited.   They   also   may   have   had   an   evolutionary   function.   High   levels   of  oxytocin  may  have  enabled  some  groups   to   thrive  better   than  others  as   they  have  been   found   to   correlate  with  different   levels  of   cooperation,   and   to   correlate  with  different  social  decisions.      

However,  research  has  also  indicated  that  while  oxytocin  can  increase  levels  of   cooperation   within   a   group,   it   can   also   promote   ethnocentric   behavior.   It   can  increase  our  suspicion  and  rejection  of  ‘others’  outside  the  group  or  the  tribe,  and  it  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  13  

makes  people  less   likely  to  cooperate  with  members  of  an  out-­‐group.  This  has  lead  to   the   suggestion   that   biologically   humans   have   evolved   for   cooperation   but   only  with   some   people,   and   not   with   others,   and   for   some   researchers   to   suggest   our  brains  are  wired  for  tribalism.    There  are  also  findings  that  suggest  that  out  group  biases   such   as   race   bias   may   be   a   fear   “prepared”   by   evolution   and   not   purely  contextual,  as  elements  of  the  amygdala  appear  to  be  responsible  for,  and  associated  with,  implicit,  as  opposed  to  explicit,  attitudes  towards  racial  out-­‐groups.    

Our   attitude   towards   out-­‐groups   is   also   affected   by   what   scientists   call  ‘mirror   neurons’.   Brain   imaging   experiments   using  functional  magnetic   resonance  imaging  (fMRI)   have   shown   that   the   human   inferior   frontal   cortex  and  superior  parietal   lobe   are   active   when   the   person   both   performs   an   action,   and   also   sees  another   individual   performing   an   action.   It   has   been   suggested   that   these   brain  regions  contain  mirror  neurons,  and  they  have  been  defined  as  the  human   ‘mirror  neuron’   system.     These  particular  neurons   in   the  brain   appear   to  be   linked   to  our  capacity   for   empathy,   the   emotion   which   enables   us   to   better   understand   other  peoples   intentions,   feelings   and   emotions   and   allow   us   to   see   the   world   from  another’s  point  of  view.    

Before   the  discovery  of  mirror  neurons,  scientists  generally  believed   that   it  was   through   the   use   of   our   brains   and   cognitive   thought   processes   that   we  interpreted  and  predicted  other  people’s  behavior.  Now,  however,  many  have  come  to  believe  that  we  actually  understand  others  not  by  thinking,  but  by  feeling  through  the  ‘mirror’  neurons  in  the  affective  brain  circuits.  These  circuits  are  automatically  mobilized  when  we  feel  our  own  pain,  and  also  feel  the  pain  of  (some)  others,  and  they  are  the  part  of  the  neural  circuitry  that  provides  our  emotional  response  to  the  distress  of  other  people.    This  ability  to  empathetically  perceive  and  respond  to  the  affective   states   of   another   emerges   as   early   as   two   years   of   age.   The   existence   of  empathy   is   a   universally   hard-­‐wired   response,   although   its   expression   varies  according   to  cultural  contexts.  Of   interest   to  note   is   that   the  part  of   the  brain   that  controls  empathy  appears  to  be  bigger  in  women,  who  show  enhanced  empathetic  ability  when  compared  to  males.  It  is  suggested  that  empathetic  responses  may  have  served  an  adaptive  evolutionary  function.    

Researchers   have  however  noted   that   empathy   is   less   commonly   exhibited  by  adult  individuals  toward  people  from  differing    out  groups.  When  the  context  is  one   of   perceived   conflict,   the   brain   appears   to   switch   off   the   empathetic   neuron  almost  completely  and  actively  resists  any  emotional  connection  with  the  perceived  ‘other’  group.  Sociological  studies  have  already  shown  that  high  levels  of  integration  or  ‘bonding’  within  a  group  seems  to  reduce  a  group’s  capacity  to  develop  ‘bridging’  and   cooperation,   between   differing   identity   groups,   and   this   is   confirmed   by   the  research  on  mirror  neurons.  

The   power   of   contact   and   dialogue   in   increasing   empathy   and   connections  has  been  examined  using  fMRI  imaging,  which  appears  to  be  successful  in  estimating  such   changes.   Encouraging   perspective   shifting   i.e.   changing   the   way   people  understand  and  think  about  other  groups  through  meeting  and  talking  can,  if  done  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  14  

effectively,   actually   alter   the   neural   circuits   concerned   with   identity   differences.  fMRI  can  also  provide  a  way  of  evaluating  such  changes.    

In  recent  years,  the  Project  on  Justice  in  Times  of  Transition  (now  renamed  Beyond   Conflict)   has   been   working   with   the   MIT   Saxe   Lab   on   investigating   the  power  of  dialogue  to  change  perspectives  about  out  groups.    While  dialogue  appears  to  have  positive  effects,  such  effects  are  highly  influenced  by  the  social  and  political  perspectives  of  groups  in  relation  to  each  other’s  perceived  status.  Positive  empathy  towards   opposing   groups   improved   most   among   members   of   the   disempowered  groups,   such   as   Palestinians   and  Mexicans,   who   told   their   own   stories,   and  were  listened  to  by  the  group  that  they  perceived  to  be  more  powerful.  On  the  other  hand,  groups  that  were  perceived  to  be  dominant  groups,  such  as  the  Israelis  and  the  US  Americans,   increased   their   empathy   by   listening   to   the   stories   of   the   groups  who  perceived   themselves   to   be   excluded   or   oppressed.   It   seems   that   the   form   of  dialogue   most   effective   in   soliciting   empathy   differed   according   to   the   perceived  power   asymmetries   between   the   groups.   These   findings   illustrate   the   need   to  address,  or  promise   to  address,   structural   societal  differences  as  a   complement   to  achieving  the  perceived  purpose  of  effective  dialogue  strategies.  If  these  changes  in  empathy  can  be  sustained  beyond  just  temporary  contact  opportunities,  and  can  be  carried  back  into  society  by  significant  actors   in  the  conflict,   they  may  provide  the  field   with   very   useful   data   about   how   best   to   arrange   for   dialogues   aimed   at  achieving  continuing  empathy  and  synergy  between  opposing  groups.    

Why  do  our  brains  exhibit  such  differing  hormonal  and  neural  responses  to  our  own  and  to  other  groups?        There  is  a  very  lively  debate  between  researchers  as  to   whether   the   evolution   of   behavior   happened   at   the   level   of   the   group   or   the  individual,  and  whether  and  when  the  individual  gene  for  survival  will  predominate  over  group  needs.  While  the  theory  for  the  success  of  individual  gene  –  the  so-­‐called  Selfish  Gene  theory  -­‐  has  been  well  addressed,  the  suggestion  of  the  genetic  nature    of   cooperation   and   altruism,   and   subsequent   success   at   group   level   is   a   relatively  new  thesis.  The  last  decade  has  seen  a  contemporary  burgeoning  of  literature  and    debate   about   the   topic.   A   robust   case   is   now   being  made   for   the   idea   that   group  survival,   as   well   as   individual   survival,   is   an   equally   powerful   genetic   factor   in  human   beings.   This   theory   suggests   that   such   group   drive,   which   goes   beyond  kinship,   is   critical   for   humans   to   live   in   groups,   to   raise   families,   to   cooperate   to  build   villages   and   cities,   and   to   defend   themselves   and   their   territories   against  threats  to  their  group.    

 Without  doubt,  the  apparently  selective  nature  of  group  survival  has  been  a  

keen   factor   in   societal   conflicts   and   wars.   The   categorization   of   out-­‐groups   into  enemies  or   friends,  part  of  our  group  or  not  part  of  our  group,   is  a  universal   trait  that   takes   place   in   all   societies   and   nations.   Such   framing   of   the   enemy   will  obviously  differ  and  change  from  context  to  context,  but  has  ever  been  part  of  our  history,  as  have  been  the  changes  in  such  categorizations.  People  from  Ireland  and  Italy  were  not  considered    ‘white’  by  most  19th  century  Americans,  and  it  took  a  US  court   to   decide   in   1909   that   Armenians   were   part   of   the   ‘white’   race.   The  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  15  

categorization  of  African  American,  Latinos  and  Native  Americans  as  societal  ‘others’  continues   in  many   contexts   in   the  United   States.     Even   the   apparently   smallest   of  differences  such  as  those  between  Sunni  or  Shia,  or  Catholic  or  Protestant,  or  French  or  English  speakers  can  provide  a  framework  for  violent  and  often  deadly  conflict.      

Such   racial,   religious,   ethnic   and   language   categorizations   at   community,  regional,   national   and   international   level   are   the   grist   for   the  mill   of  most   of   our  wars  around  the  world  today,  and  are  easily    utilised  by  leaders  for  their  own  gain.  They  appear  to  be  assisted  in  this  by  our  network  of  interacting  neural  circuitry,  and  the   hormones   that   are   responsible   for   the   emotions   with   which  we   address   out-­‐group  categorization.  Such  categorizations  often  work  outside  of  our  awareness,  but  often   have   deadly   consequences.   It   enables   our   group   to   mobilize   itself   to   kill  another  group  when   the   context  appears   to   call   for  group  solidarity   in   the   face  of  suspicion  and  fear.    

While   religion   and   ideologies   often   bind   a   group   together   they   are   not  necessary   to   the   development   of   group   boundaries.   Almost   any   form   of   group  relationships,  such  as  gangs,  cults  or  paramilitarism  can  suffice.  This  is  particularly  true  for  young  men,  where  killing  of  others  can  provide  for  a  feeling  of  cooperation  in   a   larger   cause   than   themselves,   and   a  way   to   gain   esteem   in   the   eyes   of   their  peers.   Many   psychological   experiments   have   already   proven   how   contextually  dependent   are   our   decisions   within   group   contexts   which   can   radically   alter   our  ‘normal’  pattern  of  behavior,  usually  in  favor  of  group  pressures.  Groups  can  and  do  become  our  energizer  of   ideas  and  actions,   often  beyond   individual   reasoning.     In  Section   5  we  will   see   how  we   also   appear   to   be   programmed   to   excuse   our   own  group,  and  blame  others,  to  see  the  faults  of  others  clearly,  but  be  blind  to  our  own.    

In   addition,   our   devotion   to   leaders   may   have   developed   to   facilitate  cooperation   in   large   groups.     Our   leaders   are   often   chosen   because   of   emotional  choices.   A   lot   of   this   appears   to   be   part   of   our   evolutionary   history.   Being   social  animals,   we   are   perhaps   ‘programmed’   to   try   to   lead,   and   to   be   led.   We   usually  choose  our  leaders  because  of  our  gut  instincts,  not  after  a  thorough  analysis  of  their  characters  or  strategies.  We  often  prefer  our  leaders  to  speak  with  certainty  about  issues   we   are   concerned   about   –   often   irrespective   of   the   substance   of   their  arguments.   Leaders   who   express   hesitation,   or   even   endorse   the   value   of  consultation,  can  be  anathema  to  us.    Our  need   is   for   them  to  be  clear  and  strong,  particularly   if   we   are   afraid.   This   may   have   been   the   result   of   our   evolutionary  history.  We   appear   to   be   programmed   to   be   led   and   to   have   faith   in   hierarchies,  because  of  our  desire  to  be  conform  and  be  protected.    This  followership  can  lead  us  into  excluding  others,  and  carrying  out  violent  atrocities.  The  obedience  studies  of  Stanley   Milgram   illustrate   this   tendency   to   "blindly"   carry   out   the   orders   of   an  authority,   even   when   it   means   painfully   shocking   and   potentially   harming   an  innocent  victim.  Once  people  have  adopted  a  leader,  even  new  negative  information  about  that   leader  can  often  lead  them  to  intensify  rather  than  lessen  their  support  for  him/her  (mostly  him).  Obedience  studies  show  how  easy  it  is  for  us  to  do  harm  to  others  either  by  being  ordered  to,  or  by  taking  on  the  roles  of  e.g.  prison  guards,  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  16  

and   then   adopting  practices   that   appear   to  be   contrary   to  what  we  understand   is  our  normal  behavior.  

  Once  again,   it   is   important  to  remember  that  such  tendencies  can  be  mitigated.  One  of  the  most  important  conclusions  from  neuroscience  research  is  that  the   human   brain,   throughout   life,   is   predisposed   to   be     physically   molded,   in  ongoing  ways,   by   human   interactions   in   contexts   that   provide   for   social   learning,  and   the   rewiring   of   neural   pathways.   As   we   have   seen   throughout   history,   our  definition   of   which   out-­‐groups   are   our   enemies   is   socially   learned,   and   also   that  such  enemies  can  and  do  change.  People  living  in  more  market-­‐  integrated  societies  tend  to  be  more  altruistic  and  tolerant  to  strangers,  and  more  adept  at  cooperating  with  them.  This  suggests  that  although  our  brains  are  wired  for  tribalism  they  can  be  (eventually?)  rewired  to  include  wider  groups  through  experience,  and  contexts  and   institutions   that   facilitate   tolerance.   Whether   we   can   totally   eradicate   this  tendency   appears   doubtful.   It   is   commonplace   for   peacebuilders   to   wish   for   an  overarching  enemy,  perhaps  outside  of  our  earthly  context,  to  provide  a  ‘safe’  target  against   which   humans   can   unite   and   forget   their   differences,   and   this   hope   is   a  logical   one,   based   on   an   accurate   perception   of   human  nature.   It   accords  with   an  understanding  of  the  nature  of  human  groups  and  the  wars  between  them,  and  the  usefulness  of  a  common  enemy  around  which  to  rally  groups  emotions.    

There   have   been   many   experiments   in   testing   whether   and   how   one   can  increase   a   surge   in   oxytocin   in   our   brains,   as   an   ability   to   ensure   such  may   have  some  utility  for  the  field  in  enhancing  trust  between  group.  Such  an  increase  might    help   in   managing   difficult   encounters   and   conversations   between   erstwhile   and  current  enemies.  Indeed  many  of  the  stories  of  peacebuilders  about  how  peace    agreements   were   eventually   reached   often   point   to   informal,   and   often  unrecognized,  personal  and  group  processes  that  appear  to  have  been  successful  in  increasing  oxytocin  and  trust  levels.  These  included  factors  such  as  gift  giving,  meal  sharing,  alcohol,  where  such  was  culturally  permitted  (just  a  modicum  –  too  much  can   make   us   more   belligerent!)   empathetic   responses   to   ‘others’   family   crises,  positive  physical  gestures,  expressions  of  understanding  and  appreciation,  physical  manifestations   of   listening,   sharing   of   family   stories,   group   singing,   etc.   Note   that  none,  or  almost  none  of  these  are  mentioned  in  the  mediator’s  guidebooks,  but  fMRI  and  hormonal  testing  indicate  that  perhaps  they  should  be.                      

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  17  

Section  5:  The  Believing  Brain:  How  we  fool  ourselves  and  others.    ‘The  first  principle  is  that  you  must  not  fool  yourself  -­‐  and  you  are  the  easiest  person  to  fool’  Feynman  (1974)  

‘Our   moral   thinking   is   much   more   like   a   politician   seeking   votes   that   a   scientist  searching  for  truth’  Haidt    (2012)  

Why  do  we  believe  what  we  believe?    Because  it  is  true?    Rarely,  it  appears.  Our  beliefs   in  many   cases   appear   to  be  determined  not  by   ‘truth’   but  by  our  own  physical  tendencies,  our  needs  for  beliefs,  or  moral  values  and  the  cultural  context  in   which   we   live.   In   other   words,   beliefs   often   come   first   from   our   physical  characteristics,  and  then  our  social  environment  i.e.  we  mainly  rationalize  what  our  guts  (and  fears)  tell  us.    

Experiments   have   shown   the   interdependence   of   genetic,   social   and  environmental  factors  in  the  development  of  our  beliefs  and  moralities.  Researchers  have   used   genome-­‐wide   linkage   analysis   i.e.   an   examination   of   the   many  common  genetic  variants   in  different   individuals,  in  order  to  identify  chromosomal  regions   associated   with   social   and   political   attitudes   along   the  ‘liberalism/conservatism’   scale.   In   2010   a   study   implicated   a   gene   known  as  DRD4  in  the  development  of  general  social/political  orientation.  The  DRD4  gene  encodes  a  receptor  molecule  for  a  neuro-­‐transmitter  called  dopamine.  Those  with  a  variant  of  DRD4  called  7R,  and  also  a  large  network  of  acquired  friends,  tended  to  be  more  liberal.    

The  research  suggests  that  the  development  of  a  liberal  disposition  requires  both   a   genetic   input   (the   7R   variant)   and   an   environmental   one   (the   network   of  friends)   to   take   effect.   DRD4-­‐7R   had   previously   been   associated   with   novelty-­‐seeking   behavior,   and   the   researchers   speculated   that   the   interaction   of   that  tendency,   with   its   attendant   exposure   to   lots   of   different   ideas   held   by   lots   of  different  people  might  push  an   individual   in  a   liberal  direction.  Later  research  has  found   a   further   11   genes,   different   varieties   of   which   might   be   responsible   for  inclining   people   towards   liberal   or   conservative   beliefs.   These   included   genes  involved   in   the   regulation   of   three   neurotransmitters—dopamine,   glutamate   and  serotonin—and  also  G-­‐protein-­‐coupled  receptors,  which  react   to  a  wide  variety  of  stimulants.   Many   other   studies   have   shown   that   biology   and   psychology   are  inextricably   linked.  This  means   that  when  our  physical  biology   is   altered,   this   can  lead  to  changes  in  personality  tastes,  preferences,  perceptions,  attention,  emotions,  attitudes  and  behaviors.      

While  we  appear  to  have  automatic  settings  for  our  beliefs  that  are  based  on  a  combination  of  our  neurological  architecture  and  our  social  context,  the  substance  of  what  we  believe  as   individuals   is  right  or   is  wrong   is  not  usually  something  we  learn   for  ourselves.    We   inherit   the  majority  of  our  beliefs,  perhaps  as   a   legacy  of  what  helped  our  ancestors  survive.    As  we  have  seen  in  Section  4,  our  views  as  part  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  18  

of  a  group  are  rarely  based  on  cognitive  processing  –  they  are  often  a  case  of  group  think,  i.e.  views  that  are  based  on  group  needs,  rather  than  based  on  fact  checking.  They  are  what  Haidt  calls  ‘groupish’  which  means  that  we  usually  approve  of  what  we   think,   or   are   told   is   good   for   our   group.   While   our   group   beliefs   often  successfully  serve  what   is  seen  as  an  evolutionary  need  to  bind  us  more  closely  to  each  other,  and  thus  ensure  our  group  survival,  they  often  unfortunately  blind  us  to  the  points  of  view  of  other  groups.  In  Haidt's  term,  morality  "binds  and  blinds"  i.e.  it  binds  us  to  the  group  and  blinds  us  to  the  point  of  view  of  outsiders.  

Once  group  beliefs   are   sacrosanct,   the  members  of   a  group  often   lose   their  ability  (and  their  freedom)  to  think  rationally  about  them.  This  is  helped  by  the  fact  that,  once  our  views  have  been  formed,  we  have  a  tendency  to  see  and  find  evidence  in   support   of   already   existing   beliefs,   and   ignore   evidence   that   challenges   them.  fMRI  studies  have  shown  that,  when  faced  with  logical  contradictions  to  their  deeply  held   beliefs,   people   often   feel   negative   emotions,   but   there   is   no   increase   in   the  dorsolateral  cortex,   the  part  of   their  brain  which   is  used   for  reasoning.  Most  of  us    do  all   that  we  can  to  protect  our  group  beliefs  by  watching  media  sources  such  as  Fox  News,  or  certain  programs  on  MSNBC,  or  by  reading  the  New  York  Times  or  the  Washington  Times,  or  the  London  Times  or  Guardian,  Haaretz  or  Jerusalem  Post,  etc.  often   to   solidify  what  we  believe.     Of   course,  most   of   us  may  be   unaware   of   such  biased   choices,   and   refuse   to   acknowledge   this.   Conflicting   groups   watching   the  same  media  coverage  of  their  differences  all  think  it  biased  against  them.  And  when  asked   to   choose  between  accepting   the   views  of   an   expert   (such  as  one  on  global  warming)  and  being  a  good  member  of  a  tribe,  the  latter  is  often  more  important.      

Research  seems  to  suggest  that  while  more  highly  educated  people  are  more  supportive   of   out   groups,   people   with   high   IQs   are   not   necessarily   better   at  understanding  the  other  side  in  a  moral  dispute.  They  are  however  better  at  making  the  case  for  what  they  believe.    

It   appears   that   collective   ideological   beliefs,   whether   religious,   cultural   or  social,   serve   to   strengthen   group  bonds,  which  possibly   helped   groups   to  win   the  societal   competition   for   group   level   survival.   The   function   of   such   beliefs   often  appears   to  be  more   important   than  their  content,  and  the  substance  of  our  beliefs  are  often   less   important   than  with  whom  we  share   those  beliefs.  Those  of  us  who  have   tried   to  have   theological,  moral   or   social   discussions  with   religious   or   social  fundamentalists  have  usually  found  how  unsuccessful  such  an  exchange  process  can  be.     It   seems   that   the   religious  or  political  beliefs  are  more  often  about  a   sense  of  solidarity  and  collectiveness,  than  about  truth.    Such  group  beliefs  are  seen  by  some  researchers  as  more  akin  to  ‘team  sports’  than  to  a  form  of  ‘truth’.    This  can  help  us  understand  young  loyalist  militants  in  Northern  Ireland  who  believe  that  the  1690  battle,  which  their  tribe  won,  can  be  found  recorded  in  the  bible.  Or  the  many  young  men   fighting  with   the  opposition   in  Syria,  or   currently  with   the   Islamic  State  who  often  have   little   idea   of   the  main  players   in   the  war,   but   have  blind   adherence   to  their  team  peers,  and  their  team  cause.    

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  19  

Once  adopted,  our  opinions  become  intertwined  with  our  self  or  group  image,  and  we  avoid  situations   that  may  challenge  our  deeply-­‐  held  beliefs.    Reasoning   is  therefore  often  carried  out  to  serve  the  purpose  of  persuasion,  not  of  discussion.  For  many   people   moral   reasoning   is   usually   generated   only   after   a   belief   has   been  assumed   instinctively.  We  often   lie  even   to  ourselves  because   it   appears   that   self-­‐deception   is   an   assistive   process   that   enables   us   to   more   convincingly   deceive  others.  We  usually  suffer  from  attribution  bias  i.e.  the  tendency  to  attribute  different  causes  and  justifications  for  our  own  beliefs  and  actions  than  that  of  others.  When  bad  things  happen,  it  is  always  their  fault,  not  ours,  their  violence  is  terrorism,  ours  is  justified  by  security  needs.    

Our  memories   too  are  also  notoriously   faulty  –   they  often  reframe  and  edit  events  so  as  to  create  a  story  that  will   fit  our  current  situation,  conflating  the  past  and  present  to  suggest  a  story  to  us  that  suits  our  what  we  need  to  believe  today.    

Confirmation  bias,  i.e.  the  adjustment  of  our  thinking  to  fit  what  we  want  to  believe,  appears   to  be  an  essential  part  of  an  evolutionary  adaptation  as  a  built   in  feature,  and  may  not  easily  be  removed.  

Religion  remains  the  world’s  most  dominant  group  belief    (84%  of  the  worlds  population   belongs   to   some   form   of   organized   religion)   and   it   can   be   a   very  powerful  motivator  for   individual  and  collective  violent  action.  Almost  all   forms  of  religion  –  Christianity,  Buddhism,  Islam,  Hinduism,  Judaism,  have  served  throughout  history,   and   many   still   do   today,   to   provide   frameworks   of   belief   that   justify  exclusion   and   violence.   It   appears   that   almost   any   ideological   framework   can   be  used  to  serve  as  a  binding  group  factor.      

Political   neuroscience   researchers,   who   specifically   investigate   the  relationship   between   the   brain   and   politics,   have   suggested   that   trying   to  understand   the   persistence   and   passion   of   religion   by   studying   or   arguing   about  beliefs   about   God   is   the   equivalent   of   trying   to   understand   the   persistence   and  passion  of  football  supporters  by  studying  the  movement  of  the  football.    From  this  perspective,  ideologies  are  not  systems  of  truth,  but  essentially  a  group  adaptation.  They  are  usually  born  out  of  a  particular  social  context,  allied  to  physiological  needs  such   as   a   differing   neural   sensitivity   to   threats,   a   greater   need   for   the   security   of  identity,   and   the   greater   certainty   of   belief   that   a   group   can   provide.     It   is   also  important   to   note   it   is   not   just   religion   that   can   engender   collective   support   for  violence.  The  single  most  prolific  group  of  suicide  attackers  in  the  world  has  actually  been   the   Tamil   Tigers   of   Sri   Lanka,   which   is   a   secular   movement   of   national  liberation,  with  only  nominal  religious  affiliation.  Communism,  an  ultra  secular  form  of   social   ideology,   has   been   responsible   for   many   more   millions   of   deaths   than  religion  during  the  early  part  of  the  20th  century.    

Our  biology  also  appears  to  affect  our  prioritization  of  our  social  and  moral  beliefs,   and   such   differences   can   evoke   both   group   solidarity   and   significant   out  group  misunderstanding   or   hatred.     For   some   people,   such   as   those   on   the  more  ‘liberal’   side,   care   for   others,   and   fairness   to   others,   are   priority   values,   and   they  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  20  

often  see  the  need  to  have  them  incorporated  into  a  governments  social  structures.    For   others,   whose   brains   tend   more   strongly   towards   values   such   as   loyalty,  sanctity,  authority  and  liberty,  these  values  matter  more  the   ‘fairness’  and     ‘caring’  exemplified  by    ‘big  government’,  and  they  prefer  to  undertake  their  caring  through  personal   and   group   charity   to   which   they   donate   more   often   than   liberals.   Such  findings  have  been  used  to  explain  why  working  class  and  rural  voters  often  appear  to  vote   for   their  values,   rather   than   their  economic   interests.  They  have  also  been  the  source  of  many  political  and  social  differences  in  the  USA  and  the  UK  about  the  role  of  governments.    Such  differences  in  the  prioritization  of  social  values  can  also  be  seen  cross  culturally,  and  across  nations.    

While   systems   of   belief,   and   the   shared   nature   of   values,   can   foster   great  courage   and   group   altruism,   it   seems   that   such   altruism   is   often   of   a   parochial  nature,  and  associated  with  a  particular  ideological  or  territorial  group,  rather  than  a  global  one.  How  such  group  altruism  can  fare  in  the  increasing  diversity  of  all  our  societies,   and  within   an   increasingly   globalized  world   is   a  major   question   for   the  peacebuilders  of  today.        

Conclusion:      

My  hope   is   that   a  greater  appreciation  about  how  our  genetic   and  physical  predispositions,  allied  to  environmental  factors,  can  affect  our  human  behavior,  can  strengthen   our   consideration   of   what   we   can   learn   from   the   emerging   field   of  neuroscience.    Such  learning,  if  used  wisely,  may  help  us  to  develop  more  effective  strategies   for   peacebuilding   that   can   build   on   our   greater   understanding   of   the  realities  of  human  nature,  and  its  neural  legacies,  rather  than  ignoring  them.  It  may  help   us,   and   the   groups   we   work   with   to   understand   better,   and   relate   more  empathetically,  more   realistically,   and  more   compassionately   to   conflicted   groups  who  behave  as  they  do  at  such  significant  cost  to  themselves  and  others.    Hopefully  this   will   help   us   to   craft   more   effective   peacebuilding   strategies   for   the   many  challenges  ahead  of  us  in  the  decades  to  come.    

(And  -­‐  If  we  find  such  reflections  as  are  noted  in  this  paper  distasteful   in  any  way,  can  we  say  why,  and  have  a  discussion  about  it?)  

                       

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  21  

Section  6:    Questions  for  the  Peacebuilder:    

1. How  comfortable  are  we  at  accepting  the  limitations  of  a  ‘rational’  approach  to   peacebuilding?     What   are   the   consequences   for   our   work   of  acknowledging  the  primary  importance  of  feelings  in  conflict  contexts?  

2. If  people  have  brains  that  are  predisposed  differently  towards  outgroups  and  new   ideas,   how   do   we   develop   strategies   that   take   account   of   this   in   our  work?    

3. How  do  we  work  with  groups  who  are  passionately  committed  to  only  their  own   group   vision   of   faith   or   social   ideology?   How   can   we   increase   more  constructive  group  memberships  that  increase  individual  and  group  feelings  of  positive  meaning,  safety,  and  inclusion  towards    ‘other’  communities?  

4. How  do  we  avoid  getting  caught  up  in  arguments  about  the  ‘truth’  or  ‘facts’  as  believed  by  particular  groups  ?  How  can  we  better  understand  and  respond  to  what  is  often  behind  such  arguments?  

5. How  can  we   ‘nudge’  our  societies   into   their  best   inclusive  behaviors?  What  kinds   of   local   and   international   societal   policies   and   institutions   can   we  implement  that  can  decrease  our  tendencies  towards  fear  of  the  ‘other’  

6. What   kind   of   peace   agreements   can   best   deliver   on   feelings   of   equity   and  inclusion,  as  well  as  ensuring  the  quantifiable  reality  of  such  agreements  on  issues  of  land,  rights  participation,  etc?  

7. How  do  we   change  our  peacebuilding  work   so   that   our   strategies   can   take  account  of  the  frailty  of  our  inherited  human  nature  that  tends  towards  fear  and   exclusion   of   others   -­‐   as  well   as   our   human   capacities   for   cooperation,  altruism  and  courage?  

8. How   can   help   to   create/choose/assist   leaders   who   foster   community   and  structural  inclusion  rather  than  divisions?  How  can  we  increase  the  power  of  such  leaders  with  their  constituents?  

9. How   would   we   describe   our   own   predispositions   along   a  conservative/liberal   continuum?   Do   we   have   moral   feelings   of   superiority  about  our  particular  end  (or  middle)  of  the  continuum?  Do  we  appreciate  the  need  for  society  to  have  people  who  are  both   ‘traditional/conservative’  and  ‘liberal’-­‐  despite  our  own  particular  predisposition?    

10. How  can  we  pitch/target  our  messages  and  campaigns  to  different  audiences  in   full   awareness   of   their   differing   neural   dispositions?   How   do   we   frame  messages  about  our  work  so  that  they  appeal  to  the  whole  brain,  and  not  just  the  rational  part  of  it?        

   

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  22  

Bibliography  and  Resources:      Section  1:  Introduction    Benedictus,   Leo   (2013)   The   Nudge   Unit   –   has   it   worked   so   far?   Guardian.  http://www.theguardian.com/politics/2013/may/02/nudge-­‐unit-­‐has-­‐it-­‐workedGuardian  2  May  2013  06.00  BST  

Bruneau   E,   and   Saxe   R.   (2012).   The   power   of   being   heard:   the   benefits   of  'perspective-­‐giving'   in   the   context   of   intergroup   conflict.  Journal   of   Experimental  Social  Psychology  48(4)  855-­‐866.      Chance,  Paul  (2001)  The  Barmaid's  Brain.  Freeman      

Dooley,  Roger:   (2011)  Brainfluence:  100  Ways  to  Persuade  and  Convince  Consumers  with  Neuromarketing.  John  Wiley    

Fitzduff,  Mari:  (1989)  From  Ritual  to  Consciousness  -­‐  a  study  of  change  and  progress  in  Northern  Ireland.  Unpublished  PhD.  New  University  of  Ulster.    

Fitzduff,   Mari,   and   Stout,   Chris   (eds)   (2005)   The   Psychology   of   Resolving   Global  Conflicts:  From  War  to  Peace  Palgrave  Press.      Fitzduff,   Mari   (2013)   Public   Policies   in   Shared   Societies:   A   Comparative   Approach.  Palgrave  MacMillan    

Glimcher,  Paul,  Fehr,  Ernst:  (2013)  Neuroeconomics:  Decision  Making  and  the  Brain  Academic  press  

Henrich,   Joseph   ,  Heine  Steven  J.  ,  Norenzayan,  Ara   (2009)  The  Weirdest  People   in  The  World:  How  representative  are  experimental  findings  from  American  university  studnets   ?   What  do  we  really  know  about  human  psychology?     University   of  Columbia   Website.  http://www2.psych.ubc.ca/~henrich/pdfs/Weird_People_BBS_final02.pdf  

Hutcherson,  Cendri  A.;  Seppala,  Emma  M.;  Gross,  James  J.  (2008).  "Loving-­‐kindness  meditation  increases  social  connectedness”.  Emotion  8  (5):  720–4.  

Korolak,   Rhondalynn:   (2013)   Google   Uses   Neuromarketing   To   Revolutionize   SEO.  PR   Newswire,   March   14   2013   http://imagineeringnow.com/media/press-­‐releases/google-­‐neuromarketing-­‐seo/  

Neuberg,  Andrew:      (2012)  Principles  of  Neurotheology.  Ashgate  Science  and  Religion  Series.    

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  23  

Noll,  Douglas,E:  (2006)  Neuropsychology  of  Conflict:   Implications  for  Peacemaking  in  The  Psychology  of  Global  Conflicts:  From  War  to  Peace  Vol  1.  Eds.  Fitzduff,  Mari  and  Stout,  Chris.  Palgrave  Press,  Westport.  

Organ   Donation   Taskforce:     The   potential   impact   of   an   opt   out   system   for   organ  donation   in   the   UK:   An   independent   report   from   the   Organ   Donation   Taskforce.  http://www.odt.nhs.uk/pdf/the-­‐potential-­‐impact-­‐of-­‐an-­‐opt-­‐out-­‐system-­‐for-­‐organ-­‐donation-­‐in-­‐the-­‐UK.pdf  

Sachs,  Oliver:  (2011)  The  Man  Who  Mistook  His  Wife  for  a  Hat.  Picador    Singer,   Natasha:   (2010)     "Making   Ads   that   Whisper   to   the   Brain".   The   New   York  Times.  (3  November  2010  

Sharp,  Gwen:  (2008)  Negative  Stereotypes  Of  The  Irish  http://thesocietypages.org/socimages/2008/10/06/negative-­‐stereotypes-­‐of-­‐the-­‐irish/  Sociological  Images  Oct  6,  2008,  

Spranger,   Tade   Matthias:   (2012)   International   Neurolaw:   A   Comparative   Analysis  Springer  Science  &  Business  Media,  

Sunstein,  Cass  and  Thaler,  Richard:  (2009)  Nudge:  Improving  Decisions  About  Health,  Wealth  and  Happiness.  Penguin.  

Wade,  Nicholas   (2014)  A  Troublesome  Inheritance:  Genes,  Race  and  Human  History,  by  Nicholas  Wade.  Penguin  Press.  

WIRED   UK   (2011)   How   Driving   a   Taxi   Changes   London   Cabbies’   Brains.  WIRED  http://www.wired.com/2011/12/london-­‐taxi-­‐driver-­‐memory/    Sources,  Section  2:  The  Rider  and  the  Elephant      Frith,  Chris:     (2007)  Making  Up  the  Mind:  How  the  Brain  Creates  Our  Mental  World.  Wiley  Blackwell.    Greene,   Joshua:     Beyond  Point-­‐and-­‐Shoot  Morality:  Why  Cognitive   (Neuro)Science  Matters   for   Ethics.   www.wjh.harvard.edu/~jgreene/GreeneWJH/Greene-­‐Ethics-­‐PrePub-­‐1-­‐14-­‐14.pdf    Greene,   Joshua:   (2012)  Moral  Tribes:  Emotion,  Reason  and  the  Gap  Between  Us  and  Them.  Atlantic  Books.        Haidt,   Jonathan   (2001)   The   Emotional   dog   and   its   rationalist   Tail:   A   Social  intuitionist  Approach  to  Moral  Judgement.    Psychological  Re\it"A2001.  Vol.  108.  No.  4,  814-­‐834.http://www3.nd.edu/~wcarbona/Haidt%202001.pdf  

Haidt,   Jonathan:   (2007)  The  Happiness  Hypothesis:  Finding  Modern  Truth  in  Ancient  Wisdom.  Arrow.    

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  24  

Heath,   Chip   and  Heath,  Dan   (2010)  Switch.  How  to  Change  Things  When  Change   is  Hard.  Crown  Business.  

Kahneman,  Daniel:  (2012)  Thinking  Fast  and  Slow.  Penguin    Mooney,  Chris:  (2012)  The  Republican  Brain:  The  Science  of  Why  They  Deny  Science-­‐-­‐and  Reality  Wiley  Press.    Shapiro,   Dan,   and   Fisher,   Roger:   (2007)   Beyond   Reason:   Using   Emotions   as   You  Negotiate.  Penguin  USA     Tversky,   Amos   and   Kahneman,   Daniel:   (1974)   Judgment   under   Uncertainty:  Heuristics  and  Biases.  Science,  New  Series,    Vol.  185,  No.    4157.  (Sep.  27,  1974),  pp.  1124-­‐1131    Sources:  Section  3.  Vive  La  Difference:  How  Brains  differ.      Aue,   T;   Lavelle,   LA;   Cacioppo,   JT:   (2008)   Great   Expectations:   What   Can   FMRI  Research   Tell   Us   About   Psychological   Phenomena?     International   Journal   of  Psychophysiology.  Volume  73,  Issue  1,  July  2009,  Pages  10–16  

Alford,   John   R;   Funk,   Carolyn;   Hibbing,   John   R.   :   Are   Political   Orientations  Genetically  Transmitted?  American  Political  Science  Review  Vol.  99,  No.  2  May  2005  

Amodio,   David;     Jost,   John;   Master,   Sarah;   Yee,   Cindy   (2007)   "Neurocognitive  Correlates  of  Liberalism  and  Conservatism,"  Nature  Neuroscience,  Sep.  9,  2007  

Block,   J  &  Block,   J.   H.   (2006).  Nursery   school   personality   and  political   orientation  two  decades  later.  Journal  of  Research  in  Personality,  40,  734–749    

Bonanno,  G.  A    &  Jost,  J.  T.  (2006).  Conservative  shift  among  high-­‐exposure  survivors  of  the  September  11th  terrorist  attacks.  Basic  and  Applied  Social  Psychology,  28,  311–323  

Caroll,   Joseph  (2004)  Who  Supports  The  Death  Penalty?  The  Gallup  Organisation   :  November   16,   2004   http://www.deathpenaltyinfo.org/gallup-­‐poll-­‐who-­‐supports-­‐death-­‐penalty    

Employee   Of   The   New   York   Times,  and     Hubbard,   Ben   (2014)   Life   In   A   Jihadist  Capital:   Order   With   A   Darker   Side.   New   York   Times.  Http://Www.Nytimes.Com/2014/07/24/World/Middleeast/Islamic-­‐State-­‐Controls-­‐raqqa-­‐syria.html?_r=0  

Hatemi,  Peter  K;  McDermott,  Rose;  Eaves,  Lindon   J;  Kendler,  Kenneth  S;  and  Neale,  Michael   C.   (2013)     Fear   as   a   Disposition   and   an   Emotional   State:   A   Genetic   and  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  25  

Environmental   Approach   to   Out-­‐Group   Political   Preferences.   American   Journal   of  Political  Science  Volume  57,  Issue  2,  pages  279–293,  April  2013    Hatemi,   P.   &   McDermott,   R.(Eds.)(2011).   Man   is   by   nature   a   political   animal:  Evolution,  Biology  and  Politics.  Chicago:  University  of  Chicago  Press  

 Hatemi,  Peter  K,  McDermott  R,  (2012)  The  genetics  of  politics:  discovery,  challenges,  and  progress  Trends  in  Genetics  Volume  28,  Issue  10,  p525–533,  October  2012  

Inbar,   Yoel,   Pizarro,   David,   Ravi,   Haidt,   Ravi,   (2012)   Disgust   Sensitivity,   Political  Conservatism,   and   Voting.   Social   Psychological   and   Personality   Science.  September  2012  vol.  3  no.  5537-­‐544  http://spp.sagepub.com/content/3/5/537.short  

Inbar,   Yoel;   Pizarro,   David;   Bloom,   Paul.     (2009)   Conservatives   are   more   easily  disgusted  than  liberals  Cognition  &  Emotion  Vol.  23,  Iss.  4,    

   Jeffrey,  Terence  (2010)  Conservatives  Are  More  Than  Twice  as  Likely  as  Liberals  to  Be   Strongly   Patriotic,   Says   Gallup   Poll   CNS   July   4,   2010  http://c  nsnews.com/news/article/conservatives-­‐are-­‐more-­‐twice-­‐likely-­‐liberals-­‐be-­‐strongly-­‐patriotic-­‐says-­‐gallup-­‐poll  

Jost,   John;   Frederico,   Christoper;   Napier,   Jaime.   (2009)   Political   Ideology:   Its  Structure,   Functions,   and   Elective   Affinities.   Annual.   Psychological   Review   2009.  60:307–37  

Jost,   John;   Glaser,   Jack;   Kruglanski,   Arie;   Sulloway,   Frank:     (2003)   Political  Conservatism   as   Motivated   Social   Cognition.   Psychological   Bulletin,   American  Psychological  Association,  Inc.    2003,  Vol.  129,  No.  3,  339–375.  

Jost,  John;  Kay,  A.C;  &  Thorisdottir,  H.  (eds.)  (2009)    Social  and  Psychological  Bases  of  Ideology  and  System  Justification.  (pp.  210-­‐240).  New  York:  Oxford  University  Press.    

 Jost,   J.   T;   &   Napier,   J.   L.   (2012).   The   uncertainty-­‐threat   model   of   political  conservatism.  In  M.  A.  Hogg  &  D.  L.  Blaylock  (Eds.)  Extremism  and  the  psychology  of  uncertainty  (pp.  90-­‐111).  New  York:  Wiley-­‐Blackwell.  

 Jost   J.T;   Banaji,   MR;   Nosek,   B.A.   (2004)   A   decade   of   system   justification   theory:  accumulated   evidence   of   Conscious   and  Unconscious  Bolstering   of   the   Status  Quo  Political  Psychology,  Vol.  25,  No.  6,  2004  

Jost,   J.   T.   &   Amodio,   D.   M.   (2012).  Political   ideology   as   motivated   social  cognition:  Behavioral  and  neuroscientific  evidence.  Motivation  and  Emotion,  36,  55-­‐64.  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  26  

Jost,  J.   T;  Blount,  S;  Pfeffer,  J;   &  Hunyady;  G.   (2003).  Fair   market   ideology:  Its  cognitive-­‐motivational  underpinnings.  Research    in  Organizational  Behavior  25,  53-­‐91.      

Knudson,  Kristine,  et  al.  (March  2006).  Politics  on  the  Brain:  An  fMRI  Investigation  Social  Neuroscience  1  (1):  25–40.  

Kanai,   Ryota;   Feilden,   Tom;   [Firth,   Colin],   and   Rees,   Geraint:   (2011)   Political  Orientations  Are  Correlated  with  Brain  Structure   in  Young  Adults.  Current  Biology  Volume  21,  Issue  8,  p677–680,  26  April  2011  

Mooney,  Chris:  (2012)  The  Republican  Brain:  The  Science  of  Why  They  Deny  Science-­‐-­‐and  Reality.  John  Wiley  &  Sons.  

Nosek,  B.A;  Banaji,  M.R;   Jost,   J.T.  2009.  The  politics    of   intergroup  attitudes   in   Jost,  John.  T;  Kay,  Aaron  and  Thorisdottir,  Hulda.  (Eds)  Social  and  Psychological  Bases  of  Ideology  and  System    Justification.    Oxford  University  Press.  

Oxley,   Douglas;   Smith,   Kevin;   B,   Alford,   John;   Hibbing,   Matthew,   Miller   (2008)    Political  Attitudes  Vary  with  Physiological  Traits,  Science,  Sep.  19,  2008  

Rasanayagam,  Angelo.  Afghanistan:   a  modern  history.  I  B  Tauris  &  Co  Ltd   (24  Aug  2005).  P.  143      Schreiber,  D;  Fonzo,  G;  Simmons,  A.N;  Dawes,  C.T;  Flagan  T,  et  al.  (2013)  Red  Brain,  Blue   Brain:   Evaluative   Processes   Differ   in   Democrats   and   Republicans.   PLoS  ONE  8(2):  e52970    Settle,   Jaime;  Dawes,  Christopher;   Fowler,   James.   (2010)  Friendships  Moderate   an  Association  Between  a  Dopamine  Gene  Variant  and  Political  Ideology.  The  Journal  of  Politics  01/2010;  72(4):1189-­‐1198.    

Shook,  Natalie   J  and  Fazio,  Russell   (2009)  "Political   Ideology,  Exploration  of  Novel  Stimuli,   and   Attitude   Formation,"   Journal   of   Experimental   Social   Psychology.   Vol  45(4),   Jul   2009,   995-­‐998.   http://2012election.procon.org/sourcefiles/political-­‐ideology-­‐exploration-­‐of-­‐novel-­‐stimuli-­‐and-­‐attitude-­‐formation-­‐2009.pdf  

Zamboni,   G;  Gozzi,   M;  Krueger,   F;  Duhamel,   JR;  Sirigu,   A;  Grafman,   J.   (2009)  Individualism,   conservatism,   and   radicalism   as   criteria   for   processing   political  beliefs:   a   parametric   fMRI   study.   Social     Neuroscience.  2009;4(5):367-­‐83.  http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19562629  

       

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  27  

Sources:   Section   4:   Us     and   Them.   Who   is   my   neighbor   in   today’s  world?          Alasko,   Carl:   (2011)   Beyond   Blame:   Freeing   Yourself   from   the  Most   Toxic   Form   of  Emotional  Bullsh*t.  Tarcher,      Baumgartner,   T;  Heinrichs,  M;   Vonlanthen,   A;   Fischbacher,   U;   Fehr   E   (May   2008).  Oxytocin   shapes   the   neural   circuitry   of   trust   and   trust   adaptation   in  humans.  Neuron  58  (4):  639–50.  

Berreby,  David:   (2008)  Us  and  Them:  The  Science  of   Identity   University   of   Chicago  Press.  

Brosch,   T,   Bar-­‐David,   E.,   &   Phelps,   E.A.   (2013).  Implicit   race   bias   decreases   the   similarity   of   neural   representations   of   black   and  white  faces.  Psychological  Science,  24,  160-­‐6.      

Brosch,   T;   Schiller,   D;   Mojdehbakhsh,   R;   Uleman,   J;   &   Phelps,   E.A.   (2013).  Neural   mechanisms   underlying   the   integration   of   situational   information   into  attribution  outcomes.  Social  Cognitive  and  Affective  Neuroscience,  8,  640-­‐6.      

Bruneau.   E;   Dufour,   N;   Saxe,   R.   (2012)   Social   cognition   in   members   of   conflict  groups:  behavioral  and  neural  responses  in  Arabs,  Israelis  and  South  Americans  to  each  other's  misfortunes.  Philosophical  Transactions  of   the  Royal  Society  London:  B  Biological  Sciences  367(1589)  717-­‐730.    Bruneau   E,   and   Saxe   R.   (2012).   The   power   of   being   heard:   the   benefits   of  'perspective-­‐giving'   in   the   context   of   intergroup   conflict.  Journal   of   Experimental  Social  Psychology  48(4)  855-­‐866.      Bruneau,   Emile   and   Saxe,   Rebecca:   (2010)   Attitudes   towards   the   out  group   are   predicted   by   activity   in   the   precuneus   in   Arabs   and   Israelis:  Neuroimage  (2010)  1704-­‐1711    Bruneau,  E;  Darfour,  N  and  Saxe,  Rebecca  How  we  know   it  hurts:   item  analysis  of  written   narratives   reveals   distinct   neural   responses   to   others'   physical   pain   and  emotional  suffering..  (2013)  PLoS  One.  2013  Apr  26;8(4):  e63085.      

Bruneau,  Emile;  Pluta,  Agnieszka;  Saxe,  Rebecca:  (2012)  Distinct  roles  of  the  ‘Shared  Pain’   and   ‘Theory   of  Mind’   networks   in   processing   others’   emotional   suffering.  Neuropsychologia.  Volume  50,  Issue  2,  January  2012,  Pages  219–231  

Cikara,   Mina;   Bruneau,   Emile   and   Saxe,   Rebecca   (2010)   Us   and   Them:  Intergroup   Failures   of  Empathy   (2010)   Current   Directions   in   Psychological  Science  20(3)  149-­‐153  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  28  

Damasio,  Antoniou  (2006)  Descartes'  Error:  Emotion,  Reason  and  the  Human  Brain.  Vintage  Press.    

Dawkins,  Richard  (1989)  The  Selfish  Gene  Oxford  Paperbacks.  

De   Dreu,   C.K;   Greer,   L;   Van   Kleef,   GA;   Shalvi,   S;   Handgraaf,   M.J   (January  2011).     Oxytocin   promotes   human   ethnocentrism.   Proceedings   of   the   National  Academy  of  Sciences.  U.S.A.  108  (4):  1262–6.  

Decety,   J   &   Michalska,   K.J.   (2010).   Neurodevelopmental   changes   in   the   circuits  underlying   empathy   and   sympathy   from   childhood   to   adulthood.   Developmental  Science.  13,  886-­‐899.  

Eres,  Robert  and  Molenberghs,  Pascal,   (2013)  The   influence  of  group  membership  on   the   neural   correlates   involved   in   empathy.   Frontiers   in   Human   Neuroscience.  2013;  7:  176.    

Frith,   Chris   (2007)  Making  Up   the  Mind:  How   the  Brain   Creates  Our  Mental  World  Wiley  Blackwell  

Gallese,  V;  Eagle,  M.N  and  Migone,  P.  (2007)  Intentional  attunement:  mirror  neurons  and   the   neural   underpinnings   of   interpersonal   relations.   Journal   of   the   American  Psychoanalytic  Association.  2007  Winter;  55(1):  131-­‐76.  

Goldstein,   Joshua:   (2001)  War  and  Gender:  How  Gender  Shapes  the  War  System  and  Vice  Versa.  Cambridge  University  press.    

Greene,  J.D.  (2009)  The  Cognitive  Neuroscience  of  Moral  Judgment.  In:  Gazzaniga  MS  The  Cognitive  Neurosciences  4th  Ed.  Cambridge,  MA:  MIT  Press;  2009.    Greene,   Joshua:   (2014)  Moral  Tribes:  Emotion,  Reason  and  the  Gap  Between  Us  and  Them.    Atlantic  Books.      

Haidt,  Jonathan:  (2003)  The  Righteous  Mind:  Why  Good  People  are  Divided  by  Politics  and  Religion.  Penguin    Hammerstein,   Peter   (2003)  Genetic  and  Cultural  Evolution  of  Cooperation   (Dahlem  Workshop  Reports)  MIT  Press  

Heinrichs,   Markus;   Baumgartner,   Thomas;   Kirschbaum,   Clemens;   Ehlert,   Ulrike  (2003).   Social   support   and   oxytocin   interact   to   suppress   cortisol   and   subjective  responses  to  psychosocial  stress.  Biological  Psychiatry  54  (12):  1389–1398.      

Hibbing   John;   Smith,   Kevin;   Alford,   John:    Predisposed:  Liberals,  Conservatives,  and  the  Biology  of  Political  Differences  (2103)  Routledge  Press.  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  29  

Iacaboni,  Marco:  (2008)  Mirroring  People:  The  New  Science  of  How  We  Connect  with  Others.  Farrar,  Straus  and  Giroux.    

Keysers,   Christian   and   Gazzola,   Valeria   (2010).  "Social   Neuroscience:   Mirror  Neurons  recorded  in  Humans".  Current  Biology  20  (8):  R353–354.    

Kirsch,  P;  Esslinger,  C;  Chen,  Q;  Mier,  D;  Lis,  S;  Siddhanti,  S;  Gruppe,  H;  Mattay,  VS;  Gallhofer,  B;  Meyer-­‐Lindenberg,  A;   "Oxytocin  modulates  neural   circuitry   for   social  cognition  and  fear  in  humans".  J.  Neurosciences.  25  (49):  11489–93.  .  

Kosfeld,   M;   Heinrichs,   M;   Zak,   P.   J;   Fischbacher,   U.;   Fehr,   E.   (2005).   Oxytocin  increases  trust  in  humans.  Nature  435  (7042):  673–676.      

Kubota,   J.T;   Banaji,   M.R;   Phelps,   E.A.   (2012).   The   neuroscience   of   race.   Nature  Neuroscience,  15,  940-­‐8.    

Lieberman,  Matthew:   (2013)  Social:  Why  Our  Brains  Are  Wired  To  Connect.   Oxford    UniversityPress.  

Mesquita,  B;  &  Leu,  J.  (2007)  The  cultural  psychology  of  emotions.  In  Kitayama,  S  &  Cohen,D.    (Eds.),  Handbook  for  cultural  psychology  (pp.734-­‐759).  New  York:  Guilford  Press.    McKell,  R;  Bowling,  Daniel;  Reeck,  Crystal;  Huettel,  Scott.   (2012)  A  Distinct  Role  of  the   Temporal-­‐Parietal   Junction   in   Predicting   Socially   Guided   Decisions.   Science  6  July  2012:  Vol.  337  no.  6090  pp.  109-­‐111    

Milgram,   S.   (1963).   Behavioral   study   of   obedience.  Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Social  Psychology,  67,  371-­‐378.  

Milgram,  S.  (1974).  Obedience  To  Authority:  An  Experimental  View.  Harper  Collins.  

Mukamel,  Roy;  Ekstrom,  Arne;  Kaplan,  Jonas;  Lacoboni,  Marco;  Fried,  Itzhak.  (2010)  Single   neuron   responses   in   humans   during   execution   and   observation   of   actions.  Current  Biology.  Apr  27,  2010;  20(8):  750–756.  

 Murray,  Greg  and  Schmitz,  David    (2011)  Caveman  Politics:  Evolutionary  Leadership  Preferences  and  Physical  Stature.  Social  Science  Quarterly  Volume  92,  Issue  5,  pages  1215–1235,  December  2011  

 Norenzayan,   Ara   (2013)   Big   Gods:   How   Religion   Transformed   Cooperation   and  Conflict.  Princeton  University  press.    

Olson,   Gary   In   (2007)   We   Empathize,   Therefore   We   Are:   Toward   a   Moral  Neuropolitics.  Neuropolitics    http://neuropolitics.org/defaultnov08.asp    

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  30  

Phelps,   Elizabeth:   (2012)     Neuroscience   and   Social   Conflict:   Identifying   New  Approaches  for  the  21st  Century.  Project  of   Justice   in  Times  of  Transition   (Now  re-­‐named  Beyond  Conflict)  and  MIT  conference  Cambridge,  MAFebruary  9-­‐11,  2012    Preston,   S.   D,   and   de   Waal,   F.   B.   (2002).   Empathy:   its   ultimate   and   proximate  bases.  Behavioural  and  Brain.  Sciences.  25,  1–20;  discussion  20–71.      Putnam,   Robert;   Feldstein,   Lewis   and   Cohen,   Donald.   (2005)   Better   Together:  Restoring  the  American  Community.  Simon  and  Schuster.  

Ramanchandran,   V.  (2000)  Mirror   Neurons   and   Imitation   Learning   as   the   Driving  Force   Behind   ‘the   Great   Leap   Forward   in   Human   Evolution,”   Edge,   69,   June   29,  2000.    www.edge.org/3rdculture/ramachandran/ramachandranindex.html.  

Schulte-­‐Rüther,  M;  Markowitsch,   HJ;   Shah,   NJ;   Fink,   GR;   Piefke,  M   (2008).   Gender  differences  in  brain  networks  supporting  empathy.  NeuroIimage  42  (1):  393–403.  

Shurick,   A;   Hamilton,   J.R;   Harris,   L.T;   Roy,   A.K;   Gross,   J.J;   Phelps,   E.A.   (2012).  Durable   effects   of   cognitive   restructuring   on   constructed   fear.  Emotion,  12,  1393-­‐7.    

Singer,   Tania,   and   Lamm,   Clauss,   (2009)   The   Social   Neuroscience   of   Empathy.  Annals   of   the  New  York  Academy   of   Sciences.   Volume   1156,   The   Year   in   Cognitive  Neuroscience  2009pages  81–96,  March  2009  

Steinfurth,  E.C;  Kanen,  J.W;  Raio,  C.M;  Clem,  R.L;  Huganir,  R.L  &  Phelps,  E.A.  (2014).  Young  and  old  Pavolovian   fear  memories  can  be  modified  with  extinction   training  during  reconsolidation  in  humans.  Learning  &  Memory,  21,  338-­‐341.  

Tabak,   BA;   McCullough,   ME;   Carver,   CS;   Pedersen,   EJ;   Cuccaro   M.L.   (May   2013).  Variation   in   oxytocin   receptor   gene   (OXTR)   polymorphisms   is   associated   with  emotional  and  behavioral  reactions  to  betrayal.  Social  Cognitive  Affect  Neuroscience.    

Price  Tangney,  June;  Stuewig,  Jeff;  and  Mashek,  Debra  J  (2007):  Moral  Emotions  and  Moral  Behavior.  Annual  Review  Psychology  2007:58:345-­‐372  

Tayama    &  D.    Cohen  Eds.   (2009)  Handbook    for   cultural       psychology.  The  Year  In  Cognitive   Neuroscience   2009  http://greatergood.berkeley.edu/images/uploads/Singer_2009.pdf    Tomasello,  M.  (2009).  Why  We  Cooperate.  Cambridge,  MA:  MIT  Press.  

Young,  L.  J.,  J.  T.  Winslow,  Z.  Wang,  B.  Gingrich,  Q.  Guo,  M.  M.  Matzuk,  and  T.  R.  Insel.  1997.   Gene   targeting   approaches   to   neuroendocrinology:   oxytocin,   maternal  behavior,  and  affiliation.  Hormones  and  Behaviour  31  (3):  221-­‐31.  

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  31  

Zak,  P.  J;  Kurzban,  R;  Matzner,  W.  T.  (2004).  "The  Neurobiology  of  Trust".  Annals  of  the  New  York  Academy  of  Sciences  1032:  224–227.      

Zak,   Paul.   (2012)  The  Moral  Molecule:  The  New  Science  Of  What  Makes  Us  Good  Or  Evil.  Bantam    

 

Sources:  Section  5.  How  we  fool  our  selves  and  others:  The  Believing  Brain    

Amodio,  David;   John  T  Jost,  Sarah  L  Master  &  Cindy  M  Yee  (2007).  Neurocognitive  correlates  of  liberalism  and  conservatism  Nature  Neuroscience  10  (11):  1246–1247.  doi:10.1038/nn1979.  

Ariely,   Dan   (2012)  The   (Honest)  Truth  about  Dishonesty:  How  We  Lie   to  Everyone-­‐Especially  Ourselves.  Harper      Bridge,   Donna,   Paller,   Ken   :(2012)   Neural   Correlates   of   Reactivation   and  Retrieval-­‐Induced   Distortion.   The   Journal   of   Neuroscience,  29   August  2012,  32(35):  12144-­‐12151  .    Briggs   Rachel,   Frennet,   Ross   (2014)Foreign   Fighters,   The   Challenge   of   Counter  Narratives   .   Institute   for   Strategic   Dialogue.  http://www.strategicdialogue.org/Foreign_Fighters_paper_for_website_v0.6.pdf  

Brown,   Scott   (2012)   Elections   Coming?   ‘Got   to   leave   that   brain  outside’   Global  Perspectives  https://scottbrownscerebralcaffeine.wordpress.com/2012/10/  Feynman,   Richard   (1974)     Caltech   speech  https://www.lhup.edu/~DSIMANEK/cargocul.htm  

Fine,  Cordelia  (2007)  A  Mind  of  Its  Own:  How  Your  Brain  Distorts  and  Deceives    Fowler,   James;   Darren   Schreiber   (2008).   "Biology,   Politics,   and   the   Emerging  Science  of  Human  Nature"  (PDF).  Science  322  (5903):  912–914..    

Frank,   Thomas   (2004)  What's   the  Matter  with  Kansas?  How  Conservatives  Won  the  Heart  of  America.  Holt  Mc  Dougal  

Haidt,   Jonathan   (2012)   The   Righteous   Mind:   Why   Good   People   Are   Divided   By  Politics  And  Religion.  Allen  Lane,  London.    Hanson,   Jon   and   McGroger,   Nail   (2006)   Ideology,   Psychology,   and   Law.   Oxford  University  Press.    Hatemi   et   al.   2014.   “Genetic   Influences   on   Political   Ideologies:   Genome-­‐Wide  Findings   on   Three   Populations,   and   a   Mega-­‐Twin   Analysis   of   19   Measures   of  Political  Ideologies  from  Five  Western  Democracies.”  Behavior  Genetics      

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  32  

Hatemi,  Peter,  Funk,  Carolyn,  Medland,  Sarah,  Maes,  Hermine,  Silberg,  Jody,  Martin,  Nicholas,   Lindon,J:   (2009)   Genetic   and   Environmental   Transmission   of   Political  Attitudes  Over  a  Life  Time,  The  Journal  of  Politics,  July  21,  2009    Hatemi,   P   Byrne   E,  McDermott   R   Introduction:   (2014)  What   Is   a   ‘Gene’   and  Why  Does  It  Matter  for  Political  Science?  Journal  of  Theoretical  Politics  24(3):  305    Hatemi,   Peter,   McDermott,   R:   (2011)   Man   Is   By  Nature  A  Political  Animal:  Evolution,  Biology,  And  Politics  .  University  of  Chicago.    Hatemi,  Peter:  (2012)  The  Intersection  of  Behavioral  Genetics  and  Political  Science:    ICON  Books    

Jost,   John;   Amodio,   David   (2011)   Political   ideology   as   motivated   social  cognition:Behavioral  and  Neuroscientific  evidence.  Motivation  and  Emotion,  36,  55-­‐64.   http://amodiolab.org/wp-­‐content/uploads/2012/01/Jost-­‐Amodio-­‐in-­‐press-­‐Political-­‐Neuroscience-­‐Review.pdf  

Kanai,   Ryota,   Feilden,   Tom,   [Firth,   Colin],   and   Rees,   Geraint:   (2011)   Political  Orientations  Are  Correlated  with  Brain  Structure   in  Young  Adults.  Current  Biology    Volume  21,  Issue  8,  p677–680,  26  April  2011  

McRaney,   David   (2013)   You   Are   Now   Less   Dumb:   How   to   Conquer  Mob  Mentality,  How   to  Buy  Happiness,   and  All   the  Other  Ways   to  Outsmart   Yourself.   Gildan  Media  Corporation  

Milgram,  S.   (1963).  Behavioral   Study  Of  Obedience.  Journal  of  Abnormal  and  Social  Psychology,  67,  371-­‐378.  

Runciman,   David   :   (2008)   Political   Hypocrisy:   The  Mask   of   Power,   from  Hobbes   to  Orwell  and  Beyond,  Princeton  Press.      Sacks,  Oliver:  (2011)  The  Man  Who  Mistook  His  Wife  for  a  Hat.    Picador.  Schreiber,   Darren   (2005).  Evaluating   politics:   A   search   for   the   neural   substrates   of  political  thought  (Ph.D.).  University  of  California,  Los  Angeles.    

Stern,  Jessica  (2004)  Terror  in  the  Name  of  God:  Why  Religious  Militants  Kill.  .  Harper  Perrenial.    

Trivers,  Robert  (2011)  Deceit  and  Self-­‐Deception:  Fooling  Yourself  the  Better  to  Fool  Others.  Allen  Lane  .    Westen,  Drew;  Blagov,  Pavel   S;  Harenski,  Keith;  Clint,  Kilts   and     Stephan,  Hamann  (2006).   Neural   Bases   of   Motivated   Reasoning:   An   fMRI   Study   of   Emotional  Constraints   on   Partisan   Political   Judgment   in   the   2004   U.S.   Presidential   Election  Journal  of  Cognitive  Neuroscience  18  (11):  1947–1958.    

An  Introduction  to  Neuroscience  for  the  Peacebuilder.    Mari  Fitzduff  

  33  

Westen,  Drew  (2008).  The  Political  Brain:  The  Role  of  Emotion  in  Deciding  the  Fate  of  the  Nation.  

Woollett,   Katherine,   and    Maguire,   Eleanor,     (2011)  Acquiring   “the  Knowledge”   of  London's   Layout   Drives   Structural   Brain   Changes   Current   Biology   Dec   20,   2011;  21(24-­‐2):  2109–2114.  

Zak,  Paul:  (2012)  The  Moral  Molecule:  The  New  Science  Of  What  Makes  Us  Good  Or  Evil.  Bantam  press    

Zoltan,   Fazekas   and   Hatemi,   Peter:     (2014)   The   Integration   of   Genetic   and  Environmental   Approaches   to   Political   Science:   Methodological   Advances   and  Future   Applications   in   Scott,   A.   and     Kosslyn,   S.M   (2014)   Emerging  Trends   in   the  Social  and  Behavioral  Sciences,  Oxford:  Wiley