PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in Predicting Cognitive...

22
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE This article was downloaded by: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] On: 27 February 2009 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 909101281] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Journal of Neurotherapy Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t792306937 Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in Predicting Cognitive Effectiveness and the Inefficiency Hypothesis Kirtley E. Thornton a ; Dennis P. Carmody b a The Brain Foundation, Edison, NJ b University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ Online Publication Date: 01 January 2009 To cite this Article Thornton, Kirtley E. and Carmody, Dennis P.(2009)'Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in Predicting Cognitive Effectiveness and the Inefficiency Hypothesis',Journal of Neurotherapy,13:1,1 — 21 To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/10874200802429850 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10874200802429850 Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Transcript of PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in Predicting Cognitive...

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

This article was downloaded by: [Thornton, Kirtley E.]On: 27 February 2009Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 909101281]Publisher RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House,37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Journal of NeurotherapyPublication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t792306937

Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in Predicting CognitiveEffectiveness and the Inefficiency HypothesisKirtley E. Thornton a; Dennis P. Carmody b

a The Brain Foundation, Edison, NJ b University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ

Online Publication Date: 01 January 2009

To cite this Article Thornton, Kirtley E. and Carmody, Dennis P.(2009)'Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databases in PredictingCognitive Effectiveness and the Inefficiency Hypothesis',Journal of Neurotherapy,13:1,1 — 21

To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/10874200802429850

URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10874200802429850

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial orsystematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply ordistribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug dosesshould be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directlyor indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

SCIENTIFIC ARTICLES

Eyes-Closed and Activation QEEG Databasesin Predicting Cognitive Effectiveness

and the Inefficiency Hypothesis

Kirtley E. Thornton, PhDDennis P. Carmody, PhD

ABSTRACT. Background. Quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) databases have beendeveloped for the eyes closed (EC) condition. The development of a cognitive activation data-base is a logical and necessary development for the field.

Method. Brain activation was examined by QEEG during several tasks including EC rest,visual attention (VA), auditory attention (AA), listening to paragraphs presented auditorilyand reading silently. The QEEG measures obtained in the EC and simple, non-cognitive atten-tion task that were significantly related to subsequent cognitive performance were not the samevariables which accounted for success during the cognitive task.

Results. There were clear differences between relative power, microvolt, coherence and phasevalues across these different tasks.

Conclusions. The conclusions reached are (1) the associations among QEEG variables arecomplex and vary by task; (2) the QEEG variables which predict cognitive performance undertask demands are not the same as the variables which predict to subsequent performance fromthe EC or simple, non-cognitive attention tasks; (3) a cognitive activation database is clinicallyuseful; and (4) an hypothesis of brain functioning is proposed to explain the findings. The coor-dinated allocation of resources (CAR) hypothesis states that cognitive effectiveness is a productof multiple specific activities in the brain, which vary according to the task; and (5) the averageresponse pattern does not involve the variables that are critical to success at the task, thus indi-cating an inefficiency of the normal human brain.

KEYWORDS. Attention, coordinated allocation of resources, eyes-closed and activationdatabases, memory, QEEG

Kirtley E. Thornton is affiliated with The Brain Foundation, Edison, NJ 08817, and Dennis P. Carmody isaffiliated with Institute for the Study of Child Development, Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, Universityof Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey, New Brunswick, NJ 08903.

Address correspondence to: Kirtley E. Thornton, PhD, Director, The Brain Foundation, 2 Ethel Road, Ste.203C, Edison, NJ 08817 USA (E-mail: ket@chp-neurotherapycom).

Journal of Neurotherapy, 13:1–21, 2009Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLCISSN: 1087-4208 print=1530-017X onlineDOI: 10.1080/10874200802429850

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

The relations between quantitativeelectroencephalography (QEEG) and clini-cal and cognitive problems have been inves-tigated for several decades (Evans &Abarbanel, 1999; John & Prichep, 2006).Individuals with cognitive deficits haveshown brain activation patterns that arerelated to the type and severity of theirdeficit (Thornton, 2002). A goal of theinvestigations has been to identify the devia-tions in the underlying electrophysiologicalmeasures from normative databases so thatinterventions directed towards these devia-tions will ameliorate or improve the clinicalcondition. Problematic in this assumptionis the frequent lack of consistent empiricaldocumentation that the specific cognitivedeficits are directly related to the deviationsfrom the database employed. One part ofthis problem resides in the eyes-closed (EC)QEEG data that have been employed. Forexample, in understanding memory perfor-mance within a normal population, theweakness has been the lack of a well definedset of specific QEEG variables which definehow success is achieved during a task. Differ-ent EC databases (Lubar, 2003) have beendeveloped as well as databases that engagein subject in simple attention tasks witheyes-open. Developments in the field haveled to the inclusion of cognitive tasksin database development (Brain ResourceCompany, 2007; Skil 3 http://skiltopo.com/). A subject is compared to the norma-tive databases on the QEEG values for thetask, without reference to the variables thatare critical for the task. These databasesask the question what happens rather thanwhat makes it work.

The EC database provides a set of valuesthat describe the resting state of brain activ-ity for individuals who are engaged in a ‘rest-ing’ state. While the assumption is that theresting state is a default baseline, the subjectsmay be engaging in any of a wide range of‘default’ states. This issue is of concern tothose in the field of neuroimaging (Buckner& Vincent, 2007; Raichle & Snyder, 2007).When subjects have no clear task, the restingbrain shows large variations of activity thatare not ascribed to performance (Gonzalez-Hernandez, 2005). Thus, the resting state is

at best, an estimate of how individuals ‘idle’when not required to attend, process, andremember information.

In contrast, an activation database is onethat is developed while control subjects areengaged in tasks that require attention, pro-cessing, and memory (Thornton, 2001).Under activation conditions, variations inbrain activation are related to the specifictask. In addition, subject performance oncognitive tasks allows an examination ofthe associations between brain activationpatterns and performance. For example,scores on tests of immediate and delayedrecall on a reading task are related to mea-sures of relative power and coherence inspecific locations (Thornton, 2002).

There are differences of opinion on therelative value of the EC and activation data-bases (Thatcher, 1998; Thornton, 1999,2000). An argument in favor of the EC data-base is the simple, relative uniformity of theEEG recording conditions (Thatcher, 1999)and high reliability values between evalua-tions (Niedermeyer, 1987; Oken & Chiappa,1988). The reliability values across all fre-quencies for the EC condition have beenshown to average around .7 (McEvoy,Smith, & Gevins, 2000).

In contrast to the passive EC condition,active tasks are dependent on many variablesincluding the task difficulty, the motivationof the subject, and the physical characteris-tics of the recording environment such asthe intensity of the stimuli and the roomlighting. The reliability values of the activa-tion approach for working memory andattention is .93 across the frequencies(McEvoy et al., 2000).

In addition, QEEG EC databases do nottypically collect data above the 32 Hertzrange. The Thornton activation database(Thornton, 2001) assesses the subject per-forming cognitive challenges that are diffi-cult, in order to avoid a ceiling effect andextends the frequency range to 64 Hertz,which offers considerable advantages incertain clinical situations. For example,Thornton (1999, 2000, 2003) was able todistinguish between normals and subjectswith mild traumatic brain injury (TBI)primarily on the basis of coherence patterns

2 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

in the high frequency range (32–64Hz) in theEC, simple non-cognitive visual (VA) andauditory attention (AA) tasks as well as thetask of listening to paragraphs. The resultsemphasized, as in the Thatcher et al. (1989)study, the importance of the phase andcoherence values in obtaining successfuldiscrimination between the groups.

The issue of using as a reference theQEEG from either a passive eye-closed mea-sure or from an active task measure has aparallel in the field of neuroimaging wherethere is a debate over the default state ofthe brain that is often used as a baselinefor comparison of brain activity duringtasks (Morcom & Fletcher, 2007; Raichle &Snyder, 2007). As Gonzalez-Hernandezet al. (2005) indicated, the pre-task ‘resting’condition is never truly ‘at rest.’ McKiernanet al. (2006) found in functional neuroima-ging task induced deactivation (TID), whichis a local decrease in blood flow during anactive task, relative to a ‘‘resting’’ baseline.TID may occur when resources shift fromongoing, internally generated processingtypical of ‘‘resting’’ states to processingrequired by an exogenous task. The majorcomponents of the intrinsic system have beenidentified by various investigations. Forexample, one group found the intrinsic sys-tem to include medial prefrontal areas, theposterior cingulate and the precuneus, lateralinferior parietal cortex and the anterioraspect of infero-temporal cortex (Gollandet al., 2007; Golland, Golland, Bentin, &Malach, 2008). Another group found thatthe intrinsic system involves four lefthemisphere regions, including posteriorparieto-occipital cortex, anterior cingulategyrus, fusiform gyrus, and middle frontalgyrus (McKiernan, D’Angelo, Kaufman, &Binder, 2006).

In this study we examine two methods ofunderstanding the relations between theQEEG variables and cognition and added athird method. The first two methods areexamining (1) the relation between EC dataand cognitive performance data collected ata different time and (2) the examination ofthe relation between cognitive performanceand the QEEG variables during a task. Thethird method employs the results of the

second method to guide the clinical QEEGprotocols to improve performance in thecognitive problems of the reading disabled,memory impaired and traumatic braininjured (TBI) patients. We propose the coor-dinated allocation of resources (CAR)hypothesis which states that cognitive effec-tiveness is a product of multiple specificQEEG activities in the brain for specifictasks which can involve activities of differentfrequencies at a location as well as coherenceand phase activity between locations.

In this paper we demonstrate how theQEEG measures obtained under EC, restingand simple attention tasks are not the sameas the QEEG predictors of performance dur-ing the memory tasks. In addition, QEEGstudies that measure brain activity withbandwidths from 1 to 64Hz show a differentset of relations between the QEEG variablesand cognitive functioning than the studiesthat restrict the measures of brain activityto 32Hz and less. We want to know theongoing QEEG variables during the taskwhich predict success.

RELATIONS BETWEEN MEASURES

As the research frequently examinesmicrovolts, relative power, coherence andphase relations, it is important to understandthe empirical relations between thesemeasures. Corsi-Cabrera et al. (1989) sum-marized the relations between power andcoherence across a number of studies bynoting that changes in coherence occurindependently from changes in EEG power.

Measures

Over the years research studies have gen-erally defined the frequency ranges accord-ing to standard practice and have employedthe scalp locations defined by the 10–20 sys-tem (Jasper, 1958). The frequency definitionranges have been: delta: 0 to 4 Hertz; theta: 4to 8Hz; alpha: 8 to 13Hz; beta: 13 to 25Hz.The ranges have been dependent upon hard-ware and software definitions as well as thepreferences of individual researchers. Some

Scientific Articles 3

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

studies have examined frequencies above32Hz (Thornton, 2000, 2001, 2002; vonStein et al., 2000).

There are two types of data availableto QEEG analysis. The first involves theactivity at a scalp location and examinesthe different frequencies in terms of mea-sures such as amplitude, relative power, peakfrequency, and peak amplitude. The secondmeasure quantifies the association betweenlocations with concepts of phase and coher-ence. This article will employ the presentedbolded capitalized letters to represent thevariables.

Activation Measures

M: Absolute Magnitude=Microvolts: theaverage absolute magnitude (asdefined in microvolts) of a band overthe entire epoch (one second).

RP: Relative Magnitude=Microvolt orRelative Power: the relative magni-tude of a band defined as the absolutemicrovolt of the particular banddivided by the total microvolt gener-ated at a particular location by allbands.

PA: Peak Amplitude: the peak amplitudeof a band during an epoch in micro-volts.

PKF: Peak Frequency: the peak frequencyof a band during an epoch definedin hertz.

S: Symmetry: the peak amplitude sym-metry between two locations in a par-ticular bandwidth-, i.e., defined as(A�B)= (AþB).

Connectivity Measures

The coherence and phase values obtainedin this research were generated by the algo-rithms employed in the Lexicor software.Different hardware and software companieshave employed different algorithms in calcu-lating these values. Neither the relationsbetween these different algorithms northe relations between the algorithms andcognitive effectiveness under activation con-

ditions have been studied. It is not assumedthat the results reported in this paper forcoherence and phase relationships using theLexicor software would be the same for thealgorithms provided by other equipmentmanufacturers.

C: Coherence: the average similarity betweenthe waveforms of a particular bandin two locations over the one-secondperiod of time, and conceptualized asthe strength or number of connectionsbetween two locations. Although labeledby Lexicor as coherence, from a mathe-matical point of view it would moreappropriate to refer to it as a cross spec-tral correlation.

P: Phase: the time lag between two locationsof a particular band as defined by howsoon after the beginning of an epoch aparticular waveform at one location ismatched in a second location.

The algorithms for coherence and phase,which were provided by Lexicor MedicalTechnologies, were employed in the activationdatabase by Thornton (2001). There havebeen several conceptually and mathematicallydifferent approaches to describing the rela-tionships of the frequencies between locations.Collura (2008) has provided a conceptual andmathematical discussion of these differentapproaches. There are 2944 variables for eachsubject in each task when combining all avail-able Lexicor measures. In order to reduce thelarge number of variables and to be consistentwith the generator concept in the EEG litera-ture Thornton (2002) developed the flashlightcalculation.

The concept of a flashlight assumes that aparticular location emits a signal, in definedfrequencies, which is projected to all corticallocations. The value for a flashlight variableat a specific location, and in a specific band-width, is calculated by summating the coher-ence values with the remaining 18 locations.References will employ a combination of theshorthand letters presented. For example,CA will refer to coherence alpha and RPAwill refer to relative power of alpha.

There are several problems inherent in theresearch in the area of examining the associa-tions between QEEG variables and cognition.

4 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

1. The first problem has been the implicitassumption that certain QEEG variablesrelate uniformly to all cognitive abilities.This assumption has been challenged inprevious research (Thornton, 2000, 2002).

2. The second problem is the assumptionthat the degree of activation or changesof the brain from a relevant baseline arerelated to success at a cognitive task. Thisassumption is involved in neuroimagingstudies including positron emission tomo-graphy (PET) and functional magneticresonance imaging (fMRI) when brainactivation during a cognitive task isrelated to activation at rest.

3. The third problem is the modality of theinformation presented to the participant,whether auditory or visual.

4. The fourth problem is the assumptionimplicitly made by developers of ECdatabases that subject’s relative standing,with respect to their QEEG valuesand a relevant database, will remainroughly the same when comparing valuesobtained under an EC condition and atask condition. In addition, it would beassumed that the deficits observed underthe eyes closed (EC) condition will bepresent during the activation condition.

This study examines these problems andthe associations between cognitive function-ing, assessed by reading and auditory mem-ory, and QEEG measures in order to lay thenecessary empirical groundwork to identifyeffective treatment intervention protocols.

METHODS

Participants

Forty-two right-handed participants (agerange 14 to 77 years, M¼38.4, SD¼15.98;43% female) with no previous history ofADHD, LD, or TBI participated after sign-ing consent forms. The participants underage 18 signed assent forms and the parentssigned consent forms. None of the partici-pants had a history of neurological problems,and four participants were taking medica-tions (anti-hypertensive, anti-depressants). It

is assumed that this small percentage (9.5%)of the sample would have no appreciableeffect on the overall patterns. Participantswere compensated financially and were freeto drop out of the study at any time or torefuse participation in the research.

Tasks

The participants completed several tasks inone session. Participants first engaged in anEC resting task for five minutes. This wasfollowed by an AA task with eyes closed forthree minutes. The participants then openedtheir eyes and performed a VA task for threeminutes. This was followed by listening andrecalling four paragraphs with eyes closedfor five minutes. Following each paragraphthe subject engaged in silent eyes closed recall(one minute) for each of the four paragraphswhile the QEEG is measured. Then they givea verbal report of the paragraphs with noQEEG monitoring. The next task was read-ing a full page of text for 100 seconds andthen silently, with eye closed, recalling ofthe text. Then with eyes-closed, participantsengaged in two delayed recall tasks. The firstwas a quiet eyes-closed recall of the para-graph and the second a similar approach torecall of the reading material while theQEEG is recorded. The participants then givea verbal report without QEEG recording.

EEG Recording

Brain activity was recorded using a 19channel QEEG hardware device (LexicorMedical Technology, Inc.). Bandpass filterswere set between 0.0 and 64Hz (3 dB points).The signals that passed were subjected to aFast Fourier Transform (FT) using Cosine-tapered windows, which provides spectralmagnitude in microvolts as a function offrequency. The sampling rate was set to 256to allow an examination up to 64-Hz. Thebandwidths were grouped according to thefollowing divisions: Delta: .00–4Hz, Theta:4–8Hz, Alpha: 8–13Hz, Beta1: 13–32Hz,Beta2: 32–64Hz. An Electro-Cap was fittedto the participant. The electrodes were posi-tioned at 19 scalp locations according to the

Scientific Articles 5

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

standard 10–20 system (Jasper, 1958) withear linked references. The scalp was preppedwith rubbing alcohol and Nu-Prep and the19 electrodes were filled with Electro-gel.The earlobes and forehead were prepped withrubbing alcohol and Nuprep. Impedanceswere maintained below 10K Ohm (andwithin 1.5K Ohm of each other) at all loca-tions. Gain was set to 32000, and the highpass filter was set to off. The measurementsavailable through the software provided byLexicor Medical provided the numeric valuesof the QEEG variables. The data were arti-facted for eye movements and EMG activityas well as other possible sources of contami-nation (Thornton, 1996).

RESULTS

The results are presented first by describ-ing the associations among the tasks of EC,listening, and reading. Then the results areshown for the changes in brain activity asthe participants progress from the EC taskto the attention tasks then to the cognitivetasks of listening and reading.

Associations Among Measures

To aid in understanding the research pre-sented it is important to understand howcommonly used measures relate to oneanother and to empirically describe theirassociations. Two very commonly employedmeasures are microvolts and relative power.Table 1 presents the correlations betweenthe values of relative power (RP) values andabsolute microvolts (M), which were aver-aged across the 19 scalp locations for the three

tasks of EC, listening, and reading. This studyincluded the beta2 (32–64Hz) in addition tothe commonly used beta frequency range herenamed beta1 (13–32Hz). Although manyof the relations are significant, it is clear thatthe measures cannot be considered the same.The lowest associations between RP and Mmeasures are in the delta frequency and thehighest are in the alpha and beta2 frequencies.The relations between the alpha valuesdecreases during the reading task.

Table 2 addresses the relations betweenthe RP, M, C and P variables by presentingthe correlation matrix for the EC, listening(eyes-closed) and reading (eyes open) tasksfor these variables. The only significant asso-ciations involved delta and alpha. There arepositive relations between RPA, MA andCA and negative relations with PA duringthe two eyes-closed tasks. These relationscease when participants open their eyes tobegin reading. It is unclear why there arethese inverse relations between CA, PA andthe RPA and MA variables.

Table 3 presents the intercorrelationsbetween the phase and coherence values. Asthe table indicates there are strong associa-tions between the coherence and phase valuesof the frequency measures, except for thealpha frequency under both eyes-closed tasks.

Table 4 presents the relations between ageand the RP, M, P and C values across thethree cognitive tasks. As the table indicatesage has effects on all RP values (strongestfor beta1) except alpha depending upon thetask; age has no effect on microvolt mea-sures, except for MT under reading tasks.Coherence theta (CT) was the only coher-ence variable that was directly associatedwith age under the listening task. The phasevalues that were directly related to age were

TABLE 1. Interrelations between microvolts and relative power.

Eyes Closed Listening Reading Average

MD=RPD 0.10 0.32 0.21 0.21MT=RPT 0.50 0.57 0.42 0.50MA=RPA 0.86 0.87 0.53 0.75MB1=RPB1 0.53 0.38 0.43 0.45MB2=RPB2 0.72 0.69 0.71 0.71

Note. Bold numbers are significant at .05 level, R: Relative Power, M: Microvolt, D: delta, T: theta, A: alpha, B1: beta1,

B2: beta2.

6 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

the PA value under EC and listening tasks aswell as PT under listening.

In summary, the associations between RPand M measures are strongest for alpha andbeta2 and weakest for delta across the threetasks reported in the Table 1 (EC, listeningand reading). The relations between RP,M, C and P values reflect non-significantrelations in the theta, beta1 and beta2 band-widths. Coherence and phase delta showpositive relations to relative power of deltameasures. Relative power and microvoltmeasures show positive relations to coher-ence alpha and negative relations to phasealpha. The alpha pattern doesn’t exist inthe reading task (Table 2).

Associations between coherence andphase values are high within all frequencies,except for the alpha frequency during the

EC and listening tasks (Table 3). The beta2frequency has one of the highest associationsbetween the M and RP values as well asbetween the C and P values. Some of thesephenomena have no clear explanation at thispoint in the development of this field.

Activation Patterns and PredictingCognitive Success

From a clinical point of view it is helpfulto understand what specifically occurs inthe QEEG variables as the participantsmove from an EC task to a simple non-cognitive activation task, and to identifythe QEEG variables that are related successor failure at a cognitive task. The followinganalysis will examine these changes as the

TABLE 2. Relations between relative power, microvolts, coherence, and phase.

Eyes Closed Listen(EyesClosed) Read (Eyes Open)

CD PD CD PD CD PD

RPD 0.45 0.36 0.57 0.51 0.41 0.29MD �0.02 �0.02 0.12 0.14 0.38 0.16

CT PT CT PT CT PTRPT 0.24 0.07 0.2 �0.05 0.07 �0.09MT �0.03 �0.1 �0.05 �0.16 �0.2 �0.1

CA PA CA PA CA PARPA 0.64 �0.48 0.76 �0.39 0.03 �0.08MA 0.45 �0.45 0.61 �0.42 �0.04 �0.05

CB1 PB1 CB1 PB1 CB1 PB1RPB1 0.01 0.05 �0.02 �0.04 0.27 0.26MB1 �0.1 �0.22 �0.1 0.02 �0.11 0

CB2 PB2 CB2 PB2 CB2 PB2RPB2 �0.13 �0.01 �0.01 0.08 0.11 0.19MB2 �0.25 �0.2 �0.13 �0.12 0.04 0.12

Note. Bold numbers are significant at .05 level, R: Relative Power, M: Microvolt, D: delta, T: theta, A: alpha, B1: beta1,

B2: beta2, C: Coherence, P: Phase.

TABLE 3. Interrelations between coherence and phase values in three tasks.

Tasks

Eyes Closed Listen (Eyes Closed) Read (Eyes Open)

CD PD 0.96 CD PD 0.91 CD PD 0.80CT PT 0.92 CT PT 0.72 CT PT 0.71CA PA 0.03 CA PA 0.04 CA PA 0.77CB1 PB1 0.83 CB1 PB1 0.53 CB1 PB1 0.87CB2 PB2 0.94 CB2 PB2 0.96 CB2 PB2 0.86

Note. D: delta, T: theta, A: alpha, B1: beta1, B2: beta2, C: Coherence, P: Phase.

Scientific Articles 7

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

participants (1) move across tasks fromEC to AA to listening to paragraphs and(2) move across tasks from EC to VA andthen to reading. The analysis of the datawill also (1) examine the problem of predict-ing from the EC and simple AA andVA tasks to cognitive success and (2) providea description of the state changes inbrain functioning for a group of normalindividuals.

CHANGES IN QEEG VARIABLESWITH CHANGES IN TASK

We report the changes in QEEG variablesas the group of participants progresses fromone task to the next. Selection of the vari-ables of interest was based on a criterion ofa standard deviation (SD) change of .50 orgreater, using the SD of the relevant baselinetask. Almost all of the changes were in therange of 0.50 to 1.00 SD for the auditorytask changes and up to 2.00 SD for the visualtask changes. Specifically, the QEEGobtained during AA is the relevant baselinefor auditory encoding and auditory memory.Similarly, the QEEG obtained during VA is

the relevant baseline for visual encodingand reading recall. In the first analysis, weexamine the changes in QEEG variableswhen participants move from the EC taskto the tasks of AA and VA and subsequentlyto the listening and reading tasks.

Effect Size Analysis

We will use effect size analysis to evaluatewhether the task changes QEEG measures(Cohen, 1988). In order to obtain an effectsize statistic (ES), it is necessary to havethe means and standard deviations onQEEG measures from both the EC assess-ment and the task assessment. The ES forthe task is calculated using the formula: thetask mean score minus the EC mean score,divided by the standard deviation of theEC distribution. This provides a changescore in QEEG from EC to task in standarddeviation units, thus allowing an evaluationof changes in QEEG due to the task. In addi-tion, the ES is bias-adjusted for the size ofthe sample (Hedges & Olkin, 1985). In addi-tion to the ES, we obtained confidence inter-vals that allow us to determine if the change

TABLE 4. Relations among age, relative power, microvolts, coherence and phase in three tasks.

RPD RPT RPA RPB1 RPB2

Age(EyesClosed)

�0.34 �0.29 �0.17 0.47 0.34

Age (Listening) �0.29 �0.17 �0.12 0.41 0.25Age (Reading) �0.57 �0.60 �0.08 0.47 0.48

MD MT MA MB1 MB2Age (EyesClosed)

0.00 �0.22 �0.17 0.14 0.23

Age (Listening) 0.02 �0.27 0.21 0.05 0.06Age (Reading) 0.04 �0.38 �0.25 0.02 0.21

CD CT CA CB1 CB2Age (EyesClosed)

0.13 0.21 �0.01 0.05 0.19

Age (Listening) 0.14 0.51 0.03 0.23 0.11Age (Reading) 0.07 0.14 0.22 0.17 �0.08

PD PT PA PB1 PB2Age (EyesClosed)

0.11 0.22 0.32 0.16 0.20

Age (Listening) 0.11 0.45 0.34 0.10 0.15Age (Reading) �0.05 0.20 0.26 0.11 �0.05

Note. RP: Relative Power, M: Microvolt, D: delta, T: theta, A: alpha, B1: beta1, B2: beta2, C: Coherence, P: Phase, Bold

numbers are significant at .05 level.

8 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

from EC assessment to the task assessment issignificant. Using a cutoff of 95% confidenceintervals for a sample size of 42 subjects, wecalculated the minimum ES required to besure that the QEEG measures obtainedunder task conditions differed from thosecollected under eyes-close conditions, with95% confidence. An ES of 0.5 meets theseconditions. For more information, as wellas more details on how to calculate effect sizeas applied to QEEG, see Thornton andCarmody (2008).

Changes from EC to AA

As the participants move from an EC stateto an AA state there are increases in left tem-poral lobe activity (T3) in beta variables(RPB2, PKFB1, SYMB2) and F3PA.

Changes from AA to Listening

Figure 1 shows the changes in QEEG vari-ables from the AA task to the listening task.The changes include increases in frontallocations of delta (RPD, PKAD, MD) andtheta (MT, PKAT) and occipital (O2) beta2activity (MB2, PKAB2). The variables whichdecreased included frontal RPB1, PKFT and

F3PA. The increases in delta probably repre-sent artifacting issues due to eye movements.

PREDICTING LISTENINGPERFORMANCE FROM

PREVIOUS TASKS

Predicting from EC to Listening

Figure 2 shows the predictors of auditorymemory under task, which indicate a predo-minant pattern of left hemisphere coherencealpha flashlights (F7, T3, C3, P3) and rightfrontal (F8) as well as PKFB1 at T5 andCz (Thornton, 2000). These results are arecalculation of the Thornton (2000) pub-lished results employing the flashlightmetaphor. This is the example of the exami-nation of the relations between cognitiveperformance and the QEEG variables duringa task.

Data obtained during an activationQEEG evaluation were used to develop pro-tocols for clinical patients on a case by casebasis for EEG biofeedback that was designedto improve memory. The remediation effortsimproved auditory memory (2.44 standarddeviations or 296%) with a group of 20children who had learning-disabilities and

FIGURE 1. The changes in quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) variables from the auditory attentiontask to the listening task.

Scientific Articles 9

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

ADHD (Thornton, 2006a; Thornton &Carmody, 2005). In a separate study, 19 par-ticipants with TBI improved auditory mem-ory by 2.62 standard deviations (Thornton& Carmody, 2008). This is an example ofthe third method, the effects of interventionon cognition (Thornton & Carmody, 2009).

Figure 3 illustrates the predictors of audi-tory memory from the EC task. This is anexample of the first method, predicting from

EC to a cognitive measure collected at a dif-ferent point in time. The positive predictorsinvolve frontal and central RPT and poster-ior symmetry beta measures while the nega-tive predictors are diffusely evident in thebeta2 frequency (RPB2, MB2), frontal betaactivity and posterior and central connectionprojections. As evident in this comparisonnone of the subsequent task predictors ofmemory performance were evident in the

FIGURE 3. The predictors of auditory memory from the eyes-closed task.

FIGURE 2. The predictors of auditory memory under task.

10 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

EC task. It would be expected that a partici-pant’s relative value, compared to the otherparticipant’s values, would be maintainedas the tasks change. In this auditory memorytask, the coherence alpha (CA) values of thesubsequent better performers should behigher in the EC task, and thus be a predic-tor of recall under task. This association wasnot demonstrated in the data because the ECalpha coherence values did not correlate withsubsequent recall performance.

Predicting from AA to Listening

Figure 4 shows the predictors of para-graph recall score from the AA task. Thepositive relations between AA variables andsubsequent paragraph recall ability werevery similar to the patterns in the EC data:diffuse RPT values, occipital symmetry betameasures while the negative indicatorsinvolved the beta2 frequency in diffuse loca-tions in addition to frontal beta measures. Inaddition, the CB2 activity from the rightposterior and central locations proved to be

an additional negative predictor of recallability. In summary, brain activity duringthe EC or the AA tasks was unrelated tothe subsequent predictors of auditory recallability.

ACTIVATION PATTERNS ANDSUBSEQUENT AUDITORY RECALL

Another way to examine the data is ananalysis of the activation patterns in relationto subsequent task performance. A questionthat arises is whether the participants areincreasing the value of the variables that arecritical to task success? Examining thechanges in QEEG from both the EC to AAand from the AA to listening tasks revealsno significant activation of the coherencealpha flashlights. While the change from ECto AA is not expected to induce an increasein coherence alpha values, it certainly wouldbe expected as the participants move fromthe AA to listening task. An additionalanalysis was undertaken to determine if therewas a significant change in coherence alpha

FIGURE 4. The predictors of paragraph recall score from the auditory attention task.

Scientific Articles 11

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

relationships as the participants moved fromthe EC to listening state. None of the coher-ence alpha relationships showed a significantincrease and almost all were in the negativedirection, thus negating the possibility thatthe analysis was overlooking smallerincreases as the participants moved fromEC to AA to listening, which may be, inaggregate, significant if combined.

STABILITY OF RESPONSE PATTERNACROSS DIFFERENT TASKS

Table 5 presents the correlations betweenthe EC and listening tasks to describe thestability of the variables across differenttasks for the relative power and microvoltmeasures. Table 6 presents the data for thesubsequent coherence alpha predictors. Asthe tables indicate there are significant posi-tive correlations between the variables underthe different tasks. However, it does notappear that this stability is sufficient toemploy the EC task for prediction purposesdue to variability of the response patternacross these tasks. For example, the T3CAcorrelation is .78, providing an R2 value of.61, leaving a large amount of unexplainedvariance.

Changes from EC to VA

Figure 5 presents the significant changesas the participants move from the EC to

the VA task. As there were many changesinvolving only a few locations, the descrip-tion of the results will focus on the mostdominant patterns. The change from EC toVA results in large increases in relativepower in beta2, right hemisphere microvoltsof beta2, lateral locations for peak frequencybeta1, and symmetry beta1 and beta2 mea-sures while the decreases in values involvedbroad decreases in PKFT, PKAT, RPA,PKAA, MA and posterior PKAB1 andPKAB2 and more centrally located and pos-teriorly located SYMB1 and SYMB2 mea-sures. Connection activity decreased in CAat all locations, in CB1 for frontal and cen-tral locations, in PA frontal locations andin PB1 frontal and temporal locations. Thusthe act of looking evokes the beta2 fre-quency, decreases all frequencies lower than13 hertz, and decreases connection activity,both phase and coherence, from frontal loca-tions and between all locations in the coher-ence alpha variable. The greatest changes(>1 SD) were the global decreases in alpha(RP, PKA, CA, PKFT) and increases inRPB2.

Changes from VA to Reading

Figure 6 presents the significant changesas the participants move from VA to readingsilently (RS). The change from VA to RSresults in continued posterior increases inbeta2 (MB2, RPB2) along with broadincreases in theta (PKAT), frontal theta(MT) alpha (PKAA, MA) and beta1(PKAB1), right frontal SYMB1 measuresalong with CB1 activity from posterior loca-tions (P3, T6, O2, P4) and CB2 from poster-ior locations (T5, P3, Pz, P4, T6, O1, O2).

TABLE 5. Associations of relative power and micro-volts in eyes-closed and listening tasks.

RPD 0.79RPT 0.72RPA 0.90RPB1 0.90RPB2 0.82MD 0.81MT 0.86MA 0.94MB1 0.94MB2 0.82

Note. RP: Relative Power, M: Microvolt, D: delta, T: theta, A:

alpha, B1: beta1, B2: beta2, Bold numbers are significant at

.05 level.

TABLE 6. Reliability of coherence measures acrossthe tasks of eyes-closed and listening tasks.

F7CA 0.84F8CA 0.84T3CA 0.78C3CA 0.75P3CA 0.64

Note. C: Coherence, P: Phase, D: delta, T: theta, A: alpha,

B1: beta1, B2: beta2, Bold numbers are significant at .05

level.

12 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

Decreases were evident in frontal alpha(RPA), frontal beta activity (RPB1, RPB2)and broadly located theta coherence and

phase activity, as well as frontally locatedflashlights (PA). In summary, as the partici-pant moves from VA to RS the clinically

FIGURE 5. The significant changes as the participants move from the eyes-closed to the visual attention task.

FIGURE 6. The significant changes as the participants move from visual attention to reading silently.

Scientific Articles 13

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

relevant results are the increased posteriorMB2 and RPB2, increased posterior betacoherence activity. Successful readinginvolves F7 coherence activity, a top downprocess (Figure 7). There were no variableswhose averaged value (across all 19 loca-tions) increased greater than 1 SD in thischange. An additional analysis of thechanges from EC to RS was undertaken todetermine if smaller changes were occurringas the participants moved between thesethree states, which if taken in aggregatewould be significant. As in the auditorysituation, there were no significant positivechanges in the critical variables (F7 coher-ence and phase activity; T5 coherence alpharelationships). As the participants movedfrom the EC to RS condition there were sig-nificant decreases in several of these criticalvariables: T5CA, �1.08 SD; F7CB1, �1.0

SD; F7PB1, �1.04 SD. However, much ofthis decrease can be explained by the changein state from an EC to an eyes open condi-tion. Comparing the two attention measures(VA vs. AA) indicates that these valuesdecrease as a result of opening the eyes.The following changes occur: T5CA, �.73SD; F7CB1, �1.04 SD; F7PB1, �.77 SD.

PREDICTING READING MEMORYFROM PREVIOUS TASKS

Predicting from EC to Reading Memory

Figure 7 presents the correlates of readingrecall under task (Thornton, 2002) in a nor-mal population. As the figure indicates, thesuccessful pattern is predominantly F7 beta1and beta2 coherence and phase flashlight

FIGURE 7. The correlates of reading recall under task in a normal population.

FIGURE 8. The predictors of reading memory from the eyes-closed task.

14 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

patterns along with CA from the T5 loca-tion. Thus successful reading memory is pri-marily dependent upon left hemispherecoherence activity. This is another exampleof the third method that measures the effec-tive variables under task conditions.

Figure 8 presents from the predictors ofreading memory from the EC task. The posi-tive predictors involve frontal theta (RP) andF7CA. The negative predictors involve dif-fuse sites and the beta2 frequency. This is asecond example of the first method, predict-ing from EC to a later obtained cognitivemeasure.

Predicting Reading Memory from VA Task

Figure 9 presents the correlates from theVA task to subsequent reading recall. Thepositive predictors were the MD measurein central locations. Negative predictorsinvolved PB1 from O1 and O2 and F4CA.None of these predictors accurately identi-fied the subsequent correlates under thetask.

Visual Activation Patterns and SubsequentReading Recall

The analysis of the changes from EC toVA and from VA to reading revealed thatas the participants changed from an EC toVA task they decreased the values of thepredictors indicated in Figure 7. Thechange from VA to reading does not resultin any significant improvement or decreas-ing of these values. As in the paragraphtask, one conclusion that can be assertedis that the normal brain is not particularlyeffective at activating what it needs to besuccessful at the task, the ‘‘inefficient acti-vation pattern.’’

QEEG DIFFERENCES BETWEEN TASKS

Differences in QEEG Variables Between AAand VA Tasks

It is of some clinical value to understandthe differences between the two attentiontasks and two cognitive tasks, as clinician’s

FIGURE 9. The correlates from the visual attention task to subsequent reading recall.

Scientific Articles 15

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

may have their patients in either an EC oreyes open condition during the training andmay misinterpret the meaning of the changein values. In addition, as the clinician is view-ing the EC as the comparison state, a clinicalerror of assuming improvement in a variablemay occur when, in reality, the only reasonfor the change maybe due to the patientopening their eyes. Only the most dominantdifferences will be reported. The variableswhich are greater in the AA task comparedto the VA task include alpha (RP, M,PKA, CA) and frontal beta1 flashlights(CB1, PB1), frontal phase alpha and leftfrontal CB2 flashlights, symmetry beta1measures at P3, P4, O1, Cz, Pz and SYMB2measures at Fz, Cz.. The VA task variablesare higher in all RPB2 values, frontalRPB1, SYMB1 measures at F7, F8, T3, T4and SYMB2 at T6.

QEEG Differences Between Listening Silently(LS) and Reading Silently (RS)

Figure 10 displays the variables that aresignificantly greater in the reading silentlytask (RS) compared to the listening silentlytask (LS) and Figure 11 presents the vari-ables that are greater during the listeningcompared to the reading task. Reading hasgreater values than listening in frontal betaactivity (RPB1, PKAB1, MB1, SYMB1),posterior beta (PKFB1, MB2, RPB2,SYMB2) and diffusely located higher valuesfor beta2 (PKAB2). The overall pattern isone of frontal beta1 values higher and pos-terior beta2 values higher than in the listen-ing task as well as increased CB1 fromoccipital locations.

Listening silently exhibits greater valuesthan reading for diffuse locations in the theta

FIGURE 10. Significantly greater variables in the reading silently task compared to the listening silently task.

16 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

frequency (PKFT), alpha (RPA, MA,PKAA), and increases in central and poster-ior symmetry beta1 measures. Of some inter-est to note is that the LS task evokes highervalues in the broadly located connectionvariables in the lower frequencies (CT, PT,CA) and frontal located flashlights in thebeta frequencies (PA, PB1, CB1, PB2) andincreases in central and posterior symmetrybeta1 measures. Thus the LS task engagesthe lower frequencies more as well as invol-ving more activity in the coherence andphase associations. Both tasks involvesemantic processing, which argues againstthe von Stein & Sarnthein (2000) hypothesisthat the lower frequencies are involved insemantic processing.

DISCUSSION

The findings present a complex systemthat defies adequate scientific understandingat this point in the development of the field.However, the findings do have implicationsfor how EEG biofeedback intervention

protocols should proceed. The results indi-cate (1) tasks evoke a system response whichinvolve different locations and different fre-quencies; (2) focusing on a particular loca-tion, such as Cz or frequency does notadequately address the complexity of the sys-tem; (3) the high beta2 frequency (32–64Hz)is intimately involved in brain functioning;(4) EC and simple attention data are not suf-ficient to understand or predict what isrequired to improve cognitive functioningin normal individuals; (5) The figures andtables provided also indicate to the clinicianthat an improvement (from an EC database)on a variable may not relate to the effective-ness of the intervention but merely to achange in task; (6) improvement on a parti-cular variable may have no relations toimprovement of cognition; (7) interventionsare generally conducted with eyes open andemploy an EC database to determine inter-ventions. However, merely opening of theeyes results in many reductions in the alphafrequency as well as other changes (seeFigure 5 for specifics). The failure to sup-press alpha under eyes open condition can

FIGURE 11. Significantly greater variables during the listening compared to the reading task.

Scientific Articles 17

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

be considered a clinical problem (Thornton,Carroll, & Cea, 2007).

More specifically when addressing pro-blems in auditory memory in adults, the pro-tocols should be directed towards increasingcoherence alpha relationships. When addres-sing reading problems, the F7 coherence andphase flashlights (beta1 and beta2) andT5CA flashlight may require attention.

It is relevant to note, however, an addi-tional comment. Thornton has been involvedin cases where the subject’s values on vari-ables, which are not related to successful taskperformance, were several standard devia-tions below the norm and required addres-sing. One common pattern is low posteriorcoherence beta relationships during reading.There are two ways to conceptualize thisissue. One way is to consider that variablesare necessary but not predictive of goodmemory functioning. The second way is toconsider that any variable (coherence valuesin particular) which is grossly deviant fromthe norm may function as a hindrance toeffective cognitive functioning.

The preceding discussion has focused onthe clinical value of having the subjectundergo specific cognitive tasks to under-stand the subject’s deficits in QEEGresponse pattern on the variables whichrelate to performance. In addition to thevalue of individual task QEEG analysis,there is relevant clinical information thatcan be obtained from the subject’s responsepattern across the different tasks. Two casestudies illustrate the value of the activationdatabase.

In the first case study, a woman withimpaired reading had coherence alpha valueswell above the norm in the paragraph listen-ing task and her levels of coherence beta1and beta2 were below the norm at locationF7, in addition to other locations (Thornton,2006). The difference between her auditoryand reading memory ability was 5.29 stan-dard deviations. It is instructive in this caseto ask whether the subject’s beta coherencevalues under the reading condition reflectan underlying structural deficit in themyelinated fibers or a lack of appropriateallocation. An examination of her betacoherence values under the EC to the reading

condition, indicated that the subject wasincreasing coherence values between thefrontal locations and decreasing the betacoherence values within the posterior loca-tions, while her F7CB2 (both raw and stan-dard deviation values) decreased as thetasks changed from VA to reading. This pat-tern would indicates (1) that the subject hasthe necessary physiological resources, butwas not appropriately employing them and(2) knowledge of the subject’s F7CB2 stan-dard deviation value in the VA task wouldnot have allowed accurate prediction to theF7 value during the reading task.

In the case of a 21 year old male with ahistory of severe reading disability, theexamination of the response pattern acrossdifferent tasks proved critical to rehabilita-tion efforts. The subject’s relative power ofalpha was within normal limits under ECcondition as well as all of the tasks whichinvolved the EC. Only when the subjectopened his eyes did the relative power ofalpha values increase in their standard devia-tion value to approximately 3 standarddeviations above the norm. Overall the sub-ject’s raw relative power of alpha valueincreased an average of .25 across all loca-tions, thus indicating a failure to suppressalpha under visual task conditions. Oncethe rehabilitation protocols were set toaddress this problem, the subject improvedsignificantly in his reading ability assessedby standardized testing. In this example,the subject’s standard deviation value ofalpha in the EC task would not haveindicated the appropriate intervention.(Thornton et al., 2007).

The purposes of the research were to (1)examine the relative value of databasesobtained under different conditions inimproving cognition; (2) to understand howthe brain responds to different taskdemands; (3) to understand how the QEEGvariables relate to one another. To achievethis purpose, the changes in activity levelsat locations and between locations wereexamined during several tasks includingEC, AA, VA, as well the input stages ofparagraphs presented aurally and readingpresented visually. The brain responsepatterns in each task were associated with

18 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

performance on memory tasks. The resultsshowed that the QEEG variables measuredin the recall tasks were more consistent withneuroscience research of memory responsepatterns than those measured in the ECand simple attention tasks. Specifically, theQEEG measures during recall show lefthemisphere involvement, which has beenshown by PET to be active in auditory mem-ory (Mazoyer et al., 1993). These findingssuggest that interventions using EEG bio-feedback have an advantage in obtainingtreatment success when selection is basedon an activation database. For example,there are associations between QEEG mea-sures taken under the EC condition andrecall memory. Specifically, the relativepower of the theta bandwidth is directlyrelated to memory while there are inverserelations with microvolt and relative powervalues of beta2 bilaterally in central andanterior regions. However, the theta fre-quency in the EC condition has not beenassociated historically with effective cogni-tive performance (Harmony et al., 1990)and would not be a recommended protocolto improve auditory memory.

The locations that were most stronglyassociated with memory performance wereidentified using the flashlight concept. Inthe auditory memory task, the greatest asso-ciations to performance are with the coher-ence alpha flashlight activity in the lefthemisphere and right frontal locations. Pre-vious PET research has confirmed the roleof the left temporal lobe (T3) and left frontal(F7) locations in auditory processing andauditory memory (Mazoyer et al., 1993),the role of the right frontal lobe (Henson,Shallice, & Dolan, 1999) during recall, aswell as the dominant role of the left hemi-sphere in verbal processing.

The current study identifies coherencealpha as a contributor to the left hemispherefunctioning. The predictors from EC(Figure 3) and AA (Figure 4) do not fit wellwith previous PET research, or with presentneuroscience understanding of anatomicalfunctioning and previous QEEG researchwhich has identified theta activity as anegative predictor of cognitive abilities(Harmony et al., 1990, Lubar et al., 1995).

In the reading task, improved performanceis associated with sources of coherence inbeta from left frontal region (F7) as well assources of coherence in alpha from andT5CA activity. The previously researchedidentified role of the left hemisphere in lan-guage processing overlaps with these QEEGfindings. The predictors from EC (theta) andVA (delta) do not fit well with previousQEEG research that indicated that elevatedlevels of left hemisphere theta and deltaunder EC condition predicted poor educa-tional evaluations in children (Harmonyet al., 1990).

There are also specific QEEG variableswhich have a negative correlation with recallscores (T6 PB1). In the reading task theincreased phase beta activity from the T6location is inversely related to memory. ECdata or attention task data do not providethe relevant information to formulate effec-tive interventions, while activation QEEGcorrelates of cognition provide the necessaryinformation for highly effective interven-tions. Figure 6 indicates that reading is pre-dominantly a bottom up processing task ina normal population with increased micro-volts of beta2 in posterior locations and pos-terior flashlight activity (coherence beta1and beta2). However, successful readinginvolves F7 coherence activity, a top downprocess (Figure 7).

The results presented in this paper suggesta coordinated allocation of resources (CAR)hypothesis of cognitive effectiveness. TheCAR hypothesis states that effective cogni-tive functioning is determined by multiplespecific variables acting in unison to achieveoptimal performance and that these vari-ables can be different in different tasks.The QEEG variables that are related toperformance include activity in the betafrequency at specific locations as well asthe coherence and phase relationshipsbetween locations in specific frequencies.While there are significant correlationsbetween the attention tasks and memory per-formance, the QEEG variables identified inthe attention tasks are not the variables thataccount for success during the memory taskand thus are not sufficient to develop anappropriate intervention protocol using

Scientific Articles 19

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

EEG biofeedback. Both reading andauditory memory tasks require different setsof resources for success. We cannot assumethat there is a single intervention protocolthat will broadly affect reading and auditorymemory as different tasks require allocationof different sets of QEEG variables.

In addition, the data document that thehuman brain does not activate the necessaryvariables for success in a task. For example,coherence alpha values in the auditory taskdo not increase as the subjects move froman AA task to the listening to paragraphstask. One conclusion that can be reached isthat the normal brain is not efficient or effec-tive in its activation response pattern. Thisphenomenon can most succinctly be calledthe ‘‘inefficient activation pattern.’’ Thisconclusion, if validated in a larger sample,has significant implications for the EEGbiofeedback field and education. If theresources are available but just not employedcorrectly, interventions become pragmati-cally easier to accomplish then trying to‘‘build’’ connections which don’t exist. Thenormal human mind is not efficient at acti-vating the necessary correlates of effectivecognitive functioning, as indicated by thecognitive inefficiency hypothesis.

There are, however, patterns of relationsbetween variables across tasks which areclinically important to understand in deter-mining protocol interventions. These pat-terns need to be understood in addressingthe cognitive ineffectiveness of the LD,ADHD and TBI patient if we are to obtainthe desired results.

REFERENCES

Brain Resource Company (2007). http://www.brainresource.com/

Buckner, R. L. & Vincent, J. L. (2007). Unrest at rest:default activity and spontaneous network correla-tions. Neuroimage, 37(4), 1091–1096.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for thebehavioral sciences (2nd edition). Hillsdale, NJ:Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Collura, T. F. (2008). Toward a coherent view of brainconnectivity. Journal of Neurotherapy, 12(2=3),99–100.

Corsi-Cabrera, M., Herrera, P., & Malvido, M. (1989).Correlation between EEG and cognitive abilities:Sex differences. International Journal of Neu-roscience, 45(1–2), 133–141.

Evans, J. R. & Abarbanel, A. (1999) Introduction toquantitative EEG and neurofeedback. New York:Academic Press.

Golland, Y., Bentin, S., Gelbard, H., Benjamini, Y.,Heller, R., & Nir, Y., et al. (2007). Extrinsic andintrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of thehuman brain revealed during natural sensory stimu-lation. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 766–777.

Golland, Y., Golland, P., Bentin, S., & Malach, R.(2008). Data-driven clustering reveals a fundamen-tal subdivision of the human cortex into two globalsystems. Neuropsychologia, 46(2), 540–553.

Gonzalez-Hernandez, J. A., Cespedes-Garcia, Y.,Campbell, K., Scherbaum, W. A., Bosch-Bayard,J., & Figueredo-Rodriguez, P. (2005). A pre-taskresting condition neither ‘baseline’ nor ‘zero.’Neuroscience Letters, 391(1–2), 43–47.

Harmony, T., Hinojosa, G., Marosi, E., Becker, J.,Rodriguez, M., & Reyes, A., et al. (1990). Correla-tion between EEG spectral parameters and an edu-cational evaluation. International Journal ofNeuroscience, 54(1–2), 147–155.

Hedges, L. V. & Olkin, I. (1985) Statistical methods formeta-analysis. New York: Academic Press.

Henson, R. N., Shallice, T., & Dolan, R. J. (1999).Right prefrontal cortex and episodic memory retrie-val: a functional MRI test of the monitoringhypothesis. Brain, 122(Pt 7), 1367–1381.

Jasper, H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system ofthe International Federation. Electroencephalogra-phy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375.

John, E. R. & Prichep, L. S. (2006). The relevance ofQEEG to the evaluation of behavioral disordersand pharmacological interventions. Clinical EEG& Neuroscience, 37(2), 135–143.

Lubar, J. F. (Ed.). (2003) Quantitative electroencepha-lographic analysis (QEEG) databases for neurother-apy: Description, validation, and application.New York: The Haworth Medical Press.

Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N., &Timmermann, D. L. (1995). Quantitative EEG andauditory event-related potentials in the evaluationof attention-deficit=hyperactivity disorder: Effectsof methylphenidate and implications for neuro-feedback training. Journal of PsychoeducationalAssessment, (Monograph Series Advances inPsychoeducational Assessment) ADHD Special,143–204.

Mazoyer, B. M., Tzourio, N., Frak, V., Syrota, A.,Murayama, N., & Levrier, O., et al. (1993).The cortical representation of speech. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience, 5(4), 467–479.

20 JOURNAL OF NEUROTHERAPY

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009

McEvoy, L. K., Smith, M. E., & Gevins, A. (2000). Atest-retest reliability of cognitive EEG. ClinicalNeurophysiology, 111, 457–463.

McKiernan, K. A., D’Angelo, B. R., Kaufman, J. N.,& Binder, J. R. (2006). Interrupting the ‘‘stream ofconsciousness’’: An fMRI investigation. Neuro-image, 29(4), 1185–1191.

Morcom, A. M., & Fletcher, P. C. (2007). Does thebrain have a baseline? Why we should be resistinga rest. Neuroimage, 37(4), 1073–1082.

Niedermeyer, E. (1987). EEG and clinical neurophy-siology. In E. Niedermeyer & F. Lopes da Silva(Eds.), Electroencephalography: Basic principles,clinical applications and related fields (pp. 97–117).Baltimore: Urban and Schwarzenberg.

Oken, B. S. & Chiappa, K. H. (1988). Short-term varia-bility in EEG frequency analysis. Electroencephalo-graphy & Clinical Neurophysiology, 69(3), 191–198.

Raichle, M. E. & Snyder, A. Z. (2007). A default modeof brain function: a brief history of an evolvingidea. Neuroimage, 37(4), 1083–1090.

Thatcher, R. W. (1998). Normative EEG databases andEEGbiofeedback. Journal ofNeurotherapy, 2(4), 8–39.

Thatcher, R. W. (1999). EEG database-guided neu-rotherapy. In J. R. Evans & A. Abarbanel (Eds.),Introduction to quantitative EEG and neurofeedback(pp. 29–64). New York: Academic Press.

Thatcher, R. W., Walker, R. A., Gerson, I., &Geisler, F. (1989). EEG Discriminate analysis ofmild head trauma. EEG and Clinical Neurophysiol-ogy, 73, 93–106.

Thornton, K. E. (1996). On the nature of artifactingthe QEEG. Journal of Neurotherapy, 1(3), 31–40.

Thornton, K. E. (1999). Exploratory investigation intomild brain injury and discriminant analysis withhigh frequency bands (32–64Hz). Brain Injury,13(7), 477–488.

Thornton, K. E. (2000). Electrophysiology of auditorymemory of paragraphs. Journal of Neurotherapy,4(3), 45–73.

Thornton, K. E. (2000). Exploratory analysis: Mildhead injury, discriminant analysis with highfrequency bands (32–64Hz) under attentional

activation conditions & does time heal? Journal ofNeurotherapy, 3(3=4), 1–10.

Thornton, K. E. (2001). Patent #6309361 B1 Methodfor Improving Memory by Identifying and UsingQEEG Parameters Correlated to Specific CognitiveFunctioning – issued 10–30-2001.

Thornton, K. E. (2002). Electrophysiology (QEEG) ofeffective reading memory: Towards a generator=activation theory of the mind. Journal of Neurother-apy, 6(3), 37–66.

Thornton, K. E. (2003). Electrophysiology of the rea-sons the brain damaged subject can’t recall whatthey hear. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology,18(4), 363–378.

Thornton, K. E. (2006a). No child left behind goals(and more) are obtainable with the neurocognitiveapproach, Vol. 1. North Charleston, SC: BooksurgePublishers.

Thornton, K. E. (2006b). Subtype analysis of LD byQEEG pattern analysis. Biofeedback, 34(3), 106–114.

Thornton, K. E. & Carmody, D. P. (2005). EEG bio-feedback for learning disability and traumatic braininjury. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics ofNorth America, 14(1), 137–162.

Thornton, K. E. & Carmody, D. P. (2008). Efficacy oftraumatic brain injury rehabilitation: Interventionsof QEEG-guided biofeedback, computers, strate-gies, and medications. Applied Psychophysiologyand Biofeedback, 33(2), 101–124.

Thornton, K. E. & Carmody, D. P. (2009). Traumaticbrain injury rehabilitation: QEEG biofeedbacktreatment protocols. Applied Psychophysiologyand Biofeedback. http://www.springerlink.com/content/r0234682x7737561/fulltext.pdf

Thornton, K. E., Carroll, C., & Cea, J. (May, 2007).Remediation of reading disability: contributionsof an activation database effective treatmentplanning. Neuroconnections, 9–11.

von Stein, A. & Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different frequen-cies for different scales of cortical integration: fromlocal gamma to long range alpha=theta synchroni-zation. International Journal of Psychophysiology,38(3), 301–313.

Scientific Articles 21

Downloaded By: [Thornton, Kirtley E.] At: 12:42 27 February 2009