The Nonconscious Influence of ReligiousSymbols in Motivated Performance Situations

60
http://www.academia.edu/278250/ The_Nonconscious_Influence_of_Religious_Symbols_In_Motivated_P erformance_Situations The Nonconscious Influence of ReligiousSymbols in Motivated Performance Situations Max Weisbuch-Remington Mercer University Wendy Berry Mendes Harvard University Mark D. Seery University of California–Irvine Jim Blascovich University of California–Santa Barbara Anthropological,sociological,andpsychologicaltheoriess uggest that religious symbols should influence motivational processes during performance of goal-relevant tasks. In two experiments, positive and negative religious (Christian) symbols were pre- sented outside of participants’ conscious awareness. These sym- bols influenced cardiovascular responses consistent with chal- lenge and threat states during a subsequent speech task, particularly when the speech topic concerned participants’ mor- tality, and only for Christian participants; similar images lack- ing Christian meaning were not influential. Results suggested that these effects were due to the learned meaning of the symbols and point to the importance of religion as a coping resource. Keywords:

Transcript of The Nonconscious Influence of ReligiousSymbols in Motivated Performance Situations

http://www.academia.edu/278250/The_Nonconscious_Influence_of_Religious_Symbols_In_Motivated_Performance_Situations

The Nonconscious Influence of ReligiousSymbols in Motivated Performance Situations

Max Weisbuch-Remington

Mercer University 

 Wendy Berry Mendes

Harvard University 

Mark D. Seery 

University of California–Irvine 

 Jim Blascovich

University of California–Santa Barbara Anthropological,sociological,andpsychologicaltheoriessuggest that religious symbols should influence motivational processes during performance of goal-relevant tasks. In two experiments, positive and negative religious (Christian) symbols were pre- sented outside of participants’ conscious awareness. These sym- bols influenced cardiovascular responsesconsistent with chal- lenge and threat states during a subsequent speech task, particularly when the speech topic concerned participants’ mor- tality, and only for Christian participants; similar images lack- ing Christian meaning were not influential. Results suggested that these effects were dueto the learned meaning of the symbols and point to the importance of religion as a coping resource.

Keywords: 

religion; subliminal; symbols; challenge; threat; coping; automatic 

Cultural and religious icons and symbols have beenexamined from a variety of theoretical perspectives,including anthropological (e.g., Geertz, 1973), psychological (e.g.,Jung,1964), andsociological(e.g.,Barthes,1957). According to these perspectives, objects that rep-resent religious concepts (henceforth, religious sym-bols) exert considerable influence on many social psy-chological processes, including those associated withcoping. Some (e.g., Jung, 1964) argue that consciousawareness of a religious symbol is not necessary for it toexert an influence. Similarly, Blascovich and Mendes(2000) theorized that affective symbols influence moti- vational states both consciously and unconsciously during motivated performance situations.

Religion and Coping 

 A variety of theories within and outside of social psy-chology converge on the notion that religion plays animportantrole for people coping with the demands of uncertain or otherwise difficult situations. Existentialtheorists(Becker,1973;Kierkegaard,1844/1981;Sartre,1956), for example, suggest that religion functions tohelpindividualscopewithanotherwiseunbearablyuncer-tain and potentially meaningless life. Such accountsimplythatreligionislikelytobeanespeciallyusefulcop-ingresource when existential uncertainties (e.g., death,morality)become salient. For William James (1936/1997, Lectures 4 & 5),religion offered the individual acertain optimism about life that helps to ameliorateone’s psychological and even physical problems. Simi-larly, Emile Durkheim (1912/1995) suggested that reli-gious faith could restore an individual’s sense of well-being during and after frustration and/or loss. Finally,numerous passages from the Bible and other

religioustracts recommend reliance on God in times of despair.1203

 Authors’Note:

This research was partially supported by National Sci-ence Foundation Graduate Research Fellowships to Max Weisbuch-Remington and Mark Seery and a National Research Service Award(MH12013) to Wendy Berry Mendes. We are grateful to ChristianUnkelbach, Corey Crouch, and Linda Valleni for providing assistance with data collection and administration. Correspondence concerningthis article should be addressed to Max Weisbuch-Remington, Depart-ment of Psychology, Mercer University, Macon, GA 31207; e-mail:[email protected].

PSPB,

Vol. 31 No. 9, September 2005 1203-1216DOI: 10.1177/0146167205274448© 2005 by the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc.

 

Other, more recent perspectives linking religion tocopinglargelymirrortheearlieraccounts.Forexample,in line with the existential view of religion and coping,terrormanagementtheory(TMT;Greenberg,Solomon,& Pyszczynski, 1997), “belief in a just world” theory (BJW; Lerner, 1980), as well as theories of uncertainty reduction (e.g., Kruglanski, 1989; McGregor, 2003) sug-gest that religious worldviews and beliefs help the indi- vidualcopewithanunpredictableandpotentiallymean-ingless world by offering a structured prescription forlife.Similar toJames (1936/1997) and Durkheim (1912/1995), Lazarus and Folkman (1984) have argued that religious beliefs can give

rise to hope, optimism, andmeaning in otherwise damaging circumstances. Suchhopeful beliefs may serve to sustain copingefforts inadverse conditions. However, departing from some of theearliertheorists,LazarusandFolkmanalsoexplicitly argued that religious belief could have negative conse-quences for coping, depending on the activated reli-gious beliefs. LazarusandFolkman (1984)noted that“abeliefinapunitiveGodcanleadapersontoacceptadis-tressing situation as punishment and to do nothingabout mastering or managing the situational demands”(p. 160). Pargament (1997), echoing Lazarus andFolkman,suggestedthatreligiouscopingcantakeeithera positiveor a negative form and that the active beliefsduring coping determine the relevant physical andmental health outcomes.

Religious Symbols and Coping 

 Anthropologistsandsociologistsalike (e.g.,Durkheim,1912/1995;Evans-Pritchard, 1956; Geertz, 1973; Levi-Strauss, 1964) suggest that symbols are a necessary aspect of religion. Geertz (1973) argued, for instance,that religion can be defined as “a system of symbols which acts to establish powerful, pervasive, and long-lastingmoodsandmotivationsin(people)”(p.90).Therole of religious symbols in coping with difficult situa-tions, however, is less clear than the role of religion incoping. Although the study of religious symbols in par-ticularandculturalsymbolsingeneralhasbeenanactivearea of research in other disciplines, little mainstreamsocial psychological research has focused on such sym-bols (but see Greenberg, Porteus, Simon, & Pyszczinski,1995).Giventhatreligionplaysanimportantroleincop-ing processes, it seems likely that religious symbols alsomight play a significant role. According to many scholars,the effects of religioussymbols are likely to be almost immediate, perhapsevenautomatic (cf. Bargh, 1994). Consistent with Geertz(1973), scholars across disciplines suggest that culturaland religious symbols reinforce complex ideas in

animmediate andemotionally powerfully way(e.g.,Freud,1938/2000; Jung, 1964; Ortner, 1973). Anthropologist SherryOrtner(1973)arguedthatreligioussymbolssuchas a crucifix are of a class of “summarizing symbols” that function to make the defining points of an ideology immediately salient. Ortner (1973) argued that “theimportant mode of operation for summarizing sym-bols...isitsfocusingpower,itsdrawingtogether,intensi-fying,catalyzingimpactupontherespondent”(p.1342).Indeed,theanthropologistSusanLanger(1942)arguedthatasymbolcanbedefinedassuchbyitsabilitytoauto-matically give rise to conceptions associated with (but not redundant with) the presented object (p. 60). Forexample, a cross is an object or image (i.e., two perpen-dicularlines)thatbecomesasymbolonlywhenitevokesimages of Christianity. If symbols draw together impor-tant and perhaps complex beliefs in an immediate andintense fashion, then it follows that even brief exposureto religious symbols might activate religious copingmechanisms.Consistentwiththeseideasregardingtheinfluenceof religioussymbols,CarlJung(1964)arguedthatsymbolicreligiousimagesmanifestedinreallife(notjustdreams)are likely toreflect enduring andunconscious collectiverepresentations—in other words, objects that automati-cally represent important religious themes. The mean-ing of these symbols may or may notbe evident to one’sconscious mind. Moreover, Jung (1964) suggested that given the limits of conscious perceptual ability, peopleoftenencounter suchcollective representations without anyawarenessthattheyhave,infact,beenexposedtothesymbol. He argued that encountering such religioussymbols is likely to have profound consequences forone’s experience in the world. It is hypothesized herethat exposure to religious symbols outside of consciousawarenesscaninfluenceone’sexperience,specificallyincoping with potentially threatening performancesituations.

The Biopsychosocial Model of Challenge and Threat 

The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000; Blascovich & Tomaka,1996) provides a means to assess coping responses dur-ing potentially threatening performance situations. Inthis investigation, we focus on challenge and threat cop-ing responses within motivated performance situations.Blascovich and colleagues define motivated perfor-mance situations as goal-relevant (i.e., having implica-tions for personal well-being) performance situationsrequiring instrumental (i.e., active rather than passive)cognitive responses. Examples include interviews, test taking,speeches,gameplaying,andinterpersonalnego-tiations. Within such situations, challenge occurs whenpersonal and/or situational resources (skills, knowl-1204 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

 

edge, abilities, social support) are evaluated as meetingor exceeding situational demands (e.g., danger,required effort). Threat occurs when resources do not meet situational demands. Although typically character-ized as discrete states, challenge and threat represent endpoints of a bipolar continuum, such that individualsmay be more or less challengedthan others.Blascovichandcolleagueshavevalidatedcardiovascu-lar markers of challenge and threat on the basis of pat-terns of neurally and hormonally controlled cardiovas-cular responses (for reviews, see Blascovich & Mendes,2000; Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996). Based onDienstbier’s (1989) physiological toughness pattern,challenge includes the activation of the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM) axis, which enhances cardiacperformance—particularlyheartrate(HR)andleftven-tricular contractility (VC)—and decreases systemic vas-cular resistance (total peripheral resistance [TPR]), aneffect mediated by the releaseof epinephrine at theperipheral arteries. As a result of theseeffects, cardiacoutput (CO) is increased during challenge. In contrast,threat is marked by activation not only of the SAM axis,againincreasingcardiacperformance(HRandVC),but also by

activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA; in previous work referred to aspituitary-adrenocortical or PAC)axis,which inhibits therelease of epinephrine, resulting in relatively higher sys-temic vascular resistance (TPR) and little or no changein CO during threat.

1

To meet the strict definition of a marker (i.e.,Cacioppo & Tassinary, 1990), the challenge and threat cardiovascularindexeshavebeenvalidatedinmorethan20 experiments. Initially, studies showed that withinmotivated performance situations, self-reports of chal-lenge and threat (a) were correlated with the cardiovas-cularindexes,(b)were notcausedbythecardiovascularpatterns,and(c)increasedinparallelwiththecardiovas-cular indexes following challenging versus threateninginstructional sets (Tomaka, Blascovich, & Kelsey, 1993;Tomaka, Blascovich, Kibler, & Ernst, 1997; for a review,see Blascovich & Tomaka, 1996). Because self-reportshave some limitations (e.g., social desirability, ordereffects, failures of introspection), it also was desirable toobserve whether the cardiovascular patterns of chal-lenge and threat (sans self-report) could be predictedbased on established social psychological theory.Indeed, research has shown that the cardiovascularindexesofchallengeandthreathaveconfirmedhypoth-eses derived from work on social comparison (e.g.,Mendes,Blascovich,Major,&Seery,2001),socialfacilita-tion(Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Salomon, 1999),intergroup interaction (Blascovich, Mendes, & Seery,2002;Mendes,Blascovich,Lickel,&Hunter,2002),emo-tional disclosure (Mendes, Reis, Seery, & Blascovich,2003), belief ina just world (Tomaka & Blascovich,1994), social stigma (Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter,Lickel, & Kowai-Bell, 2001), self-esteem (Seery,Blascovich, Weisbuch, & Vick, 2004), and loneliness(Hawkley, Burleson, Berntson, & Cacioppo, 2003).In the context of the current investigation, thesepsychophysiological markers provide several advan-tages.

First, appraisals including resource and demandevaluations canbe both conscious and nonconscious(Blascovich&Mendes,2000;Lazarus,1982,1984).Laza-rus (1982) has argued, for example, that responses tostimuli outside of awareness are due to preconsciousevaluationsofthosestimuli.Indeed,researchhasshownthat stimuli outside of conscious awareness can influ-ence components of resource and demand evaluations(e.g., self-evaluation: Baldwin, Carrell, & Lopez, 1990;danger: LeDoux, 1996). Similarly, and as noted above, Jung (1964) suggested that responses to religious sym-bols may not always be consciously accessible. In fact,Baldwin et al. (1990) observeddifferences in self-evaluation following subliminal exposure to a religiousicon (the Pope). Thus, the predictions noted below,derivedfromtheliteraturereviewabove,shouldholdfornonconscious as well as conscious processes. Methodsthatrequireconsciousself-reflection(i.e.,self-report)tomeasure challenge and threat may not be sensitive tochanges in nonconscious evaluation processes such asthose that may be influenced by religious symbols. Psy-chophysiological markers potentially measure noncon-scious as well as conscious responses, thereby allowingfor a more robustassessment of the influence of reli-gious symbols.Second, suchmarkers allow for the avoidance of self-presentational problems (Crowne & Marlowe, 1964;Hessing, Elffers, & Weigel, 1988; Roth, Snyder, & Pace,1986) that might be especially problematic when usingself-report techniques to measure religious persons’responses to religious symbols. Finally, such measuresallow for the assessment of coping (challenge andthreat) responses during the episode itself and thereby avoid limitations in retrospective and prospective self-reports of internal states and behavior (Newby-Clark &Ross, 2003; Ross, 1989; Vallone, Griffin, Lin, & Ross,1990).

Hypotheses 

 We proposed several hypotheses regarding the influ-ence of religious symbols during potentially stressfulperformance

situations. First, religious symbols shouldinfluence cardiovascular responses consistent with chal-lenge and threatmotivational states. Specifically, posi-tive religious symbols, signifying resources available toreligious persons (e.g., for Christians, Jesus and thechurch as supportive resources) and the rewards of sub- Weisbuch-Remington et al. /RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1205

 

scribingtoreligiousbeliefs(e.g.,ascenttoheaven),shouldincreaseresource evaluations leading to challenge. Asnoted,previousresearchhasprovidedevidencethatreli-gious icons (i.e., the Pope) can nonconsciously influ-ence self-evaluations (Baldwin et al., 1990). Negativereligious symbols,signifying the dangerous aspects of one’s religious beliefs (e.g., demons and hell), shouldincreasedemandevaluationsandleadtothreat.Indeed,researchhas shown that evaluations of danger can beinfluenced through nonconscious channels (seeLeDoux, 1996). Also, positive religious symbols may decrease demand evaluations and negativereligioussymbols may decrease resource evaluations (if the sym-bolsdiscouragetheuseofreligionasacopingresource).Taken together,thesepostulatesare consistentwithLaz-arus and Folkman(1984) and Pargament (1997), whosuggest that religion may serve both positive and nega-tive functions in coping.Second, religious symbols should evoke responsesevenwhensuchsymbolsareoutsideofparticipants’con-scious awareness. If collective representations such asreligious symbols need not reach awareness to be influ-ential, then such symbols should have the capacity toinfluence(cardiovascular components of) motivationalstates outside of participants’ conscious awareness. Fur-thermore, religious symbols are likely to convey power-ful meaning in an immediate, summary form (e.g.,Ortner, 1973) and therefore should exert an influenceeven when exposure to such symbols isbrief. Indeed, within social psychology, a large empirical literaturedemonstrates that unreportable stimuli in general

(i.e.,not necessarily religious) can be influential. Aggressivebehavior, interpretations of others’ behavior, attitudes,memory ability, and even physiological responses havebeen influenced by stimuli presented outside of partici-pants’ awareness (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1996; Chen& Bargh, 1997; Devine, 1989; Dijksterhuis, Aarts, Bargh,& van Knippenberg, 2000; Murphy & Zajonc, 1993;Ohman & Mineka, 2001;Weisbuch, Mackie, & Garcia-Marques, 2003). In addition, previous research suggeststhataffectivelyvalencedimagescaninfluencephysiolog-ical responses even when such images are unreportable(for reviews, see LeDoux, 1996; Ohman & Mineka,2001).Third, religious symbols should influence only thosepersons for whom the symbols represent meaningfulresourcesordemands.Forexample,imagesofJesusmay not offer any resource-enhancing support for individu-als who do not believe thatJesus was divine. Thus,Chris-tiansymbols shouldaffectcardiovascular responses con-sistent with challenge and threat for Christians but not non-Christians.Fourth, following the reasoning of TMT and others, we predicted that religious symbols would be especially influential during a task that addressed an existentialquestion; specifically, discussing one’s own mortality.Not only might this situation specifically call forth reli-giouscopingmechanismsbutreligioussymbolsalsomay beencodedasespeciallyimportantinlightofanexisten-tial issue. In support of this hypothesis, research hasshown thatpeople are more likely to treat religious sym-bols (a cross) as sacred following thoughts of an inevita-ble demise (vs. thinking about television; Greenberget al., 1995).

EXPERIMENT 1

Overview 

In this experiment, we addressed Hypotheses 1, 2,and 4. We reasoned that unreportable religious symbols would influence

cardiovascular responses consistent  with challenge and threatstates during a motivated per-formance situation. Participantspresented with Chris-tian religious symbols outside of their conscious aware-ness subsequently engaged in a motivated performancetask. The motivated performance task was manipulatedso that half of the participants were assigned to deliver aspeech relevant to existential issues (their own mortal-ity), whereas the other participants were assigned todeliver a less relevant (but still negatively toned) speech(visiting the dentist; for similar methodology, seeGreenberg et al., 1990). We hypothesized that participants exposed to nega-tive religious symbols would exhibit threat during thespeech,ascomparedtoparticipantsexposedtotheposi-tivereligious symbols and as measured through the car-diovascularmarkersofchallengeandthreat.Wehypoth-esized that this pattern would be more pronouncedamong participants givingthe speech on death.

Method Participants and setting 

. Participants were selectedbasedontheirresponse(duringaprescreeningsession)to thequestion, “With what religion were you raised?”OnlythosewhoreportedbeingraisedasChristianswereconsidered for selection. This selection criterion wasusedtoensurethatparticipantshadenoughexperience with Christianity such that they most likely would havelearned the meaning of these symbols. This criterionresulted in 115 undergraduate students participating inthis experiment in exchange for course credit.

2

ThesocialpsychophysiologylaboratoryintheDepart-mentofPsychologyattheUniversityofCalifornia,SantaBarbara,servedastheexperimentalsetting.Thislabora-tory contains separate control, participant preparation,and recording rooms as well as physiological recording,audiovisual, and computer equipment.1206 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

 

Stimuli 

. Stimuli were culled from a variety of religiousbooks. Criteria for selecting symbols were to choose sym-bols(a)soasnottonotconfoundfamiliaritywithvalence(i.e., only relatively obscure symbols were chosen); (b)that contained a variety of colors and complexity—so,for example, not all positive symbols were simple or allnegative symbols complex; and (c) whose positive andnegative valence and intensity were equivalent (seebelow). Selection of symbols resulted in 10 negative and11 positive symbols. Figure 1 depicts examples. Positivesymbols included images of Christ ascending to heaven,Mother Mary holding the Baby Jesus, and Christ’s heal-ingpowers.Negativesymbolsincludedimagesofsatanic worship, demons, and satanic symbols.Todetermine therelativevalenceandintensityofthesymbols, 31 undergraduates,who did not participate ineither experiment reported here, rated the religioussymbols on two 7-point, Likert-type scales (

bad 

-

good 

;

not disturbing 

disturbing 

).Foreachofthetwoitems,theaver-age ratings for the positive (

a

s = .95 and .91) and nega-tive symbols (

a

s = .85 and .76) were calculated. Asexpected, participants rated the positive symbols (

=5.1) as more positive than the negative symbols (

=3.2),

(1, 29) = 38.8,

< .001, and less disturbing (

=1.8)thanthenegativesymbols(

=4.4),

 F 

(1,29)=95.9,

 p 

<.001.Althoughbothpatternsweresignificantlystron-ger among Christian than non-Christian participants,resultingintwo-wayinteractions,

 F 

(1,29)=17.1,

 p 

<.001,and

(1, 29) = 10.7,

< .01, respectively, both patterns were significant for non-Christian-raised participants,

 F 

(1, 12) = 5.78,

< .05, and

(1, 12) = 38.65,

< .001.Hence, the positive religious symbols appeared to bepositively valenced as compared to the negative symbolsforallparticipants.Theseparticipantsalsowereaskedtoratethe relevance of each symbol—when these ratings were entered as covariates, the interaction betweenChristian background andsymbol valence was elimi-nated,suggesting thatdifferences in positive versus neg-ative ratings of the symbols were due to the strongermeaning that these symbols held for Christians relativeto non-Christians.Masks for each symbol were created by digitally alter-ingtheimages.Eachsymbolhaditsownmask,createdby “tilezing” the religious image (a special effects option in Adobe PhotoShop),which clusters adjacent colors andresults in a stained glass appearance. The resultant image was then rotated 180 degrees.

Physiological measures 

. Cardiac and hemodynamicmeasureswererecordednoninvasivelyusingequipment meeting commercial and hospital safety standards andfollowing guidelines established by the Society forPsychophysiological

Research (e.g., Sherwood et al.,1990). A Minnesota Impedance Cardiograph (Model304B),aCortronics (Model 7000)continuously inflatedbloodpressuremonitor,andaCoulbournECGamplifier/coupler(Model S75-11) provided physiological signals.The impedance signals were conditioned usingCoulbourn amplifiers (Model S79-02).Impedance cardiographic (ZKG) and electro-cardiographic (EKG) recordings provided continuousmeasures of cardiac performance. The former uses atetrapolar aluminum/Mylar tape electrode system toprovidebasaltransthoracicimpedance(Z0)andthefirst derivativeofbasalimpedance(dZ/dt).TwopairsofZKGelectrodes completely encircle the participant. Innerelectrodes are placed at the base of the neck and at thethoracic xiphisternal junction; outer electrodes areplaced on the neck and abdomen. The impedance car-diograph passes a4mA AC 100 kHz current through thetwo outer electrodes and measures Z0 via the two innerelectrodes.Electrocardiograph recordings were obtained usingeither an external EKG Standard Lead II configuration(right arm, left leg, and right leg ground) or via theimpedancecardiograph.TheCortronicsbloodpressuremonitorprovidedcontinuousnoninvasiverecordingsof bloodpressure.Aninteractivesoftwareprogram(Kelsey & Guethlein, 1990) was used to recordand later scorethe cardiac and hemodynamic data.Prior to analyses, reactivity scores (i.e., changes frombaseline resting levels) for all CV variables were calcu-lated.PatternsoffourCVvariablestypicallyidentifychal-lengeandthreatstates:heartrate(HR);ventricularcon-tractility(VC),anindex oftheleftventricle’scontractileforce;cardiacoutput(CO),theamountofbloodinliterspumped by the heart per minute; and total peripheralresistance (TPR), an index of net constriction versusdilation in the vascular system (vasoconstriction vs. Weisbuch-Remington et al. / RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1207

Figure 1ThesymbolsontheleftareexamplesofthenegativeChris-tiansymbols.Thesymbolsontherightareexamplesofthe positive Christian symbols.

 

 vasodilation). For presentational purposes, VC is calcu-latedby multiplying preejection period (PEP) by –1, wherePEPrepresentsthetimeinmillisecondsinthecar-diaccyclefrominitiationofventriculardepolarizationtoopening ofthe aortic valve and ejection of blood; alargerVCvaluethuscorrespondstogreatercontractility.TPR is calculated by dividing mean arterial pressure by cardiac output and multiplying the total by 80(Sherwood etal.,1990).Significantincreases inHRand VCindexedSAMactivation(commontobothchallengeandthreat),whereasCOandTPRwereusedtodifferen-tiate challenge and threat responses more specifically.

3

 As noted above, challenge and threat are endpoints of abipolar continuum—greater CO and lesser TPR indexgreater challenge (less threat)—it is such relativebetween-group differences that were measured here.

Procedures 

. Upon arrival, each participant entered apreparation room andwas presented an informationsheet that described the physiological measures. Theexperimenterthenappliedsensorsnecessaryforphysio-logical recording. The participant sat upright in a com-fortable upholstered chair with a computer keyboardacross his or her lap. The experimenter instructed theparticipant to sit quietlyand relax for several minutes. A 5-minbaselineperiod beganoncetheexperimenter left the room. Cardiovascular responses collected duringthis period served as baseline levels of physiologicalresponses. Physiological recording continued for theduration of the experiment.Next, the participant heard audiotaped instructionsexplaining a tile-counting task. From the participant’sperspective, this task appeared to be a decision-makingtask that involved a series ofimages that had a tile orstained-glass appearance (i.e., the masks), which werepresented on the computer screen. The participants were presented with a variety of these images presentedoff-center in random quadrants of the computer screenand estimated whether the image that they observedcontained fewer or greater than 100 tiles. The tileimages, which actually served as backward masks for theunreportable stimuli (religious symbols), were pre-sentedfor1spersymbolandwerereplacedbyaseriesof five stars in the middle of the screen following each pre-sentation.Participantswereinstructedtofocusattentionon the stars prior to each trial. Immediately followingdisappearance of the stars, a positive or negative Chris-tian symbol was presented parafoveally (minimum of a3.2-degreeangleinthiscase)for30ms.Immediatelyfol-lowing the Christian symbol, the tile mask appeared for1,000 ms. According to Bargh and Chartrand (2000),such parafoveally presented images remain outside of conscious awareness ifsuchimages are presented fornomorethan60ms(conservatively).Limitingpresentationtime to 30 ms and use of a backward mask ensured that the symbols werenot reportable. The presentationphase lasted approximately 1

min.Immediately following the presentation of the sym-bols,participantsheardinstructionsexplainingthatthey  would deliver a speech. Participants were randomly assigned to one of two speech topics—thoughts of yourown deathor visitingthedentist.For theformer,partici-pants were instructedto talk about the thoughts that cametomindwhentheycontemplatedtheirowndeath, what they thought happened to their body when they died, and what in general they believed happened totheir existence once they were dead. For the latterspeech, participants were instructed to elaborate on theprocess of visiting the dentist, what happened when heor she went there, what were some of the proceduresthey have had performed, and their general feelingsregarding visiting the dentist. For both conditions, thespeech topics were displayed on the computer monitorfortheparticipant’sreference.Participantshad1mintoprepareand2mintodeliverthespeech.Theparticipant  was cued by the experimenter, via intercom, when tobegin the preparation, delivery, and end the speech.Cardiovascular responses during the first minute of thespeech served as the critical dependentvariable forsubsequent analyses.Following the speech, the physiological sensors wereremoved from the participant who wasimmediately debriefed by the experimenter. During this debriefing,participantswereaskediftheyhadseenanythingbesidesthe tiles during the tile-counting task. No participantsreportedseeing anything else before or after the tiles.

Results Analytical strategy 

. Mean cardiovascular values werecalculated for each minute within each rest and taskperiod. Univariate outliers (those exceeding more than3.3standarddeviation unitsfrom thegrandmean)weretransformedbyassigningthedeviantrawscoretoavalueone unit larger or smaller than the next most extremescore (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996)—there were foursuch data points. Our analytic strategy included threesteps. First, we tested for baseline resting differencesbetween conditions. Second, weconfirmed SAM-axisactivation by testing VC and HR reactivity

against zero.Last, we performed ANCOVA analyses to examine theeffectsofreligious symbol(positivevs.negative),speech(deathvs.dental),andtheirinteractiononCOandTPR reactivity during the first task minute. Unless otherwisenoted, covariate-adjusted means are reported andCohen’s

is reported as the effect size estimate forrelevant comparisons.

Baseline differences 

. Four separate ANOVAs testing fordifferences in baseline physiological levels by image valence (positive vs. negative) and speech topic (death1208 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

 

 vs.control) revealed no significantmain effects or inter-actions (

 F 

s < 1). As is typical when baseline responses donotdifferamonglevelsofbetween-subjectsfactors,reac-tivityscores(differencesfrombaseline)wereusedastheprimary dependent variables (Llabre, Spitzer, Saab,Ironson, & Schneiderman, 1991). Although changescores (of which reactivity is one example) are some-timesdiscouragedonpsychometricgrounds,theiruseiscommon in psychophysiological research. Indeed,Llabre et al. (1991) concluded that change scores arenot only appropriate but preferable when analyzingphysiological data. In addition, absolute differencesfrom baseline are meaningful within the bio-psychosocial model as challenge and threat indices arebased on patterns of cardiovascular changes (e.g.,Mendes etal., 2003). Reactivity scores were

calculatedforeachcardiovascular measure bysubtractingtheaver-agevaluefromthelastminuteoftherestperiodfromtheaverage value from the first minute of the speech deliv-ery task. Because even nonsignificant error varianceassociated with baseline values can result in artificially inflated or deflated reactivity scores (thereby increasingerror variance in reactivity scores), baseline values alsoare used as covariates(removing this covariate does not change the significance patterns reported herein; seealso Seery et al., 2004).

Challenge and threat markers 

. HR and VC reactivity (indexing SAM axis activity) should increase duringboth challenge and threat. Across all four experimentalconditions,

testsrevealed thatHRdiffered significantly fromzero(i.e.,baseline;all

 p 

s<.0001;covariate-adjustedmeans reported, with standard error in parenthe-sis): Negative Symbols–Death Speech

= 21.2 (2.2);PositiveSymbols–DeathSpeech

=22.7(2.3);NegativeSymbols–Dental Speech

= 22.6 (2.2); PositiveSymbols–Dental Speech

= 20.1 (2.1). Neither themain effects nor the interaction weresignificant whenexamining religious symbol and speech effects on HR reactivity. We then examined VC reactivity during

thespeech. Significant increases in VC from zero (i.e., base-line) were observed across all four experimental condi-tions (all

s < .0001; covariate adjusted means reported, with standard error in parenthesis): Negative Symbols–Death Speech

= 12.7 (2.5); Positive Symbols–DeathSpeech

=18.6(2.6);NegativeSymbols–DentalSpeech

=13.3(2.4);PositiveSymbols–DentalSpeech

=14.9(2.4).Anexamination ofVC differences asafunctionof religious symbol and speech revealed no significant main effects or interactions. The significant VC and HR reactivity data confirmed SAM axis activation, which,according to the biopsychosocial model, justifiedfurtherexam- ination of the CV variables for challengeand threat distinctions.Recall that greater CO and lesser TPR index greaterchallenge (lesser threat). A main effect of valenceemerged for TPR and CO. TPR was significantly higher(

=35.2,

SE 

=25.9)andcardiacoutputwassignificantly lower (

= .07,

SE 

= .189) for participants exposed tonegative symbols than for participants exposed to posi-tive symbols (TPR 

= –61.9,

SE 

= 26.3; CO

= .78,

SE 

=.192),

(1, 102) = 6.84,

< .05,

= .51, and

(1, 102) =6.83,

 p 

<.05,

=.51,forTPRandCO,respectively.Thesedifferences indicate that participants exposed to nega-tive religious symbols were significantly more likely toexhibitthethreatpattern(thereforelesslikelytoexhibit thechal

lengepattern)thanparticipantsexposedtopos-itive religious symbols. Although no other main effectsor interactions approached significance (

 p 

s > .18),planned contrasts supported our prediction that theeffectofimagevalencewouldbemorepronounceddur-ing the death speech compared to the dental speech. AsFigures 2 and 3 depict, for participants giving a speechondeath,thosepresentedwithnegativesymbolshadsig-nificantly higher TPR,

= 68.9 (

SE 

= 37.4),

(1, 105) =7.35,

< .01,

= .75, and lower CO,

= –.17 (

SE 

= .27),

 F 

(1, 105) = 7.35,

< .01,

= .78, than participantsexposed to the positive symbols (TPR:

= –73.7,

SE 

=38.9; CO:

= .90,

SE 

= .28), indicating that participantsexposed to unreportable negative religious symbols were significantly more likely to exhibit the threat pat-tern than were participants similarly exposed to positivesymbols.Amongparticipantsgivingaspeechongoingtothedentist,no effectsofimage valence occurred (

 F 

s

£

1,

s<.27),althoughthedirectionofthemeanswasconsis-tentwiththatobservedduringthedeathspeech andtheeffect sizes inthis comparison were nontrivial. Post hoccomparisons within valence between speech type werenot significant (

 p 

s > .19).

 Discussion 

Consistent with Hypotheses 1 and 2, Experiment 1demonstratedthatreligiousstimulipresentedoutsideof conscious awareness influenced individuals duringmotivatedperformancesituations.Participantsexposedto unreportable negative Christian symbols exhibited acardiovascular pattern consistent with greater threat during a subsequent speech than participants exposedto positive Christian symbols. In addition, consistent  with Hypothesis 4,this effect was significant only forparticipants delivering a speech about their own death.Inherent in our explanation is that the Christianmeaning(ratherthananincidentalelement)ofthesym-bolsinfluencedparticipants.Incontrasttothisassertion,some haveargued that the influence of unreportablestimuli is limited toincidental elements of those stimuli(e.g., Abrams & Greenwald,2000). For example, onemightarguethattheinfluenceofanephemeralimageof  Weisbuch-Remington et al. / RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1209

 

Christ ascending to Heaven was due to a characteristicsuch as the image’s “blueness” rather than the complexreligious meaning of the image. To provide a more con-servativetestandrule outthepossibility thataninciden-tal element evoked by the symbols was the mechanismthrough which cardiovascular differences emerged,Experiment 2 compared the influence of unreportableChristiansymbols tonearlyidentical,scrambled images, which held no religious meaning.

EXPERIMENT 2

Overview 

The second experiment attempted to replicate theresults of thefirst experiment, refine the methodology (ruling out an alternative interpretation), and test thethird hypothesis

proposed above. To rule out the possi-bility that an incidental stimulus feature of the symbolsinfluenced participants, we introduced control condi-tions that employed images that were scrambled toremove the social meaning of the Christian symbols.These scrambled images were otherwise identical to theChristian symbols in size, shape, color, and color ratio.Hence, image type (Christian vs. control) was an addi-tionalfactorinthedesign.Twofurtherrefinementsweremade.First,toaddressthehypothesisthatChristiansym-bolsshouldonlyinfluenceChristians(thirdhypothesis), we added non-Christian participants to our sample andincluded religiousbeliefs (Christian vs. non) as a factor.In addition, only the death speech was used because it  was only during that speech that Christian participantsshoweddifferentialreactionstothenegativeversusposi-tive symbols. Our prediction was that Christian partici-pantswould exhibit a cardiovascular pattern consistent  with greater threat after exposure to the negative sym-bols than after exposure to the positive symbols but that these effects would not occur for non-Christians andthey would not occur when the images lacked Christianmeaning (control images).

Method Participants 

.Twohundredsixundergraduatestudentsfromanintroductorypsychologycourseservedaspartic-ipants in exchange for course credit.

4

 As in the first experiment, participants were selected based on theirresponse (during a prescreening session) to the ques-tion, “With what religion were you raised?” Those that respondedChristian(orsomeChristiandenomination) were included in that group; all others were included inthe non-Christian group.

Control images 

. Control images were created by alter-ing the symbols to remove Christian content. AdobePhotoShopwasusedtofirstblurandthenremovedetailsfrom each Christian symbol (see Figure 4). These con-trol images were then rotated 180 degrees. The samemasks and exposure time thatwere used for the Chris-tiansymbols were used for these control images. Hence,both positive control images and negative controlimageswereusedinadditiontothepositiveandnegativeChristian symbols.

Procedures 

. Procedures were identical to Experiment 1, with the following modifications: (a) all participantsgave death speeches, (b) half of the participants wereexposedonlytothecontrolimages,(c)halfofthepartic-ipantswere notraisedasChristians,and(d)asystematic verbaldebriefingprotocolwasused.Duringthisdebrief-ing,participantswere firstaskedifanythinghadseemeda bit odd during the experiment. Then, they were askedif they had seen anything odd during the tile-countingtask.Itwasonlythenthattheywereaskediftheyhadseenanything just before or just after they saw each tile. One1210 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

-100-75-50-250255075100Death DentalNegative

   T  o   t  a   l   P  e  r   i  p   h  e  r  a   l   R  e  s   i  s   t  a  n  c  e

Positive

Figure 2TPRasafunctionofimagevalenceandspeechcontentinExperiment 1.

NOTE:Numbershigheronthey-axisindicategreaterthreat.TPR=to-tal peripheral resistance.

-1-0.500.51

Death DentalPositive Negative

   C  a  r   d   i  a  c   O  u   t  p  u   t   (   l   /  m   i  n   )

Figure 3COasafunctionofimagevalenceandspeechcontentinEx- periment 1.

NOTE: Numbers lower on the y-axis indicate greater threat. CO = car-diac output.

 

participant had learned about the experiment ahead of time andclaimed he could see religious symbols. Thisparticipant was dropped from analyses.

Results Analytical strategy 

. Mean cardiovascular values werecalculated for each minute within each rest and taskperiod. Univariate outliers (those exceeding more than3.3standarddeviation unitsfrom thegrandmean)weretransformedbyassigningthedeviantrawscoretoavalueone unit larger or smaller than the next most extremescore(Tabachnick&Fidell,1996)—therewerefourteensuch data points. Our analytic strategy was otherwiseidentical to that of Experiment 1, save that all analysesincludedthreefactors:religioussymbol(positivevs.neg-ative), image type (religious vs. color), and participant religion (Christian vs. non-Christian).

Baseline differences 

. Four separate ANOVAs testing fordifferences in baseline physiological levels by image valence(positivevs.negative),imagecontent(Christiansymbol vs. control image), and religious upbringing(Christian vs. non) revealed no significant main effectsorinteractions(

 F 

s<1).Reactivityscoreswerecalculatedforeachcardiovascular measure bysubtractingtheaver-agevaluefromthelastminuteoftherestperiodfromtheaverage value from the first minute of the speech deliv -ery task. To control for any nonsignificant differences,baseline values were again used as covariates (removingthis covariate did not alter the patterns of significancereported below).

Challenge and threat markers 

. SAM axis activation wasconfirmedduringthespeechbytestingHRandVCreac-tivity scores against zero. Across all eight experimentalconditions, HR increased significantly from baseline(see Table 1). Neither the main effects nor the interac-tion were significant when examining condition effectson HR reactivity. Significant increases in VC wereobserved across all eight experimental conditions (seeTable 1), although there was a significant maineffect of imagecontent,suchthatparticipantsexposedtocontrolimages hadsignificantly higher VC reactivity than thoseexposed to religious symbols. No other significant maineffectsorinteractionsemergedonVCreactivity.Thesig-nificantincreases from baseline in HR and VC con-firmedthatSAMaxisactivationoccurred,whichjustifiedfurther examination of the CV variables for challengeand threat effects.Separate ANCOVA analyses were used to determine whether the predicted patterns of CV responsesoccurred as a result of image valence, image content,religion, or the interactions among these variables. WhenexaminingdifferencesinTPRasafunctionofourIVs, no significant main effects or two-way interactions were observed. However, the predicted three-way inter-action was significant,

(1, 182) = 4.92,

< .05.To understand this interaction, simple effects tests wereconducted(apriorihypothesesjustifiedthesetests— we therefore used the MSE from the overall sample). AmongChristian-raisedparticipants,thetwo-wayinterac-tionbetween image valence and content was significant,

 F 

(1, 182) = 3.76,

= .05. The nature of the interac-tion was such that Christian-raised participants pre-sentedwithnegativeChristiansymbolsexhibited higherTPR (

= 65.2,

SE 

= 31.3, consistent with greaterthreat)thanthoseexposedtothepositiveChristiansym-bols (

= –22.1,

SE 

= 31.3, consistent with challenge)

 F 

(1, 182) = 3.92,

< .05,

= .54, thereby replicating theresults of the first experiment.In contrast, Christian-raisedparticipantsdidnotdifferintheirreactionstothenegative control images (

= –7.4,

SE 

= 31.1) and posi-tive control images (

= 36.1,

SE 

= 36.4),

 F 

(1,182)= .83,

ns 

,

= .26. There were no significant differences among Weisbuch-Remington et al. / RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1211

Figure 4Controlimages:Theimageontheleftisanexampleofthecontrolimage for the religious symbol on the right.TABLE 1:MeansandUnivariateTestsFromZero(Baseline)ofHR and VC Reactivity in Experiment 2

Negative Positive 

ChristianReligious symbolsHR 17.2 19.4 VC 8.4 10.8Control imagesHR 20.9 19.2 VC 11.8 10.5Non-ChristianReligious symbolsHR 18.1 19.4 VC 7.5 9.1Control imagesHR 21.2 22.0 VC 13.9 11.8NOTE: All condition means were tested against zero todetermine sig-nificantincreasesordecreasesfrombaseline,all

 p 

s<.0001.HR=heart rate, VC = ventricular contractility.

 

any of the non-Christian participant groups, accountingfor thethree-way interaction noted above and depictedin Figure 5. AconsistentpatternemergedforCO,whichyieldedamarginal three-way interaction,

(1, 182) = 3.22,

= .07. Again, simple effects tests among Christian participants yielded a significant two-way interaction betweenimage valenceandcontent,

 F 

(1,182)=3.79,

 p 

=.05.Thenatureof the interaction paralleled the results observed withTPR reactivity. Among Christian-raised participants,those exposed to negative Christian symbols exhibitedlower CO (

= –.22,

SE 

= .26, consistent with threat)than those exposed to positive Christian symbols (

=.55,

SE 

=.26,consistentwithchallenge),

 F 

(1,182)=4.65,

 p 

< .05,

= .58. In contrast, Christian-raised participantsdid not differ in their reactions to the negative controlimages (

= .31,

SE 

= .25) compared to the positive con-trolimages(

=.08,

SE 

=.29),

 F 

(1,182)=.36,

ns 

,

=.29.Similar to the TPR data, no significant differencesemerged among non-Christian-raised participants,accountingforthethree-wayinteraction(seeFigure6).

5

 Discussion 

Experiment2replicatedthecriticalresultsofthefirst experimentandruledoutincidentalstimuluscontentasapotentialexplanationfortheobtainedCVdata.Consis-tent with the third hypothesis, Christian participantsresponded differently to the unreportable symbols thannon-Christian participants. More specifically, Christianparticipants exhibited a cardiovascularpattern moreconsistent with threat following the negative Christiansymbols than following the positive Christian symbols.This pattern did not emerge among non-Christian par-ticipants.Thisfindingsuggestsimportantboundarycon-ditions for the effects of unreportable stimuli and that the influence wasdue to the Christian content. In addi-tion,thedifferencesobservedwiththeChristiansymbols were notobservedwithcontrol images.Thisfindingfur-thers our argument that the influence of the symbolsderived from

culturally learned meaning of the symbolsrather than some incidental quality (e.g., color).

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Consistent with our hypotheses, the results of theseexperiments suggest that religious symbols can have asubstantial and nonconscious influence on coping pro-cesses. During a motivated performance situation, reli-gious stimuli that people did not consciously perceiveinfluencedmotivationalandrelatedphysiologicalstates.However, important boundary conditions apparently exist.TheresultsofExperiment1provideevidenceindi-catingthatunreportable religious symbolswere influen-tial onlywhen the subsequent task was relevant to exis-tential issues. Thus, whereas a fleeting image of Jesusmight positively influence an individual’s experience ashe or she delivers a eulogy, the same image probably  would not influence his or her experience playing a word game. The results of Experiment 2 provide evi-dence thatunreportable religious symbols were influen-tial only when they were personally relevant. Thus, whereasafleetingimageofJesus’imagemightpositively influence a Christian during a relevant task, the sameimageprobablywouldnotinfluenceanatheist.Hence,it appears that unreportable religious symbols must beboth task- and self-relevant to exert an effect during amotivated performancetask.

Possible Mechanisms 

Numerous experiments and correlational studieshave shown that religious people often use religion as acoping resource duringpotentially threatening situa-tions (Pargament, 1997), whereasnonreligious peoplerely on different (although not necessarilybetter or worse) resources. In the studies reported here, becauseChristian-raised participants would have been more1212 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

-100-75-50-250255075100Religious Colors

Positive Negative

   T  o   t  a   l   P  e  r   i  p   h  e  r  a   l   R  e  s   i  s   t  a  n  c  e

CHRISTIAN NON-CHRISTIAN

-100-75-50-250255075100

Religious Colors

Positive Negative

Figure 5TPRasafunctionofparticipantreligion,imagevalence,and image content in Experiment 2.

NOTE:Numbershigheronthey-axisindicategreaterthreat.TPR=to-tal peripheral resistance.

-1-0.500.51Religious Colors

Positive Negative

   C  a  r   d   i  a  c   O  u   t  p  u   t   (   l   /  m   i  n   )

CHRISTIAN

-1-0.500.51Religious Colors

NON-

CHRISTIAN

Positive Negative

Figure 6COasafunctionofparticipantreligion,imagevalence,andimage content in Experiment 2.

NOTE: Numbers lower on the y-axis indicate greater threat. CO = car-diac output.

 

likely to use Christianity as a coping resource than non-Christians, any changes in the utility of Christianity as

aresource should have especially affected how Christianscopedwiththespeechtask.Positivesymbolsshouldhavemadethe positive aspects of Christians’ religion (e.g.,“Jesus loves me”) more accessible for them, thereby increasing the utility of religion as a coping resource(leading to challenge). Conversely, negative symbolsshould have made the negative aspects of their religion(e.g., “If I don’t repent, I’ll go to hell”) more accessible,thereby decreasing the utility of religion as a copingresource (leading to threat). Indeed, the results of thesecond experiment suggest that the negative religioussymbols may have been more influential than the posi-tive religious symbols (see Note 5). According to previ-ousresearch(Ochsmann,1984;Osarchuk&Tatz,1973),these effects on coping resources should have beenmore likely to occur during tasks relevant to existentialissues (e.g., speaking about death).It is also possible that religious cues could affect dan-gerevaluations.BlascovichandMendes(2000)havesug-gested that danger (or lack thereof) is an important component of demand evaluations—therefore, otherevaluations being equal, any increase in danger evalua-tions should lead to greater threat.Furthermore, sub-stantialevidenceindicatesthatdangercuescanbeinflu-ential even when outside of conscious awareness (e.g.,LeDoux, 1996; Murphy& Zajonc, 1993; Ohman &Mineka, 2001).In terms of the present investigation, satanic symbolscould have triggered greater danger to Christians thannon-Christians. However, this mechanism alone cannot explain why the symbols did not significantly influenceChristian participants during the dental speech. If thesymbols elicited increased danger, then threat shouldhave resulted regardless of task differences introducedafter the presentation phase. Furthermore, tests on thereligious stimuliratingsrevealed thatnon-Christianpar-ticipants rated the negative symbols as more disturbingthan the positive symbols, suggesting that similar toChristians,non-Christiansmayhavemade dangerevalu-ations thatdifferentiated the negative from the positivesymbols. However,

non-Christian participants were not influenced by unreportablepresentation of these sym-bols. It seems reasonable to speculate that a dangermechanismisnotsolelyresponsibleforthecurrentfind-ings but rather that such a mechanism either acted inconcert with the religious coping mechanism or did not exert an influence in these experiments.Because cardiovascular measurement in this investi-gationwasnotaccompanied bybehavioral orself-report measures, it is also possible that the Christian symbolsautomatically triggered the observed cardiovascularresponseswithoutcorresponding changesintheexperi-ence of challenge and threat. However, numerous ex-perimental and nonexperimental studies have shownthatwithinmotivatedperformancesituations,thediffer-ent patterns of cardiovascular response measured hereoccur when, and only when, different patterns of challenge/threat responsealso occur (Tomaka et al.,1993,1997;forareview,seeBlascovich&Tomaka,1996). Although these patterns have been refined over several years (Blascovich, Mendes, Tomaka, et al., 2003), thebasic elements relevant to the current investigationremain,whichsuggeststhattheobservedcardiovascularresponses could not have occurred separate from chal-lenge andthreat responses. The idea that affectively  valenced symbols automatically give rise to patterns of physiological response unaccompanied by evaluationprocesses is nonetheless intriguingand worthy of futureinvestigation.

Relation to Prior Work on Religious Symbols 

Theresultsofthepresentinvestigationareconsistent  with certainaspects of traditional theories regardingreligious symbols, although these theories were not explicitly tested here. For example, there is some sup-port for the notion that religious symbols can exert anonconscious influence on coping processes (Jung,1964). However, the fact that only Christian-raised par-ticipants were influenced by the symbols in our studiessuggests that the meaning of the symbols was

learnedthroughouttheirlifetime andwastherefore transmittedculturally (not genetically, as Jung suggested). The find-ingsherealsoareconsistentwithOrtner’s(1973)theory thatcomplexsymbols(e.g.,apaintingofJesusascendingto heaven) serve a summary function.These findings also are consistent with existentialaccountsofreligioussymbols,whicharguethatreligious worldviews function to create structured, meaningfulhuman lives in an unstructured, meaningless world(e.g., Becker, 1973; Greenberg et al., 1997; Nietzsche,1887/1974;Sartre,1956).Thefactthatreligioussymbolsonly exerted an influence on motivational states duringa speech about death suggests that these symbols may function at least in part to help individuals cope withexistential issues. TMT (Greenberg et al., 1997), in par-ticular, holds that people cling to their cultural worldview (i.e.,thebeliefsheldbytheiringroup,includ-ing religious beliefs) to fend off potential terror associ-ated with knowledge of their inevitable demise. In most TMT studies, participants have been primed with deathand then their beliefsor attitudes toward outgroupmembers have been subsequently measured. The cur-rent experiments reverse that paradigm, however.Rather than prime participants with death and thenmeasure their worldview-related beliefs, we primedChristian participants with the positive or negative Weisbuch-Remington et al. / RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1213

 

aspectsoftheirworldviewandthenmeasuredtheirreac-tions as they talked about death. Consistent with TMT,obtained results indicated that the primes only exertedinfluence when participants were Christian, when thesymbols were Christian (i.e., relevantto their worldview),and when the speech was about death.

Implications for Challenge and Threat Theory 

 Although the biopsychosocial model of challengeand threat (Blascovich & Mendes, 2000; Blascovich &Tomaka, 1996) maintains that evaluations of demandsand resources can occur on both conscious andnonconscious levels, the present studies provide themostdirectevidence todateinsupportofnonconsciousevaluations. Participants could not report or recall thesymbols to which they were exposed, but cardiovascularpatterns consistent with challenge/threat responses werenonethelessaffected.Thissuggeststhatevaluationsof demands and resources must have been affected by nonconscious influences, perhaps in the form of out-right nonconscious evaluations or at least nonconsciouseffects on conscious evaluations. Although the biopsychosocial model of challengeand threatspecifically applies to cardiovascular patternsobserved during motivated performance situations, it remainspossiblethatreligioussymbolsinfluencecopingprocessesoutside of motivated performance situations.However, the data reported here cannot speak to that possibility.

Conclusions 

The findings here demonstrate that complex, cultur-ally constructed religious symbols presented outside of conscious awareness can influence psychological andphysiological states during motivated performance situ-ations. Such situations encompass many of the impor-tant events faced in everyday life(e.g., test taking, jobinterviews, and interpersonal interactions), suggestingthat the effects of symbols may have important practicalsignificance. Future research should be directed towardunderstanding the processes involved in the observedeffects and their generalization to other stimuli.

NOTES1. Challenge may result in only sympathetic-adrenal-medullary (SAM)axisactivitybecauseoftherelativelyshorthalf-life(severalmin-utes) of the neurochemical output of the SAM axis, catecholamines.The resulting fast spikeof energypresumablyfunctions to providethebody with a means for coping with events that should not requireextensive activity;

for example, events for which one is confident of one’s success. In contrast, the product of threat and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) activation is the release of cortisol, which has a half-life in the body of approximately 90 min. The HPA response would therefore appear to prepare thebody for prolongedactivity,suchasthat requiredbyhighlydemandingtasksforwhichonedoes not currently have adequate resources.2. Data from eight participants were excluded due to difficulty inconsistently identifying the “b” inflection (the opening of the aortic valve) on the dz/dt waveform. This attrition left a total of 107 partici-pantswithusablephysiologicaldata:26intheNegativeSymbols–DeathSpeech condition, 28 in the Negative Symbols–Dental Speech condi-tion,24inthePositiveSymbols–DeathSpeechcondition,and29inthePositive Symbols–Dental Speech condition.3. In the past, VC also has beenusedto differentiatechallengeandthreat (see Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, & Salomon,1999; Blascovich,Mendes, Hunter, Lickel, & Kowai-Bell,2001; Mendes, Blascovich,Lickel,&Hunter,2002;Mendes,Blascovich,Major,&Seery,2001),but it does not do so consistently (e.g., Blascovich et al., 2001, Experiment 3), although it should increasefrom baselinein both. Accordingly, forthe following experiments, cardiac output (CO) and total peripheralresistance (TPR) were used to test for differences in challenge versusthreat. However, we tested for increases in ventricular contractility (VC) (as well as heart rate [HR]) to confirm sympathetic activation.4. Data from 10 participants were excluded due to an inability toconsistently locate “b” on the dz/dt waveform. In addition, 3 partici-pants were excluded because they had irregular heartbeats and 2 par-ticipants wereexcluded because they did not speak English well (andthereforehad difficulty understanding and performing the speechtask). This attrition left a total of 191 participants with usable physio-logical data: 27 Christians and 23 non-Christians exposed to negativeChristiansymbols;27Christiansand19non-Christiansexposedtopos-

itiveChristiansymbols;28Christiansand22non-Christiansexposedtonegative control images; and 20 Christians and 25 non-Christiansexposed to positive control images.5.Thethree-wayinteractionalsomaybebrokendownbetweenpos-itive and negative images. Among participants exposed to negativesymbols or colors, the two-way interaction between symbol type andreligion was significant for TPR,

(1, 182) = 4.18,

< .05, but not forCO,

(1, 182) = 1.58,

ns 

. The nature of the interaction was suchthat Christian-raised participants presented with negative Chris-tiansymbolsexhibitedhigherTPR(

=65.2,

SE 

=31.34)andlowerCO(

= –.22,

SE 

= .26) than non-Christian participants presented withnegative Christian symbols (TPR:

= –26.84,

SE 

= 33.75; CO:

=.35,

SE 

= .27),

(1, 182) = 4.11,

< .05,

= .56, and

(1, 182) = 2.31,

=.12,

=.43, respectively, forTPRandCO.Christian-raisedparticipantspresented with negative Christian symbols also exhibited marginally higherTPRandlowerCOthanChristian-raisedparticipantspresented with the negative color controls (for this latter group, TPR:

= –22.1,

SE 

= 30.81; CO:

= .35,

SE 

= .25),

(1, 182) = 3.06,

= .08,

= .54, and

 F 

(1, 182) = 2.19,

= .13, and

= .43, respectively, for TPR and CO.There were no significant or near-significant differences between any ofthegroupsexposedtopositiveChristiansymbolsorcolorcontrols.REFERENCES Abrams, R. L., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000). Parts outweigh the whole(word) in unconscious analysis of meaning.

Psychological Science 

,

11

, 118-124.Baldwin,M.W.,Carrell,S.E.,&Lopez,D.F.(1990).Primingrelation-ship schemas: My advisor and the Pope are watching me from theback of my mind.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 

,

26 

, 435-454.Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horseman of automaticity: Awareness,intention,efficiency,andcontrolinsocialcognition.InR.S.Wyer& T. K. Srull (Eds.),

Handbook of social cognition 

(Vol. 1, pp. 1-40).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A practicalguidetoprimingandautomaticityresearch.InH.T.Reis&C.M.Judd(Eds.),

Handbookofresearchmethodsinsocialandperson- ality psychology 

(pp. 253-285). New York: Cambridge University Press.Barthes, R. (1957).

Mythologies 

. Canada: HarperCollins.Becker, E. (1973).

The denial of death 

. New York: Free Press.Blascovich, J., &Mendes, W. B. (2000). Challengeandthreat apprais-als:Theroleofaffectivecues.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),

 Feelingandthink- ing: The role of affect in social cognition 

(pp. 59-82). New York: Cam-bridge University Press.

1214 PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN

 

Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., Lickel, B., & Kowai-Bell,N. (2001). Perceiver threat in social interactions with stigmatizedothers.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

80 

, 253-267.Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., & Salomon, K. (1999).Social “facilitation” as challenge and threat.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

77 

, 68-77.Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., & Seery, M. D. (2002). Intergroupencounters and threat: A multi-method approach. In D. M.Mackie & E. R. Smith (Eds.),

From prejudice to intergroup emotions:  Differentiated reactions to social groups 

(pp. 89-109). New York: Psy-chology Press.Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Tomaka, J., Salomon, K., & Seery, M.(2003). Therobust nature of the biopsychosocial model chal-lenge and threat: A reply to Wright and Kirby.

Personality & Social Psychology Review 

,

(3), 234-243.Blascovich, J., & Tomaka, J. (1996). The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),

Advances in experimental socialpsychology 

(Vol.28,pp.1-51).SanDiego,CA:AcademicPress.Cacioppo, J. T., & Tassinary, L. G. (1990).

Principles of psychophysiology: Physical, social, andinferential elements 

.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversity Press.Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1996). Automatic activation of impression formation and memorization goals: Nonconsciousgoal priming reproduces effects of explicit task instructions.

Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

71

, 464-478.Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1997). Nonconscious behavioral confirma-tion processes: The self-fulfilling consequences of automatic ste-reotype activation.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 

,

33 

, 541-560.Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1964).

The approval motive: Studies in evaluative dependence 

. New York: John Wiley.Devine,P.G.(1989).Stereotypesandprejudice:Theirautomaticandcontrolled components.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

56 

, 5-18.Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: Impli-cationsformentalandphysicalhealth.

Psychological Review 

,

96 

,84-100.Dijksterhuis,A.,Aarts,H.,Bargh,J.A.,&vanKnippenberg,A.(2000).On the relation between associative strength and automaticbehavior.

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 

,

36 

, 531-544.Durkheim, E. (1995).

The elementary forms of religious life 

. New York:Simon & Schuster. (Original work published 1912)Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1956).

Nuer religion 

. Oxford, UK: Oxford Uni- versity Press.Freud,S.(2000).

Totemandtaboo:Resemblancesbetweenthepsychiclivesof savages andneurotics 

. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books. (Original work published 1938)Geertz,C.(1973).

Theinterpretationofcultures 

.NewYork:BasicBooks.Greenberg, J., Porteus, J., Simon, L., & Pyszczinski, T. (1995). Evi-dence of a terror management function of cultural icons: Theeffectsofmortalitysalienceontheinappropriateuseofcherishedcultural symbols.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 

,

21

, 1221-1228.Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder,M.,Kirkland,S.L.,etal.(1990).Evidenceforterrormanagement theory II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview.

Journal of Personal- ity and Social Psychology 

,

58 

, 308-318.Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (1997). Terror manage-ment theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empiricalassessments and conceptual refinements. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),

Advances in experimental social psychology 

(Vol. 29, pp. 61-139). SanDiego, CA: Academic Press.Hawkley, L. C., Burleson, M. H., Berntson, G. G., & Cacioppo, J. T.(2003). Loneliness in everyday life: Cardiovascular activity,psychosocial context, and health behaviors.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

85 

, 105-120.Hessing,D.J.,Elffers,H.,&Weigel,R.H.(1988).Exploringthelimitsof self-reports and reasoned action: An investigation of the psy-chologyoftaxevasionbehavior.

 JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsy- chology 

,

54 

, 405-413. James, W. (1997).

Varieties of religious experience 

. New York: Simon &Schuster. (Original work published 1936) Jung, C. G. (Ed.). (1964). Approaching the unconscious. In

Man and his symbols 

(pp. 18-103). London: Aldus Books Limited.Kelsey, R., & Guethlein, W. (1990). An evaluation of the ensembleaveraged impedance cardiograph.

Psychophysiology 

,

27 

, 24-33.Kierkegaard,S.(1981).

Theconceptofanxiety 

.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversity Press. (Original work published 1844)Kruglanski,A.W.(1989).

Layepistemicsandhumanknowledge:Cognitive and motivational bases 

. New York: Plenum.Langer, S. (1942).

Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism of rea- son,rite, and art 

. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotionand cognition.

American Psychologist 

,

37 

, 1019-1024.Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On theprimacy of cognition.

American Psycholo- gist 

,

39 

, 124-129.Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984).

Stress, appraisal, and coping 

. New York: Springer.LeDoux, J. E. (1996).

The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotionallife 

. New York: Simon & Schuster.Lerner, M. J. (1980).

The belief in a just world: A fundamental delusion 

.New York: Plenum.Levi-Strauss, C. (1964).

Totemism 

. London: Merlin Press.Llarbe, M. M., Spitzer, S. B., Saab, P.G., Ironson, G. H., &Schneiderman, N. (1991). The reliability and specificity of delta versus residualized change as measures of cardiovascular reactiv-ity to behavioral changes.

Psychophysiology 

,

28 

, 701-711.McGregor, I. (2003). Defensivezeal: Compensatory conviction about attitudes, values, goals, groups, and self-definition in the face of personaluncertainty. In S. J. Spencer, S. Fein, M. P. Zanna, &J. M.Olsen (Eds.),

Motivated social perception: The Ontario symposium 

(Vol. 9, pp. 73-92). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Lickel, B., &Hunter, S. B. (2002). Chal-lenge and threat during social interaction with white andblackmen.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 

,

29 

, 939-952.Mendes, W. B., Blascovich, J., Major, B., & Seery, M. D. (2001). Chal-lenge and threat responses during downward and upward socialcomparisons.

European Journal of Social Psychology 

,

31

, 477-497.Mendes, W. B., Reis, H., Seery, M. D., & Blascovich,J. (2003). Cardio- vascular correlates of emotional expressionand suppression: Docontentandgendercontextmatter?

 JournalofPersonalityandSocial Psychology 

,

84 

, 771-792.Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and aware-ness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulusexposures.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

64 

, 723-739.Newby-Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2003). Conceiving the past and thefuture.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 

,

29 

, 807-818.Nietzsche, F. W. (1887/1974).

The gay science 

. Toronto, Canada: Ran-dom House.Ochsmann, R. (1984). Belief in afterlife as a moderator of fear of death?

European Journal of Social Psychology 

,

14 

, 53-67.Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness:Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning.

Psychological Review 

,

108 

, 483-522.Ortner, S. B. (1973). On key symbols.

American Anthropologist 

,

75 

,1338-1446.Osarchuk, M., & Tatz, S. J. (1973). Effect of induced fear of death onbeliefinafterlife.

 JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology 

,

27 

,256-260.Pargament, K. I. (1997).

The psychology of religion and coping: Theory,research, practice 

. New York: Guilford.Ross, M. (1989). Relation of implicit theories to the construction of personal histories.

Psychological Bulletin 

,

96 

, 341-357.Roth, D. L., Snyder, C. R., & Pace, L. M. (1986). Dimensions of favor-able self-presentation.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

51

, 867-874.Sartre,J.P.(1956).

Beingandnothingness 

.NewYork:Simon&Schuster.Seery, M. D., Blascovich, J., Weisbuch, M., & Vick, S. B. (2004). Theeffects of self-esteem level and stability on cardiovascular reac-tionstoperformancefeedback.

Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology 

,

87 

, 133-145.

 Weisbuch-Remington et al. / RELIGIOUS SYMBOLS 1215

 

Sherwood, A., Allen, M. T., Fahrenberg, J., Kelsey, R. M., Lovallo, W.R.,&vanDooren,L.J.P.(1990).Methodologicalguidelinesforimpedance cardiography.

Psychophysiology 

,

27 

, 1-23.Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996).

Using multivariate statistics 

(3rd ed.). New York: HarperCollins.Tomaka, J., & Blascovich, J. (1994). Effects of justice beliefs on cogni-tive appraisalsof and subjective physiological, and behavioralresponsestopotentialstress.

 JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychol- ogy 

,

67 

, 732-740.Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., & Kelsey, R. M. (1993). Subjective, physio-logical, and behavioral effects of challenge and threat appraisal.

 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

65 

, 248-260.Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kibler, J., & Ernst, J. (1997). Cognitive andphysiological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal.

Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

73 

, 63-72. Vallone,R.P.,Griffin,D.W.,Lin,S.,&Ross,L.(1990).Overconfident predictionoffutureactionsandoutcomesbyselfandothers.

 Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology 

,

58 

, 582-592. Weisbuch, M., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2003). Priorsource exposure and persuasion: Further evidence formisattributionalprocesses.

PersonalityandSocialPsychologyBulletin 

,

29 

, 691-700.Received August 27, 2004Revision accepted December 15, 2004