Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus

8
Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiforrn Gyms Gregory McCarthy, Aha Puce, John C. Gore, and Truett Allison VA Medical Center, West Haven, CT and Yale University School of Medicine Abstract The perception of faces is sometimes regarded as a special- ized task involving discrete brain regions. In an attempt to identi$ face-specific cortex, we used functional magnetic reso- nance imaging (fMRI) to measure activation evoked by faces presented in a continuously changing montage of common objects or in a similar montage of nonobjects. Bilateral regions of the posterior fusiform gyrus were activated by faces viewed among nonobjects, but when viewed among objects, faces activated only a focal right fusiform region. To determine INTRODUCTION There are several reasons for believing that human faces are a biologically important class of visual objects that may be processed by specialized brain mechanisms (re- viewed by Bruce & Humphreys, 1994). Damage to oc- cipitotemporal cortex may produce an inability to recognize familiar faces (Meadows, 1974; Whiteley & Warrington, 1977; Damasio, Damasio, & Van Hoesen, 1982; Damasio,Tranel, & Damasio, 1990) with little or no deficit in recognizing other categories of objects (Farah, 1994; Newcombe, Mehta, & de Haan, 1994). Single-unit recordings from the temporal lobe of monkeys reveal cells that respond selectively to faces or face compo- nents (Perrett, Hietanen, Oram, & Benson, 1992; Gross, 1992; Wang, Tanaka, & Tanifuji, 1996). Recordings in pa- tients with chronically implanted electrodes demon- strate that discrete regions of inferior occipito-temporal cortex generate short-latencyfield potentials to faces but not to scrambled faces, letter strings, animals, or cars (AUison, Ginter, et al., 1994; Allison, McCarthy, Nobre, Puce, & Belger, 1994;Nobre, AUison, & McCarthy, 1994). Positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI demon- strate that regions of occipito-temporal cortex are acti- vated by a variety of face-processing tasks (Sergent,Ohta, & MacDonald, 1992; Haxby et al., 1994; Clark et al., 1995; Puce, AUison, Gore, & McCarthy, 1995). However, these regions are also activated by objects (Malach et al., 1995; Schacter et al., 1995; Kohler,Kapur,Moscovitch, Winocur, & Houle, 1995; Kanwisher, Woods,Iacoboni, & Mazziotta, 1997); hence activation by faces may simply reflect gen- 0 1997 Massachusetts Institute of Technology whether this focal activation would occur for another category of familiar stimuli, subjects viewed flowers presented among nonobjects and objects. While flowers among nonobjects evoked bilateral fusiform activation, flowers among objects evoked no activation. These results demonstrate that both faces and flowers activate large and partially overlapping regions of inferior extrastriate cortex. A smaller region, located primarily in the right lateral fusiform gyrus, is activated specifically by faces. W era1 object processing. Presented in isolation, faces may engender both specific and general object processing. We reasoned that a face-specificprocessing region might be revealed only if the general object recognition system was occupied by concurrent object processing.To evalu- ate this possibility, faces were periodically presented within a continuously changing montage of common objects and nonobjects on the assumption that nonob- jects would not engage object recognition processes but would control for physical stimulus characteristics such as luminance and spatial frequency. Face-specific proc- essing regions would appear as a subset of a more extensive activation evoked in the general object recog- nition system. To determine whether within-category processing of any well-known object category produces similar results as faces, the experiment was repeated substituting flowers for faces. Flowers were predicted to activate the general object recognition system when presented among nonobjects but not when presented among objects. A preliminary report of these results has been presented (McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1996). RESULTS Figure la demonstrates that faces among nonobjects evoked extensive inferior brain activation with the larg- est number of activated voxels occurring in slices 4 and 5. Figure l b demonstrates that flowers among nonob- jects also produced extensive inferior brain activation. Figure l c shows that faces among nonobjects evoked more activation in the right hemisphere, particularly in Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:5, pp. 605-610

Transcript of Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus

Face-Specific Processing in the Human Fusiforrn Gyms

Gregory McCarthy, Aha Puce, John C. Gore, and Truett Allison VA Medical Center, West Haven, CT and Yale University School of Medicine

Abstract

The perception of faces is sometimes regarded as a special- ized task involving discrete brain regions. In an attempt to identi$ face-specific cortex, we used functional magnetic reso- nance imaging (fMRI) to measure activation evoked by faces presented in a continuously changing montage of common objects or in a similar montage of nonobjects. Bilateral regions of the posterior fusiform gyrus were activated by faces viewed among nonobjects, but when viewed among objects, faces activated only a focal right fusiform region. To determine

INTRODUCTION

There are several reasons for believing that human faces are a biologically important class of visual objects that may be processed by specialized brain mechanisms (re- viewed by Bruce & Humphreys, 1994). Damage to oc- cipitotemporal cortex may produce an inability to recognize familiar faces (Meadows, 1974; Whiteley & Warrington, 1977; Damasio, Damasio, & Van Hoesen, 1982; Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1990) with little or no deficit in recognizing other categories of objects (Farah, 1994; Newcombe, Mehta, & de Haan, 1994). Single-unit recordings from the temporal lobe of monkeys reveal cells that respond selectively to faces or face compo- nents (Perrett, Hietanen, Oram, & Benson, 1992; Gross, 1992; Wang, Tanaka, & Tanifuji, 1996). Recordings in pa- tients with chronically implanted electrodes demon- strate that discrete regions of inferior occipito-temporal cortex generate short-latency field potentials to faces but not to scrambled faces, letter strings, animals, or cars (AUison, Ginter, et al., 1994; Allison, McCarthy, Nobre, Puce, & Belger, 1994; Nobre, AUison, & McCarthy, 1994).

Positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI demon- strate that regions of occipito-temporal cortex are acti- vated by a variety of face-processing tasks (Sergent, Ohta, & MacDonald, 1992; Haxby et al., 1994; Clark et al., 1995; Puce, AUison, Gore, & McCarthy, 1995). However, these regions are also activated by objects (Malach et al., 1995; Schacter et al., 1995; Kohler, Kapur, Moscovitch, Winocur, & Houle, 1995; Kanwisher, Woods, Iacoboni, & Mazziotta, 1997); hence activation by faces may simply reflect gen-

0 1997 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

whether this focal activation would occur for another category of familiar stimuli, subjects viewed flowers presented among nonobjects and objects. While flowers among nonobjects evoked bilateral fusiform activation, flowers among objects evoked no activation. These results demonstrate that both faces and flowers activate large and partially overlapping regions of inferior extrastriate cortex. A smaller region, located primarily in the right lateral fusiform gyrus, is activated specifically by faces. W

era1 object processing. Presented in isolation, faces may engender both specific and general object processing. We reasoned that a face-specific processing region might be revealed only if the general object recognition system was occupied by concurrent object processing. To evalu- ate this possibility, faces were periodically presented within a continuously changing montage of common objects and nonobjects on the assumption that nonob- jects would not engage object recognition processes but would control for physical stimulus characteristics such as luminance and spatial frequency. Face-specific proc- essing regions would appear as a subset of a more extensive activation evoked in the general object recog- nition system. To determine whether within-category processing of any well-known object category produces similar results as faces, the experiment was repeated substituting flowers for faces. Flowers were predicted to activate the general object recognition system when presented among nonobjects but not when presented among objects. A preliminary report of these results has been presented (McCarthy, Puce, Gore, & Allison, 1996).

RESULTS

Figure la demonstrates that faces among nonobjects evoked extensive inferior brain activation with the larg- est number of activated voxels occurring in slices 4 and 5. Figure l b demonstrates that flowers among nonob- jects also produced extensive inferior brain activation. Figure l c shows that faces among nonobjects evoked more activation in the right hemisphere, particularly in

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:5, pp. 605-610

slice 2. Flowers among nonobjects evoked more activa- tion in the left hemisphere in slices 3 and 4. While faces and flowers both activated slices 5 through 7, the differ- ence in their patterns of activation was small.

These results confirm our expectation that both ob- ject categories produce strong inferior occipito-temporal activation during concurrent nonobject processing. Of critical importance is whether periodic activation can be measured during concurrent object processing. Figure la shows activation by faces among objects, with the greatest activation occurring in the right hemisphere in slices 2 and 4. These voxels were a subset of those activated by faces among nonobjects.' An estimate of noise was calculated for the nonobject and object con- ditions; voxels activated by faces among objects were well above this noise level (Figure la). In marked con- trast, flowers among objects produced no activation that exceeded the noise level (Figure lb).

Figure 2 illustrates activation by faces for four individ- ual subjects and the across-subjects average for slice 2, which produced the largest relative face activation (Fig- ure lc). Faces among nonobjects produced activation of the right fusiform gyrus and produced lesser activation of the left fusiform gyrus (Figure 2b and c). Faces among objects activated a smaller region of the right fusiform gyms but did not activate the left fusiform gyrus (Figure 2e and 0.

Figure 3 presents a similar comparison for activation by flowers for slice 4, which produced the largest relative flower activation (Figure lc). Activation of the fusiform gyri and, in one subject, activation of the intra- parietal sulci was evoked by flowers among nonobjects (Figure 3b and c). However, flowers among objects evoked no fusiform activation in this or any other slice (Figure 3e and 0.

This pattern of activation is summarized in Table 1: (1) When viewed among nonobjects, faces activated the right hemisphere more than the left, and flowers acti- vated the left hemisphere more than the right, (2) when viewed among objects, faces activated a focal right fusi- form region while flowers evoked no activation in either hemisphere, and (3) the greatest activation by faces oc- curred in the lateral fusiform gyrus, while the greatest activation by flowers occurred in the midfusiform sulcus. The medial fusiform gyrus was only slightly activated by

Figure 1. Mean number of voxels (averaged across all subjects) activated by faces (a) and by flowers @) in right and left inferior occipito-temporal cortex. This measure includes all voxels within and inferior to the inferior temporal gyms and represents 86% of all activated voxels. Slice 1 was the most anterior, and slice 7 was the most posterior. Average anterior-posterior locations in the atlas of Talairach & Tournoux (1988) were, for slices 1 through 7 respective1y.y = -40, -47, -54.41, -68, -75, -82. (c) Differences in activation (mean voxel count) by faces and flowers among nonobjects. A positive difference denotes greater activation by faces than flowers, whereas a negative difference denotes greater activation by flowers. The largest effects were in slice 2, where activation by faces was greater than for flowers in the right hemisphere, and in slice 4, where activation by flowers was greater than for faces in the left hemisphere.

606 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

faces among nonobjects and not at all by faces or flow- ers among objects.

DISCUSSION

These results demonstrate that faces viewed within a complex scene of continuously changing objects acti- vate a small region of extrastriate visual cortex, limited primarily to the right fusiform gyrus (Table 1). This con- clusion supports the argument that faces are treated differently from nonface objects by the visual system and are processed in a specialized region evident when the general object recognition system is occupied.

It has long been thought that the right hemisphere is more engaged in face processing than is the left hemi- sphere. Behavioral studies demonstrate a right-hemi- sphere advantage for face recognition (reviewed by Rhodes, 1993). While lesions that produce prosopagnosia are often bilateral, lesions limited to the right occipito- temporal region can also produce prosopagnosia (Whiteley & Warrington, 1977; Damasio et al., 1990; De Renzi, Perani, Carlesimo, Silveri, & Fazio, 1994). PET @or- witz et al., 1992; Haxby, Ungerleider, Horwitz, Rapoport, & Grady, 1995), fMRI (Puce et al., 1995; Puce, Allison, Asgari, Gore, & McCarthy, 1996; Kanwisher, McDermott, & Chun, 1996; Kanwisher, Chun, McDermott, & Hamil- ton, 1996), and scalp-recorded evoked potential (Bentin, Allison, Puce, Perez, & McCarthy, 1996) studies reveal greater activation by faces in the right than the left occipito-temporal region. In this study, the volume of cortex activated by faces among nonobjects was ap- proximately twice as large in the right than in the left hemisphere. The smaller left-hemisphere activation was further diminished for faces among objects (Table l), suggesting that the right-hemisphere advantage for face processing is especially strong in complex visual envi- ronments.

It has been suggested that face recognition is a special case of object processing requiring within-category (sub- ordinate) discrimination of visually similar objects (e.g., Damasio et al., 1982; Gauthier et al., 1996; reviewed by Logothetis & Sheinberg, 1996). In this study we used unfamiliar faces and flowers in a passive viewing task that did not require subordinate-level processing of either category. Kanwisher et al. (1996) examined this issue explicitly in an fMRI study and found that a region of the right fusiform gyrus, similar in location to the region described here, was more activated by faces than by hands even when subordinate-level identification of hands was more difficult than identification of faces. While we cannot rule out the possibility that some of the activation to faces seen in this study was due to differences in depth of processing rather than to face- specific processing, we regard this explanation as un- likely.

The anatomical configuration of face-specific cortex is unclear. Each region could be composed of face-specific

Figure 2. (a) Faces among nonobjects. At any instant a random sub- set of 15 possible locations was occupied by faces and nonobjects or by nonobjects alone. Nonobjects were presented continuously while faces appeared periodically. (b) Activation by faces among nonobjects for four subjects (coronal section, slice 2 , t-values z 1.96). The right side of the brain appears on the left side of the im- age. (c) Across-subjects average activation by faces among nonob- jects for slice 2. (d) Faces among objects. (e) Activation by faces among objects; subjects and slice are the same as in (b). The fusi- form gyrus (FG) is bounded medially by the collateral sulcus (CoS) and laterally by the occipito-temporal sulcus (OTS); it is divided into medial and lateral portions by the midfusiform sulcus (MFS). (f) Across-subjects average activation by faces among objects for slice 2.

McCartby et al. 607

Figure 3. (a) Flowers among nonobjects. (b) Activation by flowers among nonobjects for four subjects (slice 4). (c) Across-subjects aver- age activation by flowers among nonobjects for slice 4. (d) Flowers among objects. (e) Activation by flowers among objects; subjects and slice the same as in (b). (f) Acrosssubjects average activation by flowers among objects for slice 4.

columns of cells 0.4 to 1.0 mm in diameter like those in monkeys (Fujita, Tanaka, Ito, & Cheng, 1992; Wang et al., 1996), interspersed among columns of different selectiv- ity. Such a dispersed pattern of activation might be ex- pected to produce a random stippling of activation throughout the fusiform gyrus rather than the focal ac- tivation that we found. Alternatively, face-specific regions could occur in patches of cortex (perhaps composed of clusters of face-specific columns), an arrangement sug-

gested by face-sensitive patches of cortex in monkey temporal lobe (Harries & Perrett, 1991). The right hemi- sphere activations in Figure 2e suggest elongated patches of face-specific cortex located in the midfusi- form sulcus and lateral fusiform gyrus.

We conclude that faces are perceived at least in part by a separate processing stream within the ventral object recognition system (Ungerleider & Mishkin, 1982; Living- stone & Hubel, 1988; Merigan & Maunsell, 1993). In humans a major component of this stream occupies lateral portions of the right fusiform gyrus.

METHODS

Twelve volunteer subjects (six males), ranging in age from 22 to 43 years, participated in this study. Eleven subjects were right handed and one was ambidextrous. The protocol was approved by the Human Investigation Committee of Yale University School of Medicine, and informed consent was obtained. A 1.5T General Electric Signa scanner with a standard quadrature head coil and ANMR echoplanar subsystem was used. The subject's head was positioned along the canthomeatal line and immobilized using a vacuum cushion and a forehead strap. T1-weighted sagittal scans were used to select seven contiguous 7-mm coronal slices beginning at the posterior edge of the splenium. Functional images were acquired using a gradient-echo echoplanar sequence (TR = 1500, TE = 45, a = GO", NEX = 1, voxel size = 3.2 x 3.2 x 7 mm). Each imaging run consisted of 128 images per slice (19Gsec scan time).

Four categories of gray-scale stimuli were back- projected onto a translucent screen that subjects viewed through a mirror mounted on the head coil: (1) Male and female faces scanned from college yearbooks (Figure 2a), (2) stock images of individual common objects, fruits, and vegetables (Figure 2d), (3) nonobjects generated by computing a Fourier transform of each object image, randomly scrambling its phase spectrum while preserv- ing its frequency spectrum and then performing an in- verse transform (Figure 2a), and (4) stock images of individual flowers (Figure 3a). The average intensity and contrast of stimuli were equalized across the four stimu- lus categories. Each run of an activation task lasted 196 sec and consisted of approximately 1100 stimuli, each presented for 500 to 1000 msec at 1 of 15 screen locations. The onsets and offsets of individual stimuli were asynchronous, resulting in a continuously changing montage in which an average of 5.4 stimuli were visible every second. Individual stimuli fit within a 1.85" x 1.85" area, and the complete display subtended horizon- tal and vertical angles of 9.3" x 5.6".

Eight runs of the face task were acquired in the first imaging session; there were four runs of objects and four runs of nonobjects. In all runs, faces appeared among the object or nonobject stimuli at predetermined periods. In half of the runs, faces appeared for a Gsec period and

608 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 9, Number 5

Table 1. Average volumes of activation (in mm3) in the right (R) and left (LJ fusiform

Faces in Faces in Flowers in Flowers in nono bjects objects nonobjects objects

R L R L R L R L

Medial fusiform gyms 60 0 0 0 13 156 0 0

Midfusiform sulcus 197 149 143 18 209 254 0 0

Lateral fusiform gyrus 299 113 137 12 91 59 0 0

Total 556 262 280 30 313 469 0 0

‘ Volume calculations based on voxel counts from average t-maps (t > 1.96). The Talairach (x, y, z) coordinates for the centroids of activation in the fusiform gyrus were: Faces among nonobjects (R: 36, -52, -19, L: -35, -56, -17); Faces among objects (R: 40, -59, -22, L: -40, -55, -15); Flowers among nonobjects (R: 30, -54, -15, L -30, -59, -20).

then disappeared for 6 sec, resulting in a 1Zsec cycle time (15 to 16 cycles per run). In the remaining runs, faces appeared and disappeared every 8.73 sec, resulting in a 17.46-sec cycle time (10 to 11 cycles per run). The total number of visible stimuli did not change with the inclusion of the periodic face stimuli. In a second imag- ing session, flowers substituted for faces using the same object and nonobject backgrounds.

For the 1Zsec cycle, the three images per slice ac- quired at the end of each face or flower 6-sec “on” period was compared to the three images acquired at the end of each Gsec “off period using an unpaired t-test on a voxel-by-voxel basis. Four images per period were compared for the 17.4Gsec cycle. For each subject, average t-maps were computed for faces and flowers among objects and nonobjects. Voxels exceeding a t- value of 1.96 were counted for each task, slice, and hemisphere. To estimate the number of false positive or noise voxels, a second set of t-maps was computed in which the 17.4Gsec cycle runs were analyzed using images selected according to the schedule of a 1Zsec cycle run. Similarly, the 1Zsec cycle runs were analyzed using images selected according to the schedule for a 17.4Gsec cycle. Thus the grouping of images was not synchronized to stimulus on and off periods, but peri- odic noise could be measured. Across-subjects t-maps were also computed. Prior to averaging, t-maps for each subject were translated, stretched, and rotated to align gyri and sulci to a reference image set. Alignment factors were calculated using high-resolution anatomical images without regard to the functional activations. Alignments were performed separately for each hemisphere and for each slice.

Acknowledgments This research was supported by the Department of Veterans Affairs and by NIMH Grant MH-05286. We thank H. Sarofin for assistance.

Reprint requests should be sent to Gregory McCarthy, Ph.D., Neuropsychology Laboratory 116B1, VA Medical Center, West Haven, CT 06516, or via e-mail to [email protected].

Notes

1. For those voxels activated by faces among objects, the corresponding voxel or its immediate neighbor was activated by faces among nonobjects 88% of the time for the right hemisphere and 65% of the time for the left hemisphere. Within the right hemisphere, the overlap was 100% for slice 2.

REFERENCES Allison, T., Ginter, H., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A,, Puce, A., Luby,

M., & Spencer, D. D. (1994). Face recognition in human ex- trastriate cortex. Journal of Neuropbysiology, 71, 821-825.

(1994). Human extrastriate visual cortex and the percep- tion of faces, words, numbers, and colors. Cerebral Cortex,

Allison, T., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A,, Puce, A., & Belger, A.

5, 544-554. Bentin, S., Allison, T., Puce, A., Perez, A,, & McCarthy, G.

(1996). Electrophysiological studies of face perception in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(6), 55 1- 565.

Bruce, V, & Humphreys, G. W (Eds.) (1994). Object and face recognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Clark, V I?, Parasuraman, R., Keil, K., Maisog, J. M., Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., & Haxby, J. V. (1995). Cortical fields for face and color perception revealed with func- tional MRI. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 21, 18.

Prosopagnosia: Anatomic basis and behavioral mechanisms. Neuvologg 32, 331-341.

Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 89- 109.

Fazio, E (1994). Prosopagnosia can be associated with dam- age confined to the right hemisphere-an MRI and PET study and a review of the literature. Neuropsycbologia,

Farah, M. J. (1994). Specialization within visual object recog- nition: Clues from prosopagnosia and alexia. In M. J. Farah & G. Ratcliff (Eds.), The neuropsycbology of high-level vi- sion (pp. 133-146). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fujita, I., Tanaka, K., Ito, M., & Cheng, K. (1992). Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cor- tex. Nature (London), 360, 343-346.

Gauthier, I., Behrmann, M., Tm, M. J., Anderson, A. W, Gore, J., & McClelland, J. L (1996). Subordinate-level categorization in human inferior temporal cortex: Converging evidence

Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Van Hoesen, G. W (1982).

De Renzi, E., Perani, D., Carlesimo, G. A., Silveri, M. C., &

32, 893-902.

McCartby et al. 609

from neuropsychology and brain imaging. Society for Neu- roscience Abstracts, 22, 8.

Gross, C. G. (1992). Representation of visual stimuli in infe- rior temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B 335, 3- 10.

Harries, M. H., & Perrett, D. I. (1991). Visual processing of faces in temporal cortex: Physiological evidence for modu- lar organization and possible anatomical correlates. Jour- nal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 9-24.

Pietrini, I?, & Grady, C. L. (1994). The functional organiza- tion of human extrastriate cortex: A PET-rCBF study of se- lective attention to faces and locations. Journal of Neuroscience, 14, 6336-6353.

Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Horwitz, B., Rapoport, S. I., & Grady, C. L. (1995). Hemispheric differences in neural sys- tems for face working memory: A PET-rCBF study. Human Brain Mapping, 3, 68-82.

Rapoport, S. I., Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1992). Functional associations among human posterior extrastri- ate brain regions during object and spatial vision. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 31 1-322.

(1996). FMRI reveals distinct extrastriate loci selective for faces and objects. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, 1937.

Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1996). A mod- ule for the visual representation of faces. Neuroimage, 3, S361.

Kanwisher, N., Woods, R. I?, Iacoboni, M., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1997). A locus in human extrastriate cortex for visual shape analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(1),

Kohler, S., Kapur, S., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., & Houle, S. (1995). Dissociation of pathways for object and spatial vision: A PET study in humans. Neuroreport, G, 1865- 1868.

Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement, and depth: Anatomy, physiology, and per- ception. Science, 240, 740-749.

Logothetis, N. K., & Sheinberg, D. L. (1996). Visual object rec- ognition. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 577-621.

Malach, R., Reppas, J. B., Benson, R. R., Kwong, K. K., Jiang, H., Kennedy, W. A., Ledden, P J., Brady, T. J., Rosen, B. R., & Tootell, R. B. H. (1995). Object-related activity revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging in human occipital cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,

McCarthy, G., Puce, A., Gore, J. C., & Allison, T. (1996). On the specificity of neural activation evoked by faces: A func-

Haxby, J. V., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Maisog, J. M.,

Horwitz, B., Grady, C. L., Haxby, J. V., Schapiro, M. B.,

Kanwisher, N., Chun, M. M., McDermott, J., & Hamilton, R

133- 142.

USA, 92, 8135-8139.

tional MRI study. Society for Neuroscience Abstracts, 22, 1937.

nosia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgeg and Psychia-

Merigan, W. H., & Maunsell, J. H. R. (1993). How parallel are

Meadows, J. C. (1974). The anatomical basis of prosopag-

t v , 37, 489-501.

the primate visual pathways? Annual Review of Neurosci- ence, 16, 369-402.

Newcombe, E, Mehta, Z., & de Haan, E. H. E (1994). Category specificity in visual recognition. In M. J. Farah & G. Ratcliff (Eds.), The neuropsychology of high-level vision (pp. 103- 132). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nobre, A. C., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (1994). Word recogni- tion in the human inferior temporal lobe. Nature (Lon- don), 372, 260-263.

(1992). Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 335, 23-30.

(1996). Differential sensitivity of human visual cortex to faces, letterstrings, and textures: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience, 16,

Puce, A., Allison, T., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G. (1995). Face- sensitive regions in human extrastriate cortex studied by functional MRI. Journal of Neuropbysiology, 74, 1192- 1199.

Rhodes, G. (1993). Conligural coding, expertise, and the right hemisphere advantage for face recognition. Brain and Cognition, 22, 19-41.

Schacter, D. L., Reiman, E., Uecker, A,, Polster, M. R., Yun, L. S., & Cooper, L. A. (1995). Brain regions associated with re- trieval of structurally coherent visual information. Nature (London), 376, 587-590.

neuroanatomy of face and object processing: A positron emission tomography study. Brain, 115, 15-36.

Talairach J., & Tournoux, I? (1988). Co-planar stereotm'c at- las of the human brain. New York: Thieme.

Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior: Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Wang, G., Tanaka, K., & Tanifuji, M. (1996). Optical imaging of functional organization in the monkey inferotemporal cor- tex. Science, 272, 1665-1668.

Whiteley, A. M., & Warrington, E. K. (1977). Prosopagnosia: A clinical, psychological, and anatomical study of three pa- tients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgev, and Psychia-

Perrett, D. I., Hietanen, J. K., Oram, M. W., & Benson, I? J.

Puce A,, Allison, T., Asgari, M., Gore, J. C., & McCarthy, G.

5205-52 15.

Sergent, J., Ohta, S., & MacDonald, B. (1992). Functional

*, 40, 395-403.

610 Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Volume 9, Number 5

This article has been cited by:

1. Matthew R. Johnson, Marcia K. Johnson. Top–Down Enhancement and Suppression of Activity in Category-selective ExtrastriateCortex from an Act of Reflective AttentionTop–Down Enhancement and Suppression of Activity in Category-selective ExtrastriateCortex from an Act of Reflective Attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, ahead of print1-8. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

2. Alison Harris, Geoffrey Karl Aguirre. 2008. The Representation of Parts and Wholes in Face-selective CortexThe Representationof Parts and Wholes in Face-selective Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 20:5, 863-878. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

3. Chun-Chia Kung, Jessie J. Peissig, Michael J. Tarr. 2007. Is Region-of-Interest Overlap Comparison a Reliable Measure ofCategory Specificity?Is Region-of-Interest Overlap Comparison a Reliable Measure of Category Specificity?. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 19:12, 2019-2034. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

4. Kartik K. Sreenivasan, Jennifer Katz, Amishi P. Jha. 2007. Temporal Characteristics of Top-Down Modulations during WorkingMemory Maintenance: An Event-related Potential Study of the N170 ComponentTemporal Characteristics of Top-DownModulations during Working Memory Maintenance: An Event-related Potential Study of the N170 Component. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 19:11, 1836-1844. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

5. Maria Ida Gobbini, Aaron C. Koralek, Ronald E. Bryan, Kimberly J. Montgomery, James V. Haxby. 2007. Two Takes on theSocial Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind TasksTwo Takes on the Social Brain: A Comparison of Theory of Mind Tasks.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:11, 1803-1814. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

6. Andrew D. Engell, James V. Haxby, Alexander Todorov. 2007. Implicit Trustworthiness Decisions: Automatic Coding of FaceProperties in the Human AmygdalaImplicit Trustworthiness Decisions: Automatic Coding of Face Properties in the HumanAmygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:9, 1508-1519. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

7. Bruno Rossion, Daniel Collins, Valérie Goffaux, Tim Curran. 2007. Long-term Expertise with Artificial Objects Increases VisualCompetition with Early Face Categorization ProcessesLong-term Expertise with Artificial Objects Increases Visual Competitionwith Early Face Categorization Processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:3, 543-555. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

8. Shlomo Bentin, Joseph M. DeGutis, Mark D'Esposito, Lynn C. Robertson. 2007. Too Many Trees to See the Forest: Performance,Event-related Potential, and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Manifestations of Integrative Congenital ProsopagnosiaTooMany Trees to See the Forest: Performance, Event-related Potential, and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Manifestationsof Integrative Congenital Prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19:1, 132-146. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

9. Galit Yovel, Brad Duchaine. 2006. Specialized Face Perception Mechanisms Extract Both Part and Spacing Information: Evidencefrom Developmental ProsopagnosiaSpecialized Face Perception Mechanisms Extract Both Part and Spacing Information: Evidencefrom Developmental Prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18:4, 580-593. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

10. Cindy M. Bukach , Daniel N. Bub , Isabel Gauthier , Michael J. Tarr . 2006. Perceptual Expertise Effects Are Not All orNone: Spatially Limited Perceptual Expertise for Faces in a Case of ProsopagnosiaPerceptual Expertise Effects Are Not All orNone: Spatially Limited Perceptual Expertise for Faces in a Case of Prosopagnosia. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18:1, 48-63.[Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

11. James P. Morris , Kevin A. Pelphrey , Gregory McCarthy . 2005. Regional Brain Activation Evoked When Approaching a VirtualHuman on a Virtual WalkRegional Brain Activation Evoked When Approaching a Virtual Human on a Virtual Walk. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17:11, 1744-1752. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

12. Galia Avidan , Uri Hasson , Rafael Malach , Marlene Behrmann . 2005. Detailed Exploration of Face-related Processing inCongenital Prosopagnosia: 2. Functional Neuroimaging FindingsDetailed Exploration of Face-related Processing in CongenitalProsopagnosia: 2. Functional Neuroimaging Findings. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17:7, 1150-1167. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDFPlus]

13. Marlene Behrmann , Jonathan Marotta , Isabel Gauthier , Michael J. Tarr , Thomas J. McKeeff . 2005. Behavioral Change andIts Neural Correlates in Visual Agnosia After Expertise TrainingBehavioral Change and Its Neural Correlates in Visual AgnosiaAfter Expertise Training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 17:4, 554-568. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

14. Brad Duchaine, Ken Nakayama. 2005. Dissociations of Face and Object Recognition in Developmental Prosopagnosia. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 17:2, 249-261. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

15. Malia F. Mason, C. Neil Macrae. 2004. Categorizing and Individuating Others: The Neural Substrates of PersonPerceptionCategorizing and Individuating Others: The Neural Substrates of Person Perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience16:10, 1785-1795. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

16. Gillian Rhodes, Graham Byatt, Patricia T. Michie, Aina Puce. 2004. Is the Fusiform Face Area Specialized for Faces, Individuation,or Expert Individuation?Is the Fusiform Face Area Specialized for Faces, Individuation, or Expert Individuation?. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 16:2, 189-203. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

17. Uri Hasson, Galia Avidan, Leon Y. Deouell, Shlomo Bentin, Rafael Malach. 2003. Face-selective Activation in a CongenitalProsopagnosic SubjectFace-selective Activation in a Congenital Prosopagnosic Subject. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:3,419-431. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

18. Ela I. Olivares , Jaime Iglesias , Socorro Rodríguez-Holguín . 2003. Long-Latency ERPs and Recognition of FacialIdentityLong-Latency ERPs and Recognition of Facial Identity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:1, 136-151. [Abstract] [PDF][PDF Plus]

19. S. Campanella , P. Quinet , R. Bruyer , M. Crommelinck , J.-M. Guerit . 2002. Categorical Perception of Happiness and FearFacial Expressions: An ERP StudyCategorical Perception of Happiness and Fear Facial Expressions: An ERP Study. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 14:2, 210-227. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

20. Thad A. Polk , Matthew Stallcup , Geoffrey K. Aguirre , David C. Alsop , Mark D'Esposito , John A. Detre , Martha J. Farah .2002. Neural Specialization for Letter RecognitionNeural Specialization for Letter Recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience14:2, 145-159. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

21. Bruno Rossion, Christine Schiltz, Laurence Robaye, David Pirenne, Marc Crommelinck. 2001. How Does the Brain DiscriminateFamiliar and Unfamiliar Faces?: A PET Study of Face Categorical PerceptionHow Does the Brain Discriminate Familiar andUnfamiliar Faces?: A PET Study of Face Categorical Perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13:7, 1019-1034. [Abstract][PDF] [PDF Plus]

22. Noam Sagiv, Shlomo Bentin. 2001. Structural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-BasedProcessesStructural Encoding of Human and Schematic Faces: Holistic and Part-Based Processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience13:7, 937-951. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

23. K. M. O'Craven, N. Kanwisher. 2000. Mental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific BrainRegionsMental Imagery of Faces and Places Activates Corresponding Stimulus-Specific Brain Regions. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 12:6, 1013-1023. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

24. Alumit Ishai , Leslie G. Ungerleider , Alex Martin , James V. Haxby . 2000. The Representation of Objects in the HumanOccipital and Temporal CortexThe Representation of Objects in the Human Occipital and Temporal Cortex. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience 12:supplement 2, 35-51. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

25. Bruno Rossion , Laurence Dricot , Anne Devolder , Jean-Michel Bodart , Marc Crommelinck , Beatrice de Gelder ,Richard Zoontjes . 2000. Hemispheric Asymmetries for Whole-Based and Part-Based Face Processing in the Human FusiformGyrusHemispheric Asymmetries for Whole-Based and Part-Based Face Processing in the Human Fusiform Gyrus. Journal ofCognitive Neuroscience 12:5, 793-802. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

26. Debra L. Mills, Twyla D. Alvarez, Marie St. George, Lawrence G. Appelbaum, Ursula Bellugi, Helen Neville. 2000. III.Electrophysiological Studies of Face Processing in Williams SyndromeIII. Electrophysiological Studies of Face Processing inWilliams Syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12:supplement 1, 47-64. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

27. Roberto Cabeza , Lars Nyberg . 2000. Imaging Cognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI StudiesImagingCognition II: An Empirical Review of 275 PET and fMRI Studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12:1, 1-47. [Abstract] [PDF][PDF Plus]

28. Susan Murtha , Howard Chertkow , Mario Beauregard , Alan Evans . 1999. The Neural Substrate of Picture NamingThe NeuralSubstrate of Picture Naming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:4, 399-423. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]

29. Isabel Gauthier , Marlene Behrmann , Michael J. Tarr . 1999. Can Face Recognition Really be Dissociated from ObjectRecognition?Can Face Recognition Really be Dissociated from Object Recognition?. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:4,349-370. [Abstract] [PDF] [PDF Plus]