The Role of Mixed Emotions in the Risk Perception of Novice and Serial Entrepreneurs

26
The Role of Mixed Emotions in the Risk Perception of Novice and Serial Entrepreneurs Ksenia Podoynitsyna Hans Van der Bij Michael Song This study examines the role of mixed emotions in the risk perception of entrepreneurs, an important determinant of entrepreneurial decision making. We extend the literature on mixed emotions by applying the cognitive appraisal tendency approach and contrasting it with ambivalence stemming from the valence-based approach. We test our hypotheses on a data set of 253 entrepreneurs from the United States. We show that mixed and conflicting emotions are an important predictor of the risk perception of entrepreneurs. At the same time, we find that emotional reactions of entrepreneurs on strategic issues change substan- tially as they found more ventures and become habitual entrepreneurs. Introduction Imagine how much easier life would be if we felt only one emotion—positive or negative—at a time. Luckily and unfortunately, this is often not the case. Entrepreneurs, in particular, are people who are likely to experience mixed rather than single basic emotions in their decision making, including their evaluation of opportunities and assess- ment of risks. In this study, we focus on the relationship between mixed emotions and the risk perception of entrepreneurs. We define entrepreneurs as individuals who recognize and exploit new business opportunities by founding new ventures (Baron, 2008; Shane & Venkataraman, 2000). Because opportunity recognition and evaluation are key phenomena of entrepreneurship (Alvarez & Barney, 2007; Shane & Venkataraman), it is important to understand what makes entrepreneurs pursue opportunities. Risk perception plays an important role in this context. Empirical research has demonstrated that risk perception strongly influences risk behavior in general (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992; Sitkin & Weingart, 1995), and opportunity evaluation and the decision to start a venture in particular (Keh, Foo, & Lim, 2002; Simon, Houghton, & Aquino, 2000). However, recent literature reviews have called for more Please send correspondence to: Ksenia Podoynitsyna, tel.: (40) 247-3640; e-mail: [email protected]. P T E & 1042-2587 © 2011 Baylor University 115 January, 2012 DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2011.00476.x

Transcript of The Role of Mixed Emotions in the Risk Perception of Novice and Serial Entrepreneurs

etap_476 115..140

The Role of MixedEmotions in theRisk Perception ofNovice and SerialEntrepreneursKsenia PodoynitsynaHans Van der BijMichael Song

This study examines the role of mixed emotions in the risk perception of entrepreneurs, animportant determinant of entrepreneurial decision making. We extend the literature on mixedemotions by applying the cognitive appraisal tendency approach and contrasting it withambivalence stemming from the valence-based approach. We test our hypotheses on a dataset of 253 entrepreneurs from the United States. We show that mixed and conflictingemotions are an important predictor of the risk perception of entrepreneurs. At the sametime, we find that emotional reactions of entrepreneurs on strategic issues change substan-tially as they found more ventures and become habitual entrepreneurs.

Introduction

Imagine how much easier life would be if we felt only one emotion—positive ornegative—at a time. Luckily and unfortunately, this is often not the case. Entrepreneurs,in particular, are people who are likely to experience mixed rather than single basicemotions in their decision making, including their evaluation of opportunities and assess-ment of risks. In this study, we focus on the relationship between mixed emotions and therisk perception of entrepreneurs.

We define entrepreneurs as individuals who recognize and exploit new businessopportunities by founding new ventures (Baron, 2008; Shane & Venkataraman, 2000).Because opportunity recognition and evaluation are key phenomena of entrepreneurship(Alvarez & Barney, 2007; Shane & Venkataraman), it is important to understand whatmakes entrepreneurs pursue opportunities. Risk perception plays an important role in thiscontext. Empirical research has demonstrated that risk perception strongly influences riskbehavior in general (Sitkin & Pablo, 1992; Sitkin & Weingart, 1995), and opportunityevaluation and the decision to start a venture in particular (Keh, Foo, & Lim, 2002; Simon,Houghton, & Aquino, 2000). However, recent literature reviews have called for more

Please send correspondence to: Ksenia Podoynitsyna, tel.: (40) 247-3640; e-mail: [email protected].

PTE &

1042-2587© 2011 Baylor University

115January, 2012DOI: 10.1111/j.1540-6520.2011.00476.x

research to refine our understanding of the role of entrepreneurial risk taking (Ireland &Webb, 2007). Until now, empirical research has emphasized risk-taking propensity(Forlani & Mullins, 2000; Mullins & Forlani, 2005) and cognitive biases (Keh et al.;Simon et al.) as determinants of entrepreneurial risk perception. A recent study by Foo(2011) finds that certain background emotions can also influence risk perception. Indeed,because entrepreneurs tend to rely heavily on their intuition in making decisions (e.g.,Simon et al.) and emotions are one of the main drivers of intuitive thinking (Epstein, 1994;Kahneman, 2003), emotions are meant to play a key role in entrepreneurial decisionmaking.

Entrepreneurs likely experience mixed rather than single basic emotions in theirdecision making. Similar to the executives of established firms, entrepreneurs often dealwith ambiguous situations (Eisenhardt & Sull, 2001; Walsh, Meyer, & Schoonhoven,2006). They operate in environments that are highly unpredictable and filled with rapidchange (Lichtenstein, Dooley, & Lumpkin, 2006), while entrepreneurial tasks are highlyvaried in nature and change significantly as the entrepreneurial process unfolds (Baron,2006). When performing variable tasks in highly uncertain and unpredictable environ-ments, an entrepreneur’s affect may play an important role in judgment and decisionmaking (Baron, 2008). Moreover, the greater the ambiguity in their working environment,the more likely that entrepreneurs will experience different emotions at the same time(Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Larsen, McGraw, Mellers, & Cacioppo, 2004). Thus, study-ing the relationship between mixed emotions and risk perception can provide valuableinsights for entrepreneurs.

The extant research on mixed emotions has concentrated on the simultaneous expe-rience of positive and negative affect (Folkman & Lazarus, 1985; Fong, 2006; Fong &Tiedens, 2002; Larsen, McGraw, & Cacioppo, 2001; Larsen et al., 2004; Priester & Petty,1996, 2001; Schwarz & Weinberger, 1980). We will build on research examining thecognitive appraisal tendency approach to emotions and contrast it with the valence-basedapproach. The former approach focuses on the differences between emotions of the samevalence, which can be attributed to various cognitive appraisal patterns associated withthese emotions, such as uncertainty and controllability, among others (Keltner, Ellsworth,& Edwards, 1993; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, 1987). Forexample, happiness and hope are two positive emotions, but happiness tends to beappraised as certain and controllable while hope tends to be appraised as uncertain anduncontrollable. Following the logic of these two approaches to affect, mixed emotionsmay emanate from the simultaneous experience of positive and negative emotions(ambivalence) or from the coexistence of emotions with different associated cognitiveappraisal patterns.

In this study, we examine 16 emotion adjectives, half of which have a positive valence(related to the basic emotions of happiness and hope) and half of which have a negativevalence (related to the basic emotions of anger and fear). The emotion adjectives wereselected in such a way that half of them also have a positive association with certainty andcontrollability cognitive appraisal dimensions (related to the happiness and anger basicemotions), and the other half have a negative association with these dimensions (related tothe hope and fear basic emotions). These dimensions are shown to mediate the relation-ship between emotions and risk estimates (Lerner & Keltner, 2001).

This study contributes to the literature by (1) conceptualizing mixed emotions usingthe cognitive appraisal tendency approach, (2) contrasting this approach with the mixedemotions conceptualization based on the valence-based approach on the example of riskperception of entrepreneurs, and (3) exploring the moderating effect of experience infounding ventures on these relationships.

116 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

Theoretical Framework and Hypotheses

Entrepreneurial Risk Perception and its DriversEntrepreneurial risk perception is defined as the entrepreneur’s assessment of the risk

inherent in a situation. This definition is consistent with previous work of Dutton andJackson (1987), Jackson and Dutton (1988), Sitkin and Pablo (1992), and Sitkin andWeingart (1995). A situation is considered risky when it is more negative than positive,there is more potential for loss than for gain, and it is seen more as a threat than as anopportunity (Highhouse, Paese, & Leatherberry, 1996; MacGrimmon & Wehrung, 1985;Sitkin & Weingart).

In the entrepreneurship and management literatures, several individual and organiza-tional drivers of risk perception have been proposed and tested. Sitkin and Pablo (1992)suggest that individuals’ risk propensity will mediate the relationship between their riskpreference, inertia, and outcome history and their risk perception. Moreover, theseresearchers suggest that organizational characteristics like control systems, problemdomain familiarity, social influence, top management team homogeneity, and problemframing can all influence an individual’s risk perception. Sitkin and Weingart (1995) alsofind empirical support for the mediation effect of risk propensity on the outcome history–risk perception relationship and the direct effect of problem framing on risk perception.Forlani and Mullins (2000) also find an effect of risk propensity on risk perception. At thesame time, Mullins and Forlani (2005) find that risk propensity influenced entrepreneurs’choices related to the probability of gain and loss, while risk perception influencedentrepreneurs’ choices related to the magnitude of gains and losses. Simon et al. (2000)and Keh et al. (2002) examine the role of cognitive biases in entrepreneurial risk percep-tion. Illusion of control and belief in the law of small numbers were found to be negativelyassociated with risk perception.

In considering affect as another antecedent of risk perception, Johnson and Tversky(1983) find that the experience of negative affect equally inflated individuals’ estimatesfor all types of negative events. Mayer, Gaschke, Braverman, and Evans (1992) findsimilar results. We refer to the review of Blanchette and Richards (2010) for an in-depthdiscussion of these two and related studies. Finally, Foo (2011) finds that the experienceof four induced emotions (anger, fear, happiness, and hope) unrelated to the venturedecision had a differential influence on students’ risk perceptions of entrepreneurialventure scenarios. Foo also finds a positive relationship between dispositional happinessand anger (i.e., happiness and anger as traits) and the risk-taking propensity of entrepre-neurs. However, despite the likelihood that entrepreneurs experience mixed emotions dueto the variable and uncertain nature of their environment (e.g., Lichtenstein et al., 2006),to date, no study has examined the relationship between mixed emotions and risk percep-tion of entrepreneurs. In our study we will focus on this link.

Valence-Based and Cognitive Appraisal Tendency ApproachesMost studies on entrepreneurship that focus on emotions and affect follow the

valence-based approach by distinguishing between positive and negative affect. Forinstance, Baron (1998, 2008) conceptually clarifies the role of positive and negativeaffect in basic cognitive processes of entrepreneurs and the potential influences on keyaspects of the entrepreneurial process. Foo, Uy, and Baron (2009) examine conse-quences of positive and negative affect and find that they both impact venture effort.Brundin, Patzelt, and Shepherd (2008) show that entrepreneurs’ displays of positiveand negative emotions influence their employees’ willingness to act entrepreneurially.

117January, 2012

Shepherd and Cardon (2009) propose that negative emotional reactions due to projectfailure impact learning from the failure and the motivation to try again. Cardon,Zietsma, Saparito, Matherne, and Davis (2005) and Cardon, Wincent, Singh, andDrnovsek (2009) explain the concept of entrepreneurial passion by using a parenthoodmetaphor in the entrepreneurial process and by linking entrepreneurial passion to threerole identities (inventing, founding, and developing) and conceptually exploring theeffects of entrepreneurial passion on goal-related cognitions, entrepreneurial behavior,and entrepreneurial effectiveness.

The cognitive appraisal tendency approach represents a viable and promising alter-native to the valence-based approach. It focuses on the differences between the emo-tions of the same valence due to dissimilar cognitive appraisals. Smith and Ellsworth(1985) summarize the patterns of appraisal for a range of emotions. Keltner et al.(1993) confirm that not only the valence of an affect, but also its associated cognitiveappraisal patterns have an important influence on causal judgments. In particular, theyfind a dissimilar impact of sadness and anger. Lerner and Keltner (2000, 2001) elabo-rate on these results and present an appraisal tendency framework, in which eachemotion is defined by a tendency to perceive new events and objects in ways that areconsistent with the original cognitive appraisal dimensions of the emotion. They findthat fearful people made risk-averse choices, while angry people made risk-seekingchoices. Moreover, risk judgments of angry people more closely resemble those ofhappy people than those of fearful people. Lerner and Tiedens (2006) elaborate furtheron the differences between anger and other emotions and depict the anticipatory plea-sure of anger that triggers the positive subjective sense. Foo’s (2011) research alsosupports the appraisal tendency approach.

Both the valence-based approach and the cognitive appraisal tendency approach canbe used to study mixed emotions, which are the focus of this study. We build further onthese two streams of research by examining mixed emotions as a potential antecedent ofentrepreneurial risk perception. In order to build our hypotheses, we use the affectinfusion model (AIM; Forgas, 1995, 1998) describing the mechanisms of how affect mayinfuse judgments.

Mechanisms Explaining the Role of Affect in Judgment andDecision Making

In this study, we define affect as a generic label to refer to both moods and emotions.Moods are low-intensity, diffuse, and relatively enduring affective states without a clearantecedent cause and therefore little cognitive content. Emotions are more intense, shortlived, and usually have a definite cause and a clear cognitive content (Forgas, 1995).Researchers on basic (as opposed to mixed) emotions have embraced the AIM depictinga set of mechanisms explaining the influence of moods and emotions on judgment anddecision making (e.g., Baron, 1998, 2008; Forgas, 1995, 1998). The AIM distinguishesbetween judgment processes where a relatively closed information search is required andprocesses where a much more open information search is needed. In general, entrepre-neurs are involved in the latter processes. These processes have a higher probability ofaffect infusion, that is, affectively loaded information exerts an influence on and becomesincorporated into the judgment process, entering into the judgmental deliberations andeventually coloring the judgment outcome (Forgas, 1995). The affect infusion worksthrough two different, but complementary mechanisms: affect as information and affectpriming.

118 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

The affect-as-information mechanism describes direct affect infusion: Instead ofmaking a judgment on the basis of recalling features of a target, the individual askshimself how he feels about it. So, in fact the affect itself is the information. In doing so,individuals may mistake feelings due to a preexisting state for a reaction to the target(Forgas, 1995). DeSteno, Petty, Wegener, and Rucker (2000) find empirical support for theaffect-as-information mechanism in making likelihood estimates of future events. Loe-wenstein, Weber, Hsee, and Welch (2001) show that emotional reactions to risky situationsoften diverge from cognitive assessments of those risks.

The affect-priming mechanism suggests that affect can indirectly inform social judg-ments by priming the encoding, retrieval, and selective use of information in the judgmentprocess. Affect priming can be enacted through four submechanisms. The first one isselective attention, which means that given the overload of information in a judgmentprocess and the necessity of making selections, particular information is selected that iscongruent with the current affect. Second, in the process of encoding new information,more attention may be given to the affect-congruent information. Third, most judgmentprocesses involve the retrieval and use of prior knowledge structures to interpret incominginformation. Affect can selectively influence these retrieval processes, because affect-congruent information may have a greater likelihood of being retrieved (Forgas, 1995).Forgas also finds empirical support for the affect-priming mechanism in complex bar-gaining tasks (Forgas, 1998).

HypothesesWe first focus on mixed emotions with dissimilar valences. In general, emotion

adjectives that are associated with the basic emotions of happiness and hope are con-sidered to be positive, while emotion adjectives associated with the basic emotions ofanger and fear are considered to be negative (see, e.g., Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001).Following the literature on attitudinal ambivalence (Fong, 2006; Fong & Tiedens, 2002;Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001), we distinguish between dominant emotions and conflict-ing emotions for each entrepreneur. Dominant emotions are those positive or negativeemotions that are experienced in greater numbers, while conflicting emotions are thosethat are experienced in lesser numbers. For example, when an entrepreneur experiencesfive positive and two negative emotions, the five positive emotions are dominant and thetwo negative emotions are conflicting. Another entrepreneur, experiencing three positiveand four negative emotions, has four dominant (negative) emotions and three conflicting(positive) emotions.

A prominent argument in the mixed emotions literature (Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001)is that the conflicting (rather than dominant) emotions make the difference in cases ofmixed emotions. Priester and Petty (1996) and Priester, Petty, and Park (2007) suggest thegradual threshold model (GTM) as the most appropriate formula to calculate mixedemotions (i.e., ambivalence) on the basis of particular reactions of the respondents. Belowa certain threshold, ambivalence is a function of both dominant and conflicting reactions.Above that threshold, ambivalence is a positive and negatively accelerating function of theconflicting reactions. That is, although feelings of ambivalence generally increase asconflicting reactions increase, the initial conflicting reactions produce a greater increase inambivalence than subsequent conflicting reactions. In the GTM, the threshold graduallyemerges as conflicting reactions become more numerous (Priester & Petty, 1996; Priesteret al.). Furthermore, it does not matter whether conflicting reactions are positive ornegative (Priester et al.)—thus, for some entrepreneurs the conflicting reactions (i.e.,emotions) will be positive, for others negative.

119January, 2012

We also consider emotions based on different associated cognitive appraisal pat-terns, in accord with the cognitive appraisal tendency approach (Keltner et al., 1993;Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985). We assume that each emotionactivates a cognitive predisposition appraising future events in line with the centralappraisal dimensions that triggered the emotion (Lerner & Keltner, 2000). In this view,there is a strong relation between how a person interprets a situation along certainemotion-relevant dimensions and what the person feels and how the person appraisesfuture events (Smith & Ellsworth, 1987). These dimensions include (1) pleasantness—the extent to which the situation is perceived as being pleasant or unpleasant; (2) antici-pated effort—the extent to which the person feels a need to expend effort (eitherphysical or mental) in the situation; (3) attentional activity—the extent to which theperson wants to attend to or shut out the situation; (4) certainty—the extent to which theperson understands or is sure of what is happening or going to happen in the situation;(5) responsibility—who or what the person perceives as having brought about the situ-ation, either oneself or someone or something else; (6) control—who or what the personperceives as currently being in control of the situation, either oneself, some other personor persons, or uncontrollable circumstances; (7) legitimacy—the extent to which thesituation is perceived as fair or unfair; and (8) perceived obstacle—the extent to whichthe person perceives problems or obstacles in the situation that are hampering the attain-ment of an desired goal. In line with Lerner and Keltner (2000, 2001) and Foo (2011),we focus on two of these dimensions, certainty and control, which mediate the rela-tionship between emotions and risk perception (Lerner & Keltner, 2001) and are thus ofgreat importance in the entrepreneurial setting (Baron, 1998). In general, emotion adjec-tives that are associated with the basic emotions of happiness and anger score high onthe control and certainty dimension, while adjectives associated with hope and fear havea low score on control and certainty (Smith & Ellsworth). For instance, fear is triggeredby the perception that negative events are unpredictable and uncontrollable (Foo; Lerner& Keltner, 2000, 2001).

The ambivalence literature is quite neutral to what constitutes conflicting and domi-nant reactions to the researched issues (Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001). Although thevalence-based approach to emotions can be more easily fit into the ambivalence concep-tualization (e.g., Fong, 2006; Fong & Tiedens, 2002), one can also distinguish dominantand conflicting reactions on the basis of the cognitive appraisal approach. If, for anentrepreneur, the number of experienced emotional adjectives that score highly on cer-tainty and controllability exceeds the number of experienced adjectives that have a lowscore on certainty and controllability, we call the former emotions dominant and the latterconflicting (and vice versa). The arguments from the ambivalence literature regarding theprominence of conflicting emotions influence should also hold in case of cognitiveappraisals approach (Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001).

Following the AIM, the effect of conflicting emotions on judgments in general andrisk perception in particular works through two complementary mechanisms. First, withdirect affect-as-information mechanism, where feelings are used as if it was informationabout the issue in question, as the level of conflicting emotions regarding a certain issueincreases, entrepreneurs will experience this odd mixture of emotions more strongly andtherefore recognize it more easily. As a result, entrepreneurs will more likely experiencethe focal issue as unusual and feel uncomfortable about it (Fong, 2006). Feeling uncom-fortable will lead to a cautious, conservative judgment on the issue, resulting in a higherperception of risk.

With the indirect affect-priming mechanism, where feelings determine the typeof information used to assess a certain issue, the more entrepreneurs experience and

120 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

recognize conflicting emotions, the more they will be sensitive to unusual associations(Fong, 2006). Facing an overload of information in the judgment process, entrepreneursimplicitly or explicitly have to make choices regarding what information to retrieve,encode, and use. This may lead to overly simplistic analyses of complex situations,directing attention to a limited set of variables and exclusion of other important variables(Simon & Houghton, 2002). Moreover, it often leads to the retrieval of success stories andpositive information, neglecting failures and negative information (Golder & Tellis, 1993;Simon et al., 2000). Normally, such selection will result in too-optimistic judgments anda lower perception of risk. However, when entrepreneurs become more sensitive tounusual associations, they are more likely to take more variables into account and accessthe less-obvious knowledge structures, thus enriching the information search process(Fong). This will lead to a richer set of associations and interpretations including bothpositive/success stories and negative/failure stories. Therefore, mixed emotions are likelyto be associated with a higher perception of risk.

Both mechanisms indicate that an increase in the level of conflicting emotions willlead to a higher perception of entrepreneurial risk. Moreover, experiencing conflictingemotions is associated with a feeling of indecisiveness (Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001). Thehigher the level of indecisiveness, the greater the level of risk entrepreneurs are likely toperceive. Therefore, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 1a: Conflicting emotions in terms of valences (i.e., positive vs. negativeemotions) will have a stronger association with the risk perception of entrepreneursthan dominant emotions in terms of valences will.Hypothesis 1b: Conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisal tendencies (i.e.,certain and controllable vs. uncertain and uncontrollable) will have a stronger asso-ciation with the risk perception of entrepreneurs than dominant emotions in terms ofcognitive appraisals will.

Although the concepts of dominant and conflicting emotions can be applied to bothvalence-based and cognitive appraisal tendency approaches, there are numerous argu-ments suggesting the superiority of the latter approach in explaining the connectionbetween emotions and risk perception. First, researchers note that the mere ability offeeling distinct emotions should result in their differential influence on many cognitiveand motivational processes (cf. DeSteno, Petty, Rucker, Wegener, & Braverman, 2004).Second, because the occurrence of a certain type of event roughly predicts the occurrenceof other events of the same type and because particular specific emotions mark suchcategories of events, the impact of affect is likely to be emotion specific rather thanvalence specific (DeSteno et al., 2000; Fessler, Pillsworth, & Flamson, 2004). It isunlikely that emotions sharing the same valence would necessarily have the same effect onrisk taking because valence is not uniformly associated with different classes of functionalgoals (Fessler et al.). Studies show that different emotions of the same valence can havecompletely opposite effects in a variety of decision-making situations and that emotionswith the same cognitive appraisal tendency are more alike than emotions of the samevalence (e.g., DeSteno et al.; Fessler et al.; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Raghunathan &Pham, 1999). Likewise, dissimilarity in terms of cognitive appraisal tendency should bemore influential than dissimilarity in terms of valence. For risk judgments, certainty andcontrollability appraisals are shown to be most essential (Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001).Because these two appraisal tendencies produce opposing effects on risk judgments (e.g.,Foo, 2011), conflicting emotions with contrasting certainty and controllability should alsoproduce a greater feeling of discomfort and irregularity than conflicting emotions basedon contrasting valences. Once again, feelinguncomfortable will lead to a cautious,

121January, 2012

conservative judgment on the issue, resulting in a higher perception of risk. Therefore, wehypothesize:

Hypothesis 2: Conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisal tendencies (i.e.,certain and controllable vs. uncertain and uncontrollable) will have a stronger asso-ciation with the risk perception of entrepreneurs than conflicting emotions in terms ofvalences (i.e., positive vs. negative emotions) will.

Promoting entrepreneurship as the driver of economic growth aims to increase thenumber of entrepreneurs founding new ventures. Having past experience as an entrepre-neur can increase the probability of actually transitioning from paid employment toentrepreneurship by 12%, while serial entrepreneurs can account for as many as 30% ofsuch transitions (Hyytinen and Ilmakunnas, 2007). Serial (also referred to as habitual)entrepreneurs therefore represent a very valuable population from a policy perspective(Wiklund & Shepherd, 2008). Moreover, serial entrepreneurs can play an important rolein promoting economic growth as they can potentially build upon their experience andstart more successful ventures. One of the executive forums of the Journal of BusinessVenturing (1986) was called “To really learn about entrepreneurship, let’s study habitualentrepreneurs,” which accurately reflects the relevance of the study of serial entrepreneursto practitioners and the popularity of success stories of serial entrepreneurs.

As entrepreneurs found more new ventures and gain more experience in creating andrunning new businesses, their intuition develops as well (Mitchell, Friga, & Mitchell,2005). Although entrepreneurs’ newly accumulated insights and expert knowledge struc-tures are pillars supporting intuition (Mitchell et al.; Shane, 2000), emotions are also oneof the characteristic features of intuition (Epstein, 1994; Kahneman, 2003). Moreover,intuition is often depicted as being primarily nonverbal and intimately associated withaffect (Denes-Raj & Epstein, 1994). Consequently, as serial entrepreneurs tend to relymore on intuition in their judgments, they are also more likely to engage emotions in theirjudgments. Accordingly, both valence-based and cognitive appraisal tendency perspec-tives on emotions should play greater roles in the judgment processes of more experi-enced, serial entrepreneurs. Thus, we hypothesize:

Hypothesis 3a: Conflicting emotions in terms of valences (i.e., positive vs. negativeemotions) will play a greater role in risk judgments of serial entrepreneurs than in riskjudgments of novice entrepreneurs.Hypothesis 3b: Conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisal tendencies (i.e.,certain and controllable vs. uncertain and uncontrollable) will play a greater role inrisk judgments of serial entrepreneurs than in risk judgments of novice entrepreneurs.

Control VariablesIn this study, we use the basic emotions of anger, fear, happiness, and hope as control

variables. On the basis of past research, we expect these emotions to be associated withentrepreneurial risk perception. However, the direction of the associations is not clear. Thevalence-based approach would predict a positive association of anger and fear withentrepreneurial risk perception and a negative association of happiness and hopewith entrepreneurial risk perception (Johnson & Tversky, 1983). The cognitive appraisaltendency approach would generally predict a positive association of fear and hope withentrepreneurial risk perception and a negative association of anger and happinesswith entrepreneurial risk perception (Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001). At the same time, not

122 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

all of the cognitive appraisal dimensions show a stable link between emotions and theirappraisal tendencies, making these predictions preliminary (Smith & Ellsworth, 1987;Tong et al., 2009). Another factor making these predictions more difficult is the nature ofour sample. We also control for entrepreneurs who are familiar with the case we presentin the study and their entrepreneurial experience.

Method

Sample and Data CollectionThe participants for this study were drawn from the list of 6,359 founders of venture-

backed firms provided by VentureOne, a leading venture capital (VC) research companybased in San Francisco. VentureOne began tracking equity investment in 1992. It collectsdata by surveying VC firms for recent funding activities and portfolio updates, gatheringinformation through direct contacts at venture-backed companies, and investigatingvarious secondary resources such as company press releases and initial public offeringprospectuses from VentureOne 2001. We randomly selected 1,100 entrepreneurs withcomplete contact information for the study.

In administering the scenario and the related questionnaire, we followed the totaldesign method for survey research (Dillman, 1978). The first mailing packet included apersonalized letter, a project fact sheet, the survey, a priority postage-paid envelope withan individually typed return-address label, and a list of research reports available toparticipants. The package was sent by priority mail to each randomly selected entrepre-neur. As a result of some packages being undeliverable for name or address reasons, theadjusted sample comprised 776 entrepreneurs.

To increase the response rate, we sent four follow-up mailings to the companies. Oneweek after the mailing, we sent a follow-up letter. Two weeks after the first follow-up, wesent a second package with the same content as the first package to all nonrespondingcompanies. After two additional follow-up letters, we received completed questionnairesfrom 255 entrepreneurs, representing a response rate of 32.9%. Two respondents indicatedthat they had no active experience as venture founders and were omitted from the analysis.In our final sample, 181 were male and 72 were female. The highest degree obtained for46 of the entrepreneurs was high school; for 124, bachelor degree; for 74, master’s degree;and for 9, doctoral degree. The mean age of the respondents was 42.4 years. On average,they founded 4.4 ventures, were currently involved in 2.1 ventures, and had 14.5 years ofentrepreneurial experience.

MeasurementsIn our study, we used existing cases and scales from the literature. We conducted a

pretest by extensively interviewing 12 entrepreneurs. At the beginning of each interview,entrepreneurs told us about the background of their ventures, how they started, how theydiscovered the opportunity, and how the business idea developed over time. This allowedus to get the conversation started and better interpret their answers on the questionnaire.In the last part of the interview, we used the protocol method and asked the entrepreneursto “think aloud” as they filled out the questionnaire (Hunt, Sparkman, & Wilcox, 1982).The interviews were recorded and two researchers made careful notes of the verbaliza-tions and the thinking process of the entrepreneurs. The analysis of interviews led to minorchanges in the wording of the instructions and cases description.

123January, 2012

For the sake of ecological validity of our results, we chose to center measures of allthe variables in our study around a strategic issue, signifying an important environmentalevent, trend, or development for which future resolution will be sought (Dutton &Jackson, 1987). The selected issue was extensively pretested in earlier studies to be surethat it represents an ambiguous event that could be interpreted both as a positive and as anegative change in the competitive environment (Highhouse et al., 1996). As a result, themeaning of the strategic issue is not inherent in the external environment and it is up tofirms’ decision makers to interpret the event (Dutton & Jackson). Meanings attached tostrategic issues color the subsequent affective and cognitive decision processes of thedecision makers. This ambiguity allowed us to maximize the chance that entrepreneurs inour sample will experience mixed emotions, the focus of our study (Folkman & Lazarus,1985; Larsen et al., 2004).

The strategic issue we chose was originally used by Dutton and Jackson (1987) as ananecdotal example that everybody can easily relate to. Considering the potential differ-ences in the backgrounds of entrepreneurs, this seems to be the most appropriate choice.Highhouse et al. (1996) adapted the issue in their study of strategic-issue framing. In ourstudy, we used the version found in Highhouse et al. It reads as follows:

Imagine that you are the owner of a retail clothing store. You have historicallyprospered by occupying a choice downtown location. You have just found out thatplans are underway to construct a large suburban shopping mall ten miles from yourcurrent place of business. (p. 97)

Highhouse et al. (1996) found that the description above is a neutral description of thestrategic issue; it is neither representing a positive situation nor a negative one. Thus,positive or negative assessments of this situation are interpretations of the decisionmakers.

Dependent Variable. To measure risk perception, we asked the respondents to character-ize on 7-point scales whether they saw the strategic issue as an opportunity or as a threatand as having a potential for loss or a potential for gain. We also asked whether theycharacterized the issue as a positive or a negative situation (Jackson & Dutton, 1988;MacGrimmon & Wehrung, 1985; Sitkin & Weingart, 1995). Cronbach’s a for the riskperception construct is 0.81.

Independent Variables. To objectively assess mixed emotions (Priester & Petty, 1996),respondents were asked to rate on 7-point scales the extent to which each of 16 emotionadjectives described how they were feeling about the strategic issue, allowing for naturallyoccurring emotions. This approach differs from the priming technique to make the par-ticipants feel a certain emotion that is unrelated to the specific issue involved (e.g., Foo,2011; Lerner & Keltner, 2001). Sixteen emotion descriptors were selected from a list of25 adjectives presented by Smith and Ellsworth (1987); eight positive and eight negativefeelings were selected to allow for contrasting the valence-based and cognitive appraisaltendency approaches. Also, eight of the 16 adjectives were associated with high certaintyand controllability appraisals and eight were associated with relatively low certainty andcontrollability appraisals (Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, 1987).Table 1 summarizes the valences and dimensions for each emotion adjective.

By employing a mean split, we determined whether a particular emotion adjective wasexperienced (1) or not (0). This served as the basis for calculating the mixed emotionsmeasures for both the valence-based and cognitive appraisal tendency approaches.Because the ambivalence literature shows that conflicting emotions drive ambivalence, we

124 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

first computed the separate dominant and conflicting emotions scores in order to test theirrelative importance for entrepreneurs. For the valence-based approach, we calculated thedominant emotions for each respondent on the basis of positive and negative valence bytaking the greater of either the sum of experienced positive adjectives or the sum ofexperienced negative adjectives (Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001). For the cognitive appraisaltendency approach, we calculated dominant emotions associated with dissimilar cognitiveappraisals by taking the greater of either the sum of experienced emotions with highcertainty and controllability appraisals or the sum of experienced emotions with lowcertainty and controllability appraisals. For conflicting emotions, we followed the sameprocedure, but took the lesser instead of the greater of the sums of experienced emotions(Priester & Petty, 1996, 2001).

In order to calculate mixed emotions for both approaches, we used the GTM formulasuggested by Priester and Petty (1996) as the most appropriate measure of ambivalence:

5C Dp 1/C−

where C is equal to the magnitude of conflicting reactions, D is equal to the magnitude ofdominant reactions, p is less than 1 (0.5 in the present instance), and a constant of 1 isadded to C and D (Priester & Petty, 2001).

Control Variables. The findings of Smith and Ellsworth (1987) demonstrate that the 16emotion adjectives correspond to four basic emotions: anger, fear, happiness, and hope.Anger was depicted by such adjectives as resentful, angry, surprised, and scornful; fearby nervous, afraid, restless, and anxious; happiness by elated, happy, amused, and proud;and hope by expectant, hopeful, challenged, and interested. After the exploratory factoranalysis we dropped the adjective “anxious” from the fear construct and the adjective“interested” from the hope construct due to their cross loadings. On the basis of the

Table 1

Characteristics of 16 Emotional Adjectives

Emotionaladjective Valence

Certaintyappraisal

Controllabilityappraisal

Afraid Negative Low LowAmused Positive High HighAngry Negative High HighAnxious Negative Low LowChallenged Positive Low LowElated Positive High HighExpectant Positive Low LowHappy Positive High HighHopeful Positive Low LowInterested Positive Low LowNervous Negative Low LowProud Positive High HighResentful Negative High HighRestless Negative Low LowScornful Negative High HighSurprised Negative High High

125January, 2012

remaining adjectives, Cronbach’s a for anger, fear, happiness, and hope were 0.83, 0.83,0.79, and 0.78, respectively, suggesting good reliabilities (Nunnally, 1978). Finally,although emotions are one of the characteristic features of intuition (Epstein, 1994;Kahneman, 2003), entrepreneurial intuition can also be based on prior knowledge andexpert knowledge structures (Mitchell et al., 2005; Shane, 2000). We thus added theentrepreneurs’ familiarity with the strategic issue and number of ventures founded ascontrols to our model in order to distinguish knowledge-based effects from our findings.We measured familiarity by the extent to which entrepreneurs could relate the strategicissue to one of the decisions they had made in the past. Number of ventures founded wasmeasured by the number of ventures in which the entrepreneurs were actively involved asa founder. Finally, we added the common control variables of age, gender, type, and levelof education.

Analysis and Results

Prior to testing the hypotheses, we examined the correlation matrix, shown inTable 2. The measures of mixed, conflicting, and dominant emotions appear to behighly correlated (0.74–0.96), so we do not include any two of these variables in thesame regression model. The correlations between these types of emotions and basicemotions are also quite large, although lower (i.e., ranging between 0.47 and 0.70 withthe majority around 0.60). In models combining basic and mixed emotions, we mean-centered all emotion variables in order to eliminate the nonessential multicollinearitydue to the computation formula of mixed emotions components (Aiken & West, 1991;Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003; Kenny & Judd, 1984). An application of Belsley,Kuh, and Welsch (1980) diagnostic tests indicated no serious multicollinearity problemsin the mean-centered regression models (maximum variance inflation factor was5.5 < 0; maximum condition index was 22.3 < 30, while the proportion of varianceinflated for emotions did not exceed 0.05 < 0.50). However, because the maximum vari-ance inflation factor does not exceed 1.83 in other models, caution is necessary wheninterpreting the results when both basic emotions and mixed emotions are entered intothe same models.

To test the hypothesized relationships, we performed hierarchical regression analysis(Cohen & Cohen, 1983; Cohen et al., 2003). This method examines the effects of addi-tional variables above and beyond the effects of the variables in the previous model. InTable 3, we present a summary of the results of eight regression models.

All the regression models were highly significant, with F-values ranging between 4.29and 8.71 (p < .001). In the first baseline model, we only examined the effects of the controlvariables on entrepreneurial risk perception. In model 2, we added the basic emotionsbased on positive and negative valence. In model 3, we added the mixed emotions basedon positive and negative valence. In models 4 and 5, we split the mixed emotions frommodel 2 into dominant and conflicting emotions in order to determine their relative effecton mixed emotions. In model 6, we added the mixed emotions associated with differentcognitive appraisals. Finally, in models 7 and 8, we split the mixed emotions from model6 into dominant and conflicting emotions in order to determine their relative effect onmixed emotions. The results from the hierarchical regressions suggest that all modelsrepresent significant improvements over model 1.

We also conducted regressions by adding the basic emotions to models 3 to 8 (notreported in Table 3). Only mixed and conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisaltendencies remained significant with a coefficient (standard error) of 0.065 (0.035),

126 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

Tabl

e2

Des

crip

tive

Stat

istic

s(N

=25

3)

Con

stru

cts

Mea

nSD

12

34

56

78

910

1112

1314

1516

1.R

isk

perc

eptio

n4.

221.

332.

Mix

ed(V

al.)

7.87

3.60

0.26

3.C

onfli

ctin

g(V

al.)

3.11

2.32

0.33

0.96

4.D

omin

ant

(Val

.)4.

892.

300.

350.

740.

825.

Mix

ed(C

ogn.

App

r.)7.

933.

530.

370.

780.

840.

826.

Con

flict

ing

(Cog

n.A

ppr.)

3.13

2.33

0.41

0.83

0.91

0.88

0.96

7.D

omin

ant

(Cog

n.A

ppr.)

4.87

2.35

0.26

0.85

0.89

0.91

0.68

0.77

8.A

nger

4.33

1.25

0.22

0.65

0.67

0.64

0.60

0.65

0.64

9.Fe

ar4.

531.

390.

260.

690.

700.

640.

620.

650.

670.

4910

.H

appy

3.68

1.48

0.43

0.47

0.56

0.57

0.56

0.61

0.50

0.32

0.32

11.

Hop

e4.

931.

140.

070.

610.

650.

610.

510.

590.

660.

430.

410.

2112

.A

ge42

.38

12.7

60.

020.

020.

040.

010.

010.

040.

020.

050.

020.

070.

0313

.B

usin

ess

degr

ee0.

600.

490.

060.

240.

290.

300.

230.

270.

310.

130.

160.

080.

350.

0114

.D

egre

ele

vel

2.18

0.77

-0.0

20.

120.

150.

140.

090.

120.

160.

090.

120.

000.

200.

090.

2015

.Fa

mili

arity

with

issu

e5.

221.

570.

070.

140.

180.

210.

150.

190.

190.

080.

15-0

.07

0.20

-0.0

60.

270.

0116

.Fe

mal

een

trep

rene

ur1.

280.

45-0

.11

0.09

0.10

0.03

0.02

0.06

0.07

0.00

0.02

0.03

-0.0

20.

050.

020.

010.

1017

.N

umbe

rof

vent

ures

4.40

2.82

0.26

0.47

0.54

0.51

0.45

0.52

0.51

0.38

0.46

0.24

0.47

0.04

0.48

0.24

0.29

0.07

Not

e:C

orre

latio

nsof

|0.1

3|an

dab

ove

are

sign

ifica

ntat

p<

.05;

corr

elat

ions

of|0

.17|

and

abov

ear

esi

gnifi

cant

atp

<.0

1.

127January, 2012

p = 0.0643, for mixed emotions and a coefficient (standard error) of 0.199 (0.065),p = 0.0026, for conflicting emotions. Mixed and conflicting emotions in terms of valencebecame insignificant after adding the basic emotions. As a result, hypotheses 1b and 2were supported while hypothesis 1a was not.

In all models, familiarity with the strategic issue is insignificant, while number ofventures founded was positive and significant. Being a female entrepreneur has a negative

Table 3

Hierarchical Regression Results for Entrepreneurial Risk Perception (N = 253)a

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Model 8

Intercept 4.317*** 3.229*** 3.993*** 4.257*** 3.875*** 3.676*** 4.183*** 4.072***(0.486) (0.562) (0.493) (0.473) (0.479) (0.485) (0.459) (0.488)

Anger 0.048(0.072)

Fear 0.084(0.067)

Happiness 0.350***(0.055)

Hope -0.181*(0.080)

Age 0.002 -0.000 0.002 0.001 0.002 0.002 0.001 0.002(0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006) (0.006)

Business degree -0.203 -0.110 -0.214 -0.232 -0.253 -0.213 -0.223 -0.244(0.190) (0.176) (0.187) (0.185) (0.183) (0.182) (0.179) (0.188)

Education level -0.139 -0.066 -0.142 -0.145 -0.150 -0.128 -0.139 -0.150(0.110) (0.101) (0.108) (0.107) (0.106) (0.105) (0.103) (0.108)

Familiarity 0.011 0.066 0.012 0.007 -0.005 0.004 -0.003 0.006(0.055) (0.051) (0.054) (0.053) (0.053) (0.053) (0.052) (0.054)

Female entrepreneur -0.373* -0.418* -0.409* -0.421* -0.368* -0.359* -0.397* -0.391*(0.180) (0.164) (0.178) (0.175) (0.173) (0.172) (0.169) (0.177)

Number of venturesfounded

0.151*** 0.094** 0.110** 0.084* 0.085* 0.085* 0.061† 0.110**(0.034) (0.035) (0.036) (0.037) (0.036) (0.035) (0.035) (0.037)

Mixed emotionsb

(Valence)0.071**

(0.025)Conflictingb

(Valence)0.159***

(0.040)Dominantb

(Valence)Mixed emotionsc

0.175***(0.039)

(Cogn. Appr.)Conflictingd

0.118***(0.025)

(Cogn. Appr.)Dominantb

0.216***(0.038)

(Cogn. Appr.) 0.107**(0.040)

R2 0.095*** 0.260*** 0.123*** 0.149*** 0.163*** 0.172*** 0.199*** 0.121***D R2 — 0.165** 0.028** 0.054** 0.068** 0.077** 0.104** 0.026**Adj. R2 0.073 0.230 0.098 0.125 0.139 0.149 0.176 0.096

† p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001a Parameter estimates are reported with standard errors in parentheses; significance levels for R2 correspond to therespective F statistics.b These variables become insignificant after adding the basic emotions to the model.c The coefficient (standard error) was 0.065 (0.035), p = 0.0643 after controlling for the basic emotions.d The coefficient (standard error) was 0.199 (0.065), p = 0.0026 after controlling for the basic emotions.

128 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

significant relationship with risk perception. Among the basic emotions, both anger andfear are insignificant, while hope has a significant negative association with entrepreneur-ial risk perception, as predicted by the valence-based approach. Happiness is stronglypositively associated with entrepreneurial risk perception. Remarkably, this positive asso-ciation is predicted by neither the valence-based approach nor the cognitive appraisaltendency approach as formulated for the emotions unrelated to the issue in question(Lerner & Keltner, 2001).

In order to test hypothesis 3a and b, we conducted a subgroup moderating analysisbecause entering an interaction term into models with both basic and mixed/conflictingemotions would raise the variance inflation factor beyond the acceptable level of 10 (Hair,Black, Babin, Anderson, & Tatham, 2005). Because our goal was to distinguish noviceentrepreneurs from more experienced counterparts, we first examined the scatter plot andthe fitted regression line between risk perception and number of ventures founded. Theplot indicated that there is a negative relationship between the two variables that turnspositive between two and three ventures founded. This supported the idea that launchingonly one venture does not make an entrepreneur experienced. Therefore, we chose to splitthe sample for the subgroup moderator analysis so that novice entrepreneurs would havea maximum of two ventures of active founding experience. Further supporting this split,the number of ventures founded was not significant in the regression models (although thecoefficients were negative) for novice entrepreneurs and was significantly positive forserial entrepreneurs. To be sure, we ran the same regression sets while drawing the linebetween three and four ventures founded. In this scenario, the number of ventures foundedbecame significantly positive for the novice entrepreneurs as well, indicating learningeffects. Thus, we decided to form the subsample of novice entrepreneurs by focusing onentrepreneurs with a maximum of two ventures of active launching experience. Thereby,we defined serial entrepreneurs as entrepreneurs that founded three or more ventures.

The moderator analysis (see Tables 4 and 5) showed that conflicting emotions are onlysignificant for serial entrepreneurs, supporting hypothesis 3a and 3b. At the same time,only the effect of conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisals remains significantafter entering the basic emotions into the same regressions. The moderator analysis alsorevealed why anger had an insignificant effect in the full sample analysis: Anger is theonly significant emotion in the novice entrepreneurs group with a negative associationwith risk perception, while it has a significantly positive association with the risk percep-tion of serial entrepreneurs, although it fades away when we add the conflicting emotions.In fact, the overall transition from novice to serial entrepreneur can be characterized as acase of anger management and starting to pay more attention to conflicting emotions andpositive emotions. Interestingly, familiarity with the strategic issue is only significant fornovice entrepreneurs. Gender effects have the same pattern, suggesting that foundingexperience equalizes the way entrepreneurs perceive the riskiness of strategic issues. Wenow further discuss the most intriguing findings.

Discussion

In this study, we conceptualized mixed emotions using the ideas from the cognitiveappraisal tendency approach and contrasted this with the more traditional conceptualiza-tion from the valence-based approach (i.e., ambivalence). To ensure that we examined atrue entrepreneurial phenomenon, we distinguished between novice entrepreneurs whohad founded one or two new ventures and experienced serial entrepreneurs who hadfounded more than two new ventures. The central argument of the mixed emotions

129January, 2012

literature is that when individuals feel a blend of different emotions, it is important todistinguish which types of emotions are dominant and which are conflicting with them.The counterintuitive of the findings in this literature is that the conflicting emotions havea profound effect on judgments rather than dominant ones (Fong, 2006; Priester & Petty,1996, 2001). Therefore, we also explicitly examined the role of dominant versus conflict-ing emotions using both the cognitive appraisal tendency approach and the valence-basedapproach. Our results show that mixed emotions under each approach taken alone showa strong positive relationship with the risk perception of entrepreneurs and thus are likelyto lead to conservative and cautious behavior. However, after controlling for the effectof basic emotions (anger, fear, happiness, and hope), only the effects of the mixed andconflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisals remain significant, with conflictingemotions tending to have the strongest effect. These results are applicable to experienced,

Table 4

Hierarchical Regression Results for Entrepreneurial Risk Perceptiona N = 75,Novice Entrepreneurs (Entrepreneurs with One or Two Ventures Where TheyActively Served as a Founder)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Intercept 4.223*** 4.919*** 5.007***(1.055) (0.901) (0.893)

Anger -0.256* -0.309* -0.299*(0.120) (0.139) (0.123)

Fear 0.199 0.118 0.099(0.129) (0.166) (0.146)

Happiness 0.182† 0.128 0.065(0.107) (0.128) (0.134)

Hope 0.049 -0.004 -0.039(0.126) (0.144) (0.140)

Age -0.010 -0.008 -0.007(0.010) (0.010) (0.010)

Business degree -0.354 -0.351 -0.268(0.322) (0.323) (0.326)

Education level -0.039 -0.037 -0.029(0.175) (0.176) (0.174)

Familiarity with issue 0.181* 0.173* 0.163†

(0.086) (0.086) (0.086)Female entrepreneur -0.553† -0.572† -0.550*

(0.287) (0.289) (0.285)Number of ventures founded 0.237 -0.229 -0.286

(0.264) (0.265) (0.264)Conflicting (Valence) 0.120

(0.156)Conflicting (Cogn. Appr.) 0.200b

(0.141)R2 0.260*** 0.267*** 0.282***D R2 — 0.007 0.022*Adj. R2 0.144 0.139 0.157

† p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001a Parameter estimates are reported with standard errors in parentheses; significance levels for R2 correspond to therespective F statistics.b Conflicting emotions were significant at p < 0.05 before adding the basic emotions to the model.

130 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

serial entrepreneurs. For novice entrepreneurs, the results reveal a substantially differentpicture where only the emotion of anger has a significant relationship to risk perception.

Theoretical ImplicationsThese findings have several theoretical implications. First, we extended the list of

antecedents of entrepreneurial risk perception. Besides basic emotions (Foo, 2011), risk-taking propensity (Mullins & Forlani, 2005), and cognitive biases (Simon et al., 2000),conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisals are an important antecedent ofentrepreneurs’ risk perception. Since the decision-making context of new venture

Table 5

Hierarchical Regression Results for Entrepreneurial Risk Perceptiona N = 178,Serial Entrepreneurs (Entrepreneurs with Three and More Ventures Where TheyActively Served as a Founder)

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3

Intercept 2.975*** 4.195*** 4.349***(0.753) (0.612) (0.597)

Anger 0.165† 0.137 0.053(0.091) (0.103) (0.105)

Fear 0.070 0.046 0.007(0.079) (0.089) (0.083)

Happiness 0.382*** 0.360*** 0.287***(0.064) (0.075) (0.078)

Hope -0.267* -0.307* -0.361*(0.105) (0.126) (0.113)

Age 0.000 0.000 0.000(0.007) (0.007) (0.007)

Business degree 0.111 0.085 0.035(0.224) (0.229) (0.225)

Education level -0.095 -0.096 -0.092(0.126) (0.126) (0.124)

Familiarity with issue 0.029 0.027 0.010(0.064) (0.064) (0.064)

Female entrepreneur -0.295 -0.318 -0.356†

(0.202) (0.207) (0.202)Number of ventures founded 0.099* 0.095* 0.094*

(0.041) (0.042) (0.041)Conflicting (Valence) 0.051b

(0.086)Conflicting (Cogn. Appr.) 0.164*,b

(0.078)R2 0.315*** 0.317*** 0.333***D R2 — 0.002 0.018*Adj. R2 0.274 0.271 0.289

† p < 0.10; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001a Parameter estimates are reported with standard errors in parentheses; significance levels for R2 correspond to therespective F statistics.b These variables were significant at p < 0.001 before adding the basic emotions to the model.

131January, 2012

founders and chief executive officers of established firms become increasingly similar(Eisenhardt & Sull, 2001; Walsh et al., 2006), mixed and conflicting emotions are likelyto play a substantial role in decisions of the latter group as well.

Second, we found that mixed and conflicting emotions associated with differentcognitive appraisals have a much stronger relationship with the risk perception of entre-preneurs than mixed and conflicting emotions based on differences in positive and nega-tive valence (traditional ambivalence). Apparently, the former mixed and conflictingemotions are considered as more atypical by entrepreneurs. At the same time, takentogether with findings of Fong (2006), these results suggest an interesting implication forthe opportunity recognition and evaluation research. In particular, Fong finds that indi-viduals who are feeling emotionally ambivalent in the traditional sense demonstrate anincreased sensitivity to associations and creativity. She also calls for more research on theconsequences of ambivalence, which do not necessarily have to be beneficial. Our studyanswers this call by showing that, although traditional ambivalence may trigger betteropportunity recognition, mixed emotions in terms of different appraisals may triggeropportunity refusal by increasing risk perception. All in all, our findings support Foo(2011) in that, in the entrepreneurial field, a further exploration of the cognitive appraisaltendency approach to emotions is a very promising direction.

Third, our findings indicate that there is a qualitative change in the way entrepreneursengage emotions in their risk judgments of strategic issues after they found their thirdventure. In particular, the sign of the association of anger with risk perception is negative fornovice entrepreneurs, consistent with the predictions and findings of cognitive appraisalstudies (e.g., Foo, 2011; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001). Interestingly, anger is the onlysignificant emotion in judgments of novice entrepreneurs. However, the sign flips over to bepositive and neutral when we consider the relationship between anger and the riskperception of serial entrepreneurs. Moreover, conflicting emotions and both positiveemotions (happiness and hope) become significant. This moderating effect of the number offounded ventures, together with its relatively high correlations with all types of both basicand mixed emotions, suggests that emotional reactions of entrepreneurs on strategic issueschange substantially as they found more ventures and become serial entrepreneurs.

However, the relationship between entrepreneurs’ cognitive appraisals and their emo-tions is still not clear. There are two possible theoretical explanations for this finding: oneis personality related and the other context related. On one hand, entrepreneurs as apopulation may have different scores on the cognitive appraisal dimensions compared topsychology students typically used for calibrating the appraisals (Smith & Ellsworth,1987). As we argued earlier, the certainty and control dimensions of emotions are essentialfor judgments of risk. These dimensions are likely to be evaluated differently by entre-preneurs because entrepreneurs score low on uncertainty avoidance and tend to have aninternal locus of control that may even become an illusion of control (Busenitz & Barney,1997; McGrath, MacMillan and Scheinberg, 1992; Mueller and Thomas, 2001). Forentrepreneurs, scores on cognitive appraisal dimensions could shift across emotions andbecome more certain and more self-controlled. On the other hand, appraisals of someemotions could change from one situation to another (for a review, see Tong et al., 2009).For example, although typically, instances of anger are associated with an appraisal ofcertainty, this appraisal is not essential to the experience of this emotion (Smith &Ellsworth), which could also explain the insignificant effect of anger (for serial entrepre-neurs) and fear in our study. Moreover, entrepreneurship is often characterized as aphenomenon centering around opportunity discovery/creation and exploitation (e.g.,Ireland & Webb, 2007; Shane & Venkataraman, 2000; Sorenson & Stuart, 2008). Thus,the setting itself where opportunities are being recognized and evaluated may shift the

132 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

cognitive appraisals of emotions associated with it. Especially, recognition of opportuni-ties arising due to the differences and incongruences in perception represent an interestingfocus for such research as they are the most subjective and perception-dependent amongthe different sources of opportunities known (Drucker, 1985; Eckhardt & Shane, 2003;Ireland & Webb). According to appraisal theorists, information conveyed by a particularaffective state can be traced back to the meaning structure underlying the typical elicita-tion of that affect (Raghunathan & Pham, 1999). Because success (or failure) in contem-porary society is one of the common metrics of emotionally loaded outcomes that shapecognitive appraisals (Fessler et al., 2004), gaining entrepreneurial experience is also likelyto change cognitive appraisals. In fact, this argument should also apply to executives of themore established firms who find themselves in increasingly similar situations (Walshet al., 2006). The interplay of experience and emotional responses is therefore an inter-esting alley of research.

Fourth, an intriguing question is whether conflicting emotions lead to higher entrepre-neurial risk perception or whether it is the other way around: A higher entrepreneurial riskperception leads to a higher level of conflicting emotions. We base our study on thevalence-based and cognitive appraisal tendency approaches, proposing that theformer assumption is true. But regulatory focus theory (Higgins, 2002; Higgins, Shah, &Friedman, 1997) has recently been applied in the entrepreneurial field (Hmieleski & Baron,2008; Wu, McMullen, Neubert, &Yi, 2008), proposing that the latter assumption is correct.Regulatory focus theory assumes that self-regulation operates differently when servingfundamentally different needs, such as distinct survival needs of nurturance or security.Nurturant social regulation engenders a promotion focus; self-regulation is concerned withthe presence or absence of positive outcomes, with advancement, aspirations, and accom-plishments. Security social regulation leads to a prevention focus; self-regulation isconcerned with the absence or presence of negative outcomes, with protection, safety, andresponsibilities (Higgins, 2002). Higgins finds that self-regulatory success or failure in apromotion focus produces emotions along the cheerfulness–dejection dimension (e.g.,happiness). Self-regulatory success or failure in a prevention focus produces emotionsalong the quiescence–agitation dimension (e.g., fear). On the basis of regulatory focustheory, one may, for instance, argue that when experienced, serial entrepreneurs with apredominant promotion focus are faced with an ambiguous strategic issue, they will mainlysee it as an opportunity to maximize returns. As a result, the emotions that are based oncertainty and controllability are activated and dominant. However, when the risks increase,protection and safety will become more important as part of a prevention focus. This willlead to additional emotions based on uncertainty and uncontrollability. In this way, a higherrisk perception may lead to a higher level of conflicting emotions. Future research isrequired to more closely examine these types of associations.

Fifth, we also controlled for basic emotions in our study. In line with the cognitiveappraisal tendency approach generally predicting opposite effects for some emotions ofthe same valence (Foo, 2011; Lerner & Keltner, 2000, 2001; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985,1987), we found a strikingly different effect of two emotions of the same positivevalence—happiness and hope—on the risk perception of serial entrepreneurs. In particu-lar, we found that feeling hopeful about the focal issue has a negative relationship with riskperception of the issue, while feeling happy about the same issue has a strong positiverelationship with risk perception. However, this does not apply to the negative emotionsof anger and fear, both of which remained largely insignificant. This implies that experi-enced, serial entrepreneurs tend to use positive emotions, but not negative ones, as aheuristic for their judgments (Slovic, Finucane, Peters, & MacGregor, 2002). This may beexplained by the relatively greater optimism of entrepreneurs. Alternatively, it may be that

133January, 2012

entrepreneurs’ dominant regulatory focus shifts to the promotion focus as they gain moreexperience, because individuals high in promotion focus direct their primary attentiontoward attaining positive outcomes and gains (Hmieleski & Baron, 2008).1 Futureresearch comparing these findings with the judgments of managers in traditional organi-zations could verify whether this explanation will hold.

Finally, our study shows that validating laboratory findings with field research is auseful methodological alteration (Simon & Houghton, 2003), raising intriguing newresearch questions. For instance, based on laboratory findings following both valence-based (Johnson & Tversky, 1983; Mayer et al., 1992) and cognitive appraisal tendencyapproaches (Lerner & Keltner, 2001), we would predict a negative impact of happiness onentrepreneurial risk perception. However, we found a strong positive impact. This resultmay be explained by prospect theory. In the past, researchers in this field have concen-trated on semantic framing, where the same issue is formulated either in negative orpositive terms. A vivid example of such framing is the widely used “Asian diseaseproblem” (Tversky & Kahneman, 1981). However, more recently, researchers startedshifting the focus to contextual and personal framing (e.g., Fiegenbaum, Hart, & Schen-del, 1996; Highhouse et al., 1996). From the perspective of personal framing, one mightargue that happiness would lead to cautious behavior in order to keep the pleasant feeling,which in turn would lead to a positive impact on risk perception. More field research isneeded to confirm and explain this finding.

Managerial ImplicationsWhat can entrepreneurs learn from our study? First, entrepreneurs should realize that

their emotions play an important role in their judgments and decision making. They alsoshould realize that the situations they face are often ambiguous. Under these circum-stances, conflicting emotions are important drivers of their risk perception. This holdsespecially for conflicting emotions associated with different cognitive appraisals, likedifferences in how certain the entrepreneur is about the situation and in the controllabilityof the situation. Whenever entrepreneurs feel that a certain event is both certain anduncertain, or both controllable and uncontrollable, such feelings make the entrepreneur’sperception of risk greater than it would be otherwise resulting in more conservative risktaking (e.g., Sitkin & Weingart, 1995).

Second, as entrepreneurs found more ventures, they learn to ignore their anger andengage their positive emotions—such as happiness and hope—as well as their conflictingemotions in their risk judgments. To the extent these emotions are grounded in entrepre-neurs’ experience in running ventures they are likely to convey relevant information. Thus,novice entrepreneurs may find it useful to pay more attention to the positive and conflict-ing emotions, as modeled by their more experienced counterparts.

At the same time, entrepreneurs should become aware of the emotional influences ontheir risk-taking behavior. After all, emotions influencing their decision making are notnecessarily based on relevant information. For example, entrepreneurs’ moods can changetheir judgments (Forgas, 1995). Also, not all positive emotions are the same: While feelinghope about a certain issue makes entrepreneur’ decisions more risk seeking, feelinghappiness about the same issue makes them more risk averse.

All in all, learning to handle their emotions is essential for entrepreneurs. After all, themore experienced the entrepreneur, the more influence emotions will have on risk-takingbehavior.

1. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this comment.

134 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

Limitations and Future ResearchThis study has several limitations. First, studies on affect differentiate between moods

and emotions, which have different characteristics as described above. Even more sub-stantial is the difference between the relatively enduring feeling of passion and the currentemotions we studied (Cardon et al., 2009), the interactions of which would be interestingto explore. Future research may also include affective reactions of entrepreneurs todifferent strategic events, for example, those related to decisions about new productdevelopment stages, marketing strategy, or financing strategy. Second, the novice entre-preneurs in our sample founded one or two ventures. Still, some of these entrepreneursmay be more successful than the others and as a result of that may decide not to found newventures. A related yet distinct aspect is the number of years the entrepreneurs are activelyinvolved in these ventures. Thus, more refined research that would also take these aspectsinto account seems valuable.

Third, in applying the cognitive appraisal tendency approach, we followed Lerner andKeltner (2000, 2001) and Foo (2011) and only focused on the certainty and controllabilitydimensions due to their links with risk perceptions. Although these appraisals emotionsare very likely to be felt by entrepreneurs in the various decisions they have to make, itwould be worthwhile to study a richer set of emotions in future research (for an overviewof dimensions, we refer to Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, 1987). For example, other positiveand negative emotions besides the ones we studied could trigger a more profound rela-tionship between ambivalence and risk perception, which could serve as an alternativeexplanation for the weak performance of the valence-based approach to mixed emotionsin our study. The first candidate would be the emotion of sadness as a more direct oppositefor happiness besides the hope and fear juxtaposition we studied.2

Finally, in our study, we were neutral to the normative interpretation of the identifiedrelationships between emotions and risk perception, because the underlying scenario wasan ambiguous strategic issue that could be seen both as a positive or a negative event(Highhouse et al., 1996). An interesting direction for future research would be to focus onnormatively good or bad decision situations and to explore the role of affect in thosesituations. This would allow for the exploration of the moods and emotions that have apolluting effect on entrepreneurial decision making. For example, emotions serve tomotivate and guide individuals with respect to salient objects in their environments, andconsequently, may increase adaptive responding to specific environmental challengesquickly through heuristic or reflexive processes. However, when the informational valueof emotions is misapplied to stimuli not relevant to the current situation, nonoptimal (i.e.,biased) judgments may result (DeSteno et al., 2000). Emotional reactions can divergefrom cognitive ones and produce behavioral responses that depart from what individualsview as the best course of action (Blanchette & Richards, 2010; Loewenstein et al., 2001).This promising line of thinking runs parallel to the research on cognitive biases, whichfocuses on deviations from a certain normative reaction or judgment in a given situation(e.g., Keh et al., 2002; Simon & Houghton, 2002, 2003; Simon et al., 2000).

Conclusion

This study identified mixed and conflicting emotions in terms of cognitive appraisalsas a predictor of entrepreneurs’ risk perceptions. However, we also found that the

2. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this comment.

135January, 2012

relationship between both mixed and basic emotions and risk perception changes sub-stantially as entrepreneurs gain more experience in founding new ventures. Finally, wefound that traditional cognitive appraisal tendencies calibrated with student samples donot fully explain the observed relationships.

REFERENCES

Aiken, L.S. & West, S.G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park,CA: Sage.

Alvarez, S.A. & Barney, J.B. (2007). Discovery and creation: Alternative theories of entrepreneurial action.Strategic Entrepreneurship Journal, 1(1–2), 11–26.

Baron, R.A. (1998). Cognitive mechanisms in entrepreneurship: Why and when entrepreneurs think differ-ently than other people. Journal of Business Venturing, 13(4), 275–294.

Baron, R.A. (2006). Entrepreneurship: A process perspective. In R. Baum, M. Frese, & R.A. Baron (Eds.), Thepsychology of entrepreneurship: Frontiers of industrial/organizational psychology series (pp. 19–40).Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Baron, R.A. (2008). The role of affect in the entrepreneurial process. Academy of Management Review, 33(2),328–340.

Belsley, D.A., Kuh, E., & Welsch, R.E. (1980). Regression diagnostics. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Blanchette, I. & Richards, A. (2010). The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of researchon interpretation, judgement, decision making and reasoning. Cognition and Emotion, 24(4), 561–595.

Brundin, E., Patzelt, H., & Shepherd, D.A. (2008). Managers’ emotional display and employees’ willingnessto act entrepreneurially. Journal of Business Venturing, 23(2), 221–243.

Busenitz, L.W. & Barney, J.B. (1997). Differences between entrepreneurs and managers in large organiza-tions: Biases and heuristics in strategic decision-making. Journal of Business Venturing, 12(1), 9–30.

Cardon, M.S., Wincent, J., Singh, J., & Drnovsek, M. (2009). The nature and experience of entrepreneurialpassion. Academy of Management Review, 34(3), 511–532.

Cardon, M.S., Zietsma, C., Saparito, P., Matherne, B.P., & Davis, C. (2005). A tale of passion: New insightsinto entrepreneurship from a parenthood metaphor. Journal of Business Venturing, 20(1), 23–45.

Cohen, J. & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences.Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G., & Aiken, L.S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis forthe behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Denes-Raj, V. & Epstein, S. (1994). Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: When people behaveagainst their better judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(5), 819–829.

DeSteno, D., Petty, R.E., Rucker, D.D., Wegener, D.T., & Braverman, J. (2004). Discrete emotions andpersuasion: The role of emotion-induced expectancies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1),43–56.

DeSteno, D., Petty, R.E., Wegener, D.T., & Rucker, D.D. (2000). Beyond valence in the perception oflikelihood: The role of emotion specificity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(3), 397–416.

136 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

Dillman, D.A. (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: The total design method. New York: John Wiley and SonsInc.

Drucker, P.F. (1985). The discipline of innovation. Harvard Business Review, 63(3), 67–72.

Dutton, J.E. & Jackson, S.E. (1987). Categorizing strategic issues: Links to organizational action. Academy ofManagement Review, 12(1), 76–90.

Eckhardt, J.T. & Shane, S.A. (2003). Opportunities and entrepreneurship. Journal of Management, 29(3),333–349.

Eisenhardt, K.M. & Sull, D.N. (2001). Strategy as simple rules. Harvard Business Review, 79(1), 106–116.

Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist,49(8), 709–724.

Fessler, D.M.T., Pillsworth, E.G., & Flamson, T.J. (2004). Angry men and disgusted women: An evolutionaryapproach to the influence of emotions on risk taking. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-cesses, 95(1), 107–123.

Fiegenbaum, A., Hart, S., & Schendel, D. (1996). Strategic reference point theory. Strategic ManagementJournal, 17(3), 219–235.

Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R.S. (1985). If it changes it must be a process: Study of emotion and coping duringthree stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(1), 150–170.

Fong, C.T. (2006). The effects of emotional ambivalence on creativity. Academy of Management Journal,49(5), 1016–1030.

Fong, C.T. & Tiedens, L.Z. (2002). Dueling experiences and dual ambivalences: Emotional and motivationalambivalence of women in high status positions. Motivation and Emotion, 26(1), 105–121.

Foo, M.D. (2011). Emotions and entrepreneurial opportunity evaluation. Entrepreneurship Theory and Prac-tice, 35, 375–393.

Foo, M.D., Uy, M.A., & Baron, R.A. (2009). How do feelings influence effort? An empirical study ofentrepreneurs’ affect and venture effort. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(4), 1086–1094.

Forgas, J.P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1),39–66.

Forgas, J.P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects on negotiator cognition andbargaining strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 565–577.

Forlani, D. & Mullins, J.W. (2000). Perceived risk and choices in entrepreneurs’ new venture decisions.Journal of Business Venturing, 15(4), 305–322.

Golder, P.N. & Tellis, G.J. (1993). Pioneer advantage: Marketing logic or marketing legend. Jornal ofMarketing Research, 20(2), 158–170.

Hair, J.F., Black, B., Babin, B., Anderson, R.E., & Tatham, R.L. (2005). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.).Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall International, Inc.

Higgins, E.T. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct values: The case of promotion and preventiondecision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(3), 177–191.

Higgins, E.T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatoryfocus as moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(3), 515–525.

137January, 2012

Highhouse, S., Paese, P.W., & Leatherberry, T. (1996). Contrast effects on strategic-issue framing. Organi-zational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(2), 95–105.

Hmieleski, K.M. & Baron, R.A. (2008). Regulatory focus and new venture performance: A study of entre-preneurial opportunity exploitation under conditions of risk and uncertainty. Strategic EntrepreneurshipJournal, 2(4), 285–299.

Hunt, S.D., Sparkman, R.D. Jr, & Wilcox, J.B. (1982). The pretest in survey research: Issues and preliminaryfindings. Journal of Marketing Research, 19(2), 269–273.

Hyytinen, A. & Ilmakunnas, P. (2007). What distinguishes a serial entrepreneur? Industrial and CorporateChange, 16(5), 793–829.

Ireland, R.D. & Webb, J.W. (2007). A cross-disciplinary exploration of entrepreneurship research. Journal ofManagement, 33(6), 891–927.

Jackson, S.E. & Dutton, J.E. (1988). Discerning threats and opportunities. Administrative Science Quarterly,33(3), 370–387.

Johnson, E.J. & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 45(1), 20–31.

Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720.

Keh, H.T., Foo, M.D., & Lim, B.C. (2002). Opportunity evaluation under risky conditions: The cognitiveprocesses of entrepreneurs. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 27(2), 125–148.

Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P.C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and angeron social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(5), 740–752.

Kenny, D.A. & Judd, C.M. (1984). Estimating the non-linear and interactive effects of latent variables.Psychological Bulletin, 96(1), 201–210.

Larsen, J.T., McGraw, A.P., & Cacioppo, J.T. (2001). Can people feel happy and sad at the same time? Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 684–696.

Larsen, J.T., McGraw, A.P., Mellers, B.A., & Cacioppo, J.T. (2004). The agony of victory and thrill of defeat:Mixed emotional reactions to disappointing wins and relieving losses. Psychological Science, 15(5), 325–330.

Lerner, J.S. & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences onjudgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14(4), 473–493.

Lerner, J.S. & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1),146–159.

Lerner, J.S. & Tiedens, L.Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shapeanger’s influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(2), 115–137.

Lichtenstein, B., Dooley, K.J., & Lumpkin, G.T. (2006). Measuring emergence in the dynamics of newventure creation. Journal of Business Venturing, 21(2), 153–175.

Loewenstein, G.F., Weber, E.U., Hsee, C.K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin,127(2), 267–286.

MacGrimmon, K.R. & Wehrung, D.A. (1985). A portfolio of risk measures. Theory and Decision, 19(1),1–29.

Mayer, J.D., Gaschke, Y.N., Braverman, D.L., & Evans, T.W. (1992). Mood-congruent judgment is a generaleffect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(1), 119–132.

138 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE

McGrath, R.G., MacMillan, I.C., & Scheinberg, S. (1992). Elitists, risk-takers, and rugged individualists? Anexploratory analysis of cultural differences between entrepreneurs and non-entrepreneurs. Journal of BusinessVenturing, 7(2), 115–135.

Mitchell, J.R., Friga, P.N., & Mitchell, R.K. (2005). Untangling the intuition mess: Intuition as a construct inentrepreneurship research. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 29(6), 653–679.

Mueller, S.L. & Thomas, A.S. (2001). Culture and entrepreneurial potential: A nine country study of locus ofcontrol and innovativeness. Journal of Business Venturing, 16(1), 51–75.

Mullins, J.W. & Forlani, D. (2005). Missing the boat or sinking the boat: A study of new venture decisionmaking. Journal of Business Venturing, 20(1), 47–69.

Nunnally, J.C. (1978). Psychometric theory (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Priester, J.R. & Petty, R.E. (1996). The gradual threshold model of ambivalence: Relating the positive and thenegative bases of attitude to subjective ambivalence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(3),431–449.

Priester, J.R. & Petty, R.E. (2001). Extending the bases of subjective attitudinal ambivalence: Interpersonaland intrapersonal antecedents of evaluative tension. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1),19–34.

Priester, J.R., Petty, R.E., & Park, K. (2007). Whence univalent ambivalence? From the anticipation ofconflicting reactions. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(1), 11–21.

Raghunathan, R. & Pham, M.T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxietyand sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision, 79(1), 56–77.

Schwarz, G.E. & Weinberger, D.A. (1980). Patterns of emotional responses to affective situations: Relationsamong happiness, sadness, anger, fear, depression and anxiety. Motivation and Emotion, 4(2), 175–191.

Shane, S. (2000). Prior knowledge and the discovery of entrepreneurial opportunities. Organization Science,11(4), 448–469.

Shane, S. & Venkataraman, S. (2000). The promise of entrepreneurship as a field of research. Academy ofManagement Review, 25(1), 217–226.

Shepherd, D.A. & Cardon, M.S. (2009). Negative emotional reactions to project failure and the self-compassion to learn from the experience. Journal of Management Studies, 46(6), 923–949.

Simon, M. & Houghton, S.M. (2002). The relationship among biases, misperceptions, and the introduction ofpioneering products: Examining differences in venture decision contexts. Entrepreneurship Theory andPractice, 27(2), 105–124.

Simon, M. & Houghton, S.M. (2003). The relationship between overconfidence and the introduction of riskyproducts: Evidence from a field study. Academy of Management Journal, 46(2), 139–149.

Simon, M., Houghton, S.M., & Aquino, K. (2000). Cognitive biases, risk perception, and venture formation:How individuals decide to start companies. Journal of Business Venturing, 15(2), 113–134.

Sitkin, S.B. & Pablo, A.L. (1992). Reconceptualizing the determinants of risk behavior. Academy of Man-agement Review, 17(1), 9–38.

Sitkin, S.B. & Weingart, L.R. (1995). Determinants of risky decision-making behavior: A test of the mediatingrole of risk perception and propensity. Academy of Management Journal, 38(6), 1573–1592.

139January, 2012

Slovic, P., Finucane, M., Peters, E., & MacGregor, D. (2002). The affect heuristic. In T. Gilovich, D. Griffin,& D. Kahneman (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 397–420). NewYork: Cambridge University Press.

Smith, C.A. & Ellsworth, C.A. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 48(4), 813–838.

Smith, C.A. & Ellsworth, C.A. (1987). Patterns of appraisal and emotion related to taking an exam. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 52(3), 475–488.

Sorenson, O. & Stuart, T.E. (2008). Entrepreneurship: Field of dreams? Annals of the Academy Management,2, 517–543.

Tong, E.M.W., Bishop, G.D., Enkelmann, H.C., Why, Y.P., Diong, S.M., Khader, M., et al. (2009). Appraisalunderpinnings of affective chronometry: The role of appraisals in emotion habituation. Journal of Personality,77(4), 1103–1136.

Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211,453–458.

Walsh, J.P., Meyer, A.D., & Schoonhoven, C.B. (2006). A future for organization theory: Living in and livingwith changing organizations. Organization Science, 17(5), 657–671.

Wiklund, J. & Shepherd, D. (2008). Portfolio entrepreneurship: Habitual and novice founders, new entry, andmode of organizing. Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 32(4), 701–725.

Wu, C., McMullen, J.S., Neubert, M.J., & Yi, X. (2008). The influence of leader regulatory focus on employeecreativity. Journal of Business Venturing, 23(5), 587–602.

Ksenia Podoynitsyna is an Assistant Professor of Entrepreneurship in the Innovation, Technology Entrepre-neurship, and Marketing Group at the School of Industrial Engineering, Eindhoven University of Technology.

Hans Van der Bij is an Associate Professor of Innovation Management in the Innovation Management andStrategy Group in the Economics and Business Department at the University of Groningen.

Michael Song is a Charles N. Kimball, MRI/Missouri Endowed Chair in Management of Technology andInnovation and Professor of Marketing in the 318 Bloch School at the University of Missouri–Kansas City.

140 ENTREPRENEURSHIP THEORY and PRACTICE