N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition

12
Ž . Cognitive Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 Research report N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition Jacques B. Debruille a, ) , Jaime Pineda b , Bernard Renault c a Centre de Recherche Fernand-Seguin, UniÕersite de Montreal, 7331 rue Hochelage, Montreal, Quebec H1N 3V2, Canada ´ ´ ´ ´ b Department of CognitiÕe Science, UniÕersity of California at San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA c Laboratoire de Psychophysiologie CognitiÕe, CNRS-URA 654, UniÕersite Paris VI, Paris, France ´ Accepted 16 April 1996 Abstract Within the theoretical framework of reference, the brain errs in processing complex stimuli, such as faces. Thus, these stimuli not only activate accurate representations but also inaccurate representations corresponding to known persons who resemble the face stimulus, and hence knowledge about these known persons. Since more errors are made in processing unfamiliar than familiar stimuli, these inaccurate activations are assumed to be more frequent, andror more intense, with unknown than with known faces. Moreover, top-down mechanisms favor representations of stimuli that are congruent with the context, and representations of known persons, even if inaccurate, receive an additional amount of activation in contexts wherein known faces are expected. Inaccurate representations have to be inhibited to achieve accurate recognition. Thus, more inhibition would be required for unknown than for known faces, and in contexts wherein known faces are expected. The aim of the present work is to study the hypothesis that the N400 component of the event-related potentials Ž . ERPs reflects the inhibition of knowledge, and to see whether this hypothesis accounts for the N400-like potential elicited by faces. To achieve that goal, ERPs to known and unknown faces were recorded while the richness in known faces of each experimental block, and thus the expectancy for known faces, was manipulated. Consistent with the hypothesis, the amplitudes of the N400-like components were greater in conditions where more inhibition was required, i.e. for unknown rather than for known faces, and in the context of the block rich in known faces. This context effect was larger for unknown than for known faces. Keywords: Event-related potential; N400; N400-like; Face; Context; Knowledge; Inhibition 1. Introduction Ž . The event-related potentials ERPs elicited by faces include a negative potential resembling the N400 compo- Žw x . nent elicited by words 16,19,34 for reviews since this potential peaks around the same latency, i.e. 450 ms post-onset, and since its amplitude can also be reduced by w x semantic priming 3,12,54 . Indeed, the amplitude of the N400-like potential elicited by the face of a known politi- cian, for instance, is smaller when its presentation has been wx preceded by the face of another known politician 3 than when preceded by the face of a known person belonging to Ž a different semantic category e.g. an actor, a sport star, . etc. . One of the most quoted hypotheses about the functional significance of the N400 component is based on this ) Ž. Ž . Corresponding author. Fax: 1 514 251-2617; e-mail: [email protected] w x semantic priming effect 5,22,23,48 . Accordingly, the component indexes the activation within semantic net- w x works 32,33 . Thus, the large amplitude of the N400s Ž . elicited by a word e.g. DOCTOR that has been preceded Ž by a stimulus that is not semantically associated e.g. . SOCKS can be interpreted as reflecting the large activa- tion that occurs when preceding stimuli did not activate semantically appropriate representations. Conversely, the small amplitudes of the N400s elicited by the same word Ž . when it is primed by a semantic associate e.g. NURSE reflects the small amount of activation that is necessary when the preceeding stimulus already carried on a part of the appropriate activation. Similarly, the large N400-like amplitudes to faces preceded by the face of a semantically unrelated person could reflect the large amount of seman- tic activation required when the preceding stimuli did not activate semantically appropriate representations. However, the semantic priming view of N400 has been challenged by several other hypotheses, including that of w x the semantic mismatch 11,21,42,46 . This alternative hy- 0926-6410r96r$15.00 Copyright q 1996 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Ž . PII S0926-6410 96 00032-8

Transcript of N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition

Ž .Cognitive Brain Research 4 1996 133–144

Research report

N400-like potentials elicited by faces and knowledge inhibition

Jacques B. Debruille a,), Jaime Pineda b, Bernard Renault c

a Centre de Recherche Fernand-Seguin, UniÕersite de Montreal, 7331 rue Hochelage, Montreal, Quebec H1N 3V2, Canada´ ´ ´ ´b Department of CognitiÕe Science, UniÕersity of California at San Diego, San Diego, CA, USA

c Laboratoire de Psychophysiologie CognitiÕe, CNRS-URA 654, UniÕersite Paris VI, Paris, France´

Accepted 16 April 1996

Abstract

Within the theoretical framework of reference, the brain errs in processing complex stimuli, such as faces. Thus, these stimuli not onlyactivate accurate representations but also inaccurate representations corresponding to known persons who resemble the face stimulus, andhence knowledge about these known persons. Since more errors are made in processing unfamiliar than familiar stimuli, these inaccurateactivations are assumed to be more frequent, andror more intense, with unknown than with known faces. Moreover, top-downmechanisms favor representations of stimuli that are congruent with the context, and representations of known persons, even if inaccurate,receive an additional amount of activation in contexts wherein known faces are expected. Inaccurate representations have to be inhibitedto achieve accurate recognition. Thus, more inhibition would be required for unknown than for known faces, and in contexts whereinknown faces are expected. The aim of the present work is to study the hypothesis that the N400 component of the event-related potentialsŽ .ERPs reflects the inhibition of knowledge, and to see whether this hypothesis accounts for the N400-like potential elicited by faces. Toachieve that goal, ERPs to known and unknown faces were recorded while the richness in known faces of each experimental block, andthus the expectancy for known faces, was manipulated. Consistent with the hypothesis, the amplitudes of the N400-like components weregreater in conditions where more inhibition was required, i.e. for unknown rather than for known faces, and in the context of the blockrich in known faces. This context effect was larger for unknown than for known faces.

Keywords: Event-related potential; N400; N400-like; Face; Context; Knowledge; Inhibition

1. Introduction

Ž .The event-related potentials ERPs elicited by facesinclude a negative potential resembling the N400 compo-

Žw x .nent elicited by words 16,19,34 for reviews since thispotential peaks around the same latency, i.e. 450 mspost-onset, and since its amplitude can also be reduced by

w xsemantic priming 3,12,54 . Indeed, the amplitude of theN400-like potential elicited by the face of a known politi-cian, for instance, is smaller when its presentation has been

w xpreceded by the face of another known politician 3 thanwhen preceded by the face of a known person belonging to

Ža different semantic category e.g. an actor, a sport star,.etc. .One of the most quoted hypotheses about the functional

significance of the N400 component is based on this

) Ž . Ž .Corresponding author. Fax: 1 514 251-2617; e-mail:[email protected]

w xsemantic priming effect 5,22,23,48 . Accordingly, thecomponent indexes the activation within semantic net-

w xworks 32,33 . Thus, the large amplitude of the N400sŽ .elicited by a word e.g. DOCTOR that has been preceded

Žby a stimulus that is not semantically associated e.g..SOCKS can be interpreted as reflecting the large activa-

tion that occurs when preceding stimuli did not activatesemantically appropriate representations. Conversely, thesmall amplitudes of the N400s elicited by the same word

Ž .when it is primed by a semantic associate e.g. NURSEreflects the small amount of activation that is necessarywhen the preceeding stimulus already carried on a part ofthe appropriate activation. Similarly, the large N400-likeamplitudes to faces preceded by the face of a semanticallyunrelated person could reflect the large amount of seman-tic activation required when the preceding stimuli did notactivate semantically appropriate representations.

However, the semantic priming view of N400 has beenchallenged by several other hypotheses, including that of

w xthe semantic mismatch 11,21,42,46 . This alternative hy-

0926-6410r96r$15.00 Copyright q 1996 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.Ž .PII S0926-6410 96 00032-8

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144134

pothesis is based upon the fact that physical mismatcheselicit negative ERP components peaking around 200 mspost-onset, the N2s, and upon the idea that ‘... semanticinformation processing generally takes longer than mosttypes of physical processing’. Hence, N400s are viewed asa type of delayed N2 components that index the semanticmismatch generated by the adjacent presentation of seman-

Ž .tically unrelated words e.g. SOCKS DOCTOR .w xKoyama et al. 30 proposed another alternative hypoth-

esis that associates N400s with decision-making processes.Their study included experiments in which N400s wererecorded in response to target Kanji words and pseudo-

ŽKanji words i.e. meaningless line drawings which looked.like Kanji ideograms in three different prime conditions: a

related condition where targets were preceded by semanti-cally related prime words; a neutral condition where theprime consisted of a drawing that pictured a pair of twoconcentric circles; and an unrelated condition where primeswere words semantically unrelated to the targets. HalfŽ .200 of these targets were real words, the other half werepseudo-words, and subjects had to perform a lexical deci-

Žsion task i.e. to decide whether the targets were or were.not real Kanji words . As in other similar experiments, the

classical effect of semantic priming on reaction times wasfound, i.e. the responses were performed more quickly fortarget words in the related than in the neutral condition. Onthe other hand, subjects took longer to respond to unrelatedtargets than to neutral targets. Prolonged decision-makingprocesses were proposed to account for these longer reac-tion times since they were observed in the conditionswhich violate the dominant associations. Indeed, in theexperiment, unrelatedness was associated with the non-word response in the majority of cases, i.e. in 160 out of200 trials. Thus, the correct decision was likely to be moredifficult to make for unrelated targets which were realwords. Since N400 amplitudes were, as reaction times,greater in the unrelated than in the neutral conditions, andsince no significant N400 amplitude differences wererecorded between targets of the related conditions andtargets of the neutral conditions, the authors also associ-ated N400 amplitude with the prolonged decision-makingprocess.

However, none of the previously described views canaccount for all of the N400 observations. The semanticactivation and the semantic mismatch views, for instance,do not make sense of the word frequency effect, i.e. of thesmaller N400 amplitudes to unprimed words of high thanto unprimed words of low frequency of occurrence in the

w xlanguage 50,55,57 . Indeed, there is no reason frequentŽ .words should induce less activation in semantic network s ,

or induce smaller mismatches than infrequent words. Asfor the decision-making process view, it does not account,for instance, for the large amplitudes of the N400s elicitedin situations where subjects were not required to make any

Ž w x.decision e.g. 58 .The aim of the present study was to test another hypoth-

esis about N400. This hypothesis stipulates that the com-ponent amplitude is generated by the inhibition of knowl-edge 1 which is not compatible with the meaning of the

w xeliciting stimulus in its context 13 . According to thisknowledge inhibition hypothesis, or N4KI hypothesis, thelarge amplitudes of the N400s elicited by words, such asDOCTOR, when preceded by semantically unrelated words,such as SOCKS, reflect the inhibition of knowledge acti-vated by these unrelated words. Conversely, small N400amplitudes elicited by the same words when preceded bysemantically related words, e.g. NURSE, reflect the smallamount of knowledge inhibition performed when preced-ing stimuli activated knowledge that is mostly compatiblewith the eliciting stimulus.

The N4KI hypothesis stipulates that N400 amplitudereflects the inhibition of any type of incompatible knowl-edge, not only incompatible knowledge activated by pre-ceding stimuli, but also incompatible knowledge activatedby the eliciting stimulus itself via inaccurate representa-tions. Indeed, according to many word-processing modelsw x4,17,29,37,38,40,41 , word presentations activate both ac-curate representations and inaccurate representations ofwords that physically resemble the stimulus word. Thepresentation of the word BRIBE, for instance, activates notonly the representation of BRIBE but also that of BRIDE 2.Hence, knowledge corresponding to these inaccurate wordscan also be activated. Since physically similar stimuli areusually not semantically similar stimuli, this knowledge isnot compatible with the one activated through the accuratestimulus representation. Thus, within the N4KI hypothesisframework, it will be inhibited and responsible for anN400 activity 3.

Since more errors, and hence more inappropriateknowledge activations, are made when processing low-

1 The concept of knowledge used here has been adapted from that ofŽ .persons identity nodes PINs included in models of person recognition

w xsuch as that of Bruce and Young 7 . These models stipulate that once theŽrecognition unit for the face of a person or the recognition unit for the

.person’s voice has been activated, knowledge about the person and thecontexts wherein he or she had previously been encountered, is, usuallyautomatically and irrepressibly, activated. Similarly, the activation of avisual or auditory word representation would induce the activation ofknowledge. Such knowledge is viewed as elementary blocks used to build

w xrepresentations of situations 29 .2 Such activations are covert, i.e. not correlated with any phenomenon

of awareness of the resembling stimuli and, similarly, to other types ofw xinappropriately activated representations 18,56 , inaccurate representa-

w xtions are inhibited 10,53 , except when an explicit error occurs, e.g.when BRIBE is mistaken for BRIDE.

3 Thus, although, the ‘usual way’ of producing incompatibility inN400 studies is the sequential presentation of two unassociated stimuliw x5,48,22 , the N4KI hypothesis stipulates that a semantic incompatibilitycan be generated by a single stimulus and, thus by a single presentation.

w xInterestingly, Anderson and Holcomb 1 recently published a studyshowing that N400s effects between semantically associated and unasso-ciated prime-target word-pairs can be obtained even when prime andtarget words both occur in a single visual presentation.

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 135

w xthan high-frequency items 20,51 , the N4KI hypothesiscan account for the fact that N400 amplitudes are greater

w xfor infrequent than frequent words 50,55,57 . Consistentwith this account of the frequency effect, N400 amplitudesto low-frequency words, such as BRIBE, have been foundgreater than to equally low-frequency words, such as SIG-NAL, which do not ortographically resemble other words

w xas much 14,15 .The aim of the present study was to further assess the

N4KI hypothesis and to determine whether it can alsoaccount for the N400-like component generated by faces.To achieve this goal, we examined the amplitudes of theN400s elicited by known and unknown faces during a‘fame decision task’. It was assumed that unknown faceswould require more knowledge inhibition, and hence elicitlarger N400 activities than known faces. Indeed, like infre-

Ž .quent words and pseudo-words , unknown faces are stim-uli that the brain is not used to process, and for which it isthus more likely to err. Unknown faces should then bemore likely to activate representations corresponding to

Ž .similar stimuli i.e. to faces of resembling known personsand hence to activate incompatible knowledge.

Žw x.According to several models of word processing 45 ,representations that are congruent with the context receive,via top-down mechanisms, an additional amount of activa-tion, whether these representations are accurate or not 4.Extended to faces, this means that the representationscorresponding to known faces which resemble the facestimulus receive an additional amount of activation incontexts where known faces are expected. Therefore, in-compatible knowledge that correspond to these knownpersons 5, is more likely to be activated. Under these

4 w x Ž .Potter et al. 42 have shown that non-words e.g. dack and anoma-Ž . Ž .lous words e.g., deck are misread as appropriate words e.g. duck in

the context of sentences that biases their processing toward the meaningŽof their orthographic neighbor word e.g. ‘The child fed the dackrdeck at

.the pond’ . These authors although showed that the bias was greater fornon-words than for words, and thus for stimuli that the brain is nottrained to process than for stimuli for which it is trained. They show howsuch misidentification can be accounted for by an additional amount ofactivation to context congruent stimuli. For faces, such an additionalamount of activation provide a way to account for the strong effect ofcontext on misidentifications. Such an effect, noticeable in everyday life,

w xhas been quantified in the Young, Hay and Ellis’ 59 study whichreported that for 33% of the misidentifications of an unfamiliar face as afamiliar person, subjects reported they ‘would definitely have expected tomeet the person they had thought they had encountered at that time andplace’.

5 The concept of knowledge used here has been adapted from that ofŽ .persons identity nodes PINs included in models of person recognition

w xsuch as that of Bruce and Young 7 . These models stipulate that once theŽrecognition unit for the face of a person or the recognition unit for the

.person’s voice has been activated, knowledge about the person and thecontexts wherein he or she had previously been encountered, is, usuallyautomatically and irrepressibly, activated. Similarly, the activation of avisual or auditory word representation would induce the activation ofknowledge. Such knowledge is viewed as elementary blocks used to build

w xrepresentations of situations 29 .

conditions, N4KI hypothesis predicts an additional N400activity, since more inhibition would be required.

In order to test this prediction, the expectancy forknown faces was manipulated by using blocks of stimulicontaining different percentages of known faces. Block A,rich in known faces, included 67% of these stimuli andonly 33% of unknown faces. Block B was neutral andincluded 50% of known and 50% of unknown faces. BlockC was poor in known faces, including only 33% of thesestimuli and 67% of unknown faces. N400-like amplitudesto known and to unknown faces were expected to be largerin the context of the block A than in other blocks, and thiseffect was expected to be larger for unknown than forknown faces. Indeed, for unknown faces, the additionalamount of activation only affects representations that have

Žto be inhibited, while, for known faces, it also and.probably mainly affects the accurate representations, given

that these latter representations are congruent with thecontext rich in known faces. Thus, it is especially forunknown faces that contexts rich in known faces shouldlead to a greater inhibition.

Manipulating context by varying the percentage of eachcategory of stimuli induces the classical effect of probabil-ity on the amplitude of the ERP component that is adjacent

w x w xto the N400, viz. the P3 28 . Indeed, Kutas et al. 31 haveshown that such category variations induce large P3 ef-fects, just like the manipulation of the probability ofoccurrence of one stimulus. Given that such effects werelikely to create a problem for the interpretation of theresults since N400 and P3 components overlap, intertrial

Ž .intervals ITIs of four s were chosen because the resultsw xof recent studies 39,44 suggest that, when ITIs equal or

are longer than 4 s, both frequent and rare stimuli elicitlarge P3s, and the classical effects of probability on P3disappear.

In sum, famous known and unknown faces were pre-sented to subjects who were asked to decide whether ornot each face was that of a famous person that he or sheknew. The predictions drawn from the N4KI hypothesis

Ž .were: a that the N400-like amplitudes should be greaterŽ .for unknown than for known faces; b that these ampli-

tudes should be larger in the contexts of the block A, richŽ .in known faces; and c that this block effect should be

larger for unknown than for known faces.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects

The subjects were six males and six females between 20and 30 years of age who watched French television at least

Ž3 h a week. They were all right-handed, had normal or.corrected to normal visual acuity, reported being free of

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144136

neurological or psychiatric disorders and had at least un-dergraduate degrees.

2.2. Stimuli

Stimuli consisted of 160 color slides portraying a frontview of different faces, chosen from the archives of two

Žmajor French press agencies Agence Gamma and Cipa.Press . Half were men, and half were women. In order to

reduce automatic semantic priming from one face to thew xnext 8 , the faces were taken from different categories,

including politicians, pop stars, sports stars and actors, inapproximately equal numbers, and, in the experiment, thecategory to which two adjacent known persons belongswas never the same. Eighty of the faces belonged to people

Ž .well known to French citizens known faces and 80 ofthem belonged to celebrities from other western countries

Ž .and unknown to French citizens unknown faces . BothŽ .classes of stimuli known and unknown faces looked

equally ‘famous’ since they had the same general presenta-Žtion and were both professionally photographed thus in-

creasing the likeliness that unknown faces activate repre-.sentations of famous known persons . Unlike other previ-

ous face experiments, slides of known faces were selectedfrom unpublished slides that did not show the person in atypical pose. This was done to reduce the likelihood thatthe subject would respond based on an immediate feelingof familiarity which would have introduced large differ-ences between the processing of known and unknownfaces. The slides were rephotographed in order to standard-ize, as much as possible, brightness, contrast, and dimen-sions of the face, and to ensure that the eyes were at thesame height on the slides. These new 160 stimuli werethen divided into two matched sets, each of which included40 known and 40 unknown faces. They will be refered toas the first and the second set.

The first set was duplicated twice, in order to have atotal of three exemplars of it. One of these exemplars of

Žthe first set was put in each of the three blocks A, B and.C . The 40 slides of known faces of the second set were

then added to block A, in order to increase its richness inknown faces. This block thus included a total of 120

Ž .slides, 80 40q40 known and 40 unknown faces, result-Ž .ing in a high 67% percentage of known faces, and a low

Ž .33% percentage of unknown faces. The 40 unknownfaces of the second set were added to block C, in order todecrease the proportion of known faces. Block C thus

Ž .included a total of 120 slides, 40 known and 80 40q40Ž .unknown faces, resulting in a low 33% percentage of

Ž .known faces and a high 67% percentage of unknownfaces. Block B was left unchanged and thus included atotal of 80 slides, 40 known and 40 unknown faces,

Ž .resulting in the medium 50r50% percentage conditions.Subjects were divided in three groups of four. The order ofthe presentation of the blocks in the first group was ABC,in the second group: BCA, and in the third group: CAB.

2.3. Procedure

Subjects sat in a comfortable chair in a sound-at-tenuated room, and were told to stare the center of a screenŽ .1.50 m away during the presentation of each rack of 40slides. They were asked to wait for two s after each slidehad disappeared before blinking. Each face subtended avisual angle of 58, and was displayed for a 400 msduration, in order to ensure that the processing would becompleted, and that the hypothesized inhibition could oc-cur. The time interval between adjacent stimuli was sys-tematically jittered and varied between 3.5 and 4.5 s.

Subjects had to respond as quickly and as accurately aspossible by moving their right index finger above twoelectrical cells, toward the right for known faces andtoward the left for unknown faces. They were asked not totry to recall names but to respond as soon as they weresure they had seen the person before.

During the debriefing session, subjects were shown theunknown faces for which they had given the motor re-sponse corresponding to known faces and asked if theyhad actually recognized a known person. If so, they wereasked who that person was. This question revealed thatamong the famous persons of other western countries usedas unknown faces, none were correctly recognized. Thesetrials were counted as false recognitions since subjectsmistook these faces for those of persons who were famousin France. The subjects were also shown the known facesfor which they had given the motor response correspond-ing to unknown faces. When they did not know the face ofthe famous person, the trial was included in the unknown

Žface trials this happened for two or three faces for each.subject . When they knew the face of the person but did

not recognize it on the particular slide used in the experi-ment, the trial was counted as an unrecognized known facetrial but excluded from the ERP averaging.

2.4. Data acquisition and processing

Ž .Reaction times RTs , number of errors and ERPs werecomputed for the slides of the first set only. ERPs wererecorded with a longitudinal and a transverse branch of

w xelectrodes placed according to the 10–20 system 24 . TheŽlongitudinal branch included 6 equidistant leads 10% of

.the inion-nasion distance : Oz, POz, Pz, CPz, Cz and FCz.The transverse branch crossed the longitudinal branch atPz, and included 5 equidistant leads which covered pari-etotemporal regions and were termed: P4, PP4, Pz, PP3and P3. Linked earlobes were used as a reference. Verticaland horizontal eyes movements were monitored by 2separate EOG bipolar recordings of electrodes placed aboveand below the right eye for vertical movements and at theouter canthi for horizontal eye movements. Rejection ofartifacted trials was done off-line and manually by examin-ing the printed EEG for each trial. EOG and EEG wereamplified with a 2 s time constant and an upper half

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 137

amplitude cutoff at 50 Hz; signals were digitized on lineby a PDP 11r60 computer at a sampling rate of 125 Hz.ERPs were calculated off-line, between 0 and 1200 msafter the onset of each slide with a 200 ms prestimulusbase line.

The correct response trials were grouped according toŽ .the category of the stimulus known or unknown . The

measures were the average amplitude within time windowscentered on each wave peak, 240–300 ms for the N270wave, 350–500 ms for the N450 wave, and 500–700 msfor the P600 wave. MANOVAs with familiarityŽ . Žknownrunknown , richness in known faces blocks A, B

.and C and electrodes as factors were used for testing thew xlevel of significance of the effects 35 . For each time

window, MANOVAs were run for the longitudinal and forthe lateral montages. Thus, for this latter montage, theelectrode factor corresponded to hemispheres. MANOVAswith a restricted number of factors were used for posthocanalyses when at least one of the factor had more that twolevels. When no factor had more than two levels, ANOVAswere used.

3. Results

3.1. BehaÕioral results

3.1.1. Debriefing sessionWhen presented again with the slides that were used in

the experiment, subjects appeared to remember quite well

Table 1Ž . Ž .Mean reaction time ms and S.D. values for known and unknown faces

Žin the block A, rich in known faces 67% known and 33% unknown. Ž .faces , in block B, neutral 50% known and 50% unknown faces and in

Ž .block C, poor in known faces 33% known and 67% unknown faces

Block Known faces Unknown faces

Ž . Ž .A 703 61 760 64Ž . Ž .B 716 74 742 84Ž . Ž .C 702 87 719 73

what happened during the presentation of many of thefaces. All subjects reported that the task was slightly

Ž .difficult. Most of them 10 out of 12 reported that a fewŽ .times four times on average they based their response on

the basis of vague resemblances.

3.1.2. RTsTable 1 shows the RTs to known and unknown faces

for the different blocks. The 2-way MANOVA, includingŽ .familiarity known and unknown faces and block as fac-

tors, revealed that there was a tendency for known faces tobe responded to faster than unknown faces, F s3.15,1,11

0.1-P-0.05. There was no significant effect of block,F s0.76, nor was there a significant interaction be-2,22

tween block and familiarity, F s1.01.2,22

3.1.3. Error ratesThe numbers of errors are depicted in Table 2. The

2-way MANOVA with familiarity and block as factor

Ž . Ž . Ž .Fig. 1. Grand mean ERPs ns12 to known left and unknown faces right . ERPs of the three upper lines are from electrodes of the midline longitudinalŽ . Ž .branch FCz, CPz and POz and ERPs of the two lower lines are from electrodes of the lateral branch P3 and P4 . Time is in s, and each tic mark

representing 0.2 s, stimulus onset is at 0 s. Thin lines correspond to block A, rich in known faces, i.e. including 67% known and 33% unknown faces.Medium lines correspond to block B, neutral, i.e. including 50% known and 50% unknown faces. Thick lines correspond to block C, poor in known faces,i.e. including 33% known and 67% unknown faces.

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144138

Table 2Ž .Number of errors and S.D. values for known and unknown faces in

blocks A, B and C

Block Known faces Unknown faces

Ž . Ž .A 3.96 3.65 2.25 2.0Ž . Ž .B 6.00 3.6 0.59 1.0Ž . Ž .C 4.75 3.07 1.08 1.1

revealed that subjects were more likely to fail to recognizethe ‘known’ faces than to incorrectly ‘recognize’ the un-known faces, F s12.5, P-0.005. It also revealed a1,11

significant interaction between block and familiarity, F2,22

s6.21, P-0.05. Separate 1-way ANOVAs for knownand unknown faces run to assess the source of this interac-tion showed that for known faces, the errors rate did notsignificantly varied across blocks. For unknown faces, thenumber of errors was significantly larger in the block Athan in the block B, F s7.85, P-0.025, and than in1,11

the block C, F s4.82, Ps0.05. Thus, unknown faces1,11

gave rise to more false recognitions when they occurred inthe block rich in known faces than in the other blocks.

3.2. Electrophysiological results

The first deflection that can be identified on the ERPŽ .waveform Fig. 1a,b is a 5 mV negative peak or frontocen-

tral N120, because it peaks around 120 ms on FCz, themost anterior lead. The second deflection is a large posi-tive component, peaking at Cz around 180 ms. Its latency,its large amplitude, and its topography identify it as the

Ž . wvertex positive potential VPP obtained with faces 6,25–x27,36,52 . The third wave is a large negative component,

peaking at Cz at around 270 ms. This central N270 beginsat about 200 ms or earlier and ends around 450 mspoststimulus but its ending overlaps with the followingcomponent. Its latency range and topography correspond tothose of the N2b but its amplitude is rather large comparedto that found in studies that did not use faces as stimuliŽ .but used LEDs or pure tones instead . Large centralN270s were also obtained in previous studies with facesw x2,3,54 . Following N270 is a widely distributed wave thatappears as a negative deflection on the downhill slopefrom the central N270 peak to the next positive compo-nent. This fourth wave peaks at approximately 450 ms andis clearly different from the central N270. Similar N450

w xwaves have been reported in ERP face studies 3,54 . Thefifth deflection is a positive wave with maximum ampli-

Ž .tude at Pz peaking around 600 ms P600 . The beginningof this component is difficult to estimate due to overlap-ping with the N450. Because of its parietal maximum andlatency, this P600 wave is assumed to be a component ofthe P3b family.

3.2.1. Central N270 time windowŽ .For known faces Fig. 1a , the amplitude of the central

N270 did not vary across blocks. In contrast, for unknownfaces, the amplitude of this component appeared larger inblock A than in the two other blocks, primarily at the FCzand P3 locations. The 3-way MANOVA for the longitudi-nal montage did not however reveal any significant effectsof block. But the same MANOVA on lateral electrodesrevealed an interaction between percentage and familiarity,F s3.28, P-0.05. There was also an interaction of2,22

the three factors, percentage, familiarity and lateral leads,F s6.34, P-0.05, in accordance with the greater2,10

amplitude of the component at the left parietotemporal siteŽ .P3 for the block A. The separate 1-way ANOVAs whichwere then run for unknown faces and for each lead toassess the source of the interaction revealed that the effectof block was significant only at that lead, F s9.4,1,11

P-0.02.

( )3.2.2. P600 P3b time windowŽ .For unknown faces Fig. 1b , visual inspection revealed

no effects of block, and thus of probability on P3 ampli-tude. For known faces, the ERPs were slightly morepositive in the block C than in the other blocks. The two3-way MANOVAs showed that the effect of block wasneither significant on the lateral nor on the longitudinalbranch. Despite this absence of significant results on theoverall analysis, and to further check the absence of aprobability effect in the P3b time window, 1-way ANOVAs

Ž .for known faces with block A and B, and A and C asfactor were run at each lead in the 560–700 ms timewindow which best captures the difference. No significanteffects were found between blocks A and C. Table 3 whichpictures the results of these ANOVAs for blocks A and Bshows that the effect reached significance only at Cz, andthat there was a trend on the two adjacent leads.

3.2.3. N450 time windowOverall, visual inspection of Fig. 1a,b showed that

ERPs to known and to unknown faces were more negativein the block A, rich in known faces than in the other

Table 3Known faces: results of the site-by-site 1-way ANOVAs on mean ERP amplitude in a 560–700 ms latency region that best captures the difference in theP600 time region between blocks A and B

FCz Cz CPz Pz POz Oz P4 PP4 PP3 P3)F 4.66 6 4.80 2.6 1.7 0.3 0.3 2.9 2.8 2.01,11

Effect quotations: P-0.05 ).

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 139

Fig. 2. Scalp distribution of the ERP differences in the N400 timewindow for unknown faces between blocks A and B.

blocks, and that this difference appeared larger for un-known than for known faces.

The 3-way MANOVA performed on the longitudinalŽ .branch in the N450 latency region 350–500 ms revealed

that the ERPs to unknown faces were significantly morenegative than the ERPs to known faces, F s21.3 P-1,11

0.001, that there was a significant interaction betweenblock and familiarity, F s6.32, P-0.025, meaning2,22

that the effect of block was significantly greater for un-known than for known faces, and that there was an interac-tion between electrodes, block and familiarity, F s10,110

2.56, P-0.025. The same MANOVA run on the lateralelectrodes revealed a significant interaction of block andfamiliarity, F s4.38, Ps0.025, but no significant in-2,22

teraction with electrode sites.Two 2-way MANOVA were run to assess the source of

the interaction of familiarity, blocks and electrode sites onthe longitudinal montage. For known faces, this analysisdid not reveal any significant interaction between blocksand electrodes. In contrast, for unknown faces, this interac-tion was significant, F s2.88, P-0.005. Separate10,110

1-way ANOVAs run to assess the source of this later

interaction revealed that the differences between blocks Aand B were significant at all leads, while the differencesbetween blocks A and C were significant at all leadsexcept POz and Oz, where there was only a trend.

Fig. 2 better illustrates the scalp distribution of theN450 difference between blocks A and B unknown facesfor the longitudinal montage. It suggests that the interac-tion between the block effect and the electrodes comesfrom the existence of two different maxima, one at FCzand one at Pz. Indeed, large at FCz, the difference wassmaller at Cz, reached a local minimum at CPz, thenincreased and exhibited a new maximum at Pz, after whatit decreased again and was small at POz and even smaller

Ž .at Oz consistent with Table 4 . Nevertheless, althoughCPz is less likely to be noisy than other electrodes since it

Žis far from the eyes and from the muscles of the head i.e.from temporal muscles and from muscles of the neck and

.of the jaw , it could argue that noise is responsible for theslightly smaller amplitude difference at Cz and CPz.

3.2.4. Effects of immediately preceding stimuli in N450time window

In block A, rich in known faces, the two precedingstimuli were more often known than unknown faces, whilein the blocks B and C, preceding stimuli were more oftenunknown faces. To assess whether the effect of block onthe N450 was due to the immediately preceding stimuli,i.e. to a sequence effect, ERPs to known and unknownfaces were reanalyzed segregating these stimuli accordingto the category of the two preceding faces. If the effect ofblock comes from an effect of sequence, then this effectshould be at least as large as that of block. Thus, for bothknown and unknown faces, we examined the conditions in

Ž .which they were preceded by two known faces KK , byŽ .one unknown and one known face UK , by one known

Ž .and one unknown face KU , and by two consecutiveŽ .unknown faces UU .

Fig. 3a,b shows these new averages. The amplitude ofthe N450 is largest for stimuli preceded by two consecu-

Ž .tive known faces KK condition , smaller for UK and KUconditions, and smallest for stimuli preceded by two con-

Ž .secutive unknown faces UU condition . The shape, laten-Ž .cies and preferential locations FCz and parietal are iden-

Table 4Unknown faces: results of the site-by-site 1-way ANOVAs on mean ERP amplitude in the 350–500 ms latency region

FCz Cz CPz Pz POz Oz P4 PP4 PP3 P3

Block ArBF 16.9 14.9 9.7 12.3 9.9 8.1 17.2 13.7 9.4 12.61,11

) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) )

Block ArCF 14.3 12.7 8.2 10.3 3.9 4.1 16.3 12.1 7.5 10.61,11

) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) )

3.23-F -4.84, 0.1-P-0.05; ) P-0.05; ) ) P-0.025; ) ) ) P-0.01; ) ) ) ) P-0.005.1,11

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144140

Ž .Fig. 3. Grand mean ERPs ns12 to known and unknown faces according to the sequence of preceding stimuli. Thick continuous lines correspond toŽ . Ž .faces that were preceded by two known faces KK , horizontal dashed lines to faces that were preceded by a known and an unknown face UK , vertical

Ž . Ž .dashed lines by an unknown and a unknown face KU and thin continuous lines by two consecutive unknown faces UU . All else is as in Fig. 1.

tical to the shape, latencies and preferential locations of theŽblock effect. But, at fronto-central and central sites Fig.

.3b , the size of the sequence effect appears smaller thanthe size of the block effect. The 3-way MANOVA with

Ž .familiarity knownrunknown , preceding stimuliŽ .KKrUKrKUrUU and midline electrodes as factors didnot reveal a significant effect of preceding stimuli. Thesame MANOVA on lateral montage revealed that theeffect of preceding stimuli were marginally significant,F s2.83, P;0.05 and tended to interact with elec-3,33

trodes F s 1.74, 0.05 - P - 0.1. Separate 1-way12,121

MANOVAs run on mean amplitude within the 350–500ms latency region for each electrode revealed that the onlysignificant effect of preceding stimuli occurred for un-known faces at the P3 electrode site, F s3.48, P-3,33

0.05.

4. Discussion

Ž .In the time region of the P3 component P600 , blockaffected the ERPs only to a minimal extent. In contrast, in

Žthe time region of the N400, familiarity knownrunknown.faces and block significantly affected the ERPs. These

latter effects were consistent with the predictions drawnfrom the N4KI hypothesis. Indeed, ERPs were more nega-tive to unknown than to known faces, and in the block A,than in the other blocks, and this effect of block interactedwith familiarity: it was large and significant for unknownfaces only.

4.1. Disentangling P3b probability effects from effects onthe N450

w xOnly a few studies 39,44 suggest that when intertrialŽ .intervals ITIs are equal or longer than 4 s both frequent

and rare stimuli elicit large P3s, and the classical effects ofprobability on P3 disappear. In the present study, ITIsvaried at random between 3.5 and 4.5 s. Thus, beforeanalyzing the variations of the ERPs within the N450 timewindow as genuine modulations of the amplitude of theN450 wave, one has first to see whether these variationswere not due to a residual probability effect on the P3bŽ . w xi.e. P600 component 28 . Indeed, both effects occur inapproximately the same time regions and may overlap.

The P3 probability effect consists of an enhancement ofthe P3 amplitudes for infrequent stimuli. For known facesŽ .Fig. 1a , probability could thus account for the differences

Ž .in the P600 time window and for the non-significantdifferences in the N450 time window, since ERPs wereslightly more positive when these stimuli were less fre-quent. However, these slight differences in the N450-P600time region did not have much of the characteristics of a

Ž .P3 effect Fig. 1a . Indeed, they were only reliable at theŽ .central site Cz , while the classical context effect on P3 is

Ž .maximal at parietal site Pz , and, instead of reaching theirŽmaximum at 600 ms poststimulus i.e. the latency of the

.P3 peak in this study , they reached their maxima at, ormore probably after, 1200 ms. Thus, these differencesappear more likely to be due to two different causes, thefirst one being a small modulation of N450 in a directionconsistent with the predictions drawn from the N4KI hy-

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 141

pothesis, and the second one, a modulation of a latecomponent, occurring later than the P3b.

Ž .For unknown faces Fig. 1b , ERPs were superimpos-able at the peak of the P600. However, this does not meanthat there was no P3 probability effect. Indeed, this effectcould be masked by an overlapping effect on N450 in theopposite direction. Such an overlapping would also dimin-ish the size of the effect on N450, which may then belarger than it appears on Fig. 1b.

Thus, the effect of block on the N450 amplitude can besummarized as follows. For unknown faces, it is large,significant, and in the directions of the predictions drawnfrom the N4KI hypothesis. For known faces, it is either

Ž .smaller as predicted , non-significant, and in the direc-tions of the predictions drawn from the N4KI hypothesis,or, less likely, totally absent since due to a P3 probabilityeffect.

4.2. N450 waÕe and N400-like

Ž .These effects on the N450 wave are at least partly dueto genuine N400-like effects. Indeed, three previous stud-

w xies 3,12,54 already found that face stimuli elicit N450waves whose amplitudes, just like that of the N400 compo-

w xnent to words 22,30,48–50,58 , are modulated by repeti-tion and semantic priming. Moreover, the analysis of theERPs to the slides of the first set, as a function of

Žrepetition each slide of the first set was presented 3 times. Ž w x.during the experiment , allowed us in 13 to see that the

amplitude of this wave significantly decreases with repeti-tion.

4.3. Could N400-like effects themselÕes be due to probabil-ity

Before interpreting this N400-like effect as a conse-quence of the context richness in known faces, one has toassess the effect that probability could have on the ampli-tude of the N400 itself, and to determine whether the largeN400 amplitudes to infrequent unknown faces in block Acould be accounted for by such an effect.

Probability has been manipulated in several word stud-w xies 9,22,30 in order to modulate the extent to which

subjects generate semantic expectancies. In these studies,subjects were presented with serial prime-target pairs and

Žhad to perform a lexical decision task i.e. to decide.whether the target was a real word or a pseudo-word . A

proportion of the primes were words which were semanti-cally associated with the target words, and probability wasmanipulated by varying this proportion. The idea was thatgreater proportions of associated pairs reinforce the gener-ation of semantic expectancies, and therefore, N400 ampli-

w xtudes. Consistently, in these three studies 9,22,30 N400amplitudes to unprimed words were found larger in theblocks including high than in the blocks including lowproportions of related pairs.

Given that the known faces belonged to the samesemantic category, i.e. that of famous faces, they could

have induced an N400 priming which could have beenmodulated by probability. However, such an N400 primingdid not appear, and N400 amplitudes to known faceswhich were immediately preceded by other known faceswere not smaller than that to known faces preceded byunknown faces. The re-averaging of the data according tothe sequence of preceding stimuli showed not only the

Žabsence of such an N400 priming because of the long.ITIs? but, moreover, it showed non-significantly larger

N400 amplitudes to such known faces than to known facesŽ .preceded by unknown faces Fig. 3a . Thus, a modulation

of an effect of semantic priming by probability was un-likely to be responsible for the N400-like effects of thepresent study.

w xAlternatively, Deacon et al. 11 claimed that the N400amplitude is, like the N2 amplitude, inversely proportionalto probability even in conditions where ‘... all of thestimuli were unprimed’, and thus, independently from anysemantic priming. They based their claim on the results of

w xa prior study 47 that included a condition in whichanimal names occurred on 20% of the trials and non-animalnames on 80% of the trials, and wherein subjects had torespond to the 20% category, and thus to the animalnames. The substractions of the ERPs corresponding to the80% category from those corresponding to the 20% cate-gory include a negativity peaking around 400 ms post-onset

Ž w x.that was reinterpreted in 11 , as an N400 component,and used to support the view that N400 amplitude isinversely proportional to the stimulus probability. How-ever, infrequent stimuli, i.e. animal names, were moreoften preceded by frequent stimuli, and thus by stimuli ofanother category, than by stimuli of their own category.The additional N400 to infrequent stimuli in that conditioncan thus be accounted for by an effect of category mis-match on N400 as well as by an effect of probability.

As a matter of fact, if probability was able to modulateN400 amplitude independently, one would see such modu-lations in a word experiment where semantic relationshipsbetween words that were adjacent in the stimulus list werenot at stake. This does not seem to be the case. Polich and

w xDonchin 43 have run such an experiment where theprobabilities of occurrence of both words and pseudo-wordswere manipulated while subjects were performing a lexicaldecision task. The inspection of the results in the N400time region shows that neither the ERPs to words, northose to pseudo-words were more negative when these

Ž . Ž .items had a low 20% than when they had a high 80%probability of occurrence.

Finally, in the present experiment, such an independenteffect of probability would have predicted larger N400amplitudes to known faces when their probability of occur-rence was low than when that probability was high. Thiswas not the case. As a matter of fact, in the N400 timeregion, the ERPs to known faces in the block C were lessnegative than those to known faces in the blocks A and B,although this difference was not significant. Therefore,

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144142

probability, as such, is unlikely to be responsible for N400effects in the present experiment.

4.4. Functional significance of the N400 effects

4.4.1. Within the framework of the N4KI hypothesisWithin that framework, the larger amplitudes of the

N400s to unknown than to known faces are due to the factthat the brain is less trained to process unknown thanknown faces. Accordingly, it more often errs, and thepresentation of unknown faces more often activates, androractivates to a greater extent, representations correspondingto resembling known faces and their corresponding knowl-edge. Hence, more incompatible knowledge would have tobe inhibited than in the case of known faces.

Although pretty rare, false recognitions of unknownfaces were significantly more frequent in the block A, richin known faces, consistent with the idea that inaccuraterepresentations corresponding to known faces which re-semble the eliciting stimulus received an additional amount

Žof activation in this block since although inaccurate, theserepresentations were consistent with the block’s context 6.The greater N400-like amplitudes to unknown faces in theblock A than in the other blocks would index the addi-tional amount of knowledge inhibition required for accu-rate processing in that context.

For known faces, the N400-like differences betweenblocks were not significant, and, as expected, significantlysmaller than for unknown faces. Within the N4KI hypothe-sis, this is due to the fact that for known faces, theadditional amount of activation can also be allocated to the

Žcorrect representations since they are that of a known.face , which does not increase the inhibition requirement.

Moreover, since known faces activate less inaccurate rep-resentations than unknown faces, this additional amount ofactivation will be more focused on those correct represen-tations.

4.4.2. Semantic actiÕationThe present N400 results cannot be made consistent

with the hypothesis that N400 indexes semantic activation

6 w x Ž .Potter et al. 42 have shown that non-words e.g. dack and anoma-Ž . Ž .lous words e.g. deck are misread as appropriate words e.g. duck in the

context of sentences that biases their processing toward the meaning ofŽtheir orthographic neighbor word e.g. ‘The child fed the dackrdeck at

.the pond’ . These authors although showed that the bias was greater fornon-words than for words, and thus for stimuli that the brain is nottrained to process than for stimuli for which it is trained. They show howsuch misidentification can be accounted for by an additional amount ofactivation to context congruent stimuli. For faces, such an additionalamount of activation provide a way to account for the strong effect ofcontext on misidentifications. Such an effect, noticeable in everyday life,

w xhas been quantified in the Young, Hay and Ellis’ 59 study whichreported that for 33% of the misidentifications of an unfamiliar face as afamiliar person, subjects reported they ‘would definitely have expected tomeet the person they had thought they had encountered at that time andplace’.

w x32 . Indeed, no faces were preceded by the face of aŽ w x.semantically associated person in contrast to 8,3 . Thus,

preceding stimuli were not able to carry on a part of thesemantic activation and all this activation had to be done atthe occurrence of each face. Accordingly, N400 amplitudeshould have always been large and there should have beenno N400 amplitude differences. This was not the case.

Nevertheless, as above, it might be argued that even ifknown faces do not picture persons that have the same

Ž .type of occupation politic, sport, etc. , they still share acommon semantic property: that of being ‘famous mediapersons’ that appear in TV shows. In that sense, theymight still semantically prime each other. But such asemantic priming effect implies that N400-like to knownfaces should have been of significantly smaller amplitudesin the priming context of the block A, rich in known faces.This was not the case since these amplitudes were eitherslightly and not significantly larger or exactly identical. Italso implies that N400-like amplitude should have beensmaller for known faces which were immediately precededby other known faces than for known faces immediatelypreceded by unknown faces. This was also not the case, asmentioned above, the reaveraged data based on the se-quence of preceding faces showed that there was a ten-dency in the opposite direction. Finally, the activationview would have predicted no N400 effects for unknownfaces since these faces cannot be primed. This was not thecase either.

4.4.3. Semantic mismatchThe view that N400 components index semantic mis-

w xmatches 11,21,42,46 would have also predicted constantand large N400 amplitudes for each stimulus, since seman-tic mismatches occurred in every cases. Indeed, none ofthe faces were primed by the face of a semantically relatedperson, and semantic mismatches were constant. In con-trast, N400-like amplitudes varied across conditions. Itcould alternatively be proposed that the face of a famousknown person does not produce a semantic mismatchwhen it occurs among other famous faces. The mismatchview then also predicts that the amplitudes of the N400-liketo known faces would be significantly smaller in the blockA, than in the other blocks. This was not the case. Asdiscussed earlier these amplitudes were either slightly andnot significantly larger or exactly identical. The semanticmismatch view would also have predicted that N400-likeamplitude would be significantly smaller for known facesimmediately preceded by other known faces than whenpreceded by unknown faces, which was neither the case.

4.4.4. Decision-making processw xKoyama et al. 30 hypothesis that N400 amplitudes

‘are associated with a prolonged decision-making processfor incongruous items’ could have accounted for a part ofthe result of the experiment. Indeed, faces were presentedthree times in the experiment. While on average, the level

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144 143

of familiarity was greater for known than for unknownfaces, unknown faces became a little familiar with repeti-tion. Thus, by the 2nd and 3rd blocks, unknown facescould have induced a tendency to give the response corre-sponding to known faces, and the larger amplitudes of theN400-like to unknown than to known faces could be dueto the prolonged decision-making process induced by thattendency. Similarly, the large amplitudes of the N400selicited by unknown faces in the block A, dominated byknown faces, could be related to the tendency to give theresponse corresponding to the known faces in that context.

But, the logic of this interpretation implies that infre-quent known faces should have also activated the responsecorresponding to unknown faces in the context of theblock C, rich in unknown faces, and thus elicited largerN400 amplitudes than known faces in the blocks A and B.This was not observed. These amplitudes were slightly andnot significantly smaller or identical. Thus, Koyama et al.w x30 idea does not account for the results, nor does itaccount for the large N400 amplitudes obtained in otherstudies wherein subjects were not required to make a

Ž w x.decision e.g. 58 .

5. Conclusion

The N4KI hypothesis thus accounts for the results in amore satisfying manner than the semantic activation, themismatch, and the decison-making process views.

Acknowledgements

This work was one among several studies done forfullfilment of a Ph.D. thesis. Jacques B. Debruille wassupported by a grant from the Fondation pour la RechercheMedicale, by a Lavoisier fellowship from the French Min-´istere des Affaires Etrangeres, by a postdoctoral fellowship` `from the McDonnell-Pew and the Fonds de la Rechercheen Sante du Quebec. Jaime Pineda was supported by a´ ´grant from the Air Force Office of Scientific ResearchŽ .AFOSR and a Career Development Award. Bernard Re-nault was supported by the Centre National de la Recherche

Ž .Scientifique CNRS . We are grateful to Jonathan W. Kingand to three anonymous reviewers for helpful commentson previous versions of this paper. The authors wish tothank Roger Grob and Jacques Martinerie for their techni-cal assistance, Brita Von Maydell, Sipa Press and Gammaagencies for providing the slides of the experiment, andEmmanuel Stip for his support.

References

w x1 Anderson, J. and Holcomb, P., Auditory and visual semantic prim-ing using different stimulus onset asynchronies: an event-related

Ž .brain potential study, Psychophysiology, 32 1995 177–190.w x2 Barrett, S.E., Rugg, M.D. and Perrett, D.l., Event-related potentials

and the matching of familiar and unfamiliar faces, Neuropsycholo-Ž .gia, 26 1988 105–117.

w x3 Barrett, S.E. and Rugg, M.D., Event-related potentials and theŽ .semantic matching of faces, Neuropsychologia, 27 1989 913–922.

w x4 Becker, C.A., What do we really know about semantic contexteffects during reading? In D. Besner, T.G. Waller and E.G. MacKin-

Ž .non Eds. , Reading Research, Vol. 5, Academic Press, San Diego,CA, 1985, pp. 125–169.

w x5 Bentin, S., Mc. Carthy, C. and Wood, C.C., Event-related potentials,lexical decision, and semantic priming, Electroencephalogr. Clin.

Ž .Neurophysiol., 60 1985 353–355.w x6 Botzel, K. and Grusser, O.J., Electric brain potentials evoked by¨ ¨

pictures of faces and non-faces: a search for ‘face-specific’ EEGŽ .potentials, Exp. Brain Res., 77 1989 349–360.

w x7 Bruce, V. and Young, A., Understanding face recognition, Br. J.Ž .Psychol., 77 1986 305–327.

w x8 Bruce, V. and Valentine, T., Semantic priming of familiar faces,Ž .Q.J. Exp. Psychol., 38A 1986 125–150.

w x9 Chwilla, D.J., Brown, C.M. and Hagoort, P., The N400 as a functionŽ .of the level of processing, Psychophysiology, 32 1995 274–285.

w x10 Colombo, L., Activation and inhibition with orthographically similarŽ .words, J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform., 12 1986 226–

234.w x11 Deacon, D., Breton, F., Ritter, W. and Vaughan, H.G., The relation-

ship between N2 and N400: scalp distribution, stimulus probability,Ž .and task relevance, Psychophysiology, 28 1991 185–200.

w x12 Debruille, B., Breton, F., Robaey, P., Signoret, J.-L. and Renault, B.,Potentiels evoques cerebraux et reconnaissance consciente et non-´ ´ ´ ´consciente des visages: application a l’etude de la prosopagnosie,` ´

Ž .Neurophysiol. Clin., 19 1989 393–405.w x13 Debruille, B., Etudes des processus de traitement des visages: apport

des potentiels evoques. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Universite Pierre´ ´ ´et Marie Curie, Paris, France, 1992, pp. 1–207.

w x14 Debruille, J.B., Knowledge inhibition and N400: a study with wordswhich look like a common word, submitted.

w x15 Debruille, J.B., Knowledge inhibition and N400: a study with wordswhich look like a common word, and with homophones, submitted.

w x16 Fischler, I., Comprehending language with event-related potential. InŽ .J.W. Rohrbaugh, R. Parasuraman and R.E. Johnson Eds. , EÕent-

Related Brain Potentials: Issue and Applications, Oxford UniversityPress, New York, NY, 1990, pp. 165–177.

w x17 Forster, K.I., Basic issues in lexical processing. In W. Marslen-Wil-Ž .son Ed. , Lexical Representation and Process, MIT Press, Cam-

bridge, MA, 1989, pp. 75–107.w x18 Gernsbacher, M.A. and Faust, M.E., The mechanism of suppression:

a component of general comprehension skill, J. Exp. Psychol.Ž .Learn. Mem. Cogn., 17 1991 245–262.

w x19 Halgren, E., Insights from evoked potentials into the neuropsycho-logical mechanisms of reading. In A.B. Scheibel and A.F. WechslerŽ .Eds. , Neurobiology of Higher CognitiÕe Function, Vol. 29, Guil-ford, New York, NY, 1990, pp 103–150.

w x20 Havens, L.L. and Foote, W.E., The effect of competition on visualduration threshold and its dependence of stimulus frequency, J. Exp.

Ž .Psychol., 65 1963 6–11.w x21 Herning, R.I, Speer, M. and Jones, R.T., Event-related potentials to

spoken equations: is the N400 really a late N200? Electroen-Ž .cephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl., 40 1987 394–398.

w x22 Holcomb, P.J., Automatic and attentional processing: an event re-lated brain potential analysis of semantic processing, Brain Lang.,

Ž .35 1988 66–85.w x23 Holcomb, P.J. and Neville, H.J., Auditory and visual semantic

priming in lexical decision: a comparison using event-related poten-Ž .tials, Lang. Cogn. Process., 5 1990 281–312.

w x24 Jasper, H.H., The ten–twenty system of the International Federation,Ž .Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 10 1958 371–375.

w x25 Jeffreys, D.A. and Musselwhite, M.J., A face-responsive visualŽ .evoked potential in man, J. Physiol., 390 1987 26.

( )J.B. Debruille et al.rCognitiÕe Brain Research 4 1996 133–144144

w x26 Jeffreys, D.A. and Tukmachi, E.S.A., The vertex-positive scalpŽ .potential evoked by faces and by objects, Exp. Brain Res., 91 1992

340–350.w x27 Jeffreys, D.A., Tukmachi, E.S.A. and Rockley, G. Evoked potential

evidence for human brain mechanisms that respond to single, fixatedŽ .faces, Exp. Brain Res., 91 1992 351–362.

w x28 Johnson, R.J., The amplitude of the P300 component of the event-re-lated potential: review and synthesis. In P.K. Ackles, J.R. Jennings

Ž .and M.G.H. Coles Eds. , AdÕances in Psychophysiology, Vol. 3,JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, 1988, pp. 69–138.

w x29 Kintsch, W., The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: aŽ .construction-integration model, Psychol. ReÕ., 95 1988 163–182.

w x30 Koyama, S., Nageishi, Y. and Shimokochi, M., Effects of semanticcontext and event-related potentials: N400 correlates with inhibition

Ž .effect, Brain Lang., 43 1992 668–681.w x31 Kutas, M., McCarthy, G. and Donchin, E., Augmenting mental

chronometry: the P300 as a measure of the stimulus evaluation time,Ž .Science, 197 1977 792–795.

w x32 Kutas, M. and Hillyard, S.A., Brain potentials during reading reflect( )word expectancy and semantic association, Nature London , 307

Ž .1984 161–163.w x33 Kutas, M., Lindamood, T.E. and Hillyard, S.A., Word expectancy

and event-related potentials during sentence processing. In S. Korn-Ž .blum and J. Requin Eds. , Preparatory States and Processes,

Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1984, pp. 217–237.w x34 Kutas, M. and Van Petten, C., Event-related brain potential studies

Ž .of language. In P.K. Ackles, J.R. Jennings and M.G.H. Coles Eds. ,AdÕances in Psychophysiology, 3, JAI Press, Greenwich, CT, 1988,pp. 139–187.

w x35 Lecoutre, B. and Poitevineau, J., Programme d’Analyse des Com-( )paraisons PAC , Groupe Mathematiques et Psychologie, CNRS-´

URA 1201, Universite Rene Descartes, Paris, France, 1991.´ ´w x36 Lu, S.T., Hamalainen, M.S., Hari, R., llmoneimi, R.J., Lounasmaa,

O.V., Sams, M. and Vilkman, V., Seeing faces activates threeseparate areas outside the occipital visual cortex in man, Neuro-

Ž .science, 43 1991 287–290.w x37 McClelland, J.L. and Rumelhart, D.E., An interactive model of

context effects in letter perception. Part 1. An account of basicŽ .findings, Psychol. ReÕ., 88 1981 375–407.

w x38 McClelland, J.L., The case for interactionism in language process-Ž .ing. In Coltheart Ed. , Attention and Performance XII: The Psy-

chology of Reading, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1987, pp. 3–36.w x39 Miltner, W., Johnson, R.J. and Braun, C., Auditory and somatosen-

sory event-related potentials: II. Effects of the inter-stimulus inter-Ž .val, J. Psychophysiol., 5 1991 27–42.

w x40 Norris, D., Word recognition: context effects without priming, Cog-Ž .nition, 22 1986 93–136.

w x41 Paap, K.R., Newsome, S.L., McDonald, J.E. and Schvanevelt, R.W.,An activation-verification model for letter and word recognition: the

Ž .word superiority effect, Psychol. ReÕ., 89 1982 573–594.w x42 Polich, J., Semantic categorization and event-related potentials, Brain

Ž .Lang., 26 1985 304–321.

w x43 Polich. J. and Donchin, E., P300 and the word frequency effect,Ž .Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol., 70 1988 33–45.

w x44 Polich, J., Probability and inter-stimulus interval effects on the P300Ž .from auditory stimuli, Int. J. Psychophysiol., 10 1990 163–170.

w x45 Potter, M.C., Moryadas, A., Abrams, I. and Noel, A., Word percep-tion and misperception in context, J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem.

Ž .Cogn., 19 1993 3–22.w x46 Pritchard, W.S., Shappell, S.A. and Brandt, M.E., Psychophysiology

of N200rN400: a review and classification scheme. In J.R. Jen-Ž .nings, P.K. Ackles and M.G.H. Coles Eds. , AdÕances in Psy-

chophysiology: A Research Annual, Vol. 4, Jessica Kingsley, Lon-don, UK, 1991, pp. 43–106.

w x47 Ritter, A., Simson, R. and Vaughan, H.G., Event-related potentialcorrelates of two stages of information processing in physical and

Ž .semantic discrimination task, Psychophysiology, 20 1983 168–179.w x48 Rugg, M.D., The effects of semantic priming and word repetition on

Ž .event related potentials. Psychophysiology, 22 1985 642–647.w x49 Rugg, M.D., Dissociation of semantic priming, word and non-word

repetition effects by event-related potentials, Q.J. Exp. Psychol.,Ž .39A 1987 123–148.

w x50 Rugg, M.D., Event-related potentials dissociate repetition effects ofŽ .high and low frequency words, Mem. Cogn., 18 1990 367–379.

w x51 Savin, H.B., Word frequency effects and errors in the perception ofŽ .speech, J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 35 1963 200–206.

w x52 Seek, M. and Grusser, J.O., Category-related components in visual¨evoked potentials: photographs of faces, persons, flowers and tools

Ž .as stimuli, Exp. Brain Res., 91 1992 338–349.w x53 Segui, J. and Grainger, J., Priming word recognition with ortho-

graphic neighbors: effects of relative prime-target frequency, J. Exp.Ž .Psychol. Human Percept. Perform., 16 1990 65–76.

w x54 Smith, M.E. and Halgren, E., Event-related potentials elicited byfamiliar and unfamiliar faces. In R. Johnson, J.W. Rohrbaugh and R.

Ž .Parasuraman Eds. , Current Trends in EÕent-Related Potential Re-search, EEG Suppl., 40, Elsevier, New York, NY, 1987.

w x55 Smith, M.E. and Halgren, E., Event-related potential during lexicaldecision: effects of repetition, word frequency, pronounceability, andconcreteness, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Suppl., 40Ž .1987 417–421.

w x56 Tipper, S.P. and Driver, J., Negative priming between pictures andwords in a selective attention task: evidence for semantic processing

Ž .of ignored stimuli, Mem. Cogn., 16 1988 64–70.w x57 Van Petten, C. and Kutas, M., Interaction between sentence context

and word frequency in event-related potentials, Mem. Cogn., 18Ž .1990 380–393.

w x58 Van Petten, C., Kutas, M., Kluender, R., Mitchiner, M. and McIsaac,H., Fractionating the word repetition effect with event-related poten-

Ž .tials, J. Cogn. Neurosci., 3 1991 131–150.w x59 Young, A.W., Hay, D.C. and Ellis, A.W., The faces that, launched a

thousand slips: everyday difficulties and errors in recognizing peo-Ž .ple, Br. J. Psychol., 76 1985 495–523.