"Everybody suffers": Karen refugee women's experiences and ...

440
"Everybody suffers": Karen refugee women's experiences and conceptualisations of wellbeing and distress Author: Watkins, Paula Grace Publication Date: 2012 DOI: https://doi.org/10.26190/unsworks/15635 License: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/au/ Link to license to see what you are allowed to do with this resource. Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/1959.4/52081 in https:// unsworks.unsw.edu.au on 2022-07-06

Transcript of "Everybody suffers": Karen refugee women's experiences and ...

"Everybody suffers": Karen refugee women's experiences andconceptualisations of wellbeing and distress

Author:Watkins, Paula Grace

Publication Date:2012

DOI:https://doi.org/10.26190/unsworks/15635

License:https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/au/Link to license to see what you are allowed to do with this resource.

Downloaded from http://hdl.handle.net/1959.4/52081 in https://unsworks.unsw.edu.au on 2022-07-06

“Everybody Suffers”:

Karen Refugee Women’s Experiences and Conceptualisations of

Wellbeing and Distress

Paula Grace Watkins

A thesis submitted in fulfilment

of the requirements for the

Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

School of Public Health and Community Medicine

University of New South Wales

Sydney Australia

October 2012

ii

iii

Originality Statement

I hereby declare that this submission is my own work and to the best of my knowledge

it contains no materials previously published or written by another person, or substantial

proportions of material which have been accepted for the award of any other degree or

diploma at UNSW or any other educational institution, except where due

acknowledgement is made in the thesis. Any contribution made to the research by

others, with whom I have worked at UNSW or elsewhere, is explicitly acknowledged in

the thesis. I also declare that the intellectual content of this thesis is the product of my

own work, except to the extent that assistance from others in the project’s design and

conception or in style, presentation and linguistic expression is acknowledged.

Signed …………………………………………

Date ……………………………………………

iv

Copyright Statement

I hereby grant the University of New South Wales or its agents the right to archive and

to make available my thesis or dissertation in whole or part in the University libraries in

all forms of media, now or here after known, subject to the provisions of the Copyright

Act 1968. I retain all proprietary rights, such as patent rights. I also retain the right to

use in future works (such as articles or books) all or part of this thesis or dissertation.

I also authorise University Microfilms to use the 350 word abstract of my thesis in

Dissertation Abstract International (this is applicable to doctoral theses only).

I have either used no substantial portions of copyright material in my thesis or I have

obtained permission to use copyright material; where permission has not been granted I

have applied/will apply for a partial restriction of the digital copy of my thesis or

dissertation.

Signed …………………………………………

Date ………………………………… …………

Authenticity Statement

I certify that the Library deposit digital copy is a direct equivalent of the final officially

approved version of my thesis. No emendation of content has occurred and if there are

any minor variations in formatting, they are the result of the conversion to digital

format.

Signed …………………………………………

Date ……………………………………………

v

Dedication

Here we live in a country that is peaceful, we don’t have to worry for anything

at all. But we worry for those left behind. We wonder how they live. We

wonder what they eat. (Mëh Luh)

This thesis is dedicated to “those left behind”, to those who struggle(d) for peace in

Burma, and to the participants and Karen community in Sydney who shared their

stories, lives and knowledge with me.

vi

Why do we tend to think in dichotomies? What is the primal factor that makes

us prefer black or white to grey, ecstatic sin or salvation to mundane daily

indecision, fervent conviction or denial to reasoned uncertainty, friend or

enemy to neutrality? Whatever it is, it often drives people to hatred and warfare

in the interpersonal and political spheres, as well as to intellectual confusion in

medicine, philosophy, and science. This love of simply splitting into extreme

groups tends to lead us to think not only in dichotomies, but also in false

dichotomies. (Andreasen, 1995, p. 963)

vii

Acknowledgements

A strong and supportive team stands behind this doctoral thesis.

I am deeply grateful to both of my supervisors, Dr Husna Razee and Associate

Professor Juliet Richters, for their expertise and astute intellectual guidance, patience

and continual encouragement. Thank you for encouraging me to be curious and

confident.

Professional editor, Dr G. A Plum, provided copyediting and proofreading services,

according to the guidelines laid out in the university-endorsed national guidelines, “The

editing of research theses by professional editors”. I wish to thank him for his technical

expertise and help.

I wish to also mention the considerable support and encouragement from other members

of the School of Public Health and Community Medicine. Special thanks to Associate

Professor Jan Ritchie, Dr Bibiana Chan and Dr Catherine Bateman Steel – thank you for

your time and valuable feedback.

To the Karen community in Sydney – ta blu doh ma. Thank you for your warmth,

generosity, trust and patience.

To my family, thank you for your love, for your tolerance in accepting my fascinations

and pursuits, and for your grace in understanding what is important to me.

To James, who witnessed all manner of joys and anxieties in the creation of this thesis.

Who watched me get stuck and unstuck and who all the while remained unwavering in

his belief in me and in my work. Thank you for your immeasurable support. Thank you

for your love.

viii

Publications

Publications arising from this thesis

Watkins, P., Razee, H., and Richters, J. (In press, accepted 14 March, 2012). “I’m

telling you … the language barrier is the most, the biggest challenge”: Barriers to

education among Karen refugee women in Australia. Australian Journal of Education.

Conference presentations arising from this thesis

Watkins, P. G., Razee, H., and Worth, H. (2009). Psychosocial distress and wellbeing

amongst Karen refugee women. Paper presented at the School of Public Health and

Community Medicine Postgraduate Research Student Conference, 23 November, 2009.

Sydney, Australia.

Watkins, P. G., Razee, H., and Worth, H. (2010). Ethnographic methods in cross-

cultural mental health research with refugee communities. Paper presented at the XXth

Congress of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology, 7–10 July,

2010. Melbourne, Australia.

Watkins, P. G., Razee, H., and Richters, J. (2010). Researching sensitive topics:

Reflexivity, ethics and a feminist perspective. Paper presented at the School of Public

Health and Community Medicine Postgraduate Research Student Conference, 22

October, 2010. Sydney, Australia.

Watkins, P. G., Razee, H., and Richters, J. (2011). ‘Feeling useless’: Listening to

refugee women. Paper presented at the International Society of Critical Health

Psychology 7th Biennial Conference, 18–20 April, 2011. Adelaide, Australia.

Watkins, P. G., Zu, D., Razee, H., and Richters, J. (2011). So you want to be useful.

Paper presented at the 2011 Refugee Conference, 14–17 June, 2011. Sydney, Australia.

ix

Table of Contents

Originality Statement.................................................................................................... iii

Copyright Statement ...................................................................................................... iv

Authenticity Statement .................................................................................................. iv

Dedication ..................................................................................................................... v

Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................ vii

Publications ................................................................................................................. viii

Table of Contents ........................................................................................................... ix

List of Figures ............................................................................................................ xviii

List of Images ............................................................................................................ xviii

List of Tables ............................................................................................................. xviii

Abstract .................................................................................................................. xix

List of Acronyms .......................................................................................................... xxi

Glossary of Karen Terms ........................................................................................... xxii

Chapter 1 Introduction ................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Background .......................................................................................................... 1

1.2 Aims of the study and research questions ............................................................ 3

1.3 Significance of the study ...................................................................................... 4

1.4 Locating the researcher ........................................................................................ 4

1.4.1 Development of topic and perspective .......................................................... 5

1.4.2 Educational and professional background .................................................... 5

1.4.3 Personal background ..................................................................................... 6

1.5 Thesis structure .................................................................................................... 6

Chapter 2 The Mental Wellbeing of Refugees .............................................................. 8

2.1 Introduction .......................................................................................................... 8

2.2 Refugees ............................................................................................................... 8

2.2.1 Current crises and solutions .......................................................................... 9

2.3 War and the psyche ............................................................................................ 11

2.4 Refugee mental health ........................................................................................ 12

2.4.1 The dominant discourse: trauma-focused psychiatric epidemiology .......... 13

2.4.2 The dominant debates: cross-cultural construct validity and category

fallacies ...................................................................................................... 19

2.4.3 Intervention issues: practical, clinical and ethical challenges ..................... 26

x

2.4.4 Developments and directions for research: the emic-etic evolution ........... 30

2.5 Addressing the gaps ........................................................................................... 35

Chapter 3 Setting the Scene: Burma and the Karen ................................................. 37

3.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................ 37

3.1.1 Burma, Burman, Burmese and Myanmar: a note on terms and usage ........ 37

3.2 Burma: an overview ........................................................................................... 37

3.2.1 Politics and power ....................................................................................... 40

3.2.2 Human rights abuses and life under the regime .......................................... 43

3.2.3 Religion and culture in Burma .................................................................... 46

3.3 Karen .................................................................................................................. 48

3.3.1 Language ..................................................................................................... 49

3.3.2 Karen culture ............................................................................................... 50

3.3.3 Karen religion ............................................................................................. 51

3.3.4 Karen traditional medicine .......................................................................... 52

3.4 The Thailand-Burma border ............................................................................... 53

3.4.1 Hosts and providers ..................................................................................... 55

3.4.2 Life in camps ............................................................................................... 57

3.4.3 Access to resettlement ................................................................................. 61

Chapter 4 The Local Context: Refugee Resettlement in Australia .......................... 62

4.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................ 62

4.2 Australia’s current refugee program .................................................................. 63

4.2.1 Onshore component .................................................................................... 63

4.2.2 Offshore component .................................................................................... 64

4.3 Initial resettlement services for refugees............................................................ 65

4.3.1 Pre-departure ............................................................................................... 65

4.3.2 Post-arrival: the humanitarian settlement services program ....................... 67

4.4 Ongoing settlement services .............................................................................. 69

4.5 The local scene ................................................................................................... 72

Chapter 5 Methodology ................................................................................................ 74

5.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................ 74

5.2 A critical realist approach .................................................................................. 72

5.2.1 Explanation and justification of a qualitative realist approach ................... 74

5.3 Theoretical perspectives ..................................................................................... 78

5.3.1 Ethnography ................................................................................................ 79

xi

5.3.2 Feminist perspective.................................................................................... 81

5.3.3 Grounded theory.......................................................................................... 84

5.4 Summary ............................................................................................................ 85

Chapter 6 Method ......................................................................................................... 86

6.1 Introduction ........................................................................................................ 86

6.2 Selecting participants ......................................................................................... 86

6.2.1 Karen women .............................................................................................. 87

6.2.2 Key informants ............................................................................................ 87

6.2.3 Saturation .................................................................................................... 87

6.3 Participant observation ....................................................................................... 88

6.4 Interviews ........................................................................................................... 88

6.4.1 In-depth interviews ..................................................................................... 88

6.4.2 Group interviews ......................................................................................... 89

6.4.3 Recording, interpreting and transcribing interviews ................................... 91

6.5 Ethics .................................................................................................................. 92

6.6 Data analysis ...................................................................................................... 93

6.7 Rigour ................................................................................................................. 95

6.7.1 Criterion 1: Clarification, justification or theoretical rigour ....................... 96

6.7.2 Criterion 2: Procedural and methodological rigour .................................... 96

6.7.3 Criterion 3: Representativeness .................................................................. 96

6.7.4 Criterion 4: Interpretative rigour ................................................................. 97

6.7.5 Criterion 5: Reflexivity and evaluative rigour ............................................ 98

6.7.6 Criterion 6: Transferability ......................................................................... 98

6.8 Logistics and limits .......................................................................................... 100

6.9 Conclusion ....................................................................................................... 100

Chapter 7 The Ethnographic Journey: A Researcher’s Reflections ...................... 101

7.1 Introduction ...................................................................................................... 101

7.2 Identifying and entering the community .......................................................... 101

7.3 Identifying and working with a bilingual research assistant ............................ 103

7.4 Informal participant observation encounters .................................................... 106

7.5 Formal interview encounters ............................................................................ 109

7.5.1 Formal encounters with groups ................................................................. 113

7.6 Transcribing speech and presenting quotations ............................................... 117

7.7 Ethics ................................................................................................................ 118

xii

7.7.1 Maintaining confidentiality and anonymity .............................................. 120

7.8 The analytic journey ......................................................................................... 120

7.9 The reflexive journey ....................................................................................... 123

7.9.1 Memo-writing ........................................................................................... 123

7.9.2 Learning patience and being honest .......................................................... 125

7.9.3 Distance and the “other” ........................................................................... 125

7.9.4 Representation and voice .......................................................................... 127

7.9.5 “Conversations with Karen”: positively using researcher power and the

reflexive journey ...................................................................................... 129

7.10 Ending the study ............................................................................................. 130

Chapter 8 “Starting at zero”: Women’s Problems and the Sources of Distress ... 131

8.1 Introduction ...................................................................................................... 131

8.2 “The biggest barrier”: language and communication....................................... 131

8.2.1 Barriers to language education .................................................................. 132

8.2.2 Barriers to communication ........................................................................ 134

8.3 “Everything is new”: lack of environmental mastery ...................................... 135

8.3.1 Mastering space: fear of freedom .............................................................. 136

8.3.2 (Un)employment ....................................................................................... 138

8.3.3 Financial hardship ..................................................................................... 141

8.3.4 Housing ..................................................................................................... 143

8.3.5 Transport ................................................................................................... 145

8.3.6 Health ........................................................................................................ 146

8.3.7 Parenting ................................................................................................... 146

8.4 “Power struggles”: gender role changes and women’s wellbeing ................... 150

8.4.1 “Too much responsibility”: increasing pressures on women .................... 151

8.4.2 “Not enough responsibility”: men’s experience of gender role changes .. 152

8.4.3 Domestic violence ..................................................................................... 153

8.4.4 Witches and bitches: single women negotiating gender ........................... 156

8.5 War, culture and daily stressors: women’s problems in context ...................... 159

8.6 Summary and reflections.................................................................................. 161

Chapter 9 A Suffering Heart-mind: Conceptualising, Experiencing and

Explaining Distress ................................................................................ 165

9.1 Introduction ...................................................................................................... 165

9.2 A suffering heart-mind: women’s idioms of distress ....................................... 165

xiii

9.2.1 Thinking too much: hso k’moe dha ........................................................... 165

9.2.2 A suffering heart-mind: tha… ................................................................... 166

9.2.3 Sickness of the heart-mind: tha dha hsar .................................................. 167

9.2.4 Madness: dha blu ...................................................................................... 168

9.3 Causes of distress ............................................................................................. 168

9.3.1 Daily stressors ........................................................................................... 169

9.3.2 Persecution and forced migration: loss and traumatic war stressors ......... 170

9.3.3 Biology and inherited vulnerability .......................................................... 172

9.3.4 Supernatural .............................................................................................. 173

9.4 Experiencing and expressing distress............................................................... 173

9.4.1 Aching heads and aching bodies: somatic complaints .............................. 173

9.4.2 “I have a problem”: social complaints and social expressions ................. 175

9.4.3 “I am useless”: expressing failure and frustration..................................... 177

9.4.4 “I remember”: remembering suffering ...................................................... 179

9.4.5 “I want to die by crying”: severe distress .................................................. 181

9.4.6 Feeling fierce: tha dha hsar and anger ...................................................... 182

9.4.7 “Eating grass”: manifestations of madness (dha blu) ............................... 185

9.5 Summary and reflections.................................................................................. 185

9.5.1 Trajectory of distress ................................................................................. 185

9.5.2 Similarities and differences ....................................................................... 187

9.5.3 The ubiquity of uselessness ....................................................................... 188

9.5.4 The importance of anger ........................................................................... 190

9.5.5 Balancing ethnopsychiatries...................................................................... 190

Chapter 10 We All Suffer. We All Cope: Managing Emotional Distress .............. 193

10.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 193

10.2 Self-control ..................................................................................................... 193

10.2.1 Thought control: “think about your thinking” ......................................... 193

10.2.2 Distraction: “think about something else” ............................................... 194

10.2.3 Acceptance: “you have to accept reality” ................................................ 195

10.2.4 Contextualising a controlled self ............................................................. 196

10.3 Collective self ................................................................................................. 200

10.3.1 The collective self and communal suffering: accepting and normalising

suffering ................................................................................................... 200

10.3.2 The collective self and communal support: “we support one another” ... 203

xiv

10.3.3 “Sharing and caring”: helping others to help oneself .............................. 206

10.4 Religious coping ............................................................................................. 209

10.4.1 “Through God’s strength” ....................................................................... 209

10.4.2 Religious community ............................................................................... 210

10.5 Professional support: coping and service providers ........................................ 212

10.5.1 Doctors ..................................................................................................... 213

10.5.2 Australian service providers .................................................................... 214

10.6 Summary and reflections ................................................................................ 215

10.6.1 Emotional and practical support .............................................................. 217

10.6.2 Shared experiences .................................................................................. 218

10.6.3 Acceptance and normalisation ................................................................. 219

Chapter 11 Bridging Idioms: Karen Women’s Explanatory Models of Western

Psychiatric Constructs ........................................................................... 222

11.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 222

11.2 Vignettes and explanatory models .................................................................. 222

11.3 Vignette A / Aunty A: DSM-IV Major Depressive Disorder ......................... 222

11.4 Vignette B / Aunty B: DSM-IV Panic Disorder ............................................. 227

11.5 Vignette C / Aunty C: DSM-IV Posttraumatic Stress Disorder ..................... 230

11.6 Summary ......................................................................................................... 233

11.6.1 Social model ............................................................................................ 233

11.6.2 Psychological model ................................................................................ 235

11.6.3 Biological model ...................................................................................... 237

11.6.4 Course, severity and impact ..................................................................... 238

11.6.5 Explanatory models and treatment .......................................................... 238

11.7 Reflections ...................................................................................................... 239

11.7.1 The ordinariness of suffering ................................................................... 241

11.7.2 Talking about trauma ............................................................................... 243

11.7.3 Illuminating diversity ............................................................................... 244

Chapter 12 Invisible Clients and Invisible Problems: Experiences and Perceptions

of Settlement Services ............................................................................ 246

12.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 246

12.2 Positive perceptions ........................................................................................ 246

12.2.1 “Here it’s the best” ................................................................................... 246

12.2.2 Community connection ............................................................................ 247

xv

12.3 Learning about limitations .............................................................................. 247

12.4 Case management and orientation support ..................................................... 248

12.5 Communication issues .................................................................................... 248

12.5.1 Not Korean, not Burmese ........................................................................ 249

12.5.2 (Mis)trust and ethnic tension ................................................................... 250

12.5.3 Interpreter availability .............................................................................. 251

12.6 Lack of bilingual, bicultural and female service providers ............................. 252

12.7 Barriers to English language education .......................................................... 253

12.7.1 Access barriers ......................................................................................... 253

12.7.2 Classroom and teaching barriers .............................................................. 254

12.8 Face value: limited perceptions of Karen women’s needs .............................. 257

12.9 Elder and service provider burnout … “the profacticals” ............................... 259

12.10Lack of awareness, understanding and cohesion between service providers . 261

12.11Summary ......................................................................................................... 262

Chapter 13 “It’s good … but it’s not very effective”: Experiences and Perceptions

of Health and Mental Health Service Provisions ................................ 264

13.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 264

13.2 “Extra special” care from GPs ........................................................................ 264

13.3 Primary health and healthcare services ........................................................... 265

13.3.1 Encountering healthcare and illness ........................................................ 265

13.3.2 Mismatched expectations ......................................................................... 265

13.3.3 “They should be fine”: communication barriers and GPs ....................... 269

13.4 Mental health services: cultural differences and paradigm clashes ................ 272

13.5 Reflections ...................................................................................................... 280

Chapter 14 Implications for Practice and Future Research ................................... 283

14.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 283

14.2 Implications and recommendations for generalist HSS service providers ..... 283

14.2.1 Case management and resettlement orientation ....................................... 283

14.2.2 Non-Karen speaking service providers .................................................... 284

14.2.3 Adult Migrant English Program .............................................................. 285

14.2.4 Poverty and housing ................................................................................. 286

14.2.5 Employment support services .................................................................. 287

14.2.6 Parenting .................................................................................................. 287

14.3 Implications and recommendations for mental health service providers ........ 287

xvi

14.3.1 Prioritise (a) prevention over treatment and (b) issues that are of concern

to the community ..................................................................................... 288

14.3.2 Build individual and community capacity and self-efficacy ................... 288

14.3.3 Work with existing community strengths and integrate interventions into

existing support networks ........................................................................ 289

14.3.4 Engage communities in critical reflection ............................................... 289

14.3.5 Address gender and work with women .................................................... 290

14.3.6 Enhance staff cultural competency training ............................................. 290

14.3.7 Improve access to bilingual, bicultural and gender-matched healthcare

staff .......................................................................................................... 291

14.3.8 Evaluate intervention and service provider efficacy ................................ 291

14.3.9 Utilise practice-based evidence ............................................................... 292

14.3.10Design culturally congruent mental health interventions ........................ 292

14.4 Implications and recommendations for future research .................................. 294

14.4.1 Use explanatory models to further explore mental health topics with

refugee women ......................................................................................... 294

14.4.2 Work sensitively with refugee communities and strive to access emic

perspectives .............................................................................................. 295

14.4.3 Investigate the roles and challenges of refugee community leaders ........ 295

14.4.4 Explore notions of power(lessness), use(lessness), dependency and

identity ..................................................................................................... 295

14.4.5 Investigate the strengths, limitations and pathways of emic coping

strategies .................................................................................................. 296

14.4.6 Explore pathways and patterns of refugee adjustment to resettlement

overtime ................................................................................................... 299

Chapter 15 Conclusions .............................................................................................. 300

15.1 Introduction ..................................................................................................... 300

15.2 Women’s conceptualisations of distress and the transferability of Western

theories ............................................................................................................ 300

15.3 Risk, resiliency and wellbeing in social, cultural and political context .......... 303

15.4 Coping: the commonsense, complex and contradictory ................................. 308

15.5 Paradigm clashes and practical problems ....................................................... 310

15.6 Final reflections .............................................................................................. 311

References ................................................................................................................. 314

xvii

Appendices ................................................................................................................. 386

Appendix A: DSM-IV criteria .................................................................................. 387

Appendix B: Interview guide for Karen key informants .......................................... 391

Appendix C: Interview guide for service providers .................................................. 393

Appendix D: Interview guide for Karen women....................................................... 397

Appendix E: Participant information and consent form (English version only) ....... 403

Appendix F: Group interview guide ......................................................................... 408

Appendix G: Conversations with Karen: art exhibition ............................................ 414

xviii

List of Figures

Figure 1: Map of Burma illustrating ethnic minority areas including Karen State ..... 39

Figure 2: Map of internally displaced people and refugee camps along Thailand-

Burma border ............................................................................................... 55

Figure 3: Four-point participant observer continuum ................................................ 106

Figure 4: Trajectory of distress .................................................................................. 186

Figure 5: Trajectory of distress .................................................................................. 233

Figure 6: Karen social, cultural and political context and associated negative

psychosocial sequela .................................................................................. 306

List of Images

Image 1: Complete storyboard regarding employment difficulties........................... 116

Image 2: Storyboard panel depicting women’s isolation .......................................... 116

Image 3: Storyboard panel depicting Karen women’s difficulties assisting their

children with homework ............................................................................ 117

Image 4: Cross-stitch depicting quotation from interview with participant Mr W. .. 129

Image 5: Karen women’s visual depiction of the emotional impact of

(un)employment and interactions with Job Services Australia .................. 140

Image 6: Picture of a salad – used by women to make an analogy between

nutritional value and the value inherent in women .................................... 178

List of Tables

Table 1: Local service providers relevant to this study .............................................. 73

Table 2: Karen women’s trajectory of idioms of distress with causes and

descriptions ................................................................................................ 192

xix

Abstract

Studies of refugee mental health have largely focused on epidemiological assessments

of posttraumatic stress disorder and other diagnoses using Western-derived assessment

measures. However, research has increasingly highlighted the role of culture in shaping

refugee mental wellbeing and suggests that local idioms of distress may be more salient

to refugee populations than Western-derived constructs of psychopathology.

The central aim of the study was to explore experiences and perceptions of suffering

and distress amongst Karen refugee women in Sydney, Australia. The Karen, an ethnic

group from Burma, have been gravely affected by decades of conflict and human rights

abuses in their homeland yet there is a dearth of information concerning their

psychosocial needs.

Informed by a critical realist theoretical paradigm, the research was guided by

ethnographic, feminist and grounded theory methodological traditions. Data was drawn

from over 200 hours of ethnographic observations and 67 interviews with Karen women

and key informants between 2009 and 2011. Data analysis was based on principles of

grounded theory.

The findings revealed important relationships between pre-migration cultural context,

persecutory experiences (including but not limited to trauma), post-migration

psychosocial stressors and current experiences of emotional distress. Women’s daily

social and material stressors, such as interacting with service providers, were central to

their distress. They conceptualised their experiences of suffering and trauma within

cultural frameworks and idioms of distress, which often differed from the Western

psychiatric models. Karen women primarily coped through traditional community and

religious networks. Language was a key barrier to seeking support from Australian

service providers. Women’s timidity and communication patterns, stemming from the

interplay of cultural, gendered and pre-migration experiences, were further barriers.

The study contributes to an empirical understanding of Karen refugee women’s

experiences and conceptualisations of mental wellbeing. It enhances existing knowledge

and developments in cross-cultural mental health and adds a new dimension by

demonstrating how pre-migration culture, refugee-specific migration experiences, post-

migration difficulties and wellbeing were linked through the interconnected issues of

xx

power(lessness), use(fulness) and identity. The findings have implications for service

provision and suggest that Western mental health practitioners should reflexively

explore prevailing discourses concerning traumatic suffering and healing.

xxi

List of Acronyms

AMEP Adult Migrant English Program

CBT Cognitive behavioural therapy

DIAC Department of Immigration and Citizenship

DSM Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

EM Explanatory model

FAAST Forum of Australian Services for Survivors of Torture and Trauma

GDP Gross domestic product

GP General practitioner

HSS Humanitarian Settlement Services

ICD-10 International Classification of Diseases-10

NAATI Australian National Accreditation Authority for Translators and

Interpreters Limited

PTSD Posttraumatic stress disorder

STARTTS Service for the Treatment and Rehabilitation of Torture and Trauma

Survivors

TIS Translating and Interpreting Service

UN United Nations

UNHCR United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees

xxii

Glossary of Karen Terms

Dha blu Mad

Hso k’moe dha Thinking too much

Tha Heart

Heart-mind

Tha ba do Hurt

Tha ba yo Worry

Tha da mu Upset

Tha dhar hsar Sickness of the heart

Sickness of the heart-mind

1

Chapter 1

Introduction

We start at zero and go – one, two, three, four. (Key informant – Mr O)

“Starting at zero” is a useful phrase to situate the experiences of Karen refugee women

who, after fleeing persecution in Burma, begin their life anew in Australia. This

ethnographic study explores Karen women’s experiences and conceptualisations of

emotional distress and wellbeing, and addresses concerns and issues that have been

raised in the literature relating to the application of Western psychiatric frameworks for

refugee mental health.

1.1 Background

For the past 25 years, mental health researchers have documented the devastating

effects of political violence and forced migration on the wellbeing of affected

populations (Porter & Haslam, 2005). The majority of these studies have focused on

epidemiological assessments of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and major

depressive disorder using Western-derived assessment measures and have consistently

reported elevated rates of these and other psychiatric disorders (Hollifield, 2005;

Hollifield et al., 2002; Miller, Kulkarni, & Kushner, 2006).

However, psychiatric scholarship of refugee populations has increasingly come under

intense criticism from both ideological (Summerfield, 1999) and methodological

(Hollifield, 2005; Hollifield et al., 2002) perspectives. Despite the plethora of

epidemiological studies, the findings and implications have been difficult to interpret

due to methodological problems and cultural differences in conceptualisations of

wellbeing, suffering and healing. Critics have argued that research to date has been too

narrow in its nearly exclusive reliance on a biomedical, epidemiological and trauma-

focused approach (Bracken, Giller, & Summerfield, 1995; Ingleby, 2010; Miller,

Kulkarni, et al., 2006). It has been argued that such an approach to understanding and

addressing human suffering and wellbeing represents an ethnocentric imposition of an

“etic” or outsider’s (in this case Western) epistemology on predominantly non-Western

populations and assumes the universal salience of psychiatric constructs (Summerfield,

2002). Critics such as Miller, Kulkarni, et al. (2006) and others (de Jong & Komproe,

2

2002; Pupavac, 2004; Wilson, 2007) have also argued that the dominant trauma-focused

discourse has resulted in culturally inappropriate and unsustainable rehabilitation

services that do not address issues of relevance to the experiences and interpretations of

refugee communities.

In recent years some researchers have begun to utilise more qualitative ethnographic

methods to identify the range, depth and meaning of mental health responses amongst

refugees (Djuretic, Crawford, & Weaver, 2007; Hubbard & Pearson, 2004; Miller,

Kulkarni, et al., 2006; Phan, Steel, & Silove, 2004). These qualitative studies have

allowed for an identification of emic or insider’s beliefs, local syndromes and idioms of

distress, as well as an understanding of strengths, forms of resilience and patterns of

help-seeking behaviour.

The ethnographically informed research to date has demonstrated that local idioms of

distress may be more salient to indigenous populations than constructs of

psychopathology identified by Western-derived diagnostic systems. Mental health

practitioners have demonstrated how these studies have the potential to assist with the

development and evaluation of psychosocial and mental health interventions that are

both clinically effective and contextually appropriate (Hubbard & Pearson, 2004).

Moreover, many of the qualitative approaches to refugee mental health research also

give voice to the refugee experience and allow for the rich texture of human life,

suffering and meaning to be heard and appreciated (Mollica, 2001).

Increasingly, rather than seen as being in opposition, ethnographic and epidemiological

approaches to refugee mental health research have been viewed as mutually supportive

(Bass, Bolton, & Murray, 2007; de Jong & Van Ommeren, 2002; Eisenbruch, de Jong,

& van de Put, 2004; Patel, 2001). From these approaches a consensus has emerged

identifying ethnographic investigations as a necessary first step towards developing

accurate, as well as culturally and contextually appropriate, bio-psychosocial

understandings of refugee communities (Petevi, Revel, & Jacobs, 2001).

There have been a variety of creative and effective solutions from researchers

committed to understanding the emic perspectives of refugee communities (Bolton &

Tang, 2002, 2004; Miller, Omidian, et al., 2006; Phan et al., 2004). However, there have

also been some consistent limitations in research to date as well as unexplored and

3

potentially useful methodologies. Limitations include an insufficient exploration and

acknowledgement of the broader sociopolitical issues affecting refugee communities, a

failure to utilise qualitative (especially ethnographic) methods as a means to explore

local explanatory models of mental illness and a paucity of information concerning

utilisation of relevant services (Summerfield, 2001), combined with a failure to report

on and involve a range of relevant service providers in order to guide policy-relevant

research. This study was undertaken to address these knowledge gaps, explore Karen

women’s emic perspectives about distress and to understand the circumstances affecting

their psychosocial wellbeing from multiple perspectives. Thus, this study provides

empirical information about Karen refugee women, a refugee population whose

wellbeing had not been explored.

The Karen people are an ethnic minority population from Burma. Following a military

coup in 1962 around 3.5 million citizens in Burma have been displaced (Refugees

International, 2010) by violence, persecution and human rights abuses including torture

and extrajudicial murder, forced labour and sexual violence against women (United

Nations General Assembly, 2010a). Since 2005 approximately 90,000 refugees from

Burma have been resettled around the world and around 10,000 now reside in Australia

(Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2011c; International Organization for

Migration, 2011b). Karen refugees represent the largest ethnic minority community of

refugees from Burma in Sydney, where this study was undertaken (M. Salgado,

Department of Immigration and Citizenship, personal communication, 25 November,

2009).

1.2 Aims of the study and research questions

The aims of this thesis are to (a) explore Karen refugee women’s experiences and

perceptions of suffering and distress, (b) analyse the women’s major psychosocial needs

within the Australian context, and (c) frame recommendations for action by community

organisations and services. The following questions guided this study:

1. How do Karen refugee women conceptualise emotional distress?

2. What issues affect Karen women’s wellbeing?

3. How do Karen women cope with emotional distress?

4

4. What are the explanatory models (beliefs regarding illness) provided by Karen

women concerning the Western psychiatric constructs of posttraumatic stress

disorder, depression and anxiety?

5. How are some of the current services provided for Karen refugee women

perceived and how might they be improved?

1.3 Significance of the study

Progress in improving the psychosocial wellbeing of refugee populations in Australia

depends on culturally accurate understandings and assessments of the problems that

need to be addressed (Kleinman, 1977, 1999; Miller & Rasco, 2004). However, there is

a dearth of knowledge regarding the experiences, needs and perceptions of the growing

population of Karen refugees from Burma in Australia. In the absence of information

about the psychosocial needs of this population there is no firm basis on which to

design programs and assess program effectiveness. This study will address this gap in

the literature and provide information to inform service providers and policy makers.

This study draws on and advances contemporary approaches to refugee mental health

research. It contributes to research and knowledge through demonstrating an approach

that integrates the experiences and perceptions of both refugee community members and

relevant service providers. The framework and methodology employed aims to gather

detailed descriptions of mental health-related needs as experienced by Karen refugee

women as well as other knowledgeable persons, such as community leaders and service

providers. Furthermore, this study employs a unique approach to utilising the

explanatory model framework from medical anthropology to understand Karen

women’s perceptions of Western psychiatric constructs. This approach allows for the

integration of emic and etic perspectives, which is essential for the development of

epidemiology studies as well as for the effective delivery of health services. The

findings will be critically discussed in relation to aspects of dominant discourses about

refugee mental health and current approaches to service provision.

1.4 Locating the researcher

This study is situated in a naturalistic, qualitative approach to research. In this tradition

researchers seek to enhance the transparency and rigour of their research through the

process of reflexivity in which they clarify how their personal beliefs, values and

5

experiences impact on the intention, implementation and interpretation of their research

(Abreu, 2001; Blignault & Ritchie, 2009; Denzin & Lincoln, 2005; Ritchie, 2001). This

section explores my personal values and prior experiences contributing to the

development of this thesis topic so as to bring about a “thoughtful, conscious self-

awareness” (Finlay, 2002, p. 532) and facilitate greater transparency of my research. A

reflexive account of the research journey is presented in Chapter 7.

1.4.1 Development of topic and perspective

Ideas for this research began to form in the winter of 2008 when I spent several months

at a locally run refugee hospital on the Thailand-Burma border assisting staff with

mental health program planning. During my time there I listened to the concerns of

local counselling staff, and observed the serious challenges they faced as they tried to

implement Western-style trauma counselling en masse to a population unfamiliar with

the concept and unwilling or unable to avail themselves of the service. The local staff

were part of a project implemented and coordinated by a Western mental health

professional who had not obtained any ethical approval and was advocating the use of

what was at the time a highly contested form of psychotherapy. The intervention, eye-

movement desensitisation therapy, had no supportive evidence regarding its use in non-

Western post-conflict settings. Moreover, a United Nations (UN) evaluation report

(Melville & Faye, 2003) argued that the technique was “not appropriate” (p. 7) in such

contexts, following field research with survivors of armed conflict in Indonesia. My

interest in undertaking research was generated by my commitment to an ethic of care

and the pressing need to understand and address the emotional experiences and needs of

refugees from a cross-cultural perspective.

1.4.2 Educational and professional background

I bring to this research perspectives and experiences from my training and work as a

clinical psychologist in New Zealand and abroad. I graduated from a firmly positivist-

oriented psychology department at the University of Otago, New Zealand, where I was

trained within the scientist-practitioner model. I completed my clinical psychology

degree and embarked on a quantitative investigation of cognitive deficits related to

schizophrenia for my master’s thesis. I was also interested in cultures and world

religions and during my undergraduate years took numerous classes through the

6

Religious Studies department. I became especially interested in South East Asia and

was fascinated by how cultural and religious concepts related to the experience of

mental wellbeing and the treatment of mental illness. After completing my clinical

training in 2005 I worked in a variety of contexts including hospitals, prisons and

private practice. In more recent years I worked with humanitarian organisations in

Cambodia and, as mentioned, on the Thailand-Burma border.

1.4.3 Personal background

Unlike many of the Karen women in this study I was fortunate to spend my childhood

in a country that enjoyed a time of peace. I was born to a middle class family in New

Zealand and spent my childhood busily engaged in educational, family-oriented, and

recreational pursuits. I was aware of mental illness from a young age through the

challenges of schizophrenia faced by members of my family and decided to study for a

career in mental healthcare.

Whilst working as a clinical psychologist in New Zealand I became involved in an

organisation assisting women and child survivors of human trafficking in Cambodia. I

left my job in New Zealand and travelled to South East Asia when an opportunity arose

to assist their team with developing ways to evaluate the outcome of their treatment

program and improve their procedures for reporting to donors. During my time in Asia I

travelled to the Thailand-Burma border and thus began my interest in working with

refugees from Burma.

My values and background have shaped the construction of this research project and I

acknowledge that other researchers, working from different perspectives, would define

and construct this project in different ways. I have reviewed my personal values and

prior experiences so as to make them a conscious element in designing, analysing and

presenting the research findings.

1.5 Thesis structure

This introductory chapter has provided a brief background to the study and introduced

the aims and research questions. The following second chapter critically explores the

field of refugee mental health and discusses the various conceptual issues and debates

that have shaped research to date. It concludes by locating this study within

7

contemporary refugee mental health literature. Chapter 3 focuses upon the background

of Karen refugee women and explores the geographic, cultural, social and political

characteristics of Burma and the Karen. An overview of the local (Australian) context is

provided in Chapter 4 where the resettlement services available to refugee entrants are

described. In Chapters 5 and 6 the research methodology and methods are detailed,

followed by an exploration of how the ethnographic journey unfolded in Chapter 7.

Chapters 8 to 13 present the research findings. These chapters loosely correspond to

each of the research questions. Each presents the relevant data and explores the findings

in relation to the literature. Chapter 14 addresses implications for resettlement service

providers and health practitioners respectively and provides recommendations for future

research. Lastly, Chapter 15 concludes the thesis, reflecting on the major findings and

implications in relation to both theory and academic discourse as well as service

provision.

8

Chapter 2

The Mental Wellbeing of Refugees

2.1 Introduction

This chapter explores the history of mental health research with refugee populations. It

begins with a sketch of the refugee phenomenon, outlining the definition of a refugee

and briefly describing the current refugee crisis. Historical conceptualisations of war-

related emotional suffering are explored briefly in Section 2.3, providing a context for

understanding contemporary approaches. Section 2.4 explores in detail the predominant

psychiatric trauma-focused approach guiding refugee mental health research and

practice, noting the key findings and contributions of this tradition. The historical

context in which the study of trauma came to be prioritised will be examined, as will the

limitations of the framework. More recent trends in approaches and methodologies will

be discussed including the contribution of medical anthropology frameworks. Finally,

areas that have received comparatively little attention will be highlighted in Section 2.5

and their significance for this study noted.

2.2 Refugees

Armed conflict and forced migration have been ubiquitous features of human history.

However, changes in 20th century conflicts have had a dramatic effect on global

migration as refugees from Third World conflicts are increasingly resettling into asylum

countries (Ingleby, 2010; Silove, 2004). Indeed, in the 50 years since the two world

wars, more people than ever have been displaced by conflict (D. Pedersen, 2002; Zwi &

Ugalde, 1989).

The present day United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees (UNHCR) was

established in 1950 and in 1951 the Convention relating to the Status of Refugees

(hereafter referred to as the Convention) was ratified. The following definition of a

refugee is set forth in Article 1 of the Convention (modified in 1967 by Article 1 of the

Protocol relating to the Status of Refugees, hereafter referred to as the Protocol)

(UNHCR, 2011b):

[Any person who] owing to well-founded fear of being persecuted for reasons of

race, religion, nationality, membership of a particular social group or political

9

opinion, is outside the country of his nationality and is unable or, owing to such

fear, is unwilling to avail himself of the protection of that country; or who, not

having a nationality and being outside the country of his former habitual

residence as a result of such events, is unable or, owing to such fear, is unwilling

to return to it. (p. 16)

Following the formation of the UNHCR and the advent of the Convention, refugees and

asylum seekers during the 1950s to mid-1970s were received with relative warmth in

host nations where they typically enjoyed full access to citizenship. Alarmingly, since

the early 1980s there as been a backlash in attitudes towards displaced people. As the

global refugee crisis has continued to grow steadily and significantly, public opinion,

political rhetoric and policy have simultaneously deteriorated. The contemporary

attitude of the international community is one of fatigue and frustration towards the

global refugee situation. Today, many nations are increasingly denying access to asylum

seekers, forcibly detaining applicants without providing adequate assessment of their

claims and enforcing repatriation policies (Silove, 2003). The increased “warehousing”

of millions of refugees in camps, sometimes for decades, is another saddening trend

(Ingleby, 2010).

2.2.1 Current crises and solutions

There are currently 15.4 million refugees and 27.5 internally displaced people

(International Organization for Migration, 2011a). The latter are essentially refugees

who have not crossed an internationally recognised state border (International

Organization for Migration, 2011a). In 2010, the top five refugee source countries were

Afghanistan (2,887,100), Iraq (1,785,200), Somalia (678,300), Democratic Republic of

Congo (455,900) and Burma (405,700) (UNHCR, 2010a).

Most refugees first cross international borders close to their country of origin and into

some of the poorest countries in the world where around 30% subsist in camps and the

rest live in urban slums or rural areas (UNHCR, 2010a). In recent decades an increasing

number of refugee situations have become protracted. The UNHCR defines protracted

circumstances as those in which 25,000 or more refugees remain in a situation that has

continued for five years or more without signs of resolution (UNHCR, 2004). There are

currently 5.5 million refugees living in such situations.

10

Today the UNHCR (2004) uses three strategies or “durable solutions” (p. I/1) in

response to the forced migration of refugees: repatriation, integration and resettlement.

Of these solutions, the UNHCR considers voluntary repatriation (return to country of

nationality) to be the most favoured but often finds it unachievable due to ongoing

conflict and safety concerns. Opportunities for legal local integration into the first

country of asylum (often countries neighbouring the refugees’ homeland) are also

limited. The third and final solution of resettlement is the UNHCR’s least favoured

option (UNHCR, 2004). It is also unavailable to most refugees and only 1% are

processed and submitted for resettlement as the demand for resettlement greatly exceeds

places made available by host countries (UNHCR, 2010b). In 2009, the UNHCR

submitted over 128,000 refugees; of those, 84,000 were accepted. When family

members are included this number rises to 112,400. Most were resettled in the United

States of America (79,000), followed by Canada (12,500) and Australia (11,000).

Smaller numbers were resettled across 16 other countries (UNHCR, 2010d).

Resettlement is often expensive and generally involves major culture change for

refugees. Its success depends heavily on the policies and services put in place by host

nations for language classes, education, interpreter services, vocational training and

employment.

The UHHCR has also developed policy and programs specific to the needs of refugee

women and girls. Female refugees are recognised to be “less likely [than men and boys]

… to have access to even the most fundamental of their rights. These include their right

to food, healthcare, shelter, nationality and documentation” (UNHCR, 2008, p. 6). In

addition, women are at risk of gender-based violence and are often restricted from

participating in education and decision-making. The protection concerns of women and

girls are addressed through the Women At Risk criteria and program. With respect to

resettlement, this program aims to identify women in particularly vulnerable situations

and fast-track their resettlement.

The impacts of mass conflicts and forced migrations are boundless. They affect

environments, economies, societies and of course individuals. Not surprisingly, the

health impact of violence for individuals is immense. Most obviously, armed conflict

causes direct violence which leads to increased mortality rates and physical injury

(Toole, 2003; Toole & Waldman, 1988) and since the 1980s civilian deaths have

increased to account for around 90% of all war-related deaths (M. G. Marshall & Gurr,

11

2003; D. Pedersen, 2002). The broader physical health impacts of war-related structural

violence such as poverty, malnutrition and disease are also well documented (Reed &

Keely, 2001; Toole & Waldman, 1990). These issues are particularly pertinent today

when an increasing number of conflicts are protracted and force civilian populations

into prolonged displacement and extended confinement in refugee camps. This thesis is

concerned with the mental health effects of armed conflict and forced displacement on

refugees.

2.3 War and the psyche

Psychiatric literature began to document the psychological consequences of conflict

during the American Civil War when soldiers who complained of aches located in the

left side of the chest and a weak heartbeat were diagnosed with “Da Costa’s syndrome”.

British soldiers in the Boer Wars were diagnosed with “debility syndrome”

characterised by joint pain and muscle weakness. During the First World War, British

and American soldiers were diagnosed according to four categories of “war neurosis”:

shell shock (literally shock to the physical and mental body following exposure to high

explosives), hysteria (loss of control over one’s motor, sensory or perceptual functions),

neurasthenia (nervous exhaustion following exposure to repeated stressors), and

disordered action of the heart (also known as Da Costa’s syndrome; characterised by

symptoms of cardiac sensations with no organic cause) (Watters, 2010; Young, 1995).

Interest in the field of post-conflict mental health grew substantially following the

return of American soldiers from the Vietnam War and research with this population

heavily influenced the development of a new diagnostic construct PTSD, which was

entered into the third edition of the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) in 1980 (Steel, Frommer, & Silove,

2004; Summerfield, 2001). The social, political and cultural atmosphere contributing to

the emergence of this disorder and the conceptual origins of the diagnosis have been

subject to much criticism (Scott, 1990; Summerfield, 2001; Yehuda & McFarlane,

1995) and much has been made of the connection between Vietnam War veterans and

the development of PTSD (see Section 2.4.2).

Today DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) retains PTSD as an anxiety

disorder characterised by significant impairment or distress due to symptoms of (a) re-

12

experiencing a traumatic event (e.g., through nightmares or flashbacks), (b) avoidance

of stimuli associated with a traumatic event and numbing of affect, and (c) persistent

increased arousal (e.g., hypervigilance or irritability). The diagnosis of acute stress

disorder is used for a similar syndrome lasting less than 30 days (the full diagnostic

criteria for these constructs can be found in Appendix A). In addition to DSM-IV, the

International Classification of Diseases-10 (ICD-10) (World Health Organisation, 1992)

diagnostic system also provides criteria for the diagnosis of PTSD. However, the DSM-

IV criteria for PTSD and other psychiatric disorders are the most commonly utilised in

refugee mental health (McFarlane, 2004) and will generally be the system referred to

throughout this thesis. As we will see, a trauma-focused framework guided by the

DSM-IV diagnosis of PTSD has radically dominated the focus of war and refugee

mental health researchers and practitioners since the field began in earnest in the 1980s.

2.4 Refugee mental health

Throughout its relatively short history, the field of refugee mental health research and

practice has been remarkably productive despite being exceptionally polarised in its

approach. At its extremes it has been governed by two approaches (Silove, 2010). The

first and dominant, known as the etic approach, is characterised by epidemiological

studies documenting the prevalence rates of psychiatric disorders among refugees. This

approach assumes that the psychiatric diagnoses of Western-derived nosologies

(represented by DSM-IV or ICD-10) are universal disorders and can therefore be

legitimately and usefully applied to non-Western cultures. Advocates of a cultural

relativist or emic perspective, strongly influenced by anthropological frameworks,

contest the validity of Western conceptualisations of mental illness for non-Western

populations. Embedded within this ongoing debate is the contested issue regarding the

relevance of a psychological trauma perspective and, more precisely, the

appropriateness of extending the Western nosological category of PTSD to non-Western

refugee populations.

The terms etic and emic are widely used in refugee mental health literature. Though first

used in linguistics (Pike, 1954), these words were adopted by anthropologists who used

them to distinguish between concepts and frameworks held by a cultural group under

study (emic) and those of an observer (etic) (Harris, 1976). In the case of refugee mental

health literature, the emic perspective reflects the beliefs held by refugee communities

13

themselves whereas the etic orientation represents that of Western psychiatry or

psychology (Weiss et al., 1992).

It is only in the last decade that a greater number of published articles in the field have

moved beyond heated oscillating debate and polarised etic versus emic opinions

towards a more balanced and integrated acknowledgement of the importance and

relevance of both frameworks to understanding and addressing refugee mental health

issues. Yet, a trauma-oriented framework has continued to shape the research agenda

and a “problematic gap” (Miller, Kulkarni, et al., 2006, p. 409) has emerged between

research and practice in which research findings seldom provide information relevant to

the needs of refugee populations and those who work with them. Understanding the

evolution of academic attention to refugee mental health is an important starting point

and leads naturally to a consideration of the contemporary challenges and opportunities

facing today’s researchers.

2.4.1 The dominant discourse: trauma-focused psychiatric epidemiology

It was not until the 1980s that the field of civilian and refugee mental health gained its

current momentum and contemporary focus on trauma (Ingleby, 2010; Miller, Kulkarni,

et al., 2006). Prior to this time, medical interventions, humanitarian efforts and research

with refugee populations had largely focused on their basic physical needs. There had

been sporadic clinical descriptions of psychic pathology, neurotic illnesses and

psychoses experienced by World War II refugees (Brill & Beebe, 1955; Eitinger, 1959;

Kardiner, 1947; H. B. M. Murphy, 1955), concentration camp survivors (Eitinger, 1961;

Krystal, 1968; Niederland, 1964) and survivors of the Hiroshima nuclear bomb attack

(Lifton, 1967), but no trauma framework as seen today had been applied. Indeed,

according to Ingleby (2010), the word “trauma” was not mentioned in published articles

on refugees between 1968 and 1977.

The present and prevailing focus on trauma in refugee mental health resulted from two

coinciding events in the 1980s. The first was the advent of PTSD in psychiatric

nosology. The second was the international focus on and proliferation of humanitarian

interventions drawn to the plight of refugees from South East Asia. Some of the first

modern refugee mental health studies emerged from collaborations between mental

health researchers and humanitarian relief organisations assisting Cambodian survivors

14

of the Khmer Rouge in South East Asia where researchers (Mollica et al., 1992; Mollica

et al., 1990) set to developing questionnaires to assess rates of PTSD symptoms.

Within two decades of its appearance in DSM, the clinical concept of PTSD was being

rapidly researched, expanded and applied in a wide range of ethnic groups, cultures and

regions where it had not previously been diagnosed or even heard of, including Sierra

Leone (S. H. Fox & Tang, 2000), Central America (Michultka, Blanchard, & Kalous,

1998), Bosnia (Weine, Vojvoda, et al., 1998), Burma (Allden et al., 1996) and Tibet

(Holtz, 1998). Today, published material concerning refugee mental health exceeds

1000 documents (Miller & Rasco, 2004), with most empirical studies focusing on PTSD

and other psychiatric sequelae (most notably other anxiety disorders and depression) of

refugee trauma.

Key findings

Epidemiology studies of refugee mental health have yielded important findings, firstly

in reporting prevalence rates of psychiatric disorders and secondly in demonstrating

several consistent risk and protective factors affecting the wellbeing of refugee

communities. This latter contribution came later in the progression of the field after

early studies had already provided evidence of a dose-dependent relationship between

exposure to traumatic experiences and PTSD symptoms (Mollica, McInnes, Pham, et

al., 1998; Mollica, McInnes, Pool, & Tor, 1998). Researchers soon moved beyond

exploring simple linear relationships between traumatic events and psychopathology

and began to address additional variables contributing to wellbeing and distress. In line

with non-refugee literature, findings began to clearly indicate that vulnerability and

resilience for mental illness (including PTSD) was influenced by complex and

interacting variables including genes, early developmental environment (Bagot et al.,

2007), cultural and social context (Lemelson, Kirmayer, & Barad, 2007), and previous

life experience with stress and trauma (Brewin, Andrews, & Valentine, 2000). It is now

widely accepted that a matrix of pre- and post-migratory factors across physiological,

social, existential, political and experiential domains affect vulnerability and resilience

for refugee populations (Mollica, Cui, McInnes, & Massagli, 2002; Porter & Haslam,

2005; Shrestha et al., 1998; J. P. Wilson & Drozdek, 2007).

15

More recent advancements in refugee mental health have emerged from behavioural

neuroscience studies of fear, anxiety and trauma. As recently discussed by Bremner

(2007), dozens of neuroimaging studies demonstrate decreased hippocampal volume

associated with impaired short-term memory in PTSD patients in comparison to both

non-traumatised controls as well as non-PTSD trauma survivors. These findings are

supported by research with refugee populations (Sabioncello et al., 2000) demonstrating

marked abnormalities in the hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid axis of East German

refugees (Bauer, Priebe, Kurten, Klaus-Jurgen, & Baumgartner, 1994) and stress

hormone irregularities amongst displaced Croatian women with PTSD (Sondergaard &

Theorell, 2003).

With regards to prevalence rates, epidemiology studies have routinely demonstrated the

presence of symptoms of PTSD, depression and other psychiatric diagnoses amongst

refugees in both clinical and community samples and across a range of cultures. A

review of interview-based prevalence studies of mental disorder amongst community-

based refugees resettled in Western countries reported prevalence rates of 9% for PTSD

and 5% for depression with high levels of comorbidity (Fazel, Wheeler, & Danesh,

2005). However, a wide variation in prevalence rates (e.g., 3% – 86% for PTSD and 3%

– 80% for depression) within and between refugee populations has been noted (Fazel et

al., 2005; Hollifield et al., 2002) and findings across populations are often difficult to

interpret. These inconsistencies may be explained by differences between the

populations studied (e.g., cultural background, war-related experiences and degree of

persecution), research methodology factors (e.g., random processes, non-equivalent

measures and translation issues), as well as problems of low validity of the constructs

measured (Bhui & Bhugra, 2002; D. Murphy, Ndegwa, Kanani, Rojas-Jaimes, &

Webster, 2002; Van Ommeren, 2003). Indeed, epidemiological studies of refugee

mental health are notoriously difficult to undertake. Researchers face enormous

methodological barriers and, increasingly, harsh criticism regarding the validity,

relevance and ethics of their approach. Today these criticisms remain highly pertinent

and, as will be in seen in the following sections, have begun to shape the progression of

the field.

16

Methodological limitations and criticisms

As noted, psychiatric epidemiology studies of refugee mental health are a challenging

venture and further attention needs to be given to sampling methods, validity and

reliability. The variety of measures employed makes it difficult to interpret findings

across studies and populations. Measures used include symptom checklists, most

commonly the Harvard Trauma Questionnaire, the Hopkins Symptoms Checklist and

the Impact of Events Scale (Aptekar & Giel, 2002; Ketzer & Crescenzi, 2002;

McFarlane, 2004; Modvig & Jaranson, 2004), semi-structured interviews and structured

clinical interviews (Silove, Sinnerbrink, Field, Manicavasagar, & Steel, 1997).

Concerningly, different measures lead to widely differing prevalence rates. For

example, the PTSD prevalence rates reported by studies using structured clinical

interviews are 411% higher than those using other measures (Hollifield, 2005). A

further and fairly consistent criticism of epidemiological research to date is that due to

the use of symptom checklists, findings are often based on symptom identification

rather than on full psychiatric diagnosis (de Jong, Komproe, & Van Ommeren, 2003;

Eisenbruch et al., 2004).

Many measures and studies are also plagued by translation and interpretation issues.

Some checklists and questionnaires are back-translated and piloted, and an

understanding of terms is reached through community consultation (Bolton &

Betancourt, 2004), but this is not always the case (Hollifield et al., 2002). Some studies

create community-specific checklists and seek to use a wording that reflects emic

conceptualisations (Miller, Omidian, et al., 2006), but again many do not (Hollifield et

al., 2002). Clinical interviews may be conducted by mental health professionals acting

with or without interpreters. In other cases the interviewer may be a trained local – often

a fellow refugee. The implications of these variations are significant. The PTSD

prevalence rate is 288% higher when studies use interpreters rather than interviewers

who share the participants’ language (Hollifield, 2005). When symptoms are identified

on the basis of back-translated and piloted instruments utilised by interviewers who

speak the local language, the measures are often still not formally validated for use with

the local population (Bolton & Betancourt, 2004). These factors are likely to contribute

to the inconsistent and varied findings that plague the now large body of literature

relating to refugee mental health.

17

There is also debate about how well PTSD measures can assess the multiple effects of

torture (Silove, 1999) and the “polytrauma” (Hollifield et al., 2002, p. 618) experienced

by refugees who do not simply suffer from isolated, appalling events but rather from

chronic dreadful situations (Ingleby, 2010). Measures of PTSD are generally designed

to assess symptoms after an isolated traumatic event (Watters, 2010) which, in

developed Western nations, is a conceivable situation. However, the experience of

isolated trauma is likely to be uncommon for refugees, many of whom continue to

endure unsafe, unsanitary and unstable environments. Studies with refugees often fail to

conceptualise psychological responses within this contextual framework and report

prevalence rates of PTSD symptoms amongst populations whose daily experiences are

not necessarily “post” traumatic.

The field also lacks longitudinal studies and desperately needs them. It is debatable

whether rates of PTSD in refugee populations are enduring or transient, as studies have

repeatedly demonstrated that the majority of civilians forcibly displaced by war do not

suffer long-term psychiatric impairment (de Jong & Komproe, 2002; Silove, 1999,

2010). The symptoms critical to the PTSD diagnosis (e.g., re-experiencing, avoidance

and arousal) are considered to be normal, evolutionarily adaptive and biologically

hardwired psychobiological responses to extremely threatening experiences (Lemelson

et al., 2007; Rau & Fanselow, 2007; Silove, 2007; Yehuda, 2003a, 2003b). For

example, remembering or re-experiencing the traumatic event can help an individual

learn from a situation and improve their ability to respond in future. Avoiding stimuli

and situations associated with the trauma, and generalising one’s learning to other

contexts, is also likely to reduce the chance of encountering such a threat again. Finally,

increased arousal (e.g., hypervigilance), the third and last symptom category associated

with PTSD, increases one’s likelihood of detecting a threat before it is too late.

Analgesic response to threat may also account for the “numbing” symptoms in the

PTSD criteria (Bouton & Waddell, 2007). Indeed, abnormality could perhaps be marked

by the absence of these symptoms (Modvig et al., 2000; Yehuda, 2003b).

This situation poses a serious problem for the interpretation of data, as elevated rates of

PTSD symptoms in such populations may simply demonstrate an expected short-term

adaptive increase in stress response. Silove (2007) suggests that immediate post-conflict

PTSD symptoms might be more accurately conceptualised as “survival overdrive state

(SOS)” (p. 249) which might better explain why the full list of PTSD symptoms have

18

been consistently observed in trauma survivors in the immediate aftermath of their

traumatic experience (Rothbaum, Foa, Riggs, Murdock, & Walsh, 1992; Shalev, 2007).

A number of recent reviews and meta-analyses have confirmed that whilst careful etic

epidemiological research has improved knowledge of refugee mental health, notorious

problems persist including a lack of attention to cross-cultural and contextual (e.g.,

sociopolitical) issues. Most critics do not question the importance of understanding the

traumatic impact of war-related violence. However, there is a growing recognition that a

considerable amount of the variance in reported distress with war-affected communities

is related not to violence experienced in the past but to cultural and contextual factors,

and to the ongoing stressors in people’s day-to-day lives (Blight, Ekblad, Lindencrona,

& Shahnavaz, 2009; Silove, 1999).

Meta-analytic data shows that residing in a country at war is associated with increased

risk for psychopathology independent of direct exposure to war events or experiencing

displacement (Porter & Haslam, 2001). Moreover, displaced persons exhibit greater

mental health distress than non-displaced persons who have also experienced direct

exposure to war. These and other findings (Miller & Rasmussen, 2010) suggest that

factors specific to the post-migratory refugee experience impact on mental health,

additional to the effects of having been exposed to war trauma (Keyes, 2000). In an

analysis of data totalling 67,294 subjects, social conditions after displacement (e.g., type

of accommodation and economic opportunities) were found to be the strongest

moderators of mental health outcomes (Porter & Haslam, 2005). Better psychological

outcomes were associated with private and permanent accommodation, less restricted

economic opportunity and resolution or stabilisation of initiating conflict.

Methodological limitations of trauma-focused refugee mental health research were

made evident in Hollifield and colleague’s (2002) landmark review of the literature. The

review highlighted cross-cultural construct validity concerns, a hotly contested issue

surrounding the validity of applying Western psychiatric diagnoses to non-Western war-

affected populations (Eisenbruch et al., 2004; Miller, Kulkarni, et al., 2006). The

problem of validity stems from a conceptual issue and it is one that deserves thorough

exploration. The issue amounts to the concern that measures of mental illness constructs

such as PTSD across societies can yield misleading results since the meaning of

symptoms may differ across societies and settings.

19

According to Miller, Kulkarni, et al. (2006), refugee mental health research presents an

extensive history of examining diagnostic categories without first establishing the

construct validity of those concepts. When Hollifield and colleagues (2002) reviewed

the range of instruments used to measure the mental health of refugees following trauma

they found that about 90% of the instruments used either did not test reliability or

lacked appropriate construct validity for use in the population of focus. A sample of 183

published studies using 125 different instruments showed that only 12 were developed

specifically for use in refugee populations (and only 22% of the studies utilised these 12

instruments) and another 8 were adapted for use with the population of focus. It has

been argued that this issue represents the most serious flaw in refugee mental health

literature, with critics claiming that sufficient reliability or other improved research

methods “cannot redeem” (Summerfield, 2007, p. 362) a study that lacks cross-cultural

construct validity.

2.4.2 The dominant debates: cross-cultural construct validity and category

fallacies

The cross-cultural validity debate has lingered as one of the most heated in the field of

refugee mental health (Miller, Kulkarni, et al., 2006; D. Pedersen, 2002; Summerfield,

2002). These concerns had previously been raised by medical anthropologists

(Eisenbruch, 1983; Kirmayer, 1989; Kleinman, 1977) but in relation to refugee mental

health, their critiques have only begun to gain sufficient impetus in the past decade. At

the heart of the issue is Kleinman’s (1987) warning against the temptation to assume the

universality of a psychiatric syndrome simply because it can be demonstrated in

different contexts. He termed this error a “category fallacy” which he described as:

the reification of a nosological category developed for a particular cultural group

that is then applied to members of another culture for whom it lacks coherence

and its validity has not been established. (p. 452)

Those who question the presumed universality and cultural relevance of PTSD as a

response to traumatic stress with war-affected populations have drawn attention to

culture-specific responses to war, persecution and forced displacement (D. E. Hinton &

Otto, 2006; Marsella, 1994; Marsella, Friedman, Gerrity, & Scurfield, 1996). They

argue that these experiences and expressions of emotional or mental distress are very

20

unlikely to be identified if the salience of PTSD (or other psychiatric diagnoses) is

assumed a priori and only symptoms fitting the diagnostic profile are assessed. Indeed,

Kleinman (1977) argued that much of Western cross-cultural psychiatric research was

not truly cross-cultural because “by definition it will find what is universal and

systematically miss what does not fit its tight parameters” (p. 4).

Although symptoms of PTSD and depression may be found in a given refugee

population, we cannot assume that the diagnosis has the same meaning or salience as it

does to Western mental health professionals. The presence of PTSD symptoms may

actually tell us little about the degree to which their behaviour is considered normative

or deviant in the local context. Nor can we presume to know how traumatic stress is

understood and experienced. For example, Liberians believe that trauma and chronic

stress can cause Open Mole – a state of suffering defined by a soft spot on top of the

skull and accompanied by additional symptoms including fatigue, somatic aches and

pains, nightmares and social withdrawal (Abramowitz, 2010). Afghans readily identify

symptoms of PTSD but are more likely to understand war-related suffering in terms of

jigar khun, an indigenous idiom of distress that shares some symptomatic overlap with

PTSD, yet is also distinct from it (Miller, Omidian, et al., 2006). Among Latinos, a

trauma survivor is more likely to suffer from ataque de nervios (attack of nerves) than

symptoms comprising the PTSD profile, though again some features of the two

constructs overlap (Lewis-Fernandez et al., 2010). Traumatised Cambodian refugees

report suffering from khyal attacks, a complex cultural syndrome related to

ethnophysiological beliefs in the concept of “wind flow” (D. E. Hinton, Pich, Marques,

Nickerson, & Pollack, 2010, p. 245).

These findings suggest that we can interpret little from an affirmative answer to the

question of whether a refugee experiences symptoms comprising the PTSD diagnosis.

For example, we do not know how the PTSD symptom of nightmares is experienced

and understood from, say, the perspective of a Sudanese woman who witnessed the

violent death of her brother and was unable to bury his body. Her nightmares may be

interpreted within a cultural context in which it is believed that the spirits of deceased

family cannot rest until burial rituals are observed, and her difficulties could be resolved

by the performance of certain rites by a local traditional healer or spiritual guide. That

her symptoms overlapped with PTSD does not negate the reality that her experience

21

was perhaps better conceptualised, and resolved, through indigenous or emic

approaches.

The unquestioned assumption that Western psychiatric diagnoses were applicable to

non-Western refugee populations came under attack shortly after the early cross-cultural

mental health studies of Cambodian refugees (Mollica, Wyshak, & Lavelle, 1987;

Mollica et al., 1990) were published. Not surprisingly, it was medical anthropologists

who were amongst the first to call for researchers to examine the cultural assumptions

underlying the diagnosis of PTSD. They appealed for a tentative approach to the use of

this Western ontology with non-Western refugees (Bracken et al., 1995; Eisenbruch et

al., 2004; Eisenbruch & Handelman, 1989; Gaines, 1992; Handelman & Yeo, 1996;

Higginbotham & Marsella, 1988; Weiss, 1997). These critics argued that assumptions

and claims of the universality of PTSD were, at best, prematurely unwarranted

examples of empiricist epistemology (Bracken et al., 1995; Summerfield, 2008) and, at

worst, examples of Western ethnocentric imperialism (Bracken et al., 1995; Eisenbruch,

1992; Summerfield, 2007).

Western epistemology

Western academically acquired knowledge cannot be separated from its cultural

base even though it presents itself as our natural way of looking at the world.

(Lutz, 1985, p. 65)

Critics argued that psychiatric explanations and the DSM concept of discrete psychiatric

disorders reflect a set of contemporary assumptions that are likely to differ greatly from

the accounts of psychological phenomena in other cultures and cannot be presumed to

have high diagnostic validity across settings (Kleinman, 1980). Anthropologists have

demonstrated that illness and distress are interpreted, experienced and expressed in

different ways across cultural settings and argued that Western psychiatry “is but one

among many ethnopsychiatries” (Summerfield, 2008, p. 993). Ethnopsychiatries differ

in their beliefs about the aetiology of emotional or mental illness and the healing

practices employed. They are also entwined with a populations’ conceptualisations of

self and identity, time, mind and memory, as well as moral authority and religious or

spiritual belief systems (Andary, Stolk, & Klimidis, 2003).

22

Several culture-bound notions have been identified as underlying Western psychiatric

approaches to mental illness. Primary amongst them is Western individualism, which

assumes that the individual is the appropriate unit of analysis in relation to mental

disorder (Karasz, 2005; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). In contrast, for the majority of the

world’s cultures, a collective ontology prevails; dependency and interdependency are

emphasised over individuality; and frameworks for mental health incorporate a more

holistic range of physical, spiritual, supernatural, moral and social domains

(Constantine, Myers, Kindaichi, & Moore, 2004; de Jong, 2004; Drozdek, 2007;

Eisenbruch, 1983; Karasz, 2005; Summerfield, 2008; Wing, 1998). Young (1995)

convincingly argues that the concept of PTSD requires acceptance of several other

uniquely Western notions, particularly those related to the idea of self, mind and

memory, and the belief that a traumatic event can result in some sort of lasting

psychological wound.

Western culture’s medicalised explanation for emotional or mental suffering (Horwitz,

2010) is also at odds with the ontological beliefs of other cultures. During the last

century, Western bureaucracies have increasingly shifted towards a medicalisation of

human experience based on models that assume states of mental illness exist in nature

independently of our definitions of them (Horwitz, 2010; Watters, 2010). Inherent in

this shift has been an intolerance towards suffering. Kleinman (1995) addresses this in

his book Writing at the Margin: Discourse between Anthropology and Medicine:

The idea in the DSM is that suffering can not and should not be endured. It

should be brought to an end. This is central to the ideology of America: there is

nothing that needs to be endured. Even memories can be “worked through”. (p.

180)

However, throughout history, world religions have provided followers with other

models for understanding their pain and suffering (Almedom & Summerfield, 2004),

including the suffering associated with war and persecution. For most non-Western

cultures today, human suffering continues to be explained as a defining characteristic of

the human condition and a vital aspect of one’s existential plight (Boehnlein, 2007).

An extension of the medicalisation of life is the perception that culture is an arbitrary

phenomenon superimposed on a universally uniform biological reality (Lewis-

23

Fernandez & Kleinman, 1994; Vance, 1991). Contemporary Western psychiatry, as

reflected in DSM-IV, largely conceptualises mental disorders as discrete entities of

organic or biological origin. However, evidence for this is lacking. Few psychiatric

constructs boast convincing evidence of biological causation or even a biological

marker, as is common for most physical illnesses and diseases (Schwartz & Corcoran,

2010). Indeed, despite the supposed universality and organic origin of many Western

psychiatric constructs, history has seen dozens of constructs come and go from

inclusion in diagnostic publications in line with social and cultural trends (Summerfield,

2008, 2010; Watters, 2010). It has been argued that “with the exception of the clearly

defined organic disorders, almost all presently used [psychiatric] diagnostic categories

are, at best, provisional” (Silove, 2010, p. 36).

Nor does the conceptualisation of mental illnesses as discrete entities receive unanimous

support within psychiatry and psychology. In fact, in preparation for the introduction of

DSM-V (due for release in 2013) there has been a resurgence of interest, controversy

and debate around the suggestion that mental health and illness are better

conceptualised as dimensional continua of affect and function rather than in terms of

discrete disease states (Bonanno & Mancini, 2012; Giles & Matthews, 2012; Horwitz,

2010). In relation to PTSD, this argument is supported by several lines of evidence

suggestive of a “biological continuum” (Konner, 2007, p. 315; Summerfield, 2010)

comprising PTSD, other anxiety disorders and mood disorders.

Picking apart PTSD: hegemony, trauma and the medicalisation of war

Amongst the critiques against the cross-cultural application of Western psychiatric

nosological constructs with refugees, much has been made of the sociopolitical context

surrounding the diagnosis of PTSD. In The Harmony of Illusions: Inventing Post-

Traumatic Stress Disorder, ethnographer Allan Young (1995) traces the development of

the diagnosis and argues:

The disorder is not timeless, nor does it possess an intrinsic unity. Rather, it is

glued together by the practices, technologies, and narratives with which it is

diagnosed, studied, treated, and represented and by the various interests,

institutions, and moral arguments that mobilized these efforts and resources. (p.

5)

24

Outspoken critic Summerfield (2001) has similarly claimed that:

A central assumption behind psychiatric diagnosis is that a disease has an

objective existence in the world … however, the story of PTSD is a telling

example of the role of society and politics in the process of invention rather than

of discovery (p. 95).

Such critiques are not universally supported and several authors believe that the

conceptual origins of the diagnosis are traceable through historic psychiatric literature

(Turnbull, 1998; van der Kolk & van der Hart, 1989; Wakefield, 2001; J. P. Wilson,

1994). Nonetheless, most accounts of the conceptual development of modern PTSD

recognise the influence of the post-Vietnam War era. According to Watters (2010), the

diagnosis was originally called “Post-Vietnam Syndrome”, an idea that originated out of

meetings held by the Vietnam Veterans Against the War and attended by anti-war

psychoanalyst Dr Chaim Shatan.

Several authors (Scott, 1990; Watters, 2010; Young, 1995) have suggested that the idea

behind post-Vietnam syndrome was not to create a diagnosis but rather to call attention

to the unique situation experienced by Vietnam veterans who felt “deceived, used and

betrayed” (Watters, 2010, p. 115) by the US government, military and society. In 1975,

Dr Shatan approached the then Chairperson of the DSM-III taskforce advocating the

inclusion of Post-Vietnam Syndrome. A DSM-III working group was assigned to

explore the concept which, during the course of research and development, was

variously renamed “Post-Combat Disorder” and “Catastrophic Stress Disorder” (Young,

1995). Psychiatrists in the working group soon linked the same set of symptoms to other

patient populations (e.g., burn victims) and by 1980, when DSM-III was first published,

the diagnosis of PTSD had been developed and extended far beyond the Vietnam

veterans’ and Dr Shatan’s original suggestion.

The inclusion of PTSD was one of many changes heralded by the publication of DSM-

III (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) which marked “a revolutionary turn in

American psychiatry” (Young, 1995, p. 100). Prior to this time there was no

standardised neo-Kraepelinian nosology in psychiatry. Neo-Kraepelinian psychiatrists

identified with the nosological perspective of the German psychiatrist Emil Kraepelin

(1856–1926) who assumed a non-arbitrary division between sanity and mental illness,

25

and argued that research would eventually demonstrate the biological origins of discrete

psychiatric disorders (Klerman, 1978). In part the DSM-III developers were pressured

to create a standardised system in order to meet the needs of government and insurance

companies whose support for psychiatry was waning (Young, 1995). However, the

taskforce was also comprised of researchers who endorsed biological explanations for

psychiatric constructs.

According to Summerfield (1999), the inclusion of PTSD into DSM-III was intended to

stimulate further research. However, he argues that the result was a near-paralysis of

inductive research into human reactions to traumatic experiences, and the advent of an

era of PTSD epidemiology research. Indeed, as we have seen, since the 1980s the

construct of PTSD has been rapidly applied to refugee populations with an often

unquestioned assumption of its scientific validity, universality and cultural relevance.

Quickly viewed as a matter of scientific technology, there was money to be made both

in research and in the development of treatments for PTSD. On the international stage,

the construct quickly became a political currency in humanitarian aid situations where

the diagnosis became, and remains, a part of regulation governing the status of migrants

fleeing violence (Drozdek, 2007). Descriptions of PTSD as a “hidden epidemic”

(Almedom & Summerfield, 2004, p. 383; Silove, 2007) and the most important public

health problem in recently war-torn countries reflect the informational sources affecting

the decision-making of donor organisations and governments involved in humanitarian

crisis intervention (Gozdziak, 2004). In 1995, the then editor-in-chief of the American

Journal of Psychiatry observed: “it is rare to find a psychiatric diagnosis that anyone

likes to have, but PTSD seems to be one of them” (Andreasen, 1995, p. 964).

In this manner it could be argued that PTSD came to life through a self-perpetuating

cycle of promotion. Several authors (Bracken, 2001; Pupavac, 2004; Summerfield,

1999) have further interpreted the rapid growth and promulgation of PTSD through the

lens of political sociology and attributed its rise to other postmodern cultural

developments including the secularisation of society, demise of traditional value

systems and subsequent loss of meaning. Alongside these changes were developments

and challenges in the field of humanitarian intervention. Pupavac (2004) describes the

politicisation and vehement criticism facing humanitarian organisations in the 1990s

26

and argues that the rise in PTSD, and more broadly refugee mental health, resulted from

the wider “crisis of purpose” (p. 497) in humanitarianism.

Notwithstanding the important points that critics have made, it should be noted that

refugee populations have benefited from trauma-focused research to date. Firstly,

through the interest associated with PTSD, the plight of refugee communities has

obtained public, professional and political recognition (Bala, 2010). Secondly, many

refugees who do experience debilitating mental health difficulties have been able to

access mental healthcare.

2.4.3 Intervention issues: practical, clinical and ethical challenges

Beyond conceptual arguments about the evidence and ethics of approaching refugee

mental health through the lens of Western psychiatry, critics have noted additional

practical limitations and expressed concern that trauma-focused research has often

failed to provide the sort of useful information that could support the development of

culturally appropriate and empirically sound mental health interventions.

From as early as the 1980s, humanitarian organisations, researchers and mental health

practitioners rapidly devised and implemented trauma-focused treatments in the

immediate aftermath of conflict and displacement. Today this practice remains common

(Watters, 2010) yet there is limited evidence for the efficacy of psychotherapy with

refugee populations (Aroche & Coello, 2004; Drozdek & Wilson, 2004; Nicholl &

Thompson, 2004; Nickerson, Bryant, Silove, & Steel, 2011; J. P. Wilson & Drozdek,

2007). Critics have argued that a medicalised trauma-focused model of refugee mental

health has had the effect of spawning “a self-sustaining and self-serving trauma

counseling industry” (Silove, 2007, p. 242) in the absence of supportive empirical

evidence.

It is beyond the scope of this thesis to detail the variety of psychotherapies implemented

in refugee communities. The terms counselling, trauma counselling, psychotherapy and

psychological support are used by a multitude of authors in a variety of ways and these

labels often tell us little of what the intervention actually involved. Different forms of

cognitive behavioural therapies (CBT), most often involving exposure therapy, appear

to be the most commonly applied psychotherapy with refugees (Murray, Schweitzer, &

Davidson, 2010; Nicholl & Thompson, 2004; Nickerson et al., 2011; Palic & Elklit,

27

2011). This is in large part due to the trauma focus of refugee mental health literature

and the fact that CBT is the most widely studied and accepted intervention for PTSD

amongst Western populations (Bisson & Andrew, 2007; Foa, Keane, & Friedman,

2000; Foa, Rothbaum, Riggs, & Murdock, 1991). Other psychotherapies commonly

utilised in refugee studies include narrative and testimony approaches (De Haene,

Grietens, & Verschueren, 2010; McPherson, 2012; Weine, Kulenovic, Pavkovic, &

Gibbons, 1998), eye-movement desentisitation (Oras, de Ezpeleta, & Ahmad, 2004), as

well as music and art therapy (Orth, Doorschodt, Verburgt, & Drozdek, 2004;

Wertheim-Cahen, van Dijk, Schouten, Roozen, & Drozdek, 2004). Despite their

differences, most trauma psychotherapies tend to be guided by the notion of “working

through” the traumatic event (Ingleby, 2010, p. 7).

The limited evidence base for interventions may be partly explained by methodological

problems including the diversity of psychotherapies. However, the efficacy of

treatments is also limited because the needs, experiences and healing approaches of

refugee populations do not always align with the orientation of Western-derived trauma-

focused interventions.

It has been argued that psychiatric (and particularly trauma-focused) approaches to

understanding and addressing human suffering and wellbeing have resulted in

rehabilitation services for refugees that (a) pathologise refugee reactions, deny

resilience and encourage a culture of passivity and victimhood (Pupavac, 2004), (b)

overlook individual and community strengths and resources (Horwitz, 2010; Rousseau

& Measham, 2007; J. P. Wilson & Drozdek, 2007), (c) lack a cultural basis in refugee

communities, (d) fail to accommodate the range of experiences and expressions of

suffering, and (e) are underutilised and unsustainable because they do not address issues

that are relevant to the experiences and interpretations of the community (de Jong &

Komproe, 2002; Miller, Kulkarni, et al., 2006).

This situation has led critics to question the ethics of trauma-oriented interventions and

point to evidence that they may be ineffective, unwarrranted and impractical (Silove,

2007; Silove, Steel, & Bauman, 2007). Worse still, trauma interventions can be

patronising, offensive, stigmatising (Boehnlein et al., 2004; Ekblad & Jaranson, 2004;

D. Pedersen, 2002) and even harmful (Kenardy, 2000; Mayou, Ehlers, & Hobbs, 2000;

Raphael & Wilson, 2000; Rose, Bisson, Churchill, & Wessely, 2009). In light of these

28

concerns it is imperative that future research explore refugees’ perceptions of service

provisions. The current study addresses this in relation to Karen refugee women.

In the past refugee populations have seldom been consulted before mental health

epidemiology studies and intervention projects are undertaken (D. Murphy et al., 2002).

Communities have generally not been asked about their needs and priorities, nor their

opinions about having their mental health framed and published in psychiatric discourse

and literature (Summerfield, 2002). Moreover, there is little evidence that these

populations are actually seeking to have access to Western psychiatric concepts and

treatments over their own traditional systems (Summerfield, 1999). Not surprisingly,

this lack of community consultation can easily result in research that does not address

the priorities of the community.

Critics also argue that the trauma-focused discourse (and industry) of refugee mental

health imposes a sort of professional imperialism in assuming that refugees (a) might

harbour hidden problems, (b) need help (from professionals), (c) are in denial if they do

not agree with outsiders’ observations about their hidden problems (Ingleby, 2010), and

(d) require Western psychiatric methods over their own healing systems. Despite

evidence that coping patterns vary across ethnic groups (Bhui, King, Dein, &

O’Connor, 2008), studies of refugees’ mental wellbeing often fail to recognise or

adequately explore emic approaches to coping with trauma and emotional distress. This

situation can lead to a gross misinterpretation of culturally (and perhaps universally)

normative patterns of behaviour and the undermining of traditional systems.

Critics have deplored Western health professionals for their “misguided image of war

and its aftermath … far removed from the actual experience of non-Westernised

societies” (Summerfield, 2002, p. 1105). They argue that the PTSD discourse amounts

to the medicalisation, sanitisation and “psychologising” (Pupavac, 2004, p. 491) of “the

misery and horror of war” (Summerfield, 2001, p. 96). This argument was put forward

in one of the earliest criticisms of psychiatry’s trauma-focused discourse on refugee

mental health; in 1988 Higginbotham and Marsella argued:

Investing authority in biomedical reasoning about human problem eliminates

explanations of disorder at levels of psychologic, political and economic

29

functioning. Consequently, problems with origins in poverty, discrimination,

role conflict and so forth are treated medically. (p. 559)

Several authors have called for recognition of the way in which war and forced

migration affect the “social fabric” (Summerfield, 2000, p. 418) of refugee communities

(Miller & Rasco, 2004). Concepts such as “collective trauma” (Somasundaram, 2010;

Somasundaram & Jamunanantha, 2002) and “cultural bereavement” (Eisenbruch, 1992)

have been offered as alternative frameworks from which to appreciate the material,

social and spiritual losses that accompany forced migration.

Psychosocial interventions have also often been promoted as a means of addressing the

broader contextual factors affecting the mental health outcomes of refugees. In contrast

to most psychotherapies, which typically focus on the individual or perhaps the family

unit, psychosocial or ecological interventions emphasise the connection between the

individual and their larger social environment. Accordingly, in addition to providing

support for psychological distress, psychosocial interventions also attempt to give

attention to the cultural, socioeconomic and sociopolitical factors affecting

psychological wellbeing (Ekblad & Jaranson, 2004). Given the cultural orientation of

these approaches, their planning and implementation typically involves more

community consultation and generally includes the use of indigenous healing strategies.

Psychosocial interventions aim to foster people’s inherent coping ability and are guided

by the notion that, given adequate social support, most refugees will cope with the loss

and trauma they have endured.

There are many versions of psychosocial interventions. According to the Psychosocial

Working Group (2003), an intervention is considered psychosocial if it promotes

“human capacity or the social ecology of a community or contribute[s] to people’s

efforts to re-establish culture and values in some way” (p. 3). Given the broad focus of

the psychosocial framework it is difficult to know what is meant when authors refer to

using psychosocial interventions. It is also difficult to measure the efficacy of such

programs (Hubbard & Miller, 2004). Unfortunately little is known about the feasibility

and outcomes of psychosocial interventions in terms of mental wellbeing. It is unclear

whether the needs of the most mentally unwell are met through this approach (Silove,

2010).

30

2.4.4 Developments and directions for research: the emic-etic evolution

The muddled situation fostered by polarised opinions about how to approach refugee

mental health could have deleterious consequences as it risks confusing planners and

funding agencies, as well as humanitarian and mental health staff (Rousseau &

Measham, 2007; Silove, 2007). It also risks harming those it aims to help when

ineffective and impertinent interventions are used.

Fortunately, the past decade has witnessed innovative solutions to the challenges and in

response to calls to “move on from head-counts and surveys demonstrating more

prevalence figures and associations” (Patel, 2000, p. 746). Increasing numbers of

researchers have implemented research that is inclusive of and sensitive to the local

needs of the community, and sought to provide useful information for practitioners and

policy makers. Such calls are of course not new. Decades earlier, medical

anthropologist (and psychiatrist) Arthur Kleinman (1977) had heralded the beginning of

the “new cross-cultural psychiatry” (p. 5) in response to the “seriously distorting

influence” (p. 9) of the dominant approach to transcultural psychiatry. He envisaged

that the new cross-cultural psychiatry would integrate anthropological and psychiatric

research.

Kleinman was perhaps premature in announcing significant change to the dominant

discourse for psychiatry in general, and particularly in relation to refugee mental health.

However, from the mid-1970s onwards we did see the development of several

influential emic-oriented frameworks in anthropology and medical anthropology

(Weiss, 2001). In the past decade several research teams have extended the framework

of refugee mental health research to include ethnographic and other emic-oriented (e.g.,

qualitative) methodologies (Bolton, 2001; Bolton, Wilk, & Ndogoni, 2004; Eisenbruch

et al., 2004; Hubbard & Pearson, 2004; Miller, Omidian, et al., 2006; Mollica, 2001;

Patel, Abas, Broadhead, Todd, & Reeler, 2001; Phan et al., 2004; Silove & Bryant,

2006). These studies have allowed for an identification of local syndromes and idioms

of distress, and an understanding of strengths, forms of resilience and patterns of help-

seeking behaviour.

Ethnographic and epidemiological approaches to refugee mental health are now widely

viewed as mutually supportive approaches (Hollifield, 2005; Hollifield et al., 2002;

Silove & Bryant, 2006) and creative mixed-method solutions have emerged (Bass,

31

Bolton, & Murray, 2007; Bolton et al., 2007; de Jong, 2002; Eisenbruch et al., 2004;

Patel, 2001). It is now accepted that ethnographic explorations of community

psychosocial concerns and beliefs about mental wellbeing are a necessary first step if

we are to address the needs of refugee communities (Bass et al., 2007; Keyes, 2000;

Marsella, Friedman, Gerrity, & Scurfield, 1996; Miller, Kulkarni, et al., 2006; Petevi,

Revel, & Jacobs, 2001; Silove & Bryant, 2006; Summerfield, 2008).

Researchers have been particularly interested in documenting refugees’ idioms of

distress which, according to the DSM-IV, should form part of clinicians’ “cultural

formulation” (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, p. 897) of patients from non-

Western cultures. Anthropologist Mark Nichter (1981) is credited with first theorising

this conceptual framework and coining the term “idiom of distress”, explaining:

In any given culture, a variety of ways exist to express distress. Expressive

modes are culturally constituted in the sense that they initiate particular types of

interaction and are associated with culturally pervasive values, norms,

generative themes, and health concerns. (p. 379)

Subsequently, in refugee mental health literature, idioms of distress have been variously

described and defined as “popular categories of mental and emotional suffering”

(Lewis-Fernandez et al., 2010, p. 220), and “those particular ways in which members of

sociocultural groups convey affliction” (D. E. Hinton & Lewis-Fernandez, 2010b, p.

210). In an Appendix, the DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) provides

a guide which encourages clinicians to note non-Western patients’ “idioms of distress”

(p. 897) including:

… the meaning and perceived severity of the individual’s symptoms in relation

to the norms of the cultural reference group, any local illness category used …

and the perceived causes or explanatory models that the individual and the

reference group use to explain the illness. (pp. 897–898)

Amongst the suggested emic-oriented frameworks and methods for exploring refugees’

idioms of distress, Kleinman’s (1980) explanatory model (EM) conceptual framework

has been especially influential. The EM framework provides a method for researchers to

explore emic beliefs about illness and healthcare. Kleinman first described EMs as

“notions about an episode of sickness and its treatment” (p. 105) formed by the various

32

cultural experiences, expectations and symptoms that are associated with a particular

illness. The original model aimed to tap respondents’ understanding or perception of (a)

what to call their illness based on their experience or dominant complaints, (b) the cause

of the illness, (c) the timeline or anticipated course of the illness, (d) the consequences

of having the illness as well as the perceived controllability of these factors, and (e)

appropriate and effective treatments (Bhui, Rudell, & Priebe, 2006).

Although Kleinman (1980) was careful to distinguish between general illness beliefs

(which he termed health ideology) and EMs (which he considered related to a specific

episode of illness), this distinction has not been upheld in subsequent research. The EM

framework has been expanded and frequently applied to explore illness beliefs in the

absence of a current specified disease or illness, particularly in the field of mental health

research (Dejman, Ekblad, Forouzan, Baradaran-Eftekhari, & Malekafzali, 2008; Patel,

Ferrao Simbine, Soares, Weiss, & Wheeler, 2007; Williams & Healy, 2001; Ying,

1990). Others have developed Kleinman’s original ideas into instruments to elicit EMs

(Lloyd et al., 1998; Weiss, 1997). These measures aim to tap collective and individual

cultural beliefs, knowledge and explanations about illness and distress.

Mental health researchers have frequently explored EMs in relation to depression

(Aidoo & Harpham, 2001; Dejman et al., 2008; Okello & Ekblad, 2006; Patel, Pereira,

& Mann, 1998; Raguram, Weiss, Keval, & Channabasavanna, 2001; Sulaiman, Bhugra,

& de Silva, 2001; Ying, 1990), although a range of psychiatric constructs have been

studied including schizophrenia (McCabe & Priebe, 2004) and somatisation

(Henningson, Jakobsen, Schiltenwolf, & Weiss, 2005). Findings indicate that patients’

causal EMs are strong predictors of impairment and short-term prognosis (Bhui et al.,

2006). For example, poor prognosis is associated with EMs emphasising biological

causes amongst patients with somatoform disorders (Henningson et al., 2005). These

and other findings indicate that EMs may complement other clinical evaluations for

understanding illness prognoses (Bann et al., 2004).

Cross-cultural EMs of mental illness have been investigated in both clinical and non-

clinical populations through the use of vignettes depicting etic mental illness constructs

(Dejman et al., 2008; Okello & Ekblad, 2006; Patel et al., 2007; Sulaiman et al., 2001;

Wig et al., 1980; Ying, 1990). Culturally grounded vignettes may assist with group

interviews regarding sensitive topics and have also been used to validate information

33

obtained from in-depth interviews (A. Bailey, 2008). Findings from studies using

vignettes have been used to select mental health service priorities according to

prevalence, perceived harmful consequences, community concern and availability of

appropriate treatments (Wig et al., 1980). While elements of the EM framework have

been used in refugee research, these studies have generally been limited to exploring

refugee communities’ casual understandings of mental illness (Eisenbruch, 1990;

Eisenbruch & Handelman, 1989; Handelman & Yeo, 1996). Moreover, the literature

review failed to identify any refugee EM studies using vignettes to explore emic (local)

conceptualisations of etic (Western) psychiatric constructs.

Despite recent advances, several other limitations of qualitative studies of refugee

mental distress remain. Like their predecessors, these studies have often retained an

individualised focus on psychological experience and given insufficient attention to the

broader sociopolitical issues affecting the community. Few have used qualitative

(especially ethnographic) methods to explore local EMs of mental illness. Moreover,

most research still “uses Western categories and looks for what are believed to be local

permutations, but assumes that Western categories and nosologies are universally

applicable” (de Jong & Komproe, 2002; Gaines, 1992, p. 4). Research beginning from

this premise risks overlooking important emic perspectives in the search for the

universal.

A further limitation of the restricted biomedical, epidemiological and trauma-focused

orientation to refugee mental health has been a failure to draw across disciplines and

engage potentially useful sociological theories and frameworks. For example, the

sociological frameworks developed by the late French anthropologist, sociologist and

philosopher Pierre Bourdieu (1977) could provide a powerful basis for understanding

the experience and psychosocial consequences of persecution, forced migration and

refugee resettlement. Through the concept of habitus Bourdieu sought to explain how

the social world is internalised and embodied in the dispositions of individual actors.

These dispositions include gendered, class and culture-based mannerisms, behaviours,

habits, beliefs and feelings (Jenkins, 2002) and are embodied in individuals and as a

collective characteristic of a particular social group. The power of the notion of habitus

lies in the way in which certain behaviours or beliefs become an automatic part of social

structure operating below a level of conscious rationale. Despite the shift toward

34

psychosocial explanations and interventions of refugee mental health, to my knowledge,

Bourdieu’s work has not been used specifically used in studies to date.

Few studies have focused on women’s specific emotional experiences and needs,

despite the challenges they face during war and forced migration. Research indicates

that refugee women are more likely to experience depression and PTSD than men

(Schubert & Punamaki, 2011) but this phenomenon is poorly understood. A recent

review found resettled refugee and immigrant women were also around 30% more

likely to suffer from postnatal depression (C. H. Collins, Zimmerman & Howard, 2011)

than non-immigrant women though here again, the studies contributing the review

suffered from methodological flaws common to studies of refugee mental health.

Indeed, the most recent review of questionnaires for measuring the health of refugee

women described those used as “suboptimal” (Gagnon & Tuck, 2004, p. 141).

During all phases of the refugee journey women are at high risk of gender-based

violence, exploitation and discrimination (Kaplan & Webster, 2003; Pittaway &

Bartolomei, 2001; Ryan, Doonley, & Benson, 2008; Zwi & Alvarez-Castillo, 2003).

Commonly, women and children are also the first to flee as men remain and fight

(Drumm, Pittman, & Perry, 2001). Pre-migration gendered experiences such as access

to education impact on women’s resettlement and affect their wellbeing (Deacon &

Sullivan, 2009). Resettled refugee women also often take on new social and family roles

as a result of forced migration and their experiences of these shifting responsibilities

require further exploration (Yakushko & Espin, 2010).

Ethnographic studies have also often overlooked the opportunity to explore refugees’

perspectives about mental health and psychosocial service provisions. A related gap

concerns the absence of emic-oriented refugee studies that take a broader focus and

include the perspectives of staff at service providers. This information is important in

order to fully understand the context surrounding the implementation of mental health

interventions and usefully guide policy-relevant research. Finally, many of the

ethnographic studies have been undertaken in refugee camps where individuals and

communities are likely to have some (albeit compromised) access to their traditional

ways of life and customs. However, as cultural beliefs and customs are not fossilised

entities, it is important to understand how cultural factors relate to the health and

wellbeing of resettled communities who are likely to be much farther removed from

35

traditional ways of life. Future ethnographic psychosocial assessments of refugee

communities must address resettled populations and the service providers who work

with them – both of whom face the challenge of adapting to each others’ foreign

language and culture.

2.5 Addressing the gaps

This chapter has outlined several limitations and gaps in refugee mental health literature

that this study aims to address. These include:

A lack of studies exploring refugees’ emic perspectives of mental or emotional

wellbeing and distress, and approaches to coping.

Insufficient attention to refugees’ EMs of mental illness as well as an absence of

studies using vignettes to explore refugees’ conceptualisations of Western

psychiatric constructs.

Inadequate consideration of gender and the gender-based challenges of refugee

women.

Insufficient attention to refugees’ perspectives about mental health service

provisions combined with an absence of studies that include the perspectives of

refugee service providers.

These gaps in mental health literature are particularly evident in relation to refugees

from Burma and, more specifically, the Karen ethnic group. Refugees from Burma

represent one of the UNHCR’s largest resettlement populations (UNHCR, 2010a).

However, only three published studies (Allden et al., 1996; Lopes Cardozo, Talley,

Burton, & Crawford, 2004; Schweitzer, Brough, Vromans, & Asic-Kobe, 2011) have

specifically explored the mental health of this population. All reported prevalence rates

of depression (ranging between 36% and 42%) and PTSD (ranging between 4.6% and

23%), which were measured according to psychometric questionnaires. Two of the

studies (Allden et al., 1996; Lopes Cardozo et al., 2004) used interview methods to

adapt the measures and identify “culturally dependent” (Allden et al., 1996, p. 1562)

symptoms and coping strategies. The most recent study (Schweitzer et al., 2011) also

examined the contribution of pre- and post-migration stressors to wellbeing amongst

Burmese refugees in Australia. However, none of the studies specifically explored emic

conceptualisations of wellbeing and distress or investigated participants’ EMs.

36

Moreover, the literature review failed to identify any studies focusing on the mental

health of Karen refugees or Karen refugee women. This study will address these gaps.

37

Chapter 3

Setting the Scene: Burma and the Karen

3.1 Introduction

This chapter sets the scene for understanding the pre-migration lives of Karen refugee

women from Burma. It describes the relevant sociopolitical history of Burma, detailing

the contemporary human rights abuse situation and the effects of oppression on the

national culture and psyche. The rich cultural tapestry of the people of Burma will be

explored, giving emphasis to the Karen. Finally this chapter attempts to depict refugee

camp life where thousands of Karen (and other) refugees from Burma have lived since

the early 1980s. An understanding of camp conditions and culture is highly relevant to

this thesis as most refugees from Burma who arrive in Australia have spent several

years, and in most cases a decade or more, living in this setting.

3.1.1 Burma, Burman, Burmese and Myanmar: a note on terms and usage

In 1989 the Burmese military junta changed the official name of the country to the

Union of Myanmar (currently the Republic of the Union of Myanmar). This thesis

adopts the position of the Australian Government, who do not recognise this name

change and continue to use the name Burma (Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade,

2011). The Burman people, or “Bamah” (Steinberg, 2010) are the dominant ethnic

group in Burma today and are differentiated in language and culture from the many

other ethnic minority groups including the Karen, Shan, Mon and Chin. Like Steinberg

(2010) and Fink (2009), this thesis uses the term Burman to refer to members of the

largest ethnic group and the term Burmese when referring to (a) the language spoken by

the dominant Burmans, and (b) all citizens of the country. However, I acknowledge that

some people from Burma reject this latter use of the term Burmese and do not like its

association with the dominant Burmans.

3.2 Burma: an overview

In approximately 2500 BC the Mon people of Central Asia are thought to have settled

throughout the lower parts of South East Asia and modern-day Burma, followed by the

Tibeto-Burmans to whom modern Burmans trace their early ancestry. Today Burma’s

total population is an estimated 54 million, of which approximately 70% are ethnic

38

Burman. There are an estimated 130 different ethnic minority communities within seven

ethnic states (Kachin, Karen, Karenni, Shan, Chin, Mon and Arakan) which make up

60% of the territory (Ekeh & Smith, 2007), as shown in Figure 1. These states are

acknowledged in Burma’s 1947 constitution which allows traditional local leaders to

manage the day-to-day affairs in ethnic regions while the central state government

maintains absolute sovereignty over the nation. In reality, however, the “regulatory

authority [of the central state government] is neither uniform, coherent, unified, nor

unchallenged” (Callahan, 2007, p. 6). Whilst there is no reliable census data on the

ethnic minority communities, the UN and human rights organisations estimate the

largest ethnic minorities are Shan (9%) and Karen (7%). The official national language

is Burmese, but most of the ethnic minority groups speak their own languages, and

many do not speak Burmese (United Kingdom Home Office, 2009).

Burma is rich in natural resources, especially timber (particularly teak), arable land,

marine fisheries and gas, but is currently one of the poorest countries in Asia and

around 30% of the population live below poverty (Department of Immigration and

Citizenship, 2011d; Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade, 2011; Central

Intelligence Agency, 2010). On the 2010 UN Human Development Index (a measure of

wellbeing), Burma ranked a low 132nd out of 169 countries (United Nations

Development Programme, 2010). The financial system is underdeveloped, plagued by

corruption and a large black market economy, and spending is skewed towards the

military (Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade, 2011). Burma is also the world’s

second largest producer of illicit opium (Central Intelligence Agency, 2010) and

methamphetamine production is increasing (Steinberg, 2010).

39

Figure 1: Map of Burma illustrating ethnic minority areas including Karen State.

Adapted from Karen Human Rights Group website (Karen Human Rights

Group, 2005.)

40

3.2.1 Politics and power

Burma was ruled under a monarchical system until 1886 when, after a series of Anglo-

Burmese wars, Britain gained control of the country. For many years the British

administered Burma as a province of India and complete colonisation lasted for 62 years

until Burma gained independence from Britain in 1948. The nation’s independence

hero, General Aung San, was expected to have led an interim government but was

assassinated some months before independence was granted. In 1948, Prime Minister U

Nu took power and established a new constitution based on a democratically elected

parliament. The nation was in a volatile situation and intense civil war soon erupted as

the government was challenged by various communist organisations as well as ethnic

minority groups (M. Smith, 1994). In 1962 the government was overthrown by a

military coup led by General Ne Win and life rapidly changed for the citizens of Burma.

Political opposition was banned, strict censorship laws were implemented and the

“Burmese Way to Socialism” began. The country plunged into a devastating economic

and social crisis.

In the decades since, the military has violently suppressed anti-government activities,

most famously opening fire and killing around 3,000 peaceful protestors during a mass

demonstration on 8 August 1988 (known as the 8888 Uprising) (Department of Foreign

Affairs and Trade, 2011). Thousands of democracy activists fled to the jungle and

neighbouring countries. Another military coup erupted the following month. General Ne

Win stepped down and the military, calling itself the State Law and Order Restoration

Council, again assumed power.

In 1990, the military junta held democratic elections in which Aung San Suu Kyi’s

(daughter of General Aung San) party, the National League for Democracy, won an

overwhelming 392 of the 485 parliamentary seats. The State Law and Order Restoration

Council refused to validate the results and soon arrested Aung San Suu Kyi, her party

members and other democracy supporters. Whilst under house arrest, Aung San Suu

Kyi was awarded the 1991 Nobel Peace Prize.

In recent years poverty and bloodshed have continued to plague the nation. In August

2007 the junta withdrew fuel subsidies, prompting a 500% fuel price increase, and an

increase in rice prices by 50% in four months. The situation prompted pro-democracy

demonstrations in September 2007. The military government again violently suppressed

41

the protests with gunfire. The nation suffered another crisis on 2 May, 2008 when

Cyclone Nargis struck the south of Burma, killing an estimated 140,000 people and

affecting a further 2.4 million (Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade, 2011). The

military government was slow to respond whilst also initially blocking international

assistance, prompting international condemnation. In the aftermath of the cyclone, on

10 and 28 May, 2008, the government also pushed forward with national referendums

for a new constitution. The regime announced its 92.48% win; “a figure no independent

observers believed was valid” (United States Department of State, 2010, para. 1).

On 7 November, 2010, Burma held its first elections in two decades. However, the

international community (United Nations General Assembly, 2010a), including the

Australian Government, later declared the elections “fundamentally flawed … and

patently unfair” (Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade, 2011, para. 17). Many

democrats chose not to participate in the election due to unfair election laws; people

also contested the final poll in which the regime’s party (the Union Solidarity and

Development Party) won 76% of seats overall, and Thein Sein became President.

According to the Human Rights Watch (2011), the situation in Burma has not

significantly improved since the 2010 elections.

On 13 November, 2010, Aung San Suu Kyi was released from house arrest. She has

remained a prominent international and local figure and was among 43 of her party

members elected to parliament in the April 2012 by-elections. The elections were part

of General Then Sein’s wave of political and economic reforms, which seek to gain

international legitimacy for Burma and overcome Western economic sanctions.

However, the military-backed party continues to effectively hold around 80% of seats in

parliament and the current constitution bars Aung San Suu Kyi from ascending to

presidency because she married a foreigner (“Burma’s NLD”, 2012).

The Yugoslavia of Asia: ethnic politics in Burma

In common with many other post-colonial societies around the world, such as

Indonesia, Burma is a somewhat artificial entity with which many of the ethnic

minorities, in particular, have great trouble identifying … As a consequence, in

what is sometimes called the “Yugoslavia of Asia”, there is the ever-present

danger of explosive political disintegration. (Philp & Mercer, 1999, p. 27)

42

Ethnic divergence and tensions have a long history in Burma (Steinberg, 2010).

However, such divisions were intensified and strategically exploited under British rule

(Fink, 2009; Steinberg, 2010). Ethnic minorities, particularly the Karen, aligned with

British forces and faith (Christianity), and many were rewarded with access to higher

education and employment status during colonial rule (Suu Kyi, 2010). During World

War II, the Karen continued to support British forces while the Burmans aligned,

temporarily, with the Japanese. Atrocities were committed on both sides and ethnic

tensions worsened (Neiman, Soh, & Sutan, 2008).

During independence negotiations some ethnic groups petitioned the British for their

own land and nation. The Karen established the Karen National Union and advocated

for a Karen nation, Kawthoolei, independent from Burma (Karen National Union,

2011). They were, however, unsuccessful in achieving their political aims. The post-

independence Burmese Government also declined requests for ethnic group autonomy.

General Ne Win and the military junta attempted to transform the multiethnic Burma

into a homogenised state through the “widespread and systematic persecution of ethnic

minority peoples” (Philp & Mercer, 1999, p. 44) and through imposing restrictions on

the use of minority languages and dress (Bisht & Bankoti, 2004). As a result, many of

the ethnic minority groups formed armed resistance movements (including the Kachin

Independence Army, the United Wa State Army, the Shan State Army, the Karen

National Liberation Army, the Karenni Army, and the Shan State National Army) and

civil war erupted again throughout the country (Kalnin, 2010).

Following General Ne Win’s seizure of power in 1962, things changed rapidly for

ethnic minority groups including the Karen. The junta implemented a policy known as

the “Four Cuts”, which aimed to cut food, funds, intelligence and recruits to the ethnic

armies. The Four Cuts campaign wreaked havoc for villagers as the military initiated

“forced relocations of entire communities … destruction of crops … and a shoot-on-

sight policy after curfew hours” (Rajah, 2002, p. 532; M. Smith, 1994). Many ethnic

minority groups fled to remote areas and zones controlled by ethnic armies where they

could continue to practise their cultural traditions and speak their indigenous

language(s).

The Karen National Union was particularly successful in protecting the cultural identity

and wellbeing of their ethnic population and established Karen schools, hospitals and

43

administrative departments. However, after several devastating military attacks in recent

decades, the now weakened Karen army has lost much of their territory and

infrastructure and today operates from remote and mobile bases on the Thailand-Burma

border (M. Pedersen, 2008) in what has become the “longest rebellion in modern world

history” (Steinberg, 2010, p. 12).

Today widespread human rights abuses perpetrated by the military continue to affect

ethnic minority (and Burman) populations leading to massive displacement and greater

civil unrest. Despite sporadic peace negotiations and temporary cease-fires, persecution

and fighting has continued. Ethnic minority communities continue to be displaced

(Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2010a) and new waves of refugees fled Burma in

the wake of the contested 2010 election results (UNHCR, 2011a). Today Burma could

still be described as a “social volcano” (Philp & Mercer, 1999, p. 24).

3.2.2 Human rights abuses and life under the regime

The international community has continued to express concern for human rights in

Burma where “the denial of economic, social and cultural rights” affect the majority of

the population (United Nations General Assembly, 2010a, p. 18). A recent United States

Department of State (2009) human rights report on Burma documented dozens of

human rights abuses including extrajudicial killings, torture, rape, disappearances,

recruitment of child soldiers, trafficking of women and girls and forced labour.

Arbitrary arrests, detentions and disappearances are also common and widespread fear

affects family and social networks as family and friends may be detained in place of

missing activists (Fink, 2009). Citizens have little hope for justice as the judiciary is not

independent of the military government. The junta has denied UN allegations that there

“are a number of human rights abuses in Myanmar that could constitute crimes against

humanity” (United Nations General Assembly, 2010b, p. 15).

Life under the regime is characterised by mistrust, uncertainty and fear as people seek to

go about their daily life without drawing unwanted attention from the military

intelligence unit who regularly conduct surveillance operations on the general public.

According to Skidmore (2003), roadside signboards declaring “Observance of discipline

leads to safety” (p. 8), “Anyone who is riotous, destructive and unruly is our enemy” (p.

10) and “Crush all destructive elements” (p. 13) are one of many forms of political

44

propaganda and social control. The military government severely restricts freedom of

speech and press and the activities of citizens are closely monitored, including the use

of all two-way electronic communication devices. Ownership of unregistered

telephones, fax machines and computer modems warrants imprisonment (Selth, 2010).

Less than 0.2% of the population use the internet, and their usage is heavily censored

(United States Department of State, 2010). Virtually all organisations must be

registered. The government officially prohibits unauthorised outdoor assembly of more

than five persons, though this is only sporadically enforced. Censorship and propaganda

prohibit free speech and “freedom of thought” (Suu Kyi, 2010, p. 203). As we will see

later in this thesis, the damaging impact of this chronic climate of fear and silence can

deeply affect refugees long after they escape persecution.

Rural village life under the regime is also extremely difficult as the military demand

labour, food and money from villagers. Livestock and crops are confiscated or

destroyed and citizens are subject to violence, arrest, torture, execution and rape.

Village populations are forcibly displaced for economic development (Bisht & Bankoti,

2004). Villages are also often simply burned to the ground as part of the Four Cuts

policy, destruction that has been corroborated by satellite imagery (United Nations

General Assembly, 2010a). As a result of the persecution and destruction, communities

flee (without adequate supplies) into the jungle, into areas littered with landmines, and

in some cases, eventually towards Burma’s borders. The United States Department of

State’s (2009) Human Rights Report on Burma has also referred to unverified reports of

“deaths and injuries caused by security forces using civilians to clear landmines,

particularly in Karen State” (para. 92).

All Karen groups and other ethnic minority communities have been subject to

persecution in Burma. Many minority communities continue to claim that a policy of

“Burmanization” (Steinberg, 2010, p. 126) is manifest and underpins many of the

human rights abuses perpetrated against them. Their armed ethnic armies continue to

fight “for autonomy, equality, and the right to maintain their own languages and

cultures” (M. Pedersen, 2008, p. 54). However, it is important to mention that, like the

military junta, the Karen National Liberation Army and other ethnic armies have been

noted to perpetrate human rights abuses, including the use of child soldiers (M. Smith,

1994).

45

In recent years the international community has gradually increased pressure for action

against Burma’s military government. Approximately thirteen countries support UN

calls for a commission of inquiry but these efforts have been blocked by China and

Russia, which have repeatedly vetoed UN Security Council draft resolutions on Burma

(“China and Russia”, 2007; Human Rights Watch, 2011).

Human rights abuses against women

Throughout Burma, poverty, discrimination and human rights abuses disproportionately

affect women (United States Department of State, 2010; Ward, 2002). In addition to

low literacy rates, women in Burma today have low participation levels in all aspects of

public, political and professional life (International Service for Human Rights, 2008).

Sexual assault of ethnic minority women and children is a serious concern of

humanitarian organisations in the area (Alternative ASEAN Network on Burma, 1999)

and many have published reports documenting acts of sexual violence perpetrated by

Burmese military troops, frequently naming perpetrators (Karen Women’s

Organisation, 2004, 2007; Shan Human Rights Foundation & Shan Women’s Action

Network, 2002). Reports of gang rape, sexual enslavement and various methods of

sexually oriented torture (e.g., genital penetration by knives and mutilation of breasts)

are common (Apple & Martin, 2003; Caouette, Archavanitkul, & Pyne, 2000; Ward,

2002). In a harrowing report, the former UN Special Rapporteur on Myanmar,

Rajsoomer Lallah (United Nations General Assembly, 2010b) suggested that sexual

violence is a strategic weapon of war, purposively used by the military government in

Burma:

These violations have been so numerous and consistent over the past years as to

suggest that they are not simply isolated or the acts of individual misbehaviour

by middle- and lower-rank officers but rather are the result of policy at the

highest level, entailing political and legal responsibility. (p. 16)

The UN Convention on the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Women

(2008) has echoed these concerns and lamented the “apparent impunity of the

perpetrators of such violence” (para. 24). According to the United Kingdom Home

Office (2009), regime authorities have been known to “arrest and prosecute women who

reported being raped by police or soldiers” (p. 24). Spousal rape is not a crime unless

46

the wife is under 14 years of age, and there are no specific laws relating to domestic

violence or spousal abuse (United States Department of State, 2010). The leading

women’s organisation operating inside Burma is chaired and controlled by wives of

military junta leaders (United States Department of State, 2010). Ethnic minority

communities are also disproportionately affected by human trafficking, forced

prostitution and sexual enslavement (Crawford, 2006; Ward, 2002). These human rights

abuses have been linked to high mortality in a population of Karen refugees (Checchi,

Elder, Schafer, Drouhin, & Legros, 2003).

3.2.3 Religion and culture in Burma

This section briefly outlines the national religious and cultural pre-migration context in

which the participants of this study lived. It is followed by a more detailed exploration

of Karen culture in particular.

Historically an animist society, Burma is now predominantly Buddhist. There is no

official state religion but the military junta have strongly demonstrated their support for

Buddhism, as well as their dismissal of Christianity and Islam, each represented by

approximately 4% of the population (Central Intelligence Agency, 2010). In practice,

Burmese citizens enjoy fluid boundaries between religions, and most follow a spiritual

practice that is an amalgam of Buddhism and animist beliefs such as nat (spirit beings)

worship (Bodeker, Neumann, Lall, & Oo, 2005; Platz, 2003). Anthropologist David

Phanner (1966) points out that it may be “artificial to try and distinguish between

Buddhist and animistic elements in the Burmese religion” (p. 93). Nat worship was

dominant in Burma prior to the introduction of Buddhism and Hinduism from the 11th

century (Suu Kyi, 2010). Today, traditional nat festivals remain important social events,

and nat spirit mediums have retained their roles as conduits between the human and

spirit world (Tint Way, 1985). Belief in spirits, ghosts and witches (humans with

supernatural powers) is common; these entities are considered to be easily offended,

sometimes malevolent, beings who punish wrongdoers and even cause illness and death

(Spiro, 1969).

Astrologers, numerologists and mediums also feature strongly in Burmese belief

systems, and have been described as the Burmese “counterpart of the Western

psychiatrist” (Singh, 1993, p. 110). Today, important events (e.g., naming ceremonies

47

for children) and large-scale decisions (e.g., military government economic decisions)

continue to be based on auspicious numbers and dates. One well-known and disastrous

example was the military leader General Ne Win’s decision to change Burma’s

currency into multiples of his auspicious number, nine (Steinberg, 2010).

For Buddhist Burmese, values and metaphysical beliefs in karma and merit are said to

underpin daily life. Merit is, in part, accumulated through donation to the monastic

orders (Tint Way, 1985). Under British rule, Christian missionaries sought to eradicate

both Buddhist and animist practices. Burmans were largely hesitant to convert to

Christianity but the missionaries were notably successful with the Karen (see Section

3.3.3), of whom between 15% and 30% are thought to be Christian (Karen Human

Rights Group, 2008; Thawnghmung, 2008).

Burmese traditional medicine similarly represents an amalgam of Buddhist and animist

belief systems, as well as aspects of Indian Ayurvedic and Chinese medical systems

(Rao, 1968). The human body is thought to be comprised of four elements (earth, water,

fire and air). Illness is caused by an imbalance of these elements, and healers (including

traditional healers, Buddhist monks and astrologers) prescribe prayers or meditation and

herbal medicines as well as dietary and lifestyle changes to bring the elements back into

alignment.

Unlike neighbouring India, Burma has no caste system. Deference to elders is a noted

feature of society (Brant & Khaing, 1951). Historically, traditional social structures

were matriarchal with women enjoying high economic, political and social status

(Khaing, 1984; H. I. Marshall, 1997; Muller, 1994; Sein, 1958, February; Steinberg,

2010). Anthropologist Melford Spiro (1997) even described Burmese women as

enjoying “economic, legal, and social equality … unsurpassed either in Asia or in

Europe and North America” (p. 11).

Numerous anthropologists have also noted that marriage and divorce customs

emphasised the equal sharing of property and other assets (H. I. Marshall, 1997; Muller,

1994; Singh, 1993; Spiro, 1997). However, Brant and Khaing (1951) have described a

marked “double standard of sexual behaviour” (p. 443) whereby a husband’s sexual

infidelity was more likely to be quietly endured than a wife’s. It also seems that gender

parity has “coexisted with a belief in the innate superiority of males” (Muller, 1994, p.

48

613) derived from Burmese Buddhist discourse which deems men to be spiritually

superior to women (Spiro, 1997).

Today, as overall human rights in Burma have deteriorated, human rights organisations

have become increasingly concerned about gender inequity and, as noted (see Section

3.2.2), particularly gender-based violence (Muller, 1994; Shan Human Rights

Foundation & Shan Women’s Action Network, 2002; Karen Women’s Organisation,

2004, 2007). The male-dominated military has inhibited women’s ability to exercise

influence in government (Muller, 1994). There are conflicting opinions about the status

of gender equality in urban centres. Steinberg (2010) believes that urban women enjoy

gender parity in education and economic participation whereas the UN Convention on

the Elimination of All Forms of Discrimination against Women (2008) has observed a

deterioration in women’s participation across all domains.

3.3 Karen

Academic literature on Burma and the Karen remains sparse (Selth, 2010; South, 2007)

and available resources are often heavily biased in favour of particular political and

religious subgroups. In the following account of the history and recent experiences of

this group of people from Burma I have sought to present a balanced perspective.

The Karen (kəˈrɛn) people of Burma are comprised of several different but related

subgroups who together represent the second largest ethnic minority group in the region

(S. Smith, 2010). Estimates of their population vary from three to six million (Barron et

al., 2007; Karen Human Rights Group & Delang, 2000; M. Smith, 1994)

They are said to descend from the same ancestors as the Mongolian people (Karen

Human Rights Group & Delang, 2000) and are thought to have migrated to Burma from

Tibet and Southern China sometime “after the Mon but before the Burmese” (Bisht &

Bankoti, 2004, p. 294). The Karen are often referred to as the hill tribe or forest people

as large numbers live(d) in isolated mountainous rural areas of eastern Burma. The term

Karen is thought to originate from derogatory Burmese names for inhabitants of the

forest and mountain areas (H. I. Marshall, 1997). At any rate, the modern usage of the

term appears to have been legitimised and made respectable by missionaries and British

colonisers.

49

The Karen State (Kawthoolei) spans northwest to southeast along the Thailand-Burma

border (Neiman et al., 2008) (see Figure 1). Geographically the Karen population may

be divided into three groups: the Sgaw and Pwo (Southern), Karenni (Central), and Po-o

(Northern) (Barron et al., 2007). The Sgaw and Pwo subgroups together constitute over

70% of the Karen population (M. Smith, 1994). These and other subgroups are

linguistically and culturally dissimilar and each Karen group also has its own name

“that is different from the name by which they are known to outsiders” (Barron et al.,

2007, p. 28). The Pwo (pronounced “poh”) call themselves G’Ploung and the Sgaw

(pronounced “skaw”) call themselves Pwakenyaw1.

3.3.1 Language

Linguistic differences between subgroups are significant and various Karen dialects

“are for the most part mutually unintelligible” (Barron et al., 2007, p. 28). There are

three main Karen dialects, Sgaw, Eastern Pwo and Western Pwo (Platz, 2003), which

are vastly different. Sgaw uses abugida Burmese script, whereas Pwo uses Mon

characters. Karen languages are monosyllabic and tonal, and are members of the Tibeto-

Burman language group (Bisht & Bankoti, 2004; Manson, 2010). Within this language

family, Karen dialects are unique in their placement of the object after the verb (Bisht &

Bankoti, 2004; Manson, 2010). Many older Karen also speak Burmese and younger

Karen are often fluent in Thai due to living in refugee camps on the Thailand-Burma

border.

According to Sino-Tibetan language researchers at the University of California

Department of Linguistics (D. Solnit, personal communication, 1 February, 2011), and

other Karennic language experts (K. Manson, personal communication, 1 February,

2011), there is no standardised Roman script for transliteration of the Sgaw or Pwo

Karen languages. There are also few published language resources. However, Christian

organisations have produced a number of Karen language materials (Gilmore, 1898)

and other resources exist online (Barron et al., 2007; Drum Publication Group, 2011).

1 There is no consensus regarding the spelling of Karen words. Variations include Pwakenyaw (Bisht &

Bankoti, 2004, p. 292) and Pga k’nyau (Bisht & Bankoti, 2004, p. 293), P’ganyaw (Platz, 2003, p. 490),

Pgha K’Nyaw (p. 8) (H. I. Marshall, 1997).

50

The traditional Sgaw Karen naming structure has only a given name (e.g., Paw, Po,

Myint) and, for adults, includes the identification of Saw (Mr) or Na (Ms). Karen names

often hold meaning (e.g., flower or moon) and may also denote a significant event that

occurred before or around the time of a child’s birth. People are also addressed

according to their kinship relationship to the speaker (e.g., aunt, grandfather) and Karen

also generally refer to unrelated older men and women as uncle and aunt respectively.

3.3.2 Karen culture

It is customary in the sparse literature to describe the Karen as shy, simple, quiet and

peaceloving (Barron et al., 2007; Bisht & Bankoti, 2004; Gilmore, 1898; Karen Human

Rights Group & Delang, 2000; Manson, 2010; Neiman et al., 2008; Platz, 2003; S.

Smith, 2010). In the academic literature, this tradition appears to have been first

articulated by the anthropologist Harry Ignatius Marshall (1997), who first published his

account of the Karen in 1922, and noted:

I once asked an educated Karen what he thought was the chief characteristic of

his race, and he immediately replied that they are a people who can be afraid.

Centuries of subjugation and oppression have filled them with fear … The

Karen is led into all sorts of difficulties by his timidity. He is apprehensive and

desirous of avoiding trouble with officers or others. (p. 22)

Karen communities are collectivist and relatively egalitarian. In common with other

collectivist societies, displays of anger and confrontation are avoided (Barron et al.,

2007; Neiman et al., 2008). Accordingly, communication is often indirect. Karen may

modestly say “no” rather than enthusiastically receive a gift. Similarly they may politely

say “yes” to a request when they in fact mean “no”. As Platz (2003) observed of Karen

communities: “A straight answer is rarely given in a society built on avoiding conflicts”

(p. 486). As in Burmese society, absolute deference to and respect for elders and

seniority are expected.

Traditional Karen villages are formed by 20 to 30 nuclear families led by a village chief

and a council of elders who provide both spiritual and political leadership (Bisht &

Bankoti, 2004). In Burma, houses are made of bamboo and wood raised on stilts, and

set in a cleared section of the forest. Parents, children and grandparents often live

51

together in one hut with a central room for gathering and smaller areas for sleeping.

Emphasising the collectivist nature of the Karen village, H. I. Marshall (1997) reported:

There was little occasion for individual initiative among the Karen, on account

of the important part played by communal activity amongst them … This

communal sharing was so much the order of the day that personal rights were

more or less disregarded. If a man got a few seeds and planted a garden near his

house, he was fortunate, as is sometimes still the case in the hills, if he gathered

half the crop he had planted. His neighbors, asking no leave, helped themselves

generously without hesitation and perhaps without intending to steal. (p. 130)

Karen villages were traditionally patriarchal, but it is often said that Karen traditional

culture was characterised by relative gender equality (Barron et al., 2007; Bisht &

Bankoti, 2004; Neiman et al., 2008). Karen families and villages are linked through

their matrilineal descendants. Most young people are free to choose their marital

partners and, after marriage, live with the wife’s family (H. I. Marshall, 1997).

Premarital sex and divorce are strongly discouraged and untoward behaviour is

addressed by the village elders. Bisht and Bantoki (2004) note that, in the rare

circumstance of divorce, property was divided equally between the couple.

Karen families are typically large, and intergenerational parenting enables younger

parents to continue daily chores related to farming, food preparation and weaving. Adult

children assume responsibility for their elderly parents (H. I. Marshall, 1997) though, in

reality, this responsibility often falls on daughters (Bisht & Bankoti, 2004; H. I.

Marshall, 1997). Farming and agriculture form the centrepoint of daily life, cultural

practices, celebrations and economy. The Karen are also noted farmers as well as

hunters and elephant handlers (Barron et al., 2007; Neiman et al., 2008).

3.3.3 Karen religion

A merged Buddhist-animist practice is the dominant religion of all Karen groups in

Burma (Barron et al., 2007; Neiman et al., 2008). According to their traditional animist

beliefs, each person has 37 souls (kla) which reside in and around their body, are

essential for wellbeing, and are at risk of being lost or stolen by other spirits. In his

anthropological study of the Karen, H. I. Marshall (1997) noted that funeral customs

52

varied amongst Karen tribes, and could include animist practices such as animal

sacrifice as well as a host of other customs derived from Buddhist practices.

More than any other ethnic group in Burma, large numbers of Karen have converted to

Christianity, particularly Baptist denominations. Sgaw Karen approached Christian

conversion with remarkable enthusiasm and “on a scale unprecedented in Southeast

Asia” (Bisht & Bankoti, 2004, p. 302). Traditional beliefs in a God (Ywa) and other

legends appear to have overlapped with the messages of missionaries and were soon

appropriated to foster conversion. Legends also reportedly included the story of a lost

book that would be returned by a white man arriving by sea (Barron et al., 2007; H. I.

Marshall, 1997; Platz, 2003).

Existing political factions and nationalist sentiments may have also paved the way for

Christian missionaries seeking to convert the Karen. Having been second-class citizens

to the dominant Burmans, the Karen were able to rapidly secure access to resources

including schools and healthcare through their Christian links. An educated Christian-

Karen elite subsequently held higher social positions than non-Christian Karen and rose

through the ranks alongside Burmans as British colonial servants. Today Christian

Karen are overrepresented in refugee camps where, according to Keenan (2005), 65%

of Karen are Christian, and only 28% are Buddhist. In comparison, an estimated 70% to

85% of Karen in Burma are Buddhist (Karen Human Rights Group, 2008;

Thawnghmung, 2008). According to Platz (2003), today animist Buddhist and Christian

Karen coexist both within nuclear family units and as a broad societal unit. However, as

we will see, religious and political tensions (though often hard to perceive and quantify)

are distinguishing features of the Karen social climate.

3.3.4 Karen traditional medicine

Like traditional Burmese cultural and religious beliefs (see Section 3.2.3), Karen

traditional medicine combines several influences, most notably, Indian Ayurvedic and

traditional Chinese medicine systems (Barron et al., 2007; Bodeker et al., 2005). In

addition, Karen traditional animist beliefs emphasised the importance of maintaining

one’s personal souls (kla) to ensure health. Various ceremonies (e.g., the wrist-tying

ceremony) and practices are traditionally performed to retain one’s souls and to

maintain good health. Traditional healers may use a variety of herbs and plants to cure

53

and ward off spirits causing illness. Turmeric is one of the most commonly used plants

and it is applied both internally and topically. Historically healers would also make

animal offerings to drive away spirits and restore health though this practice is less

likely to be practised by Christian and many Buddhist Karen. Christian Karen are also

often opposed to the herbalist practices of traditional healers.

Finally, as the Karen have inhabited Thailand-Burma border refugee camps since the

1980s, many are also familiar with Western medicine through contact with various

humanitarian organisations working in the region. Bodeker et al., (2005) interviewed 59

(including 30 Karen) outpatient refugees on the Thailand-Burma border. The

respondents were asked about their knowledge and use of traditional medicine and

cumulatively reported 271 traditional remedies. Most had tried traditional medicines but

many were presently geographically distant from traditional healers. Less commonly,

patients also reported believing in nats (spirit entities) (29%) and witches (20%), and

perceived the spirit world as a potential cause of ill health. Most of those who believed

in the nats regularly engaged in observances such as wearing amulets or making

donations to appease them.

3.4 The Thailand-Burma border

This section sets the scene for refugee camp life on the Thailand-Burma border. This is

important subject matter as most refugees from Burma arriving in Australia and around

the world have been acculturated in this environment.

The refugee camp situation along the Thailand-Burma border has been described by the

UNHCR as “one of the most protracted in the world” (UNHCR, 2010a, para. 6). Today

around 140,000 refugees live in ten camp sites (see Figure 2), many of which were

established in the 1980s. The ethnic profile of the Thailand-Burma border camps is

predominantly Karen (79%) with smaller numbers of ethnic Karenni (10%), Burman

(4%) and Shan (0.5%), as well as others (Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2010a).

Rohinga refugees are also present, though larger numbers of Rohinga refugee camps are

found in Bangladesh (UNHCR, 2010b).

Before describing refugee camp life it is important to note that the majority of refugees

from Burma do not successfully reach this relative safety. Civilians living in conflict-

affected areas frequently flee to the jungle within Burma’s borders and become

54

internally displaced people. The UNHCR estimates there are approximately 62,000

internally displaced people inside Burma, and a total of 800,000 stateless persons (not

considered as Burmese nationals according to legislation) (UNHCR, 2011a). These

people lack protection and access to food, shelter, healthcare and education. An

estimated further one to two million (Brees, 2008a, 2008b; Caouette & Pack, 2002;

Chalamwong, 2004) intermix (mostly as illegal migrants) in neighbouring countries,

with the largest numbers in Thailand and Bangladesh. Most of these refugees remain

stateless, lacking rights and protection from the host governments (Brees, 2010; Lee &

Glaister, 2008).

55

Figure 2: Map of internally displaced people and refugee camps along Thailand-

Burma border. Adapted from the camp population reports by the Thailand

Burma Border Consortium (n.d.).

3.4.1 Hosts and providers

The refugee situation is a sensitive topic for the Thai Government, who have remained

“reluctant hosts” (Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2010b, p. ix) to refugees from

Burma. Thailand is not a signatory to the 1951 Convention on Refugees (UNHCR,

2010a) and technically does not recognise refugee status. More commonly, the Thai

Government has referred to those in refugee camps as “temporarily displaced”

(Caouette & Pack, 2002, p. 7). Some asylum seekers from Burma living outside the

56

camps have official work permits but many do not (H. Lang, 2001). Consequently, they

are considered illegal immigrants and subject to arrest and deportation. As in other parts

of the world, asylum seekers in Thailand are also often scapegoats for social problems,

and are perceived as a burden on the local economy and infrastructure in spite of the

economic benefits that Thai businesses gain from their cheap labour (Lang, 2001).

Early support for refugees on the border was, and remains, provided by the Thailand

Burma Border Consortium (formally the Committee for the Coordination of Services to

Displaced Persons in Thailand). This umbrella agency is, today, a consortium of around

a dozen humanitarian organisations who provide food, shelter and other relief assistance

to the refugees and internally displaced people (Thailand Burma Border Consortium,

2007).

In 1998 the Thai Government formally permitted UNHCR operations in the refugee

camps (Lang, 2001). Many UNHCR programs are implemented with, and through, the

Thailand Burma Border Consortium who retain the role of overseeing camp

management. With official UNHCR presence, camp residents were able to apply for

Refugee Status Determination and registration. However, in 2005, the Thai Government

halted the UNHCR Refugee Status Determination programme. Refugees arriving since

this time have had no opportunity to obtain official registration, recognition or

protection as refugees. The Thai Government permitted a camp registration program

(but not official Refugee Status Determination) in 2007, but any new arrivals in the

camp since 2005 have no guarantee of protection in Thailand (Lee & Glaister, 2008).

As a further outcome, official UNHCR camp population estimates differ vastly from

those of the Thailand Burma Border Consortium, who attempt to recognise all camp

residents (registered or not). For example, the UNHCR camp figures for August 2011

were 89,512, but the Thailand Burma Border Consortium provided assistance to a camp

caseload of 142,250. Around 40% of camp residents are not officially recognised as

refugees. After more than 25 years the situation on the Thailand-Burma border has been

described as “a marathon with no sign of a finish line” (Thailand Burma Border

Consortium, 2010b, p. 1).

57

3.4.2 Life in camps

Karen refugees are birds inside a cage that get fed on a regular basis but are not

able to fly. (Karen refugee quoted by Fuertes, 2010, p. 20)

Inside the camps, refugees live in makeshift bamboo huts similar to traditional village

housing. Most camps are surrounded by barbed wire and patrolled by Thai military

personnel and police. Residents are mostly dependent on aid as they are not allowed to

work. Camps are overseen by elected camp committees who work in conjunction with

the Thailand Burma Border Consortium (and numerous other humanitarian aid

organisations) to provide access to health, education and training inside the camps.

Many of these services are explicitly linked to Christian organisations and, in turn, the

Sgaw Karen ethnic group (or rather the educated Christian Karen elite) who dominate

leadership positions in the camps (Horstmann, 2010). Food rations and other resources

are distributed through the camp committees. These rations meet the World Food

Programme and UNHCR minimum standard of 2.1000 kilocalories per person, per day

and largely consist of rice, fish paste, mung beans and salt (Thailand Burma Border

Consortium, 2009).

One concerning effect of protracted camp situations is the “culture of dependency”

(Thawnghmung, 2008, p. 21) that can be fostered by long-term dependency on aid for

rations and all means of social support. Moreover, according to Fuertes (2010),

protracted camp living can also lead to the weakening of traditional knowledge and

customs. For example, in the absence of land to farm, children inside the camp lack

knowledge of traditional farming practices. Consequently, they lack necessary skills for

rural survival should they return to their homeland in Burma.

However, the image of refugees as passive dependents languishing in refugee camps,

while true for many, overlooks many aspects of refugee life (Saltsman, 2009). Refugee

agency should not be underestimated. Refugees who leave the confines of the camp are

in grave danger of arrest, abuse, exploitation and deportation. And yet, as noted, the

majority of refugees from Burma are not in camps. Brees (2008b) explored refugees’

reasons for bypassing camps and self-settling (often illegally) in Thailand. She found

that, most commonly, “they prefer to have no support than to lose their freedom of

movement and self-reliance” (Brees, 2008b, p. 381). Other reasons included fear of

entering camps because of rebel groups, and ethnic or religious differences with the

58

camp majority. Some avoided camp confinement so they could continue to search for

family members missing along the border region. With little hope of registration or

resettlement post-2005, others saw no option but to live illegally in Thailand.

Family-splitting is a recognised strategy of populations affected by forced migration and

is commonly used by refugees from Burma. Through splitting up, families spread both

risk and opportunities (for income) and increase their chances of survival (Brees, 2010).

They risk arrest and deportation to seek employment in Thailand and Malaysia where

they often find work in farming and fishing, construction work, sewing, domestic work,

carpentry, rubbish collection, food preparation, brewing alcohol and sex work (Brees,

2008b). They often face host community hostility but experience greater opportunities

than are available in camps. They maintain strong family ties and send remittances back

into the camps. Risk is spread but not eliminated.

Social, cultural and religious customs are also often strong in the camps. As Horstmann

(2010) explains, “While the mobility in the camp is highly restricted, the relationship of

people to God is intensified” (p. 21). Refugees in camps also raise money for

themselves and internally displaced people who remain in Burma. Despite limited

resources they create business networks, sell and barter, and seek to supplement their

rations. The fortunate few with televisions advertise movie nights, others sell car

batteries to be used as electricity generators (Vogler, 2006). Some obtain stipendiary

roles within the camps through international organisations such as the Thailand Burma

Border Consortium, UNHCR and Red Cross. At night, using candles or car battery

powered light bulbs, students and intellectuals study. Moreover, many refugees

maintain strong transnational relationships with friends and family, and through

religious or political organisations.

Safety

The refugee camps on the Thailand-Burma border are also environmentally and

politically dangerous. The land area provided by the Thai Government is frequently

hilly and often unsuitable for supporting the “bamboo cities” (Thailand Burma Border

Consortium, 2010b, p. 1) built to house the refugees. Their huts are vulnerable to

natural hazards including heavy rain and landslides (Vogler, 2006). Some camps are

also inaccessible by road during the rainy season. The provision of one blanket for

59

every two refugees is often insufficient during cold periods, though most have access to

nets to protect against malarial mosquitos (Vogler, 2006). Moreover, the camps are also

“a site of mobilization of young [ethnic army] soldiers” (Horstmann, 2010, p. 17).

Consequently, they have come under attack from the Burmese military. Saltsman (2009)

notes that the camps “can be equally if not more brutal than the situation at home from

which the forced migrant has fled” (pp. 3–4).

Health issues

Most of the ethnic minority populations who arrive in the refugee camps have not had

access to proper healthcare inside Burma2. They continue to face limited access to care

within the camps where problems with sanitation, overcrowding, infection and disease

are widespread. It is important to explore the health problems and healthcare

experiences of refugees as this information is relevant to understanding their wellbeing

in Australia.

Health problems inside the camp include hepatitis B infection, HIV/AIDS, malaria,

parasitic infections (Denburg et al., 2007), tuberculosis (Power et al., 2010),

malnutrition leading to stunting, anaemia and beri-beri (vitamin B deficiency and a

leading cause of high infant mortality rates) (Luxemburger et al., 2003); and high blood

lead levels in children (thought to be caused by traditional medicines and car batteries

used to power camp huts) (Mitchell et al., 2012). The rate of methamphetamine use in

the region has been increasing (Kobori et al., 2009) but more common intoxicants are

alcohol and betel nut. Betel nut is a mild stimulant that many Karen chew. Regular use

stains teeth and gums, and can lead to dental problems and oral cancers (Neiman et al.,

2008).

Various humanitarian organisations (including Malteser of Germany, American

Refugee Committee and the International Rescue Committee) provide limited

healthcare services including testing (and sometimes treatment) for tuberculosis and

HIV/AIDS, vaccinations, family planning and maternal healthcare. These services are

2 Inside Burma, when and where accessible, internally displaced people receive care from the Backpack

Health Worker Team and the Free Burma Rangers. These groups are mobile unarmed paramilitary units

of refugees who are trained to provide education, resources, advocacy and primary healthcare (Mahn et

al., 2008).

60

generally rudimentary and vary considerably between camps. In addition, camp

residents also attend a network of traditional healers in camps and in villages along the

Thailand-Burma border (Bodeker et al., 2005).

Refugees from camps closer to the Thai town of Mae Sot frequently seek medical care

at the Mae Tao Clinic, another very rudimentary service but one of the best available.

The Mae Tao clinic boasts a blood laboratory and blood bank, eye care department,

reproductive health unit, malaria treatment unit, prosthetics unit and surgical

department. These services are indispensable to refugees on the border but are also

limited and primitive. For example, as I witnessed during my time at the clinic in 2007,

amputations (a common surgery due to landmines) were not necessarily performed by

doctors. Moreover, with no anaesthetist and no general anaesthetic, patients were

conscious. They were treated and cared for mostly by fellow refugees who were trained

as medics. Currently, the surgical ward is staffed by medics and, for six months of the

year, a volunteer surgeon (Mae Tao Clinic, 2011).

Women’s issues

Refugee women in the camps are at risk of gender-based violence including domestic

violence (Lambert & Pickering, 2003) and rape (by fellow refugees as well as Burmese

and Thai military, Thai civilians and Thai police) and there is little recourse available

for survivors (Vogler, 2006; Ward, 2002).

The close quarters and lack of privacy between huts creates a noisy and chaotic

environment, and places pressure on women who are expected to maintain modesty.

Young women also “voluntarily” (in the context of limited choice) enter into

relationships with Thai solders. Such relationships may provide women with some

relative protection and access to resources (e.g., food, gifts and money). Some women

also hope that these relationships may lead to freedom from the refugee camp.

However, Thai soldiers are prohibited from marrying camp residents and many already

have Thai wives (Vogler, 2006).

In the absence of a formal state or government, camp committees (dominated by men)

address the behaviour of camp residents. According to Lambert and Pickering (2003),

camp committees often fail to adequately address women’s concerns. In turn, women

have expressed “their frustration over the inability to address the actions or inaction of

61

the camp committees” (p. 42). Vogler (2006) found that “speaking about domestic

violence remains a taboo subject among camp residents” (p. 10). However, despite their

silence, women appeared to freely utilise a domestic violence shelter established in the

camp.

In the past decade women’s organisations have been increasingly active in camps and

have pushed to ensure women are involved in camp committees and decision-making.

The Thailand Burma Border Consortium has supported these efforts and women now

comprise 33% of stipendiary camp committee members (Thailand Burma Border

Consortium, 2011a). Increasingly vocal about their rights and concerns, these women

can be disruptive towards traditional practices as they demand participation in decision-

making and political affairs (Brees, 2010).

3.4.3 Access to resettlement

As previously noted, official UNHCR operations on the Thailand-Burma border refugee

camps began in 1998. From 2004, the UNHCR also began a resettlement program

which has since assisted approximately 90,000 refugees from Burma (International

Organization for Migration, 2011b). However, despite the resettlement program, camp

numbers have not reduced due to new influxes of refugees fleeing the military junta

(Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2011b). Moreover, since the Thai Government

halted the UNHCR Refugee Status Determination programme in 2005, refugees arriving

in the camp in the past seven years have no hope of resettlement.

This chapter has provided a thorough description of the pre-migration social, political

and cultural milieu surrounding Karen refugees. A more detailed discussion of the

Karen community in Sydney (where this research was undertaken) will follow Chapter

7, while Chapter 4 describes Australia’s refugee program and the services available to

those who resettle.

62

Chapter 4

The Local Context: Refugee Resettlement in Australia

4.1 Introduction

This chapter describes the Australian Government’s refugee-related policies and

programs, and briefly explores the prevailing social climate concerning refugee issues.

The social services provided to refugees who resettle in Australia are described in detail

in order to provide a context for understanding the research findings, particularly those

reported in Chapter 8.

Australia is a member of the UNHCR Executive Committee, and signatory to both the

1951 Convention (since 1954) and the 1967 Protocol (since 1973) (Department of

Immigration and Citizenship, 2011n). Within the international community, the

Australian Government provide international development assistance as well as

resettlement and, since the end of World War II, has formally resettled around 750,000

refugees (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2009a). Today Australia is one

of world’s top three refugee resettlement countries, as determined by the number of

places made available to the UNHCR. However, this reference is misleading as the

largest number of refugees are hosted by developing countries (Tribe, 2002; Zwi &

Alvarez-Castillo, 2003). Indeed, for each billion dollar of Gross Domestic Product

(GDP), Australia hosted 0.6 refugees and was ranked 77th in the world. In relation to

population size, Australia hosted 1.1 refugees per 1,000 head of population and was

ranked 68th (UNHCR, 2010c).

Between 1945 and the mid-1960s Australia first resettled refugees from Europe and

then from South America. Throughout this period, immigration received widespread

community support as the Australian Government actively aimed to increase population

size. In the wake of the Vietnam war, 150,000 refugees from South East Asia were

resettled. However, by 1978 the immigration program was completely reviewed as the

“demand for a new life in Australia exceeded the places available” (Department of

Immigration and Citizenship, 2010a, p. 268). The Determination of Refugee Status

committee was established and officials travelled to South East Asia to “select

refugees” and “dissuade” (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2010a, p. 268)

further migrants. Refugees from the Middle East and Africa also arrived in Australia in

63

the early 1980s, followed by refugees from Cambodia, China, Kosovar, East Timor and

Burma in the 1990s. Public and international interest in Australian immigration policy

grew in the mid-1990s as the Australian Government developed new strategies to

manage asylum seekers’ claims (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2010a).

4.2 Australia’s current refugee program3

Australia’s current humanitarian program has the following two components:

1. The offshore component for people who, whilst overseas, are found to be

refugees according to the Convention.

2. The onshore component for people who seek asylum in Australia but whose

refugee status has not yet been assessed according to the definition set forth in

the Convention.

During 2009–10 the majority (67.1%) of all humanitarian entrants to Australia were

processed through the offshore component and most refugees from Burma have entered

through this scheme. Burma was not among the top ten source countries of onshore

asylum seekers between 2007–10 and of refugees from Burma granted visas in

Australia during 2009–10, only 3.8% were processed through the onshore component

(Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2010a). In 2011, only 33 adult men (and

no women) from Burma were living in immigration detention facilities for onshore

humanitarian entrants (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2011n). From these

statistics it seems that the onshore component is not particularly relevant to this thesis.

However, onshore component issues remain pertinent because they contribute

significantly to public opinion towards refugees in general and this social climate

affects the resettlement of Karen refugee women.

4.2.1 Onshore component

Most onshore asylum seekers enter Australia by air and the remainder by boat

(officially known as “irregular maritime arrivals”). Those who arrive by boat have often

endured harrowing sea journeys organised by human traffickers. Once in Australia, the

3 The Australian Government’s humanitarian program follows the financial year 1 July to 30 June

(Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2011n). Consequently, all data related to refugees is

reported over the same twelve month period.

64

processes experienced by humanitarian entrants under the offshore and onshore

programs are vastly different. Offshore humanitarian entrants are already granted

permanent residency. They are assisted with accessing accommodation in the

community as well as other social services. In contrast, onshore asylum seekers are

typically placed into immigration detention centres or community detention (residing in

the community and moving freely but ineligible to work or access some social services)

whilst their application for a Protection visa is lodged.

In the past several years, the Australian Government has faced intense local and

international pressure and criticism of its policy and protection obligations to refugees.

The Refugee Council of Australia has reported on the negative public discourse towards

onshore humanitarian entrants (particularly asylum seekers arriving by boat), which

erupted due to political rhetoric fuelled by “sensationalist” (Refugee Council of

Australia, 2011a, p. 129) media that portrayed these entrants as criminals, “illegal

immigrants” and “queue jumpers”. According to the council, this social climate has led

to increased hostility and racism towards humanitarian entrants in general. Indeed,

research has demonstrated Australians’ polarised and alarmingly prejudicial attitudes

towards refugees (Schweitzer, Perkoulidis, Krome, & Ludlow, 2005). The government

and numerous other organisations have recently sought to shift public discourse.

However, political divisions have remained and, at the time of writing, the topic and

debate regarding Australia’s legislation and international obligations towards onshore

entrants remained heated.

4.2.2 Offshore component

The following four Australian visas are available to refugees identified and referred by

the UNHCR (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2011o):

Refugee visa for people who have received UNHCR Refugee Status

Determination and are outside of their country of origin.

In country Special Humanitarian visa for people with UNHCR Refugee Status

Determination who remain in their home country.

Emergency Rescue visa for people who remain in their home country but are in

urgent need of resettlement.

65

Woman at Risk visa for women and girls with UNHCR Refugee Status

Determination who are living outside of their country of origin and who have

increased protection and safety concerns.

In addition, the Global Special Humanitarian Program and visa was introduced in 1981

to allow citizens and residents of Australia to support applications for individuals (most

often immediate family members) who are subject to substantial discrimination in their

home country. Recipients of this visa subclass receive less support and are expected to

be assisted by their proposer in Australia (Department of Immigration and Citizenship,

2011o).

Australia has gradually increased the number of humanitarian program places over the

past few years and offered 13,770 in 2009–10. Refugees from Burma comprised the

largest refugee population (1,959) entering Australia during this period, followed by

refugees from Iraq (1,688), Bhutan (1,144) and Afghanistan (951) (Australian Bureau of

Statistics, 2009). Woman at Risk visas comprised 13.4% of all 2009–10 humanitarian

visas (Department of Immigration and Citizenship, 2010a). Refugee women from

Burma were the largest recipients of this visa during this period and second largest (next

to women from Afghanistan) during the three years prior.

4.3 Initial resettlement services for refugees

Procedures and protocols for UNHCR staff and host countries are provided in the

UNHCR Resettlement Handbook. However, the policies and procedures taken by

individual host countries are not universal and can vary considerably (UNHCR, 2004).

In Australia, the humanitarian settlement program is managed by the Department of

Immigration and Citizenship (DIAC). The following section outlines the procedures and

services DIAC provide to humanitarian entrants4. This information provides a context

for the findings, particularly those discussed in Chapters 8 and 13.

4 This description of the Australian Governments settlement program was correct at the time of writing

and generally describes the services that were available to the participants of this study. However,

refugee-related policies and services in Australia were undergoing review during the time of this research

and numerous aspects of the program were changed during the data collection.

66

67

4.3.1 Pre-departure

The following services are provided to refugees before their departure to Australia.

The Australian cultural orientation program

This one-week voluntary pre-departure program is delivered by the International

Organization for Migration on behalf of DIAC. The course is facilitated by a speaker

who shares the local language and provides refugees with information about living in

Australia including Australian geography, culture, communication, money management,

housing arrangements, public transport, law, healthcare, education and other social

services (DIAC, 2011g).

Pre-departure medical screening

Within 72 hours of departure, refugees coming to Australia undertake a mandatory

health check which, in most locations, includes: tuberculosis evaluation, testing and

treatment for malaria and parasites, and immunisations for measles, mumps and rubella.

Departure may be delayed in cases of active illness (DIAC, 2011h).

Travel and visa arrangements

Travel arrangements and visas are organised and paid for by the Australian Government

prior to departure for all humanitarian entrants (other than entrants under the Special

Humanitarian Program, who are sponsored by their proposer in Australia). As noted,

refugees arriving through the UNHCR resettlement program (e.g., refugees in the

offshore component) are given permanent residence in Australia.

4.3.2 Post-arrival: the humanitarian settlement services program

In 2005, the Australian Government implemented the Integrated Humanitarian

Settlement Strategy, the precursor to its current Humanitarian Settlement Services

(HSS) program (Refugee Council of Australia, 2011b). A range of federal, state and

local government agencies; non-government organisations; and not-for-profit

community groups are contracted by DIAC to provide HSS. Consequently, HSS

providers differ across and within states. This program is generally provided for around

six months but may be extended for refugees with greater need (DIAC, 2010c).

68

Coordinated case management model

Some refugees have considerable support from friends and family already residing in

Australia. Others may already be proficient in English, and arrive relatively ready to

enter into the workforce. Consequently, case management plans are developed and HSS

provided according to need of individuals and their families. All entrants are assigned a

local HSS case manager who speaks their language and are assessed in relation to their

HSS needs.

Initial support

Initial settlement support provided by case managers includes:

1. Arrival reception at airport.

2. Transit to short- or long-term accommodation and provision of basic household

goods including some kitchen equipment, linen and food.

3. Initial orientation to, and information about, surroundings and culture.

4. Assistance accessing social services including

Centrelink (the Australian Government service responsible for social security

payments);

Medicare (the national healthcare system);

banking;

schools;

public transport;

health services;

long-term accommodation;

English language classes (see Section 4.4); and

further additional settlement services (see Section 4.4).

Entrants are expected to have adequately adjusted to life in Australia within six to

twelve months. According to DIAC, markers of refugee adjustment are:

residing in long-term accommodation;

linked to required social services;

school-age children enrolled and attending school; and

an understanding of “the critical messages of the orientation program [and] the

skills and knowledge to independently access services” (DIAC, 2011f, para. 14).

69

Finances

Through Centrelink, refugees are entitled to the same social security payments as other

Australian permanent residents. As of March 2011, the maximum weekly allowance for

unemployed adults was $237.45 and $345.45 for pensioners, disability pensioners and

carers (e.g., people who care for family members with a severe disability or medical

condition) (DIAC, 2011l). Parents and guardians were also eligible for a range of

childcare rebates and payments (Department of Human Services, 2011).

Location

According to DIAC (2011l), “Refugees are often located close to family members”

(para. 10). Entrants without existing family links in Australia are, when possible,

located in metropolitan areas with low population growth.

Housing

Entrants are assisted with short-term accommodation but must begin looking for longer-

term solutions and must join waiting lists for public housing with other Australian

permanent residents.

4.4 Ongoing settlement services

The following additional settlement services are also provided by DIAC but differ in

that they are also available to other migrants (i.e., not just humanitarian entrants)

settling in Australia (DIAC, 2011i).

Adult migrant English program (AMEP)

English language tuition is provided under the AMEP, which is Australia’s largest HSS

program. Under the AMEP, eligible migrants and refugees are provided with 510 hours

of English language training, career counselling and general settlement assistance.

Participation in the AMEP is voluntary. Eligible migrants are required to register for the

AMEP within three months of arrival and commence classes within twelve months.

Free childcare is available for under school-age children during class times. Thirteen

service providers are contracted to provide AMEP services nationally and all follow the

Certificate in Spoken and Written English curriculum (DIAC, 2011b). Clients are first

assessed according to three levels: Level 1 classes provide education for students who

70

cannot speak or write English, Level 2 classes are directed at post-beginner students and

focus on basic transactional proficiency whereas Level 3 classes are for students with

functional English (DIAC, 2011k).

According to DIAC (2010b), clients may choose from the following range of learning

options:

Full- or part-time classroom tuition in formal or community-based settings; full-

time tuition requires a minimum of 20 hours of study.

Distance learning (supported by telephone contact with a qualified teacher).

The Home Tutor Scheme, which provides language assistance by a trained

volunteer in the client’s home.

Self-paced e-modules (a supplementary package for independent learning).

Through AMEP providers, refugees may also access the following programs and

services:

Fee-free translation of personal documents such as birth and marriage

certificates, medical reports and qualifications (DIAC, 2011m).

Settlement courses designed to assist with settlement and orientation to

Australian culture and society (including information about laws and social

services).

The Special Preparatory Program. This program provides additional hours of

education to entrants with limited pre-migration education or particularly

difficult pre-migration experiences (e.g., torture). Entrants under 25 years old are

eligible for an extra 400 hours of tuition. Older adults may be eligible for 100

additional hours. Students in this program are provided with bilingual support

during their initial weeks in language classes (DIAC, 2011k).

The Settlement Language Pathways to Employment and Training Program. This

program provides entrants with 200 hours of employment-focused English

language training including 80 hours’ work experience.

Translating and Interpreting Service (TIS) National

TIS is a nation-wide 24-hour service providing on-site and telephone interpreting

services in approximately 170 languages. These services are available to humanitarian

71

entrants as well as other Australian residents. Services may be free of charge or user-

pays depending on the circumstances which differ in public and private sectors. For

example, private medical practitioners providing Medicare rebateable services and

pharmacies are eligible for free TIS services (DIAC, 2009b). TIS contracts interpreters

who are accredited or recognised by the Australian National Accreditation Authority for

Translators and Interpreters Limited (NAATI).

Complex case support program

This program provides specialised case management for entrants with exceptional needs

including mental health, physical health and family violence difficulties for up to five

years after arrival (DIAC, 2011c). At the time of writing, this program was undergoing

significant change. The program had been implemented in 2008 and reviewed in 2010.

In 2011 DIAC announced a request for tender to redevelop a humanitarian service

panel. The panel will be comprised of organisations that are specialised to provide

services for clients with complex problems.

Subgroup support

Additional services and programs were available to subgroups of humanitarian entrants

and asylum seekers. For example, the unaccompanied humanitarian minors program

provides support to refugee children without parents or relatives in Australia (DIAC,

2011i). The community assistance support program assists asylum seekers with

complex physical, social or mental health needs whilst their applications for protection

are processed (DIAC, 2011e).

Settlement grants program

This program provides services to help entrants for up to five years after arrival, and

targets those with low English proficiency. Two types of services are provided through

this program:

1. Generalist assistance services including further orientation to Australia,

development of life skills, referral to social services and assistance engaging

with mainstream Australia.

72

2. Specialist assistance services including support with immigration and housing

difficulties. In addition, ethno-specific funding is available for groups who wish

to build the capacity of newly arrived communities.

Torture and trauma counselling

The Forum of Australian Services for Survivors of Torture and Trauma (FASSTT) is

the umbrella agency for mental health services for humanitarian entrants and comprises

eight specialist rehabilitation services, representing one in each state and territory

(FASSTT, 2006). According to the FASSTT website, the services offered include

psychological assessments, individual psychotherapeutic interventions, group and

family therapy, youth activities, natural therapies and community development.

The local provider of torture and trauma counselling services in the study site (see

Section 7.2) was the New South Wales Service for the Treatment and Rehabilitation of

Torture and Trauma Survivors (STARTTS). In the years preceding and during data

collection, Karen refugees were routinely referred to STARTTS by their HSS case

managers as part of their needs assessment. Until 2011 DIAC funded torture and trauma

counselling services through the HSS program. However, at the time of writing the

funding and structure of this aspect of refugee service provision was under review

(DIAC, 2012).

4.5 The local scene

As we have seen, a wide range different service providers hold contracts with DIAC to

provide the HSS and other settlement services. Table 1 outlines the services provided in

Western Sydney at the time data collection began. Only those services are listed that

were used or mentioned by participants in this study.

73

Table 1: Local service providers relevant to this study

Provider HSS target area ResolveFM Accommodation Navitas Ltd (previously known as ACL) Adult Migrant English Program

Case management Auburn Diversity Services Incorporated Settlement Grants Program Liverpool Migrant Resource Centre Settlement Grants Program The Hills Holroyd Parramatta Migrant Resource Centre

Settlement Grants Program

New South Wales Service for the Treatment and Rehabilitation of Torture and Trauma Survivors

Trauma and torture counselling

74

Chapter 5

Methodology

5.1 Introduction

Empirical researchers are guided by paradigms or worldviews which shape how they

approach their field of study and illuminate their underlying beliefs regarding (a) the

nature of reality and all that exists within it (ontology), (b) the relationship between the

knower and what can be known (epistemology), and (c) how we gain knowledge of the

world (methodology) (Denzin & Lincoln, 2005). This chapter outlines the theoretical

paradigms and perspectives relevant to this research. The approach taken will be

justified for its ability to adequately address the research aims and questions discussed

in Chapter 1.

5.2 A critical realist approach

As a researcher I situate myself as most closely aligned with a critical realist paradigm

which spans the “either-or” divide (Danermark, Ekstrom, Jakobsen, & Karlsson, 2002,

p. 2; Oliver, 2011) that has traditionally split the natural and social sciences into

positivist and quantitative, and constructivist and qualitative approaches respectively

(Onwuegbuzie & Leech, 2005). The critical realist framework referred to here was

developed by British philosopher Roy Bhaskar (Bhaskar, 1978, 1986, 1989) and shares

theoretical orientations with pragmatism (Dewey, 1922, 1929, 1938; Dewey et al.,

1917; G. H. Mead, 1917). Subsequent developments and related perspectives include

the subtle realism approaches of Hammersley (1992) and Willig (1999) but it is

Bhaskar’s critical realism that has been most widely promoted and used by researchers

(Banfield, 2004).

Bhaskar first outlined the foundations for his critical realist framework in the book A

Realist Theory of Science (1978) and began his argument with answering the

ontological question of how reality is constructed (Bhaskar & Lawson, 1998). He

argued that the natural and social worlds are comprised of multiple levels of reality – an

empirical level (experienced events), an actual level (all events whether experienced or

not) and a causal level (consisting of the mechanisms by which events are generated)

(Bhaskar, 1978). All levels are recognised as real though not necessarily observable. For

75

example, in the social world, psychological mechanisms (e.g., self-control) and social

structures (e.g., racism), though not necessarily observable, may be mechanisms that

casually impact on peoples’ lives and are real in their consequences. Similarly, in the

natural world, the presence and activity of neurochemicals at nerve terminals in the

human body was real (and real in its consequences) before it was revealed in 1903

through scientific research (Danermark et al., 2002).

Having outlined his ontological stance, Bhaskar then turned to the epistemological

questions of how we can acquire knowledge about reality. He criticised the “epistemic

fallacy” (1978, p. 36) of scientific research for reducing ontological arguments to only

the empirical level of reality, consequently reducing “what is to what we can know

about it” (Danermark et al., 2002, p. 21). With regard to epistemology, critical realism

distinguishes between two dimensions or objects of science – intransitive objects,

objects of reality (mostly in the natural world) which exist independent of human

knowledge about them; and transitive objects, the knowledge and theories produced by

researchers. Although (unlike natural intransitive objects) social phenomena exist only

because of human activity, critical realists do not consider them to be “only in people’s

minds … social constructions are constructions of something” (Danermark et al., 2002,

p. 30). Bhaskar acknowledged differences between natural and social objects of

scientific investigations, but maintained that the principles concerning reasoning or

theorising about external phenomena remain the same.

Bhaskar argued that the transitive objects of science – the theories scientists produce

about intransitive objects – are (like all forms of knowledge) influenced by the cultural,

political and economic conditions of the time. By extension, he maintained that

“Established facts are social products” (Bhaskar, 1978, p. 196) and, like all social

products, they are fallible and changeable. The effect of culture on perception and

experience can be marked and Danermark and colleagues (2002) argue “it is often not a

question of different ways of seeing things, but that we see different things” (p. 29).

Theories also rely on “the linguistic world of meaning” (Danermark et al., 2002, p. 29)

as they are shared through the use of language. When theories are formed and expressed

they already represent one biased interpretation or conceptual framework because, most

commonly, they employ one language (typically, in Western academia, English).

Language, however, is partly social and cultural in nature and the viewpoints or

76

conceptual frameworks of members of one language group may be vastly different from

those of another (Willig, 1999).

Despite acknowledging the inherent constructivist nature, and fallibility, of human

knowledge, critical realism also rejects naïve relativism and opposes the premise that all

truths are equally valid. Bhaskar viewed knowledge of reality as imperfect as it is

influenced by human perception and meaning making. However, he also stressed the

importance of striving towards some degree of objectivity in order to produce the best

understanding possible (Houston, 2001). For critical realists “knowledge is always

fallible, yet all knowledge is not equally fallible” (Danermark et al., 2002, p. 15).

Indeed, Bhaskar (1986) argued that there can remain “rational grounds” (p. 72) for

preferring one explanation or description of events to another. Critical realism does not

dictate any set of research methods (Danermark et al., 2002) or system to assess the

plausibility of different accounts. Rather, researchers informed by this philosophy seek

to generate a deep understanding of the mechanisms interacting with their object or area

of study (Connelly, 2001). They may utilise both qualitative and quantitative

methodologies and frequently conduct work that surpasses traditional disciplinary

boundaries.

Finally, a “centerpiece” (Houston, 2001, p. 851) of Bhaskar’s work has been his

criticism of value-free approaches to inquiry and insistence that the purpose of his

philosophy is not only to illuminate the mechanisms of life but to expose and challenge

institutions and mechanisms leading to inequity and oppression. In his more recent

works, Bhaskar (1998) has repeated his objection to “intellectualism or theoreticism” (p.

410) and called for an enhanced relationship between research and practice. This

emphasis on the “practical relevance” (Danermark et al., 2002, p. 24; Willig, 1999) of

inquiry has made critical realism a particularly useful and suitable framework for

researchers in healthcare, social work and health promotion settings (Connelly, 2001;

Houston, 2001; Oliver, 2011; C. Taylor & White, 2001).

5.2.1 Explanation and justification of a qualitative realist approach

As a pragmatic ontological middle path, a critical realist paradigm is relevant to this

inquiry. Firstly, an aim of this study is to frame recommendations for improving

services. This aim represents a practical real-world focus best achieved through the

77

application of a realist-oriented approach. In this sense I hold a realist ontology – I am

interested in understanding and explaining Karen women’s wellbeing and distress. I

believe that there are some observable truths and real phenomena in nature. More

importantly, for me, research involving the explanation, prediction and control of

certain phenomena can result in facts or truths that are of benefit to humans. Lives can

be improved and people can be helped. I want my findings to reveal probable truths that

are relevant to helping these women.

I also share social constructivists’ viewpoints and believe that all knowledge is partial

and socially constructed, and that a certain degree of relativism is thus unavoidable.

Human beings cannot be devoid of the contexts that surround them and these contexts

are not universal – rather they include social, cultural, political and gendered factors that

are in states of flux throughout history. I believe that humans perceive, construct and

interpret reality in patterns that are shaped by these contexts. I agree with the point that

“What is defined or perceived by people as real is real in its consequences” (Thomas &

Thomas, 1928, p. 572). Humans may believe in phenomena that are not physically real

and their belief in those phenomena may significantly shape their experience and

behaviour – in short, they are real to them. In this study I aimed to understand the

contexts, experiences and perspectives of the research participants. I view myself as an

active participant in the research process from the initial inception of the research

questions and aims through to data collection and analysis. However, I reject naive

relativism because, although knowledge of reality is always imperfect, I believe we can

strive to attain some truth in our research findings. Within our imperfect knowledge of

human life, especially the human psyche, I believe that there are important contributions

to be made by representing truths and having something to say about them.

Lastly, critical realism is germane to my research, not only because it provides a

balanced approach between realist and relativist perspectives, but also because the

moral and emancipatory aspects of the perspective are aligned with the motivations

underpinning my inquiry. I reject the purely objectivist epistemological posture

concerning value-free, objective inquiry.

78

5.3 Theoretical perspectives

In addition to their overarching philosophical paradigms, researchers are also situated

amongst a number of other theoretical perspectives and methodological traditions, many

of which are derived from different disciplines such as biology, sociology, psychology

and anthropology. Further traditions within these disciplines include feminist,

postcolonial, ethnographic and neo-Marxism perspectives to name a few (Carter,

Ritchie, & Sainsbury, 2009; Ritchie, 2001). Each approach includes particular methods

(or tools) for research, though these often overlap across perspectives. Researchers

choose amongst the variety of these perspectives by assessing which best addresses

their research question. Morse and Richards (2002) emphasise that no one particular

approach is perfect for a given research purpose and that researchers must choose which

is “most appropriate” (p. 6). Likewise, Patton (2002) strongly emphasises pragmatism

as the foundation of research, rather than sworn allegiance to particular theoretical

traditions. Denzin and Lincoln (2005) explain that researchers frequently use “more

than one interpretive practice in any study” (p. 4) as a means to better understand the

subject of inquiry in different ways. In this section the theoretical frameworks and

methods chosen as most relevant to this research will be briefly outlined and justified.

To begin with, from amongst the broad research traditions, qualitative research

methodologies are the most germane to this thesis as the primary axiological goal of the

study is one of understanding. Qualitative approaches are inductive and emergent

(Carter et al., 2009) and “seek to discover understanding or to achieve explanation from

the data instead of from (or in addition to) prior knowledge or theory” (Morse &

Richards, 2002, p. 2). The relevance of a qualitative approach to this study is indicated

by the several key characteristics of the study. Firstly, the study is exploratory due to the

absence of any previous studies exploring the suffering and distress of Karen refugee

women. Secondly, there are no existing valid or reliable measures for assessing the

psychosocial experiences of Karen refugee women. Qualitative methods are

recommended for contexts where existing measures are inadequate or non-existent

(Bolton & Tang, 2004; Creswell, 1998). Thirdly, this thesis seeks to explore cross-

cultural experiences and perceptions of emotional wellbeing and distress. The research

aims are concerned with developing an emic (insider’s) understanding which, as Pope

and Mays (1995) argue, is best achieved through qualitative research methods. Lastly,

qualitative methods aimed at documenting the truth of people’s experiences may

79

become a source of empowerment by affirming their reality, raising group

consciousness and facilitating dissemination of their needs and concerns to a wider

audience (Banyard & Miller, 1998). These are important considerations for research

with disadvantaged populations such as refugee women.

5.3.1 Ethnography

The relevance of an ethnographic framework is suggested by the research aims and

questions, which are oriented towards understanding Karen women’s emic perspectives.

Ethnography is a theoretical perspective and set of methods that originated in the field

of cultural anthropology and focuses upon the identification and “thick” (Spradley,

1979, p. 3) or detailed description of patterns and themes within a cultural group from

the emic (insider) perspective. An ethnographic approach also situates this study within

contemporary approaches to refugee mental health research. According to Gozdziak

(2004), “ethnography is the real bridge to multicultural mental health practice with

refugees” (p. 207) and, as discussed in Chapter 2, researchers in this field have

increasingly used ethnographic methods to understand issues relevant to refugee

communities.

Participant observation has been described as the hallmark of ethnography and it is

through this process ethnographers come to immerse themselves in the culture of study

as fully as possible in order to gain a deep understanding of their participants’ lives,

meanings, actions, perspectives, behaviour and environment in context. Alongside

participant observation, ethnographers produce detailed field notes recording what they

learn and experience during their research. Field notes are typically written (or

recorded) as soon as possible during or following encounters and contain not only

observations and records of events but also the ethnographer’s reflexive account of their

experience. In addition to participant observation, interview methods feature strongly in

ethnographic research. Interviews are conducted with both “ordinary” community

members as well as key informants who provide a deeper level of access, insight and

interpretation of meaning to the ethnographer (Liamputtong, 2009).

Historically, most ethnographers immersed themselves in their culture of study for

extended periods of time, learning the local language and living within the local

community. Examples of this approach include the works of anthropologists

80

Malinowski (1922) and M. Mead (1928). However, as Denzin and Lincoln (2005)

argue, “It is no longer possible to take for granted what anyone means by ethnography”

(p. xvi). Modern ethnographers have questioned the notion of immersion, and

contemporary ethnographic approaches have evolved to address the time-limited nature

of some research contexts (Bolton & Tang, 2004; Handwerker, 2001). Moreover, cross-

cultural researchers are now “Often … linguistically and culturally distant from their

research participants” (Liamputtong, 2010, p. 135).

Traditionalist ethnographers may be dismayed at some contemporary ethnographic

works and the rationale behind them, which sometimes fail to make any distinction

between general qualitative methods and ethnographic approaches and also fail to locate

the perspective within a scholarly anthropological setting. For example, contemporary

researchers have described their research as encompassing “ethnographic (or

qualitative) methods in that the interviewers rely on open-ended questions; avoid using

leading questions, comments or reactions; use non-leading probes; and record responses

verbatim” (Bolton & Tang, 2004, p. 98). Many ethnographers would argue that these

interview techniques are neither limited to an ethnographic approach nor encompass

ethnographic inquiry.

The present study represents a contemporary approach to ethnography that draws from

and builds upon recent ethnographies with populations affected by political violence

(Hubbard & Pearson, 2004; D. Pedersen, Kienzler, & Gamarra, 2010; Zarowsky, 2004).

I planned for a level of participant observation immersion adequate to the research aims

and questions but noted from the outset that the usefulness of this method was limited

by my inability to fluently speak all participants’ mother languages. Consequently, I

planned to draw heavily on interview methods in order to access richer and deeper

information.

Finally, my approach to ethnographic research is informed by a critical realist

perspective. Purely relativist ethnographers refrain from “taking a stand of any kind in

relation to groups of individuals” (Griffin & Bengry-Howell, 2008, p. 21). However,

according to Manicas (1998), the critical realist ethnography “needs to do more than

give a description of the social world as seen by its members … it needs to also ask

whether members have an adequate understanding of their world, and if not, explain

why not” (p. 315). Moreover, as I situate my inquiry also within a feminist perspective I

81

view my research as, in part, committed to giving voice to women’s experiences and

taking a stand on issues related to their wellbeing and distress.

5.3.2 Feminist perspective

A feminist perspective is an ideological orientation indicated by several key

characteristics of the study. Firstly, in researching women’s perspectives and needs, this

study “presumes the importance of gender in human relationships and societal

processes” (Patton, 2002, p. 129). I was guided by a desire for gender equity and the

position that mental health support services for women should be built on and from

women’s experiences and needs. Several authors have pointed to the androcentric and

ethnocentric aspects of the psychological and psychiatric disciplines (Fine & Gordon,

1992; G. L. Fox & McBride Murry, 2000; Mirza, 1998; Scheper-Hughes, 1983) and

these issues have also plagued refugee mental health research. As we saw in the

literature review, the field of refugee mental health has been dominated by White

biomedically-oriented researchers whose agendas have shaped how refugees have been

represented (Warren, 2000) while indigenous knowledge and the experiences and

knowledge of refugee women was seldom represented.

Feminism is a diverse field which has been defined as both a political movement and a

theoretical orientation (Sparks & Park, 2000). At its core it is about human rights for

women and it is current wherever women’s issues are relevant. Like Reinharz (1992) I

embrace the notion that feminism is a perspective, not a research method, and the

feminist researcher may utilise a multiplicity of theoretical perspectives (including

ethnography) and research methods across multiple disciplines (Gergen, 2008).

G. L. Fox and McBride Murry (2000) contend that feminist research tends to be

collaborative, more reflective, change-oriented and questioning of existing disciplinary

paradigms. Whereas for Gergen (2008) feminist research “is designed to seek social

justice, to enhance women’s voice and influence in society, and to explore alternative

ways of understanding the world” (p. 280). These are all important aspects within the

aims of this research. This inquiry is located within Reinharz’s (1992) description of

feminist ethnography, which she argues seeks:

82

(1) to document the lives and activities of women, (2) to understand the

experience of women from their own point of view, and (3) to conceptualize

women’s behavior as an expression of social contexts. (p. 51)

As a cross-cultural feminist ethnography, both postmodern (Olesen, 2005) and

postcolonial (Pilcher & Whelehan, 2004) feminist perspectives are also relevant to this

inquiry, which is an encounter between myself – a White, middle class researcher and

psychologist from New Zealand – and Karen refugee women from Burma. A feminist

questioning of the universal sisterhood is relevant to this research and has guided my

approach to inquiry. Feminist anthropologists “originally assumed the existence of a

universal sisterhood of womankind” (Scheper-Hughes, 1983, p. 112); however, these

assumptions were soon challenged by differences in class, culture and ethnicity

(Hammarstrom, 1999). In stark contrast to the ideal of the universal sisterhood, Meara

and Day (2000) argued that the power difference between White women and women of

Colour had grown wider as White women have gained more parity with White men. I

set out expecting that “a bond of sisterhood must be earned” (Reinharz, 1992, p. 65) and

used reflexivity to explore my own cultural perspectives (Harding, 1991; Scheper-

Hughes, 1983). Moreover, I made no assumption that by virtue of my gender as a

woman I would (a) be guaranteed access to Karen women’s lives and perspectives, or

(b) necessarily understand their lived experience. My approach to inquiry was also not

limited to observing and interviewing women. Many feminist ethnographers solely

research women but some also observe and interview both sexes (Reinharz, 1992).

Postcolonial feminists have also questioned the application of Western feminist models

with women from other cultures, and pushed for consideration of different forms of

feminism based on culture and context (Hartsock, 1983; Olesen, 2005). Reinharz (1992)

outlines the following assumptions guiding postcolonial feminist cross-cultural

research:

(1) the importance of cultural specificity, (2) the necessity of intensive study, (3)

the possibility of commonalities among women of different cultures, and (4) the

need for a critical evaluation of study materials. (p. 111)

Each of Reinharz’s (1992) guiding principles is relevant to this research project, firstly

because a commitment to recognising cultural specificity within mental health

83

underpins the aims of the study. Secondly, as an ethnographic inquiry, this study is

oriented towards intensive study. Thirdly, the research design leaves open the

possibility of both disparate and common voices concerning distress and suffering

amongst women from different cultures. Lastly, a critical evaluation of available study

materials prompted the need for qualitative research methods to be utilised. This

approach invited Karen women to give voice to their experiences. I sought to avoid

imposing predetermined frameworks or hypotheses for conceptualising Karen women’s

realities and especially avoided assessing their wellbeing and distress according to the

imposed constructions of Western psychiatric diagnoses. Yet, at the same time I have

aimed to provide a “mirror” or point of comparison between the different cultural and

perhaps gender perspectives that might arise.

Nevertheless, an awareness of postcolonial feminist issues does not negate the fact that

cross-cultural research involves the translation of discourses from other cultures into

knowledge forms recognisable within a Western framework (Edwards & Ribbens,

1998). The White feminist cross-cultural researcher does not easily escape the influence

of ethnocentric and colonial frameworks. Indeed, it has been argued that “relationships

between First World feminist researchers and Third World subjects will inevitably be

exploitative, whatever the good intentions of the researcher” (Edwards & Ribbens,

1998, p. 3) and despite the use of reflexive research practices. There have been

criticisms of researchers for not studying women from other cultures and for

undertaking such research as outsiders. Reinharz (1992) has commented:

Western feminists are criticized for not studying “Third World” women (i.e.,

ethnocentrism) and criticized for doing so as outsiders to those cultures (i.e.,

colonialism). Feminists studying women in cultures other than their own are

criticized if they accept that culture’s way of subordinating women (i.e.,

misogyny) and are criticized if they repudiate the culture’s subordination of

women (i.e., ethnocentrism). (p. 122)

According to Olesen (2005), we “can never resolve all ethical dilemmas that arise” (p.

255) but a consideration of the feminist perspective encourages considerable reflexivity

and allows for the many issues of power and privilege in research to be considered and

actively tackled.

84

5.3.3 Grounded theory

Finally, this inquiry is influenced by the tradition of grounded theory. From the outset it

must be made clear that this inquiry is not a grounded theory study per se. Rather,

aspects of the grounded theory approach are relevant to this inquiry because of their

ability to “add rigor to ethnographic research by building systematic checks into both

data collection and analysis” (Charmaz, 2006, p. 23). Grounded theory provides a set of

methods of managing and analysing qualitative data (Glaser, 1978; Glaser & Strauss,

1967; Strauss & Corbin, 1990, 1998). Most significantly, for the present inquiry, it

offers a collection of tools that encourage understanding and reasoning beyond existing

subjectivities to ground analysts’ findings in the real world (Strauss & Corbin, 1998).

As I aimed to understand the emic perspective of research participants, such tools were

necessary to somewhat manage my existing notions and subjectivities, and allow me to

compare, contrast and challenge my conceptualisations and interpretations of the data.

Grounded theory is a useful framework for qualitative researchers who are not

committed to purely constructivist or objectivist paradigms (Charmaz & Henwood,

2008) and consequently “highly compatible” (Oliver, 2011, p. 371) with a critical realist

philosophical framework. Moreover, it has long been considered well-matched to

ethnography (Spradley, 1979) and is now frequently used “in combination with other

approaches … as part of a flexible toolkit of methods” (Charmaz & Henwood, 2008, p.

254). In conjunction with the success and popularity of grounded theory there has been

a generic overuse of the label (Carter et al., 2009) as well as misunderstandings

(Suddaby, 2006) and incorrect application of its methods (Glaser, 1969). Perhaps

because grounded theory methods share many similarities with a range of other

qualitative inquiry approaches, the phrase is sometimes mistakenly used

interchangeably to refer to inductive, qualitative approaches (Carter et al., 2009).

Confusion and criticism arose from this scenario (Liamputtong, 2009). Moreover, the

divergent approaches (known as the Glaserian and Straussian versions) taken by the

founders of grounded theory have further fuelled confusion regarding conceptual

underpinnings and methods. The current inquiry borrows several basic research

processes from grounded theory including memo-writing, coding, categorising and

theoretical sampling to assist with the management and interpretation of the data. These

methods are described in detail in the following chapter.

85

5.4 Summary

In this chapter I have outlined the theoretical paradigms and perspectives relevant to this

inquiry, and evaluated and justified the relevance of the approaches taken. I have chosen

a combination of ethnography, feminist and grounded theory perspectives and methods.

These frameworks encapsulate a critical approach to understanding the psychosocial

world of Karen refugee women and provide suitable tools to meet the research aims.

They also situate this inquiry firmly within the field of contemporary refugee mental

health research and allow this study to contribute to current research and academic

debate across the various disciplines that the inquiry transcends. The perspectives

guiding this study share a questioning of reliance on prior and dominant theory and

frameworks. In particular, ethnographic inquiry and grounded theory are useful methods

for mental health practitioners who find themselves “too constrained by psychology’s

traditional experimental and psychometric outlook” (Charmaz & Henwood, 2008, p.

249). A feminist perspective is current wherever women’s issues are relevant – a point

too long overlooked in the field of refugee mental health where a trauma discourse has

largely disregarded gender and context.

86

Chapter 6

Method

6.1 Introduction

This chapter discusses the methods or tools used to address the research aims and

questions. As a qualitative inquiry, the approach to data collection was inductive and

emergent. Consequently, as the research evolved the focus and methods used were also

adapted. This chapter addresses the initial research and early processes. It is

complemented by a description of, and reflection on, the unfolding research journey in

Chapter 7.

6.2 Selecting participants

This study used several purposive sampling strategies commonly used to guide

emergent research (Blignault & Ritchie, 2009; Carter et al., 2009; Liamputtong, 2009;

Ritchie, 2001). Convenience sampling was used to launch the inquiry. This method

involves the use of participants who are easily accessible, available and often already

known to the researcher, and is a helpful means of “getting in” to hard-to-reach

communities (Liamputtong, 2009). Snowball sampling is another strategy often used in

studies of refugee and other “hidden” populations (Liamputtong, 2008; Spring et al.,

2003; Sulaiman-Hill & Thompson, 2011) and was used throughout the study. This

method requires participants to propose other potential participants who are then

contacted and invited to participate. As an ethnography, this research also involved the

use of opportunistic sampling methods in which data was collected in the form of

observations or impromptu interviews (Ritchie, 2001).

As the study progressed, additional purposive and theoretical sampling strategies were

used to follow-up hunches, ideas and emergent themes. Intensity sampling

(Liamputtong, 2009) helped to identify people with detailed or expert knowledge by

asking participants to list the most knowledgeable or respected individuals in relation to

specific topics. Revelatory case sampling (Liamputtong, 2009) enabled access to single

case participants who could represent previously undocumented issues (e.g., women

who suffered particular Karen idioms of distress). In addition, negative, extreme and

deviant case sampling as well as confirming and disconfirming cases (Charmaz, 2006;

87

Liamputtong, 2009) were used to make comparisons of issues across participants and

add depth to emerging themes.

6.2.1 Karen women

Operationally, for this study, Karen women participants are defined as “ordinary”

community members (as distinct from others who held positions of significant

authority, trust or respect within the community). In the ethnographic sense, these

women were also key informants in that they may have been able to provide unique,

rich insight on a topic. For example, a woman suffering from a Karen idiom of distress

may be in a unique position to provide an insider view that others are unlikely to share.

However, for the purpose of clarity I have utilised a system of referring to these women

as “Karen women”, distinct from key informants (described below). As noted, Karen

women who participated in interviews were initially selected through convenience and

snowball sampling strategies on the basis of their availability and willingness. Later

women were purposively targeted based on their knowledge or experience of specific

topics of interest.

6.2.2 Key informants

Lincoln and Guba (1985) describe an informant as a committed and accepted member in

the local context who is able to provide inside views of norms, attitudes, constructions,

processes and culture. Operationally, for this study, key informants are defined as (a)

Karen community members who occupied positions of trust and respect, or (b)

individuals whose work (e.g., service providers) or other duties brought them into

regular contact with Karen women. Inevitably, some Karen women were also key

informants (e.g., Karen women employed as bicultural workers). For the purpose of this

study, these women are referred to herein as key informants to distinguish them from

“ordinary” women in the community.

6.2.3 Saturation

Redundancy or saturation is regarded as the most suitable criterion for appropriate

sample size in qualitative research. It is described as the point at which “a thorough

understanding of the phenomenon under study has been reached” (Kuper, Lingard, &

Levinson, 2008, p. 687), and when “no new information is forthcoming from new

88

sampled units” (Lincoln & Guba, 1985, p. 202). Grounded theorists refer to saturation

as the point at which no new information regarding categories or the relationships

between categories emerges (Charmaz, 2006). However, saturation is a thorny concept

for ethnographic researchers who do not seek an endpoint but rather aim for continued

immersion into deeper and richer findings, learning more as they observe more. Having

said that, the pragmatics of modern research (and doctoral studies) require data

collection to be contained in some form. I employed saturation as a feature of analysis

and essentially continued collecting information until the data was rich enough to

support the desired analyses of the dimension(s) of interest. I continued to engage in

theoretical sampling of participants for interviews for a period of nineteen months

(November 2009 to June 2011) at which point I considered theoretical saturation of

categories to have been reached.

6.3 Participant observation

Participant observation is the primary method of data collection for ethnographers and

involves “systematically watching and recording people’s behaviours, clothing,

expressions, and interaction in a particular location or several settings” (Liamputtong &

Ezzy, 2005, p. 110). The participant observation approach used was informed by

Spradley (1980) and Strauss, Schatzmann, Bucher, Ehrlich and Sabshin (1964). Field

notes were written on the same day as observations or interviews occurred and noted

observations concerning the physical environment, social interactions (verbal and non-

verbal), activity, persons and emotional tone of interactions as well personal reflections

about the observed interaction and my place and performance within it. If I was too

busy or tired to write notes (for example, after a day of multiple interviews followed by

a community soiree in the evening) I simply dictated my notes in the car whilst driving

home and typed the saved recording at a later date.

6.4 Interviews

6.4.1 In-depth interviews

In-depth interviews were approached from an ethnographic and feminist perspective

and were oriented to fit with Spradley’s (1980) description of the ethnographic

interview as “a series of friendly conversations” (p. 58). Where possible I planned to

89

meet with interview participants on more than one occasion (Rosenwald & Wiersma,

1983; Schweitzer & Steel, 2008) but I anticipated that time limitations on service

providers, particularly healthcare practitioners, might restrict their availability to a

single interview.

Interview guides

The semi-structured interview guide for service providers and key informant interviews

(see Appendices B & C) was loosely based on those recommended by Larson, van

Kooten-Prasad, Manderson and Frkovic (1998). The guide for Karen women (see

Appendix D) included extensive lists of themes, which were taken or adapted from

previous psychosocial research with refugee communities (Bolton & Tang, 2004;

Bolton et al., 2004; Miller, Omidian, et al., 2006). This guide was translated and back-

translated to ensure that key terms were accurate. The interview guide also prompted

my observations, reflections and notes regarding the interview context and participant

behaviour such as body language and tone. The bilingual research assistant’s

observations regarding cultural nuances were also recorded.

6.4.2 Group interviews

Group interviews are seen as particularly useful in exploratory research in different

social and cultural contexts, and in addressing sensitive research topics (Flick, 2002;

Stewart & Shamdasani, 1990). This method of data collection has also been particularly

embraced by feminist researchers as groups can represent a more natural part of

participants’ social life (Bryman, 2008), as well as a more collaborative and less power-

structured approach to inquiry (Kamberelis & Dimitriadis, 2005; Patton, 2002). The

approach taken was guided by Willgerodt (2003) and Kitzinger (1994) and aimed for

pre-existing and homogeneous groups of Karen women (i.e., subgroups who already

knew each other and shared characteristics such as membership of an organisation or

attendance at a regular support group) for the following reasons:

1. Previous research indicates that a shared socioeconomic characteristic amongst

group members is conducive to facilitating better communication and interaction

(Kitzinger, 1994; Stewart & Shamdasani, 1990).

2. Pre-existing social networks are likely to represent natural environments in

which one might discuss psychosocial issues (Kitzinger, 1994).

90

3. The community of Karen refugee women in Sydney was so small that most

people knew of each other and consequently it would not be possible to conduct

a group with people unfamiliar to one another.

4. Mistrust is inherent in communities subjected to persecution. In such a context it

would not be permissible to introduce strangers into a group situation and expect

the group members to openly discuss their experiences and perceptions.

An intermediary guided group participant selection. She identified between six and

eight participants for each group, provided them with information about the study (see

Appendix E) and confirmed their willingness and availability to attend the interview at

the arranged time, date and location. Transport requirements were discussed via the

intermediary and, when necessary, I personally transported women who could not

otherwise attend the meeting. As the group members formed part of a pre-existing

social network, their regular childcare arrangements were discussed with the

intermediary. A disadvantage of the use of intermediaries is the possibility that “the

intermediary may act as an unwanted ‘screening device’” (Bloor, Frankland, Thomas, &

Robson, 2001, p. 32). To mitigate this potential problem, additional snowball sampling

strategies were also used.

Group interview guide

The purpose of the group interviews was to explore participants’ EMs of mental illness

and was guided by Kleinman’s (1980) EM framework (see Section 2.4.4). Vignettes

meeting DSM-IV diagnostic criteria were created to explore women’s EMs about

Western psychiatric constructs commonly reported in epidemiological studies of

refugee mental health: PTSD, depression and panic disorder. The vignettes depicted

aspects and features relevant to the lives of Karen refugee women so that they might

find the cases meaningful and easy to relate to. The characters were named “Aunty” as

it was common practice in Burma (Steinberg, 2010) and amongst the Karen to use the

positioning word for aunt to respectfully denote adult women in the community. The

diagnostic accuracy of each vignette was independently checked by a clinical

psychologist and a Burmese psychiatrist. The full interview guide, including vignettes,

is provided in Appendix F.

91

A template was created to assist with identifying speakers during group interviews. The

two hour interview period was divided into five minute intervals in the left margin and

when participants spoke I noted their name in the corresponding time block along with

the first few words of the bilingual research assistant’s English interpretation of their

comments. I also adopted Stevens’s (1996) approach to guide my reporting of group

interaction, spending time alone or with the interpreter (when her time availability

permitted it), noting observations about the group process and participants’ interactions.

6.4.3 Recording, interpreting and transcribing interviews

Where participants consented, and environmental circumstances allowed, I planned to

digitally record interviews with a dictaphone.

A bilingual research assistant was employed (see Section 7.3) to interpret all group

interviews and in-depth interviews with participants who did not speak fluent English or

who wished to converse in their first language.

The approach taken to interpretation was guided by Hale (2010) who encourages the use

of consecutive (as opposed to simultaneous) interpreting in which each person has a

separate turn at speaking then pauses while the bilingual research assistant interprets.

Languages cannot be translated verbatim and complex and subtle differences such as

non-equivalence in words, concepts and assumptions must be navigated. The interpreter

was encouraged to prioritise conveying the meaning of speech in context rather than just

the literal meaning of words. I also planned to meet with the bilingual research assistant

outside of interviews to reflect on the process and trouble-shoot problems. Moreover, I

anticipated that interpretation dilemmas would arise and planned to resolve them with

the assistance of community leaders.

As discussed elsewhere (see Section 3.3.1), according to Sino-Tibetan language

researchers at the University of California Department of Linguistics (D. Solnit,

personal communication, 1 February, 2011) and other Karennic language experts (K.

Manson, personal communication, 1 February, 2011) there is no standardised Roman

script for transliteration of Sgaw or Pwo Karen languages. The approach taken towards

Romanisation of Karen terms in this thesis was developed in collaboration with the

bilingual research assistant; and with input from other Karen interpreters, translators

and knowledgeable key informants. It is not the aim of this inquiry to create a

92

comprehensive typology of Karen idioms of distress but rather to explore Karen

women’s experiences and perceptions of emotional wellbeing and suffering. The

transliteration process of idioms of distress for this inquiry adequately meets the aims of

this research.

The conventions of Bloor et al. (2001) guided transcription of interviews. Emphasis was

given to content and overarching meaning, a level of analysis appropriate to the aims of

the study. Only spoken English was transcribed because I did not have the necessary

Karen linguistic skills to transcribe Karen dialects.

6.5 Ethics

Ethical approval for this project was sought and obtained from the Human Research

Ethics Committee of the University of New South Wales. Additional approval and

assent was also obtained from Karen community leaders and elders (Liamputtong,

2008).

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained according to the requirements of the Human Research

Ethics Committee. Participants were given an information and consent form in English,

Sgaw Karen or Burmese (see Appendix E). All Karen refugee women participants also

had the information and consent form read aloud to them by the bilingual research

assistant. Key informants and service providers who wished to read the form without

assistance were able to do so. Participants were provided with an opportunity to discuss

any questions about the study and consent process before being asked to provide verbal

(recorded) or written (signed) consent. Participants were also asked if they were

comfortable with a dictaphone being used to record the interview.

Maintaining confidentiality and anonymity

Participant confidentiality and anonymity was ensured by (a) concealing the identity of

refugee women participants, key informants and organisations by using pseudonyms in

all written documents (field notes and interview transcripts), (b) ensuring the bilingual

research assistant understood and agreed to strict conditions of confidentiality, (c)

storing all audio-recordings, consent forms and transcripts in a confidential and secure

format and location, and (d) ensuring all publicly available information (such as

93

publications and this thesis) was free from information that could potentially lead to the

identification of participants.

Protecting participants from risk and harm

A reasonable foreseeable risk of this study was psychological distress amongst

participants and strategies were planned to minimise the risk of adverse outcomes.

Firstly, all interviews would be carried out by myself (a trained clinical psychologist

with experience interviewing refugees). Secondly, participants who discussed

potentially distressing information would be followed up two weeks after their

interview to ensure they were not experiencing ongoing distress as a result of the

material discussed and, if necessary, referred to an appropriate service provider.

Protecting myself and the bilingual research assistant from risk and harm

Several risk-minimisation strategies were used to protect myself and the bilingual

research assistant from risk and harm. With regard to physical safety, the ethics

committee required that I carry a mobile phone at all times and provide contact details

to another person should a problem arise.

Additional strategies were used to mitigate and manage the potential stress associated

with researching marginalised and vulnerable groups (Dickson-Swift, James, &

Liamputtong, 2008). For myself these included: (a) reflection through the use of field

notes, audit trail and personal writings including poetry, (b) use of doctoral supervision

sessions to monitor my emotional responses, and (c) use of independent and

confidential support services should I feel overwhelmed and unable to manage the

emotional stress associated with the research. For the bilingual research assistant I

planned to (a) give great care to remain open, empathic, sensitive and patient to her

emotional journey, (b) engage in debriefing following each interview, (c) make myself

personally available should she need to call me for support outside of work hours, and

(d) encourage her to seek independent professional support if she experienced difficulty.

6.6 Data analysis

As noted (see Section 5.3.3), analytic methods borrowed from grounded theory guided

data analysis throughout this inquiry. NVivo 8 software was used to assist with the

management of data.

94

In the grounded theory method, data analysis begins as soon as data collection

commences. Initial basic open coding or line-by-line analysis aims to identify recurrent

patterns present in the data. The analytic process then proceeds through stages in which

data is compared and categorised to explore higher-level relationships and theory. There

are a host of strategies and approaches to guide grounded theorists though they are not

prescriptive (Strauss & Corbin, 1998); this study was most significantly guided by

Charmaz’s (2006) constructivist approach.

Data analysis began during the earliest interactions in the field and whilst transcribing

the interviews. Initial coding (also known as open coding) consisted of line-by-line

analysis of notes and transcripts. This was done by hand and impressions and categories

were noted in a second column. This process resulted in rudimentary descriptive codes

to describe chunks of data (e.g., “transport problems” and “difficulty with children’s

homework”).

Additional coding processes shift analysis to a more conceptual level where properties,

conditions and relationships within and between such conceptual categories are

explored. At this stage, categories are developed into subcategories and their

corresponding dimensions until no new categories (or subcategories) are identified from

the data. As coding progressed I examined the codes, developed categories and concepts

and explored the properties of these concepts. I looked at codes I had created that could

be loosely grouped together and then I asked myself “Is this a type of [another code]?”

For example, I looked at the code “Prayer meetings” and asked myself “Is this a type of

coping strategy?”. I had coded other references to “coping” but began to note the

different ways in which women coped. Sometimes women shared their distress with

others and sometimes they did not. Moreover, to whom, where, when and what they

shared also varied. I developed subcategories for coping which included not only

approaches to coping (e.g., praying, seeking advice) but also coping contexts to explore

how women’s circumstances influenced their perceptions and experiences of coping.

Constant comparison – an analytic process in which comparisons are made between

“data with data, category with category, and concept with concept” (Charmaz, 1990, p.

1168) in order to test emerging themes and theory – was used to look at patterns, refine

and extend categories into subcategories. For example, I compared different

participants’ discussions of “freedom” and also compared the various categories that I

95

had created concerning “freedom”. I realised that some notions of freedom were related

to parenting concerns and parent-child disagreements regarding appropriate levels of

children’s “freedom” in Australia. But there was also a lot more going on there. As I

compared categories I realised that, within “freedom” categories, a theme of “fear of

freedom” represented one aspect of a societal consequence of persecution. In addition to

these comparisons, I also compared different people’s situations and accounts.

The method of theoretical sampling is central to grounded theory. In this process,

analysis and data collection (including participant selection) occur simultaneously and

participants are selected to help advance the developing conceptual analysis. Theoretical

sampling of participants is continued until theoretical saturation is reached. This is

defined as the point whereby no new information regarding categories or the

relationships between categories emerges. As the study progressed and I constructed

concepts and themes from the data, I began to utilise extreme and deviant case sampling

to seek out participants who represented information-rich cases of the topics of interest.

The entire analytical process of grounded theory is supported by the writing of memos

which are essentially analytical field notes containing the researchers’ thinking process

throughout the study. I used memo-writing throughout the study, noting my

impressions, ideas, notes and drawing diagrams. I also used memo-writing to

reflexively explore my experiences, feelings and notions throughout the research

journey (Charmaz, 2006; Strauss & Corbin, 1998) (see Section 7.9.1).

6.7 Rigour

Qualitative researchers use a variety of methods to enhance the rigour or trustworthiness

of their research. However, there remains no universally agreed upon approach to

addressing rigour (Carter et al., 2009; Ritchie, 2001). Like all elements of research

design, the approach taken must be viewed against the backdrop of the theoretical

perspective(s) adopted (Barbour, 2001; Patton, 2002). As we have seen, critical realists

and qualitative researchers enjoy an extensive range of theoretical viewpoints. They

also hold multiple perspectives concerning rigour. Notable differences are observed

between qualitative researchers more informed by a constructivist philosophy, and those

whose viewpoint is located towards a realist philosophy (Cohen & Crabtree, 2008).

96

An aim of my inquiry was to frame recommendations for improving psychosocial

support services for Karen refugee women, a real-world focus best achieved through the

application of “realist” approaches to rigour. As a result I adopted Kitto, Chesters, and

Grbich’s (2008), six-point realist-oriented criteria for rigour. This section describes their

approach including how each criterion was met in this study.

6.7.1 Criterion 1: Clarification, justification or theoretical rigour

This criterion requires demonstration and justification of the theoretical approach

guiding research; in this thesis this is addressed in Chapter 5.

6.7.2 Criterion 2: Procedural and methodological rigour

This criterion is met through explanation of procedure and methods including rapport

building; participant selection; and data collection, storage, coding and analysis (Kitto et

al., 2008). In this thesis, the present chapter explains the methods of participant

selection (Section 6.2), data collection (Section 6.3) and data analysis (Section 6.6)

employed. Further descriptions of the unfolding research process in relation to rapport,

ongoing participant selection and data analysis are described in Chapter 7. Together

these chapters provide a thorough audit trail (Flick, 2002); these are augmented by

additional documents and recordings including:

raw data (audio-recordings, participant consent forms, completed interview

forms, transcriptions, emails and letters);

participant observation field notes;

interview summaries and field notes;

research diary and memos (including decision-making steps during research

planning; development of methods; data collection, analyses and interpretation;

and mistakes, surprises and challenges);

minutes from supervision and decision-making meetings.

6.7.3 Criterion 3: Representativeness

Representativeness requires the justification of the participant sampling techniques used

and is addressed in Section 6.2 of the present chapter as well as in Chapter 7 (see

Sections 7.2, 7.4 and 7.5).

97

6.7.4 Criterion 4: Interpretative rigour

Interpretive rigour was addressed through triangulation, respondent validation and

attention to negative cases (Kitto et al., 2008). Triangulation involves the utilisation of

multiple research methods or theories to offset the limits and reduce the biases that

come with the use of only one research method or theory (Blignault & Ritchie, 2009;

Guba & Lincoln, 2005; Ritchie, 2001). Ethnographic, grounded theory and feminist

methodologies provided a means of methodological triangulation and multiple data

collection methods (e.g., different types of purposeful sampling, participant observation,

in-depth interviews and group interviews) which further enhanced the rigour of the

inquiry. I did not have co-researchers to allow for intensive investigator triangulation.

However, my doctoral supervisors, colleagues and the bilingual research assistant did

provide some degree of investigator triangulation through their feedback on my research

and analysis.

In addition to triangulation, the trustworthiness of research may also be enhanced

through prolonged engagement and observation (Lincoln & Guba, 1985) – procedures

which are also central to ethnography. By contemporary ethnographic standards I

conducted prolonged fieldwork in the community by engaging in fourteen months of

rapport building and orientation to the community before the research formally began.

Once ethical approval was obtained I also engaged in over 200 hours of ethnographic

fieldwork for a period of twenty-four months (September 2009 – September 2011).

Member checking, respondent validation and inter-rater coding are further approaches

to establishing the rigour of qualitative research though these techniques are variably

defined and their use is widely debated (Morse, 1997; Ritchie, 2001). Guided by a

critical realist paradigm, I reject the positivist notion of objectivity and did not

undertake forms of member checking or inter-rater coding in an attempt to achieve

complete objectivity (Glaser, 1969). However, I did include these strategies to help

minimise the “intrusion of bias” (Strauss & Corbin, 1998, p. 43) in data analysis.

Ritchie (2001) suggests a practical approach to member checking which may occur at

any stage of the research and which may also be a formal or informal encounter. In the

process of interviews I frequently used summaries and reflections to check my

understanding and seek feedback from participants. In this manner I gave them informal

opportunities to correct my understanding of their experiences and perceptions. Lincoln

98

and Guba (1985) further suggest checking transcripts with participants for accuracy and

general comment. As discussed above (see Section 6.4.3), transcriptions were made of

spoken English only. As a result, copies of transcriptions were provided only to

participants who were able to read English and who wished to receive one. This process

was helpful in prompting discussions with these participants at subsequent interviews

but participants did not generally comment on or question the accuracy of transcripts in

any significant way. In addition, I engaged in formal member checking and respondent

validation at the completion of the inquiry. With the help of community leaders, a

feedback meeting (see Section 7.9.4) was arranged and participants (as well as the wider

Karen community) were invited to attend.

For inter-rater checking, Ritchie (2001) suggests the use of peer researchers at different

phases of research (particularly during the analysis phase) and promotes the benefits of

shared interpretation. I engaged in frequent debriefing with the bilingual research

assistant immediately after interviews. Issues of meanings and translations were

discussed, checked and rechecked at every point in the research process; at the time of

interviews; and when interviews were transcribed, translated and analysed. In addition, I

met with three colleagues who each separately coded sections from my interview

transcripts. We then shared our codes. It was encouraging to see that all three coders

identified variables comparable to mine. Moreover, a discussion of these codes helped

me to articulate and further explore conceptualisations I noted in my memos.

6.7.5 Criterion 5: Reflexivity and evaluative rigour

Reflexivity requires explicit acknowledgement of the researcher’s values and

sociocultural position as relevant to the research inquiry and evaluative rigour requires

that the ethical and political aspects of the research are considered and addressed (Kitto

et al., 2008). In this thesis, reflexivity is first addressed in Chapter 1 (see Section 1.4)

and again in Chapter 7, where the ethical and political aspects of the research are also

explored in detail.

6.7.6 Criterion 6: Transferability

This criterion is understood and addressed in multiple ways. Kitto et al. (2008) provide

a twofold understanding of transferability or generalisability as (a) “the capacity to

make conceptual generalisations from the local context of a qualitative study to other

99

settings” (p. 243), and (b) the applicability and usefulness of the findings to current

research knowledge, policy and practice. Contextually, Kitto and colleagues present

these criteria for authors and assessors of articles submitted to the Medical Journal of

Australia. In this biomedical milieu, positivist-oriented approaches to the transferability

of qualitative research findings may be more obligatory than in other qualitative

research environments.

There is considerable debate surrounding the concepts of transferability and

generalisation in qualitative research. The findings of qualitative research are often

made with reference to a specific setting in a specific social, historical and political time

period, and researchers may be “less interested in distribution across a population and

more concerned with a detailed understanding of people’s experience” (Daly, Kellehear,

& Gliksman, 1997, p. 99). Researchers may also wish to generalise in the qualitative

sense described by Flick (2002) as “the gradual transfer of findings from case studies

and their context to more general and abstract relations, for example a typology” (p.

232). Blignault and Ritchie (2009) explain that researchers may wish to explore a

phenomenon “for its own sake” (p. 142). Carter et al. (2009) urge qualitative

researchers to refrain from making apologies about the inability to generalise their

findings and maintain that “if you succeed in ‘proving’ that your participants were

‘average’ or ‘typical’ … you will probably thereby demonstrate that your sampling was

misdirected” (p. 109). Similarly, Patton (2002) argues that the aims, contexts and

questions of individual studies may not consider maximum variation to be a goal of the

research. Ultimately, the approach taken must be appropriate to the research aims and

researchers must be transparent about whether they seek to generalise their findings and

the degree to which this is achievable (Flick, 2002).

An aim of my inquiry is to frame recommendations for improving psychosocial support

services for Karen refugee women. In expressing this aim I am moving beyond an

exploration of the psychosocial experiences of Karen women “for its own sake”

(Blignault & Ritchie, 2009, p. 142). However, I am also not aiming to generalise my

findings beyond the research context, which would be “inconsistent with the principles”

(Carter et al., 2009, p. 109) of qualitative research and unjustified given the participant

selection strategies employed. In making clear my aims regarding generalisation and

transferability, I return to Kitto et al.’s (2008) approach whereby research is critically

100

evaluated by its applicability and usefulness to current research knowledge, policy and

practice.

6.8 Logistics and limits

As a doctoral study, this inquiry was limited to a three-year duration. It was supported

by financial grants from the Australian Psychological Society, the New South Wales

Institute of Psychiatry and the University of New South Wales. Resources and technical

support such as office space and computer equipment were provided by the School of

Public Health and Community Medicine, University of New South Wales. Throughout

the duration of the research I also needed to work part-time and continue my

commitment to my business (in which I was self-employed). Through these financial

and resource means I was able to commit to a long-term period of involvement with the

community which spanned the three-year duration of the doctoral study as well as

several months of involvement with the community prior to undertaking the research.

6.9 Conclusion

This chapter has described the methods used to address (a) the research questions, (b)

ethics, and (c) rigour. It contributes to the audit trail for this inquiry and is augmented

by the following Chapter 7.

101

Chapter 7

The Ethnographic Journey: A Researcher’s Reflections

7.1 Introduction

This chapter demonstrates the inductive and emergent nature of the inquiry. In it I will

provide a rich account of how the research unfolded and explore the difficulties and

challenges encountered along the way.

7.2 Identifying and entering the community

Identifying the Karen community was not a straightforward task and it took me a while

to do this. Like other resettled refugee populations (Spring et al., 2003), the Karen were

a relatively hidden community and, in Australia, published statistical information about

them was limited. The Australian Government did not require humanitarian entrants

from Burma to identify their ethnic affiliations and, consequently, could not

authoritatively determine the number of entrants from different ethnic minority

communities. As a result, some Karen (and other ethnic minority) entrants were likely

to be listed as Burmese. Sydney Karen community leaders were also unsure of the

precise size of the Karen community and, in 2009, estimated around 600 mostly Sgaw

and some Pwo Karen speakers. The community leaders confirmed information obtained

from the Australian Government’s DIAC database (2009c, 2011p) which indicated that

refugees from Burma residing in Sydney primarily lived in the western suburbs of

Parramatta (29%), Canterbury (20%), Auburn (16%), Blacktown (16%) and Fairfield

(10%). These suburbs are approximately 20 to 30 kilometres from Sydney city and are

amongst the most socially and economically disadvantaged in the state of New South

Wales (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2006; Department of Education, Employment

and Workplace Relations, 2011).

The Karen community in Sydney was most easily identified by its political, social and

religious organisations. The Australian Karen Organisation and its subgroups (e.g.,

Youth and Women’s Group) and affiliates (the Australian Karen Cultural Association)

formed the largest of these groups and held regular meetings and celebrations. The

Australian Karen Baptist Church was the largest Karen-specific religious organisation

and community members were also established in the congregations of other Australian

102

churches (e.g., Baptist, Anglican, Catholic and Seventh Day Adventist). Although

Karen in Burma were (and are) predominantly Buddhist (see Section 3.3.3), a

disproportionate number of refugees from Burma in Australia were Christian (DIAC,

2006).

Entering the community and building rapport was facilitated through my contact with a

few groups and community leaders as well as other individuals who served as “culture

brokers” (Liamputtong, 2010, p. 67). Through these contacts I was able to immerse

myself in the community. With the approval and endorsement of community leaders I

was introduced at a multitude of community events where I had the opportunity to

explain to the audience who I was and the nature of my research. It was critical for me

to establish rapport and approval through community leaders. However, I kept in mind

the numerous demands that researchers and organisations place on these individuals

(Liamputtong, 2008) and tried not to burden them unnecessarily. Moreover, I did not

rely on these contacts as sole gatekeepers and was aware that my connections to these

leaders, who were mostly men, might be detrimental to me gaining acceptance and trust

from women. I might – in the eyes of Karen women – be aligned with the elder Karen

men, their political and religious factions, and potentially other variables I could not

foresee. I therefore also made an effort to contact senior Karen women and arranged to

meet with a small group of them to discuss my research, answer their questions and gain

their approval and support.

It took approximately fourteen months to formally set up the study and obtain the

necessary financial support. During this time I established a relationship with the

community, built rapport, learnt about and mapped community sub-groups, attended

Karen language lessons, read literature to improve my knowledge of Karen culture and

history, and endeavoured to learn more about strengths, politics and factions (Morse &

Richards, 2002) within the community. By the time interview data collection began I

was already quite familiar with my research setting, had been invited to numerous

community functions and regularly attended weekly religious events. I had established a

reputation as a Westerner who was concerned for the welfare of the Karen people – “a

friend of our community” as one leader commonly introduced me. Towards the end of

the study, during a community feedback meeting, the most senior elder extended his

trust to me and described me as an “honorary Karen sister”. Though not an “insider” to

the community per se, such examples suggest that I gained access beyond that of an

103

“outsider” (Merriam et al., 2001; Ritchie, Zwi, Blignault, Bunde-Birouste, & Silove,

2009).

My strategies for maintaining relationships and rapport with research participants

included compensation and reciprocity (Liamputtong, 2010). I viewed reciprocity as not

only a means of maintaining relationships and rapport but also as an ethical duty and

guiding principle within my methodological framework. In appreciation of Karen

women’s time and contribution I provided each with a $20 grocery voucher. I also was

able to help community members who asked for my assistance and engaged in many

forms of reciprocity including responding to requests for advocacy, writing letters of

recommendation for education and employment applications, providing help with

completing documents and assisting with transportation. These reciprocal activities are

common amongst researchers working in refugee communities (P. Guerin & Guerin,

2007). Many of these encounters were very useful – often as useful as in-depth

interviews – in helping me to learn about Karen women’s lives. For example, through

assisting a Karen woman who needed help accessing English language classes I was

able to observe the challenges faced by both herself and the service provider. I was also

able to witness how Karen women’s approaches to communication affected their ability

to engage with services and supports. Finally, research reciprocity included reporting

research findings to participants in an appropriate manner. At the conclusion of my

research I provided visual and verbal (with an interpreter) feedback to the community

(see Section 7.9.4).

7.3 Identifying and working with a bilingual research assistant

Not being fluent in Karen dialects meant I needed to find someone to assist with

interpreting and navigating cultural nuances (Hennink, 2008; Liamputtong, 2008,

2010). Interpreters and translators are not invisible conduits of information (Angelelli,

2004; Tribe 1999a). They are an active part of cross-cultural communication encounters

and add an additional layer of perception and meaning through which information is

conveyed (J. Bailey, 2008). In response to Hennink’s (2008) call for “greater

transparency” (p. 27) in how language and communication issues are addressed in

cross-cultural research, the following sections describe how the bicultural research

assistant was selected, her role(s) and our experiences during the study.

104

The recruitment and selection process was lengthy and at times frustrating. I initially

aimed to use a qualified professional interpreter (Kapborg & Bertero, 2002) but found

there were no female Karen interpreters in Sydney accredited by the NAATI (R.

Ganesan, personal communication, 3 December, 2010). I next attempted to recruit

potential interpreters through the guidance of community leaders. This process took a

number of months but none of the proposed women were suitably available and some

lacked sufficient English language skills. I was also concerned about existing

community factions and wished to find an interpreter whose relationships within the

community would be less likely to jeopardise my ability to gain rapport and

information.

There are advantages and disadvantages to working with bilingual research assistants

who are distant or on the fringe of participants’ daily lives. Although participants may

feel more comfortable discussing topics with a relative outsider, the assistant may not

understand contemporary cultural meanings, expression or experiences of particular

groups (Edwards, 1998). However, as Liamputtong (2010) notes, “Small minority

communities may distrust cultural insiders because they fear that their issues may be

gossiped about” (p. 68). This perspective was endorsed by a male Karen key informant

who was also a well respected community leader and experienced interpreter.

After six months of failing to find a suitable bilingual research assistant I tried another

approach and asked local service providers to recommend Karen-speaking women they

employed for interpreting. My new recruitment process resulted in the recommendation

of two women, both of whom had considerable professional experience. I subsequently

invited Ms Naw to work with me in conducting the research based on (a) her personable

and empathic nature, (b) her relevant experience and competence (in multiple Karen

dialects) as an interpreter and health professional (nurse), (c) her strong bilingual and

bicultural background, (d) her time availability, (e) her relative distance from the

research participants, and (f) her interest and commitment to the research.

Ms Naw was well briefed on the purpose of the research and given training in the

interview methods and confidentiality (Tribe & Keefe, 2009). As we worked together

we trouble-shooted different problems and challenges. Of course, language cannot be

translated directly and inevitably Ms Naw needed to “actively select words or phrases to

accurately convey equal meaning” (Hennink, 2008, p. 26) when no exact translation of

105

words, idiomatic expressions or concepts was possible. For example, there was no direct

translation for the term or concept of psychological trauma so Ms Naw had to convey

the meaning through describing the impact of extremely awful life experiences on the

wellbeing of one’s heart-mind. As the research progressed we became familiar with

common words and concepts that presented difficulty and spent time together

discussing meanings. I also explored the meaning(s) of these words and concepts in

meetings with Karen key informants including other interpreters.

Dialectal differences pose a serious challenge to researchers working with communities

of refugees from Burma and a “common interpretation problem is expecting one person

from Burma to understand another” (S. Smith, 2010). Mr W, a Karen key informant,

confirmed this problem when he explained: “Karen is not only one language. It is like

Aborigines here [in Australia]. So many dialects they don’t understand each other”. In

addition to recognised differences in dialects, participants also reported that Karen

speakers who were institutionalised in camps for long periods of time tended to speak

“camp Karen” versions of different dialects. Key informants explained that even within

the main dialect of Sgaw, different expressions could be used to describe emotion states

by persons from different villages or camps. Research participants in this inquiry were

predominantly Sgaw Karen speakers and a few spoke Pwo Karen. Ms Naw was fluent

in both dialects but still had to frequently check her understanding with participants to

ensure she interpreted as accurately as possible. Participants themselves negotiated and

navigated dialect differences during group interviews and key informant interviews with

two or more Karen speakers.

Ms Naw’s community and cultural knowledge and expertise were invaluable. In

addition to assisting with interviews she helped to plan and organise interview

opportunities and introduced me to some key informants. She actively contributed to the

ongoing research dialogue through interpreting and conveying cultural meanings but

was not involved in any other technical, administrative or analytic tasks and did not

influence any other decision-making throughout the research process. Debriefing after

interviews, and especially after potentially distressing encounters, is an important

process when working with bilingual research assistants (Liamputtong, 2008, 2010) (see

Section 6.5). After potentially upsetting interviews I contacted Ms Naw to (a)

acknowledge that the information we heard was potentially distressing, and (b) offer

support. Ms Naw did not wish to engage in extensive discussion about the sensitive

106

information to which she was exposed. However, she appeared grateful that I checked

on her wellbeing.

7.4 Informal participant observation encounters

I made over 200 hours of observations over a twenty-four month period (September

2009 – September 2011) during which I attended dozens of cultural and religious events

as well as smaller gatherings with Karen women and their families. In addition to

interviews, I socialised informally with Karen women in their homes, in restaurants and

in cars while travelling. I also joined them in attending service providers, community

consultations and education classes. Across these various experiences the extent to

which I was a participant or observer was in a constant state of flux. In reflecting on my

place on the participant observer continuum, I refer to Gold’s (1958) description, which

I have adapted as a figure below:

Complete participant Participant-as-observer Observer-as-participant Complete observer

Figure 3: Four-point participant observer continuum (Adapted from Gold, 1958)

At the beginning of the inquiry I was almost always a complete observer. I knew few

people and at functions often felt unable to adequately communicate with the people I

met. Consequently, field notes from these early encounters were highly descriptive.

Though I was not able to access women’s perceptions during these encounters, I was

able to make behavioural observations of issues such as evident gender role rules and

expectations. As the inquiry progressed I became, at times, much more of a participant-

as-observer. For example, I helped a Karen woman access social services; physically

assisting her (she had several severe health problems and was unable to walk far) and

advocating on her behalf. I was a participant in her experience and at the same time I

was a researcher observing and trying to comprehend the encounter from the

perspectives of the various parties involved.

Although I wished to participate and observe women’s lives, it was not always feasible

to do so out of respect for community leaders. For example, at particularly important

community gatherings I was often separated from “ordinary” Karen women (who were

often working in the kitchen) as leaders requested that I sit at the front of the room with

107

community leaders and distinguished non-Karen guests, such as local politicians. I was

often the only woman at the table. If I refused the request to sit at the guests’ table I

would have disrespected the leaders and the cultural custom of demonstrating esteem

towards people with graduate education (or other sources of authority). I also missed

other opportunities for participant observation due to personal and work commitments.

I found myself pondering ethical dilemmas early on in participant observation.

Presumably not everyone present at a community gathering had been informed about

my research by the leaders, and yet, here I was observing them. What about their

consent? Furthermore, what about the children I observed interacting with their mothers

– I am sure no one had told them about my research or sought their assent. The method

of participant observation is not without controversy as it raises complex ethical

questions regarding the rights and abilities of participants to consent to being observed

by the researcher. Deception and covert participant observation are hotly debated issues

and participant consent in relation to the method is “seldom clear-cut” (Griffin &

Bengry-Howell, 2008, p. 24). Covert research observation without the explicit consent

of those observed is considered unethical by many (Patton, 2002; Woods, 1992) while

others accept the use of this longstanding social science research strategy (Douglas,

1976). Some argue that absolute open consent is unachievable in the context of

ethnographic fieldwork as it is unclear to what extent consent from every single

community member included in the observation is obtainable (Daly et al., 1997).

Questions arise regarding the nature of consent and whether a hierarchy of consent is

acceptable (e.g., when senior members approve the observation on behalf of the

community) (de Laine, 1997). I referred to literature (Atkinson & Hammersley, 1994;

Li, 2008) and eventually I came to accept that I would do no harm provided I applied

the same ethical guidelines to participant observation as to interviews (including being

sensitive when writing up findings).

Ethnographers may face additional ethical challenges in relation to deception as their

research unfolds. For example, in order to participate in activities they may pretend “to

share values and beliefs” (Patton, 2002, p. 271). I grappled with this dilemma when I

found myself participating in political and religious activities that I did not personally

endorse. Was I deceiving those I observed by partaking in their customs and traditions?

I felt uncomfortable about this but ensured that I always informed people about my

108

research. I also decided that should I be asked about my personal religious and political

affiliations I would be honest – throughout the research no-one asked.

Grills (1998) views the heart of ethnography as providing “perspective, or how people

see their world” (p. 4) and sees participant observation as central to that endeavour.

During the study I often wondered if the method resulted less in an understanding of

Karen participants’ perspectives, and perhaps, as much (or more in some cases) in

gaining understanding or insight into how their world perceived them. Indeed, perhaps

due to the limitation of not fluently speaking their language, my field notes were often

behaviourally descriptive. I saw how women behaved in their daily environment(s) and

I saw how others (e.g., native Australians, service providers, restaurant waiters, etc.) in

their environment(s) behaved towards them. For example, whilst joining Karen women

in an English language class I observed both the passivity of the women’s interactions

with their non-Karen classmates and teachers, but also, and most alarmingly, the often

negative responses of these other Australians to them. Such observation was still very

helpful and conducive to addressing the aims of my research as I was able to better

understand limitations with service provisions by observing interactions rather than just

interviewing women and asking about their perspectives of such encounters.

Over a two-year period through my engagement in Karen women’s lives I developed

strong relationships with many community members. However, I am not an insider in

the Karen community, rather I remain an accepted – perhaps even trusted by some –

outsider, fortunate enough to be allowed to partake in insider perspectives, activities and

experiences on occasion. Nevertheless, before and during research I also continually

questioned the degree to which my approach to participant observation aligned with

traditional ethnographic fieldwork and notions of what constitutes a “full ethnography”

(Griffin & Bengry-Howell, 2008, p. 17). Traditional ethnographers typically learn the

local language before undertaking research (Fielding, 2001) and engage in extensive

field work of a suggested minimum of six months to two years or more (Griffin &

Bengry-Howell, 2008; Liamputtong, 2009). I did not acquire sufficient knowledge of

the Karen language (or various dialects) to work with non-English-speaking Karen

without an interpreter but I did fulfil the recommended two years of prolonged

engagement in the field. I was also unsure about the extent of my immersion in Karen

women’s culture. According to Goffman (1989), participant observation fieldwork

involves:

109

Subjecting yourself, your own body and your own personality, and your own

situation to the set of contingencies that play upon a set of individuals, so that

you can physically and ecologically penetrate their circle of response to their

social situation, or their work situation, or their ethnic situation. (p. 125)

I did not subject myself to the set of contingencies and ecological contexts that

characterised the lives of Karen women. I did not live in the same sociopolitical,

economic and built environment that my participants lived in and I did not experience

the joys and challenges of parenting their children day after day. I did, however, spend

time in their homes, in their broader physical communities and in their churches. I

joined in their community events and celebrations and I held their babies and played

with their children. I participated in women’s interactions and one day, en route to

attend a community gathering, a participant invited me to her home and bedroom where

she dressed me in traditional Karen garments. I joined Karen women in their education

classes and other social situations such as going out for food. But I cannot say that I

“ecologically penetrated” their lives. However, by engaging in activities alongside the

Karen women I have achieved extended immersion through what Daly et al. (1997)

describe as “periodic observations” consisting of “dropping in to see what is happening

at different points in time” (p. 158). The time limits imposed on a doctoral research

project and other practical limitations prevented me from a fuller immersion in the lives

of Karen women. My approach to participant observation is tempered somewhere

between Goffman’s and Daly et al.’s perspectives and was adequate for the purpose of

addressing the research aims and questions.

7.5 Formal interview encounters

I interviewed 10 Karen refugee women over a twelve month period from February 2010

to February 2011. Most women were interviewed only once, but some participated in

several interviews. I had initially envisaged the interpreter’s presence at all interviews

with Karen women who did not fluently speak English. However, after meeting with

each participant once or twice with the interpreter present and covering the key themes

of the interview guide I then engaged in informal encounters with participants who

spoke English reasonably well. Women generally preferred to be interviewed in their

own homes when other adults or school-age children were away. The average duration

of formal encounters with Karen women was two hours.

110

All women consented to an audio-recording. Like Booth and Booth (1994), I did not

audio-record informal encounters during my participant observations. This gave me the

freedom to move around and talk in noisy or busy environments (e.g., when women

were cooking). After these encounters I wrote brief field notes of key themes or dictated

my observations into the audio-recorder, as soon as I returned to my car. These notes

were later (within 24-hours) expanded.

Over the period of November 2009 to February 2011, I formally interviewed 23 key

informants. These encounters took place at the participants’ own place of residence or

work and lasted, on average, one and a half hours. Though most key informants were

interviewed on one occasion only, four were interviewed on two or more occasions.

Interviews with English-speaking key informants and service providers were undertaken

in English and an interpreter was not present. The final selection of key informants

included:

Community leaders (e.g., pastors and elders)

Healthcare providers (e.g., doctors, psychiatrists and counsellors)

HSS providers (e.g., case managers, interpreters, counsellors and bicultural

workers)

Social service providers (e.g., interpreters and political advocates)

All but three key informant interviews were audio-recorded. Only one, a healthcare

practitioner, denied consent. On another occasion, technical difficulties with the

dictaphone prevented recording and during another interview I offered to cease

recording when I observed that the informant was uncomfortable with the dictaphone.

During my formal encounters I used interview guides that I refined as the research

progressed, research questions evolved and I began to focus on new areas not identified

in the initial plan. These new directions were guided by ideas and concepts generated

from data analysis (see Section 7.8). In hindsight, the thorough development and

translation of the guide for Karen women reflected my anxiety and desire to be prepared

prior to beginning interviews. The process of back-translation of interview guides was

lengthy and expensive. It was also ultimately largely unnecessary as I found a less

structured interview approach to be most helpful in establishing rapport and eliciting

information. Although I had begun with a semi-structured translated interview guide,

111

my approach rapidly developed into a form more like the theme lists described by

Liamputtong and Ezzy (2005).

Like Liamputtong and Ezzy (2005), I found in-depth interviewing to be “a privilege” (p.

55). Aside from a few participants who appeared guarded, most were forthcoming with

information and I felt privileged that they trusted me with their stories and perspectives.

I believe that my involvement in their community fostered rapport with Karen

participants.

At the beginning of interviews I took care to situate each participant as the expert of

their own experience and positioned myself as a concerned and interested learner-

listener seeking to understand their story and viewpoint. During interviews (particularly

with Karen women) I also encouraged a “two-way relationship” (Egharevba, 2001, p.

233) in which I encouraged women to ask questions of me. Frequently, women wanted

to know personal information such as whether I had children. Many also asked about

my home country (New Zealand) and about my time on the Thailand-Burma border. I

answered their questions freely and believe that such self-disclosure encouraged

rapport. On many occasions women also asked questions to which I did not know the

answers. For example, women asked questions about immigration processes or social

welfare payment rules and regulations. When I did not know the answer to their

questions I freely admitted my ignorance. In some instances I was able to seek out the

information they needed or refer them to someone who could help. One participant

expressed suicidal ideation and I subsequently made a referral to a doctor and mental

health service.

I approached particularly sensitive topics with caution and, naturally, participants

differed in their level of knowledge and willingness to discuss issues such as sexual

violence. Karen women were often hesitant to openly discuss sensitive topics and I soon

learnt that contexts and physical gestures conveyed messages different to the verbal

content tied to them. For example, when asked if some Karen women experienced

domestic violence, a previously forthcoming participant might lower her head and eyes

and respond “no” or simply “some might, I don’t know”. Nevertheless, little by little,

these and other more puzzling communications became clear and I found myself

learning when “yes” meant “no” and “no” meant “yes”. In contrast, I found that key

informants were often relieved to be invited to articulate their thoughts about sensitive

112

issues. For example, after a conversation in which a Karen leader discussed community

attitudes to single mothers, I pointed out that we had been discussing a difficult topic.

He replied: “It is, Paula. But thank you for asking me about these kinds of things; these

things are coming to me [from community members]”. Karen women also frequently

thanked me for visiting and talking with them: “Thank you for coming. I’m so glad that

you want to know what our problems are, I’m glad to talk about my problems” (Hpä

Hpaw Mu).

In reading over my transcripts I realised that, despite my best intentions, I had not

always listened well, especially in the earlier interviews when I had often been nervous.

Sometimes I was anxious about going over time and how I would afford the interpreter.

Other times I had not always heard the participant’s response because I was anxiously

trying to categorise their comments and plan the use of the remaining interview time – a

pitfall described by Liamputtong and Ezzy (2005). The quality of earlier interviews was

markedly different from the later ones. As the study progressed, I relaxed and allowed

myself to respond with a more flexible interview style, and match my approach to each

participant. For example, I found that some women did not feel confident directly

outlining the properties of particular idioms of distress. However, if I approached the

interview in a different manner, asking them to describe stories about third persons who

suffered from different conditions, they happily provided detailed accounts.

Balancing engagement and detachment during interviews became an ongoing process.

This was not difficult during all interviews but featured strongly in some. I had to be

careful not to slip into patterns of psychological therapy. I was there to learn and had to

ensure my research was rigorous. At the same time I had to be careful not to detach

myself so much that distance between myself and participants would reduce

understanding and rapport (Patton, 2002). As a feminist researcher I was interacting

with women on a personal level. I allowed myself to empathise with women. For

example, when Hpä Hpaw Mu described her experience of social rejection and isolation

I empathised and confirmed that her experience sounded very hard. I let her know that if

I was in her shoes I would have struggled with the experience too. I also validated

women’s bravery. When Htee Gloh shared her story of protecting her children from

bullets, I attested to her strength and courage. When I was humbled by women’s stories

I let them know. When I sat on the floor beside Lah Luh as she cried and spoke of

wanting to die it seemed appropriate to physically comfort her.

113

During interviews with key informants, especially non-Karen service providers, I

generally found it easy to simply listen and observe even when I personally disagreed

with their comments – I was focused on understanding their perspective as this helped

me to better understand what was going on in Karen women’s lives in Australia.

However, in hindsight I wonder whether I should have taken on more of an advocacy

role. For example, during one participant observation encounter, I witnessed a service

provider make offensive and misinformed comments about Karen women in their

presence (see Section 12.7.2). At the time I felt embarrassed and angry but I followed

the lead of the Karen women I was with and did not challenge the service provider’s

point of view. The encounter gave me insight into Karen women’s lives and experiences

with this service provider. From one perspective doing nothing was the right thing to do

– I think the Karen women I was with would have been very uncomfortable to disagree

or challenge a service provider and I could have made them uncomfortable by doing so

in their presence. On the other hand I failed to show respect and dignity towards the

women I was with by not advocating on their behalf. I also failed to demonstrate to

them Australian cultural norms in which it is both safe and appropriate to disagree with

a service provider if they express offensive, prejudicial and incorrect views about an

ethnic group. Some weeks later I had the opportunity to meet the service provider in

private. I did not specifically refer back to the incident but, during our conversation,

provided some education about the experiences and context of refugees fleeing Burma.

7.5.1 Formal encounters with groups

Five group interviews were conducted between April 2010 and June 2011 to explore

vignettes. A total of 28 Karen women attended these interviews. One group interview

was held at a woman’s home whilst all others were conducted at a community centre

that the women were familiar attending. All group participants provided informed

consent and agreed to the audio-recording. One man attended the second group

interview and had been invited by one of the women. The group agreed that he should

stay and join the discussion. On reflection, his presence did not appear to alter the

information obtained from the women as the women’s discussions were comparable to

other group interviews and included the topics of menstrual blood flow and its

relationship to physical and emotional wellbeing. Each group interview was two hours

in duration; this was just sufficient to explore the three vignettes from the interview

guide (see Section 6.4.2).

114

Rapport was easily established in group interviews. Most participants freely discussed

their perspectives about the vignettes and many also revealed details of their own lives

related to the vignettes. I believe this apparent comfort was largely due to the fact that

the intermediary, who assisted organising the groups, ensured that each group was

relatively homogeneous whilst demographic variation (e.g., age, political or religious

affiliation) was apparent across the different groups.

Time and participant lateness were the most persistent problems affecting group

interviews. Room availability was limited and we were often unable to go over time so

interviews were affected when women arrived twenty or thirty minutes late and then

needed to go through consent forms after everyone else had completed theirs. This

problem was not unique to my group interviews. Several Karen community leaders

joked about “Karen time”, meaning that Western concepts of time and punctuality are

not familiar to the Karen. Other organised events within the community often began up

to one hour after the scheduled start. In hindsight, this problem could have been

somewhat averted if I had been able to secure a venue for three to four hours rather than

the two hours available at the centre used.

Theoretical saturation of EM concepts (see Section 6.4.2) was thoroughly achieved

following three group interviews. Subsequently, I utilised the fourth interview to delve

deeper into (a) idioms of distress, and (b) properties of other concepts generated from

data analysis. To do this I moved beyond the planned methods for group interviews and

employed the storyboard interview techniques utilised by the Centre for Refugee

Research (n.d.). This approach has been used to identify, analyse and problem solve

issues of concern for refugee communities, particularly refugee women, and involves

asking small groups of participants to prepare panels (using drawing, pictures or

collages) to address questions that pertain to an issue of concern. Each group then

creates one large collage of their panels and presents their work to the wider group.

According to the Centre for Refugee Research, the six stages must take participants

through describing a problem or issue to be worked on, to a solution and an outcome.

I asked women to separate into groups of three or four people and select an issue of

concern from a summary list of problems identified from data analysis (e.g., parenting,

domestic violence and unemployment). Each group then prepared five pictures to

address the following questions about their chosen issue:

115

1. What is the impact of this problem on the individual, families and community?

(How does it affect the people concerned? What happens to them? How does

this affect their families and their communities?)

2. What happens to these women and girls now? (What help is available to them

and do you feel it is effective?)

3. If you were in charge of services for refugees what would you provide for these

women and girls, their families and the community in order to address this

issue?

4. Who do you think could provide these services? Who would you want to do

this? What help would the community need to address it?

5. If all these services were available what would be the best outcome for these

women and girls?

The five pictures were then pasted onto one large piece of cardboard which the group

presented as a storyboard. The peer audience and I asked questions and gave feedback.

Women worked together very hard during the storyboard group session and appeared to

enjoy both the opportunity for group work and the chance to constructively discuss

problems that affected them. The level of detail obtained from this exercise assisted me

in understanding the emotional impact of life stressors on Karen women in a much

richer way and allowed me to explore more fully the ways in which women perceived

the problems in their lives. The following images present some of the storyboard panels

from the group interview.

116

Image 1: Complete storyboard regarding employment difficulties

Image 2: Storyboard panel depicting women’s isolation

117

Image 3: Storyboard panel depicting Karen women’s difficulties assisting their

children with homework

Lastly, I used the fifth and final group interview to check and further explore my

understanding of the experiences, perceptions and problems faced by single Karen

mothers. This group was a rare and valuable opportunity to meet with a homogenous

group of recipients of the Women at Risk visa subclass (see Section 4.2.2). I opened the

group discussion summarising what I had learnt from women so far, seeking feedback

and moving towards clarification of topics. Perhaps because of women’s familiarity

with each other, and their shared context (being single mothers), this group was one of

the most relaxed, frank and emotive of all group interviews.

7.6 Transcribing speech and presenting quotations

All audio-recorded interviews were transcribed in full, a process which took

approximately three to four hours per hour of recorded speech, depending on quality of

recording and differing accents. This process was most complex for group interview

118

recordings where interruptions and multiple simultaneous speakers were common and it

was not always possible to identify all speakers (Bloor et al., 2001; Bryman, 2008).

Transcriptions were typed and saved with as little editing as possible. Published

quotations from interpreted interviews represented the bilingual research assistant’s

English articulation and interpretation of the participant’s statement and were tidied to

omit incorrect literal spellings and verbal noises such as “um”. I faced a dilemma in

deciding how to present quotations from interviews where an interpreter was not

present. Many participants (including key informants) were from non-English speaking

backgrounds and the literal transcriptions of their interviews were often clumsy and

difficult to ready. Of course, linguistic variety “is an important feature of cultural and

subcultural identity” (J. Bailey, 2008, p. 129) and there was also an argument to be

made for reliably conveying my encounters with key informants from non-English

speaking backgrounds in order to contextualise the interview data. However, these

literal transcripts risked portraying these respondents as inarticulate or less educated

than was the case (J. Bailey, 2008). I decided to employ standard English grammar and

spelling conventions for all published quotations. False starts, repetition and noises such

as “ummm” were omitted. Other verbal interactions such as laughs were included as

these were important to understanding the context and meaning conveyed in the verbal

message.

7.7 Ethics

Social scientists, and particularly those who work with vulnerable populations, are

encouraged to consider dimensions of ethical research practice beyond the aspects

traditionally addressed by ethics committees (e.g., informed consent, protection from

harm and protection of privacy) (Allotey & Manderson, 2003; Angrosino, 2005;

Fontana & Frey, 2005; Hugman, Pittaway, & Bartolomei, 2011; Liamputtong, 2010;

Liamputtong & Ezzy, 2005). Broader ethical and moral issues, encompassing what is

sometimes termed the “progressive social agenda” (Angrosino, 2005, p. 734) concern

the values that guide our work as well as our sensitivities to how our research is placed

in the local, cultural and political research environment in which we and our

participants live. The practicalities and challenges of addressing these broad dimensions

of ethics are explored in this section.

119

As discussed (see Section 6.5), ethical approval for this project was sought and obtained

from the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of New South Wales.

Despite our shared interest in protecting the wellbeing of participants I experienced

some difficulty balancing my commitment to conducting moral and culturally ethical

research with the procedural requirements of the ethics committee – particularly those

relating to informed consent.

The process of informed consent is important and seeks to ensure that informants and

participants are fully aware of the nature and purpose of the research including who is

undertaking it, what it involves and how the information will be disseminated.

However, informed consent is also a “Thorny Issue [which] can be intimidating to many

cultural and ethnic groups, particularly those who are doubly vulnerable like refugees”

(Liamputtong, 2008, pp. 12–13) and is made difficult by first language illiteracy as well

as negative experiences and fear caused by living under a repressive regime. Previous

research with Karen populations had encountered challenges with the Western concept

of written informed consent (Fuertes, 2007). I explained these concerns about the

written consent process to the ethics committee and suggested that refugee participants

be able to provide verbal consent. The committee initially rejected this approach but

eventually agreed to a process in which participants could provide written or verbal, but

audio-recorded, consent.

In most cases Karen women did not ask questions about consent – I think that some

would have signed the consent form without reading it – but I ensured that they held a

copy of the form whilst Ms Naw (bilingual research assistant) explained the content. All

of the women were happy to sign the form so I did not have to use the option of verbal

consent. My experience of willingness to provide written consent differs to that of other

researchers who have worked with Karen refugee communities (Fuertes, 2007) and I

cannot be sure of the reasons for this. However, I suspect that participants in my study

were more familiar with the process of signing forms as they now live in Australia

(Fuertes undertook data collection in refugee camps). It is also possible that my

extended period of contact and rapport building prior to data collection, as well as the

support I obtained from community leaders, also contributed to women’s willingness to

provide consent.

120

7.7.1 Maintaining confidentiality and anonymity

The Karen community and surrounding community of support services was very small

and I needed to be sensitive to confidentiality and ethics. All Karen women were given

Karen pseudonyms (e.g., Mu Htòh) taken from Sgaw Karen names of flora, fauna and

other natural elements. However, I spent considerable time grappling with the task of

grouping or representing key informants in a manner that (a) protected their identity, but

(b) allowed for the data and different subgroups (see Section 7.5) to be accurately

conveyed. Some of my earlier attempts at protecting participants’ anonymity resulted in

a host of difficult to read acronyms (e.g., HCKI for healthcare key informant) and others

failed to protect participant anonymity. Ultimately I decided on a system whereby the

term “key informant” was used sparingly as a sole descriptor. Additional descriptors

conveyed other relevant participant demographics. For example, Karen (as opposed to

non-Karen) key informants were always identified as such. Cases where a key

informant was also a Karen woman were made explicit. The gender and ethnicity of

other key informants was reported if relevant to the inquiry. Key informants who were

doctors were identified as such. Due to the exceptionally small number of other

healthcare key informants (e.g., psychiatrists and counsellors) in the study, these

participants are not routinely distinguished from other service provider key informants

unless it was safe and necessary to do so.

7.8 The analytic journey

At times the cyclical pattern of grounded theory analysis seemed to be moving too

quickly. There were moments when I would have liked to have spent more time

analysing and developing key concepts before undertaking further theoretical sampling

but sometimes interviews with participants (especially busy key informants such as

doctors) had to be grabbed when the opportunity arose. Charmaz (1990) suggests that

“delaying focused theoretical sampling fosters gaining an in-depth understanding” (p.

1163). Whilst I agree with this sentiment, the reality of balancing opportunities and

involvement in the field with an ideal research process did not conform to such a

system. On the other hand, there were also times when I experienced forced delays in

theoretical sampling. For example, some informants who I sought to select on the basis

of my developing analysis were difficult to contact and even more difficult to arrange

an interview with.

121

Early line-by-line analysis of transcripts helped me to immerse myself in the data and

also provided an opportunity to critique my interview technique. After a few interviews

I had dozens of descriptive codes, some of which were lengthy and vague. One early

code included “If it doesn’t work don’t do it” which referred to participants’ cultural

wisdom and commonsense advice that unhelpful mental practices, such as ruminating

over sad thoughts, should not be continued. Charmaz (1990) warns against unnecessary

jargon and encourages researchers to label their categories with simple, direct and vivid

words. Gradually, the labels and codes themselves were refined as common and

distinguishing characteristics of each code were formed and rudimentary categories

developed. I also soon realised that some codes seemed to be “types of” a number of

things and often these particular concepts continued to retain their importance as

analysis progressed. For example, “mistrust” was a type of problem encountered in

relation to “interpreters”. I also coded it as a property of “societal effects of war” and

additionally as a feature of “relationship difficulties” with peers. I then developed

further questions about mistrust – I wanted to know about the origins, experience and

impact of this issue.

As analysis progressed I developed and refined the relationships or connections between

categories. I found that many of my findings were interrelated. For example, health

issues were related to education and employment problems, which were, in turn, central

to financial woes and poverty. Refugee researchers P. Guerin and Guerin (2007) have

argued that it is “imperative that topics about refugee communities … cannot be treated

as separate or ‘compartmentalized’” (p. 154). As I analysed and wrote about the data, I

aimed to convey the interrelationship of many of the issues related to Karen women’s

lives and wellbeing.

The initial interviews led me to sense that the concept of feeling useless encompassed

far more for participants than frustration at their inability to achieve goals. But what was

it? I raised these observations and questions in my memo-writing and went on to gather

more data as well as re-analyse previously coded data. I went back through earlier

interviews and sorted material on “use / uselessness” and personal and collective value.

Looking back through the interviews with this interest now in mind prompted me to

look again at comments and accounts that reflected this topic. Such data had often been

coded as something else and now became coded in additional ways. For example, I had

previously created a code titled “bored” in which some women had made comparisons

122

to their level of activity (e.g., work or volunteer involvement) pre-migration to their

current status and the boredom they experienced in Australia – I wondered if this

actually represented one aspect of “uselessness”. Subsequently, I built questions about

“use / uselessness” into later interviews, allowing me to gather a more complete picture

of the topic. As categories grew more detailed I proceeded to ask questions of the data

and of subsequent participants in order to more fully understand the properties of

emerging categories and concepts. Eventually I realised that both “use / uselessness”

and “identity” were interrelated concepts that were central to Karen women’s wellbeing.

As conceptual categories were developed I shifted into areas I had not expected to

study. I did not intend to study societal level relationships between culture and

experiences of persecution. However, I developed an interest in it as topics around

women’s daily struggles were located within this milieu. Key informants attached

significance to these issues, which, in turn caused me to look at them more

systematically, to look again at what women were telling me, and then raise new

questions such as how religious belief systems related to women’s beliefs about

suffering. Later on, how these issues intertwined with other themes and categories

became evident.

I occasionally found myself with disparate accounts within my data. For example, some

non-Karen key informants thought that Karen women did not particularly experience

domestic violence or other social problems whereas other participants did. The

following quotation from Charmaz (1990) was helpful in guiding me through analysing

disparate accounts:

Unlike some qualitative researchers, grounded theorists may show less concern

with ascertaining the “accuracy” of a specific piece of data than with its

theoretical relevance. Two people, such as an ill person and his or her caregiver,

may give different account for the ill person’s activities. Here, whose rendering

of reality comes closer to “truth” has less importance than the analytic issues

raised within each view … (p. 1167)

Through exploring conflicting accounts I was able to understand the processes by which

key aspects of women’s expressions of distress led to incomplete understandings of

their situation by others. I was able to draw together issues such as religion and culture,

123

widespread or societal reactions to protracted persecution, and daily post-resettlement

challenges in order to see how these disparate views actually reflected the interaction of

these variables. In short, it was not about establishing accuracy, but rather gaining

understanding.

Writing draft after draft of documents for publication, conference presentation and

thesis assisted me to develop further insights and refine my analysis. Re-writing early

drafts following feedback from others also helped me to identify problems in my

analysis and make explicit my assumptions. Though I did not impose my preconceived

categories emanating from my disciplinary background during analysis, I did attempt to

view these categories through the lens of my data to try to explain both any similarities

and differences in concepts. Occasionally, pre-existing concepts fitted with the data I

observed. I felt confused about this – was I imposing bias and forcing the data into pre-

existing categories? I returned to the grounded theory literature and found guidance

from Glaser and Strauss (1967):

The core categories can emerge in the sociologist’s mind from his reading, life

experiences, research and scholarship; [furthermore] no sociologist can possibly

erase from his mind all the theory he knows before he begins his research.

Indeed the trick is to line up what one takes as theoretically possible or probable

with what one is finding in the field. (p. 253)

Similarly, Charmaz (1990) addressed this issue explaining that “Should grounded

theorists apply such concepts and categories – even their own previous ones – to new

data, they must justify them” (p. 1170). Where pre-existing theories appeared to fit with

the data (see Section 10.6.1) I explained and justified their use. The following findings

chapters demonstrate the “line-up” between the data obtained from participants and

various theoretical interpretations that aid explanation and understanding.

7.9 The reflexive journey

7.9.1 Memo-writing

Through the process of memo-writing I explored emerging ideas regarding data

analysis, both free from my theoretical background and within it. The “on-going dialog

124

with self” (Charmaz, 1990, p. 1169) that arose produced many of the questions and

ideas which led to the creation of key insights and conceptual understandings.

My memos also contained comments that I later recognised to be my own stance and

allowed me to monitor myself as a research tool. Re-reading these memos I was able to

observe tendencies towards “going native” (Liamputtong & Ezzy, 2005, p. 169) (i.e.,

losing all objectivity). I consequently came to understand the aspects of women’s lives

that I was most able to integrate myself within, as well as those I could not. The process

also provided me with space for reflexivity in which I could express my frustrations,

doubts and self-observations:

I think that I’ve been bogged down in analysis. I need to pull my head up for

some air. I’ve been too caught up in descriptive aspects of this topic and not

thinking conceptually. Yet if I allow myself to simply write, I realise the

conceptual links that are there. (Memo: 12 October, 2010)

Further examples of my reflexive reaction to the research process can be seen in the

following two memos:

I’m struggling with contact with the community and guilt – [name removed]

seemed disappointed that I won’t be able to visit her or conduct another group

interview with the women until October. I feel bad. Inevitably as data analysis

proceeds I have periods where I spend less time in the community. I feel guilty

about this. I’m conscious of how this could be perceived – as though I came and

took what I wanted and then I left. This isn’t the case from my perspective and it

isn’t my intention. (Memo: 28 August, 2010)

I’ve been thinking about bias. By virtue of the kind of research question or

general research aim I begin with I am imposing my stance on the research.

Because of my background in mental health I’m interested in emotional

wellbeing. I bring this focus to my research. If the Karen community were to be

contacted and offered a doctoral student or researcher from the School of Public

Health and Community Medicine who would conduct research into a topic of

their choosing for a period of three years would they choose to focus on

emotional health? No. I don’t think so. There are so many immediate pressing

needs of physical wellbeing impacting this community. Sometimes I feel guilty

125

to have all these resources and then to come to them with my bias. (Memo: 4

November, 2010)

7.9.2 Learning patience and being honest

Liamputtong (2008) suggests that cross-cultural researchers must have (a) tolerance for

ambiguity, (b) patience, (c) adaptiveness, (d) capacity for tacit learning, (e) courtesy,

and (f) respect for cultural beliefs and practices (p. 5). Similarly, Schweitzer and Steel

(2008) emphasise the importance of establishing trusting relationships and a willingness

to flexibly adapt to the needs and cultural approaches of refugee participants.

Throughout the research process I found that I was challenged and called upon to

develop these skills and qualities many times. I was particularly challenged with regards

to patience as my participants were accustomed to a more casual pace of life and

subsequently meetings and appointment times were often delayed and, consequently,

went longer than expected. In respect for Karen cultural practices I had to be flexible in

my expectations and courteous in my responses. To develop good rapport with

participants and the broader community I accepted many invitations to share meals,

attend family events and partake in community activities. Like Liamputtong, I found

that accepting such invitations enhanced my relationships and also contributed to my

goals of conducting ethical research practices by respecting the cultural practices of

research participants.

Throughout the research process I had to be very careful to ensure that I was honest and

transparent in my role and capabilities as a researcher. I frequently had to reiterate that I

was unable to make any promises that my research would directly benefit any

individuals or the community in any way. Similarly, I was always careful not to

contribute to disempowerment or dependency and situated the Karen as experts on their

own experiences and culture. I had to be careful not to claim to have authority or expert

knowledge regarding how to solve the complex problems faced by the community.

7.9.3 Distance and the “other”

As I grappled with the ethics of my research I also encountered confronting concepts

and questions from postcolonial (Macleod & Bhatia, 2008) and feminist (Gergen, 2008)

literature. My research was borne from my observation and experience of the hegemony

of Western mental health knowledge during my time on the Thailand-Burma border.

126

Although ethnic hostilities were present between the Karen and dominant Burman long

before their more recent colonial history, British colonisation also fuelled and created

further disharmony (see Section 3.2.1). Moreover, as for many other postcolonial

nations, Burma’s current civil war may be most recently traceable to ramifications of

neocolonial domination. All of these things bother(ed) me, and yet, here I was (a White,

middle-class, able-bodied and educated woman) effectively attempting to “capture” the

“other” and represent and interpret her experience(s). Western imperialism exists in my

research. Linguistic imperialism is also present. There are inevitably contradictions here

that I cannot fully resolve. I can only explicate them and in doing so attempt to be as

sensitive, humble and careful as possible in all aspects of my research.

Sparks and Park (2000) describe the ethical dilemmas faced by feminist cross-cultural

psychologists working with non-Western refugee and immigrant women as work at the

“border” (p. 203). They recommend researchers have (a) an understanding of cultural

context, (b) an awareness of oppressive forces in the Western context, (c) enhanced self-

awareness, and (d) a flexible approach to solution finding. Reinharz (1992) describes

the challenge as one of “confronting two competing sets of ethics – respect for women

and respect for culture” (p. 123) and acknowledges that there is no one correct way of

working through these complexities. These feminist perspectives were highly relevant

to my inquiry.

Throughout my research and especially during analysis and writing up I continued to

question my legitimacy in researching and writing about the cultural “other”. I was, and

remain, external to the Karen community. Potential benefits of an “outsider perspective”

(Liamputtong, 2010, p. 115) include the discovery of new perspectives due to the

researcher not taking for granted the assumptions of participants, and participants

feeling safer confiding in someone external to their community. Ultimately, debate

remains as to the limitations and advantages of insider-indigenous versus outsider

researchers (Banks, 1998; Liamputtong, 2010) and I cannot be sure whether Karen

women would have shared different information if I had been an indigenous researcher,

though I imagine they might have been more forthcoming with their complaints and

perhaps more transparent about political, gender and ethnic tensions.

Gender, education and age are further features and differences that may be important

influences in ethnographic fieldwork (Liamputtong & Ezzy, 2005). I believe that a

127

shared gender was helpful in establishing rapport and interviewing Karen women about

their personal lives. Having high academic status can serve to create distance

(Liamputtong, 2010). However, I believe that my credentials were mostly reassuring to

the Karen who demonstrate a great respect for education and balanced the potentially

negative effects of my younger age. Other social class differences also differentiated me

from Karen refugee women participants. I tried to minimise these differences through

dressing in a tidy yet relaxed manner. I also comfortably participated in aspects of

women’s lives (e.g., meals and helping children) to reassure them of my ease in their

surroundings and I shared my own experiences and stories where appropriate in order to

relate our lives. However, I remained very conscious of my social position and the class

differences between us. After interviews I left women’s homes (which sometimes

lacked heating or appropriate ventilation and had broken windows or no curtains) in low

socioeconomic areas and proceeded to drive between one and two hours home to the

suburb where I lived (where such poverty is practically non-existent). As an outsider I

worried that I might misunderstand and therefore misrepresent Karen women. Again, I

could only take steps to minimise this potential.

7.9.4 Representation and voice

Through my interpretation and writing I hold a position of power and authority. With

this power come several ethical dilemmas including issues of voice and representation

as well as the potential political uses of the research findings and their effect on public

opinion and dialogue. I was acutely aware of the need to take great care. During

analysis and early writing I became concerned that rather than convey participants’

voices, they would rather lose their voice as I presented and then interpreted their

messages, and became the “speaker” of the knowledge gained. As Harrison (2003)

notes:

Knowledge and power are produced through the ethnographer’s methods of

organising and interpreting the data through a metaphorical structure where the

participant’s position is inevitably displaced by that of the ethnographer. (p.

102)

Liamputtong (2010) raises the question of how we can avoid marginalising our

participants in the process of writing. This issue is highly relevant to my research. She

128

suggests staying “closer to their lived experiences and the true meanings of their

experiences” (p. 213).

Research rigour, including member checking, is crucial here but there is more that can

be done. I sought to be accountable and transparent in my writing and therefore

established a community meeting for people to hear and discuss my research findings.

In total approximately 25 people attended the meeting, including community leaders,

service providers and four of the ten women who participated in in-depth individual

interviews. I presented the research findings visually (using PowerPoint) and verbally

(with the assistance of an interpreter) and sought feedback from the audience. I also

provided stamped, self-addressed envelopes so people could provide anonymous

feedback. Finally, copies of the feedback presentation were made available in printed

and electronic format to the audience and other interested people.

The feedback obtained from this meeting was invaluable and provided a firm source of

rigour through member checking (see Section 6.7.4) thereby enhancing the

trustworthiness of this inquiry. At the conclusion of the presentation one community

leader stated that my description of the community and their strengths and weaknesses

“was like looking in a mirror” for him. Others provided anonymous written feedback

stating:

I personally believe that this research provided and explore[d] true accounts of

refugee women’s experiences and their struggle in the third-world country. The

comparison of Western and traditional means of coping with stress and “Tha Ta

Hsar” is valuable for the service providers to adequately help those that are in

need … You’ve mentioned very notable points in your presentation (research)

and I hope that these will one day be put in action. (Community leader –

anonymous written feedback)

I was also concerned with how to manage data that might portray the community in a

negative light and thus effect their reputation. In some ways potential effects are so

varied that they may not be foreseeable. However, researchers have a duty to anticipate

probable misuses and attempt to counteract the possibility of misuse (Liamputtong,

2009). For example, a penetrating analysis of refugee life in Australia may have

implications for public dialogue about asylum seekers, race relations, poverty and

129

politics concerning refugee communities. Again an ethic of care must dictate how

researchers convey their research findings in public media. I kept these issues in mind

when writing and publishing material from the study and did not, for example, call

attention to issues of domestic violence, alcohol or gambling when these matters were

not relevant to the topic being explored.

7.9.5 “Conversations with Karen”: positively using researcher power and the

reflexive journey

Besides memo-writing, I found alternative ways to engage in reflexivity and process my

reaction to some of the sensitive information that participants shared with me. Some

women’s situations seemed quite helpless and I, in turn, experienced feelings of sadness

and frustration. Poetry, writing and art helped me to express my thoughts. I used various

art mediums including felt typography and cross-stitch as depicted in the following

image:

Image 4: Cross-stitch depicting quotation from interview with participant Mr W

During the production of these art works I read Liamputtong (2010) and Hartnett and

Engels (2005) who encourage researchers to use power and representation positively

and to access wider audiences in new and creative ways through forums and mediums

130

such as photography, short stories, poetry, theatre and art (Hartnett & Engels, 2005;

Liamputtong, 2010; L. T. Smith, 1999). I was inspired to hold an art exhibition titled

“Conversations with Karen” to present the works and provide education about the

Karen people and their plight. The exhibition was held during Refugee Week 2011 and

the artworks depicted the struggles and strengths in the lives of Karen refugees. Further

details of the exhibition are presented in Appendix G.

Positive and unforeseen events arose from this exhibition. To an audience of

approximately eighty people, a young Karen woman opened the exhibition and shared

her experience of forced migration. Many in the audience cried, the young girl and her

mother were also moved by the experience and afterward expressed their desire to

engage in further opportunities to educate the Australian public about their lives. Media

advertising for the exhibition led a number of people to contact me and I was able to

create relationships between the Karen community and people interested in helping

them. Profits from the sale of artworks were donated to Karen women who then chose

to purchase materials for a craft group activity.

7.10 Ending the study

During the research I developed many strong and trusted relationships with some

community members. Cutliffe and Ramcharan (2002) discuss the importance of ending

these relationships sensitively, especially when working with vulnerable populations.

Similarly, Patton (2002) recommends developing a disengagement plan. I kept

community members updated about the progress of my research. During feedback

meetings I also explicitly explained that my study was coming to an end. However,

other than no longer collecting data, my social relationships with the Karen community

have not significantly changed. At the time of thesis submission my social involvement

and political advocacy for the Karen community had not ceased though it had shifted

from the many personal visits I undertook during data collection to attending large-scale

community events as well as meetings with community leaders.

131

Chapter 8

“Starting at zero”: Women’s Problems and the Sources of Distress

8.1 Introduction

This chapter presents and discusses some of the findings related to the question: What

issues are perceived to affect distress and wellbeing of Karen refugee women? Multiple

issues over multiple phases of the refugee experience led to emotional distress for Karen

women. Layer-upon-layer of distressing circumstances marked their refugee journey

through pre-migration (flight), refugee camp (limbo) and post-migration resettlement.

Women described their experiences of distress within the daily challenges of

resettlement – adjusting to life Australia and “starting at zero”. This chapter explores

women’s daily stressors, grouped around the key themes of (a) language and

communication problems, (b) lack of environmental mastery, and (c) women’s

challenges. The chapter concludes by drawing together the findings and further

examining how women’s encounters with these daily stressors may be understood

against their broad sociocultural background and through the Karen experience of

persecution.

8.2 “The biggest barrier”: language and communication

I’m telling you … the language barrier is the most, the biggest challenge. (Karen key

informant – Ms S)

Participants unanimously described difficulty with English language proficiency and

communication as the “number one” problem affecting women’s wellbeing in Sydney.

For them, language was a universal barrier that obstructed access to all other domains of

resettlement, generating or exacerbating further resettlement challenges and

psychosocial difficulties. This finding is hardly surprising (Higgins, 2009) but the

compounding factors, additional difficulties generated by it and the significant impact

on emotional wellbeing are important matters. This section addresses practical

challenges as well as the gendered, social and cultural and factors that contribute to this

issue. These issues (a) contextualise language problems within the unique sociocultural

position of the Karen, and (b) suggest possible areas for change within service provision

and policy.

132

8.2.1 Barriers to language education

Australia’s AMEP (see Section 4.4) provides eligible migrants and refugees with 510

hours of English language training. However, many women experienced multiple pre-

and post-migration barriers affecting their ability to access and benefit from AMEP

classes.

Pre-migration barriers

Many women had not received formal education in Burma and were unable to read or

write in their native tongue. Their pre-migration education opportunities had been

limited by the sociopolitical situation as well as gender role beliefs and practices

locating women’s roles within the home:

In our culture the woman looks after the kids and stays at home and because of that

you don’t have a chance to learn English … Most of the women have lived more than

ten years in the refugee camp and before we came to the camps we have been running

around in the jungle moving here and running there. Many women have not even had

a chance to learn to read and write Karen. (Key informant – Ms S)

Lack of pre-migration opportunities for education and literacy affects people’s ability to

learn English (Hou & Beiser, 2006; van Tubergen & Kalmijn, 2009) and adapt to the

Australian education system. Low literacy and education meant many Karen women

were unfamiliar with holding pens and unable to utilise Karen-English / English-Karen

dictionaries to assist with communication. These limitations hindered their ability to

succeed within the AMEP. Many also struggled to adapt to an alien and formal

education experience in an unfamiliar environment (e.g., classroom), requiring an

unfamiliar sitting position (e.g., sitting on chairs rather than on the floor) and interacting

with unfamiliar persons (e.g., teachers and fellow students). These issues are common

to refugee students with low literacy (Adult Migrant English Program Research Centre,

2006).

133

Post-migration barriers5

Gendered barriers continued to affect women’s access to education after arriving in

Australia. Women struggled to attend to their own education needs whilst caring for

children, acting as full-time carers for elderly parents or in-laws, as well as managing

household finances, groceries and maintenance. Typical of women’s comments, Htòh

remarked:

I already completed 510 hours [at AMEP], now I need to look after my father-in-law.

He is elderly and can’t stay at home alone, he has medical problems. (Htòh)

Additional cultural factors further encumbered women’s ability to benefit from English

language education. English language teachers described their Karen students as

“polite” and “compliant”, and noted that they expressed great respect for authority.

However, these cultural traits and social interaction patterns also impeded women’s

education experiences and led to behaviour that did not foster learning. For example,

women’s respect for teachers, and anxieties about failure and authority figures, led

some of them to avoid class or copy homework when they thought they were not

succeeding or believed they had not adequately completed their assignment. Women

also appeared embarrassed to practise their English and key informants spoke of the

“shame” of speaking English inadequately. During ethnographic observation I

repeatedly encountered how these factors impeded their development of English

language skills. For example, I often experienced great difficulty building the

confidence and trust of Karen women to communicate or practise their spoken English

with me. Women’s shame may also be understood through Lebra’s (2010)

conceptualisation of the role of self, shame and embarrassment in collectivist societies.

Lebra argued that in collectivist societies the ideal self remains inconspicuous.

Consequently, when one is focused upon – such as when getting feedback from a

teacher or practising English with a native speaker – one feels (and is expected to

convey) embarrassment or shyness.

Teachers, particularly those who appeared to lack sufficient understanding of Karen

culture, became frustrated with their Karen students’ avoidant and unassertive

5 The findings also brought to light service provider limitations and barriers. These and other issues

related to service provisions are explored in Chapter 12.

134

behaviour. The following quotation from an interview with an exasperated teacher

reflects these challenges:

Sometimes I think they are on a different planet, not with the classroom … They don’t

want to participate in group activities [with students from different cultural

backgrounds] … Sometimes I think I don’t want to waste my time or their time doing

this work because they are not going to be happy. (Key informant / teacher – Ms M)

Hofstede (2001) notes that such complaints are common of teachers from individualist

cultures who work with students from collectivist cultural contexts. Karen participants

further contextualised their social anxiety and lack of assertiveness within experiences

of (a) persecution, (b) protracted camp confinement eroding opportunities for

interaction with a broader world, and (c) gender roles limiting women’s opportunities

for interaction beyond the home environment. The latter is depicted in the following

quotation:

They are scared of people, how to talk to them, how to deal with them. Because some

women have been living in the kitchen for a long time they have no confidence in

dealing with people outside. (Mu Htòh)

Finally, for many women, access to (and interactions with) AMEP services were further

limited by age, physical illness and disease. For example, older participants were

unfamiliar with classroom chairs and described experiencing physical discomfort. Key

informants reported that some parents and older refugees resigned themselves to

minimal or mediocre levels of educational and occupational achievement but

encouraged and expected their children to forge ahead in these domains. These findings

resonate with research that documents high unemployment and low education amongst

first generation humanitarian entrants, but higher than national average levels of tertiary

education and employment amongst the second generation (DIAC, 2011a).

8.2.2 Barriers to communication

Language and communication problems complicated resettlement through exacerbating

women’s difficulties navigating unfamiliar and complex social, legal, educational and

governmental systems and structures. Consequently, women felt confused, stressed,

isolated and helpless to the point that some, like Ta Heh Sōo, wanted to “go back to the

refugee camp” because they “can’t communicate” or “do anything” in Australia.

135

Written correspondence from service providers was problematic for women, as most

were unable to read English adequately and consequently relied on translation help from

children, friends and community leaders. Staff at refugee support services also

frequently assisted with written correspondence, especially in situations that had

escalated into late payment fees and legal problems. A few women were proactive and

self-sufficient in resolving written communication challenges and used English-Karen

dictionaries to decipher letters from service providers. These women had received some

pre-migration education, were able to read Karen and were consequently more

confident about their English language skills.

Some social services depended heavily on telephone communication but did not use

interpreters. Women struggled to communicate with English speakers and often hung up

or relied on their children to interpret. In addition, the telephone was an unfamiliar

object of daily use for many women, especially older women whose lives had been

primarily village and refugee camp-based. Some service providers sought to

communicate with Karen women through interpreters but were often unsuccessful in

securing one due to unavailability. When attending services, Karen women were also

often assigned an interpreter who did not speak their language (e.g., a speaker of

Burmese). Moreover, women’s cultural communication patterns, shyness and mistrust

further obstructed face-to-face encounters with service providers and interpreters. These

factors compounded language and communication problems and were a source of stress

for women. This raises the issues of service provider oversight and limitations (see

Chapter 12).

8.3 “Everything is new”: lack of environmental mastery

For Karen women “everything is new” in Australia, the physical environment, the

culture, as well as the political and legal system. Not having implicit knowledge of the

Australian way of life, these women faced enormous challenges and “culture shock”, as

vividly portrayed by a key informant:

They came from the villages, get displaced, along the line they have a series of losses,

loss of husband, there are a lot of stories like that, people killed by landmines. They

lose children through malaria infection and having to wait in the refugee camps. Some

people spent about ten years in uncertainty not knowing where to go … During the

refugee period there is a lot of anxiety. And they are coming here unprepared. That’s

136

what I find. Vast differences between the cultures. Culture shock is there, more so

than for some other groups. Because they come here really, really unprepared, not

informed of the legal system, welfare system, medical system, service delivery, their

rights, you know, all those things. They end up having to fill out a lot of forms,

Centrelink, housing, imagine all those things. Because of the knowledge gap, they

don’t speak the language. So they struggle, they struggle … So, looking at the

problems they’re facing here I will look at a very obvious area – the vast difference

between their life over there and getting resettled in a new country like metropolitan,

sophisticated, multicultural society like Australia with its complexities. (Key

informant – Dr F)

Women’s accounts confirmed that they struggled to master life in Australia. Besides

language and communication difficulties, they reported being most significantly

distressed by problems with (un)employment, transport, poverty and parenting in a new

cultural context. Their resettlement challenges seem to arise from what has been

described in the literature as “lack of environmental mastery” (Khawaja, White,

Schweitzer, & Greenslade, 2008, p. 493). In employing this phrase I wish to locate the

problems experienced within the Australian context and not within Karen individuals or

communities who may indeed lack mastery of life in metropolitan Sydney, but who

unquestionably mastered the skills necessary for survival in Burma and in refugee

camps.

8.3.1 Mastering space: fear of freedom

I think giving them the freedom to move is also restricting them and making them feel

insecure as well. (Karen key informant – Ms K)

Many women lacked environmental mastery of functioning in sizeable physical space

and experienced anxiety about (a) their distance from other Karen community members,

and (b) their “freedom to move”. Separation and distance from other Karen community

members greatly increased the distress experienced by new arrivals. Upon arrival in

Australia some women were sent to temporary accommodation in Mount Druitt –

approximately 30 kilometres away from the Sydney Karen community hub around

Guildford, Yennora and Fairfield. In another instance, two single mothers travelled to

Australia together but were then placed in separate accommodation suburbs apart. The

women had hoped to support one another through resettlement and described fearing for

137

their safety and security when they were separated. In the following quotation, a Karen

key informant, Ms S, employs a Karen proverb to emphasise the importance of close

physical proximity to the Karen community:

You know, in Karen language, we say “You never live alone because if the enemy

wants to attack you they can easily swallow you, but if you stay together, no one can

come and destroy you”. (Key informant – Ms S)

This proverb contextualises the experience of separation and distress within a

sociopolitical history characterised by continual persecution. Many women also

struggled to adapt to their newfound freedom and had difficulty adjusting to an

unrestricted and unfamiliar physical environment. This struggle is clearly reflected in

the following quotation from an interview with Ms K, a Karen key informant:

Here it’s like open space. You don’t know who your neighbour is. You don’t know

whether someone will help you or not if something happens, how can you find

support? … Even though in the camp you don’t have a lot of space, you are cramped

in together but you still feel that your neighbour next door is there and they will hear

you [if you need help]. (Key informant – Ms K)

As Ms K suggested, these issues must be contextualised within women’s refugee

experiences of conflict and restrictive camp environments. During conflict, being

caught in an unfamiliar environment could be a dangerous situation. As we saw in

Chapter 3, being caught outside a refugee camp in Thailand could be equally dangerous.

After years or even decades in camps with up to 37,000 people restricted to a maximum

land-space of 4km2 (Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2010b), unfamiliar and open

space in Australia appeared to provoke what I call a “fear of freedom”. Interestingly,

when I used this expression with Karen women they enthusiastically concurred with my

understanding and formulation of the phrase. Many women also expressed a desire to

overcome this fear. For example, during a group interview, women reported that they

were aware they were living within their “comfort zone” and desired to “get to know

what is happening in the outside world”. They explained that their children were able to

have contact with the outside world and interact with Australians of various ethnic

backgrounds at school but lamented that their own lives were more limited.

138

8.3.2 (Un)employment

Overwhelmingly Karen women wanted and felt obliged to work but were held back by

the language barrier and their lack of work experience. This is clearly reflected in

Htòh’s words:

We need to work but on the other hand we can’t speak, we can’t communicate with

others – how can we get a job? Since we came here the government has looked after

us very well. We know we have to work, we have to do something to give back to the

government, but we can’t speak or communicate with others, this is the problem.

Language is not learned overnight. (Htòh)

The language barrier also affected women’s ability to interact effectively with staff at

Job Services Australia (the network of organisations contracted by the Department of

Education, Employment and Workplace Relations to help jobseekers find employment).

The following quotation describes a typical encounter with Job Services Australia and

the emotional impact on Karen women:

Every day [the Job Services Australia staff] provided me with the newspaper

advertisement and I had to call up for any job I could see. I tried, with whatever

knowledge I had, but when people answered I couldn’t really understand what they

were saying and when I asked about the job, they said “No job available”. One day

one of the managers asked me, “Are you sure you really looked for a job?” and I said

“Yes, here are all the numbers I called”. Then he called the numbers to check whether

I told him the truth and yes, it was true. Then he told me “You know, my mother

couldn’t speak much English when she came here, she couldn’t really speak at all, but

she was able to get a job, how come you can’t get a job?” I was so upset and hurt by

this. (Htee Gloh)

Htee Gloh requested help of a counselling service and a staff member there

communicated with Job Services Australia on her behalf. However, later on, she and her

husband chose to forgo receiving financial assistance to which they were entitled

through Centrelink in order to avoid further interactions with Job Services Australia. As

a result, their family of four relied on the limited and sporadic work income of Htee

Gloh’s husband to support the household.

For many women, negative interactions with Job Services Australia staff members

compounded the emotional impact of repeated failures at attempts to gain employment.

139

Key informants, when asked about this issue, tried to balance the perspectives of both

sides. For example, Mr P, a Karen key informant reported:

I have been an interpreter at those interviews, [the Job Services Australia staff

members are] not too friendly but they are doing their job, their duty … that’s their

obligation. (Key informant – Mr P)

Community leaders also identified Karen women’s lack of experience with the demands

of a competitive individualistic employment environment as a problem. Moreover, like

other refugee communities in Australia (Colic-Peisker & Tilbury, 2007; Refugee

Council of Australia, 2010), they faced obstacles having their pre-migration

qualifications and skills recognised. Most lacked official documents evidencing their

qualifications. Key informants explained that qualified community members were

generally unable to afford the cost, and time, of university bridging courses whilst also

needing to support their families. Consequently, and consistent with a recent Australian

report (DIAC, 2011a), there were unemployed and under-employed nurses, doctors,

teachers, musicians and engineers within the community.

In a group interview Karen women visually depicted their feelings in relation to job

seeking and interacting with staff at Job Services Australia (see Image 5). The image

clearly reflected the immense emotional impact of (un)employment related difficulties.

140

Image 5: Karen women’s visual depiction of the emotional impact of (un)employment

and interactions with Job Services Australia

The following explanation, offered by a speaker for the group, calls attention to both the

individual and collective impact of women’s (un)employment-related stress:

This picture is about individual stress. You came here and you need to look for a job

but you can’t get it because you come from an uneducated background. And you have

to attend a meeting [with Job Services Australia] every fortnight. You feel stressed.

You feel pressured. Some feel traumatised by this. It also impacts on the family, on

the children because the parents have to attend all these appointments [with Job

Services Australia]. It’s not only one person’s problem, it becomes the whole family’s

problem. The whole family becomes unhappy. Also, it puts pressure on the

community leaders because they need to help you to find a job … The colour

represents the sun it is very hot – it’s about heat. And not one sun – three suns! You

141

feel pressure, sometimes you not only feel numb, you feel blank – although someone

is talking to you, you don’t listen, you just feel blank. (Participant in Group D)

Participants’ accounts of (un)employment-related problems were marked by fear,

failure and frustration. Women feared the consequences of unemployment and worried

“what will happen if we can’t get a job?”. Their stress was so great that some wanted to

“return to living in the refugee camp”. Again, key informants conceptualised women’s

fear within cultural attitudes towards authority as well as the experience of persecution.

Furthermore, women’s employment-related fear and stress was also conceptualised

within gender role practices that limited women’s opportunities for work and

interactions outside the home. Consequently, women lacked experience interacting with

officials or government agencies.

8.3.3 Financial hardship

What we get in income and our expenses becomes equal, so no extra money. (Mëh

Luh)

Financial hardship and lack of knowledge about financial systems was a major source of

stress for Karen women. Women’s accounts of their financial problems were related to

language and communication problems, their husband’s behaviours and their concern

for family left behind.

Like most stressors, financial difficulties were related to language and communication

problems as well as lack of experience. With few banks and virtually no automatic teller

machines in rural Burma (and certainly none inside refugee camps), many Karen

women had never used these kinds of financial systems. Their unfamiliarity led to

confusion and presented practical problems such as difficulty accessing money. For

example, Ms S, a Karen key informant reported: “they put it in the bank but they don’t

know how to take out the money”. Similarly, Pàw Lèh explained: “we don’t know our

PIN. In the bank there are withdrawals, enquiries and balances – we don’t know which

one to press”. Women were also stressed as a result of their unfamiliarity with

managing a budget and paying for services such as electricity, water and telephones.

Basic budgeting tips and example budgeting forms were available from HSS case

managers in addition to services and one-off seminars arranged by various other service

142

providers such as Migrant Resource Centres. However, many women desired to learn

more about financial matters.

Participants described struggling to afford their basic needs including housing, food and

electricity, as well as paying for children’s school books. Most reported that if they

“planned well” they were able to adequately feed their families. Echoing findings from

a food security study of refugees in Australia (Gallegos, Ellies, & Wright, 2008), others

raised food security concerns and reported that they sometimes lacked enough food to

feed their family. During the course of this study, the Karen community were provided

with a community garden plot through Haven Australia – a consortium of refugee and

other social service organisations (Cachia, Zu, Chahola, & Meale, 2010) – but no

participants reported having accessed food through this channel.

Several women reported their financial hardships were worsened by their husbands’

alcohol and gambling problems. These findings add to literature documenting

disordered gambling amongst resettled refugees from South East Asia (Centre for

Culture, Ethnicity and Health, 2008; G. N. Marshall, Schell, Elliott, Berthold, & Chun,

2005; Petry, Armentano, Kuoch, Norinth, & Smith, 2003). Women attempted to assert

their family’s financial rights and developed strategies to control the budget (including

hiding credit cards). However, they inevitably felt unable to address their problems. Key

informants and service providers were also aware of gambling problems within the

community but noted that it was largely a hidden problem. They expressed concern that

Karen men were being introduced to poker machines and beginning to gamble at

younger ages.

Furthering their financial difficulties, women described regularly sending money to

friends and family left behind in Burma and in the refugee camps. Homeland remittance

is common amongst refugee communities (Refugee Council of Australia, 2010) and,

according to the World Bank (2011), outward remittances from Australia totalled US$3

billion in 2009. Brees (2010) has described this practice as one way resettled refugees

can meet their expected obligations within a collectivist culture. Similarly, in the

following interview quotation, Ms L, a Karen key informants, describes the cultural

context surrounding homeland remittance amongst the Karen:

143

We are always sending financial [support] back home, there is never enough money

… With the [Australian] government, [family means] immediate family, like your son.

But with us, [family means] extended family … Everybody that comes here is using

some of their income to send money back there … For them to have their mental

balance, they need to know that their family members back home are okay. So the way

to address that is to send money, but it makes a big financial hole here when most of

them are in debt. (Key informant – Ms L)

Concern for loved ones left behind in the homeland is commonly associated with

psychological distress including stress and depression amongst refugees (Atwell,

Gifford, & McDonald-Wilmsen, 2009; Schweitzer, Melville, Steel, & Lacherez, 2006).

In this study Karen women desired to help others but also described feeling guilty if

they did not share their relative good fortune with their extended family. It appeared that

most were more distressed and concerned for those left behind than they were by their

own financial hardship and poverty in Australia.

Women developed strategies to survive and described borrowing money from friends to

pay bills, as well as seeking assistance from service providers such as Migrant Resource

Centres. However, they also spoke of the shame of poverty and expressed their unease

and reluctance to burden others with their difficulties.

8.3.4 Housing

Why does Australia continue to take refugees if there is not enough housing and jobs

available? (Karen key informant – Ms A)

Women described housing and tenancy problems as a significant source of stress. Like

other refugee communities in Australia (DIAC, 2011q; Ernst & Young, 2011; Higgins,

2009), they experienced problems with (a) availability of affordable and appropriate

housing, (b) increasing rental costs, (c) difficult landlord-tenant relationships including

landlords failing to meet necessary home maintenance obligations, and (d) poor

understanding of rights, responsibilities and laws in relation to tenancy and debt,

resulting in worry and fear of persecution. A further issue in relation to housing was the

Karens’ wish to live in close proximity to other Karen families (see Section 8.3.1).

The scarcity of appropriate and affordable housing resulted in Karen refugees accepting

unsuitable (e.g., barely affordable, old and dirty) rental properties and frequent property

144

shifts, which often entailed further challenges such as children’s school changes.

Participant observation and interviews revealed women living in homes without

adequate insulation or heating and with holes in walls. Some participants attributed their

health problems (e.g., colds and headaches) to living in unsuitable, old and damp

homes.

Housing concerns impacted women’s mental or emotional wellbeing in multiple ways.

Most women feared homelessness and felt uncertain about their future ability to afford

housing. Their lack of environmental mastery in relation to language, tenancy laws and

practices also resulted in worry and fear of persecution. This scenario is conveyed in the

following quotation from an interview with a young Karen woman:

The number one problem is that you can’t communicate – you can’t discuss issues

with your landlord. So you might be scared that this landlord might come back

anytime to kill you or might hire someone to kill you. Even when you walk on the

street you might think, oh, my landlord might hire someone to attack me. (Participant

in Group B)

This quotation captures an extreme example of fear that can be contextualised within (a)

the broader problems of the language barrier and associated communication difficulties,

(b) culture shock and lack of knowledge with complex new social and legal systems,

and (c) fear of authority caused by pre-migration experiences. Accommodation

difficulties, like other social problems, created a reliance and burden upon community

leaders for advocacy and practical assistance. During the course of this research Karen

community leaders formed the Karen Community Housing Cooperative Limited (2011)

to address some of these issues and within a year ninety-five families had registered to

receive assistance.

No hot water in the jungle

Women also reported experiencing difficulties with housekeeping and managing the

family home – gender role responsibilities that they retained in the Australian context

(see Section 8.4.1). Many housekeeping problems were entwined with the language

barrier. For example, women were unable to read product labels or instructions and felt

burdened by the task of communicating with shopkeepers. In addition, most women

were unfamiliar with home appliances and other technology and had difficulty using

145

household items. The following quotation from an interview with Ms C, a Karen key

informant and service provider, describes some of the many new household items and

practices women encountered in Australia:

We [HSS case managers] teach them how to use and eat things … They are provided

with some rice but we have to teach them how to cook it here, it’s a different way …

We have to teach them how to use cleaning products like floor cleaning, glass

cleaning, toilet and dishwashing. Because in the camp they didn’t have to take care of

this. We have to teach them how to use the water. Here they have cold and hot water –

in the jungle no hot water. (Key informant – Ms C)

8.3.5 Transport

Transport problems were another source of stress for Karen women. Their transport

difficulties were largely underpinned by factors already identified in this chapter, most

notably language and communication limitations, lack of familiarity or environmental

mastery, as well as shyness and fear of freedom. For example, women had difficulty

reading and understanding transport timetables as well as communicating with public

transport staff. They were also worried about getting lost in an unfamiliar environment.

Service providers also bemoaned the physical distance between the community and

some service provider facilities. They reported that transport problems hindered

community involvement with programs and interventions.

HSS case managers demonstrated the use of public transport during refugees’ first few

months of resettlement but many women still lacked the skills and confidence to do so

on their own. It is possible that newly arrived refugee groups such as the Karen could

experience anxiety and difficulty in relation to transport and travel due to a different

spatial ontology. Cognitive conceptualisations of route directions are variable (Richter

& Klippel, 2007). According to Hallowell (2010), one’s “spatiotemporal frame of

reference” (p. 35) is oriented by culture. It seems conceivable that Karen women from

rural Burma might hold cognitive spatial maps vastly different from those employed by

urban Australians.

A minority of women proactively addressed transport problems. These women sought

opportunities to practise using the public transport system and overcome their worries

about communication and getting lost. For example, Htee Gloh Poh reported:

146

For us it is fine to travel by bus and train. Anywhere within Sydney, I can go

anywhere by train! I try to go everywhere so that I can know places. When people go

to the hospital or something by train I just go with them because I want to know how

to go everywhere by public transport. (Htee Gloh Poh)

Women like Htee Gloh Poh appeared to experience a sense of mastery from their

achievements and other women often requested their assistance. These women were

lucky to have had pre-migration literacy which had enabled them to make use of maps

and take detailed notes when travelling with their HSS case managers. In contrast,

others felt that they required greater support from HSS case managers to learn the skills

necessary for using public transport.

8.3.6 Health

Like other refugee communities (Higgins, 2009; A. Wilson, Renzaho, McCabe, &

Swinburn, 2010), the Karen were troubled by health problems particularly chronic

diseases such as diabetes and high cholesterol. Communication problems and lack of

familiarity with medical services and treatments compounded their health-related

difficulties and women expressed a desire to learn more about preventing and managing

illness and disease in the Australian context. Karen women did not appear to utilise the

free New South Wales Refugee Health Service general practitioner (GP) clinics as, at

the time of this research, refugee women from Burma only comprised between 1.1%

and 2.5% of patients attending the various clinic locations (H. Corner, personal

communication, 13 January, 2010). However, they did attend and actively participate in

health information sessions specifically created for them. For example, during the

course of research (February 2010) I was a participant observer during a sexual health

workshop co-hosted by the New South Wales Refugee Health Service and the Hills

Holroyd Parramatta Migrant Resource Centre, and facilitated by a staff member from

the Family Planning Association (assisted by an interpreter).

8.3.7 Parenting

All Karen participants spoke of the tremendous difficulty they experienced parenting in

the Australian context. Like other stressors explored in this chapter, women’s parenting

difficulties are shared by other refugee communities in Australia (Lewig, Arney, &

147

Salveron, 2010) and were entwined with language and communication barriers, as well

as cultural issues.

The language barrier meant parents were unable to assist and monitor their children’s

progress at school, resulting in women feeling “frustrated” and “useless”. Women also

felt their authority slip away due to their growing dependence on their children and

often relied upon them to interpret at health and social service appointments. Some

children intentionally manipulated their parents’ dependency and, for example,

misinformed organisations (including schools) who attempted to contact their non-

English-speaking parents by phone. Equally, parental dependence was also detrimental

to children who occasionally missed school to accompany their parents to appointments.

Key informants prioritised parenting problems as central to Karen women’s distress.

The following quotation from an interview with Dr F, a key informant, illustrates

clearly the psychological implications of parenting problems, including feeling useless

and loss of identity:

Even though she was illiterate in Thailand she could look after [her children] and

manage her own money. Here the children learnt English very quickly. Now the

money is controlled by the son because she doesn’t know how to go to the bank, use

credit cards, withdraw the money and things like that. Her 18-year-old son is not too

bad but he does not explain things to her in a compassionate way. Her complaint is

that she feels useless. She doesn’t even know how much money she’s got. She’s got a

benefit – she knows that. The son will give her the money for the day. She doesn’t

trust the son, so that’s a problem. A social worker should be involved and she should

be given information, training. I tried to empower her in that area … [She had a] loss

of identity, needed to be empowered as a mother. (Key informant – Dr F)

Most Karen women came from collectivist, village-based communities where they had

enjoyed the benefits of collectivist childcare (whereby their children were watched and

cared for by numerous adults and older children) (see Section 3.3.2) and were

unfamiliar with monitoring the activities of all of their children at any one time. In

Australia they reported struggling to understand and follow vastly different childcare

practices and laws. In one instance, child protection services were notified when a

Karen child was injured whilst cooking unsupervised at home. The mother had been

unaware of her legal parenting responsibilities in Australia. In the refugee camp her

148

children were frequently unsupervised and capable of cooking. Community Services

was the local government agency responsible for addressing the safety and wellbeing of

children. According to their website, they provided information about childcare and

explained Australian rules regarding child abuse in seventeen languages excluding

Burmese and Karen (New South Wales Department of Community Services, n.d.).

Karen cultural approaches to parenting were further challenged by older children who

were quick to adopt some Australian cultural values and attitudes leading to cultural

generation gaps and family tension. Intergenerational conflict due to mismatched

expectations and cultural dissonance has been frequently identified in migrant and

refugee communities (McMichael, Gifford, & Correa-Velez, 2011; Merali, 2004; P. V.

Nguyen, 2008). In Western society, adolescent challenges against parental authority are

viewed as a normative aspect of development (Allison, 2000; Bongers, Koot, van der

Ende, & Verhust, 2003). However, within their Karen community, such behaviour was

deemed unacceptable. Karen parents frequently lamented that their children were

abusing their new-found freedom and safety in Australia:

The teenagers are another problem … they see freedom. I see it like a freedom of

speech, a freedom of the mind. They see it as a freedom to do anything you want to do

– mess yourself. (Key informant – Ms L)

These findings parallel those of Atwell and colleagues (2009) who conducted

qualitative interviews with refugee families in Melbourne, Australia. Parents in Atwell

et al.’s study spoke of young people misunderstanding the meaning of freedom and

using the term to excuse themselves of family and cultural expectations and behave

improperly. In my study, parents and community leaders feared that Karen children

might neglect their cultural values, language and family responsibilities. As reflected in

the quotation below, their concerns also reflected differences between individualistic

and collectivist cultural values (Hofstede, 2001):

My concern is that they might follow the world, that is my concern. When I say the

world, [I mean] this world based on self. All about self, self, self, self, self, self. And

other things happen because of this self. This is my concern, their spirituality … It’s a

part of individualism: “I don’t care, I have a right to do”, that sort of thing – self. For

example, back home, whatever we do we think “I might make my family lose face, or

my nation, or my tribe”. We have that kind of etiquette. (Key informant – Mr M)

149

Mothers also expressed astonishment about the sexual health education provided at

Australian schools:

When they are in high school they teach them about sexual relationships … They

come home and say they have a project to do about this, as a mother we feel shy … At

their age we never knew [about that]. Some people in Burma graduate from university

without knowing about this. Asians are very private about this. I said “How come the

school taught you all this?” … Is it part of syllabus? (Htee Gloh Poh)

Karen women’s concerns about sex education reflected in the above quotation signalled

their own pre-migration experiences and cultural beliefs. Their concerns are

contextualised by a recent study of Karen women’s reproductive health experiences.

Ussher et al. (2011) interviewed Karen women in Sydney and found that the majority

“had no knowledge about coital sex before their wedding night” (p. 6).

Karen adults were also concerned about the myriad of distractions and “immoral” “un-

Christian” temptations (such as alcohol, gambling and media laden with sexual and

violent imagery) available to children in Australia’s technologically advanced multi-

media culture. Parents also appeared to lack the necessary experience, skills and

confidence to effectively negotiate with their children in relation to new activities and

distractions such as television, mobile phones and money. Moreover, poverty and

welfare-dependency created circumstances where parents had little financial power over

their adolescent children. In some families children demanded full access to their

portion of the family Centrelink allowance. Mothers were unsure about their rights and

responsibilities in relation to their children’s allowances and described feeling “useless”

as parents; some even regretted bringing their children to Australia.

Women stated that their parenting problems were compounded by Australian laws and

“children’s rights”, which they perceived were opposed to their parental authority. They

explained that in Burma absolute obedience and respect for all elders was expected and

enforced. Talking back or arguing with parents or other adults was not tolerated.

Physical force was not condoned as a first solution to children’s disobedience but was

used if deemed necessary, with no social or legal ramifications for parents. Many

parents lamented Australian children’s rights and legal restrictions against physical

punishment and felt unable to discipline their children effectively. Parents’ problems

were compounded by their lack of knowledge about Australian parenting laws. Most

150

were frustrated, confused and nervous about parenting in this context and struggled to

access parenting information and advice. In contrast, their children were given

information about children’s rights at school. As a result, some children confidently

asserted their rights to their bemused and confused parents, resulting in family conflict

and occasionally intervention from the police and justice system. The following

quotation from an interview with a Karen key informant highlights this troubling

scenario:

[Karen mothers occasionally smack or hit their children] … Initially the children will

not inform the police but when they grow up – maybe twelve, thirteen or fourteen –

they challenge: “Mum, don’t do that again, I will call 000, you will be in trouble” …

Burmese and Karen mothers give excuses like “I had you, I look after you, I take care

of you, why are you challenging me? I got a lot of worries about you because of your

attitude and your behaviour. Your response is quite rude. I’m a responsible person, a

responsible mother to teach you not to do that. That’s the reason I beat you. I will beat

you again if you call the police. I will go to the police station, I can go to the court, I

can go to the jail. It doesn’t matter. Mothers never surrender, never give up”. (Key

informant – Mr J)

8.4 “Power struggles”: gender role changes and women’s wellbeing

Participants explained that new gendered opportunities in Australia created (a) tension

within women (and men) themselves as they struggled with their options,

responsibilities and decisions, and (b) “power struggles” within couples, families and

communities as they adapted to Australian gender role beliefs and practices. Most

women (and men) struggled to strike a delicate balance between Karen and Australian

cultural values and, in some cases, gender role conflicts led to domestic violence.

Most Karen women did not complain about gender politics or the status of women

within their community. However, a number of key informants, including Karen men

and women, raised this topic and reported that Karen women were subjugated within a

patriarchal social structure. For example, Ms R, a Karen key informant, reported that

some Karen men “speak softly and they say they love their women but they tell them

what to do and don’t let them make decisions”. Ms R recalled an instance in a refugee

camp in which Karen elders (all men) complained about humanitarian organisations

who were providing information about “women’s rights”. The elders argued that such

151

education dishonoured traditional customs. Ms R believed that such patriarchal attitudes

continued (though to a lesser degree) within the Karen community in Australia. She

reported that despite the presence of women’s groups and organisations, community-

level decision-making remained largely controlled by a small group of men. As an

outspoken, educated and political woman Ms R represented a minority of the Karen

women interviewed. Most participants felt that their rights as women were well

respected in Australia. However, a number of other Karen key informants corroborated

Ms R’s perspective and my ethnographic observations supported her views regarding

community decision-making. For example, I dealt almost exclusively with Karen men

when formally meeting with the community leaders whilst setting up this study.

In this discussion of gender relations and hierarchies I am particularly aware of walking

in a cross-cultural borderland. It should be borne in mind that I do not see Karen

women’s position “as a unidirectional, linear process, from ‘traditional’ women who

lack ‘freedom and control’ to ‘modern, autonomous women”(Mumtaz & Salway, 2009,

p. 1354). Rather, I have aimed to present and explore the complexities of Karen

women’s gendered lives and the post-migration challenges of renegotiating gender

roles.

8.4.1 “Too much responsibility”: increasing pressures on women

Many women were positive about their new opportunities for education and

employment in Australia, while others met them with a certain bemusement. However,

like other resettled refugee women (Kaplan & Webster, 2003; Tankink & Richters,

2007), most lamented that their gender role demands had increased post-migration

where, in addition to their new roles and demands outside of the home, they continued

their traditional roles as housekeepers and carers. Like Htee Gloh, many reported

feeling stressed and upset by these pressures:

You must study full time, five days per week from nine until two, as well as show Job

Services Australia that you are looking for a job. You also need to look after the

children and do the housework. It’s too much pressure, I even cried, I can’t take it. It’s

too much for me. (Htee Gloh)

In the face of increasing responsibilities some women also described feeling

unappreciated by their families. For single mothers, the daily stressors and parental

152

difficulties described in this chapter were compounded. As Hpä Hpaw Mu exclaimed:

“you have to do everything by yourself”. In addition, single mothers described their

children’s behaviour and obedience as worsening in the absence of a father figure. Their

adolescent children no longer listened to them and mothers described feeling “useless”.

Women also faced additional gender role pressures from the Karen community.

Participants reported that some of the older women disparaged the Australian cultural

way in which they saw “women competing with men”. Younger women who embraced

Australian cultural values and stepped outside traditional boundaries were sometimes

met with gossip and scorn from other community members.

8.4.2 “Not enough responsibility”: men’s experience of gender role changes

Participants explained that Karen men struggled with a loss of identity as a result of

gender role changes in Australia and the resulting power struggle was viewed as a key

cause of domestic violence:

The interesting thing is, for the girls, once they arrive here, most experience this

complete role reversal in their marriage and relationship. Back home in Burma, most

girls are expected to have a housewife role. But here, the circumstances push you to

go out, learn and earn. Their confidence level somehow gets higher and higher. It’s

really encouraging and that’s the trend we are seeing. At the same time, marital

discord and domestic violence rates are on the rise … It has a huge impact on some

families. We have domestic violence cases because of this role reversal. The girls

become more assertive and more confident. So in order to hold onto their ego and to

exercise power and control, some men start to react violently and use physical force.

(Key informant – Dr V)

A Karen man concurred with Dr V:

When they come here the language changes, the world view changes, the women here

have more rights so that’s different to the [Karen] man’s view of the world … When

the man is feeling like his identity is affected then his anger is difficult to control.

(Key informant – Mr H)

These findings parallel those of Tankink and Richters (2007) who found that post-

migration changes to the gender roles amongst Dinka refugees in the Netherlands led to

domestic violence. Karen key informants also traced men’s loss of responsibility and

153

identity to pre-migration experiences and protracted camp confinement. Protecting and

providing for his family and community had marked a Karen man’s role. For many

men, their experiences were characterised by repeated failures in fulfilling this identity

role. They may have failed to protect their land, livestock and village. They may have

failed to protect their wives, sisters, mothers and daughters from rape. In the camps

many had failed again and become dependent on camp authorities. In contrast, it

appeared that within the limitations and conditions of the camps, women were still able

to be relatively effectual mothers and homemakers to the extent that their usefulness and

gender role identity had remained somewhat intact.

8.4.3 Domestic violence

Many Karen participants appeared uncomfortable discussing the topic of domestic

violence. This is not surprising for many reasons including (a) the sensitive nature of the

topic, (b) cultural expectations and customs, (c) mistrust, (d) perceived authority and

power imbalance during interviews, and (d) concern for community credibility.

Consequently, it was difficult to get first-hand accounts of domestic violence.

All Karen women interviewed denied first-hand experience of domestic violence in

Australia (one spoke of experiencing domestic violence in Burma and Thailand) and yet

most reported that it was an issue for “other women” in their community. According to

key informants, instances of domestic violence within the Karen community in

Australia included forced isolation, restricted access to money, verbal abuse, threats of

violence (e.g., “threatening, I will kill you”) and physical assault resulting in broken

bones. Though by far most instances of domestic violence described by participants

involved male perpetrators, one instance of a female perpetrator was also reported.

One Karen woman, who was also a key informant, readily described various forms of

gender-based violence within the Karen community but emphasised that non-physical

forms (e.g., men controlling finances and decision-making) were more common. She

also reported that Karen women were socialised to believe that a “good Christian wife”

must “never refuse” her husband’s sexual advances. These reports are corroborated by

the findings of the recent reproductive health study conducted with Karen women in

Sydney. The researchers reported that Karen women said they were able to refuse sex

154

but also that they were “expected” to have sex when their husbands wanted to (Ussher

et al., 2011, p. 7).

Some women and service providers denied or minimised domestic violence issues. In

the following interview quotation Ms S, a Karen key informant, confirmed the presence

of domestic violence in the refugee camps but denied that it occurred in Australia. I was

later told that Ms S was herself romantically involved with an abusive partner.

There is no violence. And especially I heard that here men cannot hit the women and

you cannot slap or hit or torture your wife. But there [in Burma], I tell you, men can

be the king in the house and they can abuse the wife, the children, whatever –

especially when they’re drunk. But when they come to Australia, they’ve been told

that it’s unacceptable and they don’t do it. (Key informant – Ms S)

Key informants described a traditional cultural context in which domestic violence was

not considered “a legal issue or social problem”. Shame and stigma – noted features of

collectivist societies (Hofstede, 2001) – also appeared to contribute to a culture of

silence and victim-blaming surrounding domestic violence amongst the Karen.

If a woman had been sexually harassed or assaulted, she doesn’t want to mention

those things over here so she blocks those things out. We call it denial, they call it

blocking out. So I would say sexual harassment and sexual abuse either by outsiders

or within the family is underreported that’s for sure. It’s a very judgmental society and

even if its not the women’s fault society tends to point a finger “Well you must have

encouraged him” or “You didn’t fight hard enough” – something like that. The poor

woman cops it all the time. (Key informant – Dr G)

Australian social and legal systems related to domestic violence were perceived to

encourage or result in family breakdowns and divorce which, for the Karen, was an

unacceptable solution and considered a “shameful” public affair. Consequently, women

avoided approaching the police or other Australian social services for help with

domestic violence problems. As reflected in the following quotations from interviews

with Karen key informants, marriage was “for life” and a woman should tolerate any

suffering in order to uphold this custom:

155

Religiously, particularly for the Christians, once you get married that is for life … it’s

like a tattoo – you cannot erase it. Even if you have an abusive husband or wife, you

have to accept it. (Key informant – Mr H)

Elderly women, say [of domestic violence] “That happened all the time, why you

complain? Why you call the police? It means you try to destroy the family. Once your

husband hits you, you need to listen to him next time so you can prevent him from

hitting or smacking”. It’s not a solution Paula. But, some elderly people accept that

kind of minor domestic violence issues. (Key informant – Mr J)

Family and community leaders attempted to resolve marital discord and domestic

violence within the community as they had done traditionally (Ward, 2002) through

instructing the community in moral behaviour, verbally reprimanding and instructing

abusive parties, and seeking marital reconciliation6. Leaders had also adapted their

strategies to deal with domestic violence in the Australian context. They reported

educating community members about their rights and responsibilities under Australian

law. Though they encouraged adherence to Australian law this was finely balanced with

upholding traditional values and practices. This balance is reflected in the following

quotations from interviews with two Karen community leaders:

We have some problem with domestic violence … rarely, but we’ve got some …

Because we have got a very close knit community we take it by two prongs, the

church and the community. The church teaches them about morals, ethics and how to

be a Christian family. And in our community we talk about our national and cultural

value, to respect the law, to respect this country’s law and be appreciative. (Key

informant – Mr P)

Sometimes they call me from the court! [They say] “Now my husband is about to be

charged, what should I do?” What do you say Paula? … We become a judge

sometimes so we have to be very wise … I refer it back to them in a nice way, [I say]

“His future is in your hands and he has been kind to you quite a number of times.

Look at him, is he remorseful? Does he really regret his actions? Always remember he

is the father of your children”. I make her be aware of this. (Key informant – Mr W)

6 Leaders considered marital reconciliation as the best solution. However, in one case of severe physical

assault they encouraged divorce.

156

Some service providers and Karen key informants were frustrated with how leaders

managed domestic violence but also empathised with their efforts. They believed that

the leaders genuinely wanted to stop family violence and also perceived that they

worked hard to present a positive image of the Karen community to the wider

Australian society. However, many also expressed concern about the effectiveness of

traditional approaches to domestic violence in the Australian context. They explained

that the Australian context did not permit leaders the same degree of monitoring and

control over the community as they had held in pre-migration contexts:

Some community elders, they are conservative. They would convince the wife or

maybe the daughter … “Okay, next time I will guarantee that he will not do that to

you”. That’s it. That’s action. He warns the husband, or maybe father, or maybe uncle,

“Don’t do that again, this is your wife, your daughter, or you niece, something like

that, don’t do that again” … But, who can guarantee it will not happen again? (Key

informant – Mr J)

8.4.4 Witches and bitches: single women negotiating gender

Single, separated and widowed women encountered additional gendered concerns.

Amongst the single women I met were (a) women whose husbands had died in battle or

during the refugee journey due to illness or accident, (b) mothers whose husbands had

abandoned them and their children for other women, (c) some who were separated from

their husbands after years of domestic violence and abuse, (d) single mothers who

raised children born as a result of rape and coercive relationships, and (e) women who

were unsure if they were widowed or abandoned by husbands from whom they had not

heard in years. Some of these women wanted to find new partners and most worried

about their physical safety:

They are concerned that they have no husband, there is no male in their house. They

are afraid that somebody might come in the night and touch them or look down on

them. (Pàw Lèh)

Single women were also concerned about financial security. Karen key informants

described the tenuous social position facing divorced and widowed women:

157

In your opinion, what are the main emotional issues facing the women?7

Security. Security.

What do you mean by that?

In Western thinking … we talk about individualism. You know as you are a Western

woman, you don’t care if you have a husband, or if you have someone to protect you.

But in Asian or Karen culture, women’s status is weak and, you know, traditionally

Karen women would like to depend on a man. (Key informant – Mr B)

Kohrt and Hruschka (2010) observed similar gendered beliefs and practices in Nepal,

where the loss of a husband was perceived to be more traumatic than the loss of a wife

due to the “significant livelihood instability” (p. 330) endured by Nepali widows.

Many single and married women corroborated Mr B’s view. However, a variety of

opinions emerged when I discussed the topic of remarriage with a group of Karen single

mothers. On the one hand the group predominantly reported that they were presently

content with their situation, felt well supported by the Australian Government, and

therefore did not desire to meet a new partner or husband. Some also referred to Karen

traditional customs and noted that remarriage was uncommon in their culture. On the

other hand, many women contemplated their future marriage prospects during the group

discussion. Some reported feeling presently content but thought that in the future they

might want to (re)marry. However, they felt that their marriage prospects were limited.

They reported that single men within the Karen community were either “kids” or “too

old”. Women also believed that they could be “worse off” if they failed to find a “good

partner” and instead met a man who had problems with gambling and alcohol. Women

unanimously felt they were unable to meet men outside of the Karen community due to

language and cultural barriers. They were also certain that they would face community

gossip if they chose to date or (re)marry. In the following interview quotation Mr B, a

Karen key informant, described the dilemma faced by widowed and divorced women:

As a Karen you know, if they are remarried, or have a relationship without official

marriage, the people look down [on them] … In the camps they can’t do it. Here [in

Australia] are they going to do the same as Western women or will their culture or

traditions stop them? That’s what I want to know. Because as a woman they might

need a man to protect them … You see, Western women, after they break up they

7 In extended quotations from interviews researcher’s voice is given in bold.

158

have another relationship, another marriage. But, how about Karen women, can they

do that or not? Will the elders and the community accept or not? … They are stuck in

between. To openly describe, they might need a sexual relationship as well … So, they

are in between two cultures … Only one or two [women] asked me [about this issue].

[They want to know] if they marry again will the community put them aside? If they

marry a white man are they going to look down on us? Can we go to Church again?

(Key informant – Mr B)

Similarly, Mr W, a Karen key informant, reported single women who engaged in sex

outside of marriage were likely to face community gossip:

So it really sounds like there is a negative label attached to these women?

I think so. Society is going to brand them as a witch. Witch or bitch or whatever. (Key

informant – Mr W)

Gossip served to monitor women’s behaviour and punish violations of accepted cultural

standards. Given the insularity of the Sydney Karen community, this method of social

surveillance appeared quite effective in controlling the behaviour of single women. The

detrimental effects of negative gossip have also been documented amongst Somali

women living in Melbourne, Australia (McMichael & Manderson, 2004). Ms R, an

outspoken Karen key informant, spoke of the vulnerable position of single women on

the Thailand-Burma border. She reported that single Karen women (especially those

who were separated or divorced as opposed to widowed) were seen as “available” for

“use” by men as legitimate objects for their sexual gratification. In addition, these single

women were ostracised by other women, who worried that they might lure away their

husbands. I was unable to obtain such frank opinions on this topic from other women

who, due to the sensitivity of the issue, often politely avoided my questions.

Single Karen women reported experiencing gossip in both the refugee camp and in

Australia. They described feeling hurt and upset by these experiences and explained that

some single mothers felt as though they did not “belong” within the Karen community.

However, most single mothers felt that community gossip was more significant in the

camps where they were more dependent on others for survival. In Australia they felt

well supported by the government and reported that gossip did not “really have a direct

impact” on their lives. Single mothers emphasised that they were “never excluded” from

community activities and were often “the first people to be invited” to events because

159

the community was aware of their difficult circumstances. Moreover, during a group

interview with Karen women recipients of the Woman at Risk visa, a number of single

mothers spoke not only of the challenges of single motherhood but also the motivation

and courage they derived from their situation. These women appeared to have

developed a sense of identity, responsibility and strength from their situation.

8.5 War, culture and daily stressors: women’s problems in context

In addition to the daily stressors affecting women’s wellbeing, the findings –

particularly those from key informants – also call attention to the unique relationship

between daily stressors and large-scale social, cultural, spiritual and political issues

affecting the wellbeing of the community. These various issues intermixed and often

could not be separated. The political had shaped the spiritual, the spiritual had shaped

the cultural and so on. This section brings together the findings discussed in this chapter

and interprets women’s experiences of daily stress in Australia within the pre-migration

spiritual-socio-cultural-political melting pot described by key informants. This melting

pot – and in particular persecutory experiences – was thought to have led to extreme

compliance and tolerance of hardship and discrimination, fear of authority, apathy,

sponsored dependency and disempowerment. These macro or societal-level

consequences of the Karen experience of forced migration are highly relevant to

understanding the daily problems women encountered and how they reacted and

responded to those problems. They can be thought of as the collective trauma of a

community that has undergone persecution.

Here an oversimplified reorientation to Karen cultural patterns or “national character”

(Bhugra, 2005, p. 86) is warranted. As we saw in Chapter 3 (see Section 3.3.2), key

cultural characteristics of the Karen are their (a) communal, collective ideology and

social practices, (b) respect for wisdom, age, religion and authority, and (c) compliance,

quietness and avoidance of confrontation. Like other collectivist societies (Hofstede,

2001; Kagitcibasi, 1999) the Karen seek to avoid “losing face” and are reluctant to

make a scene. Direct confrontation is thought rude and disagreement in conversation

may not be represented by a clear “no”, but rather by a diversion of topic or a polite

“maybe” or even “yes”. According to Hofstede (2001), these various practices may be

160

seen as a “consequence of living in a society that is very conscious of social contexts”

(p. 230)8.

The experience of persecution and war was layered on upon this pre-existing cultural

context. The protracted conflict and persecution experienced by the Karen was (and

remains) characterised not only by violence and warfare but also by social and

psychological tactics employed to instil mistrust and disrupt the cultural and social

fabric of the community (see Chapter 3). Expressing one’s opinion under the regime

was not just culturally discouraged: it was dangerous as it could easily disturb others

and upset a precarious political environment in which strength was necessary for

survival.

Under the rule of the junta’s soldiers, the Karen learnt (and learn) to adapt and survive.

Within their religious context they had already acknowledged suffering as a part of life.

Such religious and cultural practices continued but, according to key informants, as the

persecution persisted through generations, what was once aberrant became normal.

According to Karen community leaders, the bar of what became acceptable was lowered

and many accepted their “fate”:

Karen people said “Oh this is our fate”, general Karen people feel like that. Even

though they are not satisfied or content with that, they will come to think it’s

acceptable. “This is our fate, or, our way of life has to be like this all the time because

we have no legal powers for our rights”. So this becomes a limitation for us, the whole

nation. This kind of thinking is one of the effects of living in this situation for quite a

long time. (Key informant – Mr H)

Elders and key informants explained that, for some, acceptance of one’s fate and long-

term refugee camp confinement led to apathy, dependency and demoralisation. They

believed that the disabling nature of protracted camp confinement eroded self-worth and

self-efficacy. Except for the few who were employed by humanitarian agencies (as

stipendiary staff), most people could not work and most had no opportunity to study.

They were dependent on camp authorities for everything. As Dr G, a key informant,

8 These traits are mentioned here as cultural psychological patterns (Sapir, 2010) and this characterisation

is not meant to define the personality of any given Karen individual.

161

noted, “there is a sponsored dependency on governments and organisations”. Other

Karen key informants also lamented the creation and effects of such dependency:

Most people, their mindset is “we are refugees, we need to be fed, the sun will rise

tomorrow, rice, fish paste and oil are going to be there and we are going to grow up

and live like that”. (Key informant – Mr W)

And because so many of them have been 15, 20, in this case 32 years in the camp,

they have got used to the Thai authorities and the NGOs [non-government

organisations] providing them with their food and other necessities. (Key informant –

Dr G)

Kibreab (1993) has criticised the “so-called ‘refugee dependency syndrome’” (p. 330)

discourse as it was applied to Somalian refugees in the 1990s. However, aid

organisations and researchers have continued to observe institutionalisation (Abdi,

2005; Lautze & Hammock, 1996), “forced idleness” (Colic-Peisker & Tilbury, 2003, p.

77) and “aid dependency” (Kett, 2005, p. 99) resulting from prolonged refugee camp

confinement. Karen key informants narratives suggest that protracted camp confinement

had the effect of disabling opportunities for individuals and communities to experience

efficacy, that is, to experience their ability to produce necessary or desired results. Their

conceptualisations share features with Bandura’s (1977) theoretical framework of self-

efficacy.

For Bandura “The capacity to exercise control over the nature and quality of one’s life

is the essence of humanness” (2001, p. 1). Bandura postulated that a person’s

expectations of self-efficacy (their personal capacity for effectiveness) determine the

degree to which they will take actions to change their circumstances. He predicted that

experiences of mastery or accomplishment (including seeing others demonstrate

success) are critical to perceived self-efficacy and that repeated failure lowers such

efficacy. Moreover, Bandura hypothesised that emotional arousal (e.g., states of stress)

can affect perceived self-efficacy and that individuals are less likely to expect success

when feeling agitated or stressed.

The empirical literature exploring self-efficacy amongst refugees remains sparse,

despite hints towards its potential relevance (Benight & Bandura, 2004). The most

recent review of self-efficacy and trauma (Luszczynska, Benight, & Cieslak, 2009)

found that low self-efficacy was associated with greater PTSD symptom severity,

162

depressive feelings and somatic symptoms. However, of the twenty-seven studies

reviewed, only one (Ferren, 1999) concerned a refugee population. Moreover, Ferren’s

(1999) study indicated that, amongst refugees, the relationship between the effects of

forced migration and self-efficacy may not be as linear or as one-directional as might be

predicted as adolescent Bosnian and Croatian refugees with symptomatic PTSD

exhibited higher positive self-efficacy than their non-PTSD and non-traumatised peers.

Ferren surmised that survival of trauma might actually protect and maintain high self-

efficacy because, in the context of ethnic persecution, survivors of particularly horrific

trauma are recognised by the community and more likely to receive social support. In

contrast, other research (Colic-Peisker & Tilbury, 2003) supports the view of Karen

leaders and suggests that the refugee experience, and particularly prolonged refugee

camp confinement, leads to hopelessness and loss of belief in one’s capabilities. It

seems that further exploration of refugee wellbeing from a self-efficacy perspective is

warranted.

Karen key informants also described mistrust and fear of authority as “undeniable, very

common” problems impacting on community resettlement as depicted in the following

quotations:

They are very compliant with everything. For example, you have to comply with

Centrelink, you have to comply with your doctors appointment, you have to comply

with any group trying to meet you even though you don’t know who they are and what

they are doing. You have to, you have to, you have to. Because that’s what they have

experienced in the camp where you are conditioned to comply or else you aren’t going

to get supplies or you can’t stay in the camp. So I think the fear [is] that if you don’t

comply you are going to suffer. (Key informant – Ms K)

Because [of] the language barrier it’s hard to understand how the system works – how

to find your way through this legal system, or the government system – so then they

have no confidence to complain, or to express their feelings, emotions towards

hierarchy … They don’t want to talk to officers [in] coats [uniforms]. (Key informant

– Mr H)

These findings locate the social effects of persecution and camp confinement as critical

determinants of how Karen women experienced the stress of resettlement and bring

attention to three cross-cutting themes of culture, gender and environmental mastery.

This context helps to explain women’s interactions with service providers in Australia,

163

particularly their fear of Job Services Australia staff (see Section 8.3.2), as well as their

pervasive feeling of uselessness and the emotional impact of failure and unemployment.

8.6 Summary and reflections

This chapter has presented findings related to issues perceived to affect distress and

wellbeing of Karen refugee women. Women overwhelmingly gave attention to

problems that are relatively predictable in underprivileged populations, and which have

been documented before in relation to refugee women (James, 2010; Pittaway &

Bartolomei, 2001) including refugee communities in Australia (Allotey, 1999; Sheikh-

Mohammed, MacIntyre, Wood, Leask, & Isaacs, 2006; J. Taylor, 2004) and Karen

refugees in the United States (Kenny & Lockwood-Kenny, 2011). Literature supports

their perspective that post-migration problems with education, poverty and financial

stress, food insecurity, housing, social class, interpersonal conflict, parenting and

separation from family are pertinent sources of psychological distress (Lund et al.,

2010; Marmot, 2005; Nicassio & Pate, 1984; Porter & Haslam, 2005).

Due to lack of local (i.e., Australian) knowledge women struggled to effectively access

and utilise a range of systems, services and resources. While these issues appear against

the backdrop of language and communication difficulties, they are also characterised by

additional pre- and post-migration factors. This is dense and salient subject matter. A

visual model of these issues would be one with many arrows as the factors interact in

multiple ways. For example, language difficulties and anxiety result in reluctance to use

public transport. Reduced mobility results in less access to education and supports. Lack

of education reduces chances of employment, which in turn hampers financial security.

Lack of money leads to failure to access necessary medical care. Cycles of poverty and

disempowerment are created and perpetuated by multiple factors and players, including

but not limited to those mentioned in this example. But multiple factors and multiple

players mean multiple possible areas and entry-points for change and improvement to

take place. In order to explore solutions it is first necessary to understand the problem.

Unfortunately, as flagged throughout this chapter, many elements of women’s daily

stressors were generated and exacerbated by the limitations of service providers. Such

limitations will be explored in detail in Chapter 12.

164

This chapter has largely been about problems. It is important to remember that the

Karen are a relatively recent arrival population in Australia’s humanitarian program.

Getting “a feel for the game” of Australian daily living is most difficult for first

generation arrival populations. Most participants reported that they sought resettlement

to provide a better life for their children and they held high expectations for their

children’s education and futures. This finding resonates with recent Australian research

demonstrating “the significant contribution” of humanitarian entrants in Australia. The

government-funded research found that humanitarian entrants place high value on the

education of their children (DIAC, 2011a). The research highlighted the substantial

barriers facing first-generation entrants but noted that second-generation humanitarian

entrants enjoy educational and occupational achievements on par, and better than, their

non-immigrant, same-age Australian-born peers. Such findings are encouraging but

should not negate the need for further efforts to enhance the wellbeing of humanitarian

entrants. These findings bring attention to the need for research and improvements in

relation to HSS policy and service provision (see Chapter 14).

165

Chapter 9

A Suffering Heart-mind:

Conceptualising, Experiencing and Explaining Distress

9.1 Introduction

This chapter further explores women’s conceptualisations and experiences of distress

through examining their idioms of distress.

9.2 A suffering heart-mind: women’s idioms of distress

In this section the Karen idioms hso k’moe dha, tha, tha dha hsar and dha blu are

introduced and their meanings described.

9.2.1 Thinking too much: hso k’moe dha

Women spoke of their distress using the Sgaw Karen phrase hso k’moe dha. This phrase

may be broken up into hso k’moe: to think, thinking, give concern to; and dha: to

excess, too much, now, in the present. Listening to women’s descriptions of hso k’moe

dah it seems that English words such as fretting, worrying or ruminating might be the

most precise translations. However, the more literal translation “thinking too much” was

the most commonly used by bilingual Karen-English speakers. Moreover, this literal

translation allows for ease of reading and links to literature documenting “thinking too

much” idioms amongst refugees from Burma (Lopes Cardozo et al., 2004), other South

East Asian populations (Frye & D’Avanzo, 1994; D. E. Hinton & Otto, 2006) and

populations elsewhere (Guarnaccia, Lewis-Fernandez, & Marano, 2003; Kohrt &

Hruschka, 2010; Parker, Chan, & Tully, 2006; D. Pedersen et al., 2010).

Hso k’moe dha (thinking too much) was characterised by persistent worrying thoughts

about past or current stressors. The chronic daily stressors discussed in Chapter 8 were

the most commonly cited causes. Women considered hso k’moe dha to be a mild state

of distress experienced by everyone at some stage in life but also explained that

excessive or chronic thinking could lead to more serious states of distress, sickness and

even mental illness:

166

Everybody can have thinking, but some people think and think and think and the

thinking never comes out of their mind and becomes sickness. (Participant in Group

B)

9.2.2 A suffering heart-mind: tha…

Karen women used expressions relating to the heart (tha) to describe further states of

emotional distress:

Everything is about the heart, everything is about the heart … when your heart is

heavy then your whole body is heavy. (Key informant – Ms S)

The word tha refers to both the physical heart organ and the symbolic heart as the seat

of emotions. When used to denote emotional suffering it is more aptly translated as

“heart-mind” as women perceived emotions and thoughts to be interrelated. A variety of

suffixes were added to tha (heart-mind) to describe different states of physical and

emotional distress. For example, ba do means hurt, thus, tha ba do is a state in which

one’s “heart-mind is hurt”. Most women’s descriptions of tha (heart-mind) distress were

limited to tha da mu (upset), tha ba do (hurt) and tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-

mind). Karen key informants and women with more education also referred to several

other states; however, they emphasised that many terms overlapped or reflected

differences in dialect. Interestingly, participants emphasised that in comparison to the

English language, Karen dialects do not have so many different words or phrases to

describe emotions.

Women explained that common states of tha distress – such as feeling tha ba do (hurt)

or tha da mu (upset) – were frequently caused by daily life stressors and were not

abnormal or debilitating forms of suffering. Rather, the experience was part of “normal”

life experience and encompassed a range of experiences and reactions from acute

instantaneous shock or fright reactions through to prolonged worry, anxiety and grief

after receiving no news from a loved one missing in the war. These common forms of

tha distress represented the most undefined of the various idioms and could potentially

be likened to Western idioms used to express stress and worry. For example, many

English words are used to refer to a state of stress though they may emphasise different

aspects of the cause or experience (e.g., stressed, nervous, hassled, frazzled, worried or

pressured). These states were also potential precursors to more severe suffering. If a

167

person should continue to hso k’moe dha (think too much), their acute and common

state of tha distress could become a more severe “sickness” of the heart-mind (tha dha

hsar) or mental illness (dha blu).

9.2.3 Sickness of the heart-mind: tha dha hsar

The idiom tha dha hsar described a more serious state of heart-mind distress. Dha hsar

means any disease or sickness, thus, the term tha dha hsar means “sickness of the

heart”. It related to physical heart sickness such as cardiovascular disease as well as to a

state of emotional distress characterised by irritability, anger and somatic complaints:

Tha dha hsar, directly it is heart disease but it can also mean emotional, [there are]

two sides to it. (Key informant – Mr H)

Women had unanimously agreed on the universal prevalence of more common heart-

mind distress (e.g., tha da mu and tha ba do) but their perceptions differed regarding the

prevalence and seriousness of emotional tha dha hsar. Some thought it was a more

severe but still common state of distress whereas others believed it was rare and quite

debilitating, though still something you could recover from. Participants also expressed

a gendered notion of distress and believed that women were more likely to suffer tha

dha hsar. Some attributed this to the stress of women’s social roles and responsibilities.

Others, such as Mu Htòh, believed that women were “weak” and simply less able to

cope with stressors:

Women can get [tha dha hsar] more … The men can think about the bright side and

relax more than the women who worry too much. (Mu Htòh)

Tha dha hsar shared similarities with common (and unstigmatised) conditions such as

tha ba do (hurt). However, it was also considered to be closer to the state of dha blu

(mental illness). As Pàw Lèh stated, tha dha hsar “means mostly a mental problem”.

Consequently, it was more highly stigmatised, which might explain women’s reluctance

to identify their suffering in this way.

168

9.2.4 Madness: dha blu

The idiom dha blu described the most severe state of suffering, which is translated here

as madness or mental illness9. It was characterised by inappropriate and incoherent

behaviour and speech, such as walking in public naked and talking nonsense. Dha blu

was distinguished from other idioms by an emphasis on “mental” as opposed to

emotional (e.g., heart-mind) features. Moreover, it was considered to be relatively

untreatable. Participants also clearly conceptualised this state of suffering as an

extension of unresolved tha distress and explained that very severe tha dha hsar

(sickness of the heart-mind) could be indistinguishable from dha blu (mental illness or

madness).

Women’s idioms of distress could be placed along a continuum from normal reactions

to life stressors (e.g., hso k’moe dha / thinking too much and common states of tha /

heart-mind distress) through to rarer (e.g., tha dha hsar / sickness of the heart-mind) and

eventually debilitating (e.g., dha blu / mental illness) forms of distress. Ms K, a Karen

key informant, explained that the idioms could not be clearly categorised as distinct

entities:

If you don’t care for yourself here [points to tha dha hsar written on a piece of paper]

you can go there [points to dha blu written on a piece of paper] … We can’t

differentiate [distinctly between states] because we just don’t have that way of

thinking. (Key informant – Ms K)

In most instances the gamut of idioms of distress shared underlying causal explanations

and experiential features. These are explored in the following sections.

9.3 Causes of distress

Women’s aetiological beliefs about emotional distress can be grouped into four

categories: daily stressors, persecution and forced migration, biological and

supernatural.

9 Dha blu was overwhelmingly the most common Sgaw Karen term employed to describe this state of

mental illness. However, participants explained that other dialects used additional and different terms

such as tha te tea.

169

9.3.1 Daily stressors

All participants described the daily life stressors detailed in Chapter 8 as the most

significant causes of their emotional and mental (e.g., heart-mind) distress. They

believed that daily stressors caused all people to occasionally hso k’moe dha, saying:

“[it’s] normal, too much thinking – you have, I have”. The following quotations were

typical of women’s discussions about daily stressors and emotional suffering. Htòh

located the source of her hso k’moe dha within daily resettlement concerns:

We came here as refugees and we can’t even sleep, we think a lot and worry … We

came here empty handed, we have nothing and we know nothing. We feel really

down. People are pressuring you to look for a job. We are thinking what will happen?

What will happen if we can’t get a job? That is our main worry … It will become a

sickness if you go on thinking a lot like that. Even I get headaches when I think too

much. (Htòh)

Married women described changing gender role practices and marital discord as central

causes of suffering. Like Cambodian refugee women (D. E. Hinton et al., 2010;

Liamputtong Rice, 1999a), they described suffering heart-mind distress due to their

husband’s excessive alcohol consumption, gambling and sexual infidelities. Typical of

their comments, Ta Heh Sōo described marital and parenting problems as the cause of

her friend’s distress:

She’s got a lot of thinking regarding her relationship with her husband. Before she

came here to Australia her husband had an affair … She asked him to come back but

he won’t … The children want their father … This makes her stressed, she’s thinking

too much. Also … she can’t discipline the children. When she was back in the camp at

least she can discipline the children, but here it’s hard. (Ta Heh Sōo)

Women also drew attention to other interpersonal stressors, particularly community

gossip and peer rejection. As the Karen community was small, collectivist and relatively

insular, it is understandable that social exclusion was a significant stressor.

Others, like Mu Htòh, described real and perceived experiences of outsider

discrimination as resulting in hso k’moe dha and isolation:

Sometimes when we are shopping, especially in the Asian shops, they look at how we

are dressed and talk unkindly about us. A few feel afraid by this … They think “We

170

are refugees, we can’t talk the language and we have no job”. Some people think

about that and then later they move away from those who discriminate and they want

to interact only within their Karen community. (Mu Htòh)

Typical of many women separated from their children, Mëh Luh described her suffering

as caused by worry for her friends and family in the homeland:

Sometimes if we think a lot we tend to have sadness. Here we live in a country that is

peaceful, we don’t have to worry for anything at all … But when we think about

people who live in either the refugee camp or in Burma we feel sad … I feel sad when

I hear about the situation in Burma, especially when I hear that the military is

targeting the ethnic groups and burning down their villages. (Mëh Luh)

9.3.2 Persecution and forced migration: loss and traumatic war stressors

Although women emphasised the prominence of daily stressors, they also explained that

losses and traumatic war experiences caused varying degrees of emotional suffering

including hso k’moe dha, common heart-mind distress, tha dha hsar and dha blu.

Loss, war and common heart-mind distress

Women experienced hso k’moe dha and common tha distress as a result of their

cumulative “series of losses” (i.e., loss of land, culture, possessions and loved ones), as

well as from exposure to general war trauma such as hearing gunfire. They did not

pathologise their suffering but considered it to be an understandable consequence of

their experiences. They also used the idiom of the heart-mind to describe a wide range

of encounters and emotion states including acute fright reactions (e.g., the psycho-

physiological response experienced when faced with a menacing dog or incoming

gunfire) and memories of loss and trauma. Tha ba do (hurt) was the most common

idiom used to describe distress caused by loss and trauma.

Tha ba do … is suffered by anyone affected by some distressing memory … When a

person suffers tha ba do [he is] upset, he is hurt, his mind is hurt. (Key informant – Mr

H)

Women typically portrayed their experience of these memories in the same matter-of-

fact manner as Ta Heh Sōo and Htee Gloh who described the impact of past traumas as

occasionally making them feel tha da mu (upset):

171

I grew up with bullets all around me. When I first came here it was Christmas time

and there were a lot of fireworks and I thought is it a gun? I was scared because the

sound of fireworks is a lot like gunshots. We can still think a lot and because of that

thinking you feel scared and upset.

What is the Karen word for what you felt?

Tha da mu [upset]. Tha da mu. Tha is heart. (Ta Heh Sōo)

Your memory is not lost … BOOM, I heard big bombs too many times. And then

when we moved here I heard the thunder and I thought the bombing had come here. I

don’t like loud noises. It makes me feel afraid. (Htee Gloh)

Like other participants, these women did not consider their reactions to be abnormal or

alarming.

Loss, war and more severe suffering

Women believed that particularly awful experiences (both war-related and chronic daily

stressors) could cause more severe heart-mind distress (e.g., tha dha hsar) and all

believed there were grades or “degrees” of loss and trauma. For them, everybody

suffered but some people suffered “too much”. Loss(es), trauma(s) and chronic daily

stress(ors) with serious negative social consequences were deemed worse than those

without and considered more likely to cause severe emotional distress.

The chronic daily stressors (described in Chapter 8) considered most severe and likely

to cause severe distress were interpersonal; specifically domestic violence, marital

discord and alcoholic or sexually unfaithful husbands. When participants recalled

instances of severe tha dha hsar and suicide in the refugee camp(s), they described

women who were married to abusive or “alcoholic” men. With regard to war, loss and

trauma, participants explained that witnessing the death of an unrelated individual was

unlikely to have significant social consequences and was not as severe as enduring the

death of one’s husband – a situation with grim social ramifications for Karen women

who were culturally prohibited from remarriage and yet were considered to need a

husband for physical safety. In the following interview quotation Mr P, a Karen elder,

succinctly illustrated this view:

It’s a normal life in our country, tha ba do [feeling hurt] and everything. Only when

your relative, husband or children are killed, that one is a big issue. But when you only

172

have to flee or you see someone being killed but physically they are not related to you,

it’s a normal thing. (Key informant – Mr P)

Witnessing the murder of one’s family member was ranked as highly distressing but,

again, there were degrees depending on the social implications of the traumatic event.

Enduring the loss of children was considered to be harder if one was unable to conceive

any more children. Losing all of one’s children or all children of one gender (e.g., death

of all daughters) more so. Severe tha dha hsar could also result from one’s children

being raped or abducted and sold to human traffickers for prostitution. Again the

relative salience of this experience appeared linked to its social ramifications; rape and

prostitution had serious social implications for both mother and child.

Interestingly, women tended to locate current life stressors as a more significant cause

of tha dha hsar than previous life difficulties and believed that war-related loss and

trauma could not cause severe emotional suffering in the absence of additional chronic

daily stressors:

I think in our community, in the Karen situation we don’t think about the past

background. Only about these problems, current problems [pointing to our list of daily

post-migration stressors].

Is tha dha hsar sometimes caused by old memories?

No [laughs]. Old memory is the thing about the past?

Yeah.

No, no. (Mu Htòh)

9.3.3 Biology and inherited vulnerability

Women did not attribute biological causes to common types of distress (e.g., hso k’moe

dah and tha distress). However, many reported that more severe suffering (e.g., tha

dhar hsar and dha blu) resulted from an interaction between life adversity and an

inherent vulnerability or predisposition towards emotional instability. Participants

proposed some sort of mixed biologically and socially inherited familial predisposition

to psychological fragility. Similar beliefs regarding inherited vulnerability have been

documented in other cultures (Guarnaccia et al., 2003; Liamputtong Rice, 1999a,

1999b) and share features with a Western stress-diathesis model (Rabkin, 1982) of

mental illness. Additional biomedical explanations for dha blu included brain injury,

epilepsy, cerebral malaria and poor prenatal nutrition.

173

9.3.4 Supernatural

Women attributed supernatural causes only to dha blu, explaining that it could be

caused by witchcraft, black magic (e.g., curses by enemies) and bad fates (caused by

offending spirits). Mr H, a Karen key informant, explained: “when the cause is

unknown people generally believe this is black magic or spirits”.

9.4 Experiencing and expressing distress

9.4.1 Aching heads and aching bodies: somatic complaints

Women gave precedence to somatic symptoms over affect states. They explained that

hso k’moe dha often naturally resolved over a matter of days but reported that it also

commonly led to a temporary depletion of physical and mental energy and to somatic

complaints including headaches, sleeping difficulties, loss of appetite, lethargy,

tachycardia, and aches and pains. Like participants in Rosbrook and Schweitzer’s

(2010) study of refugees from Burma, Karen women also described experiencing their

distress in physical terms “as exhaustion, heaviness, insomnia and poor appetite” (p.

164). Mëh Luh described the symptoms of hso k’moe dha, reporting:

They won’t feel well, they will feel sick … Once you have too much thinking you

have no appetite … Once you lose your appetite you will not be able to sleep because

you think a lot … and you feel lethargic … Then it’s very easy to get sick and catch

diseases … People with this issue are impatient and get angry easily … They will feel

their body aching … maybe their hands, legs, and sometimes the whole body aches

and pains. (Mëh Luh)

Common tha (heart-mind) distress caused women to feel “shaky” and “dizzy”, and

experience tachycardia and “chest pain”. These somatic complaints were often

experienced as acute instantaneous reactions to stressors. For example, women

described feeling acute shakiness after misunderstanding road rules and nearly getting

hit by traffic or after hearing thunder and being reminded of bomb blasts and gunfire.

The same somatic complaints were also reported for more enduring heart-mind distress

caused by chronic daily stressors such as marital discord. Women who suffered tha

distress due to complex social stressors explained that their distress was characterised

by additional difficulties including irritability, poor memory, poor blood circulation and

174

persistent worry. In the following quotation Hpä Hpaw Mu describes feeling tha ta mu

(upset) due to social exclusion and relationship difficulties:

I feel upset, I lose my appetite, I feel dizzy … I can’t sleep. I also have difficulty

breathing, especially at night. I’m afraid that if I die at night no one will know. When I

want to think of something I can’t recall the things – loss of memory. The heart is

beating fast. My feet and fingertips are very cold … Sometimes I feel like I don’t want

to do anything and become lazy. The blood circulation doesn’t work well and I feel

completely heavy. (Hpä Hpaw Mu)

Hpä Hpaw Mu had informed her GP of her symptoms but lamented that he could not

find anything “wrong” with her. Women experiencing more severe distress also spoke

of their distress in relation to specific bodily organs and gave graphic depictions to

convey the physical embodiment of their distress. For example, Lah Luh reported:

Whenever my daughter [misbehaves] I feel like something pops in my stomach,

something like a bullet pops in my stomach and goes straight to my head and I have a

big headache … Sometimes without thinking or without having any problems I feel

something pop in my tummy and pop into my liver or my heart. A sharp pain radiates

up to my head and I feel headaches. (Lah Luh)

Karen women also experienced psychological distress as dysregulation of blood flow.

They considered blood pressure, menstrual blood flow and blood circulation to be

important factors for physical and emotional health. These beliefs appear to have their

origins in traditional Karen medicine, which includes components of both Ayurvedic

and traditional Chinese medicine, both of which stress the importance of unrestricted

blood flow (Bodeker et al., 2005). Women believed that blood pressure problems were

caused by both physical illness and social stress. They feared that blood dysregulation

from thinking too much and emotional distress could lead to serious health

consequences:

Some people might even get high blood pressure from hso k’moe dha [too much

thinking]. If you can’t control the blood pressure you could even get a stroke.

(Participant in Group C)

Cambodian refugees have described similar fears of strokes and heart-attacks (D. E.

Hinton et al., 2010), attributable to their comparable traditional medical

175

conceptualisations about blood flow. Anthropologist Monique Skidmore (2002)

similarly reported that, for Burmese women, the regularity and volume of menstrual

blood was a primary indicator of health. Abortion and childbirth were thought to be

particularly vulnerable times for women. Her description of women’s

conceptualisations of blood flow shares similarities with Chinese medical systems in

which the metaphor of unrestricted blood flow is more akin to the notion of a flowing

river than a heart-pump system. Similar to both Ayurvedic and Chinese systems,

traditional Burmese medicine addressed women’s blood-flow difficulties through diet

and the nutritional and taste qualities of hot, cold, sweet, sour, bitter and salty foods. In

my study women tended to attribute their blood flow concerns to poverty, social

stressors and medical problems.

Women’s descriptions of somatic symptoms paralleled comments made by GPs who

explained that their Karen clients primarily focused on the somatic aspects of their

emotional distress:

They somatise … They don’t really come straight out and report to you “I’m

depressed” or “I have difficulty dealing with this”. They present with a headache, or

tummy ache, or chest pain, or fatigue, lethargy. The problem is that they are not

coping. [The somatic complaint] means that she is tired. It means “I’m not coping”.

(Key informant – Dr F)

Somatisation of distress has been consistently observed amongst Asian (D. E. Hinton &

Otto, 2006; Kohrt & Hruschka, 2010) and other populations (Escobar & Gureje, 2007;

Gureje, 2004; Gureje, Simon, Ustan, & Goldberg, 1997; Kirmayer, Groleau, Looper, &

Dominice Dao, 2004; A. Kleinman & Becker, 1998; Lock, 1993) but is also common in

the West where people often speak of stressors as a “pain in the neck”. Chronic low

back pain is another common somatic manifestation of psychosocial distress amongst

White Western patients (Sikorski, Stampfer, Cole, & Wheatley, 1996). Indeed, A.

Kleinman and Kleinman (1985) argue: “from the cross-cultural perspective, it is …

psychologization in the West that appears unusual and requires explanation” (p. 435).

9.4.2 “I have a problem”: social complaints and social expressions

Women also described a social conceptualisation of distress. Social stressors were

perceived as a leading cause of emotional suffering and were often the means by which

176

women conveyed their distress to close friends or family women. Some key informants

interpreted women’s tendency to emphasise the social causes of their distress as

reflective of an inability to express one’s feelings. Others, like Dr G, believed that both

somatic and social expressions of distress served as a means of avoiding stigma

associated with emotional and mental problems:

To label oneself as suffering from a mental health problem creates a stigma and

people tend to find somatic or bodily symptoms to explain these things. They might

say “Oh, I’m worried about my children and their schooling” or something, they won’t

label that as anxiety. They will put the cause onto the children or the husband and then

we have to tell them very gently “Look, OK, we take those into account but you are

the one who is suffering from this so this has become your problem so we will have to

help you deal with that”. So it’s about ongoing education. (Key informant – Dr G)

Although stigma may contribute to how women express and experience distress, women

also appeared to genuinely perceive their distress as intimately entwined with damaged

social relationships and other environmental or social stressors. Emotional suffering

occurred not only within their bodies and minds, but within their social environment.

Some social causes of distress were considered to be more private affairs (e.g., serious

marital discord) and consequently were unlikely to be expressed openly, though women

might privately discuss their difficulties with closely trusted friends, family or

community leaders. In contrast, other stressors (e.g., parenting problems, financial

difficulty and grief) were not shrouded in such weighty stigma and embarrassment, or

the need for privacy. Overt social expressions of grief (e.g., crying) were culturally

approved reactions to loss but disproportionate or ongoing expressions were considered

abnormal and problematic:

The way of grieving is you quietly cry and you don’t make a big scene out of it, you

know, not the physical hitting and stuff like that. You pick yourself up afterwards and

you don’t go on forever even though you are going to sort of, but yeah, you don’t

show it. (Key informant – Ms K)

Ms K, a Karen key informant, went on to describe the difference between normal grief

and “crazy” reactions to loss:

177

The duration [is important]. If people behave in a certain [way for a certain] period of

time, in a very short time they are not considered crazy. [If you lost your child] and

you saw the child that you just lost and if you behave [grieve overtly e.g., crying] then

people do understand that this is your loss. But if you start to behave [in a manner

that] they can’t rationalise, for example, you might have lost a brother or somebody

else or something and grieved for a period of time but six months down the track it

comes up again, then they couldn’t find a reason why the person is behaving that way,

then they start to think that person is crazy … They won’t see any relevance to it [the

loss of the loved one]. (Key informant – Ms K)

Ms K’s description relates to the notion of degrees of loss and trauma described above.

Expressions of distress were expected to vary according to the degree of loss or trauma

and had to fit a culturally approved form and time course or else individuals risked

being identified and stigmatised as suffering from dha blu (mental illness). The

American Psychiatric Association (2000) similarly distinguishes between normal and

pathological grief. Bereavement is expected to resolve within two months and not be

characterised by functional impairment or severe problems such as suicidal ideation and

psychomotor retardation.

9.4.3 “I am useless”: expressing failure and frustration

“Feeling useless” could be considered a cause of distress, but women generally talked

about it as an experience and expression of their heart-mind distress. They felt useless in

a number of areas including language, education, employment and performing daily

tasks such as shopping. Some felt useless in contrast to their pre-migration life

including their previous occupational and social roles (e.g., professional and volunteer

positions). As we saw in Chapter 8 (see Section 8.3.7), women felt particularly useless

as parents in the Australian context. Similarly, when Karen key informants spoke of

Karen women who suffered emotional distress they also described them as feeling

useless:

Like, the old woman sitting in the corner … she always says “I’m useless”. She feels

that she is useless and her life is useless … [Her] daughter does not listen [to her]

anymore. This is the difference. When they were in the refugee camp, then they might

not be different because of the tradition, because of the culture. But here, you know,

you go your way, I go mine. So, the culture has been changed and she feels isolated.

She says “I am useless. I am useless”. (Key informant – Mr B)

178

The bilingual research assistant predominantly used the English word “useless” when

interpreting women’s discussions. Similarly, key informants speaking English

employed the same term when conveying what Karen women had said to them about

their distress. However, through examining women’s accounts of feeling “useless” it

seems that multiple meanings may be inferred. Women described experiences related to

feeling purposeless (without purpose), unoccupied (not used by anyone or anything),

worthless (of no social value) and incompetent (lacking necessary skill or ability).

The following quotation comes from a group interview in which women had chosen to

discuss unemployment. I had asked the women to explain the emotional impact of

unemployment. To answer they cut out a magazine picture of a salad (see Image 6) and

explained the symbolism of the image:

Image 6: Picture of a salad – used by women to make an analogy between

nutritional value and the value inherent in women

That bowl of food represents a woman. That food is nutritious, it gives you energy,

you will be healthy. If a woman or a person is strong enough to stand by herself it will

be very helpful for the community … Food has value, it has things like vitamins that

are beneficial. As women we have that power but nobody takes that power to be used.

If someone takes that power it will be helpful for her and everyone.

179

Okay, so everybody has skills but nobody is able to give their skills or use their

skills because they can’t find work.

The woman or the person is represented by the bowl of food, someone must use her

for her to be beneficial. If you don’t use it, it is useless. It must be used. Food is for

someone to eat. A person is for someone to use in some way. Otherwise they are

wasted. (Participants in Group D)

Helplessness and cessation of action were consequences of feeling useless. For

example, when discussing parenting and feeling useless, Mu Htòh reported that some

parents gave up trying to instruct and discipline their children and simply “let them go

and do whatever they want”.

Key informants spoke of the importance of “self-esteem” and “self-reliance”. They

emphasised that emotional wellbeing significantly depended on people feeling effective

in their daily lives. These findings share similarities with other ethnographic research.

Failure in gendered life domains was identified as a cause of distress amongst Nepalis

(Kohrt & Hruschka, 2010). Nepali women emphasised the negative emotional impact of

failing to maintain good household and family relationships, and produce male children.

Men were more negatively affected by professional failure or loss of social status.

Similarly, Watters (2010) reported that failure to fulfil one’s social role is central to

distress for Sri Lankans. The pivotal role played by the discourse of feeling useless

suggests that understanding this idiom of distress could provide policy makers,

researchers and service providers with new ideas for enhancing the wellbeing of Karen

women.

9.4.4 “I remember”: remembering suffering

Tha (heart-mind) distress was also experienced as memories of loss, persecution and

trauma. For example, Mëh Luh described seeing memories of fighting in her eyes:

I myself had to flee from one place to another. I have seen fighting and burning

villages. Even I sometimes remember that and I see it in my eyes.

Is that a problem for you?

Not really. Not really. Sometimes maybe I might feel a bit sad. But it doesn’t really

hurt me a lot or interrupt me at all. Because we always learnt not to think about things

that are meaningless and things that don’t help you at all. (Mëh Luh)

180

Mëh Luh reported that her memories were not problematic despite causing short-term

feelings of sadness. Her matter-of-fact approach to memories was also conveyed by

other participants including Pàw Lèh who reported that dreams about fighting did not

affect her daily functioning:

I always have dreams about my homeland, the Karen State … [Dreams of] always

running, fleeing to another place. Dreaming that I am not in Australia, that I am in the

Karen State … I dream about the running and I dream about the property, and the

fighting and war there … It’s not a problem. After I’m dreaming about it I’m okay,

dream is dream! (Pàw Lèh)

Several Karen key informants corroborated this pragmatic and accepting approach to

war-related memories and dreams. In the following quotation, Mr M, a Karen leader,

described his own experience of “a few years” of nightmares following a traumatic

experience in which he was wounded. Interestingly, Mr M’s words suggest that

remembering may not necessarily be functionally impairing:

When I first came [to Australia] I had a nightmare in which I had the same scenario

but with different settings where I got wounded. But it never came back after a few

years …

When you were having this did it cause you some concern, were you worried or

upset?

Only for a while, only when I woke up and remembered about the nightmares but later

on it does not affect me badly.

Did you or do other people in your community go and get help for this kind of

problem?

No, I didn’t think I needed help [laughs] …

Is there a word for it [your nightmares]?

Yeah, remember, remembering.

So remembering is not considered a problem?

No, not if it doesn’t affect your day-to-day work.

Does it affect some people’s day-to-day work?

It might, I don’t know. (Key informant – Mr M)

Consistent with prior research (Parker et al., 2006), acculturation or familiarity with

Australian culture influenced participants’ descriptions of local idioms of distress. In

contrast to Karen women, other Karen and non-Karen key informants, particularly those

181

employed in health professions, were more likely to employ Western idioms of distress

(e.g., “flashbacks”) to describe the emotional impact of war and trauma, and

conceptualise these experiences as psychological problems. The different perceptions of

professional and lay participants is consistent with ethnographic research in Nepal

where counsellors, health professionals and program administrators gave greater

importance to traumatic experiences causing mental health difficulties than did Nepali

lay-persons who emphasised the importance of chronic daily life stressors (Kohrt &

Hruschka, 2010). A Karen key informant, Mr P, hypothesised that heart-mind distress

resulting from trauma could result in some functional impairment:

I was imprisoned as a political prisoner. After I was released I experienced nightmares

all the time when I was in Burma. When I came to Australia I even experienced the

nightmares for another three years. At night I dreamt about being in prison. I was very

angry in the middle of the night, [I thought] “why do you keep on troubling me, I’m in

a free country already, why do you keep troubling me here? That was my bad

experience in my past” [laughs]. But now I am not troubled by them any more … In

our country we don’t know how to get assistance for it [mental health], this is a

normal life. Only when we are here because of the information given to us we know

that we need help, to get help regarding our mental health. Mental health in our

country is poorly looked after … Karen people just think it’s a normal life. But

actually it’s not. It will affect them, their quality of performance. If they have bad

dreams all the time they can’t perform well at work or study, they can’t concentrate.

(Key informant – Mr P)

To summarise, women experienced memories and nightmares of their suffering(s)

(including loss, trauma and daily stress) but did not perceive these to be problems and

did not pathologise their experience. It appeared that Karen cultural wisdom prescribed

that it was nonsensical to dwell on unhelpful emotions, thoughts or experiences (e.g.,

nightmares). This seemed to promote a pragmatic approach to how women experienced

the psychological sequelae of loss and trauma.

9.4.5 “I want to die by crying”: severe distress

More severe heart-mind distress (e.g., tha dha hsar / sickness of the heart mind) was

experienced and expressed through the same somatic and social manifestations as

common states of suffering (e.g., hso k’moe dha / thinking too much and common tha /

182

heart-mind distress). In addition, it was also characterised by greater overt expressions

of distress (e.g., complaining, crying and distressed “facial expressions”), self-isolation,

sensitivity to bad news and suicidal ideation. Hpä Hpaw Mu explained “they might go

into shock and even faint. Some even can’t control themselves and they lose their mind

and become dha blu [mad]”.

In the following quotation Lah Luh describes wanting to die by crying. She experienced

considerable physical health problems, social stressors and emotional distress, and her

peers identified her as suffering from tha dha hsar. Her words reflect the confusion she

felt in defining her experience:

Sometimes I even think I want to die. I came here and now I have so many problems

… Because of [my son’s bad behaviour] I have a lot of headaches. I hso k’moe dha

[think too much] … I feel tired and weak, my heartbeat is very fast. Sometimes I cry

and I think that if I cry out loud and burst out I will die. I want to die by crying. I cry

out loud but I am still not dying … I’m not sure how to give a name to this problem,

I’m just suffering … Is it because of the hso k’moe dha? Or am I having physical

problems? … I’m not sure whether this is tha dha hsar or not. I did ask my doctor if I

have tha dha hsar and he said no. I am afraid of having tha dha hsar. I don’t want to

have that.

Why don’t you want to have tha dha hsar?

I don’t know someone who suffers from tha dha hsar, what does that look like? How

do they feel? I don’t know … I know that I am suffering but I don’t know how to

name it. (Lah Luh)

9.4.6 Feeling fierce: tha dha hsar and anger

Anger was another distinguishing feature of more severe emotional distress and women

described tha dha hsar as being characterised by feeling “fierce” and “hateful”. Some

Karen key informants emphasised the role of persecutory and traumatic experiences in

causing anger and emotional distress. Here too these informants were all fluent English

speakers with greater exposure to Western worldviews including models of mental

health and trauma. They often did not refer specifically to Karen idioms of distress

unless explicitly asked to do so. Rather, they spoke of “unresolved” issues relating to

war, especially “trauma” and ongoing anger. They expanded the meaning of tha dha

hsar beyond its common use by Karen women. For example, Mr P, a Karen key

183

informant, explained that there are both positive and negative sides to tha dha hsar. He

depicted it as the energy and passion that provided purpose and fuelled survival through

extremely difficult circumstances. However, he also illustrated an emotional or mental

flipside and described the suffering caused by an inability to forget injustice and the

desire “to kill … want revenge”. Mr P emphasised that forgiveness and healing was

critical to psychological wellbeing and necessary for overcoming the negative aspects of

tha dha hsar. In the following quotation, recounting his own personal journey, he

clearly illustrates the role of forgiveness for his recovery:

Tha dha hsar … it keeps on encouraging you to live on – that’s one thing … That’s

why I survived, without that I wouldn’t have survived … But [it] also emotionally

hurts you because you can’t forgive, you are never free … Even me for example,

when I came out of prison my main objective was to get back at them … One of the

pastors asked me, “If General Ney Win [former Prime Minister of Burma] came to

you and let you chop off his neck, would you chop it off?”. I said, “Oh no – more than

one cut – I will chop and chop”. But now that sort of mentality is gone for me and

then it made me free. We only know that one day the injustice will be gone and

freedom and justice will prevail. As Christians or as Karen people we have to stand

for justice and freedom, this is our duty, not out of hatred not out of tha dha hsar. That

is our duty. Also, the International Declaration of Human Rights [says that] all are

created equal, so we have all equal rights, this is universal rule of law. Only then I can

free myself emotionally. (Key informant – Mr P)

Mr P described tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind), and specifically the

component of anger, as an instrument of survival and social change. Other oppressed

and disenfranchised groups have similarly used their expressions of anger as tools for

social movements (e.g., civil and gay rights). In many circumstances anger is a

justifiable response to discriminatory sociopolitical conditions but it can also lead to a

“social psychology of victimhood” (Bumiller, 1988, p. 3). Mr P warned that such anger

must eventually subside. As an educated community leader he was easily able to engage

in advocacy for Karen refugees and directly oppose the oppression of the people of

Burma. He said he had forgiven his oppressors and that his continued commitment to

social change was now fuelled by his enthusiasm for the International Declaration of

Human Rights. But resolution of anger and forgiveness might be harder for community

members who are deprived of social and political roles within their own disenfranchised

184

communities. As Dr F’s words below illustrate, when there is no alterative way of

channelling or venting anger it could result in women feeling “helpless”:

A lot of them have been through trauma [but] trauma need not be pathologised – not

everyone will have PTSD. We don’t have to pathologise … These women say “I’m

very angry”. [One Karen woman was talking about] how she had to flee because of

military onslaught and how she lost her son. She was talking about the whole village

getting burnt down, even the livestock. [She talked about] the scare techniques and the

gang rapes, you know like the women being raped, and the husbands being taken as

porters, enslaved there. She had all those sort of things happen and then she came here

and she was very angry. Angry about how the military dictator can do this ethnic

cleansing … She’s very angry about it but she said “There’s nothing I can do. I want

to retaliate but I feel helpless”. (Key informant – Dr F)

Numerous Karen key informants conceptualised anger as a central characteristic of

more severe emotional distress. They described a severely emotionally distressed

woman who had survived a violent traumatic experience in Burma and afterwards

began to physically assault her family members. Mr W, a Karen key informant,

described witnessing Karen women crying and banging their heads against walls, which

he attributed to “suppression of their anger”. His description suggests that women turn

on themselves when they have no other means of externally expressing or acting upon

their distress:

They had to run for their lives over there … [it’s] about injustice. Unjustly being

treated and not being able to express their own emotional pain and stress. The

emotional outlet has been blocked. They never had a chance to express [their pain].

(Key informant – Mr W)

Mr K, another Karen key informant, explained that although Karen women generally

attributed the cause of their distress to their social world, he felt that an important

underlying issue of anger and injustice remained. Similarly, Dr N reported that some

Karen refugee patients “are angry, the hatred is there but they try to avoid [expressing

it]”. Mr M emphasised anger as an effect of war and also explained that it could

manifest as ethnic tension. His words illustrate how forgiveness is a necessary aspect of

coping with anger and demonstrate the relationship between unforgiveness and mental

illness as well as ethnic tension:

185

Emotional effects [of the war and fighting] – the first thing I can see is hatred. That is

the most obvious thing … They can’t let it go, even though I preach [that] we have to

forgive. Even some people say, I can accept this but I can’t do it [forgive]. (Key

informant – Mr M)

9.4.7 “Eating grass”: manifestations of madness (dha blu)

Individuals with dha blu (mental illness or madness) were described as “crazy” people

who “eat grass”, “walk around naked” and “won’t even remember their husband or

children”. Like sufferers of tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind), they were also

considered at risk of suicide. In addition, dha blu was described as stigmatising for

sufferers and their families.

Participants described having seen people with dha blu in Burma and in refugee camps

but explained that no Karen in Sydney suffered this problem. They attributed this to the

pre-migration screening process saying: “I don’t think Australia will take you … they

will screen you very good” and “if you have something like that … you can’t come

in”10. When participants recalled instances of dha blu (mental illness or madness) the

individuals they described were often married women experiencing severe marital

difficulties or men whose wives and children had died. As with other idioms of distress,

some key informants gave more detailed descriptions and occasionally compared and

contrasted emic (Karen) and etic (Western) idioms of distress. They explained that the

duration of symptoms or behavioural change determined whether someone was

considered to have dha blu or a less severe, acute grief reaction. Participants were not

optimistic that a person suffering from dha blu could ever recover.

9.5 Summary and reflections

9.5.1 Trajectory of distress

This chapter has explored the collection of idioms women used to talk about distress.

Women did not actively impose a formalised classification system in regard to their

emotional world. Rather, they described a trajectory of emotional distress (see Figure 4)

10 The Australian Government’s Humanitarian Program does not specifically exclude applicants with

mental illness. Applicants must be free from tuberculosis and any disease or condition that is a risk to

public health and safety (UNHCR, 2004).

186

from hso k’moe dha (thinking too much) through various states of tha (heart-mind)

distress such as tha ba yo (worry) and tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind),

culminating in dha blu (madness or mental illness). As the trajectory of distress

suggests, Karen women’s conceptualisations of suffering could not be separated into

“discrete Linnaean classification schemata” (Guarnaccia et al., 2003, p. 340) but rather

overlapped and encompassed the experience of suffering in many forms. Normality and

abnormality appeared to be relative terms and, in the Karen context, existed on a

continuum.

Most common and least severe Least common and most severe Hso k’moe dha Tha… Tha dha hsar Dha blu

Thinking too much

(Common) Heart-mind distress

Sickness of the heart-mind

Crazy

Figure 4: Trajectory of distress

As I learnt women’s conceptualisations and sought to understand their idioms of

distress I initially fell into a trap whereby I attempted to neatly match expressions with

translations and tidily group these representations. Ultimately, I came to understand the

trajectory or continuum inherent in women’s discussions. I also came to understand that

their idioms were ambiguous in nature; each had multiple interpretations. Linguistically,

the collection of idioms provided the foundation for discourse on a variety of forms of

suffering and violence. The effects of poverty were described with the same terms used

to express physical disease of the heart organ. The loss of a child, a brutal murder or the

disappearance of a loved one was conveyed with the same expressions used to address

marital conflict and social isolation.

Women’s idioms of distress were embedded in the physical, gendered and socio-

political circumstances of their lives. The most significant of these was the social and

their sense of wellbeing emanated from their collective connections with others; their

interpersonal and emotional worlds were closely tied. Women believed (according to

their experience and cultural wisdom) that some degree of emotional affliction was

normal in the context of their lives as persecuted people. Consequently, experiences

such as nightmares were not deemed abnormal or necessarily concerning.

187

Karen women experienced emotional distress through the body (e.g., physical aches and

pains) and the mind (e.g., thinking too much or remembering past suffering) but

avoided a Cartesian division of the two. Rather, their heart-minds were hurt (e.g., tha ba

do / hurt heart-mind). They also expressed their distress somatically (e.g., physical

complaints such as dizziness) and socially (e.g., reporting problems and grieving

openly). Guarnaccia et al. (2003) note that such “somatopsychosocial” (p. 348)

conceptualisations and culturally prescribed patterns occur in many cultures, but differ

in terms of the “particular configuration of experiences” (p. 348). For Karen women, the

perceived severity of a loss or traumatic experience – and in turn the expected and

accepted level of overt expression of distress – appeared to be largely determined by the

social implications of the event. Women who broke from accepted patterns of

experiencing and expressing distress and engaged in culturally inappropriate (e.g.,

excessive overt expression of distress or anger) or abnormal (e.g., disoriented behaviour

such as eating grass and talking nonsense) behaviour risked stigma and being labelled

crazy (dha blu).

9.5.2 Similarities and differences

There are similarities between hso k’moe dha (thinking too much) and idioms of distress

in other cultures. Thinking too much (fikuru boduvun) has been documented as an idiom

of distress amongst women in the Maldives (Razee, 2006). In South East Asia the same

idiom is commonly reported amongst Cambodian (Frye & D’Avanzo, 1994; D. E.

Hinton et al., 2010) and Karenni refugees (Lopes Cardozo et al., 2004). The idioms

padecer de los nervios (suffering from nerves) and estar enfermo de los nervios (being

ill with nerves) described among Caribbean Latinos are characterised by having “too

many thoughts” (Guarnaccia et al., 2003, p. 351). Similarly, D. Pedersen et al. (2010)

describe the Quechua idiom pinsamientuwan (full of worrying thoughts) as a key

category of distress linking worrying thoughts to more severe states of distress.

Moreover, factor analysis of Chinese and non-Chinese Australian participants’

responses to the 35-item Depressive Symptom Questionnaire has indicated that

“thinking too much” (Parker et al., 2006, p. 144) is part of a group of core symptoms

accounting for greater variance than either somatic or cognitive (e.g., anhedonia and

thinking about death) symptoms. Karen women’s notion of a separate though

interrelated heart-mind also parallel conceptualisations of distress reported in Uganda

(Okello & Ekblad, 2006) and Zimbabwe (Patel, Musara, Butau, Maramba, & Fuyane,

188

1995). In Cambodia, researchers have reported a similar progression of mental distress

entailed by “thinking too much” and leading to “broken down heart-mind” (van de Put

& Eisenbruch, 2004, p. 137).

It is noteworthy that in contrast to other research with South East Asian refugees (Kohrt

& Hruschka, 2010; Liamputtong Rice, 1999a) no participants in this study reported

karma (the Buddhist concept of good or bad deeds determining prosperity or suffering)

as a factor influencing vulnerability to negative life events or emotional distress. It is

also important to note that participants overwhelmingly did not report traditional Karen

animist beliefs in spirits or souls. The absence of key Buddhist and animist beliefs is

most probably explained by the long-held Christian faith of many community members.

Large-scale Christian conversion occurred in Vietnamese and Khmer refugee camps as

refugees scrambled to access Christian-sponsored resources and resettlement

opportunities (Horstmann, 2010; McLellan, 2004; Smith-Hefner, 1994). Post-

resettlement conversion to Christianity was also common amongst Hmong refugees

(Winland, 1994). It seems likely that these later converts would retain aspects of their

culture’s traditional religious beliefs. In contrast, the Karen are not new to Christianity

and many refugees are likely to have been raised in Christian homes. As Horstmann

(2010) notes: “the Christian Karen are old Christians who have a tradition of

proselytizing among their own ethnic group and other ethnic minorities” (p. 8).

9.5.3 The ubiquity of uselessness

Women’s expressions of heart-mind distress often represented “commentaries on a

social world out of control” (Guarnaccia et al., 2003, p. 348). Women felt “useless” in

their social roles and in their daily lives in Australia. The salience and configuration of

suffering through this idiom is unique and interesting, particularly in the Karen context

of persecution, protracted camp confinement and forced migration. This short plaintive

expression says much about Karen refugee women’s social position. This phrase may

also convey the personal experience of what key informants termed the sponsored

dependency of protracted camp confinement. As we saw in Chapter 8 (see Section 8.5),

Karen elders lamented the societal effects of refugee camp institutionalisation and spoke

of disempowerment, low self-efficacy, apathy and sponsored dependency.

189

The notions of uselessness, power(lessness) and loss of self-efficacy share features with

Seligman’s (1975) theory of learned helplessness model which postulates that people

whose efforts to control or improve their lives have been futile may respond by giving

up. Learned helplessness refers to “the feeling that no amount of effort can lead to

success” (Eggen & Kauchak, 1999, p. 412) and may result from repeated failure at

tasks. Few studies have specifically explored the notion of learned helplessness in

relation to refugee populations (John-Langba, 2004). Other research has, however,

noted the potential relevance of the framework for refugee mental health (Colic-Peisker

& Tilbury, 2003; Goran, 2005; Nicassio, 1985). The learned helplessness model seems

particularly suited to contributing to understanding findings from this study with Karen

women where the issues of protracted camp confinement, low self-efficacy, sponsored

dependency and feeling useless were critical to understanding community and

individual wellbeing. Moreover, there is now a substantial body of literature supporting

this model which refugee mental health researchers might draw on in developing further

insights in order to delve deeper into understanding different dimensions, experiences

and needs of refugee populations.

The concepts of demoralisation, entrapment and defeat may provide further useful

frameworks for conceptualising Karen women’s accounts of feeling useless and

distressed. Demoralisation has been described as “one reaction people exhibit when

basic defensive maneuvers – symbolic mastery, escape, or attack – seem futile against a

serious threat” (Frank & Frank, 1991, p. 445; Kissane, 2001; Kissane, Clarke, & Street,

2001). Few studies have explored demoralisation in relation to refugee wellbeing.

However, Briggs and Macleod’s (2006) findings support the notion that, amongst

refugees, depression and demoralisation may indeed be different constructs. The notion

of demoralisation also shares features with Gilbert and Allan’s (1998) argument that

entrapment and defeat (specifically failure in conflict situations) are important factors in

explaining psychological distress. I was unable to locate further published

investigations of Gilbert and Allan’s model in refugee mental health literature aside

from Bhugra’s (2004) suggestion that their model might be useful to refugee

psychology.

190

9.5.4 The importance of anger

Participants conceptualised anger (both internalised as hate and externalised as

aggression or suicidal ideation) as a distinguishing feature of more serious states of

emotional distress. These findings have implications for healthcare professionals as,

among Karen, anger may be a better indicator of emotional distress than Western

symptoms such as nightmares. These findings also resonate with anthropology research

documenting cross-cultural differences in the meaning assigned to experiences and

expressions of anger (Briggs, 1970; Lindholm, 2005). Research with Cambodian (D. E.

Hinton, Hsia, Um, & Otto, 2003; D. E. Hinton, Rasmussen, Nou, Pollack, & Good,

2009; Nickerson & Hinton, 2011) and other (Basoglu et al., 2005; Lopes Cardozo,

Kaiser, Gotway, & Agini, 2003) refugee populations also suggests that anger is a

common expression of distress for refugees experiencing psychological problems.

Research with Western trauma survivors has similarly documented a relationship

between anger and PTSD (Chemtob, Hamada, Roitblat, & Muraoka, 1994; Nickerson &

Hinton, 2011). For the Karen, the experience of anger appeared to pose a dilemma. On

the one hand expressions of anger contravene accepted and expected cultural modes of

communication. On the other hand suppression of anger is thought to be harmful and

pose risks to one’s emotional or mental wellbeing. Disentangling this scenario may help

to further understand the emotional experiences and expressions of Karen women.

9.5.5 Balancing ethnopsychiatries

Table 2 provides a summary of the trajectory of distress described in this chapter and

presents a synopsis of the causes, expressions and experiences associated with each

idiom. Like Gaines (1992) and Guarnaccia et al. (2003) I encourage a cultural

constructivist approach to mental health research and intervention which views Western

psychiatry and indigenous ethnopsychiatries as equally culturally constructed.

Nevertheless, I also agree with Miller and Rasco (2004) who contend that “although it is

important to avoid cultural and psychological imperialism, we must be careful not to go

to the opposite extreme and romanticize all things indigenous” (p. 406). In this chapter I

have sought to take seriously and respect Karen women’s conceptualisations of

suffering and the experiences that contribute to their distress without romanticising their

emic beliefs and practices. Kohrt and Hruschka (2010) also caution that idioms of

distress:

191

may easily become a vocabulary list without a cohesive framework detailing the

significance of these terms and how they fit into a broader understanding of

suffering and wellbeing in a given cultural context. (p. 326)

Women’s conceptualisations of emotional distress reflected their cultural understanding

that suffering is a universal and inevitable aspect of life. In recent decades Karen culture

and society has encountered rapid change; unsurprisingly I found that participants

expressed “a strange amalgam of modern and traditional beliefs, values, and

institutions, held together in varying patterns of assimilation, complementarity, conflict

and contradiction” (Kleinman, 1980, p. 37). The findings demonstrate that Karen

women’s explanations and experiences of emotional distress reflect cultural values (e.g.,

collectivist social attitudes) and beliefs (e.g., Buddhist and Christian) within Karen

society and are embedded in the context of sustained persecution. Women appeared to

hold a sociocentric and interdependent conceptualisation of self, a notion shared by

other collectivist societies (Geertz, 1979; Hollan, 2010). This collectivist orientation

could explain women’s concern with social harmony and emphasis on interpersonal

explanations for suffering.

192

Table 2: Karen women’s trajectory of idioms of distress with causes and descriptions

Trajectory

of distress

Most common and least severe Least common and most severe

Hso k’moe dha Tha… Tha dha hsar Dha blu

Thinking too much Common heart-mind distress Sickness of the heart-mind Crazy

Idiom Hso k’moe dha Thinking too much

Tha… Common heart-mind distress (e.g., tha ba do /

hurt, tha ta mu / upset)

Tha dha hsar Sickness of the heart-mind

Dha blu Crazy

Causes Daily stressors (marital conflict, parenting problems, daily resettlement concerns, worry for those left behind and discrimination) Loss and traumatic war experiences

Daily stressors with emphasis on resettlement stressors especially language, responsibilities of women and interpersonal relationship difficulties (e.g., marital discord and social rejection) Loss, grief (“series of losses”) and trauma (with emphasis on losses and traumas with negative, serious and often permanent social consequences)

Daily stressors with emphasis on marital and family conflict Loss and trauma with emphasis on losses and traumas with negative, serious and often permanent social consequences Inherent vulnerability towards emotional instability

Unresolved tha dha hsar Trauma (witnessing or surviving murder) Genetic or inherited predisposition Supernatural (e.g., witchcraft, spirits and curses)

Experience and expression

Physical complaints (headaches, sleeping difficulties, loss of appetite, lethargy, tachycardia, aches and pains, and high blood pressure)

Physical complaints with emphasis on feeling shaky, dizzy, chest pain and poor blood circulation Focus on social cause of distress Feeling “useless”

Physical complaints Focus on social cause on distress Feeling “useless” Crying and other overt expressions of distress Wanting to die Anger, hatred or desire for revenge

Incoherent speech, talking nonsense Abnormal, disoriented behaviour (e.g., public nakedness or eating grass)

193

Chapter 10

We All Suffer. We All Cope: Managing Emotional Distress

10.1 Introduction

This chapter addresses the findings related to the question: How do Karen women

cope with emotional distress? Four categories reflect women’s coping strategies: (a)

self-control, (b) collective self, (c) religion, and (d) professional support. Certain

cultural, spiritual and sociopolitical themes – including those introduced in Chapters 3

and 8 – are reflected throughout women’s discussions on how they deal with distress.

10.2 Self-control

Women firmly located responsibility for coping with emotional distress within

themselves and spoke of the need to “control”, “console” and “calm” oneself when

suffering. This approach is captured in the following quotation from an interview with

Htee Gloh Poh:

I don’t think anyone can help, this just depends on yourself. If you have too much

worry and think a lot you will suffer. You have to control yourself and talk to

yourself; you need to calm yourself down. (Htee Gloh Poh)

Being “strong”, not complaining, concealing or suppressing distress and seeking to

resolve one’s problems before asking others were highly valued virtues. Women

generally had confidence in their own personal strength as well as that of other

women and the wider Karen community and emphasised that Karen women “do not

complain” but simply get on with their daily lives. Women described using several

intrapersonal, cognitive strategies to achieve self-control. In the following sections

these are grouped according to the themes of (a) thought control, (b) distraction, and

(c) acceptance.

10.2.1 Thought control: “think about your thinking”

Women used culturally commonsensical strategies such as thinking about one’s

thinking and then seeking to curb unhelpful thoughts. One group participant

explained: “for me I never keep on thinking things that will make me sick or become

194

a problem, I never think of it”. Others in the group nodded in agreement but also

explained that such willpower is easier for some than others.

Leaders reiterated women’s reports and explained that the Karen were familiar with

questioning whether their thought patterns are beneficial and then attempting to

change unhelpful patterns through willpower and recognition of the harm that

unhelpful thinking can cause. Such conscious analysis of the value and quality of

one’s thoughts was promoted as a venerated approach to coping with distress. Mr M,

a Karen key informant, described this cultural wisdom as the primary antidote to hso

k’moe dha (thinking too much): “they put it into practice they think ‘does it work if I

keep feeling like that?’”.

10.2.2 Distraction: “think about something else”

Women also commonly used distraction to avoid troubling thoughts and emotional

distress. By keeping their minds occupied with other activities they found temporary

relief from their suffering. Approaches to distraction included explicitly solitary

activities, some of which were religion-oriented (e.g., praying or reading the bible)

and some of which were not (e.g., reading a secular book or watching television).

Women also distracted themselves through their religious and secular interpersonal

connections.

Both women and Karen key informants implied that some methods of distraction

(e.g., media, gambling, sex and alcohol) were “not the right way” and considered

them to be less desirable than sheer willpower or religion-oriented approaches. This is

reflected in the following quotation from an interview with Mr M, a Karen leader:

They try to forget about [their problems] … by watching movies in Thai, Burmese,

even Korean … Even I find myself [distracting] this way. Whenever I have a lot of

pressure when I look at the TV I forget about that. I know that’s not the right way.

(Key informant – Mr M)

Opportunities for “quick fix” distractions though frowned upon strategies had

proliferated in resettlement. Accessible movies, television and pokies were a recent

phenomena for many Karen. Most were originally farmers or rural villagers prior to

spending years or even decades in refugee camps where access to technology was

195

very limited though substances such as betel nut and alcohol were readily available

(see Section 3.4.2).

In contrast to the “negative” distraction strategies, community-oriented and

interpersonal distractions were generally described in wholly positive terms. Women

described “keeping busy” by visiting friends and family, and joining in community

and religious activities. This helped them to distract from their problems and

overcome or avoid isolation. Moreover, through their connections they found

opportunities to help others, which in itself was viewed as a coping strategy (see

Section 10.3.3). In the following quotation Dr V captures the various benefits of

distraction through connection:

Coping strategy? They keep themselves busy. I encourage them to join any

volunteer groups just to keep them busy. Those who are depressed or have anxiety

tend to see and think negatively once they have spare moment. Some of the

survivors of domestic violence and past trauma are now quite happily involved with

their own religious group activities. They feel part of the community and part of the

events. It is quite a rewarding experience to see them doing something for others –

which helps them at the same time. (Key informant – Dr V)

These solitary and collective approaches to coping were implicit in women’s

discussions though they did not explicitly distinguish between them. Collective or

interpersonal approaches to coping are explored in greater detail in Section 10.3.

10.2.3 Acceptance: “you have to accept reality”

Women also coped through acceptance and frequently spoke of “accepting the

reality” of their situations. In the following quotation, Hpä Hpaw Mu articulates this

widespread commonsense approach to coping with distress:

You talked about your experiences after your son died. Did you have tha dha

hsar [sickness of the heart-mind] then?

For those who are not able to control their mind they will get tha dha hsar … For

example if one of your loved ones dies, you need to accept the reality. You need to

know that we are just human beings, we are born one day and we will die one day,

acceptance. (Hpä Hpaw Mu)

196

Women described coping through accepting both negative internal (physical,

psychological and emotional) and external (e.g., interpersonal and environmental)

experiences as unavoidable aspects of human life.

10.2.4 Contextualising a controlled self

To better understand the importance, complexity and role of self-control for the

Karen, it is necessary to examine their religious and sociopolitical context.

Karen women’s matter-of-fact approach to self-control through (a) sheer willpower,

(b) distraction, and (c) the application of cultural beliefs regarding the acceptance of

suffering shares distinct similarities with Buddhist principles and teachings which

promote:

(a) adopting an observational attitude to affective and mental processing, (b)

controlling attentional focus, [and] (c) maintaining a present-oriented mental

state (in contrast to a past-oriented mental state, e.g., anxious expectation). (D.

E. Hinton, Safren, & Pollack, 2006, p. 279)

Kleinman (1995) argued that all of the world’s major religions consider human

misery as “a defining condition of people’s existential plight” (p. 181). However,

acceptance of the “ubiquity of human suffering” (Hayes, 2002, p. 58) appears to be

made most explicit in Buddhist doctrine (Kinzie & Friedman, 2004; Levine, 2006;

Rotem, 2003). In her research with Cambodian refugees, Liamputtong Rice (1999a)

described “acceptance and detachment” as “distinctly Buddhist approaches to mental

health” (p. 215). However, it is important not to over-emphasise religious

characteristics as the central mechanism steering women’s approaches to coping. It is

seductive and easy to fall into the trap of conceptualising refugee communities as

culturally static (P. Guerin & Guerin, 2007) and reducible to traditional

characteristics. Eisenbruch (1983) also warned against making assumptions about the

importance and relevance of traditional or religious belief systems in the lives of

migrants. He wrote:

Individual patients are usually unaware of the details of their “great tradition”,

whereas the well-intentioned Western observer, having read the classic texts

… may superimpose his own construction of Eastern beliefs upon his

197

preexistent Western medical viewpoint. This may well distort, rather than

clarify. (p. 325)

Karen culture is dynamic and currently in rapid transition as a result of forced

migration and the majority of participants did not conceptualise acceptance as a

religious or spiritual practice. Karen culture is, historically, firmly rooted in a

Buddhist tradition and though many Karen refugees are now Christian, long-held

Buddhist beliefs appear to be reflected in their cultural wisdom. It is sometimes

difficult, and perhaps artificial, to make distinctions between culture and religion as

the two are so deeply entwined and multiple religious perspectives so readily

combined amongst the Karen (Platz, 2003) and Burmese (Pfanner, 1966).

A sociopolitical lens provides further insight into the origin and role of self-control,

acceptance and stoic silence. The findings concerning the importance of stoic silence

and self-control resonate with research from anthropology and psychology which

demonstrates that Asian and other collectivist cultures encourage emotional

suppression more than Western-European populations (Butler, Lee, & Gross, 2007;

Goodman, 2004; Liamputtong Rice, 1999b; Wierzbicka, 1994).

In collectivist societies powerful expressions of distress or anger are typically

discouraged and behaviour kept in order by the threat of shame to the guilty party as

well as her family (Gudykunst & Bond, 1999; Hofstede, 2001). Karen key informants

similarly located cultural attitudes towards the expression of affect within this

religious and cultural milieu:

This is a cultural issue, Paula. Purely cultural issue. Even the Karen women who are

Christians, the Buddhist teaching is deeply rooted in the society. So that’s the reason

they don’t express their, I think, real feelings and real emotions in front of the other

people … they never or they never shout or cry … Karen women never do that.

(Key informant – Mr J)

Karen women similarly conceptualised the practices of controlling one’s thinking and

expressions of distress as a means of avoiding shame and stigmatisation. Expressed

psychological distress was seen as a weakness and women reported that it was

“shameful” to indulge in talking about one’s own distress (except perhaps to one’s

closest confidant). Stoic silence demonstrated strength and women expressed

198

admiration for people who showed patience and endurance in the face of adversity.

As discussed in Chapter 9 (see Section 9.4.2), women who overtly expressed distress

or reacted excessively to a situation considered traumatic were likely to be labelled

“crazy”; a label they would carry “for the rest of their life”:

We have this conception that if you are feeling something you don’t show it, you

don’t express it, you sort of keep it to yourself … By showing emotional pain or

stress or anxiety, it’s sort of [like], people see that it’s a weakness. So when a

woman starts to, you know, behave outside of that perception then they would

automatically say that she is having a mental breakdown, so it’s sort of like she’s

going crazy. That’s why people don’t often show their distress. (Key informant –

Ms K)

Some key informants described self-control, and particularly stoic silence and

acceptance, as a “kind of denial” creating “hidden problems inside”. They were

concerned about the potential ramifications of not expressing distress and thought that

women’s ability to cope could in fact be hindered by unexpressed and unresolved

emotional suffering.

The development and strengthening of a “controlled self” may also be seen as a

consequence of (and an approach to managing) conflict and persecution. In short,

stoic silence and self-control may be necessary for survival. Within the Karen context

of persecution, overt expression of distress could easily disturb others and agitate the

insecure political environment. Unstable and overtly distressed individuals could

place the safety of the wider community in jeopardy. These factors are reflected in the

following quotation from an interview with Ms K, a Karen key informant:

I think it’s more than [loss of] face … Because we have been in war, if you look at

the history generation after generation have been through war so people have to be

strong. It’s become hit into your head so much that you don’t show weakness, you

have to be strong for your family … Karen people are very collective people they

are always together, do stuff, work together. So if one person doesn’t show

weakness, other people won’t either. They might be doing it [suffering] quietly, you

don’t know. (Key informant – Ms K)

Stoic and phlegmatic reactions to war-related suffering are not unique to the Karen.

For example, in his World War II memoirs, Scotsman George Fraser (1992),

199

described the potential harm and social disruption of overt expressions of distress. For

Fraser “The celebrated British stiff upper lip, the resolve to conceal emotion which is

not only embarrassing and useless, but harmful, is just plain common sense” (1992, p.

89).

Protracted war is likely to have influenced Karen women’s beliefs about, and

approaches to coping with, distress and suffering in other ways also. As we saw in the

previous chapter (see Sections 9.3.2 and 9.4.4) women accepted sleeping difficulties,

nightmares and sadness as normal given their experience of persecution and forced

migration. Experiences such as nightmares were described as “very small things”

while, as Mr P, a Karen key informant, noted: “the big thing is to survive”. Mr P

located the experience of nightmares and emotional distress within a context in which

daily survival needs remained a priority, consequently reducing the perceived impact

and attention paid to sufferings less vital to maintaining life. This understanding is

analogous to Maslow’s (1954) hierarchy of needs. Maslow postulated that basic

physical needs must be adequately addressed before individuals and communities

address other aspects of human life. The historically recent Western pastime of

analysing one’s emotional experiences is not likely to be relevant in situations where

survival is the number one priority. As Ms K, a Karen key informant, reported:

The Karen people who were born on the borderline before they went into the camp,

normally they have lived through the whole revolution, the whole civil war, they

might consider that this is you fighting for your freedom and everyone goes through

it, they don’t see any effect. Not that it isn’t affecting them, but they just don’t have

the time to analyse or process it, they are just trying to live. Trying to live and trying

to cope. (Key informant – Ms K)

Mere survival has dominated life for Karen refugees, thus reducing opportunities for

intensive self-reflection. Resettlement in Australia may be viewed as another phase of

learning to “survive”. Many participants still struggled with daily living, poverty and

the acquisition of skills required to simply survive in the Australian context (see

Chapter 8). Those who were focused on addressing their daily needs were perhaps

unlikely to have the time or inclination to analyse their emotional worlds. Moreover,

hyper-analysis of suffering is not a feature of Karen culture. Their emic models of

distress and mental illness (see Chapter 9) dictated that thinking about one’s suffering

200

was nonsensical and was both a cause and symptom of mental illness. Emotional

suffering such as periods of sadness were normalised and generally regarded as likely

to “settle down” with the passing of time. An understanding of these various issues

helps to contextualise women’s approaches to coping, particularly their emphasis on

self-control.

10.3 Collective self

A sense of collective solidarity and communal identity or self was also implicit in

women’s approaches to coping.

10.3.1 The collective self and communal suffering: accepting and normalising

suffering

The concept of acceptance was extended beyond a diffuse acknowledgment of human

suffering into a specific acceptance and normalisation of the suffering of the Karen

people. Women were able to normalise their own adversities and distress within that

of their peers and reached acceptance through this shared suffering. For example,

when Lah Luh described witnessing the brutal murder of her family she did not

ruminate over the severity of her experience or her suffering. Rather, she concluded:

“my experience was the same as other refugees”. Karen key informants reiterated that

women found some relief in knowing they were not alone in their suffering:

Traditionally Karen women are very quiet. And, they strongly believe that people

always suffer from different kinds of things. So, they are not alone. So, let’s say

[their] husband [is] killed, or maybe [their] children [are] killed, they are thinking

“Ah, I am not alone”. (Key informant – Mr J)

Suffering was not only accepted within women’s cultural worldview, it was their

dominant sociopolitical experience. Participants emphasised that encountering

extremely traumatic and difficult experiences was the normal state of affairs and had

been so for generations of Karen. It was accepted and normalised because it was the

normal experience, as clearly reflected below in Ms K’s words:

They start to see traumatic experiences as part of their daily life, so they sort of

normalise [it] and they wouldn’t see anything [wrong with it]. It’s [not] un-normal

201

because this is what they would grow up with, their parents and their grandparents

[too]. (Key informant – Ms K)

Leaders and other Karen key informants concurred with Ms K. In the following

quotation, Mr J illustrates how one woman coped through contextualising and

normalising her suffering within that of her peers:

She said “my husband was killed by the Burmese soldiers in front of me, my

daughter was raped by many Burmese soldiers in front of me, my children were

killed by Burmese soldiers in front of me. I feel terribly sad for my family. But,

what can I do?” … Even she can say, “I can cope with that kind of situation because

nobody [is] free from suffering, so many Karen women have similar experiences

like mine”. (Key informant – Mr J)

Women often compared their loss(es) and trauma(s) to those of their peers and went

on to minimise their own adversities through explaining that certain features of other

people’s loss(es) were more severe than their own problems. Here again, women’s

accounts conveyed the degrees discourse of stress, loss and trauma (introduced in

Section 9.3.2) in which the seriousness of a stressor correlated with the negative

social implications associated with it.

The strategy of normalisation is shared by other populations affected by political

conflict for whom “collectivizing personal trauma” (Guribye, Sandal, & Oppedal,

2011, p. 2) is identified as a coping strategy. For example, Tamils perceive of

themselves within a larger traumatised community rather than as traumatised or

suffering individuals (Guribye et al., 2011; Somasundaram, 2010).

Key informants likened acceptance and normalisation to “psychological

desensitization” that made one “immune” to severe mental health problems. To these

participants such coping is quite predictable and common to war-affected populations

as reflected below:

I’d say that we all have to desensitise ourselves, we all do. Particularly those who

are coming out here as refugees. It’s a coping mechanism for them. I doubt that we

would be any different to the millions of Poles who went through the second World

War and the Nazis in the camps over there. (Key informant – Dr G)

202

The findings suggest, perhaps paradoxically, that some people may cope better with

widespread suffering than individualised and isolated negative experiences. Indeed,

the collective and universal nature of Karen women’s suffering appeared to help them

make meaning of their own personal adversities:

There are so many [people with] similar problems in the community so they think “I

am not alone”. It is a kind of solution. If a Karen woman faces that kind of problem

alone – if other women [were] free from that kind of problem – [then] maybe [she

would feel] very sad when she [looks at] other women happy and with children and

husband … [Then she would think] “Oh, I lost my husband, I lost my children, I lost

my property, my rice, my paddy, everything, you know”. But, in [the] Karen

community, most of the Karen women suffer you know, similar problems, so [she

thinks] “I am not alone. So many people suffer like that. It’s normal. So I have to

face it, I have to adapt”. It’s a kind of solution I think. (Key informant – Mr B)

However, key informants often also conveyed a flipside to the benefits of such

collective acceptance and normalisation and expressed conflicted views about its

function and value. Some hypothesised there could be psychological consequences

“happening inside” because of accepting, normalising and not expressing one’s

troubles, although they were unclear on how that might manifest:

Karen women feel “everybody suffers like that so I am not alone”. So, it’s a good

thing in one way but I feel we should try to help them because of hidden problems

inside. (Key informant – Mr J)

Key informants described acceptance as (a) a cultural characteristic of the Karen, (b)

a protective and adaptive coping strategy, and (c) a harmful consequence of forced

passivity in refugee camps. As discussed in Chapter 8 (see Section 8.5), these leaders

explained that cultural attributes such as acceptance, quietness and reluctance to

complain were strengths that had partly helped the Karen to survive war and forced

migration. However, they lamented that years of persecution and particularly

protracted camp living had warped these cultural traits into forms of fatalistic

acceptance and apathy, idleness, helplessness and emotional distress. Some leaders

also indicated that coping strategies such as acceptance were not as effective in the

resettlement context. They bemoaned instances of fatalistic acceptance and apathy

amongst community members and perceived this as a major hindrance to them

203

successfully navigating life in Australia’s “individualistic” and “competitive” society.

Their reports echo Bhugra’s (2004) argument that acculturation into individualistic

cultures may be particularly difficult for migrants from sociocentric societies.

The paradox of acceptance and normalisation was articulately described by Mr W, a

Karen key informant. He began by describing the Karen as accepting and “easy

people” who “are content” and will endure the hardship of persecution and camp

living if their basic needs are met. However, he then described the demoralised

“mindset” and dependency of those who lived in refugee camps for decades as

characterised by “lack of ambition, lack of purpose, just one day at a time”. To him

their acceptance of an inhumane and restricted existence had gone too far. Yet, Mr W

acknowledged “acceptance” of fate and taking “one day at a time” as a coping

mechanisms that enabled people to “still sing” despite their suffering:

[Acceptance and taking one day at a time] can be negative and yet can be positive

too. Why are they still singing when they don’t have eyes? They don’t have legs.

They don’t have hands. You have been there on the border, you’ve seen the

landmine victims – they are still singing. It’s heart breaking isn’t it? But inside them

there is a part that can still sing. (Key informant – Mr W)

10.3.2 The collective self and communal support: “we support one another”

Collective solidarity and the collective self also helped women cope by providing

them with access to emotional support as well as material necessities and practical

help. Within their community, Karen women described turning to family, friends and

community leaders. They reflected on such social support as a fundamental and

critical component of their wellbeing and daily survival in Australia and explained

that these support networks were generally their first point of contact for material and

practical needs such as money, childcare, transport or help translating a document.

Karen women seldom publicly expressed their distress though they described

exposing their intimate feelings to family members, same-sex peers and, occasionally,

community leaders. Women’s wider group of friends and family also provided them

with casual support via distraction through engaging in pleasurable activities such as

picnics, as well as emotional support through informal discussions of their problems

and sharing experiences. Women chatted and shared their problems informally during

204

communal activities such as food preparation. Participant observation provided

several opportunities to witness and partake in women’s collective discussions about

their problems. In these group contexts women were forthcoming in sharing their

common daily stressors (e.g., parenting) and often used humour to lighten the load of

their shared fate. The following quotation comes from an interview with Mu Htòh in

which she described a group of women discussing their marital problems whilst

cooking food and washing dishes at a community gathering:

They express their feelings sometimes, [they say] “her husband is like that”, “[my]

husband is like this”. But we cannot solve the problem, only talking, expressing

their feeling. (Mu Htòh)

In the above quotation Mu Htòh emphasised that the women were “only talking”

rather than solving the problem. This distinction was another important theme in

women’s conceptualisations of coping. It brings to light distinguishing features of

how women sought support and the type(s) of support or service they expected to

obtain. Women had difficulty articulating exactly how “only talking” helped them to

cope. Like Mu Htòh they referred to “expressing their feeling”. When “only talking”,

women occasionally received some advice, information or ideas in relation to their

own problems. However, obtaining advice on problem solving did not appear to

define the benefit women obtained from sharing experiences with their friends.

Rather, relating to each other’s “shared” experience appeared to be a critical factor for

coping through “only talking”.

Community leaders also made clear the subtle but crucial distinction between (a)

“only talking” or emotional support, and (b) practical support. They reported being

most commonly called upon to provide practical support including information,

referrals and advice but also noted that some people sought their emotional support.

When women sought support from leaders, the implicit or explicit acknowledgement

of a shared experience remained vitally important. Women valued their leaders’

advice because they had experienced comparable situations and could offer their

wisdom, experience and knowledge of how to cope. This suggests that, for Karen

women, emotional or psychological support (i.e., talking therapies) are perceived as

beneficial when provided by one who shares the same social (i.e., cultural,

experiential or political) or circumstantial experiences and understandings. Leaders

205

also explained the ways in which women expected and accepted their support.

Women were unlikely to schedule regular meetings but rather sought “informal”,

irregular contact when needed. These findings have implications for service providers

which will be explored in Chapters 12 to 14.

Karen communities have traditionally sought support from community leaders for a

variety of matters from relationship disputes to macro-level decision-making affecting

the community (H. I. Marshall, 1997). In the refugee camps, Community Elders’

Advisory Boards, in conjunction with other organisations, oversee the management of

camp communities (Thailand Burma Border Consortium, 2011a). In the Australian

resettlement context, community use of leaders also appeared to reflect the sponsored

dependency and helplessness that leaders spoke of (see Section 8.5) as many women

expected quite exhaustive support from their community leaders. Community leaders

confirmed that they had become a “one-stop shop” for resettlement needs including

immigration issues, resolving family disputes, parenting, translating documents,

arranging transport and finding housing.

The demand and need for support from community leaders was patently clear during

participant observation encounters. When I visited their homes, new requests for their

assistance would come in regularly. Community leaders were also proactive in

providing support. In some instances their help was not directly sought but, rather,

they heard of people or families in difficulty and consequently visited their home.

During those visits the specific problem or suffering would not always be explicitly

discussed. Sometimes the leaders would simply provide a social visit and all present

would pray together, an approach depicted in the following quotation from an

interview with a Karen leader:

Even when there is a problem between kids and mum or kids and dad and we hear

about that sometimes we go. We don’t say anything about it [the problem], we just

go and pray with them and give support. (Key informant – Mr P)

In the following quotation another leader, Mr H, acknowledged the role of religion in

the support provided by elders, as well as the importance of shared experience:

What sort of help would they be seeking?

206

To console them … maybe through words and maybe through some, how do you

say, some treatment we will say – you know, being friendly in manner, empathy and

sympathy, something like that, consoling words with some scripture or something

like that … Not only with these scripture things, they [the elders] console them with

sharing their experience, what they have been through, what they think, how they

coped with the situation, that sort of thing. (Key informant – Mr H)

10.3.3 “Sharing and caring”: helping others to help oneself

Social engagements in the Karen community often involved activities directed at

helping others. For example, most community celebrations aimed to raise funds

(through food stalls) to send back to people in Burma or on the Thailand-Burma

border. As discussed (see Section 8.3.3), women also personally sent money back to

help friends and family. In addition, women helped fellow Karen in Australia with

financial support, accommodation, understanding correspondence, childcare, transport

and language difficulties. During the course of the research some Karen women (and

other community members) also travelled back to the border temporarily in order to

help and share professional skills and knowledge they had acquired in Australia.

Women explained their desire to help and their happiness to do so though a few also

acknowledged the stress this placed on them and their families.

Seems like I’m a busy person now – no time to stay home. After I drop the children

at school someone will call, definitely someone will call. They ask “will you be

available to accompany me somewhere?” Even today, the neighbour has asked

whether I will be available this evening. One of his sons has an appointment and

wants to go and look for the venue tonight so he doesn’t get lost tomorrow. I want

to help people wherever I can. (Ta Heh Sōo)

Because of her English language proficiency Ta Heh Sōo was often asked to spend

entire days assisting community members. She frequently acted as an unofficial

interpreter at hospital and GP visits. Whilst Ta Heh Sōo sometimes found this

demanding, she also appeared to gain pleasure from helping and a sense of self-worth

from possessing skills that others valued.

Karen key informants highlighted the value of helping others as an important feature

of Karen cultural approaches to coping and healing. This may be understood as an

207

extension of a collectivist culture whereby “maintaining the group’s wellbeing is the

best guarantee for the individual” (Hofstede, 2001, p. 211). It is also a distinct

characteristic of Buddhist (Nash, 1966) and Christian (Wuthnow, 1991) religious

traditions, which are prominent among Karen. One Karen key informant who had

suffered imprisonment and torture due to his political opinions spoke of the

transformation of such a negative experience into “positive” emotions through a

commitment to helping others:

I was detained. I had some sort of maybe serious anger against authorities,

interrogators, men with uniforms, dark rooms, solitary cells. I still remembered

those things … When I met with [other] former political prisoners in Thailand I

asked them, did you visit counselling services once or twice? I learnt a lot from

them … [They said] “we don’t solve anything medically [or] psychologically” …

They told me that as a former political prisoner the best way for me [to cope] is to

help other political prisoners. I’m very happy to know that. So what I mean is … the

Karen women face, I think, very terrible situations. They lost their husbands, they

lost their children. So many terrible things, the best thing [for them is to] join some

sort of community group to support [other] women and children who lost their

families. Sharing and caring … We transform our emotion into a positive way. This

is the reason I stayed doing this advocacy. In my case, somebody asked me “why

are you crazy about advocacy?”. I am not crazy about advocacy! I am trying to help

my people be free from torture and free from disappearing without informing their

family members. Free from arbitrary detention. As long as I help them I am happy. I

don’t have money but I am happy. (Key informant – Mr J)

Mr J’s words reflect the notion of altruism and his observation of the positive impact

of altruism on mental health is supported by previous research (Baetz & Toews,

2009). Like Mr J, other participants contextualised their altruism through their

politically-motivated concern for the Karen people. In addition, several Karen key

informants reported being guided by their religious convictions (i.e., to serve others as

God’s work):

In every way that we possibly can we’re involved and I try to help, that’s our

mission, that’s all I can say. It’s God working through us. (Key informant – Dr G)

According to research, such strong political and religious beliefs may enhance

psychological wellbeing. Brune et al. (2002) conducted a retrospective analysis of

208

therapy outcomes for 141 refugees treated at therapy centres in Germany and Sweden.

The refugees had received psychotherapy accompanied by medical and social support

for an average of 21.5 months. There were no significant differences in patient

outcome (as measured by psychometric test scores) between the type or duration of

psychotherapy offered or the use of psychopharmacological treatment. A firm belief

system (either political or religious) was the only predictor of symptom reduction.

Altruism as a coping strategy has been reported in research with Burmese political

dissidents living in exile (Allden et al., 1996), Sudanese refugee youth in America

(Goodman, 2004) and Holocaust survivors (J. P. Wilson, 1995). In his thesis on Karen

refugees on the Thai-Burma border, Fuertes (2007) reported:

Helping is a dominant theme among Karen refugees in terms of making them

feel useful about themselves and validate their belief that refugees as they are,

they can still make a difference in the lives of other people … [it] gives many

of them a sense of meaning and purpose in their life (p. 367).

Refugees in Australia are noted to regularly engage in volunteering activities

(Refugee Council of Australia, 2010) and refugee women who volunteer report

gaining benefit from viewing themselves as actively shaping and improving society

(Working Lives Research Institute, 2005).

Whilst women did enjoy altruistic and loving relationships within their families and

community, these findings do not negate (and should not obscure) the reality of

intragroup tensions and divisions that restricted some women from accessing social

support. The social relations of resettled refugee communities are characterised by

continuations of war-related partitions and hostilities, and the repercussions of broken

trust. Women’s social networks were also conditioned by gendered and sociopolitical

structures that often limited their ability to assume roles and responsibilities within

their social networks. Like McMichael and Manderson (2004) I found that gossip

served as a source of distress and “a mode of community surveillance” (p. 95) of

women who violated accepted cultural mores. Tilden and Galyen (1987) have called

for attention to the “darker side of social support” (p. 9) after reviewing evidence that

social conflicts may relate more strongly to wellbeing than social supports.

209

This section has explored how Karen women coped through their collective

community networks. Women expressed a strong sense of connection to the wider

Karen community in the homeland and abroad and this was visibly apparent in their

homes. In every home I entered, Karen national calendars, posters of Karen national

leaders, and maps of Burma and the Karen nation hung on the walls. Women made

meaning of their problems through accepting and normalising the collective suffering

of the Karen community. They sought support from their friends, family and

community leaders. They also coped through being of use to others. All Karen

participants distinguished between (a) “only talking” or emotional support, and (b)

practical or material assistance. Both types of support were primarily obtained within

the Karen community and both were underpinned by the value placed on the shared

experience. These themes continue to weave throughout the remaining sections of this

chapter, which explore how women coped through religion and by way of

professional support services.

10.4 Religious coping

The only way to heal is through the spiritual – Buddha or Christ (Statement heard

during participant observation on 20 September, 2009).

10.4.1 “Through God’s strength”

Most participants (whether Buddhist or Christian) spoke of their religious faith as

central to their emotional strength and wellbeing. The following quotation from an

interview with a Karen key informant clearly suggests the importance of religion to

most Karen:

[Those who] cannot accept [their suffering], just yield it to God or to the righteous

one, something like that. Some spiritual figure, or spiritual belief. Bear what we can

bear, but if we can’t, just hand it over. Where? Who? We don’t know exactly, but

the righteous one, the almighty one, creator, or God almighty. Even with animist

Karen, it’s like sort of, creator or the owner of the universe or this land, something

like that … They try to console themselves, or comfort themselves, because of their

spiritual belief. I don’t want to say religious belief, but spiritual belief. (Key

informant – Mr H)

210

Participants’ accounts suggested that, for some Karen women, encountering adversity

may have led to a deepening of their religious faith. Models of posttraumatic

psychological (Tedeschi & Calhoun, 2004a, 2004b) and spiritual (Bray, 2010; Grof &

Grof, 1989) growth similarly suggest that people can experience positive changes in

their personal philosophy or worldview and religious or spiritual understanding after

severe life stressors. Karen women reported that their faith provided them with a

sense of “strength” and many spoke of “surrendering” their problems to God:

If that person is a Christian, it is our religious point of view that they should

surrender their sickness, their worry or their thinking to God. Also, in the Bible, it

says you don’t have to worry, you surrender everything to God and you will be free.

So, pray harder. (Mëh Luh)

Like Mëh Luh, several other Christian Karen women reported that they were

encouraged to “pray harder” as prayer was expected to resolve their suffering:

The other thing is, before you can have a nightmare, you are always told, I think

everyone is told this, not only women, if you are Christian you are told to pray

before you go to bed so you don’t have a nightmare. So, that’s one of the reasons

that the first question someone’s going to ask you if you have nightmares is “Did

you pray last night?” (Key informant – Ms K)

These comments suggest that people who suffer nightmares might be blamed for

failing to protect themselves through religious practices. Baumeister and Exline

(1999) note that individual judgment and guilt with regard to failures to exert self-

control are prominent in Christian thought. In his review of religion, spirituality and

mental health, Koenig (2009) also noted that religious teachings (of any tradition)

have the potential to exacerbate fear and guilt amongst followers. I did not

specifically inquire as to whether participants felt blamed or guilty in relation to their

nightmares. However, the apparent ease with which a large number of participants

(including key informants) provided first-hand accounts of nightmares suggests that

they did not feel ashamed or stigmatised for having failed to prevent their dreams.

10.4.2 Religious community

Religious organisations formed the centre of the Karen community in Sydney and

religious gatherings and practices marked some of the most widely attended and

211

regular social activities for most Karen women. Participant observation confirmed the

critical importance of the religious community. I not only attended dozens of religious

events but also noticed that women often introduced or identified other Karen women

by clarifying which religious subgroup they belonged to. Most religious events in the

community were Christian-oriented. Buddhist participants appeared to attend fewer

faith-based social events but still regularly visited their local Buddhist temples and

also meditated and prayed in their homes. In the following quotation, Ms K, a Karen

key informant, emphasises the significance of the religious community. The practice

of normalisation is also woven into her account of coping through religious

involvement:

Grief is part of life … everybody’s going to go through it and when it happens your

whole community or the church, monastery, temple will support you … Mostly they

cope with religion, even the Buddhists cope with religion as well. The Christian too

… The other thing is the degree of getting involved with the religion, some women

sort of devote their life to church and church activities. For them it’s a coping

strategy. (Key informant – Ms K)

Others such as Dr F similarly emphasised the importance of the religious community

in women’s lives:

What are women’s main coping styles?

Spiritual, spiritual coping fits with their lifestyle. The lifestyle for women is more

about communal activities, supporting others, extended families, friends, that sort of

thing. So the church or the monastery is the main one … Rituals are very important.

Church, congregation, support, pastoral care, I think these are very important. (Key

informant – Dr F)

For Christians, pastoral care was an important aspect of religious-oriented coping.

Pastors played a pivotal role in the community and often visited (or were visited by)

congregation members experiencing stressful life circumstances or emotional distress.

Like other community leaders (see Section 10.3.2), pastors were quick to highlight

that, in the Australian context, they were increasingly called upon to help with

practical resettlement concerns such as housing and document translation as well as

legal and medical emergencies.

212

The research findings suggest that religion served multiple functions in the lives of

Karen women. Women’s religious faith appeared to give meaning to their suffering.

Religious organisations also offered distraction, normalised problems and suffering,

provided opportunities for self-efficacy (e.g., through helping others) and gave

opportunities for informal problem solving. Pastors, like other community leaders,

were seen as a “one stop shop” for spiritual care, advice and practical assistance with

a host of resettlement needs. Religion also permeated women’s intrapersonal coping

strategies where they used prayer and scripture to aid their self-control and acceptance

of hardship. These findings are consistent with Pargament, Koenig and Perez’s (2000)

model of religious functions as sources of meaning, control, comfort, spirituality,

intimacy and life transformation.

These research findings also resonate with coping research around the world and

amongst both general and psychiatric populations (Koenig, 2009). According to

Schuster et al. (2001), 90% of Americans surveyed after the terrorist attacks in 2001

reported coping through “turning to religion” (p. 1507). Similarly, research across

multiple refugee populations has reliably identified religious coping as a primary

means of accessing spiritual, emotional and practical or material support (Brune et al.,

2002; Goodman, 2004; Halcon et al., 2004; Khawaja et al., 2008; Pahud, Kirk, Gage,

& Hornblow, 2009; Sachs, Rosenfeld, Lhewa, Rasmussen, & Keller, 2008;

Schweitzer, Greenslade, & Kagee, 2007; Wuthnow, 1991). Religious involvement

post-resettlement may also help refugees to re-establish identity and can provide both

physical and mental links to the homeland (Mayer, 2007). Notably, a recent

ethnographic study of the resettlement processes of Karen refugees in the United

States similarly found that Christian organisations were central providers of support

(Kenny & Lockwood-Kenny, 2011).

10.5 Professional support: coping and service providers

Women described seeking external help from service providers only as a last resort.

They also again made a subtle but important distinction between practical (e.g.,

financial or medication) and emotional (e.g., “only talking”) support, noting that

professional service providers were most relevant to their practical needs only. This

section explores how and when women sought support from Australian service

providers.

213

10.5.1 Doctors

Doctors were seen as essential to wellbeing and all women interviewed reported

attending a GP. Women were adamant that anyone suffering from tha (heart-mind)

distress or severe mental illness should immediately visit a doctor. They also almost

universally expressed the belief that medical care could only address the physical

aspects of their suffering. Consequently, they thought it was nonsensical and

inappropriate to discuss their broader social problems with their GP:

Definitely the doctor can’t help with family issues, children issues or husband

problems – he can’t help with this. She should go to the community and some of the

organisations that specifically help children. (Participants in Group C)

Similar attitudes have been documented in other populations. Like Chinese

Australians, Karen women primarily viewed doctors as a source of medication only

(Chan & Ritchie, 2011). Al-Busaidi (2010) found that some Omani women believed it

was “inappropriate culturally to talk to the doctor about their private lives” (p. 8)

despite viewing their psychosocial stressors and physical health complaints as

interrelated. This distinction between the physical and social was made clear in the

narrative of Lah Luh, whose somatic and emotional expressions of distress included

“popping” sensations in her stomach and sharp pains radiating to her head as detailed

in Chapter 9 (see Section 9.4.5). In addition to suffering from hypertension, Lah Luh

attributed many of her physical complaints to the stress of her son’s behaviour

problems. She frequently cried and spoke of wanting to die but although she regularly

visited her GP she did not consider it appropriate to tell him about her emotional and

social distress. Instead she spoke only of her “body” problems. Likewise, Mu Htòh

clearly explained that such concerns were not topics to discuss with one’s doctor:

Not about that, no! Never, never! This is our private affair, we only go to the doctor

for our headaches or body problems. For the mental we never go … never tell him.

We only tell him “I can’t sleep, I can’t eat”. (Mu Htòh).

Some GPs were aware that their Karen patients were unlikely to freely disclose their

emotional suffering and social problems. They explained that Karen women more

commonly sought their help for the somatic aspects of their distress – particularly

headaches, high blood pressure and abdominal pains. Dr N reported that medical

214

professionals must be attuned to Karen women’s cultural expressions of distress or

they could misunderstand their patients’ problems:

They dare not express it, they won’t say “I’m depressed”, [or] “I’m down”. No

[instead] they will say “I’ve got a headache, stomach-ache” because the mental

illness is stigmatised. But if the person giving the treatment isn’t familiar with this it

will be missed. (Key informant – Dr N)

Other key informants also highlighted the role of stigma and shame. They believed

that Karen women were particularly unlikely to discuss their problems associated with

rape and domestic violence:

For the women to come out and tell you openly that she has been raped or sexually

harassed, oh no, we have to be very subtle about it … [Also] there is a stigma

attached to mental illness and they suspect that. They will tell you that they can’t

sleep and then when you take it further the insomnia might be flashbacks actually,

or it might be certain deep anxieties or whatever. You have got to probe a little bit.

(Key informant – Dr G)

10.5.2 Australian service providers

In addition to doctors, participants suggested using a range of other service providers

(e.g., psychologists, psychiatrists and counsellors) to help with emotional distress.

Women speculated that these service providers might be “helpful” but were generally

unsure of “how” such professionals could help other than providing medication or

helping to practically deal with one’s problems. I was unable to obtain any first-hand

accounts from women who would disclose having sought emotional support (rather

than practical assistance, e.g., document translation) from counsellors, psychologists

or psychiatrists outside of the standard mental health assessments undertaken as part

of resettlement and orientation (described in Chapter 4). Unanimously, women did not

perceive these services as relevant to their own personal needs and therefore did not

identify them as strategies for coping with their own distress. However, when

discussing idioms of distress, women explained that a psychiatrist or psychologist

might be necessary to help someone with severe tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-

mind) or dha blu (mental illness). Typical of women’s comments, Mëh Luh reported:

215

I’ve never been there. I’ve never heard of my community people having been to

counselling but this is what they [the service providers] have said – for those who

are having these problems, especially thinking a lot on their problems, to go to a

counsellor and talk about your problem. It is helpful, this is what they say but I’m

not sure, I’ve never been. (Mëh Luh)

These findings are similar to those of Whittaker, Hardy, Lewis and Buchan (2005)

who explored Somali refugee women’s perceptions of psychological wellbeing. Their

participants listed psychologists and counsellors as sources of help but reported that

they did not personally utilise those services. Karen women may have been reluctant

to admit utilising these services due to shame and stigma. Participants’ perceptions of

mental health services are discussed in greater detail in Chapter 13.

10.6 Summary and reflections

Women coped with hardship and emotional distress through controlling their minds.

They sought to identify and challenge unhelpful thinking patterns and also distract or

avert their minds from troubling thoughts. Women also coped with hardship and

distress through stoic silence and acceptance, and a strong conviction that human life

is characterised by suffering which one must endure. These approaches to coping are

situated within a religious, cultural and sociopolitical milieu of persecution and forced

migration. Interpersonal emotional and material support from family, friends and the

wider community (including community leaders) was also highly valued. Feelings of

collectivity and community provided women with support (emotional and physical)

and protection against the hardships of resettlement. Helping others provided

distraction from one’s own suffering, a sense of purpose and value as well as an

opportunity for social interaction. Religion and spirituality permeated both

intrapersonal and social coping strategies. Women prayed for strength and their faith

gave meaning to their suffering. In addition, the religious community also provided

them with access to social networks as well as material and emotional support. Karen

women sought assistance from Australian service providers to cope with practical,

physical and material concerns but rarely for emotional support. Rather, they

perceived that sharing experiences with fellow Karen was the most appropriate means

of addressing emotional needs.

216

In relation to coping, here too (see Section 8.5) Bandura’s theoretical accounts of

efficacy provide a platform for conceptualising the research findings. In his more

recent works, Bandura (2000, 2001) extended his theory to account for interpersonal

or socially mediated forms of efficacy: proxy and collective agency. Proxy agency is

actioned by people who rely on capable others to mediate or obtain the outcome they

desire. Collective efficacy or agency refers to a group’s shared belief in its

interdependent ability to achieve a desired aim. The research findings suggest that

Bandura’s model may not only provide a useful framework for conceptualising the

negative effects of forced migration and camp confinement (as discussed in Section

8.5), but the notions of collective efficacy and interdependent agency might also be

helpful in understanding women’s resilience and approaches to coping. For example,

the findings discussed in this chapter demonstrate that despite low individual self-

efficacy, participants conveyed a strong sense of proxy agency and collective efficacy

(within the Karen community). They relied on (and desired help from) other

community members, particularly leaders, to assist them with their problems. Teasing

out such findings and further exploring self-efficacy and wellbeing may help to

elucidate not only pathways to suffering but mechanisms of strengths and resilience

also.

As demonstrated in Chapter 9, women’s conceptualisations of emotional distress and

suffering could not ignore the body, the psyche and the sociocultural context.

Similarly, their approaches to coping with emotional distress encompassed strategies

that sought to manage the psychological, political (e.g., helping others through

advocacy), economic, spiritual and sociocultural dimensions of the refugee

experience.

My literature search located only three published studies that have discussed coping

strategies amongst refugees from Burma. Lopes Cardozo et al. (2004) assessed mental

health problems among Karenni refugees in camps along the Thailand-Burma border

using psychometric questionnaires (the Hopkins Symptom Checklist-25 and the

Harvard Trauma Questionnaire). They also interviewed key informants to identify

coping mechanisms and included additional questions about coping in their survey of

495 participants. These authors reported coping mechanisms of sleeping, thinking

about their homeland, singing or playing music, visiting the clinic and drinking rice

217

wine. The predominance of distractive (over informational or practical help-seeking)

approaches to coping may reflect the camp context of the research, where seeking

assistance was unlikely to result in significant changes to one’s problems or lifestyle.

In another study, also using the same psychometrics, Allden et al. (1996) reported on

the coping mechanisms of Burmese political dissidents living in Thailand. Seventy-

five per cent of participants reported “continuing the pro-democracy struggle” as a

strategy to deal with life and suffering in exile. Allden et al. also mentioned “desiring

to help the people of Burma” (p. 1567) as another coping strategy. Participants who

reported coping through contact with friends or other comrades in exile, and those

who coped through weira (a Burmese Buddhist concept), had lower symptom scores.

The authors described weira as a “strategy of self-confidence … a Buddhist concept

of resolve and determination to use one’s innate talents and strengths to deal with

one’s karma” (p. 1567). Like politically active participants in my study, Allden et al.’s

participants emphasised coping through collective solidarity and helping others.

Finally, Fuertes (2004, 2007) similarly found that helping others was the dominant

approach to coping among the Karen on the Thailand-Burma border. In his study,

Karen refugees conveyed a concept of healing that entwined social, economic and

political restoration and “transforming the negative impact of their war experience

into something beneficial” (2007, p. 301). Collective solidarity and the reintegration

into community life as a nation were part of their paths to healing. Likewise, my

research findings indicate that Karen women in Australia coped through collective

solidarity, transforming and making meaning of negative experiences, and helping

others.

10.6.1 Emotional and practical support

Women’s division of emotional and practical support shares a likeness to the Western

model of emotion-focused and problem-focused coping styles. Western psychological

literature on coping commonly distinguishes between these approaches and considers

problem-focused coping to be more effective for dealing with traumatic experiences

(Agaibi & Wilson, 2005; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). When using problem-focused

strategies a person might learn skills or seek information in order to resolve a stressor.

Emotion-focused strategies, as the name suggests, include a host of techniques to

218

manage the emotions associated with a stressor. Techniques include distraction,

relaxation and cognitive restructuring (e.g., re-appraising the meaning of the stressor).

The emotion- and problem-focused coping framework has been adapted and applied

to conceptualise refugees’ coping strategies. Colic-Peisker and Tilbury (2003)

identified “active” (p. 61) refugee coping styles such as political involvement and

goal-oriented resettlement activities. They found that refugees who used active coping

strategies were generally those who had spent less time in refugee camps. Refugees

who had endured a protracted migration were more likely to demonstrate a passive

approach to coping with resettlement and tended to conceptualise themselves as either

“endurers” or “victims” (p. 72). Similarly, Khawaja and colleagues (2008) described

passive approaches to coping including resignation, avoidance of confronting

problems and framing the situation as out of one’s control in their study of Sudanese

refugees.

10.6.2 Shared experiences

The importance of seeking help from someone with shared sociocultural and

situational experiences was prominent in women’s narratives of coping. This process

appeared to help women through (a) accepting, normalising and making meaning of

their adversities, and (b) problem solving and learning through the experiences of

others.

One refugee study has specifically drawn attention to the importance of the shared

experience within social support and intracommunity help-seeking (Simich, Beiser, &

Mawani, 2003). That research similarly documented that refugees in Canada sought

support within their own communities and valued help from others who shared their

experiences.

The importance of the shared experience to help-seeking was explicitly addressed

over two decades ago by Thoits (1986) who sought to uncover the processes by which

social support protects against the adverse mental health effects of stressful life

events. She explored the aspects of supportive social relationships that promoted

coping and argued that “sociocultural and situational similarity” (p. 420) enhance the

perceived acceptability and effectiveness of coping assistance. Of the two, Thoits

considered a shared situational experience to be most critical. She argued that the

219

nature of the shared experience provides help-seekers with a comparison for self-

evaluation and modelling, as well as empathic understanding, guidance and

reassurance that is considered valid. Thoits also identified exceptions to the shared

sociocultural and situational model in cases whereby shame, embarrassment or

anticipated rejection might prevent an individual from seeking help within their social

network. This is an important reminder, as the Karen collective (like any community)

is not devoid of intragroup tensions and divisions. This area warrants further

investigation.

10.6.3 Acceptance and normalisation

The themes of acceptance and normalisation were woven throughout women’s

accounts of coping. These practices appear underpinned by traditional religious and

cultural worldviews and the sociopolitical experiences of recent generations. Previous

research with Karen refugees has also reported acceptance as an aspect of coping.

Fuertes (2007) reported that Karen refugees “accept the reality” (p. 356) of being

refugees, share their experiences with fellow refugees and pray to cope with their

situation. He explained that “acceptance of reality serves as an overarching form or

mechanism that enables them to cope with the reality of being refugees” (p. 357).

Fuertes argued that this approach was positive, explaining “acceptance of reality does

not mean giving up their hopes and dreams of a better future” (p. 357-358).

Participants in Allden et al.’s (1996) study of Burmese political dissidents in Thailand

also reported coping through accepting their inability to return to Burma as well as

continuing their political activities and helping others. These coping strategies did not

show significant association with any of the symptom scales measured by the

psychometric questionnaires. However, Allden et al. did not explore the broader

concept of acceptance beyond “accepting one’s inability to return to Burma” (p.

1567). Nor did they further explore relationships between acceptance and domains of

distress and wellbeing as measured by the psychometric tests.

For Karen women, the psychological and behavioural outcomes of acceptance and

normalisation such as stoic silence appeared to move along a continuum. At one end

they are positive and adaptive as they foster resilience and enable women to endure

and survive hardship. At the other end they may lead to apathy and demoralisation.

220

Western psychiatric literature has typically associated accepting one’s fate, stoic

silence and emotional suppression with negative social and psychological outcomes

(Gross & John, 2003). However, the consequences of stoic silence or suppression

appear to differ across cultures and research has demonstrated that it may be an

effective coping strategy for Asian populations (Butler et al., 2007). In Sri Lanka, G.

A. Fernando (2005) demonstrated the role of religious faith in mediating vulnerability

to depression amongst children exposed to war and violence. She found that Christian

children were more vulnerable to depression than Hindu and Buddhist children and

speculated that active acceptance of pain and suffering might be a protective factor.

Researchers have proposed that Japanese traditions such as accepting one’s fate,

masking feelings and enduring adversity may partially account for lower rates of

PTSD amongst Japanese American Vietnam Veterans (Freidman, Schnurr, Sengupta,

Holmes, & Ashcraft, 2004).

Research also suggests that Western approaches seeking to alter practices such as

stoic silence can be harmful. For example, D. Pedersen et al. (2010) observed that

silence and stoicism can function to preserve community cohesion. In Sri Lanka, a

complex cultural system of silence and speaking in euphemisms allowed members of

Sinhalese Buddhist communities to refer to traumatic events during the civil war

without giving explicit detail (Argenti-Pillen, 2003). This system also kept control of

revenge violence quite successfully. The community members who violated the social

code of silence and euphemistic speech were seen as careless, “fearless” (Argenti-

Pillen, 2003, p. 162) and dangerous. When Western trauma counsellors entered the

village they legitimised the behaviour of the fearless outspoken community members,

encouraged verbalisation about traumatic events and suffering, and pathologised the

local custom of silence and euphemistic speech. A delicate social balance was

damaged without insight into the potential consequences.

Karen women also normalised their suffering within their collective community and

coped through knowing that they were not alone in the problems they faced. Such

normalisation is a documented coping strategy amongst other populations affected by

war and persecution (Goodman, 2004; Guribye et al., 2011; Schweitzer et al., 2007;

Somasundaram, 2010). Goodman (2004) described a similar theme of collectivity and

the communal self and explained that young Sudanese men coped through recognition

221

of their shared experience of suffering. Like the Sudanese men that Goodman

interviewed, Karen women also conveyed a sense of a collective self and referred to

the suffering of “we” (the collective community of Karen refugees). However, the

current research findings have demonstrated that in addition to normalising and

accepting suffering, Karen women also appeared to minimise their losses and

traumatic experiences relative to those of their peers (see Section 9.3.2).

Recent research with 769 Tibetan refugees provides some insight into the potential

role and function of minimisation for refugee populations whose cultural traditions

include Buddhism. Sachs et al. (2008) asked the Tibetan refugees to provide a social

comparison rating in which they appraised “the severity of their experiences

compared to those of other Tibetans” (p. 204). Most participants (including survivors

of severe torture) rated their experiences as less severe than their peers. Subjective

appraisals of trauma experience as less severe than that of others mediated the impact

of trauma on psychological functioning. Those who minimised their own problems

had better mental health outcomes than those who did not.

The Tibetan refugee study provides a point of interpretation for the findings of

normalisation and minimisation as a coping strategy amongst Karen women. Few

studies have noted the role of “social comparisons” or minimisation and even fewer

have explored the relationship of this factor to emotional suffering. Negative social

comparisons (in which a trauma survivor ranks their own suffering as less than that of

others) would generally be referred to negatively as minimisation or even “denial” in

Western psychiatric and psychological literature. However, Sachs et al.’s (2008)

findings suggest that minimising one’s suffering in relation to others may reduce

symptom severity and thus represent an adaptive coping strategy. It is unclear how

such social comparisons might enhance resilience. Acknowledgement of the suffering

of others may provide (a) religious or cultural meaning-making (e.g., “it is a universal

truth that everyone suffers”), (b) a means of distraction, (c) social connection through

shared experiences and care giving, or (d) opportunity to develop an identity role and

self-efficacy (e.g., “I can be useful”, “I can help someone else”). The clinical and

theoretical implications of my research findings, and the congruence of my findings

with those of other research such as Sachs et al.’s, will be discussed further in

Chapters 13 to 15.

222

Chapter 11

Bridging Idioms:

Karen Women’s Explanatory Models of Western Psychiatric

Constructs

11.1 Introduction

This chapter presents and discusses the findings related to the question: What are

Karen women’s explanatory models (beliefs regarding illness) for the Western

psychiatric constructs of depression, panic and PTSD? It begins with a brief review of

the methods used to address this aspect of the research before presenting the findings

in relation to each of the vignettes. The findings will be discussed in relation to the

literature and the implications of women’s EMs explored.

11.2 Vignettes and explanatory models

As detailed in Chapters 6 and 7 (see Sections 6.4.2 and 7.5.1), for this aspect of the

study women participated in group interviews during which they were asked

questions to elicit their EMs about three separate case vignettes. The vignettes each

met DSM-IV (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) diagnostic criteria for either

PTSD, major depressive disorder or panic disorder, and the stories depicted features

relevant to the lives of Karen refugee women.

11.3 Vignette A / Aunty A: DSM-IV Major Depressive Disorder

Aunty A arrived in Sydney from a refugee camp on the Thai-Burma border in the last

few years. Recently, she appears to be sad most of the time. Sometimes she

experiences headaches, poor sleep, tiredness, backaches, stomach-aches and other

physical discomforts. Much of the time Aunty A seems to have lost interest in life and

seems to think too much. She does not seem to be able to think clearly or concentrate

and appears restless. It seems as though Aunty A has no hope for the future and thinks

that it would be better if she were not alive.

All participants identified the vignette symptoms using the label tha dha hsar

(sickness of the heart-mind) and believed that this was a case of tha dha hsar caused

by hso k’moe dha (thinking too much). They associated the condition with everyday

223

problems of living, particularly resettlement difficulties and poverty. Women also

speculated that Aunty A probably lived alone and struggled with daily problems due

to lack of family support. They explained that women without family and older

women were more likely to worry and suffer tha dha hsar due to resettlement stress

and an uncertain future. Marital conflict and family problems were also identified as

pertinent in the aetiology of the symptom picture:

By looking at their family situation you can tell [if] a particular person is more

likely to have tha dha hsar. (Participant in Group C)

Additional psychosocial precursors to Aunty A’s symptoms included related elements

of homesickness, loneliness, lack of community support and isolation (particularly

separation from immediate family members). Women emphasised that stress and hso

k’moe dha (thinking too much) caused by everyday problems were not sicknesses but

could lead to sickness of the heart-mind (tha dha hsar):

Stress is not a sickness. No, stress is not a sickness. Sometimes if you carry on

having stress long-term then it will become a sickness. (Participant in Group B)

Women also related their own experiences in response to the vignette:

When I think a lot I feel like that – stomach-ache, sometimes backache. When you

think a lot if affects you physically. I have three sons. One is in Australia, one is in

Burma and one is in the refugee camp. When I think of my sons it makes me sick –

I miss them. I feel worried, especially for the second son in Burma. I have no idea if

he is alive or dead due to the political situation. We had to flee and now I have no

idea if my son is alive. I can’t sleep at night. I wake up in the middle of night and

just read the bible or pray to get all the thinking out of my mind but it is still there,

especially about my son. As a mother you think a lot when you can’t get news from

your child … I myself might even go mad soon. (Participant in Group A)

A number of women disclosed having had similar problems. They described acute

episodes of distress (which they commonly termed tha ba do / worried) after the

death(s) of their children and reported having been inconsolable during those times.

Participants drew direct relationships between social causes, thinking too much (hso

k’moe dha) and physical symptoms:

224

Whatever circumstances you suffer and your body is related to each other. When

you think a lot you can’t sleep. If you can’t sleep you get weak. If you feel sad you

don’t want to eat. If you don’t eat you get a stomach-ache. (Participant in Group A)

Social isolation was identified as a serious cause and consequence of Aunty A’s

problems. Physical effects were lack of appetite, trouble sleeping, stomach-ache and

headaches. Women explained that someone with tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-

mind) could also become sensitive to further stressors, easy to frighten, tearful,

irritable, distracted and could fail to find pleasure through activities with other people.

They believed that Aunty A’s level of functioning might potentially be impaired and

she “might not be interested in looking after her children”. Her family would also be

affected due to their “worry” about her.

Aunty A’s thoughts of death were cited as indicative of a serious level of distress and

women thought that she could become mentally ill (dha blu) and commit suicide. A

number of women recounted instances of suicide in the refugee camps. They reported

that the deceased had suffered tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind) but had been

unable to control themselves and became mad. They attributed one woman’s suicide

to her “family situation”, particularly her husband’s alcoholism. Most other suicides

were noted to occur after the death of a spouse or child(ren). Participants reported that

women were more likely to suffer tha dha hsar but men were more likely to commit

suicide: “men are worse, when they experience tha dha hsar they commit suicide”.

Reflecting self-control coping practices described in Chapter 10 (see Section 10.2),

women unanimously reported that Aunty A needed to have controlled her thinking

especially in the early stages of stress and hso k’moe dha (thinking too much) to

prevent deterioration into tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind):

The emotional issue is not caused by itself. It’s caused by you because you are the

one who thinks a lot. You cannot control yourself. Always worrying, always

thinking. That’s what causes emotional problems. It’s how you manage it.

(Participant in Group A)

They stressed the importance of personal responsibility: “if you do nothing it will get

worse”, and spoke of their religious faith as helping them cope with stress and

thinking too much:

225

For me a lot has happened but I never keep it in my mind. I never put it in my mind

and save it. I hear news, especially from my children, that never makes me happy

but I never think of it. I’ve got children but God will take care of them. I read the

bible and the bible makes me calm and happy. There is no point keeping things in

your mind – no point keeping on thinking those things – it will give you sickness.

(Participant in Group A)

Another participant in the same group agreed that it was important to try and alleviate

troubling thoughts through religious coping but reported that she was unable to stop

her worrying and believed she might go mad:

I pray and pray to God to give me some news about my daughter. But maybe God

never listens to me. (Participant in Group A)

Community support through visits and prayers from friends and religious leaders

formed the next approach to coping suggested for Aunty A’s difficulties:

We Karen people, if we have a problem or face difficulties our close friend used to

visit our house and sleep over with us. (Participant in Group C)

All participants agreed that overcoming isolation through social support was central to

coping with the symptoms of tha dha hsar. One participant said: “by talking to

someone her problem will be less”. Two younger and outspoken women in Group C

reported that they would not hesitate to ask friends and community members for

support. Other participants, particularly older women, spoke of feeling embarrassed

about revealing their emotional distress to their peers. However, most reported that

they would tell their best friend. Women’s descriptions of coping through social

support again revealed the importance of the shared experience (see Section 10.6.2) as

well as self-disclosure on behalf of the supporter. In addition to sharing feelings,

participants felt that the community should also help Aunty A resolve the specific

causes of her tha dha hsar:

If this problem is because of the children she might need community help to

recorrect the children’s problems. Or if the husband is the one who is the problem

then the community should help her. (Participant in Group C)

226

Women in all groups agreed that Aunty A should consult a doctor, particularly

because she expressed a desire to die. They felt that the doctor could relieve her

somatic complaints but not other aspects of her distress:

The doctor, the most he can treat this person is only physical sickness. For mental or

for emotional [problems] the doctor may not be able to help this lady. So even

before she consults a doctor, community people should visit her … Religious

leaders should visit and pray for her. (Participant in Group A)

In Group B one participant disclosed that she had visited her GP specifically due to

emotional distress. She described feeling dissatisfied with seeking support this way:

“when I go to the doctor if they treat me like nothing [is wrong], if they don’t console

me, if they don’t treat me I may get angry and leave”. This woman had hoped to

receive either emotional support (e.g., “console me”) or medical treatment but was

disappointed and frustrated after receiving neither.

In summary, all groups conceptualised external stressors, especially loss and

interpersonal conflict, as the primary cause of Aunty A’s suffering. They described a

psychocentric element in the aetiology of these problems as hso k’moe dha (thinking

too much) caused her emotional (heart-mind) distress. They believed that an element

of self-control was crucial to good mental health and described a notion of meta-

cognitive awareness whereby thinking about one’s thinking was an important

component of psychological wellbeing. With support and self-control women

expected Aunty A to get better. However, they felt that her thoughts of wanting to die

were indicative of severe suffering and feared that she could progress towards mental

illness and possibly suicide.

227

11.4 Vignette B / Aunty B: DSM-IV Panic Disorder

Aunty B arrived in Sydney from a refugee camp on the Thai-Burma border in the past

few years. Recently, she appears to be worried and is scared as if something terrible

is going to happen. Sometimes her body trembles and she also experiences dizziness,

sore muscles and feels that her heart is beating very fast. Aunty B also has trouble

concentrating, has sleeping problems and frequently appears tired or low in energy.

Sometimes she is worried that something is wrong with her, is scared of something

bad happening or keeps thinking the same worrying thought and seems unable to

control her worry. Aunty B also avoids situations that she is scared of.

Women easily related to the symptoms described in the case vignette of Aunty B.

They described their own similar stories but struggled to identify any particular name

for the symptom picture. The majority of participants regarded the symptoms as

indicative of some type of common emotional (heart-mind) distress caused by

physical health problems, hso k’moe dha (thinking too much) about daily social

stressors or both. Women noted that the symptoms described did not yet constitute tha

dha hsar. As one participant observed: “[Aunty B has] too much hso k’moe dha but

not yet tha dha hsar … if she doesn’t control herself she will get tha dha hsar”.

Group members talked amongst themselves as they considered a variety of possible

terms for Aunty B’s symptoms. They debated with each other and lively discussion

followed but ultimately no specific Karen term could be identified. They put forward

a variety of stress-like states including tha ba yo (worry) and explained the difference

between these and tha dha hsar:

Sometimes you have too much tha ba yo [worry]. Especially about your children.

They want to go somewhere and you worry but they go anyway. If nothing happens

you had tha ba yo. If something happens you will get tha dha hsar [sickness of the

heart-mind]. Now you know the difference [laughs]. (Participant in Group C)

In addition to worrisome thoughts, hso k’moe dha and stress caused by social

problems (particularly parenting and interpersonal conflict), participants also gave

attention to Aunty B’s somatic problems. In contrast to their discussions of the

depression vignette (Aunty A), women were more likely to cite physical or biological

causes for Aunty B’s symptoms. They related her physical symptoms to pre-migration

228

nutritional deficiencies, especially for people who spent long periods of time in

refugee camps. They also related the symptom picture to health problems such as high

blood pressure and diabetes, which they partly attributed to post-migration lifestyle

and nutrition changes. Women appeared to have gained some familiarity with

Western biomedical models and terminology through their contact with humanitarian

aid organisations in Thailand as well as their experiences with healthcare providers in

Australia. Social stress and hso k’moe dha were also described as causes of high

blood pressure. Finally, and consistent with women’s idioms of distress discussed in

Chapter 9 (see Section 9.4.1), a minority of participants reported poor blood

circulation (including irregular menstruation) as a potential cause and consequence of

Aunty B’s symptoms.

As with the first vignette, women speculated that the Aunty was likely to be an older

woman who experienced “family problems” or lived alone. The old, lonely woman

with family problems and emotional distress appeared to be a cultural stereotype

amongst Karen participants. They conveyed an acceptance of Aunty B’s symptoms as

relatively normal and believed that she could still perform her daily tasks and function

in the community. Participants recounted their own similar experiences. Older women

related Aunty B’s symptoms to their experiences of high blood pressure and

worsening physical health during times of increased social stress. Younger women

reported experiencing similar symptoms which they attributed to external stressors

and their fears of “something” bad happening to them in Australia:

This story is similar to mine. I have also experienced this. Here in Australia

everyone has human rights and you can report or complain if something happens to

you. You are told you can call the police. But if you call, something might still

happen to you. If the police don’t come what will you do? I’m afraid that something

might happen. (Participant in Group B)

Women reported that they read the bible, prayed, listened to music and visited their

best friends when they experienced Aunty B’s problems. Again they emphasised the

importance of social support and kind words from friends and family. Group C also

explained that Aunty B should continue to avoid the situations that make her feel

afraid or exacerbate her symptoms.

229

Having emphasised the role of physical or medical factors in the symptom profile,

women also thought it was important for Aunty B to visit her doctor. However, a

number of women reported experiencing similar symptoms but were dissatisfied with

the medical assistance they had received: “but when we see a doctor they say nothing

is wrong with you”. After mentioning self-control, social support, religion and

medical care, participants in Group C also suggested counselling, though they

continued to place importance on self-control:

Is there anyone else she should see to get help?

(GC:1) Counselling – see a counsellor.

Can a counsellor fix her problem?

(GC:1) No.

(GC:5) Not one hundred per cent. But maybe the counsellor can talk to her and say

“you are not the only person, we have many people like you”. But the main thing is

that she must control herself.

In the preceding quotation, the concept of normalisation (see Section 10.3.1) is again

apparent. In relation to Aunty B’s difficulties, participants in Group C believed that

counselling could be beneficial if the therapist normalised Aunty B’s experiences and

problems. When symptoms were explained as physical health problems (including

physical tha / heart problems such as high blood pressure) women expected that

Aunty B would require medication but would also need to “control” herself and her

thinking in order to avoid deterioration in her condition. Similarly, when social

stressors and hso k’moe dha were cited as aetiological factors, women again

emphasised the need for self-control in order to avoid further deterioration.

In summary, when discussing Vignette B women conveyed an implicit

conceptualisation of mind (psyche) and body (soma) as intimately interrelated.

However, they also distinguished between somatic illnesses that are independent of

the mind-self (e.g., diabetes) from those that may have a psychological aetiology

(e.g., high blood pressure from thinking too much). Physical and psychological

distress involved one another, and when women described psychological causes of

distress (e.g., hso k’moe dha) they endorsed physical consequences (e.g.,

palpitations). Similarly, physical causes of distress (e.g., high blood pressure) were

conceptualised as involving psychological consequences (e.g., difficulty

230

concentrating). Aunty B’s symptoms of common tha (heart-mind) distress were

perceived as less severe than Aunty A’s. This was, in part, because Aunty A wanted

to end her life but also because Aunty B’s somatic symptoms were so familiar to

participants. Importantly, women did not have a specific label or idiom unique to this

particular vignette and often struggled to differentiate aspects of Vignette B from

Vignette A. They employed a variety of idioms of distress, all of which related to the

heart (tha). This again (see Section 9.5.1) suggests that the women’s EMs for mental

illness do not view the Western psychiatric constructs of depression and anxiety as

radically different but rather perceive variation in symptoms along a continuum of

suffering.

11.5 Vignette C / Aunty C: DSM-IV Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Aunty C had awful experiences before arriving in Australia last year. She feared for

her life in Burma and saw a lot of violence including seeing her brother killed. Aunty

C continues to have thoughts or memories of seeing her brother killed, and then soon

feels scared and tense. Sometimes she has nightmares and she often has trouble

sleeping. Recently Aunty C has been acting differently, she is avoiding certain people,

activities and situations that remind her of the terrible things she saw in Burma and

she appears to be very fearful. Aunty C sometimes seems unable to experience normal

happy emotions and she seems to have a deep sadness. Sometimes she complains that

her heart is beating fast and sometimes she also feels dizzy, trembling or choking

sensations.

Women in all groups called the symptom picture presented in Vignette C (Aunty C)

severe tha dha hsar (sickness of the heart-mind). They perceived the symptoms as

similar to Aunty A’s (depression vignette) but many felt that Aunty C’s symptom

profile was more serious and closer to the stage of mental illness or madness (dha

blu):

This is more than tha dha hsar already … we can’t say tha dha hsar or dha blu –

What shall we call it? What can we call it? (Participant in Group B)

This is severe tha dha hsar but the next step is dha blu. (Participant in Group B)

231

In Group C participants also struggled with labelling the symptom profile but

collectively decided that it was probably closer to dha blu (mental illness or

madness). A participant in Group B who spoke fluent English and was familiar with

Western psychiatric conceptualisations of mental illness reported: “in our country we

don’t have the [term] trauma but we still have this kind of experience, we call it

‘suffer too much’”. This comment parallels findings from individual interviews (see

Section 9.3.2) in which women described severe suffering as caused by “too much

experiencing” and “too much suffering”.

Most of the women’s causal explanations for Aunty C’s problems were the same as

those identified for the preceding vignettes (A and B) (e.g., thinking too much about

one’s stressful experiences). However, Aunty C’s problems were distinguished as

being caused by “too much experience”. Her experiences were considered more

severe and likely to overwhelm an individual’s ability to cope. This might have

suggested that the women conceptualised a direct relationship between Aunty C’s

symptoms and the traumatic experience of witnessing her brother murdered.

However, as with the previous vignettes, women speculated that Aunty C’s

experience was also characterised by a lack of family support and social isolation.

Here again, Karen women’s collectivist cultural context framed their interpretations

of distress. Women routinely emphasised social and relational factors over all others.

As we saw also in Chapter 9 (see Section 9.3.2), when describing instances of “too

much experience” women consistently included severe family (or social) difficulties

or consequences in their narratives. Women also again explained that the social

implication of the event indicated its severity and the likelihood of the survivor

suffering severe heart-mind distress. Some suspected that Aunty C might have only

had one brother and consequently expected her suffering would be worse than that of

someone with several brothers. Some participants conceptualised Aunty C’s severe

tha dha hsar in response to traumatic experiences as normal and believed her

symptoms would naturally abate with time:

Maybe her experience is very recent and it is still warm so she remembers it. She

can still feel it but she needs to take time, you’ll see. (Participant in Group B)

Women did not spontaneously attribute other causes to Aunty C’s problems.

However, when asked to list all potential causes of dha blu (mental illness or

232

madness) they cited additional biological (e.g., genetics) and supernatural (e.g.,

witchcraft) factors, as reported in Chapter 9 (see Section 9.3.4). It remains unclear

why women who identified Aunty C’s suffering as dha blu did not spontaneously list

these other causes. It appeared that they were hesitant to attribute her suffering to

causes generally reserved for cases of dha blu because they were unsure whether

Aunty C had tha dha hsar or dha blu. In addition, as women’s concept of genetics

(see Section 9.3.3) appeared to include both biologically and socially inherited

familial predispositions, it seems plausible that they would have needed further

contextual information about Aunty C and her family before considering genetic

factors.

Participants recited stories from the refugee camp(s) to describe people with problems

similar to those of Aunty C:

It’s like mental illness. You think too much and become mentally ill. I saw a man

whose wife and son died. He got crazy from this experience. When you are

experiencing a lot, when you think a lot and experiencing awful situations and you

can’t control yourself you may lose your mind. We call this dha blu [mental illness

or madness]. (Participant in Group C)

Aunty C’s symptoms were perceived as suggestive of mental illness and a loss of

control of the mind. Consequently, women did not suggest self-control as a coping

strategy. Rather, they emphasised the importance of the community in providing

physical and emotional support. They thought that family members should strictly

manage and monitor Aunty C’s eating and sleeping and take her to church.

Furthermore, participants agreed that the symptoms would likely require care from a

doctor and perhaps admission to a psychiatric hospital:

Maybe the doctor can give her tablets for her heart beating fast. Also sleeping pills

to help her sleep. (Participant in Group C)

Women believed that Aunty C’s degree of emotional distress was severe and

potentially life threatening. Participants in Group B mentioned the need for good

blood circulation and reported that if you think a lot, about current or past “awful”

experiences, your blood circulation may be affected and your heart will beat fast. As

233

the group discussed the consequences of poor blood circulation they described the

potential seriousness of this problem:

If you think a lot, worry a lot, fear a lot and your heart is beating fast there might be

a point in time where the heart stops beating. It is beating fast and then might stop.

(Participant in Group C)

Those who thought the symptom profile indicated dha blu reported that Aunty C

could eventually lose knowledge of reality, cry and laugh irrationally, talk nonsense

and walk around not recognising anyone. Women unanimously spoke of witnessing

such symptom profiles amongst men and women in the refugee camps but again (see

Section 9.4.7) reported no such instances within the community in Australia.

11.6 Summary

The findings from this exploration of EMs resonate with data collected for other parts

of this study and provide a form of triangulation of methods. The trajectory of distress

(see Figure 5) introduced in Chapter 9 (see Section 9.5.1) is again apparent, this time

in relation to women’s EMs of Western psychiatric constructs.

Most common and least severe Least common and most severe Hso k’moe dha Tha… Tha dha hsar Dha blu

Thinking too much

(Common) Heart-mind distress

Sickness of the heart-mind

Crazy

Figure 5: Trajectory of distress

Three EMs emerge from women’s responses. In order of importance these are: (a) the

social model, (b) the psychological model, and (c) the biological model. Women’s

narratives revealed a strong interconnection between these models and, in practice,

they held multiple causal models simultaneously – a finding consistent with previous

EM research in a number of populations (Bhui et al., 2006; Williams & Healy, 2001).

11.6.1 Social model

Women made sense of problems described in vignettes through the characters’ social

environment (past and present) – through poverty, loss and interpersonal conflicts.

This finding is consistent with EM research in Zambia (Aidoo & Harpham, 2001),

234

India (Patel et al., 1998) and amongst Western populations (Bhui et al., 2006;

Williams & Healy, 2001). Women’s social narratives also revealed two further social

subcategories consisting of (a) material and (b) relational factors. Material factors

included post-resettlement hardships such as housing, unemployment, transport and

difficulty accessing support services. Relational explanations included family and

community-wide relationship problems (especially household problems, parenting

and the quality of marital relationships). In almost all interviews group participants

attributed multiple material and relational resettlement stressors to the vignettes.

As discussed previously (see Section 9.3.2), some external social stressors were seen

as more severe than others. Upsetting (e.g., death of a family member) and traumatic

(e.g., witnessing the murder of a family member) experiences with significant social

consequences were deemed more severe. For example, the death of a spouse was

perceived as more serious in the absence of other family members to care for the

widow/er. The impact of any post-resettlement stressor was perceived to be greater in

the absence of family support. Marital disharmony was a particularly pertinent and

serious source of stress. Women’s emphasis on social, particularly socioeconomic and

interpersonal, stressors is consistent with other EM mental health research (Aidoo &

Harpham, 2001; Chipimo, Tuba, & Fylkesnes, 2011). Community surveys in Western

societies also show that the majority of people similarly believe that mental illness is

socially rather than biologically rooted (McKeon & Carrick, 1991; Priest, Vize,

Roberts, Roberts, & Tylee, 1996).

It is striking that interpersonal conflicts played such a significant role in women’s

EMs even though they were not explicitly suggested in the vignettes. Karen women’s

emphasis on social and particularly relational factors may be understood as a feature

of their collectivist cultural context. Women’s identities were bound to others through

interpersonal networks and interdependency was highly valued. Consequently, they

felt vulnerable and stressed when experiencing threats to their interdependency and

collective identity. Material resettlement stressors (e.g., housing and poverty) were

also used to describe the vignette characters’ emotional distress but were generally

given less emphasis.

Notably, drugs and alcohol were not identified as factors directly causing the

symptoms described in the vignettes and this could be explained by the use of a

235

female character in the vignettes. Participants reported that substance use problems

were markedly more common amongst Karen men and identified their husbands’

alcohol problems as a source of stress (see Section 8.3.3). My study focused on

women and revealed gendered aspects to emotional distress for both men and women.

Future research is needed to explore issues with Karen men.

11.6.2 Psychological model

Psychological factors played a central role in women’s causal explanations for all of

the vignettes. Symptoms of emotional distress were conceptualised as problems

related to mind and cognition – most commonly as thinking too much (hso k’moe

dha) about one’s difficulties. Thus, the relationship between social stressors and

emotional distress was not seen as direct. Rather, the effects of external (particularly

interpersonal) stressors were mediated by the ability of the mind-self to control the

impact of these experiences.

Concepts of self and mind appeared implicit within Karen women’s EMs of mental

illness. However, we should be wary of making assumptions about the nature of their

perceived mind and self. Several lines of empirical evidence suggest that notions of

self and mind differ both cross-culturally and historically (Cromby & Standen, 1999;

Hollan, 2010). Specifically, Western cultures emphasise a Cartesian dualism between

mind or self and social persona. In a widely quoted portrayal, Geertz (1979) described

the Western notion of self as a:

bounded, unique, more-or-less integrated motivational and cognitive universe,

a dynamic centre of awareness, emotion, judgement and action, organized into

a distinctive whole and set contrastively against other such wholes and against

a social and natural background. (p. 229)

In contrast, several other cultural groups tend to convey a sociocentric concept of self

and emphasise a sense of interdependent personhood (Bhugra, 2004). Similarly,

Karen women’s narratives tended to integrate discussions of self with their collective

and social context.

Karen women’s psychological narratives for depression were similar to those of the

Baganda people in Uganda. Okello and Ekblad (2006) used a vignette of depression

236

and found their Baganda participants identified the problem as an “illness of

thoughts” (p. 290), an affliction but not a medical condition caused by thinking too

much about social problems, especially (amongst women) marital relationship

problems. The Baganda women believed that a person should seek to control their

thoughts. Similarly, Karen women invoked themselves as contributing to emotional

distress and mental illness through a failure to control one’s thinking. These findings

also share similarities with EM research in Western populations (Bhui et al., 2006).

For example, first-time patients attending a community mental health service in

Wales described an implicit sense of “mind/self” which lacked “strength” (Williams

& Healy, 2001, p. 471) to control cognitive functioning.

The causal notion of failing to control one’s thinking holds potentially interesting

moral consequences and implications. In the Karen cultural context psychological

causal explanations of distress and the concept of the mind-self might be understood

as a feature of religious context (see Section 10.2.4). Meta-cognition (thinking about

thinking) and mental regulation is a central tenet of Buddhist doctrine (Toneatto,

2002). Similarly, self-control as a “moral muscle” (Baumeister & Exline, 1999, p.

1165) is also a noted feature of Christian thought. In this study participants did not

appear to make a moral distinction between those who could control their own minds

and those who failed. During group interviews some women freely, and without an

apparent sense of self-deprecation, noted their inability to control their thinking. Other

women openly expressed confidence in their ability to control their minds. However,

women who explicitly acknowledged their failure to control their mind also tended to

emphasise the severity of their external stressors. As Karen women’s causal

explanations for emotional and mental distress are multifactorial it is possible that

emphasising the contributory nature of external causes buffered moral judgment

related to weak self-control.

It is noteworthy that women’s EMs, particularly for the depression vignette, did not

refer to guilt, which is a cognitive feature of mood disorder in Western psychiatry

(American Psychiatric Association, 2000, p. 356). In her research with Chinese-

American women, Ying (1990) also noted the absence of guilt as a feature of EMs of

depression. She suggested that Chinese cultural conceptions of mental health

emphasised both intra- and interpersonal harmony whereas Western psychiatric

237

frameworks are influenced by Northern European Protestant traditions that are

individualistic and oriented towards guilt as a feature of emotional distress.

Baumeister and Exline (1999) similarly note that individual judgment and guilt with

regard to failures to exert self-control are prominent in Christian thought. Collectivist

cultures have been described as “shame cultures” (Hofstede, 2001, p. 229) where

socially undesirable behaviour is kept in order by the threat of shame to the guilty

party as well as her family. Controlling one’s thinking and expressions of distress

may be a means of avoiding shame and stigmatisation. Lebra (2010) came to a

different conclusion, arguing, with reference to Japanese society, that guilt is at least

as prevalent as shame. However, Karen women’s EMs seem best accounted for by

Hofstede’s model and, in part, Ying’s elucidation.

11.6.3 Biological model

Women’s biological or biophysical narratives attributed worry, stress and symptoms

of distress such as sleeping problems directly to physical ailments such as heart

disease. In addition, women’s bio-narratives ascribed physical illness and symptoms

to social stressors and psychological problems. For example, thinking too much about

one’s marital problems could cause high blood pressure.

Irregular menstruation and general blood circulation (including high blood pressure)

were identified as potential causes and consequences of the symptoms described in

the vignettes of depression and panic disorder. Women feared that blood

dysregulation from thinking too much and emotional distress could lead to serious

health problems including strokes. As previously noted (see Section 9.4.1), such

beliefs appear to have their origin in traditional Karen medicine which, having

components of both Ayurvedic and traditional Chinese medicine, stresses the

importance of adequate unrestricted blood flow (Bodeker et al., 2005). Women

reported “genetics” as a cause of dha blu but did not offer this cause as an explanation

for any of the vignettes including Aunty C (who some believed was suffering from

dha blu). As discussed, this may reflect their uncertainty as to whether Aunty C

actually had dha blu and women’s hesitancy to suggest genetic causes in the absence

of additional information about Aunty C and her family.

238

11.6.4 Course, severity and impact

Women’s narratives about the vignettes revealed EMs in which mind (psyche or

thinking), body (soma) and environment (material and relational) were separate

though interrelated domains. Each domain could cause, exacerbate or help heal

emotional distress. Although women described psychological impacts (e.g., difficulty

concentrating, irritability and anhedonia) of emotional distress and mental illness,

most postulated the experience and impact of these problems as somatic (e.g., aches

and pains) and interpersonal (e.g., impaired social relationships). Participants believed

that Aunty A and C would likely be impaired performing their daily tasks such as

parenting and education, but thought that Aunty B would be able to carry on despite

her difficulties. Social isolation was deemed a serious cause and consequence relevant

to all the vignettes.

11.6.5 Explanatory models and treatment

All participants described community support as essential regardless of the vignette or

casual explanation assigned to the symptoms. Triangulating the findings reported in

Chapter 10, Karen women described lay help from family, friends and community

leaders as the primary source of interpersonal help. Women also ascribed a

psychological model to healing and emphasised that people suffering from the

symptoms described in the depression and panic disorder vignettes should seek to

control their own minds. However, self-coping was not suggested as a feature in

relation to the PTSD vignette which was identified as both severe tha dha hsar

(sickness of the heart-mind) and dha blu (madness).

That community support was central to recovery from emotional distress and mental

illness is not surprising given that interpersonal conflict was identified as a salient

cause of suffering. The women valued strong social networks and viewed isolation is

both a cause and symptom of suffering. Participants’ perceived causes and relevant

coping or treatment strategies were multifactorial which meant that their EMs were,

by and large, internally consistent. EM research suggests that this is not always the

case and beliefs regarding causes and treatments of mental illness do not necessarily

always correlate. For example, in Bhui et al.’s (2006) study White British participants

who endorsed spiritual causal explanations nevertheless preferred medical treatment.

239

Among Black Caribbean participants who endorsed spiritual causes, self-coping and

complementary therapies were the preferred treatments.

Women did not tend to emphasise a need for professional medical or mental help.

When doctors or hospitals were suggested, it was also understood that simultaneous

community support would be crucial to recovery. However, most participants

believed that some of the physical symptoms of emotional distress and mental illness

could be helped by Western biomedicine. This was emphasised more for the PTSD

vignette as well as for the physical complaints identified in the panic vignette.

Western medicine and medical treatment were thought to be beneficial for physical

ailments causing distress, as well as for some psychological symptoms (e.g., women

thought that medication to aid sleeping might assist someone who is thinking too

much). However, women did not generally ascribe the need for psychopharmacology

for suffering attributed to social stressors. They appeared to view such symptoms as

transient problems likely to resolve by an improvement in social circumstances.

11.7 Reflections

This account of EMs has described how these Karen women interpreted and

organised mental phenomena. The findings demonstrate that the emic (Karen

women’s ideologies) and etic (Western ideologies) notions of emotional and mental

distress shared many similarities. For example, community surveys in White Western

countries have similarly found that anxiety and depressive disorders are most likely to

be attributed to social rather than biological causes (McKeon & Carrick, 1991; Priest

et al., 1996). However, there were also some important distinctions. The Karen

women did not endorse a clear dichotomy between mind and body but viewed the

two, as well as the broader social environment, as interrelated. Harmony in all

domains (including blood flow) was considered important to emotional and mental

wellbeing. Although the symptoms of common mental disorders (anxiety and

depression) may be cross-culturally valid, the conceptualisation of these problems as

psychiatric disorders appears less applicable to the Karen.

The information obtained from these EM group interviews reinforced data collected

for other parts of the study and again confirmed that serious mental illness such as

psychosis (e.g., dha blu) were not perceived to be prevalent in the Karen community

240

in Australia. These findings in no way establish that Karen refugees in Australia do

not experience severe mental illness. However, it is conceivable that (as Karen

participants suggested) people and families with such problems do not tend to resettle.

It is also possible that participants wished to present a positive image of their

community and were hesitant to discuss instances in Australia. This latter explanation

seems less likely as participants freely recounted numerous stories suggestive of

serious mental illness in the refugee camps. Although UNHCR resettlement

determination criteria do not explicitly exclude applicants with severe mental illness

(UNHCR, 2004), it seems feasible that these individuals fail to adequately complete

the application and screening process. It therefore seems that participants were

describing a kind of healthy migrant effect in which immigrants are, on average,

healthier than the native-born population (Fennelly, 2007; Razum, Zeeb, &

Rohrmann, 2000). Notwithstanding that refugees experience poor physical health in

most domains (Hollifield et al., 2002), the notion of a “mentally healthy refugee

effect” warrants further investigation. It has implications for humanitarian and health

service policy and service provision.

It is noteworthy that the supernatural did not feature strongly in women’s accounts of

the vignettes. Murdock, Wilson and Frederick (1978) developed a system for

categorising causation theories of illness. According to their model, supernatural

beliefs include mystical (e.g., fate, astrology, luck and ominous visions), animistic

(e.g., spirit possession and soul loss), and magical (e.g., witchcraft and sorcery)

explanations. Murdock et al.’s dimensions do not exhaustively cover all EMs

documented in cross-cultural research (Eisenbruch & Handelman, 1989) and have

been criticised as “anthropologically unacceptable” (Eisenbruch, 1990, p. 713) due to

the insistence on fitting emic concepts into Western categories. However, their

framework provides a point of reference for understanding different types of

supernatural explanations.

Karen women did not describe witchcraft, magic, spirits and God as causal

explanations for the vignettes although they considered supernatural factors

(especially God) as highly relevant to healing or coping and reported supernatural

causes as relevant to dha blu (see Section 9.3.4). One participant mentioned that God

never listened to her when she conveyed her distress about her missing children, but

241

none directly cited God as causing their emotional distress. These findings contrast

with EM studies in a number of African, Asian and White Western populations

(Aidoo & Harpham, 2001; Bhui et al., 2006; Eisenbruch, 1983; Okello & Ekblad,

2006; Patel, 1995). Zambian women’s EMs for common mental disorders included

causal explanations based on fate, spirits and God (Aidoo & Harpham, 2001). In the

United Kingdom Bhui et al. (2006) compared the EMs of Bangladeshi, White British

and Black Caribbean participants. They found that independent of ethnic group,

participants with a common mental disorder (depression or anxiety) were more likely

to give a spiritual explanation for their distress.

The relative absence of supernatural causal explanations in Karen women’s EMs of

mental illness may be attributable to genuine changes to traditional Karen cultural and

religious beliefs (see Section 9.5.2). A second possible explanation is that women

were reluctant to acknowledge such beliefs due to social pressure and the

predominance of Christians amongst Karen in Sydney (and consequently in the group

interviews). Karen women may have been reluctant to express viewpoints or endorse

perspectives that are not supported by the Christian church as most were emotionally

and practically supported by Christian organisations. It is therefore conceivable that

participants might have held traditional supernatural beliefs relating to emotional

distress and mental illness but abstained from expressing these beliefs in a public

forum.

11.7.1 The ordinariness of suffering

Refugee researchers P. Guerin and Guerin (2007) have spoken of the importance of

showing the “ordinariness” (p. 159) of refugee communities as well as highlighting

their unique characteristics. Accordingly, this chapter has identified parallels between

Karen women’s EMs and those of White Western lay participants in other EM studies

(Bhui et al., 2006; Williams & Healy, 2001). It is apparent that these groups shared

many similarities when it came to beliefs about the cause(s), course and treatment of

emotional distress. The findings support the observations of Williams and Healy

(2001) who stated:

Although cultural variation in definitions of mental illness do exist this does

not necessarily support the argument that mental illness is entirely socially

242

constructed … Interpretations may be influenced by cultural factors but the

inherent nature of the phenomena may constrain the possible variety of those

interpretations. (p. 466)

Women’s models described stress and pressure as building up and resulting in

emotional distress as well as a trajectory of distress along which a person with a

vulnerable personality or disposition might travel. They believed that social factors

led to stress manifested somatically (aches and pains), psychologically (as thinking

too much) and socially (as interpersonal anhedonia and social withdrawal). Failure to

control thinking and seek resolution of social stressors (through problem solving and

support from others) in turn led to a worsening of somatic, social and psychological

symptoms. Here Karen women’s prominent conceptualisations share features with the

Western folk notion of the nervous breakdown which, as Williams and Healy (2001)

suggest, “is likely to be the most familiar folk illness” (p. 466) in Western society. In

an American community survey, EMs for nervous breakdowns emphasised stress and

pressure as central causes (Omark, 1980).

Prior to 1980, when it was controversially removed from official psychiatric

nomenclature (American Psychiatric Association, 1980), the concept of neuroses had

been central to Western psychiatric discourse (Bayer & Spitzer, 1985; Koppe, 2009).

The term had been used to represent the “nervous” personality and encompassed a

broad range of mental illnesses (now mostly assumed under the rubric of mood and

anxiety disorders), specifically excluding psychotic states. The Oxford English

Dictionary defines neurosis as any “psychological disorder in which there is disabling

or distressing anxiety, without severe disorganisation or distortion of behaviour or

personality” (“Neurosis”, 2011). In contemporary psychiatric discourse many

researchers now refer to “common mental disorders” as a “a group of distress states,

previously termed neuroses, that describe states of anxiety and depression” (Cooper et

al., 2008; I. A. Lang, Llewellyn, Hubbard, Langa, & Melzer, 2011; Patel et al., 1998,

p. 135). Karen women’s EMs contribute to cross-cultural research suggesting that the

concept of neurosis aptly captures the complaints of primary healthcare patients

across a range of cultures (Aidoo & Harpham, 2001; Ormel et al., 1994). Similarly,

when researchers explored the emic Zimbabwean concept of kufungisisa / thinking

243

too much they suggested that it seemed to be “closest to the Euro-American concept

of neurotic illness” (Patel, Gwanzura, Simunyu, Lloyd, & Mann, 1995, p. 1192).

11.7.2 Talking about trauma

The vignette EM method allowed for Karen women’s discourse about the impact of

discrete war-related stressors and daily social stressors to be examined. Their

emphasis on the salience of post-migration problems echo findings from a recent

investigation into the relationships between psychometric test scores and pre- and

post-migration stressors. Carswell, Blackburn and Barker (2011) found an association

between higher numbers of trauma events and greater emotional distress. However,

when post-migration problems were added to their regression models, number of

traumas was no longer a significant predictor of distress. Rather, resettlement

difficulties were associated with higher PTSD symptoms. Loss of culture and low

social support was predictive of emotional distress.

Karen women believed that most survivors of war-related trauma could cope well

with their life circumstances. They acknowledged that under certain circumstances a

minority of community members would suffer severe and chronic distress in part

related to traumatic war events but the social consequence of a trauma appeared to

mediate the perceived severity of the experience.

Women’s accounts also indicated that traumatic experiences exist “along a

continuum” (Kohrt & Hruschka, 2010, p. 339) and suggest that the “trauma”

experienced by refugee populations may be very different to the discrete horrific

incidents experienced by the majority of Western-European trauma-survivors who

comprise the bulk of participants in trauma-related research (and whose experiences

have guided the development of contemporary treatment protocols). It would be

wrong to assume that the post-resettlement lives of Karen women are characterised by

the absence of traumatic stress. Acute traumatic life events (as commonly

conceptualised in Western psychiatric nosology) represent one end of the spectrum.

At the other end of the continuum are poverty and other daily stressors which, for

refugees, include continued fear for the safety of friends and family left behind in

refugee camps and war zones.

244

Women’s accounts of the PTSD vignette revealed that Karen conceptualisations of

suffering and distress did not explicitly account for anything similar to the Western

psychiatric diagnosis of PTSD. Indeed, it seemed difficult to speak about the

emotional effects of trauma due to the lack of a linguistic corollary. Researchers have

noted the absence of linguistic corollaries for psychological trauma in other Asian

populations (A. Hinton, 2007; Kohrt & Hruschka, 2010). Previous research with

refugee populations from Burma has also reported the absence of an exact translation

for the term trauma. Lopes Cardozo and colleagues (2004) studied 495 Karenni

refugees and noted the absence of a PTSD-like concept. In his doctoral thesis with

Karen refugees in Thailand, Fuertes (2004, 2007) too documented the lack of a

linguistic corollary and conceptual crossover. However, he noted that contact with

humanitarian organisations had led some Karen refugees to come up with a local term

to match the foreign term utilised by these agencies. Fuertes worked with his Karen

participants to develop a working definition of trauma and arrived at a phrase

meaning “scar of suffering” (2007, p. 185). In the present study I did not ask

participants to come up with a Karen word for the English term trauma and

participants did not spontaneously offer one. In the Karen context, the experience of

trauma was absorbed within existing cultural idioms of distress.

Participant observation provided triangulated confirmation of the findings from

interviews. At a local service provider’s community consultation with Karen

community members, a Karen man described the lack of cultural fit for the term: “we

are not familiar with ‘trauma’. I don’t think it is useful for us, it doesn’t relate to our

daily experiences” (Statement heard during participant observation on 28 March,

2009). The inner logic of trauma as a wound to the psyche does not appear to be

apparent in Karen conceptions of their emotional world. These findings add support to

reservations about the utility and ethics of imposing a Western psychiatric nosological

category in other cultures where it may not correlate with local understandings of

distress (D. E. Hinton & Lewis-Fernandez, 2010a; Kleinman, 1980, 1995; Kohrt &

Hruschka, 2010; Marsella, Friedman, & Spain, 1996).

11.7.3 Illuminating diversity

These findings illuminate the depth and diversity of cultural idioms and EMs. They

also provide a lens through which to illuminate Western cultural assumptions and

245

beliefs about emotional distress and mental illness and the human mind. In

documenting the diversity of Karen women’s idioms of distress I remain wary of their

potential for misuse. Ethnographic methods and understandings of local idioms of

distress have produced, in some instances, a rather confused logic with potentially

harmful ramifications. Service providers of refugee mental and psychosocial

healthcare have been noted to simply use “culturally sensitive” idioms of distress as

linguistic tools to pair with Western psychiatric diagnostic categories (Abramowitz,

2010). It is important to preserve cultural models of mind and human behaviour. As

Hollifield (2005) has stated “The world loses culture when dominant countries export

their views and countries lose historical artifacts” (p. 1284). Similarly, Watters (2010)

warns that “Indigenous forms of mental illness and healing are being bulldozed by

disease categories and treatments made in the USA” (p. 3). He refers to cross-cultural

mental health researchers (psychiatrists, psychologists and anthropologists) as

“psychology’s version of botanists in the rain forest, desperate to document the

diversity while staying only a few steps ahead of the bulldozers” (p. 7). With refugee

mental health services continuing to be framed within a trauma discourse, the

diversity of distress is likely to be compacted. These research findings suggest that a

more collectivist and socially located discourse of human suffering might better

capture Karen women’s experience of emotional and mental suffering.

246

Chapter 12

Invisible Clients and Invisible Problems:

Experiences and Perceptions of Settlement Services

12.1 Introduction

This chapter begins to explore the findings related to the research question: How are

some of the current services provided for Karen refugee women perceived and how

might they be improved?

When participants were invited to speak about service provisions they did so broadly,

discussing both generalist resettlement services such as housing provided under HSS

as well as specific health and mental health services. This thesis focuses on mental

health and did not aim to comprehensively explore issues related to the generalist

HSS program. However, as discussed in Chapter 8, many of the causes of women’s

distress were social and related to environmental mastery, language and

communication as well as limitations within the HSS programs designed to address

these issues. Given the importance of these issues to women’s wellbeing, it would be

irresponsible to dismiss the study insights relating to service limitations. Hence this

chapter addresses the findings relating to generalist HSS services and Chapter 13

focuses upon health and mental health services. Implications and recommendations

for both generalist and mental health service providers are discussed in Chapter 14.

12.2 Positive perceptions

12.2.1 “Here it’s the best”

Overall, women were extremely grateful to the Australian Government and all HSS

providers. They wished to “say a big thank you” because they felt “well looked after

and supported in many ways”. A significant strength of current service provisions was

assistance with meeting women’s critical basic needs of shelter, safety and food, as

well as access to education and healthcare. Though many women continued to

experience difficulty in these domains (see Chapter 8), all felt “secure and safe” in

comparison to living in Burma and refugee camps where they “always had to be

afraid of people”.

247

12.2.2 Community connection

Women unanimously perceived the Sydney Karen community as a critical provider of

support and a key link connecting them to HSS providers. A major strength of HSS

providers was their association with Karen community groups and leaders. In many

cases these links were created and maintained through the employment of Karen

community members as staff. These links appeared to be crucial to the ability of

organisations to disseminate information and provide support to the Karen refugee

community.

12.3 Learning about limitations

Karen women praised helpful and pleasant service providers but they often hesitated

to verbalise direct criticism when dissatisfied. Instead, they employed certain phrases

and gestures to make non-committal or ambiguous responses when asked about

negative experiences. For example, if asked about problems with GPs, a woman

might say “some women experience that” or “yes, something like that happens”, but

not give a detailed account. In this manner, women managed their discussions of

negative or sensitive topics. Very few women took a clear-cut stance on potentially

sensitive issues. Those who did had often worked with or been trained by human

rights organisations in the refugee camps. Most women spoke allusively and

cautiously, and avoided directly criticising service providers. As Hofstede (2001) has

noted, these patterns of communication are common in collectivist societies.

However, socio-political explanations might better account for women’s reluctance to

criticise service providers. In Burma fear, distrust and danger have dominated social

life (Suu Kyi, 2010) and, as Argenti-Pillen (2003) explains, equivocal discourse may

be employed as an “efficient safety mechanism” (p. 116) in such contexts.

Despite their evasive communication, women did, in their own way, express their

attitudes. They remained silent when asked whether an encounter with a service

provider had been satisfactory, pleasant or helpful encounter. Their silence stood in

stark contrast to their praise, providing a tacit understanding of their perspective. I

positioned women’s silences and ambiguous discourse alongside the comments of key

informants to understand perceptions of service providers. Interpreters were

particularly pertinent sources of information as they had witnessed the encounters

248

first-hand and were also often privy to women’s remarks that were not expressed to

the service provider.

12.4 Case management and orientation support

HSS case managers were most women’s first point of contact in Australia, meeting

them at the airport upon their arrival. As outlined in Chapter 4 (see Section 4.3.2),

case management was provided through DIAC’s HSS and consisted of six

components: (i) needs assessment and case management plan, (ii) transit assistance,

(iii) reception, property induction and initial food provision, (iv) essential

registrations, (v) orientation, and (vi) health services (Refugee Council of Australia,

2011b).

Most women reported positively on the quality of case management support that they

received but felt that the duration was too short. Case managers assisted women with

tasks such as using public transport, attending a GP, going to Centrelink and using a

bank. However, most women explained that they were assisted with each of these

activities “once or twice”. This duration of support seemed to be inadequate as several

women reported that they did not feel comfortable to repeat them alone. Moreover,

although recipients of visa subclass 204 (Women at Risk visa) were reportedly told

they would receive an additional six months’ support, many reported that they did not.

Finally, a minority of women perceived that their case manager provided them with

less support because they did not share their religious or political ideologies. The

presence and ongoing impact of political factions is common to all communities and

has been noted to affect other resettled refugee populations (McMichael &

Manderson, 2004).

12.5 Communication issues

Difficulty accessing appropriate interpreters was one of the most salient problems

affecting Karen refugee women when using service providers. As discussed in

Chapter 4 (see Section 4.4), DIAC funded the TIS to assist people who do not speak

English and the service providers who work with them. TIS provided the following

interpreting services: (a) pre-booked telephone interpreting (available for all

languages), (b) 24-hour automated telephone interpreting (in 18 “high-demand”

languages not including Karen), and (c) on-site interpreting (which may be booked up

249

to 28 days in advance). Doctors were eligible for free TIS services and access to a 24-

hour priority line (DIAC, 2009b). TIS contracted interpreters who were accredited or

recognised by NAATI. At the time this research was undertaken NATTI reported 11

Accredited and 8 Recognised Karen interpreters in Australia (three of the eight

Recognised interpreters were in NSW) (Personal communication, R. Ganesan,

NAATI, 1 December, 2010). The following were the key communication- and

interpreter-related problems Karen women experienced.

12.5.1 Not Korean, not Burmese

According to participants, some service providers lacked awareness of the Karen

ethnic group. They misunderstood when Karen refugees requested a Karen interpreter

and provided them with a Korean interpreter instead. Participants often discussed this

problem with humour – “they think we are Korean! [laughs]”, an observation also

made by Karen refugees in Minnesota (S. Smith, 2010). For Karen women in Sydney

such problems led to lost time, interruptions to education and employment and

reduced access to quality healthcare. This problem also negatively impacted families

and communities as women relied more heavily on leaders and the few community

members who were proficient English speakers. The following quotation from a

group interview highlights the frustration that many women felt:

We request for an interpreter but we get the wrong one. We feel very angry and

upset because we have to return home empty handed without getting any assistance.

We feel that they don’t want to attend appointments any more. You don’t know –

next time it may happen again then you have wasted your time, your day, and you

return home with nothing. We are supposed to go to school but we have to cancel

our class to attend appointments yet we end up with nothing. It’s very frustrating. It

happens again and again. (Participant in Group D)

A related issue was lack of understanding of ethnic differences amongst refugees

from Burma. Some service provider staff assumed that all people from Burma speak

Burmese. Karen with very limited English language proficiency had trouble

articulating the difference. They were then provided with a Burmese-speaking

interpreter whom they could not understand.

250

12.5.2 (Mis)trust and ethnic tension

According to Dr N, phone interpreters may be good for a “non-traumatised

population” but perhaps not refugees from Burma who were accustomed to

surveillance by military intelligence services. She reported: “they are paranoid, they

don’t know who it is. Is it government intelligence? How can they know who they are

talking to?”. Another key informant reported that her refugee clients did not always

trust that their interpreter(s) accurately conveyed their conversations:

I have referred them to [mental health services] but their feedback is “I don’t know,

the interpreter in between is saying things, like mixing it up”. (Key informant – Ms

L)

Dr N and Ms L raise issues of (mis)trust that resonate with research by Hynes (2003)

who referred to a “boundless social universe of mistrust” (p. 2) affecting refugees

from Burma. Ethnic and political mistrust amongst populations affected by

persecution and forced migration has been widely documented (Daniel & Knudsen,

1995) and the potential effects on interactions between refugees and service providers

noted (Thampy & Buhgra, 2004; Tribe, 1999a). Karen refugees have suffered years of

persecution under the Burmese military regime and for some this had resulted in

hostility towards Burmese as reflected below:

Is there a problem with ethnic issues, like not trusting Burmese?

Oh, yeah, yeah. Some are still there! We should change our mind but some are still

there. They come from the hills and mountain areas so they don’t have much

education or experience dealing with other [non-Karen] people. They came from

there and ran, ran, ran. They came to refugee camps and then here to Australia. In

their mind they think “Burmese is not good” because they had a brother or father

killed by a Burmese soldier, they still have it in their mind. (Mu Htòh).

The hostility towards Burmese that Mu Htòh alludes to above was also noted by Dr F

who spoke of “problems trusting the interpreter” in situations where “the interpreter

belongs to a different group like Burmese” because of the “atrocity [the Karen]

experienced”.

Interestingly, none of the Karen women explicitly verbalised their own prejudice

against Burmese (or other ethnic groups) and appeared uncomfortable when explicitly

251

asked about this issue. This is likely due to women’s general reluctance to overtly

criticise or complain and their avoidance of political dialogue. However, when given

a choice of preference between Burmese-Karen and Karen interpreters, all reported

that they wished to utilise Karen-speaking interpreters. I did not inquire as to whether

the women preferred a Karen-speaking interpreter whom they personally knew within

their community or not. However, some key informants highlighted issues of

confidentiality, pointing out that in certain circumstances women preferred Karen-

speaking “outsiders” (i.e., community members not personally known to them).

12.5.3 Interpreter availability

Participants described being unable to secure an interpreter when needed and

experienced this problem at Job Services Australia, HSS providers, hospitals and GP

clinics. The findings from this study contrast greatly with the TIS 2010 client

satisfaction survey findings which reported that 98% of respondents were able to

engage a TIS telephone interpreter every time or most times they requested one

(“Client satisfaction survey”, 2010). Some women preferred to use on-site interpreters

when available and found this service more reliable than telephone interpreting. The

following quotation from an interview with a key informant depicts a “catch-22”

situation in relation to women’s communication with Centrelink:

Centrelink say to call this number if you need an interpreter … but they can’t get the

Karen interpreter so we don’t know what’s going on … [However], Centrelink [are]

not very happy if you bring your own interpreter. They want their interpreter yet

they can’t call their interpreter for you! (Key informant – Ms S)

Other women similarly reported that Centrelink staff became frustrated when friends

or family members assisted them with interpretation and translation. This situation

was especially problematic for illiterate women who reported that staff were irritated

by their need for assistance to complete forms. Women were upset when Centrelink

staff reacted negatively to them bringing their friends to interpret. Typical of many

women, Lah Luh reported that such reactions from staff made her “get a headache”

and “want to cry”. However, the Centrelink staff were unlikely to perceive such an

impact as many of the women avoided overt expression of their distress. Interactions

with government service providers such as Centrelink and Job Services Australia

252

were a significant source of distress amongst Karen women (see Sections 8.2.2 and

8.3.2).

12.6 Lack of bilingual, bicultural and female service providers

Karen women expressed a preference for female service providers – especially when

addressing sensitive (in this context meaning body or private) topics – but seldom had

access to them at social support services. Male Karen HSS providers explained that

their female Karen clients were uncomfortable discussing sensitive issues such as

relationship problems or sexual violence with them. For example, typical of male

HSS providers, Mr H acknowledged that Karen women “culturally speaking don’t

want to talk about [sensitive issues] at all” but explained that they are even less likely

to disclose problems to a male service provider. Another male Karen service provider

also explained that discussing such topics placed him in a thorny social position as he

risked offending leaders and being perceived to support behaviour(s) (e.g.,

remarriage) at odds with traditional Karen cultural customs (see Section 8.4).

When given the opportunity to suggest positive changes to HSS, a number of key

informants identified the employment of Karen women as cultural brokers to

disseminate information and provide support to the community. For example, Dr N

encouraged the use of Karen health educators to provide information and education

about nutrition. She described her Karen patients as “very quick learners”, adding:

“but it has to be from someone who speaks their language”. Other key informants

emphasised that cultural brokers shared more than language – they shared a “way” of

understanding and a cultural context. For example, Dr F argued that bilingual,

bicultural staff could bridge cultural gaps and provide support within Karen clients’

“model” of mental health:

Otherwise what happens is that this [Western] model and that [Karen] model are

different. The patient’s construct of illness and of mental health is different. (Key

informant – Dr F)

Literature supports Dr F’s view that shared language alone is not a magic solution to

communication in mental health contexts. For example, Sue (1998) argued that shared

cultural characteristics (e.g., attitudes and identity) may be more important in

therapeutic encounters.

253

Karen bilingual service providers confirmed that their clients were grateful and “more

comfortable” when able to communicate in their first language. They also reported

that shared knowledge of cultural idiosyncrasies helped them to understand their

Karen clients. However, even bilingual female service providers did not assume that

ethnic matching would automatically bring rapport. They emphasised that, from a

cultural perspective, Karen women were reluctant to disclose personal problems in a

professional context and also drew attention to their pervasive mistrust of others due

to their experiences of persecution.

12.7 Barriers to English language education

Language barriers were the most significant resettlement stressors encountered by

Karen women, especially those who arrived with low literacy due to gendered and

other pre-migration factors (see Section 8.2). This section explores further service

provision barriers including accessibility, availability and quality that compounded

women’s language problems.

As discussed (see Section 4.4), the Australian Government provided 510 hours of

English language tuition to humanitarian entrants through the AMEP. An additional

100 hours of tuition was available through the Special Preparatory Program to

humanitarian entrants with special needs including those with limited pre-migration

education and survivors of torture and trauma. Refugees were required to register for

the AMEP within six months of arrival and commence classes within twelve months.

According to DIAC, AMEP clients could choose from a range of learning options

including full- or part-time classroom tuition, telephone supported distance learning

or home tutoring with volunteer teachers (DIAC, 2011b). At the time of this study

thirteen service providers were contracted to provide AMEP services nationally.

12.7.1 Access barriers

Most Karen women (assisted by their HSS case managers) began classes at the AMEP

within their first few months in Australia. However, health or social factors restricted

some women’s ability to engage with AMEP at this time and they subsequently

slipped through the cracks. Unfamiliar with their rights and the services available, and

without advocacy, they had not instigated further educational support. Other women

254

had been unsuccessful in accessing home tutoring and encountered long wait-lists,

hindering their ability to adequately adjust to resettlement.

For example, Pàw Ta Shu had been living in Australia for over a year but had not had

access to AMEP services. Upon her arrival in Australia she had been unable to attend

AMEP classes due to serious health problems. Her case manager had explained that

she could receive English tuition from a home tutor but this had not been followed up.

During fieldwork I assisted and observed Pàw Ta Shu as she attempted to register

with and engage AMEP services. During her initial AMEP assessment there was no

bilingual assistant available despite Pàw Ta Shu having requested one when the

appointment was made. She was assessed as eligible for the Home Tutor Scheme and

informed of a three month waiting list, as volunteers must be matched with a student

by geographic area and gender. Seven months later Pàw Ta Shu had still not been

contacted by the AMEP service provider.

Prior to beginning classes, AMEP service providers conducted an assessment of each

individual’s English language skills. Despite women’s limited pre-migration

education none of the women I interviewed had received additional tuition through

the Special Preparatory Program. This suggests that the Special Preparatory Program

was failing to identify and provide services to eligible Karen refugees.

12.7.2 Classroom and teaching barriers

Cultural and societal characteristics (e.g., compliance, fear and saving face) appeared

to impair women’s ability to benefit from AMEP services because of (a) how they

utilised these services, and (b) how service providers perceived and reacted to their

behaviour (see Section 8.2.1). Women were reluctant to overtly express emotional

distress or dissatisfaction and, as a result, some AMEP providers overlooked their

needs and difficulties. The following participant observation field note regarding an

AMEP classroom encounter demonstrates this issue. It makes evident the necessity of

cross-cultural training and sensitivity to different refugee communities.

Inside the classroom there were 12 students, all sitting in rows facing the

teacher and white board. There were 4 Karen students and 8 from another

country sitting on different sides of the room. The lesson was about how to get

a ticket to catch a train, students were writing on paper and also on the white

255

board. Teacher D sat down and spoke with me briefly while the students

worked on their writing. He asked about my research and I explained my

interest in the emotional wellbeing of Karen women. He immediately

responded by saying that I should be studying the refugee women from the

other country and not the Karen. He reported that refugee women from that

other country had “been through a lot” and remarked that some of them had

“lost their children”. He reported that they cry a lot when they think of or

speak about their past and are sometimes so distressed that they need to leave

the class. Teacher D went on to say that the Karen “don’t have those

problems”. He made these comments in front of Karen students, implying that

their experiences and needs were not as critical as those of their classmates.

(Participant observation field note: date removed to protect confidentiality)

This encounter at an AMEP service provider raises doubt about the cross-cultural

competency of the teacher observed. The DIAC Review of the Adult Migrant English

Program (DIAC, 2008b) addressed the “need for support in the training of teachers in

areas of: more information on clients’ cultural backgrounds and experiences, and

cross-cultural training” (p. 26). This ethnographic observation demonstrates that this

issue remains an important and urgent problem.

Further factors in the AMEP learning environment emerged as additional barriers to

language proficiency and were reiterated through ethnographic observations of AMEP

classes where Karen students participated. Firstly, most teachers were not native

English speakers, consequently students learnt pronunciation in heavily accented

second language English. Moreover, during observation I encountered teachers

frequently communicating in their own native language during class-time despite the

presence of students from dissimilar linguistic backgrounds. For example, Teacher C

frequently spoke in his native language, sometimes combining his native language

and English in a sentence. Many of the students in the class spoke the same native

language as Teacher C; however, many did not. Most did not have access to a

bilingual assistant teacher (from their own native language). Teacher C was

employing bilingual teaching methods in a multilingual class. Such observations draw

attention to some serious impediments affecting the ability of students to benefit from

the tuition provided and describe a classroom culture that fails to provide a respectful

256

learning environment. According to Karen women and AMEP teachers, limited

access to bilingual assistants in AMEP classes impacted negatively on women’s

ability to participate in, and benefit from, language tuition:

From my point of view, if there were an interpreter at [AMEP] school it would be

great. At the moment we attend and there is no interpreter there. Once in a while we

have one but there is only one interpreter and she needs to attend different classes so

she can’t be with us all the time … Although they teach us using pictures we still

can’t understand … we need someone to interpret. (Htòh)

Similarly, AMEP teachers valued the input of bilingual assistants but noted that

funding limited their availability:

They need bilingual support just to understand. [For example], if I ask them to

please read the questions and answers, they don’t know what it means to read the

questions and answer them so the bilingual support can explain that to them. (Key

informant / AMEP teacher – Mr E)

Karen women avoided directly criticising the Australian Government and AMEP

services and staff, aside from expressing their desire for more language tuition and

greater access to bilingual support. When Karen key informants were asked to

comment on the aforementioned participant observation encounters they concurred

with my trepidation regarding the AMEP service provider as well as the teachers

observed. One community leader explicitly stated that despite concerns he did not

make use of known avenues for complaint. Here too it seems that cultural patterns are

relevant to understanding their resettlement experiences and difficulties. This quiet,

compliant conduct may be detrimental to the wellbeing of the Karen community in

Sydney as their needs are easily overlooked and their problems unvoiced.

Various reports (DIAC, 2008b; Maher, 2006; Nega, 2008) have questioned the

adequacy of the number of hours of English language education provided to refugees

in Australia. This study suggests that number of hours is only one component of

effective education. Students’ learning is likely to remain hindered, despite increased

hours of tuition, if classroom practices and teaching methods are not also addressed.

Findings of this study also strongly indicate that the gendered aspects of refugee

education are important. Women had less pre-migration education than men and faced

257

difficulties both attending and engaging with English language classes in Australia.

This is similar to what is already documented in literature (Hatoss & Huijser, 2010).

In light of the limitations that Karen women have in accessing and utilising language

education services in Australia a “one size fits all” approach to refugee education is

likely to be suboptimal (Hatoss & Huijser, 2010, p. 149). The Australian Government,

education providers and researchers should explore barriers to language proficiency

and alternative solutions to meet the learning needs of different refugee communities.

Here too (see Section 12.3) this research has highlighted the need for future

researchers to use sensitive research methods to overcome refugees’ reluctance to

complain or criticise service providers. The National Client Satisfaction Survey report

(DIAC, 2008a) found 84% of clients reported overall happiness with their AMEP

classes. However, a twelve month DIAC-funded AMEP Research Centre

ethnographic study of AMEP clients completed in 2009, while reporting high client

satisfaction (DIAC, 2010a) noted that few participants, particularly students with

lower English language proficiency, criticised their AMEP experience.

12.8 Face value: limited perceptions of Karen women’s needs

Some cultural characteristics of the Karen appeared to contribute to inequalities in

access to services. Service providers’ failure to understand these cultural

characteristics is a limitation in current HSS service provisions for Karen refugee

women.

Karen participants collectively referred to themselves as “shy” and “easy people” (see

Section 8.5) and, in literature, are often portrayed as quiet and avoidant of conflict

(see Section 3.3.2). Typical of their comments, Ms K, reported: “[people] see the

Karen and they say, ‘Your people – how come [they are] so laid back?’”. Many of

these traits are common to collectivist societies (Gudykunst & Bond, 1999; Hofstede,

2001; Kagitcibasi, 1999). These customary characteristics also appear to have been

strengthened and in some cases distorted by persecution, prolonged political conflict

and forced migration. This is reflected in the following quotation from an interview

with Mr H, a Karen key informant.

It’s the culture … Karen people love to live quietly, simply, they don’t want to

disturb others … So when they are oppressed and when they are mistreated … they

258

try to control their feelings and emotions, rather than expressing and bursting out

with emotion. (Key informant – Mr H)

These patterns of behaviour were confirmed by my observations at service providers

where other community groups were far more vocal and visible whereas Karen clients

were reserved. The Karen community were perceived by both Karen and non-Karen

service providers to be adaptable and resilient. Service providers linked Karen

resilience to their strong religious faith, collective- and community-oriented approach,

and also to their normalisation of suffering. Most also described the Karen as “easy-

going”, compliant and non-complaining. Overall, the service providers I met liked

working with Karen clients, describing them as “proactive”, “hard working”, “a

pleasure to work with”, “adaptable”, “receptive”, “easily content” and “shy”.

Concerningly, some non-Karen service providers took their Karen clients’ self-

presentation at face value. These participants often described the Karen as “simple”,

“compliant”, “polite”, “smiling” and socially unsophisticated. They presented

positively skewed perceptions of the community and some lacked knowledge of

serious problems and issues affecting Karen women. For example, one non-Karen

service provider reported that domestic violence was not a problem in the community

“because they are Christian”. But, as explored in Chapter 8, domestic violence was

indeed a problem amongst the Karen community. The AMEP encounter with Teacher

D (see Section 12.7.2) also revealed that some service providers perceived the Karen

to be “fine” in comparison to other refugee communities, and believed they had not

endured suffering as severe as other (more vocal and emotionally expressive) refugee

populations.

The perceptions of Karen service providers differed from the face value approach

adopted by some non-Karen staff. Some Karen service providers believed that Karen

approaches to coping and resilience (such as not expressing distress and normalising

extreme suffering) could mask deeper emotional suffering and pain (see Sections

10.2.4 and 10.3.1). Karen service providers tended to view the community as shy and

troubled but also stoic and resilient. These findings point to the need for adequate

cultural competency training in order to orient service providers to the unique

circumstances of different refugee communities.

259

12.9 Elder and service provider burnout … “the profacticals”

As we saw in Chapter 10 (see Section 10.3.2), Karen leaders were central to

supporting the Karen community and were frequently the first point of contact for

women experiencing problems. They were relied upon for both practical assistance as

well as guidance and emotional support. It was therefore important to address their

perceptions of current service provisions as well as their perceptions of their own

roles. Service providers confirmed the important role of community leaders. For

example, a health service provider said his Karen patients were more likely to attend

follow-up appointments when their pastor encouraged attendance and helped to

arrange appointments. A limitation of current service provisions appeared to be the

excessive workloads and lack of support for Karen HSS staff and Karen community

leaders who often struggled to meet the demands of their roles.

Some of the Karen and Burmese community members employed as service providers

experienced a dissonance between the expectations and demands of their employers

and those of their community and clients:

I can’t, I just can’t, I [feel like] crying, I can’t explain. She [my employer] doesn’t

realise that I’m burning out. (Key informant – Ms L)

[My employer says] you work too much overtime. You have to take time in lieu.

How can I take time in lieu? They are ringing me all the time … I mean [you could]

say that we help the community, 24-hours, 8 days a week [laughs]! (Key informant

– Mr B)

Participants viewed their roles in the community as crucial. They explained that

effective support for community members required outreach and could not just be

provided in an isolated professional environment. Moreover, they felt that they would

lack credibility and “lose trust” in their professional roles if they attempted to distance

themselves or cease to meet the “after hour” demands of their clients. One key

informant coined the term “profactical” to describe the role of within-community

service providers:

We need to be practical and realistic … You are between professional and practical

… that’s why I have created a [new] word … profactical … As a [service provider]

your role is professional [but] from the community perspective you are doing

260

practical things. The word professional, professionalism talks about boundaries and

this and that. But when you are working in the community that doesn’t work

always. That is why I created the word [profactical]. (Key informant – Mr H)

Some leaders described a cumulative effect of pressure building from responding to

the needs of the community here in Sydney as well as listening and responding to the

needs of those who remained in Burma and on the Thailand-Burma border. These

pressures also affected their families and, in one case, severely affected a community

leader’s ability to maintain other commitments such as employment. In contrast, non-

Karen / non-Burmese service providers did not mention these problems. The

following quotations depict Karen key informants’ experiences and expressions of

burnout:

People talk to me unofficially, which makes me tired. I’m tired of this stuff … It’s

heavy. I get pain. I get neck pain. (Key informant – Ms L)

Even Saturday, Sunday, early morning, midnight, they ring me. You know, I can’t

handle it any more. (Key informant – Mr B)

You know they are quite demanding to the leaders and elders, so these people

become burnt out or, you know, exhausted … My position is with the community

[here and] also back there [in the refugee camps]. I [still] have a lot of connections

back there. So from the camps, over the border from time to time they contact me

with information, particularly needs and concerns so then it is hard to forget and

how do you have weekends or respite time? I mean 24/7 is not enough. During the

weekend, even regardless of day or night-time, the phone is ringing, information is

coming. (Key informant – Mr H)

Karen leaders and within-community service providers spontaneously explained their

motivations for continuing to help the community in their professional and

community-based volunteer capacities. For some their reasoning was based around a

moral desire to help others. Others perceived a “duty” and “obligation” related to the

historical and political context of the Karen people. Some helped as a means of

honouring those who had died throughout the many years of conflict:

[We leaders tell each other] don’t be demoralised because some people [in Burma]

gave their life and are fighting the Burmese [military]. They even gave their lives

261

and died … In serving them [the community] we don’t expect them to praise us, it’s

because of our duty and obligation. (Key informant – Mr P)

A few community leaders mused over their concern for each other and worried that

their commitment to the community was in fact (a) “burning” the community out with

too many events and expectations, and (b) “burning” the leaders out because their

motivation, although “good”, also stemmed from their own suffering. Participants

suggested that this problem is shared by elders in other refugee communities. The

potential for refugee community leaders and bilingual staff to face burnout has been

noted before (Baron, 2002; de Jong, 2002; Fadiman, 1997) but is seldom further

explored. These research findings suggest that this topic should be addressed in future

research.

12.10 Lack of awareness, understanding and cohesion between service

providers

During the course of research I heard of, and observed, some positive examples of

community organisations, HSS providers and other service providers (e.g., GPs)

working together well. For example, one health service provider shared a strong

relationship with a Karen community leader who regularly referred clients for

healthcare appointments and also monitored and encouraged ongoing attendance.

However, I often encountered misunderstandings, disparities and confusions between

different service providers. Most were aware of other organisations but did not always

fully understand precisely what was available from other providers. For example, an

AMEP teacher said he did not know what kind of support HSS case managers

provided. Some service providers lamented that short funding rounds hindered (a)

their capacity to provide and plan services, and (b) the ability of organisations to

become familiar with one another. Moreover, some HSS providers had doubts about

the appropriateness or ability of others to address the needs of their Karen clients. The

following quotation comes from an interview with a service provider who expressed

confusion and doubt about the relevance of another refugee support organisation and

their ability to meet her clients’ needs:

I really don’t know about how STARTTS work? How would [they] assess her?

That’s what I don’t understand. (Key informant – Ms L)

262

This issue was particularly pronounced in relation to informants’ opinions about

mental health support services. Further questioning revealed that health service

informants perceived a mismatch between services offered and the cultural beliefs of

their clients. A mismatch of perspectives and a lack of cultural fit regarding mental

health service provisions for Karen was also highlighted by Karen women and a

number of Karen key informants. These issues are addressed further in Chapter 13

(see Section 13.4).

12.11 Summary

Women generally expressed positive perceptions of services that helped to address

their most pressing needs. Their less positive opinions were not always transparent

but women did express their dissatisfaction with the duration of management support

and AMEP as well as their access to interpreter services. Other limitations noted by

key informants, some of whom were also Karen women, included ethnic tension

between Karen clients and Burmese service providers, lack of female and especially

female Karen service providers, quality and duration of English language tuition,

elder and service provider burnout, and, lastly, a lack of awareness and cohesion

between service providers. Implications and recommendations for generalist HSS

service providers are addressed in Chapter 14.

Many of the material and systemic sources of stress and suffering experienced by

Karen refugee women are hardly surprising and certainly not unique. They are the

shared fate of the disenfranchised and the systemic socio-political factors affecting

these communities have been well documented (Albrecht, Albrecht, & Murguia,

2005; Blakemore, 2000; Chow, Jaffee, & Snowden, 2003; K. S. Collins et al., 2002;

James, 2010; Pittaway & Bartolomei, 2001; Sheikh-Mohammed et al., 2006; J.

Taylor, 2004). The shared fate of poverty and sociopolitical inequality amongst many

communities has provided frameworks, models and examples of interventions that

might be applied to the Karen context. For example, we might be able to learn from

programs implemented in disadvantaged Indigenous Australian Aboriginal

communities to enhance social and emotional wellbeing (Purdie, Dudgeon, & Walker,

2010; Tsey et al., 2007).

263

However, whilst the systemic aspects of poverty in Australia are shared, the refugee

experience is not common to all communities of poverty. Refugees’ experience of

poverty in resettlement is positioned between pre-migration experiences and post-

migration sociopolitical prejudices and problems specific to humanitarian migrants

such as increasing public and media discrimination against asylum seekers (N. Grove,

Zwi, & Allotey, 2007; J. Taylor, 2004; Tribe, 2002). We might therefore also look to

ecological interventions with other refugee communities (Goodkind, Hang, & Yang,

2004). Yet, each refugee community is unique in its sociopolitical and cultural

context. In this thesis I have sought not only to report women’s stressors and concerns

but also to examine how their encounters with daily stressors might be understood

within the Karen-specific context – the broader spiritual-socio-political-cultural

(including gendered) melting pot. These issues need to be considered if service

providers and policy makers wish to work with Karen women to enhance their

wellbeing. The findings demonstrate that women’s problems intersected with gender,

culture, politics and persecution. Moreover, this thesis has demonstrated how fear of

freedom, fear of authority, absolute compliance, and sponsored dependency or

helplessness generate or exacerbate daily stressors faced in the Australian context.

264

Chapter 13

“It’s good … but it’s not very effective”:

Experiences and Perceptions of Health and Mental Health Service

Provisions

13.1 Introduction

This chapter explores Karen women’s experiences and perceptions of primary and

mental healthcare services in Australia.

13.2 “Extra special” care from GPs

Women perceived a number of organisations and individuals to have been particularly

sensitive to their situation. In addition to many HSS providers, a small handful of GPs

who did not work exclusively with refugee clients were particularly sensitive to the

needs of Karen refugee women and provided “extra special” care and attention. When

interviewed, these GPs – in contrast to others – reported that more time, extra work,

education, social support and advocacy (e.g., budgeting, parenting and assertiveness

advice) was required to adequately care for their refugee patients. For example, Dr G

reported:

Another problem is advocacy for these people … It’s about giving them information

and ringing up on their behalf and making appointments. Dealing with immigration

and the health bureaucracy on their behalf. (Key informant – Dr G)

It is possible that GPs with a special interest in Karen or refugee patients were more

likely to participate in the research as an equal number of doctors declined or did not

respond to repeated invitations to participate. In order to seek out negative cases

(Charmaz, 2006), GPs about whom participants or key informants had expressed

dissatisfaction were specifically targeted and recruited. These GPs explained that they

did not require any extra time for consultation with refugee patients and did not report

assisting refugee patients with advocacy or accessing social services.

265

13.3 Primary health and healthcare services

13.3.1 Encountering healthcare and illness

Karen women suffered from numerous (and often serious) illnesses and diseases and

valued their access to healthcare in Australia. A recent study of 156 refugees from

Burma living in Australia reported high rates of Helicobacter pylori infection (80%),

latent tuberculosis (70%), vitamin D deficiency (37%) and strongyloidiasis (26%)

(Chaves et al., 2009). Most Karen women were encountering complete primary

healthcare for the first time and, while grateful for receiving assistance, many were

saddened to discover that they had disorders and chronic diseases, something they

were not previously aware of. Some women were fearful of the implications for their

wellbeing and, as one GP explained, were often seriously alarmed because diseases

such as diabetes are “death sentences” in Burma and in the refugee camps.

Some Karen women experienced frustration accessing services and particularly with

service protocols and waiting times. Htee Gloh explained that some women did not

comprehend the custom of making appointments. GPs did not raise any concerns in

relation to Karen patients accessing and attending their services other than language

and communication. These findings suggest that Karen women might benefit from

more education about (and assistance accessing) Australian health services than is

currently provided through the HSS orientation.

13.3.2 Mismatched expectations

GPs varied greatly in terms of their level of interest, knowledge and involvement in

the political, social and medical issues of their Karen refugee patients. Most appeared

attuned to their Karen patients’ expectations regarding treatment. However, a number

of Karen women were dissatisfied with the care they received. Women expressed

stress and dissatisfaction with GPs when they felt that they were not properly assessed

and when no medicine, intervention or referral was provided. Women were especially

frustrated and disheartened when their doctors could not find “anything wrong”

despite their experience of various aches and pains. At the same time, Karen women

were also bewildered by what they perceived to be Australian health service

providers’ preoccupation with unnecessarily analysing emotional or psychological

suffering:

266

When I go to [the] antenatal clinic, the first time they interview you, you have to fill

out the form to say that, “I feel very sad, very often”. We laugh because for [us]

that’s normal. Sometimes you feel sad and then you can’t sleep, those things for [us]

it’s normal … For the Westerner, they think everything is serious, especially when

you’re pregnant, they’re really concerned about it. (Key informant – Ms S)

Karen key informants and women participants located their experience of general

healthcare in Australia against the backdrop of their encounters with health services in

rural villages and refugee camps and explained that most Karen were not familiar

with attending medical services. One participant in Group B explained: “in our

country we rarely go to see the doctor, only when we are near death”. Consequently,

when women attended Australian GPs, they expected some form of tangible treatment

or referral for further testing. Moreover, women’s pre-migration experiences of

treatment were aggressive by Australian standards (in large part because healthcare in

the camps was rudimentary and patients were only likely to receive medical care in

the later stages of illness). Many were familiar with using antibiotics and other

medications rarely prescribed in Australia. For example, during participant

observation an elderly Karen man lamented the “weak” pharmaceuticals available in

Australia and praised the benefits of isoniazid (a first-line anti-tuberculosis treatment)

for curing a variety of ailments including sore throats and asthma. Most doctors were

aware of their Karen patients’ expectations concerning treatment. For example, as Dr

G explained:

They all expect some kind of treatment … They love injections … [and in the

refugee camps] have been made to rely on [them] … it might just be a B-12 shot or

some kind of B-complex mixed up in twenty millilitres of water and each person is

getting one tenth or one twentieth but the colour is nice and yellow so they think

they are getting something good … That’s part of the education that they need in

Australia, that in Australia we don’t just give injections willy-nilly. (Key informant

– Dr G)

Some doctors had developed creative solutions to meet their patients’ “high”

expectations and prescribed multivitamins as a “placebo” treatment and means of

reassurance. GPs highlighted the need to give explicit information about treatment

regimens for chronic diseases as many of their Karen patients expected treatment to

be “one-off”.

267

GPs were aware that Karen patient dissatisfaction led to frequent “GP-hopping”,

stating: “[they think] if this doctor doesn’t give me a treatment I’ll go to another and

he might give me an injection”. They also noted that, in part as a consequence of

dissatisfaction, some women accessed traditional Burmese medicine. Resettled Karen

refugees in America have been noted to arrive with, import and use a variety of

traditional medicines (Power et al., 2010). Karen key informants in this study

confirmed Power et al.’s (2010) findings and explained that many arrive with

numerous traditional medicines as well as medications such as antibiotics obtained in

Thailand. GPs differed in their perceptions of the use and availability of traditional

medicines amongst Karen refugees. Some believed that traditional medicines are not

available in Sydney whereas others were aware of their use. Research in America

found high levels of lead in some traditional medicines used by refugees from Burma

(Mitchell et al., 2012) and some service providers were worried about the contents of

traditional medicines used in Australia. Several months after the interviews with

service providers in Sydney, the local government health authority released a health

warning to avoid Burmese traditional medicines due to concerns they might contain

high levels of arsenic (New South Wales Department of Health, 2012).

Women experiencing more severe emotional distress were especially dissatisfied with

the care offered by their GPs. It is possible that, in addition to cultural factors,

language and communication barriers limited discussions between Karen patients and

GPs such that only physical health issues have been discussed in detail. An additional

possibility is that GPs may not have sought to explore their patients’ EMs for their

distress due to lack of time in short consultations. A third possibility is that some

doctors have limited awareness of the importance of a cross-cultural appreciation of

EMs. Finally, GPs may have felt unable to adequately address the psychosocial needs

of these patients and thus avoided opening a can of worms. Recent research with GPs

treating ethnic minority patients in Australia found that doctors felt unable to address

the structural issues affecting refugee patients and offered medication instead (Furler

et al., 2010).

As discussed in Chapter 9, women conceptualised their physical and emotional or

mental health as interrelated and often attributed their distress to social and

psychological (e.g., thinking too much) causes. However, when it came to physical

268

manifestations of distress (e.g., aches and pains) they expected a physical biomedical

solution. Moreover, women often sought medical treatment for their physical

symptoms of distress but explicitly separated their physical and psychosocial

complaints to the degree that they did not inform their doctor of the broader

circumstances of their distress. This scenario is exemplified in the following quotation

from an interview with Lah Luh, a woman who experienced severe emotional

difficulties. During this section of our conversation I was concerned about Lah Luh’s

mental health needs and sought to understand the degree to which she had informed

her GP of her distress:

I am not well most of the time. If possible I want to find out what is wrong with my

whole body, I want to check it out. I always feel sick … I have no idea what’s

wrong with me. Is it my gastric problems? Is it liver problems? What part of my

body has something wrong? … I want to find out what the problem is …

Did you tell him [your GP] about the body problems?

Yes.

And did he give you help?

He said it’s because my diabetes affects other parts of my body.

Did you tell your doctor about your crying?

No.

Did you tell the doctor about sometimes thinking that it would be better if you

were not alive?

No point talking to the doctor because I am the one who suffers, not them! If they

don’t understand my suffering they might scold me. I am the one who suffers, not

him.

Why might he scold you?

Sometimes when I see my GP he says, “You have so many problems, I can’t find

your problem, I can’t help any more, I am not God, I don’t have a power like God

so I can’t help you”. (Lah Luh)

Part of Lah Luh’s anxiety about her doctor’s reaction may relate to fear of authority.

Her behaviour might also have reflected a despondency towards medical treatment as

a result of her pervasive and chronic undiagnosed health problems.

The issues and mismatched expectations noted here speak, in part, to the notions of

cultural congruence and cultural competency. The construct of cultural congruence

269

refers to the cultural competence of a health provider (or organisation) in relation to

the cultural needs of the client (Betancourt, Green, Carrillo, & Park, 2005;

Constantino, Malgady, & Primavera, 2009; Hardy & Laszloffy, 1995; Hwang, 2006;

Manderson & Allotey, 2003). Here Kleinman’s (1980) EM framework is relevant. It

seems that GPs and Karen women often held different EMs in relation to women’s

suffering. That is, their cultural conceptualisations of women’s experiences were

incongruent. In many cases, the cultural competence of GPs was insufficient to bridge

their differences.

13.3.3 “They should be fine”: communication barriers and GPs

Upon Karen women’s arrival in Australia HSS case managers assisted them with

arranging and attending GP appointments. These appointments were (without

exception amongst participants in this study) with Burmese-English speaking doctors

practising in Western Sydney. Many HSS case managers spoke multiple languages

and could converse with the GPs in either Burmese or English. However, a number of

their Karen clients spoke neither of these languages and were reliant on their case

manager for communication. Officially, case managers were prohibited from

interpreting at medical appointments though in reality all women reported that they

did because no other interpreter was made available.

Women’s communication with their GPs became difficult when assistance from HSS

case managers expired. Doctors and Karen participants overwhelmingly reported that

they did not use professional interpreters for GP consultations. Karen women who

could not speak Burmese or English enlisted friends or family members to assist them

with communication. Women were more likely to continue to attend Burmese-

speaking GPs because they could more readily find friends who spoke proficient

Burmese (as opposed to English). In this way they made a choice between two

imperfect alternatives. As Pàw Lèh explained: “if you go to an English Australian GP

you need an interpreter [who speaks English], it’s more difficult”. Echoing prior

research (Rosenberg, Richard, Lussier, & Schuldiner, 2011), participants in my study

also reported that women were less likely to discuss sensitive health concerns when

assisted by informal family interpreters.

270

Unfortunately, women were not always able to find a friend to translate. As a result

they attended appointments alone and communicated in whatever limited Burmese or

English they possessed:

For those who can’t speak Burmese or English, [Dr X] knows so he doesn’t say

much, he just does whatever is necessary and prescribes the medicine that the

patient needs [laughs]. I think most Karen people, although they don’t speak

Burmese, they can understand a little bit of Burmese, so they should be fine. (Ta

Heh Sōo)

In these circumstances things were not always “fine” and women were not always

satisfied with the level of communication. Moreover, they often did not understand

the content of the consultation. On occasion miscommunication resulted in

mismanagement. For example, Lah Luh experienced serious physical and mental

health problems – including thoughts about killing herself – and attempted to convey

her distress to her GP but did not have access to an interpreter and had not received

adequate care. This raises a crucial issue in terms of the implications of GP

communication for the wellbeing of Karen women, particularly those who may be

suffering from mental health issues, and could mean that women’s mental wellbeing

is compromised. Interestingly, despite feeling dissatisfied, women did not complain to

their doctors about communication issues. They frequently continued to attend the

same GP or changed to another Burmese-speaking doctor.

Most GPs reported having difficulty using the TIS service and, in this context,

considered the use of family and friends as interpreters to be an adequate solution to

communication problems. These findings parallel research with GPs treating refugee

patients in Ireland where doctors believed that they could sufficiently communicate

using sign-language, minimal English and informal assistance from others

(MacFarlane, Glynn, Mosinkie, & Murphy, 2008). Like Ta Heh Sōo, most GPs in this

study were optimistic that their methods of communication “should be fine” for their

patients. However, some acknowledged having had difficulty explaining treatment

regimes. Others queried the ethical and legal implications of the scenario. These

issues are depicted in the following two quotations from interviews with doctors:

Often times I’ve seen from my own practical experience when you ask someone a

question and they reply sometimes the interpreter, their friend, tells you what they

271

think you are asking and not what the patient is replying… That is why I feel the

phone interpreters are better … However, trying to get a phone interpreter is not

easy and having an interpreter visit unless you are a specialist is not allowed … I

think it has to do with funding. (Key informant – Dr G).

The first time they [Karen refugee patients] are brought to me by their case

managers. Most of the case managers can speak Karen and Burmese. For the second

visit they bring their friend who can speak Karen and Burmese.

Have you ever used the telephone service?

No, I’ve never used [it] … Time is very important to us. I can get any [information]

I need because when they come to see me most of their complaints are very simple.

For example, blood tests, simple complaints, fever, coughing, running nose – I can

easily understand that [without an interpreter]. Some of them, if they can’t talk

[Burmese] I think they can understand what I say in Burmese … So far I don’t have

any problem [using their friends as interpreters] but I don’t know exactly from the

legal point of view whether I am right or not … maybe I’m wrong or risky, I don’t

know. In our centre the specialists don’t use any interpreter [either]. (Key informant

– Dr L)

These findings add to a growing body of literature indicating that telephone

interpreting services are sub-optimal for healthcare providers in Australia (Atkin,

2008; Bird, 2008; Department of Immigration and Multicultural and Indigenous

Affairs, 2003; Huang & Phillips, 2009).

Numerous reports describe problems with interpreting services from the perspectives

of doctors, service providers and (occasionally) clients. However, the voices of

interpreters are largely and remarkably absent. Interpreters are not invisible conduits

of information (Angelelli, 2004). They both affect and are affected by the

communication encounters in which they work. In this study the perspectives of

Karen interpreters offered valuable additional information as to why interpreting

services appear to be so difficult to utilise. Many of the NAATI interpreters in the

Karen community were themselves refugees, often struggling to address their own

resettlement concerns. Interpreters were paid ten dollars per fifteen minute phone call.

The work was sporadic and consequently many interpreters took on other

employment. As one interpreter stated: “we cannot sit around all day in case we might

get a thirty-five minute job”.

272

Adding to GP communication problems, the impact of ethnic tension and mistrust

(see Section 12.5.2) was also apparent in relation to Karen women’s interactions with

their doctors and was identified as a cause of “GP-hopping” whereby Karen

community members utilised multiple GPs:

They don’t normally talk about that [ethnic-related mistrust of Burmese GPs] but I

can imagine that they have it in mind. So … they just find another [doctor] … They

don’t openly say “Oh, this is because [they are a] Burmese GP”, but they have it in

mind. (Key informant – Mr H)

All but one of the Burmese doctors minimised the issue of ethnic prejudice,

explaining that this was not an issue with their clients. Typical of their responses, Dr

N reported: “they don’t see me as Burmese, they see me as their doctor”. Taken

together, these findings point towards an urgent need to address the effectiveness of

primary healthcare services for Karen refugee women, including current interpreter

services for GPs as well as GPs’ awareness of ethnic (mis)trust issues.

13.4 Mental health services: cultural differences and paradigm clashes

Refugees in Australia could access a range of mental health services, most notably:

Mainstream mental health services such as psychologists, psychiatrists and

counsellors in private or public practice;

Specialist mental health providers for people from non-English-speaking

backgrounds (e.g., the Transcultural Mental Health Centre); and

Specialist DIAC-funded services for refugees and torture survivors. As noted

in Chapter 4 (see Section 4.4), FASSTT was a network of eight such specialist

rehabilitation services representing one in each state and territory (FASSTT,

2006). In New South Wales this service had been provided by STARTTS

since 1988. In addition to STARTTS services, refugees were able to access

education-related counselling through the AMEP. In New South Wales,

Mission Australia provided counsellors to the AMEP program. These

counsellors assisted with educational counselling, referrals and “personal

problems” affecting learning (ACL, n.d.).

273

During interviews all participants were asked about their experiences and perceptions

of Australian mental health service providers. Their feedback centred around nine key

areas of concern which highlighted cultural differences between Karen and Western

psychotherapeutic approaches to healing and emotional or mental wellbeing. In this

section I will use the nine key areas as a platform to present participants’ experiences

and perceptions of mental health services.

Overwhelmingly, participants referred to STARTTS when discussing their

perceptions of mental health services. This is not surprising, because, as discussed

(see Section 4.4), refugees were routinely referred to STARTTS as part of the needs

assessment undertaken by HSS case managers. Most Karen women also knew that

psychologists, psychiatrists and counsellors were also available in the healthcare

system and through other services such as adult education institutions. No participants

specifically mentioned the Transcultural Mental Health Centre.

Nine points of difference

1. The first factor: “what is counselling?”

Karen women (including key informants) were generally aware of the availability of

mental health services but noted that they did not necessarily understand how such

services purportedly helped people. Similarly, healthcare key informants who worked

with Karen refugees observed that their clients often did not understand what

counselling was about despite regularly attending sessions:

Do you think they [Karen clients attending counselling] know what is being

offered to them?

Not really. I don’t think so. Even those experienced [counsellors] – ten or fifteen

years working here – they have some clients who after ten or twelve sessions [still]

don’t know what counselling is. (Key informant – Mr H)

Karen women and Karen key informants frequently explained that counselling “is a

Western concept”. Indeed, with “no traditional term” for counselling it could not be

directly or even easily translated in their language.

274

2. The prebooked healing hour

At Australian service providers refugees were expected to self-disclose and address

their suffering (at least in part) within regular sixty minute pre-booked intervals. This

contrasted strongly with Karen forms of healing whereby supportive interactions took

place when and as needed, for unspecified durations and at locations determined by

the level of need and situation of the distressed person. For example (see Section

10.3.2), Karen leaders and pastors visited homes, sometimes in small groups, to

provide support and healing. Women contacted their peers, leaders and pastors when

they needed support and, according to leaders (and service providers), were

particularly unlikely to attend follow-up or prescheduled appointments. Karen key

informants explained that Australian psychotherapy sessions were viewed as “very

formal” and women were described as “reluctant to join that formal session”. One

Karen leader went on to explain: “even if you have counsellors no one will go to him

or her”.

3. Disclosure to a professional stranger

When seeking help, women valued the opinions and empathy provided by others who

shared their situational and sociocultural circumstances. In contrast, Western

counselling is often an interaction with a stranger who does not necessarily share

similar experiences, beliefs or cultural nuances. Findings from this study resonate

with anthropology and cross-cultural mental health literature (Calabrese, 2010;

Gudykunst & Bond, 1999; Sue & Zane, 2009), including refugee studies (B. Guerin,

Guerin, Diiriye, & Omar Yates, 2004), and suggest that Karen women do not

understand or perceive value in disclosing one’s problems to a stranger.

4. One-on-one individual healing versus communal group process

Though the distinction between individualist and collectivist societies is often

overemphasised, it is helpful for understanding differences in therapeutic systems

(Calabrese, 2010). Karen society is collective. Moreover, because of widespread

persecution, most of Karen women’s problems, losses and traumas were shared by

others in their community. Consequently, when women did engage in interpersonal

approaches to coping, they tended to do so in groups. Even formal support from

elders and pastors was obtained in people’s homes where families would often

275

converse and pray together. In contrast, Western psychotherapeutic interactions (with

the exclusion of family and group therapies) are typically characterised by one-on-one

encounters. Moreover, Western trauma-oriented psychotherapies for refugees

personify political violence and transform social problems into problems of the

individual (Kleinman, 1980). Notably, STARTTS did offer some community-oriented

group activities such as women’s swimming classes. Interestingly, Karen women

reflected positively on such activities but did not conceptualise them as counselling

per se. Rather, they accepted and enjoyed these activities as opportunities for social

interaction and the chance to learn new skills.

5. Verbalisation and self-disclosure

Researchers have long noted that the cultural attitudes of several communities

(Moodley & West, 2005) – including Hispanics (Cortese, 1979; Meadow, 1982),

Philipinos (Ponce, 1974), Asians (Kim, Sherman, & Taylor, 2008) and Blacks (Calia,

1966) – contradict the perceived value of talk therapies that encourage self-disclosure

and introspection about emotional suffering. Indeed, the belief in the need for

disclosure and the catharsis of confessing one’s most intimate thoughts and feelings

has been attributed to Western and Judaeo-Christian thought (Summerfield, 2000).

Foucault (1998) used psychoanalysis as an example of how previous Christian

practices of confession have spread to secular private life. Today, confession through

verbalisation remains a noted feature of Western society (C. Taylor, 2009). Within

this setting, modern Western therapies have tended to emphasise the need to confess,

control and analyse one’s thoughts, feelings and behaviours. This approach is distinct

from the healing methods and therapies common to most other cultures which are

oriented towards acceptance, harmony, understanding by awareness, contemplation

and mind-body-spirit unity (S. Fernando, 2010). As van de Put and Eisenbruch (2004)

have argued: “There is certainly no cultural analogue in any of the Asian healing

ceremonies to the type of self-disclosure encouraged in psychotherapy” (p. 152).

Karen leaders and women described established patterns of help-seeking in which the

help-seeker could say very little if desired and the help-provider could give explicit

and specific advice and instruction if that was deemed appropriate. The healer was

expected to have, at some time, endured comparable problems. Consequently, through

their experience and wisdom they could illustrate pathways (via self-disclosure) for

276

the help-seeker to overcome their difficulties. In contrast, Western psychotherapy also

tends to eschew therapist self-disclosure. Seen from a cultural perspective, it would be

unsurprising that some Karen women might perceive confessing intimate feelings as

nonsensical or counterproductive (Ekblad & Jaranson, 2004). The findings suggest

that Australian mental health services, particularly talking therapies such as

counselling and psychotherapy, were not well matched to Karen women’s approaches

to healing and beliefs regarding coping.

6. Secular versus spiritual

Karen women emphasised the centrality of religious beliefs and the religious

community to healing – approaches that contrasted sharply with the secular

orientation of mental health services. It is only relatively recently in Western history

that mental and emotional healing has become devoid of spiritual or religious aspects

and this division is rarely found elsewhere (Calabrese, 2010; Moodley & West, 2005).

The differences between Western secular and Karen spiritual approaches to coping

can be framed as cultural incongruences and mismatched EMs. Anthropologists,

psychologists and psychiatrists (Kleinman, 1980; Lawler-Row & Elliott, 2009;

Thoresen, 1999) have consistently drawn attention to the significance of spirituality

for wellbeing and Nagayama Hall (2001) has pointed to the importance of a shared

spirituality as an aspect of culturally competent psychotherapy with ethnic minorities.

7. Crazy people

When we discussed the presence of mental health services women were often eager to

report that they had not personally utilised these services beyond attending required

resettlement assessments or when seeking assistance with some practical problem

such as liaison with immigration services. This likely reflects the shame and stigma

surrounding the topic of mental illness amongst the Karen. For example, key

informants reported that, because of stigma, they had difficulty encouraging Karen

community members to seek help from mental health services:

They won’t go if I refer them to a psychiatrist. They at once assume that I am

considering them crazy. (Key informant – Ms L)

277

You don’t want to label them because labelling is not good at all. When you label a

Karen woman, that label sticks to her forever, for the rest of her life. (Key informant

– Ms K)

In contrast, there was no stigma attached to traditional models of help-seeking. Karen

community members could obtain help from leaders and pastors without risking

gossip or social exclusion. Of course, stigmatisation of mental illness and

psychotherapy is not unique to the Karen or even refugee communities and such

attitudes are common amongst the Australian public also (Griffiths et al., 2006).

However, the collectivist cultural framework of the Karen and the small size of the

community could potentially contribute to increased stigmatisation due to fears of

social judgements, gossip and rejection.

8. Don’t just sit there – do something

Karen women believed that explicit help-seeking encounters (such as counselling)

should result in some sort of practical solution and failed to perceive any benefit if

they did not receive some material or tangible help. As Htee Gloh explained:

Counselling shouldn’t be about just talking. For example, if I had a problem with

my children, the counsellor should give me some ideas for how to solve the problem

– some ideas for action. (Htee Gloh)

Women reflected positively on their experiences with service providers who had been

able to assist them with their practical concerns during their needs assessment period.

However, at the conclusion of the assessment (and after receiving the desired or

maximum practical help available), most declined offers of continued appointments

for further counselling. A rare few had proceeded beyond the required attendance and

engaged in voluntary counselling appointments. However, they had attended with a

lack of knowledge about the type of help available, or what the term counselling

meant, and had hoped to receive assistance with practical problems (such as liaison

with government agencies like Centrelink). If their practical needs were not met

women generally did not attend further appointments. Karen women’s expectations

for assistance are not unique. For example, Sue and Zane (2009) similarly noted that

clients from other Asian backgrounds generally expect some sort of assistance or

direct benefit from treatment from the outset. Similarly, a recent study of Chinese

278

Australians (Chan & Ritchie, 2011) reported that participants expected mental health

professionals to be directive and instructive rather than reflective or facilitative.

Karen leaders also highlighted the mismatch and confusion between Karen

community members’ understandings and expectations of what Australian mental

health services could and should offer and the approach taken by such services:

I don’t know exactly what they [mental health services] are doing [laughs]. My

friend told me “the [mental health workers] are always coming to ask me what

happened”. What happened! [laughs] My friend was angry … They [mental health

services] ask us “do you have any problem?”. If you say “oh yes, I need money”

they say “I can’t support you with money” [laughs]. You can’t support me so okay,

don’t ask me any more! (Key informant – Mr O)

The quotation from an interview with Mr O conveys participants’ frustration with

mental health service providers and their sense of annoyance that these organisations

were unable to address their most pressing needs such as poverty.

9. Ontology of suffering: now versus then

Women’s expectations of practical help related to another difference in approach:

focusing on the now versus the then. Women reported that they primarily sought help

from others to cope with and resolve their current problems. In contrast, they

perceived their spiritual faith and informal support networks as the key sources of

help for suffering related to past events (e.g., grief) that could not be resolved through

material means. For the Karen it was nonsensical to talk about past events that caused

suffering. This mismatch was vividly apparent during interviews with Karen key

informants who had experience interpreting for service providers and, in their

interpreting roles, had been privy to Karen clients’ perceptions of and experiences

with Australian mental health services. The following quotations clearly show that

patients often did not understand the purpose of recounting past traumas:

Privately [after the STARTTS appointment] the woman said she could not

understand why she was being asked to repeat her story and discuss her bad

experiences. The woman did not find the process helpful but agreed to return to

future appointments simply because the staff member asked her to. (Key informant

– Ms Z)

279

I talked to one Karen woman and she said, “They [STARTTS] invited me [to

appointments] two or three times and the first time I told them all my stories and I

cried already. Why do they ask me to come back again and again and talk about that

issue? I don’t want to go any more”. So after the third time she ignored the

invitation. (Key informant – Mr B)

Some people just openly say, “No, we want to forget these past experiences and go

ahead with life here [but] STARTTS are making us tell the story back”. (Key

informant – Mr H)

The first two quotations also relate to the role of compliance in Karen women’s

interactions with service providers. Women continued to attend appointments if asked

to, even when they were offended or upset by a service provider. These quotations

also strongly suggest that the Karen do not perceive a need for disclosing past losses

and traumas and may not find the therapeutic encounter beneficial. Observing a

similar scenario in relation to a Rwandan refugee, Summerfield (2010) pondered:

“The question also [raised] here is what constitutes informed consent when asylum

seekers have so little social clout” (p. 105). If Karen women do not see benefit in

psychotherapy and do not wish to attend but lack confidence to cease attending

appointments, then service providers must critically reflect on the ethical predicament

they find themselves in.

Some Karen and non-Karen key informants expressed concern that therapeutic

cultural mismatches could be harmful. For example, Dr G expressed his reluctance to

refer clients for counselling after one of his patients experienced negative

consequences from therapy:

You know, because when I did refer somebody and he went to STARTTS when

they were taking him back over that [his experiences] he started having flashbacks

and things like that. Those people, not knowing the language and culture, they

couldn’t cope with him. This man, he didn’t know what they were doing [or] why

they were making him regress back to that and deal with that. So in the end we had

to get him to stop going. (Key informant – Dr G)

Again these findings are not unique to the Karen. A present-time orientation has been

observed in other populations (Sue & Zane, 2009). Cambodian monks have reported

that Khmer refugees, although sometimes angry about the injustices caused by Pol Pot

280

and the Khmer rouge, tended to seek help for current problems rather than past

suffering (Nickerson & Hinton, 2011). The monks perceived that talking about past

suffering and injustice could actually be harmful to one’s mental balance and

wellbeing. Karen key informants similarly expressed concern that trauma-focused

mental health interventions could negatively affect individual and community

wellbeing through contributing to dependency. Somali refugees in New Zealand have

reported that Western treatments “focus too much on past experiences (trauma), rather

than problems in the current situation” (B. Guerin et al., 2004, para. 29). According to

Bemak, Chung and Bornemann (1981), the frustration is also mutual and mental

health professionals similarly find that refugees do not want to delve into the past:

Many professionals find themselves faced with refugee clients who are

initially far more interested in understanding and effectively adapting to the

world around them than in delving into intrapsychic and interpersonal

problems or psychosocial adjustment issues. (p. 250)

These differences constitute what Calabrese (2010) refers to as a “paradigm clash” (p.

311). Paradigm clashes or cultural mismatches in psychotherapy have been

consistently linked to limitations and inadequacies in mental health service provisions

for non-Western populations. They lead to under-utilisation of services, higher

prevalence rates, treatment dropouts, misdiagnosis and mismanagement. Doctors and

other service providers may become reluctant to refer Karen patients for mental

healthcare for fear of mismanagement and harm to patients. Karen patients may also

be offended, as depicted in the following quotation:

The psychologist [at Centrelink] asked so many questions: “Do you want to harm

yourself? Please tell me if you want to commit suicide”. He [the Karen refugee

client] was shocked! I was the interpreter and he said to me “Why do they ask me so

many silly questions? I am a normal person” … The man [Karen client] said “I feel

you insult me; I can cope with that kind of situation, no problem. I spent twenty

years in a refugee camp. Don’t worry about me”. (Key informant – Mr J)

13.5 Reflections

The cultural mismatch fuelling the negative encounters experienced by Karen

refugees attending Australian mental health service providers was aptly conveyed in

281

an early study by Frye and D’Avanzo (1994) who compared and contrasted Khmer

and Western psychotherapeutic approaches to healing. They observed that:

Western scientific medicine tends to approach the resolution of painful

psychological experience with a confrontative approach. Identification and

dissection of the psychological trauma is encouraged, and introspection is

expected to be followed by verbalization. Somatization is considered

maladaptive and is not rewarded. Confrontation of the psychological pain is

considered courageous by the patient and skillful by the therapist. (p. 94)

Frye and D’Avanzo’s (1994) conclusion that these particular aspects of Western

psychotherapy contrasted sharply with the healing approaches of Khmer refugees is

uncannily relevant to the situation of Karen refugees. The present research findings

have demonstrated that a confrontative, verbal approach to psychological suffering

and trauma is antithetical to Karen culture. Like the Khmer, the Karen perceived the

act of confrontation as rude. Introspection was part of the symptomatology of hso

k’moe tha (thinking too much). Somatisation was an expected part of suffering. The

outpouring of feelings was not encouraged nor rewarded. Social contact, participation

and support were thought to be integral to healing. These findings, and those of other

refugee mental health researchers, imply that prevailing Western mental health

discourses must be re-examined. Moreover, these combined findings imply that

professionals must give attention to the possibility that Western “therapeutic”

treatments may indeed be unhelpful, offensive and even harmful for refugee

populations.

Western healers tend to rightly favour evidence-based interventions but unfortunately

non-Western alternatives are overlooked or dismissed as nonsense often simply

because they have not been researched. As J. P. Wilson and Drozdek (2007) argue:

“[the] opportunity to do scientific research is a privilege of rich societies” (p. 379).

Patient harm and marginalisation of indigenous knowledge systems are further risks

associated with the use of Western mental health interventions with refugee

populations (Stubbs, 2010). The full ramifications of transmitting Western psychiatric

models and treatments are, as yet, unknown. Watters (2010) argues that indigenous

experiences and expressions of distress and mental illness could indeed be critically

changed:

282

How a people in a culture think about mental illnesses – how they categorize

and prioritize the symptoms, attempt to heal them, and set expectations for

their course and outcome – influences the diseases themselves. In teaching the

rest of the world to think like us, we have been, for better and worse,

homogenizing the way the world goes mad. (p. 2)

Demonstrating further exactly how Western models of mind are imposed onto a

population at a time of upheaval, Watters (2010) points out that the pharmaceutical

industry has funded trauma-oriented training for health professionals in post-disaster

settings. Moreover, PTSD research is often combined with aid relief and clinical care

and thus may become a kind of currency for refugees seeking help (Summerfield,

2010). Individuals in such circumstances are likely to engage in Western programs

and mental health interventions not because such programs align with their experience

or perspective of their emotional world but as a strategy to obtain other forms of

assistance and maintain relationships with those who retain the power, money and

access to resources. My research findings add to Watters’s concerns and suggest that

even resettled refugees engage with services that they perceive to be useless or

offensive in an attempt to access needed resources and because they lack the skills

and confidence to disengage from contact with these organisations.

283

Chapter 14

Implications for Practice and Future Research

14.1 Introduction

This chapter addresses the major implications and recommendations of the findings

for (a) generalist HSS service providers, (b) mental health service providers, and (c)

future research.

14.2 Implications and recommendations for generalist HSS service providers

Drawing upon the research findings presented in Chapters 8 and 12, this section

provides concise recommendations for Australian resettlement service providers as

well as policy makers and researchers interested in refugee resettlement in Australia.

Recommendations are grouped according to areas of service provision and key topics

of concern.

14.2.1 Case management and resettlement orientation

a) Evaluate the effectiveness of HSS services and the duration and consistency of

the case management services provided to new arrivals. In particular, the

needs and availability of services for recipients of the Women at Risk visa

subclass should be further investigated.

b) Assess the availability and efficacy of DIAC’s Australia – a new home DVD

(DIAC, 2009a) and Beginning a life in Australia booklet (DIAC, 2010b).

These information sources were both available free of charge and in Karen,

and provided information on many of the environmental mastery issues

identified in this thesis (e.g., banking systems, housing, parenting, Australian

law and healthcare). However, no participants in this study mentioned having

or using them.

c) Investigate humanitarian entrants’ needs in relation to housekeeping and home

safety in Australia. Women lacked environmental mastery in these areas and

were stressed by these issues (see Section 8.3.4) but there is a paucity of

research on this topic. The findings from this study support those of a recent

Australian study of case managers perspectives about home safety (Campbell

284

& Turpin, 2010) and suggest that intervention programs and further research

on this issue is warranted.

d) Investigate alternative means of providing orientation or settlement support

during the resettlement period in Australia which tap into existing cultural

approaches to education, support-seeking and information sharing (see

Chapter 10).

14.2.2 Non-Karen speaking service providers

a) Ensure that handouts and information provided to Karen are in Karen and are

heavily based on visual communication of ideas.

b) Independent empirical research should investigate the interpreting services

available to refugees. This research should explore the broad issues affecting

the interpreter scenario including intra- and inter-ethnic mistrust (e.g., within

the Karen community, and between Karen and Burmese) and service

providers’ lack of cross-cultural competency and familiarity with the Karen

people (e.g., assuming they speak Burmese or misunderstanding them and

their language to be Korean).

c) Assess the feasibility of innovative solutions to interpreter problems such as

the implementation of designated days when on-site interpreters are available

for specific ethnic groups to attend social services and health clinics11. Such a

solution is likely to reduce the distress caused by negative interactions,

improve efficiency and decrease staff frustration for organisations. Clinics

could be mobile outreach vans or medical centres in Western Sydney.

Appropriate interpreters and bilingual, bicultural health workers could be

booked in advance of regular ethnic clinic days to ensure that Karen (and

other) patients are able to adequately understand their interactions with

healthcare providers. An added benefit of designated ethnic clinic days is that

transport and childcare problems would likely be reduced for patients as

parents could carpool (or use public transport together) and share childcare

arrangements. Moreover, another positive side effect would likely be the

11 At the time of writing these research findings and recommendations were being utilised by a working

group comprised of Karen service providers, staff from New South Wales Refugee Health Service and

the researcher.

285

provision of another collective, group-oriented activity. A clear finding

throughout this research is that Karen women enjoyed, and benefited from,

group activities with other Karen women (see Section 10.3). Rather than

organising and attending healthcare appointments in isolation, Karen women

are likely to enjoy undertaking such activities as a collective.

14.2.3 Adult Migrant English Program

a) Develop ways to harness the motivations of refugee communities, understand

and sensitively negotiate cultural customs, and take into account the effects of

culture, gender and context on student learning.

b) Investigate how candidates for the Special Preparatory Program are identified

and seek to understand why and how some women slip through the cracks.

c) Education programs must do no harm and a basic level of cross-cultural

respect and understanding must be a prerequisite for all encounters between

teachers and learners. Women’s encounters with their teachers did not always

meet this basic level of care and instances of discrimination and poor service

provision were noted. As humanitarian arrivals encounter human rights,

possibly for the first time, their experience with resettlement service providers

must reinforce the dignity and respect for all persons that underpin these

rights.

d) Develop further opportunities for informal, practical and visual learning

(Maher, 2006).

e) Teachers should teach English in English and refrain from using other

languages not spoken by all students (except where teaching in the students’

native language).

f) Provide greater access to bilingual assistants in classes where teachers do not

speak their students’ native language and when students’ are not competent in

basic level English. Auerbach (1993) has argued that “bilingual options are not

only effective but necessary for adult ESL students with limited L1 literacy or

schooling” (p. 9).

g) Bilingual English language assistants should be supported in obtaining

educational qualifications to enable them to become teachers. Bilingual

interpreters are often also qualified teachers (or hold other tertiary

286

qualifications) in their country of origin and the recognition of their

qualifications, fully or partially, needs to be facilitated (McKay, 2009; United

Nations Educational Scientific and Cultural Organization, & International

Institute for Education Planning, 2009).

h) Independent program evaluation research should explore the level of English

proficiency acquired by humanitarian entrants following completion of current

service provisions and situate findings within an international context.

i) Best practice guidelines should be developed for teachers and organisations

providing AMEP services in Australia. Until 2009 DIAC funded the AMEP

Research Centre to provide research and professional development services to

AMEP providers. There is currently no research centre or best practice

guideline informing AMEP service providers. Some of the research findings

from this study can form a foundation for guiding best practice.

14.2.4 Poverty and housing

a) Refugee poverty and housing should be priority issues at all levels of

government. These problems were pertinent sources of stress for Karen

women and negatively affected their wellbeing. This is unsurprising as

poverty itself is known to adversely effect mental wellbeing (Marmot, 2005).

The mental health of refugee communities cannot be adequately addressed

without removing their realistic fears of homelessness.

b) Regular and independent monitoring of HSS providers is needed to ensure that

refugees receive appropriate and adequate assistance from contractors. Some

participants’ homes were in states of disrepair and the women experienced

considerable difficulty securing adequate (affordable and appropriate)

housing. Findings from this study and other recent reports (DIAC, 2011q;

Ernst & Young, 2011) demonstrate the need for urgent monitoring.

c) Food security issues amongst refugees in Australia should be further

researched and addressed (Gallegos et al., 2008).

d) Settlement service providers should develop further strategies for equipping

Karen refugees with budgeting and home-management skills. The research

findings indicated that women’s home-management and financial management

problems intersect with additional challenges including gambling and gender

287

role beliefs and practices. Future research and interventions should address

these related issues also.

14.2.5 Employment support services

a) Job Services Australia should monitor and review staff interactions (see

Section 8.3.2) and ensure that services are provided in a respectful manner.

b) Staff should be provided with adequate cultural competency training to alert

them to the cultural differences and needs of refugee clients.

14.2.6 Parenting

a) Investigate and address refugee parenting issues. Women’s parenting

difficulties resonated with those of other resettled refugee families in Australia

(Atwell et al., 2009; Lewig et al., 2010). The intervention recommendations of

Lewig et al. (2010) seem applicable here. These researchers question the

appropriateness of Western models of social work for refugee parents and call

for closer collaboration between social workers, refugee elders, refugee

communities, schools and police to address parenting issues.

14.3 Implications and recommendations for mental health service providers

This section draws on the research findings to provide recommendations for mental

health service providers. In formulating these recommendations, I frame them within

the principles of the ecological model for refugee mental health (Miller & Rasco,

2004) and the Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion (World Health Organization,

1986). The recommendations are summarised below and then expanded in subsequent

sections.

Recommendations for service providers

Prioritise (a) prevention over treatment and (b) issues that are of concern to the

community. Address the material and systemic sources of suffering.

Build individual and community capacity and self-efficacy.

Work with existing community strengths and integrate interventions into

existing support networks.

Address gender and work with women.

288

Engage communities in critical reflection.

Enhance staff cultural competency training.

Improve access to bilingual, bicultural and gender-matched staff.

Evaluate intervention and service provider efficacy.

Utilise practice-based evidence.

Design culturally congruent mental health interventions.

14.3.1 Prioritise (a) prevention over treatment and (b) issues that are of

concern to the community

Much of women’s distress was caused by a mismatch between the demands of

resettlement life and women’s resources to meet those challenges. Preventative and

relevant interventions for Karen women should focus on addressing their most

pressing concerns of (a) English language proficiency, (b) communicating with

service providers (e.g., interpreter-related problems), (c) employment, (d) parenting,

and (e) environmental mastery.

14.3.2 Build individual and community capacity and self-efficacy

The research brought to light the pervasive and complex effects of low self-efficacy

or agency and loss of identity, which might be targeted as part of preventative

interventions. These problems seem to have arisen from Karen refugees’ current

social circumstances, cultural context and prior experiences. A thorough and

theoretical understanding of these issues, such as that provided by the current study

(see Sections 8.6, 12.8 and 14.4.4) could be of interest and use to mental health policy

makers and service providers.

Interventions that extend the dependency fostered in refugee camps are harmful and

should be avoided. Rather, interventions should focus on (a) building community

capacity, (b) strengthening existing support networks, (c) establishing individual and

collective ownership, and (d) enabling Karen women to achieve improved efficacy in

their lives. Moreover, services built by and for refugee communities are more likely to

be utilised and perceived as effective than the provision of services by mental health

professionals (Miller & Rasco, 2004).

289

14.3.3 Work with existing community strengths and integrate interventions

into existing support networks

A central theme throughout this thesis has been the critical importance of Karen

collective and intracommunity support. Service providers need to harness this

community strength and interventions should (a) be integrated into existing networks,

and (b) replicate emic support networks. Policy makers and service providers should

seek to work with community leaders, harness their energy and determination, and

rethink mutually beneficial strategies for structuring service provisions to use their

skills in a sustainable way.

Existing settings such as organisations that provide language training, vocational

training and other services are likely to be ideal sites to provide interventions

(Jaranson & Bamford, 1987). Such an approach would likely be convenient for clients

and would also minimise shame and stigma associated with attending the intervention

(Tribe, 2002). Practitioners seeking to integrate interventions into existing community

activities must become familiar with local leaders and work with them to integrate

information or skills training into existing community events.

The research findings suggest that group-based, communal interventions are likely to

be most relevant and effective for Karen women. Health service providers could assist

the community in creating and facilitating support groups for members with shared

experiences or problems (e.g., single mothers experiencing parenting problems). To

avoid stigmatisation, groups could be based around activities such as gardening,

sewing, poetry, cooking or craft activities. For individuals whose experiences or level

of distress lie outside of the collective group’s understanding, facilitators could

provide help to seek assistance from specialists (pastors, community leaders, healers,

therapists or doctors). Karen women should also be empowered to form their own

collective support groups to provide emic healing strategies (e.g., prayer, sharing of

experiences and altruism).

14.3.4 Engage communities in critical reflection

Refugee communities should also be assisted in making sense of their unique

struggles in the resettlement environment. Moreover, they should be supported in

overcoming the harmful risks associated with particular cultural practices or

290

characteristics whilst also respecting and maintaining their culture. Their capacity to

interact and “adapt effectively” (Miller & Rasco, 2004 p. 35) to existing supports and

environments must also be enhanced. One intervention and empowerment model that

seems applicable here is Chan’s (2010) Assertiveness Communication Training

Program.

14.3.5 Address gender and work with women

The research findings demonstrated the gendered aspects of Karen women’s

suffering. Service providers should work with communities to address gender

ideologies that are harmful and limit women’s participation in life. Women expressed

a desire for “improved communication” within the Karen community as well as

greater flexibility and unity across gendered, political and religious divisions.

Gendered and intracommunity divisions are complex matters. However, as a female

Karen key informant suggested, women leaders are important for creating unity. She

described the present Karen community as characterised by a “top-down” approach

largely controlled by men and suggested a more “grass-roots approach” in which

women united to form forums and address issues. Service providers should help

Karen women to facilitate such forums and empower them with skills to address the

issues of concern to them. One approach might be to empower respected and senior

Karen women to mobilise community supports, liaise with service providers and

provide information and support.

14.3.6 Enhance staff cultural competency training

Staff should receive adequate training on the sociocultural and political backgrounds

of their refugee clients and be sensitised to cultural differences in expression of affect.

Mental health practitioners should seek to gain a thorough understanding of their

refugee clients’ EMs of mental illness to avoid mistaking culturally appropriate

expressions of distress for manifestations of psychopathology. They should also

become familiar and competent in working with interpreters (Tribe, 2002, 2007,

2009; Tribe & Lane, 2009). The conceptualisations, terms and idioms of distress

described in this thesis provide rich data for this particular group of Sgaw-speaking

and (primarily) Christian Karen refugees from the Thailand-Burma border. However,

the umbrella term “refugees from Burma” encompasses several ethnic subgroups with

291

vastly different religious and political perspectives. Muslim Rohinga refugees from

the Burma-Bangladesh border may hold very different conceptualisations of distress

as “ethnic composition influences ethnopsychological models significantly” (Kohrt &

Hruschka, 2010, p. 345). It is necessary to emphasise that idioms of distress must be

explored anew within different settings and populations of refugees from Burma.

14.3.7 Improve access to bilingual, bicultural and gender-matched healthcare

staff

Service providers should seek to provide refugee communities with greater access to

bilingual, bicultural and gender-matched staff. Research suggests that ethnic minority

patients prefer bilingual, bicultural therapists and respond better to treatment provided

by ethnically matched therapists (Griner & Smith, 2006; Walton, Berasi, Takeuchi, &

Uehara, 2010; Ziguras, Klimidis, Lewis, & Stuart, 2003). The recently released

Refugee Health Plan (2011–2016) aims to ensure accessible, culturally and

linguistically competent health services (New South Wales Department of Health,

2011). Many of the strategic priorities and actions in the plan address concerns and

recommendations arising from this study. In particular, the planners aim to recruit and

train bilingual community educators to provide health education and information and

recognise that “Bridging the language barrier is clearly fundamental to effective

communication” (p. 27). Findings from this study support the urgent need for these

changes and bring attention to the importance of addressing communication from a

gender perspective also.

14.3.8 Evaluate intervention and service provider efficacy

Monitoring and independent audits of generalist HSS services for refugees appear to

be warranted. There is also a distinct lack of available evaluative reports from

services in relation to refugee mental health and the efficacy of specialist services for

refugees should be urgently examined. Some existing programs (e.g., the Karen

Community Housing Cooperative and the Haven Australian community garden plot

described in Chapter 8, Sections 8.3.3 and 8.3.4) were viewed positively by the

community and also appeared to build community capacity and enhance self-efficacy.

However, typical of interventions with refugee communities (Miller & Rasco, 2004),

these interventions lacked formal evaluation.

292

14.3.9 Utilise practice-based evidence

Mental health practitioners are advised to challenge their professional bodies to

address institutional biases and draw upon a wider field of empirical knowledge to

guide interventions with refugee communities. This recommendation resonates with

Birman et al.’s (2008) call for policy makers, researchers and practitioners to better

utilise “practice-based evidence” (p. 122) to explore interventions that already exist

and occur in the real world. Their study of the International Family, Adult, and Child

Enhancement Services for refugee children in America provides a framework for

further efforts in this field.

14.3.10 Design culturally congruent mental health interventions

The current findings do not mean that adapted forms of talk-oriented approaches to

healing are always unwarranted for refugees (de Jong, 2002). Researchers van de Put

and Eisenbruch (2004) found that despite the lack of a cultural framework for talking

about one’s problems, Cambodian women reported finding relief from sharing their

history of war-related suffering in a group therapy settings.

In my study, some healthcare key informants reported that their Karen clients had

benefited from mental health services including individual interventions based on

Western psychological therapies (e.g., CBT techniques such as relaxation, activity

scheduling for pleasant events and cognitive restructuring of traumatic events).

However, these key informants also reported that, generally speaking, their Karen

clients did not wish to talk about their traumatic experiences, did not understand the

benefit in doing so, and more commonly found support for their wellbeing through

community interaction and addressing resettlement challenges (such as

unemployment).

The findings concerning women’s approaches to healing call for therapeutic pluralism

(Tribe, 1999b) rather than a “one size fits all” approach to enhancing the wellbeing of

Karen refugee women and echo Jacobs’ (2007) caution to:

Beware of those who advocate that something is better than nothing and who

wish to forge ahead with interventions that have not been well planned and

prepared with the local community. (p. 934)

293

Not only do therapeutic modalities need to be reconsidered, but the inappropriateness

of the Western model of the professional sixty minute consultation with a stranger

also needs to addressed. Karen women are more likely to benefit from healing

approaches that are appropriate to their needs, and cultural beliefs and traditions.

While understanding and providing (or enhancing) culturally relevant supports is

complicated (Bemak et al., 1981), it is likely to result in services that are better

utilised and more helpful.

The research findings raise the paradox of how Karen women valued support yet also

valued concealment of emotional distress and stoic silence (see Chapter 10). This

presents challenges for service providers but other findings related to coping suggest

several suitable approaches (e.g., group-based interventions) and flag some potential

pitfalls for health professionals. These findings can be used to create or adapt

interventions to align with women’s perspectives.

Women’s main psychological coping strategies (e.g., self-control, distraction and

acceptance) (see Section 10.2) are strikingly similar to the approach taken in

mindfulness-based CBT (Cayoun, 2011), which suggests that culturally adapted CBT

and particularly mindfulness-based approaches (Cayoun, 2011; Lau & McMain,

2005) might be acceptable and relevant to this population.

Mindfulness-based interventions are informed by a philosophy that espouses the

acceptance of suffering and an interrelated conceptualisation of body and mind. They

teach cognitive behavioural techniques to decrease isolation and harmful behaviour

patterns, increase pleasant and interpersonal activities, and observe thought patterns.

This approach normalises and validates emotional suffering at the same time as

employing strategies to improve one’s situation. Karen women’s emphasis on

understanding and accepting the ubiquity of human suffering is a central tenant in

mindfulness-oriented interventions. It seems that the cognitive reappraisal strategies

espoused in mindfulness therapies (e.g., acceptance) are also possibly better suited to

the Karen cultural and political context than cognitive reappraisal techniques

emphasised in other psychotherapies (e.g., reframing negatives to positives and

decreasing the personal importance of events). These latter strategies have been

associated with negative psychological functioning among oppressed populations

(Perez & Soto, 2011). D.E. Hinton, Pich, Hofmann and Otto (2011) have recently

294

published treatment guidelines and case studies advocating mindfulness and

acceptance-oriented CBT interventions with refugee populations. Further research in

this field, and with Karen refugee populations, should be undertaken.

14.4 Implications and recommendations for future research

The implications and recommendations of the findings for future research are

summarised below and then expanded in subsequent sections.

Recommendations for future research

Use explanatory models to further explore mental health topics with refugee

women.

Work sensitively with refugee communities and strive to access emic

perspectives.

Explore notions of power(lessness), use(lessness), dependency and identity.

Investigate the strengths, limitations and pathways of emic coping strategies,

particularly acceptance, stoic silence and shared experiences as well as both

material and spiritual approaches to coping.

Explore pathways and patterns of refugee adjustment to resettlement overtime.

14.4.1 Use explanatory models to further explore mental health topics with

refugee women

In the refugee mental health literature that I reviewed, this study was unique in its

combined use of vignettes with an EM interview framework. The findings suggest

that the use of case vignettes is well suited to understanding refugees’ EMs of mental

illness. The vignettes provided a way of portraying Western psychiatric categories

without using technical language and participants appeared comfortable discussing

the topics in a group environment. Vignettes may be a promising research technique

for accessing refugee women’s experiences, expectations and beliefs about health and

other topics. Future studies could explore a greater range of psychiatric constructs or

diagnoses including psychoses and substance abuse and longitudinally explore mental

health literacy (Goldney, Fisher, Dal Grande, & Taylor, 2005).

295

14.4.2 Work sensitively with refugee communities and strive to access emic

perspectives

In this study, Karen women’s experiences and perceptions, particularly those of

service providers in Australia, were not always transparent and easily accessible. This

experience demonstrates that researchers, evaluators and planners must be sensitive to

the cultural and socio-political context that may limit the information obtained

through survey and interview methods. It is important for such future research to use

sensitive research methods to access the “backstage” (Miller, 2004, p. 217) opinions

of refugee communities rather than superficial positive endorsements reminiscent of

their reluctance to criticise or complain. This study has demonstrated several useful

approaches to address these concerns. These include the triangulation of multiple

perspectives as well as ethnographic data collection methods.

14.4.3 Investigate the roles and challenges of refugee community leaders

The research findings concerning burnout of elders and community leaders (see

Section 12.9) warn of the negative flipside to a strong community network and

warrant further investigation. The findings from the present study suggest that current

settlement service provisions do not make the best and most sustainable use of these

existing community support people. Further research is necessary to ensure that this

important resource is supported and maintained in Karen and other refugee

communities.

14.4.4 Explore notions of power(lessness), use(lessness), dependency and

identity

The interconnected issues of power(lessness), use(lessness), dependency and identity

were critical factors affecting women’s wellbeing but these concerns are rarely

discussed in refugee mental health literature and future research seems warranted. The

research findings on these issues share features with several Western theoretical

models, namely Bandura’s (1977) theoretical framework of self-efficacy (see Section

8.5), Seligman’s (1975) model of learned helplessness and conceptualisations of

demoralisation (Frank & Frank, 1991, 1996; Kissane, 2001; Kissane et al., 2001) (see

Section 9.5.3). These frameworks might be useful for future research.

296

Why might self-efficacy, helplessness, defeat, entrapment and demoralisation be

useful concepts from which to understand refugee mental health issues? As we have

seen, a lack of personal and collective autonomy, control and self-determination

characterised the entire refugee journey for Karen women. One could argue that

everyday life for most refugees is characterised by defeat or lack of accomplishment

of one’s desired outcomes and (for those in camps) entrapment. Indeed, Karen

refugees on the Thailand-Burma border have been described as “birds inside a cage”

(Fuertes, 2010, p. 20). Refugees by definition do not wish to leave their homes – they

are forced. Those who seek refuge do not wish to live in camps but have few choices.

As we saw in Chapter 2, over five million refugees have lived in a state of limbo for

five or more years. In several conflict situations, including Burma, camps have

housed populations since the 1980s. For those who spend decades in camps it seems

likely that most will neither believe nor experience that they can do much to influence

the major outcomes in their lives. Moreover, their problems are colossal and,

according to Bandura (2001), the “magnitude of human problems also undermines

perceived efficacy to find effective solutions for them” (p. 18). With regard to

refugees, Abdi (2005) has argued that self-worth is destroyed by the dependency

fostered in camps. It seems that more so in camps, than during initial flight, human

defensive manoeuvres are rather futile. There is nothing to master except adjusting to

daily camp life, nothing much to escape from (relative to fleeing inside Burma) and

nothing to attack.

14.4.5 Investigate the strengths, limitations and pathways of emic coping

strategies

Acceptance, stoic silence, shared experiences and the division of support systems into

material and emotional or spiritual ones respectively dominated Karen women’s emic

approaches to coping. The current findings highlight the urgent need for studies

exploring these emic pathways to healing.

Interestingly, there are some important parallels between the coping processes

described in this study and conceptual models of social support described in Western

literature. House (1981) described the following four aspects or types of supportive

social interactions:

297

1. Emotional: verbal and nonverbal communication of love, empathy, trust, care

and sense of belonging.

2. Informational: provision of advice or guidance to problem-solve a difficulty.

3. Instrumental: provision of tangible, material goods or physical assistance

completing a task (e.g., translating a document).

4. Appraisal: communication for self-evaluative (rather than problem-solving)

purposes (e.g., constructive feedback, affirmation and social comparison).

House’s (1981) operational framework of social support has since been applied to a

range of populations (Helgeson & Cohen, 1996; Langford, Bowsher, Maloney, &

Lillis, 1997) and his model may provide a useful lens through which to understand

and further explore social support in refugee communities.

The notion of appraisal support (also referred to as affirmational support) (Kahn &

Antonucci, 1980) seems analogous to the key notion of the shared experience which

was central to Karen women’s help-seeking encounters. When women sought help

from their peers and leaders, the tacit knowledge or open discussion of their shared

experiences appeared to provide them with reassurance and a source of social

comparison and opportunity for self-evaluation. Women appeared to seek and obtain

emotional and appraisal types of support solely within the Karen community.

Informational and instrumental support was also predominantly sought within the

community, especially in relation to problems that were more culture-specific in

nature (e.g., adapting to parenting in the Australian context). However, in relation to

information and instrumental support, Karen women did also access and utilise

service providers.

Across Karen women’s approaches to coping, the importance of the shared

experience was apparent. There is sparse mention of the concept of shared experience

(in relation to help seeking and coping) in refugee mental health literature and

seemingly none exploring the mechanisms by which a shared experience is helpful,

nor the potential implications for service provision. The processes outlined by Thoits

(1986) (see Section 10.6.2) could serve as a lens through which to understand and

further research coping strategies among refugees. Women’s division of emotional

and practical support also shares similarities with the Western emotion- and problem-

focused coping framework (Agaibi & Wilson, 2005; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) (see

298

Section 10.6.1) and this model could be useful for furthering refugee mental health

coping research.

The research findings highlight the need for a cautious approach to investigating the

strengths and weaknesses of emic coping strategies. On the one hand Karen women’s

coping strategies such as acceptance and stoic silence might, from a Western

perspective, be considered maladaptive, passive, victim-like or even pathological

(Basoglu et al., 1997). Indeed, some Karen key informants interpreted these strategies

as potentially harmful and noted the potential for these practices to become warped

into unconditional acceptance, apathy, idleness and helplessness in the context of

persecution and protracted camp confinement. As noted, this perspective shares

similarities with Seligman’s (1975) theory of learned helplessness, Bandura’s (1977)

theoretical framework of self-efficacy and conceptualisations of demoralisation

(Frank & Frank, 1991, 1996; Kissane, 2001; Kissane et al., 2001). Thus it seems that

cultural practices and traditional coping strategies may, in certain circumstances,

actually predispose people to experience low self-efficacy and learned helplessness.

In contrast, as we saw in Chapter 10 (see Sections 10.2.4 and 10.3.1), acceptance and

normalisation of suffering was also associated with the ability to tolerate and survive

hardship and was seen to have its origins in venerated cultural and religious beliefs

and values. It is perhaps harmful to label these approaches disordered. Literature also

supports Karen women’s claims that acceptance and normalisation was, for them,

helpful and effective in reducing distress (Sachs et al., 2008). These discussions are

largely absent in mainstream mental health literature and these findings suggest that

researchers should work to better understand other emic coping strategies as well as

the role of these variables within different cultural contexts. At present, there is

insufficient research on suppression, acceptance and other emic approaches to healing

in refugee populations to make statements on their functions and effects. Future

research is warranted as the findings from this study suggest that these areas of

inquiry may help refugee mental health service providers to better understand the

experiences and needs of refugee communities.

299

14.4.6 Explore pathways and patterns of refugee adjustment to resettlement

overtime

Finally, this study has highlighted the centrality of post-migration factors to Karen

women’s wellbeing. Most psychological research with refugees has focused on

trauma, but given the apparent importance of post-migration resettlement factors

future research should also explore how refugees cope with the demands of

resettlement including their pathways and patterns of adjustment overtime. The

current literature lacks dedicated research in this field. Several models have been

proposed and each has received modest attention. Berry’s (1992) model of

acculturation provides one potential framework. Gonsalves (1992) proposed the

application of C. L. Grove and Torbiorn’s (1985) model for cultural sojourners. S. D.

Ngyuen’s (1982) three-stage model of refugee adjustment might also be useful.

Longitudinal studies could helpfully explore the process of resettlement and

contribute to the development of models appropriate to refugee communities.

300

Chapter 15

Conclusions

15.1 Introduction

In concluding this thesis, it is pertinent to firstly revisit the study’s overall aims which

were to (a) explore Karen refugee women’s experiences and perceptions of suffering

and distress, (b) analyse the women’s major psychosocial needs within the Australian

context, and (c) frame recommendations for action by community organisations and

services.

This original study contributes to knowledge in a number of ways. Firstly, it

contributes to an empirical understanding of Karen refugee women’s experiences and

conceptualisations of mental wellbeing. This contribution is significant and necessary

as, to the best of my knowledge, there are no previously published investigations of

Karen refugee women’s psychosocial wellbeing. By not imposing a pre-determined

psychiatric framework on Karen women’s experiences of distress and wellbeing, the

findings add to (and enhance) academic knowledge of refugee mental health issues

and prompt a reconsideration of existing theories and models. The findings contribute

to the field of psychological and medical anthropology, particularly in adding to the

body of knowledge concerning cultural idioms of distress and EMs. From a practical

perspective, this study provides a foundation for public and mental health

professionals to develop more culturally appropriate ways of addressing the mental

health of Karen refugee women.

15.2 Women’s conceptualisations of distress and the transferability of Western

theories

The prevailing discourse of refugee mental health research and practice has

emphasised the importance of traumatic war experiences. However, the findings

presented here have brought to light a clear disparity between prevailing trauma-

focused approaches and Karen women’s conceptualisations and EMs of their distress.

The findings strongly indicated that, for Karen women adjusting to life in Australia,

single adverse life events were of less relevance to their overall wellbeing than the

chronic daily stressors they encountered after resettlement. Women conceptualised

301

their psychological suffering as, in large part, caused by their social circumstances

including poverty, persecution, social disharmony, unemployment, exclusion and

discrimination and gender role pressures. They highlighted the suffering caused by

their inability to communicate effectively and thus navigate life in Australia, as well

as their ongoing grief and uncertainty about loved ones who remain in their homeland

or who are scattered across refugee camps. Their idioms of distress were embedded in

the physical, spiritual, gendered and socio-political circumstances of their lives.

Moreover, the mental health impact of feeling useless dominated their descriptions of

both individual and collective suffering.

No conceptually equivalent term for PTSD or even trauma existed in Sgaw Karen.

Indeed, a PTSD-like construct (as distinct from other terms) was not present in the

women’s discussions or conceptualisations of suffering. PTSD may not be an

appropriate illness construct for Karen refugees and is somewhat incongruent with

their conceptualisation of mind, body, environment and self in relation to others.

Karen women attributed different meanings, values and causal attributions to

symptom constellations similar to those of PTSD according to a broader framework

of personal, social and spiritual suffering.

However, these findings should not be taken to suggest that symptoms of traumatic

stress are not present among Karen women. Women did grieve, feel angry and have

nightmares as a result of past traumatic experiences. Indeed, women’s narratives

vividly illustrated that they experienced many of the symptoms indicative of PTSD.

For example, they described re-experiencing, avoidance and hyper-arousal symptoms

which they associated with traumatic as well as other social stressors. The

preponderance of trauma-focused refugee mental health research would appear to

indicate that such “symptoms” of traumatic stress are the most pressing concerns for

refugee communities. However, Karen women’s discussions of psychological distress

strongly suggest that psychological trauma was not among their most urgent concerns

in Australia. Further, not all people who experienced symptoms of traumatic stress

found their symptoms distressing. Moreover, the presence of such symptoms does not

necessarily appear to imply impaired functioning. The research firmly indicates that

the mere identification of PTSD symptoms amongst the Karen does not necessarily

lead to an accurate understanding of their experiences or needs, nor to an indication of

302

appropriate interventions. The observation of PTSD symptoms in this population

could lead mental health professionals towards a category fallacy (Kleinman, 1980),

that is, the false belief that a category has coherence and validity in a new setting

simply because its constituents can be observed. The findings of this study therefore

may help mental health professionals to provide more culturally competent care for

Karen women in Australia.

Women contextualised suffering within their cultural and sociopolitical milieu and

generally understood their distress to be a normal response to their unenviable

experiences (see Chapters 9 and 10). Their conceptualisation of the normality of

psychological suffering after forced migration raises some rather radical questions.

What is a normal response to human suffering on such a grand scale? Is normality the

absence of PTSD symptoms such as nightmares, recollections of the past,

hyperarousal and fear of one’s surroundings? Would it be normal to be “symptom”

free following the experience of fleeing a burning village, witnessing torture and

learning about disappearances? Would it be normal to remain free of anxiety and

depression whilst members of your family remain in refugee camps where they are

hungry, vulnerable and facing an uncertain future? When psychiatric disorders such

as depression and PTSD are applied uncritically to refugees these important questions

are avoided. Key informants identified sponsored dependency, fear of authority,

disempowerment, mistrust, absolute compliance and apathy, tolerance or acceptance

of hardship as the collective “trauma” suffered by the Karen community, and

experienced again by individuals in relation to resettlement stressors encountered in

Australia. These findings fit with Kleinman’s (1995) argument that “collective

trauma” (p. 177) might better encapsulate the experiences of refugee communities

rather than medicalised and individualised psychiatric frameworks such as PTSD.

The findings concerning Karen women’s discourse of stress, loss and trauma (see

Section 9.3.2) also provide further evidence that the impact(s) and experience of

traumatic events are mediated by the meaning assigned to them (Hagengimana &

Hinton, 2009; Park, Mills, & Edmondson, 2012; Sachs et al., 2008; Terheggan,

Stroebe, & Kleber, 2001), and support the notion that cultures differ in the events they

perceive to be traumatic (D. E. Hinton & Lewis-Fernandez, 2010a). For Karen

women, traumatic events with serious and long-lasting social ramifications were

303

amongst the most pertinent of trauma-related sufferings. Non-trauma-related

destruction of social networks and social capital appeared to be viewed as similarly

severe. Karen cultural and religious conceptualisations of a world where suffering is

normal may have mediated the effects of their traumatic experiences. Moreover, in

the Karen context, the long-running civil war and decades of persecution may have

provided them with realistic (to their context) beliefs about unfairness and injustice

such that individual traumatic encounters did not substantially violate their

worldviews. Such cultural, religious and socio-political psychological preparation

may have served to buffer or protect them from the impact of traumatic experiences.

Taken together, these findings strongly suggest that Western mental health

practitioners should reflexively explore prevailing discourses concerning traumatic

suffering and healing.

Finally, as we saw in Chapters 9 and 11, women’s idioms for, and conceptualisations

of, distress describe a trajectory or continuum of human emotional and mental

suffering. In contrast, Western psychiatric constructs are based on dichotomies and

taxonomies that distinguish between categories of disorder or disease. Despite this

major difference there were also several important similarities between Karen

women’s conceptualisation of distress and those found in both Western popular

culture and academic literature. The findings suggest that the concepts of neurosis and

common mental disorders (see Section 11.7.1) align with Karen women’s

conceptualisation of emotional and mental distress along a related continuum rather

than as discrete states of suffering. It appears that a continuum of distress may be a

more accurate model of the range of variation in emotional and mental states found in

everyday life, not only for the Karen, but for other cultures (including Western

societies) also.

15.3 Risk, resiliency and wellbeing in social, cultural and political context

In terms of addressing the question of what issues affect Karen women’s wellbeing,

this study has raised some important issues. The findings strengthen the growing

literature (Carswell et al., 2011; Laban, Gernaat, Komproe, van der Tweel, & de Jong,

2005; Miller & Rasmussen, 2010; Porter & Haslam, 2005; Steel, Silove, Bird,

McGorry, & Mohan, 1999) suggesting that post-migration difficulties are key

determinants of mental health difficulties for resettled refugee populations and may

304

mediate the effects of pre-migration trauma. Much of this literature has been

quantitative whereas the current study adds emic and qualitative evidence to the body

of knowledge. Moreover, this study adds a new dimension by demonstrating how pre-

migration culture, refugee-specific migration experiences, post-migration difficulties

and wellbeing were linked through the interconnected issues of power(lessness),

use(fulness) and identity – all of which must be understood within a cultural and

historical context.

As we have seen (see Sections 3.3.2 and 8.5), compliance with rules, containment (or

suppression) of distress, respect for authority and avoidance of confrontation appear

to be central to the Karen “national character”. They also appear to have socio-

political origins and probably served adaptive functions for the Karen throughout their

experiences of persecution. Such behaviour may have aided survival and prevented

violence and conflict, as non-compliant and confrontational individuals (and villages)

would quickly meet the heavy hand of the military junta. These traits also probably

served to maintain social cohesion and encourage tolerance of hardship during crises.

Quiet endurance of suffering contributes to risk as well as resilience when, through

repeated experiences of powerlessness, it extends to demoralisation, fear of authority,

apathy, submission, dependency and fatalistic tolerance of hardship (see Section 8.5).

Powerlessness was a central component of the refugee journey. Refugees have been

powerless in effecting their desire to remain in their homes and country of origin.

They have often been powerless in protecting not only their material possessions but

also their loved ones from persecution or death. Many have been powerless to alter

the undesirable experience of living in a confined camp where, as leaders suggested,

capacity and identity were stripped away. As the leaders noted, camps deny self-

sufficiency and create dependency. In a word, they are dehumanising. Camp

dependency disables opportunities for building self-efficacy and perpetuates a cycle

of reliance on others. It effectively paralyses initiative and confidence to succeed in

one’s own endeavours. In short, it teaches helplessness.

Beliefs, behaviours and mannerisms associated with Karen culture and the warped

manifestations of these practices in response to protracted persecution exacerbated

resettlement stressors in Australia, particularly when service providers took Karen

people’s compliant and shy behaviour at face value and overlooked their needs and

305

difficulties. People who are both fearful of and dependent on authorities are unlikely

to thrive in Australia’s competitive and individualistic culture. Although certain

problems, such as low rates of health service use, may be common to many resettled

refugee communities, cultural characteristics of the Karen place them at further risk of

neglect by service providers. Figure 6 schematises a possible model for use in

understanding this Karen context. It demonstrates, using five concentric circles, the

layered process by which individual features, cultural characteristics, pre-migration

experiences, prolonged refugee camp confinement and post-migration resettlement

factors interconnect and manifest as issues of powerlessness, identity and uselessness.

306

Individual

CulturalTraits /Values

Defined genderroles

Pre-MigrationExperiences

ProtractedCamp Life

Resettlement

Shy / quiet

Fear of authority

Fear of people & environment

Failure / useless

Respect authority

PhysicalPhysical / Social / Cultural/ Social / CulturalContextContext

Negative Mental /Negative Mental /Emotional ContextEmotional Context

Useless & wasted

C o m p l i a n c y

Mistrust

Fear offreedom

Powerlessness

C o m p l a c e n c y

Demoralisation

Confusion

Sponsoreddependency

DeathDestruction

DisappearancesPersecution

Poverty

Lack of: safety, healthcare &opportunities (education /

employment)

Limbo

Poverty

Relative SafetyGreater Safety

Poverty

Culturechange:

gender andparenting

Discrimination

Religious

Language & lackof environmental

mastery

Identity / roleloss

Helplessness

Accept suffering

Strengths &weaknesses

Apathy

Figure 6: Karen social, cultural and political context and associated negative psychosocial sequela

307

The model indicates a broadening of the theoretical base guiding refugee mental

health research. Using the sociological frameworks of Bourdieu (see Section 2.4.4) it

is possible to deconstruct the prevailing psychiatric narrative(s) concerning refugee

mental health and present and analyse responses from refugees themselves in regard

to their individual and collective experience and perspective. It is possible to marry

together an understanding of large social issues central to the refugee experience (e.g.,

power) with the daily experiences, behaviour patterns and emotional experiences of

Karen women.

Women who grew up in Karen villages or in refugee camps learnt and internalised a

set of practical cultural competencies, beliefs and embodied dispositions. They

acquired a social identity within that space and mastered living in that environment.

As Bourdieu (1990) states:

The practical mastery of the logic or the imminent necessity of a game – a

mastery acquired by experience of the game, and one which works outside

conscious control and discourse (in the way that, for instance, techniques of

the body do) [sic]. (p. 61)

It seems that getting “a feel for the game” of the Australian lifestyle was problematic

for many Karen women. They lacked knowledge of systems, procedures, laws, rights

and responsibilities, cultural norms and patterns of interpersonal communication, as

well as tools and physical objects. They also lacked experience, and confidence, to

interact effectively with service providers. Much of their knowledge of the world,

even their practical skills (such as how to wash clothing), was irrelevant in their new

environment. The research to date has been insufficient in recognising and accounting

for these social and physical processes and their impact on wellbeing.

The concept of habitus serves to “connect the biologic being with the social world via

physical and psychic embodiment” (Carrington & Luke, 1997, p. 101). This is

especially salient in the field of refugee mental health, as most non-Western cultures

experience the emotional world in a more somatic manner (Kleinman & Kleinman,

1985). The notion of habitus also provides a framework for understanding physical,

practical challenges of resettlement such as adjustment to an urban spatial orientation

as well as to appliances, furniture and technical equipment and services. For example,

308

Karen women’s traditional bodily habitus is significantly different from the physical

postures and movements assumed in an Australian classroom.

The habitus is developed in a broader structured social space or “field” characterised

by certain rules, class systems, and systems of power and domination affecting access

to resources. Bourdieu exemplified the concept of habitus through pointing out how

the politics of gender amongst the Kabyle peoples of Algeria are embodied in ways of

talking, walking and speaking. Similarly, Karen women’s embodiment of gender and

their traditional pattern of social relationships were remarkably different from the

gendered social relationships, expectations and experiences they encountered in

Australia. Their culturally accepted gender rules and logic did not necessarily transfer

easily to their post-resettlement environment. This was partly conveyed through Mu

Htòh’s description of Karen women as having previously been “living in the kitchen”

rather than attending school or engaging in paid work. This accepted field of gendered

behaviour was one of the first to be disrupted in Australia, where all adults, including

women, were expected to attend English language classes and seek employment. Here

too research to date has failed to adequately address the embodiment of gender

amongst refugee women and the implications for their wellbeing and resettlement.

15.4 Coping: the commonsense, complex and contradictory

Karen women had their own strategies for coping with stress and mental health

problems (see Chapter 10). They sought practical and informational support to

manage situations when they believed that they (or someone else) had the capacity to

influence or resolve the stressor. When women were faced with situations they could

not (or believed they could not) influence (e.g., the death of a child) they sought

emotional support from others with shared experiences and used coping strategies

such as prayer, distraction and self-control. Across interpersonal approaches to

coping, a mutual shared experience appeared to be a critical ingredient to women’s

perception and experience of a help-seeking encounter as beneficial. This feature of

help-seeking is likely to have origins in cultural traditions that value and respect the

experience and wisdom of elders. These findings have implications for service

providers (see Chapter 14).

309

Karen women’s EMs espoused a strong connection between mind, body and

behaviour. They described the ability of the mind-self to observe and overcome

unhelpful cognitive patterns as central to wellbeing. Acceptance, stoic silence, self-

control, minimisation and normalisation of distress were emic practices, expressions

and manifestations of distress and coping that featured heavily in the research

findings. Aspects of these practices do, of course, share features with popular

approaches to managing and expressing distress in various Western populations

including the notion of the British stiff upper lip (Fraser, 1992). Normalisation of

emotional suffering is also a practice actively encouraged in the West as a means of

destigmatising mental illness (Hinshaw & Cicchetti, 2000). However, from the

perspective of mental health professionals, individual coping strategies such as stoic

silence and minimisation have been largely deemed unhelpful and even pathological.

Western psychiatric literature has typically associated emotional suppression with

negative social and psychological outcomes (Gross & John, 2003). The findings from

this research, as well as other cross-cultural studies of refugee mental health (Sachs et

al., 2008; Perez & Soto, 2011), indicate that the relationship between psychological

coping strategies (e.g., expression of emotion, introspective analysis and cognitive

reappraisal) and psychological functioning is moderated by culture and context.

Western mental health literature has, as yet, failed to sufficiently explore the role,

function and implications of such stoic silence, minimisation and other associated

practices in non-Western (and possibly Western) populations. This study with Karen

women has contributed to filling this gap.

As discussed throughout this thesis (see Sections 8.5, 10.2.4, 10.3.1 and 10.6.3) the

findings from this study have demonstrated that the practices of acceptance, stoic

silence, minimisation and normalisation have complex origins and multifarious (as

well as sometime contradictory) implications for Karen women’s wellbeing. Women

found solace through accepting and normalising distress as a universal feature of

human life, as well as through minimising the circumstances of their suffering in

relation to their peers and others. However, as discussed in Chapter 12 (see Sections

12.7.2 and 12.8), we have also seen how these practices appear to contribute, in the

context of protracted persecution and forced migration, to a worsening of

psychosocial problems and become hindrances to effective resettlement. There has

310

been a general failure to consider the complex pathways and interactions between

these practices and wellbeing.

15.5 Paradigm clashes and practical problems

Women positioned community-based social supports as more salient to their

emotional and mental health needs than outside agencies. Of external support

services, those that addressed women’s most pressing concerns and assisted them in

developing skills to adapt effectively and feel effectual in their lives in Australia were

perceived to be most helpful. However, women’s ability to access and benefit from

health and social support services was limited by several persistent inadequacies and

problems within service provisions (see Chapters 12 and 13). Most notably, women’s

interactions with service providers were plagued by communication problems. A

further problem was culturally incongruent methods and models of service delivery.

Most Karen women did not see Western psychotherapy as relevant to their needs in

large part because (a) they believed it could not solve their most pressing problems,

(b) they did not relate to talking to strangers, and (c) there was too much focus on

things they did not want to talk about. Various other cultural mismatches (or

paradigm clashes) also appeared to act as barriers to Karen women accessing and

benefiting from mental health services. Some participants also believed that Western

mental health services and diagnoses were potentially limiting and unhelpful to the

Karen community. They argued that these approaches weakened the strength of

individuals and the community as a whole and viewed the trauma discourse as

contributing to apathy, dependency and helplessness amongst community members.

Practitioners are advised to do no harm and yet, paradoxically, their therapeutic

strategies may do just that. Service providers have largely addressed interventions

from an ethnocentric and monolithic view, using “therapeutic” strategies with refugee

populations for whom those strategies were not developed. The research findings,

particularly those discussed in Chapter 13, suggest that prevailing Western mental

health discourses should be re-examined and professionals must give attention to the

possibility that Western “therapeutic” treatments may indeed be unhelpful, offensive

and even harmful for refugee populations. Strategies that are beneficial for Western

populations enjoying a period of political peace are not necessarily helpful – and

311

indeed may be harmful – for Asian (or other) populations experiencing conflict,

oppression and persecution, and forced migration. Individualistic models of therapy,

particularly those that emphasise the importance of recounting and analysing past

traumatic experiences, are unlikely to meet the complex somatopsychosocial sources

and expressions of suffering experienced by Karen women. An understanding of the

importance of the shared experience in Karen women’s approaches to coping may

help to explain some of the difficulties experienced when Karen community members

attended Australian mental health services.

The implications of the paradigm clashes were many and most obviously involved

Karen patients avoiding or merely superficially entertaining mental health service

providers, for example, politely attending appointments but without truly engaging or

agreeing with the necessity of the visits. Participants often regarded psychotherapies

as irrelevant or useless but did not tend to describe them as harmful. The effect of

misaligned cultural perspectives between mental health practitioners and Karen

community members occasionally resulted in frustration and offence. Apart from Dr

G’s account (see Section 13.4), in which he had to intervene and stop a patient

attending counselling, there were no other direct allegations of harm caused to Karen

patients. However, these findings still point towards the need to ask: “Are trauma

counseling services as beneficial, or even as harmless, as they seem at first sight?”

(Argenti-Pillen, 2003, p. 10). The findings discussed in Chapter 13 suggest that they

are not.

15.6 Final reflections

The findings strongly suggest that refugee mental health needs to be viewed through a

social determinants of mental health framework such as that recommended by Barry

and Jenkins (2007) who divide these determinants into the individual, community and

structural.

As individuals, Karen (and other) refugees have demonstrated their personal

resilience and ability to deal with stress. Many have managed to survive situations of

unimaginable suffering. They have demonstrated their will to live through their

persistence and their fight to survive. They have resettled in Australia because they

have hope. They are agents, immensely resourceful, and extremely wilful. It is not

312

easy to plunge into the unknown of resettlement even when the known is a dark and

dangerous place. Their communities are strong, and they continue to place a high

value on social support and community cohesion.

Whilst most Karen refugees enjoy a strong sense of belonging within their own ethnic

communities, the same cannot necessarily be said for the sense of inclusion and social

participation afforded them within the broader Australian context where structural

factors act as barriers to their wellbeing. Indeed, privileged societies are becoming

increasingly intolerant and unwelcoming towards refugees at a time when 44 million

displaced people do not wake up each morning feeling safe.

In Australia the issue of humanitarian entrants is a politically loaded topic attracting

heated public debate (Mares & Allotey, 2003). Media-fuelled public resistance

towards “boat people”, “queue jumpers” and “economic migrants” has created an

atmosphere of distrust, resistance and discrimination towards all humanitarian entrant

populations (Murray, Davidson, & Schweitzer, 2008; Refugee Council of Australia,

2011a). They are often conveyed as vulnerable hapless victims in need of assistance,

or as sneaky asylum seekers and economic migrants threatening to take jobs from

Australians. Highly qualified humanitarian entrants face often insurmountable barriers

to getting their skills recognised in Australia. Many of those with tertiary

qualifications are not even asked about their skills (Refugee Council of Australia,

2011a). Those with adequate English struggle to gain jobs let alone meaningful

employment. Those without English are marginalised on the fringe of Australian

society and face discrimination when seeking adequate housing. The structural

determinants of health including poverty and discrimination are weakening the

capacity and wellbeing of individuals and communities from refugee backgrounds.

The research findings from this study with Karen women demonstrate the need for

political and civic social action to address the complex and systemic factors affecting

the wellbeing of refugees in Australia. Moreover, the current study clearly emphasises

the need for a strength-based, culturally congruent community development approach

to both service provision and research. They reiterate Minas’ comments, made over

20 years ago, in The Health of Immigrant Australia: A Social Perspective:

313

The maintenance of good mental health requires, amongst other things,

freedom from injury, poverty and discrimination, the opportunity for

productive work, education and leisure … Bringing about change in such

noxious circumstances is clearly a political process. (Minas, 1990, p. 280)

314

References

Abdi, A. M. (2005). In limbo: Dependency, insecurity and identity amongst Somali

refugees in Dadaab camps. Refuge, 22(2), 17–34. Retrieved from

http://refuge.umn.edu/staff/index.html

Abramowitz, S. A. (2010). Trauma and humanitarian translation in Liberia: The tale

of the open mole. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34(2), 353–379. doi:

10.1007/s11013-010-9172-0

Abreu, M. J. (2001). Theory and research on stereotypes and perceptual bias: A

didactic resource for multicultural counseling trainers. The Counseling

Psychologist, 29(4), 487–512. doi: 10.1177/0011000001294002

ACL. (n.d.). AMEP Counselling Service. Retrieved 23 June, 2011, from

http://www.acl.edu.au/amep_counselling.html

Adult Migrant English Program Research Centre. (2006). Learners with low literacy

in the Adult Migrant English Program (AMEP). Retrieved 19 January, 2012,

from http://www.ameprc.mq.edu.au/resources/amep_fact_sheets/amep_fact_

sheets_-_by_title

Agaibi, C. E., & Wilson, J. P. (2005). Trauma, PTSD, and resilience: A review of the

literature. Trauma, Violence and Abuse, 6(3), 195–216. doi: 10.1177/

1524838005277438

Aidoo, M., & Harpham, T. (2001). The explanatory models of mental health amongst

low-income women and health care practitioners in Lusaka, Zambia. Health

Policy Plan, 16(2), 206–213. doi: 10.1093/heapol/16.2.206

Albrecht, D. E., Albrecht, C. M., & Murguia, E. (2005). Minority concentration,

disadvantage, and inequality in the nonmetropolitan United States. The

Sociological Quarterly, 46(3), 503–523. doi: 10.1111/j.1533-8525.2005.

00024.x

Al-Busaidi, Z. Q. (2010). A qualitative study on the attitudes and beliefs towards help

seeking for emotional distress in Omani women and Omani general practitioners:

Implications for post-graduate training. Oman Medical Journal, 25(3), 1–9. doi:

10.5001/omj.2010.55

315

Allden, K., Poole, C., Chantavanich, S., Ohmar, K., Aung, N. N., & Mollica, R. F.

(1996). Burmese political dissidents in Thailand: Trauma and survival among

young adults in exile. American Journal of Public Health, 86(11), 1561–1569.

Retrieved from http://ajph.aphapublications.org

Allison, B. N. (2000). Parent-adolescent conflict in early adolescence: Research and

implications for middle school programs. Journal of Family and Consumer

Sciences Education, 18(2), 1–6. Retrieved from http://www.natefacs.org/

JFCSE/jfcse.htm

Allotey, P. (1999). Travelling with “excess baggage”: Health problems of refugee

women in Western Australia. Women & Health, 28(1), 63–81. doi: 10.1300/

J013v28n01_05

Allotey, P., & Manderson, L. (2003). From case studies to casework: Ethics and

obligations in research with refugee women. In P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of

refugees: Public health perspectives from crisis to settlement (pp. 200–211).

Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

Almedom, A. M., & Summerfield, D. (2004). Mental well-being in settings of

‘complex emergency’: An overview. Journal of Biosocial Science, 36(4), 381–

388. doi: 10.1017/S0021932004006832

Alternative ASEAN Network on Burma. (1999). Report card: The situation of women

in Burma. Bangkok, Thailand: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of

mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of

mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Andary, L., Stolk, Y., & Klimidis, S. (2003). Assessing mental health across cultures.

Bowen Hills, Australia: Australian Academic Press.

Andreasen, N. C. (1995). Posttraumatic stress disorder: Psychology, biology, and the

manichaean warfare between false dichotomies. American Journal of Psychiatry,

152(7), 963–965. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/

journal.aspx?journalid=13

Angelelli, C. V. (2004). Medical interpreting and cross-cultural communication.

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

316

Angrosino, M. V. (2005). Recontextualizing observation: Ethnography, pedagogy,

and the prospects for a progressive political agenda. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S.

Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 729–

745). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Apple, B., & Martin, V. (2003). No safe place: Burma’s army and the rape of ethnic

women. Retrieved 11 November, 2011, from Refugees International website:

http://repository.forcedmigration.org/show_metadata.jsp?pid=fmo:3162

Aptekar, L., & Giel, R. (2002). Walks in Kaliti, life in a destitute shelter for the

displaced. In J. T. V. M. de Jong (Ed.), Trauma, war and violence: Public mental

health in socio-cultural context (pp. 337–366). New York, NY: Kluwer

Academic / Plenum.

Argenti-Pillen, A. (2003). Masking terror: How women contain violence in Southern

Sri Lanka. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Aroche, J., & Coello, M. J. (2004). Ethnocultural considerations in the treatment of

refugees and asylum seekers. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken

spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture

victims (pp. 53–80). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Atkin, N. (2008). Getting the message across: Professional interpreters in general

practice. Australian Family Physician, 37(3), 174–176. Retrieved from

http://www.racgp.org.au/afp

Atkinson, P., & Hammersley, M. (1994). Ethnography and participant observation. In

N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 248–

261). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Atwell, R., Gifford, S. M., & McDonald-Wilmsen, B. (2009). Resettled refugee

families and their children’s futures: Coherence, hope and support. Journal of

Comparative Family Studies, 40(5), 677–697. Retrieved from http://soci.

ucalgary.ca/jcfs

Auerbach, E. R. (1993). Reexamining English only in the ESL classroom. TESOL

Quarterly 27(1), 9–32. Retrieved from http://www.tesol.org/s_tesol/seccss.

asp?cid=209&did=1679

Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2006). Socio-economic indexes for areas. (Cat no.

2033.0.55.001). Retrieved 21 May, 2012, from http://www.abs.gov.au/

AUSSTATS/[email protected]/DetailsPage/2033.0.55.0012006?OpenDocument

317

Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2009). Migration. (Cat no. 3412.0). Retrieved 18

April, 2012, from http://www.abs.gov.au/AUSSTATS/[email protected]/DetailsPage/

3412.02007-08?OpenDocument

Baetz, M., & Toews, J. (2009). Clinical implications of research on religion,

spirituality, and mental health. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54(5), 292–

301. Retrieved from http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/sections/0

Bagot, R., Parent, C., Bredy, T. W., Zhang, T., Gratton, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2007).

Developmental origins of neurobiological vulnerablity for PTSD. In L. J.

Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating

biological, clinical and cultural perspectives (pp. 98–117). New York, NY:

Cambridge University Press.

Bailey, A. (2008). Let’s tell you a story: Use of vignettes in focus group discussions

on HIV/AIDS among migrant and mobile men in Goa, India. In P. Liamputtong

(Ed.), Doing cross-cultural research: Ethical and methodological perspectives

(pp. 253–264). In Social Indicators Research Series. London, England: Springer.

Bailey, J. (2008). First steps in qualitative data analysis: Transcribing. Family

Practice, 25(2), 127–131. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmn003

Bala, J. (2010). Beyond the personal pain: Integrating social and political concerns in

therapy with refugees. In D. Ingleby (Ed.), Forced migration and mental health:

Rethinking the care of refugees and displaced persons (pp. 169–182). New York,

NY: Springer.

Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.

Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/

pubs/journals/rev/index.aspx

Bandura, A. (2000). Exercise of human agency through collective efficacy. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 9(3), 75–78. Retrieved from http://cdp.

sagepub.com

Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review

of Psychology, 52, 1–26. Retrieved from www.annualreviews.org

Banfield, G. (2004). What’s really wrong with ethnography? International Education

Journal, 4(4), 53–63. Retrieved from http://iej.cjb.net

Banks, J. (1998). The lives and values of researchers: Implications for educating

citizens in a multicultural society. Educational Researcher, 27(7), 4–17.

Retrieved from http://edr.sagepub.com

318

Bann, C. M., Parker, C. B., Bradweijn, J., Davidson, J. R. T., Vitiello, B., & Gadde,

K. (2004). Assessing patient beliefs in a clinical trial of hypericum perforatum in

major depression. Depression and Anxiety, 20(3), 114–122. doi: 10.1002/

da.20036

Banyard, V. L., & Miller, K. E. (1998). The powerful potential of qualitative research

for community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 26(4),

485–505. doi: 10.1023/A:1022136821013

Barbour, R. (2001). Checklists for improving rigour in qualitative research: A case of

the tail wagging the dog? British Medical Journal, 322(7294), 1115–1117. doi:

10.1136/bmj.322.7294.1115

Baron, N. (2002). Community based psychosocial and mental health services for

Southern Sudanese refugees in long term exile in Uganda. In J. T. V. M. de Jong

(Ed.), Trauma, war and violence: Public mental health in socio-cultural context

(pp. 157–203). New York, NY: Kluwer Academic / Plenum.

Barron, S., Okell, J., Yin, S. M., VanBik, K., Swain, A., Larkin, E., … Ewers, K.

(2007). Refugees from Burma: Their backgrounds and refugee experiences

(Culture Profile No. 21). D. Ranard & S. Barron (Eds.). Retrieved 18 January,

2012, from Center for Applied Linguistics website: www.cal.org/co/pdffiles/

refugeesfromburma.pdf

Barry, M. M., & Jenkins, R. (2007). Implementing mental health promotion. Oxford,

England: Elsevier.

Basoglu, M., Livanou, M., Crnobaric, C., Franciskovic, T., Suljic, E., Duric, D., &

Vranesic, M. (2005). Psychiatric and cognitive effects of war in former

Yugoslavia: Association of lack of redress for trauma and posttraumatic stress

reactions. Journal of the American Medical Association, 294(5), 580–590. doi:

10.1001/jama.294.5.580

Basoglu, M., Mineka, S., Paker, M., Aker, T., Livanou, M., & Gok, S. (1997).

Psychological preparedness for trauma as a protective factor in survivors of

torture. Psychological Medicine, 27(6), 1421–1433. doi: 10.1017/

S0033291797005679

Bass, J. K., Bolton, P. A., & Murray, L. K. (2007). Do not forget culture when

studying mental health. The Lancet, 370(9591), 918–919. doi: 10.1016/

S0140-6736(07)61426-3

319

Bauer, M., Priebe, S., Kurten, I., Klaus-Jurgen, G., & Baumgartner, A. (1994).

Psychological and endocrine abnormalities in refugees from East Germany: Part

I. Prolonged stress, psychopathology, and hypothalamic-pituitary-thyroid axis

activity. Psychiatry Research, 51(1), 61–73. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90047-7

Baumeister, R. F., & Exline, J. J. (1999). Virtue, personality, and social relations:

Self-control as the moral muscle. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 1165–1194. doi:

10.1111/1467-6494.00086

Bayer, R., & Spitzer, R. L. (1985). Neurosis, psychodynamics, and DSM-III: A

history of the controversy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42(2), 187–196.

Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org

Bemak, F., Chung, R. C. Y., & Bornemann, T. H. (1981). Counseling and

psychotherapy with refugees. In P. B. Pedersen, J. G. Draguns, W. J. Lonner & J.

E. Trimble (Eds.), Counseling across cultures (4th ed., pp. 243–265). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Benight, C. C., & Bandura, A. (2004). Social cognitive theory of posttraumatic

recovery: The role of perceived self-efficacy. Behaviour Research and Therapy,

42(10), 1129–1148. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2003.08.008

Berry, J. W. (1992). Acculturation and adaptation in a new society. International

Migration, 30(s1), 69–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2435.1992.tb00776.x

Betancourt, J. R., Green, A. R., Carrillo, J. E., & Park, E. R. (2005). Cultural

competence and health care disparities: Key perspectives and trends. Health

Affairs, 24(2), 445–505. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.24.2.499

Bhaskar, R. (1978). A realist theory of science. Brighton, England: Harvester Press.

Bhaskar, R. (1986). Scientific realism and human emancipation. Norfolk, England:

Thetford Press.

Bhaskar, R. (1989). The possibility of naturalism. Hemel Hempstead, England:

Harvester Wheatsheaf.

Bhaskar, R. (1998). Facts and values: Theory and practice. In M. Archer, R. Bhaskar,

A. Collier, T. Lawson & A. Norrie (Eds.), Critical realism: Essential readings

(pp. 409–417). London, England: Routledge.

Bhaskar, R., & Lawson, T. (1998). Introduction: Basic texts and developments. In M.

Archer, R. Bhaskar, A. Collier, T. Lawson & A. Norrie (Eds.), Critical realism:

Essential readings (pp. 3–15). London, England: Routledge.

320

Bhugra, D. (2004). Migration and mental health. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,

109(4), 243–258. doi: 10.1046/j.0001-690X.2003.00246.x

Bhugra, D. (2005). Cultural identities and cultural congruency: A new model for

evaluating mental distress in immigrants. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,

111(2), 84–93. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00454.x

Bhui, K., & Bhugra, D. (2002). Explanatory models for mental distress: Implications

for clinical practice and research. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181(1), 6–7. doi:

10.1192/bjp.181.1.6

Bhui, K., King, M., Dein, S., & O’Connor, W. (2008). Ethnicity and religious coping

with mental distress. Journal of Mental Health, 17(2), 141–151. doi: 10.1080/

09638230701498408

Bhui, K., Rudell, K., & Priebe, S. (2006). Assessing explanatory models for common

mental disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67(6), 964–971. Retrieved from

http://www.psychiatrist.com

Bird, S. (2008). Lost without translation. Australian Family Physician, 37(12), 1023–

1024. Retrieved from http://www.racgp.org.au/afp

Birman, D., Beehler, S., Harris, E. M., Everson, M. L., Batia, K., Liautaud, J., …

Cappella, E. (2008). International Family, Adult, and Child Enhancement

Services (FACES): A community-based comprehensive services model for

refugee children in resettlement. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78(1),

121–132. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.78.1.121

Bisht, N. S., & Bankoti, T. S. (2004). Encyclopaedia of South-east Asian

ethnography: Communities and tribes (Vol. 1). Delhi, India: Global Vision.

Bisson, J., & Andrew, M. (2007). Psychological treatment of post-traumatic stress

disorder (PTSD). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2007(2), 1–110.

doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000560

Blakemore, K. (2000). Health and social care needs in minority communities: An

over-problematized issue? Health and Social Care in the Community, 8(1), 22–

30. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2524.2000.00224.x

Blight, K. J., Ekblad, S., Lindencrona, F., & Shahnavaz, S. (2009) Promoting mental

health and preventing mental disorder among refugees in Western countries.

International Journal of Mental Health Promotion, 11(1), 32–44. Retrieved from

http://www.ijmhp.co.uk

321

Blignault, I., & Ritchie, J. (2009). Revealing the wood and the trees: Reporting

qualitative research. Health Promotion Journal of Australia, 20(2), 140–145.

Retrieved from http://www.healthpromotion.org.au/journal

Bloor, M., Frankland, J., Thomas, M., & Robson, K. (2001). Focus groups in social

research. London, England: Sage.

Bodeker, G., Neumann, C., Lall, P., & Oo, Z. M. (2005). Traditional medicine use

and healthworker training in a refugee setting at the Thai-Burma border. Journal

of Refugee Studies, 18(1), 76–99. doi: 10.1093/jrs/18.1.76

Boehnlein, J. K. (2007). Religion and spirituality after trauma. In L. J. Kirmayer, R.

Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological,

clinical and cultural perspectives (pp. 259–274). New York, NY: Cambridge

University Press.

Boehnlein, J. K., Kinzie, J. D., Sekiya, U., Riley, C., Pou, K., & Rosborough, B.

(2004). A ten-year treatment outcome study of traumatized Cambodian refugees.

Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 192(10), 658–663. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Bolton, P. (2001). Local perceptions of the mental health effects of the Rwandan

genocide. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 189(4), 243–248. Retrieved

from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Bolton, P., Bass, J., Betancourt, T., Speelman, L., Onyango, G., Clougherty, K. F., …

Verdeli, H. (2007). Interventions for depression symptoms among adolescent

survivors of war and displacement in northern Uganda: A randomized controlled

trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 298(5), 519–527. Retrieved

from http://jama.ama-assn.org

Bolton, P., & Betancourt, T. S. (2004). Mental health in postwar Afghanistan. Journal

of the American Medical Association, 292(5), 626–628. doi: 10.1001/

jama.292.5.626

Bolton, P., & Tang, A. M. (2002). An alternative approach to cross-cultural function

assessment. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37(11), 537–543.

doi: 10.1007/s00127-002-0580-5

Bolton, P., & Tang, A. M. (2004). Using ethnographic methods in the selection of

post-disaster, mental health interventions. Prehospital and Disaster Medicine,

19(1), 97–101. Retrieved from http://pdm.medicine.wisc.edu

322

Bolton, P., Wilk, C. M., & Ndogoni, L. (2004). Assessment of depression prevalence

in rural Uganda using symptom and function criteria. Social Psychiatry and

Psychiatric Epidemiology, 39(6), 442–447. doi: 10.1007/s00127-004-0763-3

Bonanno, G. A., & Mancini, A. D. (2012). Beyond resilience and PTSD: Mapping the

heterogeneity of responses to potential trauma. Psychology Trauma: Theory,

Research, Practice, and Policy, 4(1), 74–83. doi: 10.1037/a0017829

Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., van der Ende, J., & Verhust, F. C. (2003). The normative

development of child and adolescent problem behavior. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 112(2), 179–192. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.2.179

Booth, T., & Booth, W. (1994). Parenting under pressure: Mothers and fathers with

learning difficulties. Buckingham, England: Open University Press.

Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. New York, NY: Cambridge

University Press.

Bourdieu, P. (1990). In other words: Essays towards a reflexive sociology.

Cambridge, England: Polity.

Bouton, M. E., & Waddell, J. (2007). Some biobehavioral insights into persistent

effects of emotional trauma. In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.),

Understanding trauma: Integration biological, clinical and cultural perspectives

(pp. 27–40). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Bracken, P. J. (2001). Post-modernity and post-traumatic stress disorder. Social

Science and Medicine, 53(6), 733–743. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00385-3

Bracken, P. J., Giller, J. E., & Summerfield, D. (1995). Psychological responses to

war and atrocity: The limitations of current concepts. Social Science and

Medicine, 40(8), 1073–1082. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00181-R

Brant, C. S., & Khaing, M. M. (1951). Burmese kinship and the life cycle: An outline.

Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 7(4), 437–454. Retrieved from

http://www.jstor.org

Bray, P. (2010). A broader framework for exploring the influence of spiritual

experience in the wake of stressful life events: Examining connections between

posttraumatic growth and psycho-spiritual transformation. Mental Health,

Religion and Culture, 13(3), 293–308. doi: 10.1080/

13674670903367199

Brees, I. (2008a). Forced displacement of Burmese people. Forced Migration Review,

30, 4–5. Retrieved from http://www.fmreview.org

323

Brees, I. (2008b). Refugee business: Strategies of work on the Thai-Burma border.

Journal of Refugee Studies, 21(3), 380–397. doi: 10.1093/jrs/fen02

Brees, I. (2010). Refugees and transnationalism on the Thai-Burmese border. Global

Networks, 10(2), 282–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0374.2010.00286.x

Bremner, J. D. (2007). Does stress damage the brain? In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson

& M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical and

cultural perspectives (pp. 118–141). New York, NY: Cambridge University

Press.

Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Valentine, J. D. (2000). Meta-analysis of risk factors

for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. Journal of Consulting

and Clinical Psychology, 68(5), 748–766. Retrieved from

http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/ccp/index.aspx

Briggs, J. (1970). Never in anger: Portrait of an Eskimo family. Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Briggs, L., & Macleod, A. D. (2006). Demoralisation - A useful conceptualisation of

non-specific psychological distress among refugees attending health services.

International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 52(6), 512–524. doi:

10.1177/0020764006066832

Brill, N. Q., & Beebe, G. W. (1955). A follow-up study of war neuroses. Washington,

DC: Government Printing Office.

Brune, M., Haasen, C., Krausz, M., Yagdiran, O., Bustos, E., & Eisenman, D. (2002).

Belief systems as coping factors for traumatized refugees: A pilot study.

European Psychiatry, 17(8), 451–458. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(02)00708-3

Bryman, A. (2008). Social research methods (3rd ed.). Oxford, England: Oxford

University Press.

Bumiller, K. (1988). The civil rights society: The social construction of victims.

Maryland, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

Burma’s NLD in parliament ‘boycott’ over oath. (2012, April 23). BBC News.

Retrieved 21 May, 2012, from http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world-asia-17803628

Butler, E. A., Lee, T. L., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion regulation and culture: Are

the social consequences of emotion suppression culture-specific? Emotion, 7(1),

30–45. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.30

Cachia, G., Zu, D., Chahola, B., & Meale, R. (2010, October). Haven Australia. Paper

presented at the Community Garden Conference, Canberra, Australia.

324

Calabrese, J. D. (2010). Clinical paradigm clashes. In R. A. Le Vine (Ed.),

Psychological anthropology: A reader on self in culture (pp. 309–323).

Chichester, England: Wiley-Blackwell.

Calia, V. F. (1966). The culturally deprived client: A reformulation of the counselor’s

role. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 13(1), 100–105. Retrieved from

http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/cou/index.aspx

Callahan, M. P. (2007). Political authority in Burma’s ethnic minority states:

Devolution, occupation, and coexistence. Washington, DC: East-West Center.

Campbell, E. J., & Turpin, M. J. (2010). Refugee settlement workers’ perspectives on

home safety issues for people from refugee backgrounds. Australian

Occupational Therapy Journal, 57(6), 425–430. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1630.

2010.00882.x

Caouette, T. M., Archavanitkul, K., & Pyne, H. H. (2000). Sexuality, reproductive

health and violence: Experience of migrants from Burma in Thailand. Mahidol

University, Institute for Population and Social Research. Retrieved 18 April,

2012, from http://whothailand.healthrepository.org/handle/

123456789/1116

Caouette, T. M., & Pack, M. E. (2002). Pushing past the definitions: Migration from

Burma to Thailand. Washington, DC: Refugees International and Open Society

Institute. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from www.unhcr.org/refworld/

pdfid/47a6eb9d0.pdf

Carrington, V., & Luke, A. (1997). Literacy and Bourdieu’s sociological theory: A

reframing. Language and Education, 11(2), 96–112. doi: 10.1080/

09500789708666721

Carswell, K., Blackburn, P., & Barker, C. (2011). The relationship between trauma,

post-migration problems and the psychological well-being of refugees and

asylum seekers. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 57(2), 107–119. doi:

10.1177/0020764008105699

Carter, S. M., Ritchie, J. E., & Sainsbury, P. (2009). Doing good qualitative research

in public health: Not as easy as it looks. New South Wales Public Health Bulletin,

20(7–8), 105–111. doi: 10.1071/NB09018

Cayoun, B. (2011). Mindfulness integrated CBT: Principles and practice. Chichester,

London: Wiley-Blackwell.

325

Central Intelligence Agency. (2010). The world factbook, East & Southeast Asia,

Burma. Retrieved 2 December 2010, from https://www.cia.gov/library/

publications/the-world-factbook/geos/bm.html

Centre for Culture, Ethnicity and Health, Victorian Multicultural Gambler’s Help

Program. (2008). Problem gambling in new and emerging refugee communities.

A research report on the Liberian, Somali, Iraqi Muslim and Sudanese Dinka

communities. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from

http://www.ceh.org.au/about/projects/pages/problem_gambling_new_emerging.a

spx

Centre for Refugee Research. (n.d.). Sri Lanka tsunami data collection training kit:

DC session 7 – Storyboard sessions. [PowerPoint slides]. Retrieved 3 February,

2011, from http://www.crr.unsw.edu.au/education-and-training/training-

resources/

Chalamwong, Y. (2004). Government policies on international migration: Illegal

workers in Thailand. In A. Ananta & A. Eva Nurvidya (Eds.), International

migration in Southeast Asia (pp. 352–373). Singapore: Institute of Southeast

Asian Studies.

Chan, B. (2010). Assertive communication program for Chinese Australians – Final

report. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from University of New South Wales, Faculty

of Medicine, Centre for Primary Health Care & Equity website:

http://unsworks.unsw.edu.au/vital/access/manager/Repository/unsworks:8378

Chan, B., & Ritchie, J. (2011). Perceptions of Chinese Australians on seeking help for

mental health problems: A qualitative study. International Journal of Mental

Health Promotion, 13(4), 36–45. doi: 10.1080/14623730.2011.9715667

Charmaz, K. (1990). ‘Discovering’ chronic illness: Using grounded theory. Social

Science and Medicine, 30(11), 1161–1172. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)

90256-R

Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through

qualitative analysis. London, England: Sage.

Charmaz, K., & Henwood, K. (2008). Grounded theory. In C. Willig & W. Stainton-

Rogers (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research in psychology (pp.

240–260). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

326

Chaves, N. J., Gibney, K. B., Leder, K., O’Brien, K. L., Marshall, C., & Biggs, B.

(2009). Screening practices for infectious diseases among Burmese refugees in

Australia. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 15(11), 1769–1772. Retrieved from

doi:10.3201/eid1511.090777

Checchi, F., Elder, G., Schafer, M., Drouhin, E., & Legros, D. (2003). Consequences

of armed conflict for an ethnic Karen population. The Lancet, 362(9377), 74–75.

doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13813-5

Chemtob, C. M., Hamada, R. S., Roitblat, H. L., & Muraoka, M. Y. (1994). Anger,

impulsivity, and anger control in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder.

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62(4), 827–832. Retrieved from

http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/ccp/index.aspx

China and Russia veto US/UK-backed Security Council draft resolution on Myanmar.

(2007, January 12). UN News Centre. Retrieved 30 September, 2011, from

http://www.un.org/apps/news/story.asp?NewsID=21228&Cr=myanmar&Cr1

Chipimo, P. J., Tuba, M., & Fylkesnes, K. (2011). Conceptual models for mental

distress among HIV-infected and uninfected individuals: A contribution to

clinical practice and research in primary-health-care centers in Zambia. BioMed

Central Health Services Research, 11(7): 7. doi: 10.1186/1472-6963-11-7

Client satisfaction survey 2010. (2010, Spring). Talking TIS Newsletter. Retrieved 10

September, 2011, from Department of Immigration and Citizenship website:

http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-australia/help-with-english/help_with_

translating/talking-tis/

Chow, J. C.-C., Jaffee, K., & Snowden, L. (2003). Racial/ethnic disparities in the use

of mental health services in poverty areas. American Journal of Public Health,

93(5), 792–797. Retrieved from http://ajph.aphapublications.org

Cohen, D. J., & Crabtree, B. F. (2008). Evaluative criteria for qualitative research in

health care: Controversies and recommendations. Annals of Family Medicine,

6(4), 331–339. doi: 10.1370/afm.818

Colic-Peisker, V., & Tilbury, F. (2003). “Active” and “passive” resettlement: The

influence of support services and refugees’ own resources on resettlement styles.

International Migration, 41(5), 61–89. doi: 10.1111/j.0020-7985.

2003.00261.x

327

Colic-Peisker, V., & Tilbury, F. (2007). Integration into the Australian labour market:

The experience of three “visibly different” groups of recently arrived refugees.

International Migration Journal, 45(1), 59–82. Retrieved from

http://au.wiley.com/WileyCDA/WileyTitle/productCd-IMIG.html

Collins, K. S., Hughes, D. L., Doty, M. M., Ives, B. L., Edwards, J. N., & Tenney, K.

(2002). Diverse communities, common concerns: Assessing health care quality

for minority Americans: Findings from the Commonwealth Fund 2001 health

care quality survey. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from Commonwealth Fund

website: http://www.commonwealthfund.org/Content/Publications/

Fund-Reports/2002/Mar/Diverse-Communities--Common-Concerns--Assessing-

Health-Care-Quality-for-Minority-Americans.aspx

Collins, C. H., Zimmerman, C., & Howard, L. M. (2011). Refugee, asylum seeker,

immigrant women and postnatal depression: Rates and risk factors. Archives of

Women's Mental Health, 14(1), 3–11. doi: 10.1007/s00737-010-0198-7

Connelly, J. (2001). Critical realism and health promotion: Effective practice needs an

effective theory. Health Education Research, 16(2), 115–120. doi: 10.1093/

her/16.2.115

Constantine, M. G., Myers, L. J., Kindaichi, A., & Moore, J. L. (2004). Exploring

indigenous mental health practices: The roles of healers and helpers in promoting

well-being in people of colour. Counseling and Values, 48(2), 110–125. doi:

10.1002/j.2161-007X.2004.tb00238.x

Constantino, G., Malgady, R. G., & Primavera, L. H. (2009). Congruence between

culturally competent treatment and cultural needs of older Latinos. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77(5), 941–949. doi: 10.1037/a0016341

Conversations with Karen. (2011, September). The Globe, 10, 4. Retrieved 18 April,

2012, from http://www.sphcm.med.unsw.edu.au/sphcmweb.nsf/page/

SPHCMReports

Cooper, C., Bebbington, P. E., Meltzer, H., Bhugra, D., Brugha, T., Jenkins, R., . . .

King, M. (2008). Depression and common mental disorders in lone parents:

Results of the 2000 National Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Psychological

Medicine, 38(3), 335-345. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001493

Cortese, M. (1979). Intervention research with Hispanic Americans: A review.

Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 1(1), 4–20. Retrieved from

http://hjb.sagepub.com

328

Crawford, C. F. (2006). Cultural, economic and legal factors underlying trafficking in

Thailand and their impact on women and girls from Burma. Cardozo Journal of

Law and Gender, 12(3), 821–853. Retrieved from

http://www.cardozolawandgender.com/index.html

Creswell, J. W. (1998). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among

five traditions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cromby, J., & Standen, P. (1999). Taking ourselves seriously. In J. Cromby & D.

Nightingale (Eds.), Social constructionist psychology: A critical analysis of

theory and practice (pp. 141–156). Buckingham, England: Open University

Press.

Cutliffe, J. R., & Ramcharan, P. (2002). Leveling the playing field? Exploring the

merits of the ethics-as-process approach for judging qualitative research

proposals. Qualitative Health Research, 12(7), 1000–1010. doi: 10.1177/

104973202129120313

Daly, J., Kellehear, A., & Gliksman, M. (1997). The public health research: A

methodological guide. Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

Danermark, B., Ekstrom, M., Jakobsen, L., & Karlsson, J. C. (2002). Explaining

society: Critical realism in the social sciences. London, England: Routledge.

Daniel, E. V., & Knudsen, J. C. (1995). Introduction. In E. V. Daniel & J. C. Knudsen

(Eds.), Mistrusting refugees (pp. 1–12). Berkeley: University of California Press.

Deacon, Z., & Sullivan, C. (2009). Responding to the complex and gendered needs of

refugee women. Affilia, 24(3), 272–284. doi: 10.1177/0886109909337401

De Haene, L., Grietens, H., & Verschueren, K. (2010). Holding harm: Narrative

methods in mental health research on refugee trauma. Qualitative Health

Research, 20(12), 1664–1676. doi: 10.1177/1049732310376521

Dejman, M., Ekblad, S., Forouzan, A., Baradaran-Eftekhari, M., & Malekafzali, H.

(2008). Explanatory model of help-seeking and coping mechanisms among

depressed women in three ethnic groups of Fars, Kurdish, and Turkish in Iran.

Archives of Iranian Medicine, 11(4), 397–406. Retrieved from

http://www.ams.ac.ir/AIM

329

de Jong, J. T. V. M. (2002). Public mental health, traumatic stress and human rights

violations in low-income countries: A culturally appropriate model in times of

conflict, disaster and peace. In J. T. V. M. de Jong (Ed.), Trauma, war and

violence: Public mental health in socio-cultural context (pp. 1–91). New York,

NY: Kluwer Academic / Plenum.

de Jong, J. T. V. M. (2004). Public mental health and culture: Disasters as a challenge

to Western health care models, the self and PTSD. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek

(Eds.), Broken spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees,

war and torture victims (pp. 159–178). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

de Jong, J. T. V. M., & Komproe, I. H. (2002). Closing the gap between psychiatric

epidemiology and mental health in post-conflict situations. The Lancet,

359(9320), 1793–1794. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)08729-9

de Jong, J. T. V. M., Komproe, I. H., & Van Ommeren, M. (2003). Common mental

disorders in postconflict settings. The Lancet, 361(9375), 2128–2130. doi:

10.1016/S0140-6736(03)13692-6

de Jong, J. T. V. M., & Van Ommeren, M. (2002). Toward a culture-informed

epidemiology: Combining qualitative and quantitative research in transcultural

contexts. Transcultural Psychiatry, 39(4), 422–433. doi: 10.1177/

136346150203900402

de Laine, M. (1997). Ethnography: Theory and applications in health research.

Sydney, Australia: MacLennan and Petty.

Denburg, A., Rashid, M., Brophy, J., Curtis, T., Malloy, P., Audley, J., … Banerji, A.

(2007). Initial health screening results for Karen refugees: A retrospective

review. Canada Communicable Disease Report, 33(13), 16–23. Retrieved from

http://www.phac-aspc.gc.ca/publicat/ccdr-rmtc

Denzin, N. K., & Lincoln, Y. S. (Eds.). (2005). The Sage handbook of qualitative

research (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Department of Education, Employment and Workplace Relations. (2011). Socio-

economic indexes for areas. Retrieved 10 September, 2011, from

www.skillsinfo.gov.au/NR/rdonlyres/F57F6EB7-339A-41CF-8AF3-

28A6ACF194DD/0/NSWSEIFA.pdf

Department of Foreign Affairs and Trade. (2011). Burma country brief. Retrieved 29

September, 2011, from http://www.dfat.gov.au/geo/burma/burma_brief.html

330

Department of Human Services. (2011). Centrelink. Retrieved 29 September, 2011,

from www.centrelink.gov.au

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2006). Burmese community profile.

Retrieved 2 March, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-australia/

delivering-assistance/government-programs/settlement-planning/community-

profiles.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2008a). National Client Satisfaction

Survey, Adult Migrant English Program, 2007 Report. Retrieved 4 December,

2010, from www.immi.gov.au/.../client/.../AMEP_client_survey_report_

2007.pdf

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2008b). Review of the Adult Migrant

English Program. Retrieved 4 December, 2010, from www.ames.net.au/media/

pdf/11_amep_discussion_paper.pdf

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2009a). Annual report 2008–09.

Retrieved 30 August, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/about/reports/

annual/2008-09/html/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2009b). Fact sheet 91 – Translating and

interpreting Service (TIS) national. Retrieved 3 November, 2010, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/91tis.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2009c). Total number of Burma born

permanent arrivals in NSW by LGA, migration stream and year of arrival.

Retrieved 19 November, 2009, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-

australia/delivering-assistance/settlement-reporting-facility/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2010a). Annual report 2009–10.

Retrieved 1 December, 2010, from www.immi.gov.au/about/reports/annual/

2009-10

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2010b). Beginning a life in Australia.

Retrieved 22 November, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-

australia/settle-in-australia/beginning-life/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2010c). Settlement programs. Retrieved

1 December, 2010, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-australia/

delivering-assistance/government-programs/settlement-programs/

331

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011a). A significant contribution: The

economic, social and civic contributions of first and second generation

humanitarian entrants. Retrieved 2 April, 2012, from http://www.immi.gov.

au/media/publications/research/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011b). AMEP facts & figures.

Retrieved 29 June, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-australia/

help-with-english/amep/facts-figures/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011c). Complex case support services.

Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-

australia/delivering-assistance/government-programs/settlement-

programs/ccs.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011d). Country profile: Burma

(Myanmar). Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from www.immi.gov.au/media/

statistics/country-profiles/_pdf/burma.pdf

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011e). Fact sheet 64 – Community

assistance support program. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/64community-assistance.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011f). Fact sheet 66 – Humanitarian

settlement services. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/66hss.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011g). Fact sheet 67 – The Australian

Cultural Orientation (AUSCO) program. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/67ausco.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011h). Fact sheet 67a – Pre-departure

medical screening. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.

au/media/fact-sheets/67a_pdms.html

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011i). Fact sheet 69 – Caring for

unaccompanied minors. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/69unaccompanied.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011j). Fact sheet 92 – Settlement

grants program. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/

media/fact-sheets/92funding.htm

332

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011k). Fact sheet 94 – English courses

for eligible migrants and humanitarian entrants in Australia. Retrieved 29

September, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/media/fact-sheets/

94amep.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011l). Fact sheet 98 – Settlement

services for refugees. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from http://www.immi.

gov.au/media/fact-sheets/98services.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011m). Help with translations.

Retrieved 30 September, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-

australia/help-with-english/help_with_translating/translation_help.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011n). Immigration detention statistics

summary. Retrieved 26 October, 2011, from http://www.immi.

gov.au/managing-australias-borders/detention/facilities/statistics/

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011o). Refugee and humanitarian

issues: Australia’s response. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/publications/refugee/ref-hum-issues/ref-hum-

issues-june11.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011p). Settlers by country of birth

(settlement) by state: Settlement report. Retrieved 14 March, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/settlement_external/SettlementServlet?ACTION=GETR

EPORT&REPORTID=hgOTFd0ni5JrnU8FhUjWr2MtUREkoPltvrnjLq8m

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2011q). Urgent action on Newcastle

settlement services following independent review. [Press release]. Retrieved 18

April, 2012, from http://www.minister.immi.gov.au/media/cb/2011/

cb165699.htm

Department of Immigration and Citizenship. (2012). Torture and Trauma Counselling

(TTC) Grants Program. Retrieved 22 April, 2012, from http://www.immi.gov.

au/living-in-australia/delivering-assistance/government-programs/settlement-

programs/torture-trauma-counselling.htm

Department of Immigration and Multicultural and Indigenous Affairs. (2003). Report

of the review of settlement services for migrants and humanitarian entrants.

Retrieved 22 June, 2011, from http://www.immi.gov.au/living-in-australia/

delivering-assistance/government-programs/settlement-policy/review-settlement-

services.htm

333

Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature in conduct. New York, NY: Henry Holt.

Dewey, J. (1929). The quest for certainty. New York, NY: G. P. Putnam.

Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York, NY: Henry Holt.

Dewey, J., Moore, A. W., Brown, H. C., Mead, G. H., Bode, B. H., Stuart, H. W.,

Tufts, J. H., & Kallen, H. M. (1917). Creative intelligence: Essays in the

pragmatic attitude. New York, NY: Henry Holt.

Dickson-Swift, V., James, E. L., & Liamputtong, P. (2008). Undertaking sensitive

research in the health and social sciences: Managing boundaries, emotions and

risks. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Djuretic, T., Crawford, M. J., & Weaver, T. M. (2007). Role of qualitative research to

inform design of epidemiological studies: A cohort study of mental health of

migrants from the former Yugoslavia. Journal of Mental Health, 16(6), 743–755.

doi: 10.1080/09638230701496394

Douglas, J. D. (1976). Investigative social research: Individual and team field

research. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Drozdek, B. (2007). The rebirth of contextual thinking in psychotraumatology. In B.

Drozdek & J. P. Wilson (Eds.), Voices of trauma: Treating psychological trauma

across cultures (pp. 1–25). New York, NY: Springer.

Drozdek, B., & Wilson, J. P. (2004). Uncovering: Trauma-focused treatment

techniques with asylum seekers. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken

spirits: The treatment of traumtized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture

victims (pp. 243–276). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Drumm, R., Pittman, S., & Perry, S. (2001). Women of war: Emotional needs of

ethnic Albanians in refugee camps. Affilia, 16(4), 467–487. doi: 10.1177/

08861090122094370

Drum Publication Group. (2011). Transliteration word list. Retrieved 18 April, 2012,

from http://www.drumpublications.org/index.html

Edwards, R. (1998). A critical examination of the use of interpreters in the qualitative

process. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 24(1), 197–208. doi:

10.1080/1369183X.1998.9976626

Edwards, R., & Ribbens, J. (1998). Living on the edges: Public knowledge, private

lives, personal experience. In J. Ribbens & R. Edwards (Eds.), Feminist dilemmas

in qualitative research (pp. 1–23). London, England: Sage.

334

Eggen, P., & Kauchak, D. (1999). Educational psychology: Windows on classrooms

(4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Egharevba, I. (2001). Researching an-‘other’ minority ethnic community: Reflections

of a black female researcher on the intersection of race, gender and other power

positions on the research process. International Journal of Social Research

Methdology, 4(3), 225–241. doi: 10.1080/13645570010023760

Eisenbruch, M. (1983). ‘Wind illness’ or somatic depression? A case study in

psychiatric anthropology. British Journal of Psychiatry, 143(4), 323–326. doi:

10.1192/bjp.143.4.323

Eisenbruch, M. (1990). Classification of natural and supernatural causes of mental

distress: Development of a Mental Distress Explanatory Model Questionnaire.

Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 178(11), 712–719. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Eisenbruch, M. (1992). Toward a culturally sensitive DSM: Cultural bereavement in

Cambodian refugees and the traditional healer as taxonomist. Journal of Nervous

and Mental Disease, 180(1), 8–10. Retrieved from http://journals.

lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Eisenbruch, M., de Jong, J. T. V. M., & van de Put, W. (2004). Bringing order out of

chaos: A culturally competent approach to managing the problems of refugees

and victims of organized violence. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17(2), 123–131.

doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000022618.65406.e8

Eisenbruch, M., & Handelman, L. (1989). Development of an explanatory model of

illness schedule for Cambodian refugee patients. Journal of Refugee Studies,

2(2), 243–248. doi: 10.1093/jrs/2.2.243

Eitinger, L. (1959). The incidence of mental disorders among refugees in Norway.

Journal of Mental Science, 105, 326–338. doi: 10.1192/bjp.105.439.326

Eitinger, L. (1961). Pathology and the concentration camp syndrome. Archives of

General Psychiatry, 5(4), 371–379. Retrieved from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.

org

Ekblad, S., & Jaranson, J. M. (2004). Psychosocial rehabilitation. In J. P. Wilson & B.

Drozdek (Eds.), Broken spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers,

refugees, war and torture victims (pp. 609–636). New York, NY: Brunner-

Routledge.

335

Ekeh, C., & Smith, M. (2007). Minorities in Burma. Retrieved 4 December, 2010,

from Minority Rights Group International website: http://www.minorityrights.

org/3546/briefing-papers/minorities-in-burma.html

Ernst & Young. (2011). Independent review: Provision of humanitarian support

services in the Hunter region. Retrieved 18 July, 2011, from

http://www.immi.gov.au/media/publications/settle/

Escobar, J. I., & Gureje, O. (2007). Influence of cultural and social factors on the

epidemiology of idiopathic somatic complains and syndromes. Psychosomatic

Medicine, 69(9), 841–845. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31815b007e

Fadiman, A. (1997). The spirit catches you and you fall down: A Hmong child, her

American doctors, and the collision of two cultures. New York, NY: Farrar,

Straus and Giroux.

Fazel, M., Wheeler, J., & Danesh, J. (2005). Prevalence of serious mental disorder in

7000 refugees resettled in western countries: A systematic review. The Lancet,

365(9467), 1309–1314. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)61027-6

Fennelly, K. (2007). The “Healthy Migrant” effect. Minnesota Medicine, 90(3), 51–

53. Retrieved from www.minnesotamedicine.com

Fernando, G. A. (2005). Interventions for survivors of the tsunami disaster: Report

from Sri Lanka. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 18(3), 267–268. Retrieved from

http://au.wiley.com/WileyCDA/WileyTitle/productCd-JTS.html

Fernando, S. (2010). Mental health services in the UK: Lessons from transcultural

psychiatry. In D. Ingleby (Ed.), Forced migration and mental health: Rethinking

the care of refugees and displaced persons (pp. 183–192). New York, NY:

Springer.

Ferren, P. M. (1999). Comparing perceived self-efficacy among adolescent Bosnian

and Croatian refugees with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of

Traumatic Stress, 12(3), 405–420. doi: 10.1023/A:1024749118463

Fielding, N. (2001). Ethnography. In N. Gilbert (Ed.), Researching social life (2nd

ed., pp. 145–163). London, England: Sage.

Fine, M., & Gordon, S. M. (1992). Feminist transformations of/despite psychology. In

M. Fine (Ed.), Disruptive voices (pp. 1–25). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan

Press.

Fink, C. (2009). Living silence in Burma: Surviving under military rule (2nd ed.).

London, England: Zed Books.

336

Finlay, L. (2002). “Outing” the researcher: The provenance, process and practice of

reflexivity. Qualitative Health Research, 12(4), 531–545. doi: 10.1177/

104973202129120052

Flick, U. (2002). An introduction to qualitative research. London, England: Sage.

Foa, E. B., Keane, T. M., & Friedman, M. J. (2000). Effective treatments for PTSD.

New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Foa, E. B., Rothbaum, B. O., Riggs, D. S., & Murdock, T. B. (1991). Treatment of

posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive-

behavioral procedures and counseling. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 59(5), 715–723. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/

pubs/journals/ccp/index.aspx

Fontana, A., & Frey, J. H. (2005). The interview: From neutral stance to political

involvement. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of

qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 695–727). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Forum of Australian Services for Survivors of Torture and Trauma. (2006). Out of the

abyss: Australia’s program of assistance for survivors of torture and trauma.

Retrieved 23 June, 2011, from http://www.fasstt.org.au/home/index.php

Foucault, M. (1998). The history of sexuality Volume 1: An introduction. London,

England: Penguin.

Fox, G. L., & McBride Murry, V. (2000). Gender and families: Feminist perspectives

and family research. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 62(4), 1160–1172. doi:

10.1111/j.1741-3737.2000.01160.x

Fox, S. H., & Tang, S. (2000). The Sierra Leonean refugee experience: Traumatic

events and psychiatric sequelae. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 188(8),

490–495. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/

default.aspx

Frank, J. D., & Frank, J. B. (1991). Persuasion and healing: A comparative study of

psychotherapy (3rd ed.). Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.

Frank, J. D., & Frank, J. B. (1996). Demoralization and unexplained illness in two

cohorts of American soldiers overseas. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,

184(7), 445–446. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/

pages/default.aspx

Fraser, G. M. (1992). Quartered safe out here: A recollection of the war in Burma.

London, England: Harvill.

337

Freidman, M. J., Schnurr, P. P., Sengupta, A., Holmes, T., & Ashcraft, M. (2004).

The Hawaii Vietnam veterans project: Is minority status a risk factor for

posttraumatic stress disorder? The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,

192(1), 42–50. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/

default.aspx

Frye, B. A., & D’Avanzo, C. (1994). Themes in managing culturally defined illness in

the Cambodian refugee family. Journal of Community Health Nursing, 11(2), 89–

98. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/toc/hchn20/current

Fuertes, A. B. (2004). In their own words: Contextualizing the discourse of (war)

trauma and healing. Conflict Resolution Quarterly, 21(4), 491–501. doi:

10.1002/crq.77

Fuertes, A. B. (2007). Community-based warviews, resiliency and healing among the

internally displaced persons in Mindanao and the Karen refugees on the Thai-

Burmese border. (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations

and Theses database. (UMI No. 3289656)

Fuertes, A. B. (2010). Birds inside a cage: Metaphor for Karen refugees. Social

Alternatives, 29(1), 20–24. Retrieved from http://www.socialalternatives.com

Furler, J., Kokanovic, R., Dowrick, C., Newton, D., Gunn, J., & May, C. (2010).

Managing depression among ethnic communities: A qualitative study. Annals of

Family Medicine, 8(3), 231–236. doi: 10.1370/afm.1091

Gagnon, A., & Tuck, J. (2004). A systematic review of questionnaires measuring the

health of resettling refugee women. Health Care for Women International, 25(2),

111–149. doi: 10.1080/07399330490267503

Gaines, A. D. (1992). Ethnopsychiatry: The cultural construction of professional and

folk psychiatries. Albany: State University of New York.

Gallegos, D., Ellies, P., & Wright, J. (2008). Still there’s no food! Food insecurity in a

refugee population in Perth, Western Australia. Nutrition and Dietetics, 65(1),

78–83. Retrieved from http://www.ajnd.org.au

Geertz, C. (1979). From the native’s point of view: On the nature of anthropological

understanding. In P. Rabinow & W. M. Sullivan (Eds.), Interpretive social

science (pp. 225–241). Berkeley: University of California Press.

Gergen, M. (2008). Qualitative methods in feminist psychology. In C. Willig & W.

Stainton-Rogers (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research in psychology

(pp. 280–295). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

338

Gilbert, P., & Allan, S. (1998). The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in

depression: An exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological Medicine,

28(3), 585–598. Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/action/

displayJournal?jid=PSM

Giles, C., & Matthews, R. (2012). Diagnostic dilemmas: DSM-5 review and

development. InPsych, 34(1), 22–23. Retrieved from http://www.psychology.

org.au/publications/inpsych

Gilmore, R. D. (1898). A grammar of the Sgaw Karen. Retrieved 24 February, 2011,

from http://openlibrary.org/books/OL14010187M/A_grammar_of_the_Sgaw_

Karen

Glaser, B. G. (1969). The constant comparative method of qualitative analysis. In G.

J. McCall & J. L. Simmons (Eds.), Issues in participant observation (pp. 216–

228). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Glaser, B. G. (1978). Theoretical sensitivity. Mill Valley, CA: Sociology Press.

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies

for qualitative research. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Goffman, E. (1989). On fieldwork. Journal of Contemporary Ethnography, 18(2),

123–132. doi: 10.1177/089124189018002001

Gold, R. (1958). Roles in sociological field observation. Social Forces, 36(3), 217–

223.

Goldney, R. D., Fisher, L. J., Dal Grande, E., & Taylor, A. W. (2005). Changes in

mental health literacy about depression: South Australia, 1998 to 2004. Medical

Journal of Australia, 183(3), 134–137. Retrieved from https://www.mja.com.au

Gonsalves, C. J. (1992). Psychological states of the refugee process: A model for

therapeutic interventions. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice,

23(5), 382–389. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/pro/

index.aspx

Goodkind, J., Hang, P., & Yang, M. (2004). Hmong refugees in the United States: A

community-based advocacy and learning intervention. In K. E. Miller & L. M.

Rasco (Eds.), The mental health of refugees: Ecological approaches to healing

and adaptation (pp. 295–334). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Goodman, J. H. (2004). Coping with trauma and hardship among unaccompanied

refugee youths from Sudan. Qualitative Health Research, 14(9), 1177–1196.

Retrieved from http://qhr.sagepub.com

339

Goran, B. (2005). Life-threatening loss of function in refugee children: Another

expression of pervasive refusal syndrome? Clinical Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 10(3), 337–350. doi: 10.1177/1359104505053753

Gozdziak, E. M. (2004). Training refugee mental health providers: Ethnography as a

bridge to multicultural practice. Human Organization, 63(2), 203–210. Retrieved

from http://www.sfaa.net/ho

Griffin, C., & Bengry-Howell, A. (2008). Ethnography. In C. Willig & W. Stainton-

Rogers (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative research (pp. 15–31). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Griffiths, K. M., Nakane, Y., Christensen, H., Toshioka, K., Jorm, A. F., & Nakane,

H. (2006). Stigma in response to mental disorders: A comparison of Australia and

Japan. BioMed Central Psychiatry, 6(21), 1–12. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-6-21

Grills, S. (1998). Doing ethnographic research: Fieldwork settings. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Griner, D., & Smith, T. B. (2006). Culturally adapted mental health intervention: A

meta-analytic review. Psychotherapy Theory, Research, Practice and Training,

43(4), 531–548. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/

pst/index.aspx

Grof, S., & Grof, C. (1989). Spiritual emergency: When personal transformation

becomes a crisis. New York, NY: GP Putnam’s Sons.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation

processes: Implications for affect, relationships and well-being. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362. Retrieved from

http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/psp/index.aspx

Grove, C. L., & Torbiorn, I. (1985). A new conceptualization of intercultural

adjustment and the goals of training. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 9(2), 205–233. doi: 10.1016/0147-1767(85)90008-2

Grove, N., Zwi, A. B., & Allotey, P. (2007). Other of refugees: Social exclusion and

public health. In S. Douglas, S. Earle, S. Handsleym, C. E. Lloyd & S. Spurr

(Eds.), A reader in promoting public health: Challenge and controversy (pp.

213–224). London, England: Sage.

Guarnaccia, P. J., Lewis-Fernandez, R., & Marano, M. R. (2003). Toward a Puerto

Rican popular nosology: Nervios and ataque de nervios. Culture, Medicine and

Psychiatry, 27(3), 339–366. doi: 10.1023/A:1025303315932

340

Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (2005). Paradigmatic controversies, contradictions, and

emerging confluences. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage

handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 191–215). Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

Gudykunst, W. B., & Bond, M. H. (1999). Intergroup relations across cultures. In J.

W. Berry, M. H. Segall & C. Kagitcibasi (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural

psychology: Social behavior and applications (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 119–161).

Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Guerin, B., Guerin, P., Diiriye, R., & Omar Yates, S. (2004). Somali conceptions and

expectations concerning mental health: Some guidelines for mental health

professionals. New Zealand Journal of Psychology, 33(2), 59–67. Retrieved from

http://www.psychology.org.nz/NZ_Journal

Guerin, P., & Guerin, B. (2007). Research with refugee communities: Going around

in circles with methodology. The Australian Community Psychologist, 19(1),

150–162. Retrieved from http://www.groups.psychology.org.au/ccom/

publications

Gureje, O. (2004). What can we learn from a cross-national study of somatic distress?

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56(4), 409–412. doi: 10.1016/S0022-

3999(03)00623-8

Gureje, O., Simon, G. E., Ustan, T. B., & Goldberg, D. P. (1997). Somatization in

cross-cultural perspective: A World Health Organization study in primary care.

Amercian Journal of Psychiatry, 154(7), 989–995. Retrieved from

http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/journal.aspx?journalid=13

Guribye, E., Sandal, G. M., & Oppedal, B. (2011). Communal proactive coping

strategies among Tamil refugees in Norway: A case study in a naturalistic setting.

International Journal of Mental Health Systems, 5(9), 1–13. doi: 10.1186/1752-

4458-5-9

Hagengimana, A., & Hinton, D. E. (2009). Ihahamuka, a Rwandan syndrom of

response to the genocide: Blocked flow, spirit assault and shortness of breath. In

D. E. Hinton & B. J. Good (Eds.), Culture and panic disorder (pp. 205–229).

Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

341

Halcon, L. L., Robertson, C., Savik, K., Johnson, D. R., Spring, M. A., Butcher, J. N.,

… Jaranson, J. M. (2004). Trauma and coping in Somali and Oromo refugee

youth. Journal of Adolescent Health, 35(1), 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.

2003.08.005

Hale, S. (2010). Court interpreting. The need to raise the bar: Court interpreters as

specialized experts. In M. Coultard & A. Johnson (Eds.), The Routledge

handbook of forensic linguistics (pp. 440–454). New York, NY: Routledge.

Hallowell, A. I. (2010). Culture and experience. In R. A. Le Vine (Ed.),

Psychological anthropology: A reader on self in culture (pp. 30–52). Chichester,

England: Wiley-Blackwell.

Hammarstrom, A. (1999). Why feminism in public health? Scandanavian Journal of

Public Health, 27(4), 241–244. doi: 10.1177/14034948990270040601

Hammersley, M. (1992). What’s wrong with ethnography? London, England:

Routledge.

Handelman, L., & Yeo, G. (1996). Using explanatory models to understand chronic

symptoms of Cambodian refugees. Family Medicine, 28(4), 271–276. Retrieved

from http://www.stfm.org/fmhub

Handwerker, W. P. (2001). Quick ethnography. Lanham, MD: AltaMira Press.

Harding, S. (1991). Whose science? Whose knowledge? Thinking from women’s lives.

New York, NY: Cornell University Press.

Hardy, K. V., & Laszloffy, T. A. (1995). The cultural genogram: Key to training

culturally competent family therapists. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,

21(3), 227–237. doi: 10.1111/j.1752-0606.1995.tb00158.x

Harris, M. (1976). History and significance of the emic-etic distinction. Annual

Review of Anthropology, 5, 329–350. doi: 10.1146/annurev.an.05.100176.

001553

Harrison, N. (2003). Grounded theory or grounded data? The production of power and

knowledge in ethnographic research. The Australian Journal of Indigenous

Education, 32, 101–106. Retrieved from http://www.atsis.uq.edu.au/ajie

Hartnett, S. J., & Engels, J. D. (2005). “Aria in time of war”: Investigative poetry and

the politics of witnessing. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage

handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 1043–1067). Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

342

Hartsock, N. (1983). Money, sex, and power: Toward a feminist historical

materialism. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press.

Hatoss, A., & Huijser, H. (2010). Gendered barriers to educational opportunities:

Resettlement of Sudanese refugees in Australia. Gender and Education, 22(2),

147–160. doi: 10.1080/09540250903560497

Hayes, S. C. (2002). Buddhism and acceptance and commitment therapy. Cognitive

and Behavioral Practice, 9(1), 58–66. doi: 10.1016/S1077-7229(02)80041-4

Helgeson, V. S., & Cohen, S. (1996). Social support and adjustment to cancer:

Reconciling descriptive, correlational, and intervention research. Health

Psychology, 15(2), 135–148. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.15.2.135

Henningson, P., Jakobsen, T., Schiltenwolf, M., & Weiss, M. G. (2005). Somatization

revisited: Diagnosis and perceived causes of common mental disorders. Journal

of Nervous and Mental Disease, 193(2), 85–92. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Hennink, M. M. (2008). Language and communication in cross-cultural qualitative

research. In P. Liamputtong (Ed.), Doing cross-cultural research: Ethical and

methodological perspectives (pp. 21–33). In Social Indicators Research Series.

London, England: Springer.

Higginbotham, N., & Marsella, A. J. (1988). International consultation and the

homogenization of psychiatry in Southeast Asia. Social Science and Medicine,

27(5), 553–561. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.01.024

Higgins, C. (2009). Offshore refugees: Settlement experiences of humanitarian

arrivals in Victoria. Melbourne, Australia: Parliament of Victoria, Department of

Parliamentary Services, Parliamentary Library Research Service.

Hinshaw, S. P., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Stigma and mental disorder: Conceptions of

illness, public attitudes, personal disclosure, and social policy. Development and

Psychopathology, 12(4), 555–598. Retrieved from http://journals.

cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=DPP

Hinton, A. (2007). Terror and trauma in the Cambodian genocide. In L. J. Kirmayer,

R. Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological,

clinical and cultural perspectives (pp. 433–450). New York, NY: Cambridge

University Press.

343

Hinton, D. E., Hsia, C., Um, K., & Otto, M. W. (2003). Anger-associated panic

attacks in Cambodian refugees with PTSD: A multiple baseline examination of

clinical data. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(6), 647–654. doi:

10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00035-9

Hinton, D. E., & Lewis-Fernandez, R. (2010a). The cross-cultural validity of

posttraumatic stress disorder: Implications for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety,

28(9), 1–19. doi: 10.1002/da.20753

Hinton, D. E., & Lewis-Fernandez, R. (2010b). Idioms of distress among trauma

survivors: Subtypes and clinical utility. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34(2),

209–218. doi: 10.1007/s11013-010-9175-x

Hinton, D. E., & Otto, M. W. (2006). Symptom presentation and symptom meaning

among traumatized Cambodian refugees: Relevance to a somatically focused

cognitive-behavior therapy. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 13(4), 249–260.

Retrieved from http://www.journals.elsevier.com/cognitive-and-behavioral-

practice

Hinton, D. E., Pich, V., Hofmann, S. G., & Otto, M. W. (2011). Acceptance and

mindfulness techniques as applied to refugee and ethnic minority populations

with PTSD: Examples from “culturally adapted CBT”. Cognitive and Behavioral

Practice. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.09.001

Hinton, D. E., Pich, V., Marques, L., Nickerson, A., & Pollack, M. H. (2010). Khyal

attacks: A key idiom of distress among traumatized Cambodian refugees.

Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34(2), 244–278. doi: 10.1007/s11013-010-

9174-y

Hinton, D. E., Rasmussen, A., Nou, K., Pollack, M. H., & Good, M. (2009). Anger,

PTSD, and the nuclear family: A study of Cambodian refugees. Social Science

and Medicine, 69(9), 1387–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.08.018

Hinton, D. E., Safren, S. A., & Pollack, M. H. (2006). Cognitive-behavior therapy for

Vietnamese refugees with PTSD and comorbid panic attacks. Cognitive and

Behavioral Practice, 13(4), 271–281. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2006.04.008

Hofstede, G. (2001). Culture’s consequences. Comparing values, behaviors,

institutions and organizations across nations (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

344

Hollan, D. (2010). Cross-cultural differences in the self. In R. A. Le Vine (Ed.),

Psychological anthropology: A reader on self in culture (pp. 295–308).

Chichester, England: Wiley-Blackwell.

Hollifield, M. (2005). Taking measure of war trauma. The Lancet, 365(9467), 1283–

1284. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)61003-3

Hollifield, M., Warner, T. D., Lian, N., Krakow, B., Jenkins, J. H., Kesler, J., …

Westermeyer, J. (2002). Measuring trauma and health status in refugees: A

critical review. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288(5), 611–621.

doi: 10.1001/jama.288.5.611

Holtz, T. H. (1998). Refugee trauma versus torture trauma: A retrospective controlled

cohort study of Tibetan refugees. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease,

186(1), 24–34. Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/

default.aspx

Horstmann, A. (2010). Confinement and mobility: Transnational ties and religious

networking among Baptist Karen at the Thailand-Burma border (MMG Working

Paper 10-16). Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from Max Planck Institute for the Study

of Religious and Ethnic Diversity website: http://www.mmg.

mpg.de/publications/working-papers/2010/

Horwitz, A. V. (2010). An overview of sociological perspectives on the definitions,

causes, and responses to mental health and illness. In T. L. Scheid & T. N. Brown

(Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and

systems (2nd ed., pp. 6–19). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Hou, F., & Beiser, M. (2006). Learning the language of a new country: A ten-year

study of English acquisiton by South-East Asian refugees in Canada.

International Migration, 44(1), 135–165. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2435.2006.

00358.x

House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Boston, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Houston, S. (2001). Beyond social constructionism: Critical realism and social work.

British Journal of Social Work, 31(6), 845–861. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/31.6.845

Huang, Y.-T., & Phillips, C. B. (2009). Bridging the barrier to using telephone

interpreters in general practice. Australian Family Physician, 38(6), 443–446.

Retrieved from http://www.racgp.org.au/afp

345

Hubbard, J., & Miller, K. E. (2004). Evaluating ecological mental health interventions

in refugee communities. In K. E. Miller & L. M. Rasco (Eds.), The mental health

of refugees: Ecological approaches to healing and adaptation (pp. 337–374).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hubbard, J., & Pearson, N. (2004). Sierra Leonean refugees in Guinea: Addressing

the mental health effects of massive community violence. In K. E. Miller & L. M.

Rasco (Eds.), The mental health of refugees: Ecological approaches to healing

and adaptation (pp. 95–132). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hugman, R., Pittaway, E., & Bartolomei, B. (2011). When ‘do no harm’ is not

enough: The ethics of research with refugees and other vulnerable groups. British

Journal of Social Work, 41(7), 1271–1287. doi: 10.1093/bjsw/bcr013

Human Rights Watch. (2011). World report 2011: Burma. Retrieved 18 April, 2012,

from http://www.hrw.org/world-report-2011/burma

Hwang, W. (2006). The psychotherapy of adaptation and modification framework:

Applications to Asian Americans. American Psychologist, 61(7), 702–715. doi:

10.1037/0003-066X.61.7.702

Hynes, T. (2003). The issue of ‘trust’ or ‘mistrust’ in research with refugees:

Choices, cavaets, and considerations for researchers (Working Paper No. 98).

Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees

website: http://www.unhcr.org/cgi-bin/texis/vtx/home/

opendocPDFViewer.html?docid=3fcb5cee1&query=Hynes%20The%20issue%2

0of%20trust

Ingleby, D. (2010). Editor’s Introduction. In D. Ingleby (Ed.), Forced migration and

mental health: Rethinking the care of refugees and displaced persons (pp. 1–27).

New York, NY: Springer.

International Organization for Migration. (2011a). Global estimates and trends.

Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from http://www.iom.int/jahia/Jahia/pid/241

International Organization for Migration. (2011b). IOM resettles 90,000 refugees

from Thai camps [Press briefing]. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from

http://www.iom.int/jahia/Jahia/media/press-briefing-notes/pbnAS/cache/offonce/l

ang/en?entryId=29806

346

International Service for Human Rights, Committee on the Elimination of

Discrimination against Women. (2008). Concluding observations of the

committee on the elimination of discrimination against women: Myanmar. In

Human Rights Monitor Series. Retrieved 4 December 2010, from

http://www.ishr.ch/treaty-body-monitor/cedaw

Jacobs, G. A. (2007). The development and maturation of humanitarian psychology.

American Psychologist, 62(8), 929–941. Retrieved from

http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/amp/index.aspx

James, K. (2010). Domestic violence within refugee families: Intersecting patriarchal

culture and the refugee experience. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Family Therapy, 31(3), 275–284. Retrieved from http://www.anzjft.com/

pages/index.php

Jaranson, J. M., & Bamford, P. (1987). Program models for the mental health

treatment of refugees. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota, Mental Health

Technical Assistance Center, Refugee Assistance Program.

Jenkins, R. (2002). Pierre Bourdieu (Rev. ed.). London, England: Routledge.

John-Langba, J. (2004). The relationship of sexual and gender-based violence (SGBV)

to sexual-risk behaviors among refugee women in Botswana: The mediating role

of depression. (Doctoral dissertation). Available from ProQuest Dissertations and

Theses database. (UMI No. 3188275)

Kagitcibasi, C. (1999). Individualism and collectivism. In J. W. Berry, M. H. Segall

& C. Kagitcibasi (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology: Social behavior

and applications (2nd ed., Vol. 3, pp. 1–49). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Kahn, R. L., & Antonucci, T. C. (1980). Convoys over the life course: Attachment,

roles and social support. In P. B. Baltes & O. Brim (Eds.), Life span development

and behavior (pp. 253–286). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Kalnin, E. C. (2010). Displaced Burmese in Thailand: Refugee policies and impact on

access and rights, 1988–2008. (Master’s thesis). Available from ProQuest

Dissertations and Theses database. (UMI No. MR67515)

Kamberelis, G., & Dimitriadis, G. (2005). Focus groups: Strategic articulations of

pedagogy, politics and inquiry. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage

handbook of qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 887–907). Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

347

Kapborg, I., & Bertero, C. (2002). Using an interpreter in qualitative interviews: Does

it threaten validity? Nursing Inquiry, 9(1), 52–56. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-

1800.2002.00127.x

Kaplan, I., & Webster, K. (2003). Refugee women and settlement: Gender and mental

health. In P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of refugees: Public health perspectives

from crisis to settlement (pp. 104–122). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University

Press.

Karasz, A. (2005). Cultural differences in conceptual models of depression. Social

Science and Medicine, 60(7), 1625–1635. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2004.08.

011

Kardiner, A. (1947). War stress and neurotic illness. New York, NY: International

Universities Press.

Karen Community Housing Cooperative Limited. (2011, Winter). Karen Settlement

Grant Program – The Hills Holroyd Parramatta MRC: Karen Community and

Settlement Update, 1(2), 3. Retrieved 21 May, 2012, from

http://www.docstoc.com/docs/83878397/Karen-Community-_-Settlement-

Update-E-Newsletter-Winter-2011

Karen Human Rights Group. (2005). KHRG sketch map of Burma. Retrieved 18

April, 2012, from http://www.khrg.org/maps/2006maps/Burma2006.jpg

Karen Human Rights Group. (2008). Village agency: Rural rights and resistance in a

militarized Karen state. (n.p.): Author.

Karen Human Rights Group, & Delang, C. O. (2000). Suffering in silence, the human

rights nightmare of the Karen people of Burma. Parkland, FL: Universal.

Karen National Union. (2011). Aims, policies, and programmes. Retrieved 2 March,

2011, from http://www.karen.org/knu/knu.htm

Karen Women’s Organisation. (2004). Shattering silences: Karen women speak out

about the Burmese military regime’s use of rape as a strategy of war in Karen

state. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from http://www.peacewomen.org/

portal_initiative_initiative.php?id=119

Karen Women’s Organisation. (2007). State of terror: The ongoing rape, murder,

torture and forced labour suffered by women living under the Burmese military

regime in Karen state. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from

http://karenwomen.org/reports

348

Kenardy, J. (2000). The current status of psychological debriefing. British Medical

Journal, 321(7268), 1032–1033. doi: 10.1136/bmj.321.7268.1032

Kenny, P., & Lockwood-Kenny, K. (2011). A mixed blessing: Karen resettlement to

the United States. Journal of Refugee Studies, 24(2), 217–238. doi: 10.1093/

jrs/fer009

Kett, M. (2005). Displaced populations and long term humanitarian assistance. British

Medical Journal, 331(7508), 98–100. doi: 10.1136/bmj.331.7508.98

Ketzer, E., & Crescenzi, A. (2002). Addressing the psychosocial and mental health

needs of Tibetan refugees in India. In J. T. V. M. de Jong (Ed.), Trauma, war and

violence: Public mental health in socio-cultural context (pp. 283–315). New

York, NY: Kluwer Academic / Plenum.

Keyes, E. F. (2000). Mental health status in refugees: An integrative review of current

research. Issues in Mental Health Nursing, 21(4), 397–410. doi: 10.1080/

016128400248013

Khaing, M. M. (1984). The world of Burmese women. London, England: Zed Books.

Khawaja, N. G., White, K. M., Schweitzer, R., & Greenslade, J. (2008). Difficulties

and coping strategies of Sudanese refugees: A qualitative approach.

Transcultural Psychiatry, 45(3), 489–512. doi: 10.1177/1363461508094678

Kibreab, G. (1993). The myth of dependency among camp refugees in Somalia 1979–

1989. Journal of Refugee Studies, 6(4), 321–349. doi: 10.1093/jrs/6.4.321

Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., & Taylor, S. E. (2008). Culture and social support.

American Psychologist, 63(6), 518–526. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/

pubs/journals/amp/index.aspx

Kinzie, J. D., & Friedman, J. M. (2004). Psychopharmacology for refugee and

asylum-seeker patients. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken spirits: The

treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture victims (pp.

579–600). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Kirmayer, L. J. (1989). Cultural variations in the response to psychiatric disorders and

emotional distress. Social Science and Medicine, 29(3), 327–339. doi:

10.1016/0277-9536(89)90281-5

Kirmayer, L. J., Groleau, D., Looper, K. J., & Dominice Dao, M. (2004). Explaining

medically unexplained symptoms. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49(10), 663–

672. Retrieved from http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/sections/0

349

Kissane, D. W. (2001). Demoralisation – A useful conceptualisation of existential

distress in the elderly. Australasian Journal on Ageing, 20(3), 110–111. doi:

10.1111/j.1741-6612.2001.tb01770.x

Kissane, D. W., Clarke, D. M., & Street, A. F. (2001). Demoralisation syndrome – A

relevant psychiatric diagnosis for palliative care. Journal of Palliative Care,

17(1), 12–21. Retrieved from http://www.liebertpub.com/jpm

Kitto, S. C., Chesters, J., & Grbich, C. (2008). Quality in qualitative research.

Medical Journal of Australia., 188(4), 243–246. Retrieved from

https://www.mja.com.au

Kitzinger, J. (1994). The methodology of focus groups: The importance of interaction

between research participants. Sociology of Health and Illness, 16(1), 103–121.

doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.ep11347023

Kleinman, A. (1987). Anthropology and psychiatry. The role of culture in cross-

cultural research on illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 151(4), 447–454. doi:

10.1192/bjp.151.4.447

Kleinman, A. (1977). Depression, somatization and the “new cross-cultural

psychiatry”. Social Science and Medicine, 11(1), 3–10. doi: 10.1016/0037-

7856(77)90138-X

Kleinman, A. (1980). Patients and healers in the context of culture: An exploration of

the border-land between anthropology, medicine and psychiatry. Berkeley:

University of California Press.

Kleinman, A. (1995). Writing at the margin: Discourse between anthropology and

medicine. Berkeley: University of California Press.

Kleinman, A. (1999). Social violence: Research questions on local experiences and

global responses. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(11), 978–979. Retrieved

from http://archpsyc.ama-assn.org

Kleinman, A., & Becker, A. E. (1998). “Sociosomatics”: The contributions of

anthropology to psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatic Medicine, 60(4), 389–

393. Retrieved from http://www.psychosomaticmedicine.org

Kleinman, A., & Kleinman, J. (1985). Somatization: The inter-connections in Chinese

society among culture, depressive experiences, and the meanings of pain. In A.

Kleinman & B. Good (Eds.), Culture and depression: Studies in the anthropology

and cross-cultural psychiatry of affect and disorder (pp. 429–290). Berkeley:

University of California Press.

350

Klerman, G. L. (1978). The evolution of a scientific nosology. In C. J. Shershow

(Ed.), Schizophrenia: Science and practice (pp. 99–121). Cambridge, MA:

Harvard University Press.

Kobori, E., Visrutaratna, S., Maeda, Y., Wongchai, S., Kada, A., Ono-Kihara, M., …

Kihara, M. (2009). Methamphetamine use and correlates in two villages of the

highland ethnic Karen minority in northern Thailand: A cross sectional study.

BioMed Central International Health and Human Rights, 9(11). doi: 10.1186/

1472-698X-9-11

Koenig, H. G. (2009). Research on religion, spirituality, and mental health: A review.

The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54(5), 283–291. Retrieved from

http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/sections/0

Kohrt, B. A., & Hruschka, D. J. (2010). Nepali concepts of psychological trauma: The

role of idioms of distress, ethnopsychology and ethnophysiology in alleviating

suffering and preventing stigma. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34(2), 322–

352. doi: 10.1007/s11013-010-9170-2

Konner, M. (2007). Trauma, adapatation, and resilience: A cross-cultural and

evolutionary perspective. In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.),

Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical and cultural perspectives

(pp. 300–338). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Koppe, S. (2009). Neurosis: Aspects of its conceptual development in the nineteenth

century. History of Psychiatry, 20(1), 27–46. doi: 10.1177/

0957154X08092426

Krystal, H. (1968). Massive psychic trauma. New York, NY: International

Universities Press.

Kuper, A., Lingard, L., & Levinson, W. (2008). Critically appraising qualitative

research. British Medical Journal, 337, 687–689. doi: 10.1136/bmj.a1035

Laban, C. J., Gernaat, H. B. P. E., Komproe, I. H., van der Tweel, I., & de Jong, J. T.

V. M. (2005). Postmigration living problems and common psychiatric disorders

in Iraqi asylum seekers in the Netherlands. Journal of Nervous and Mental

Disease, 193(12), 825–832. doi:10.1097/01.nmd.0000188977.44657.1d

Lambert, C., & Pickering, S. (2003). Domestic violence on the Thai-Burma border:

International humanitarian rights implications. Forced Migration Review, (17),

41–42. Retrieved from http://www.fmreview.org

351

Lang, I. A., Llewellyn, D. J., Hubbard, R. E., Langa, K. M., & Melzer, D. (2011).

Income and midlife peak in common mental disorder prevalence. Psychological

Medicine, 41(7), 1365–1372. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710002060

Lang, H. (2001). The repatriation predicament of Burmese refugees in Thailand: A

preliminary analysis. (New Issues in Refugee Research: Working Paper No. 46).

Retrieved 21 May, 2012, 2011, from United Nations High Commissioner for

Refugees website: www.unhcr.org/3b7d24214.pdf

Langford, C. P., Bowsher, J., Maloney, J. P., & Lillis, P. P. (1997). Social support: A

conceptual analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 25(1), 95–100. doi:

10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.1997025095

Larson, A., van Kooten-Prasad, M., Manderson, L., & Frkovic, I. (1998). Assessing

needs for mental health in culturally and linguistically diverse communities: A

qualitative approach. Melbourne, Australia: University of Melbourne, Australian

Transcultural Mental Health Network.

Lau, M. A., & McMain, S. F. (2005). Integrating mindfulness meditation with

cognitive and behavioural therapies: The challenge of combining acceptance- and

change-based strategies. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 50(13), 863–869.

Retrieved from http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/sections/0

Lautze, S., & Hammock, J. (1996). Coping with crisis: Coping with aid, capacity

building, coping mechanisms and dependency, linking relief and development.

Retrieved 18 April, 2011, from Forced Migration Online website:

http://repository.forcedmigration.org/show_metadata.jsp?pid=fmo:1685

Lawler-Row, K. A., & Elliott, J. (2009). The role of religious activity and spirituality

in the health and well-being of older adults. Journal of Health Psychology, 14(1),

43–52. doi: 10.1177/1359105308097944

Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York, NY:

Springer.

Lebra, T. (2010). Shame and guilt in Japan. In R. A. Le Vine (Ed.), Psychological

anthropology: A reader on self in culture (pp. 102–111). Chichester, England:

Wiley-Blackwell.

Lee, C. C., & Glaister, I. (2008). Burmese asylum seekers in Thailand: Still nowhere

to turn. Forced Migration Review, 30, 33–34. Retrieved from

http://www.fmreview.org/burma.htm

352

Lemelson, R., Kirmayer, L. J., & Barad, M. (2007). Trauma in context: Integrating

biological, clinical, and cultural perspectives. In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson &

M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical and

cultural perspectives (pp. 451–474). New York, NY: Cambridge University

Press.

Levine, M. (2006). Contributions of modern psychological methods to the attainment

of Buddhist goals. In D. K. Nauriyal, M. S. Drummond & Y. B. Lal (Eds.),

Buddhist thought and applied psychological research: Transcending the

boundaries (pp. 491–501). New York, NY: Routledge.

Lewig, K., Arney, F., & Salveron, M. (2010). Challenges to parenting in a new

culture: Implications for child and family welfare. Evaluation and Program

Planning, 33(3), 324–332. doi: 10.1016/j.evalprogplan.2009.05.002

Lewis-Fernandez, R., Gorritz, M., Raggio, G. A., Pelaez, C., Chen, H., & Guarnaccia,

P. J. (2010). Association of trauma-related disorders and dissociation with four

idioms of distress among Latino psychiatric outpatients. Culture, Medicine and

Psychiatry, 34(2), 219–243. doi: 10.1007/s11013-010-9177-8

Lewis-Fernandez, R., & Kleinman, A. (1994). Culture, personality, and

psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(1), 67–71. Retrieved

from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/abn/index.aspx

Li, J. (2008). Ethical challenges in participant observation: A reflection on

ethnographic fieldwork. The Qualitative Report, 13(1), 100–115. Retrieved from

http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR

Liamputtong, P. (2008). Doing research in a cross-cultural context: Methodological

and ethnical challenges. In P. Liamputtong (Ed.), Doing cross-cultural research:

Ethical and methodological perspectives (pp. 3–20). In Social Indicators

Research Series. London, England: Springer.

Liamputtong, P. (2009). Qualitative research methods (3rd ed.). Melbourne,

Australia: Oxford University Press.

Liamputtong, P. (2010). Performing qualitative cross-cultural research. Cambridge,

England: Cambridge University Press.

Liamputtong, P., & Ezzy, D. (2005). Qualitative research methods (2nd ed.).

Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

353

Liamputtong Rice, P. (1999a). How do perceptions and experiences from a homeland

influence perceptions of mental illness in a new country? The case of Cambodian

young people. In P. Liamputtong Rice (Ed.), Living in a new country:

Understanding migrants’ health (pp. 213–226). Melbourne, Australia: Ausmed

Publications.

Liamputtong Rice, P. (1999b). Multiculturalism and the health of immigrants: What

public health issues do immigrants face when they move to a new country? In P.

Liamputtong Rice (Ed.), Living in a new country: Understanding migrants’

health (pp. 1–24). Melbourne, Australia: Ausmed Publications.

Lifton, R. J. (1967). Death in life: Survivors of Hiroshima. New York, NY: Simon

and Schuster.

Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Lindholm, C. (2005). An anthropology of emotion. In C. Casey & R. B. Edgerton

(Eds.), A companion to psychology anthropology: Modernity and psychocultural

change (pp. 30–47). Oxford, England: Blackwell.

Lloyd, K. R., Jacob, K. S., Patel, V., Louis, L. S., Bhugra, D., & Mann, A. H. (1998).

The development of the Short Explanatory Model Interview (SEMI) and its use

among primary-care attenders with common mental disorders. Psychological

Medicine, 28(5), 1231–1237. Retrieved from http://journals.

cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=PSM

Lock, M. (1993). Cultivating the body: Anthropology and epistemologies of bodily

practice and knowledge. Annual Review of Anthropology, 22, 133–155. Retrieved

from http://www.annualreviews.org/journal/anthro

Lopes Cardozo, B., Kaiser, R., Gotway, C. A., & Agini, F. (2003). Mental health,

social functioning and feelings of hatred and revenge of Kosovar Albanians one

year after the war in Kosovo. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 16(4), 351–360. doi:

10.1001/jama.284.5.569

Lopes Cardozo, B., Talley, L., Burton, A., & Crawford, C. (2004). Karenni refugees

living in Thai-Burmese border camps: Traumatic experiences, mental health

outcomes, and social functioning. Social Science and Medicine, 58(12), 2637–

2644. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.09.024

354

Lund, C., Breen, A., Flisher, A. J., Kakuma, R., Corrigal, J., Joska, J. A., … Patel, V.

(2010). Poverty and common mental disorders in low and middle income

countries: A systematic review. Social Science and Medicine, 71(3), 517–528.

doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.04.027

Luszczynska, A., Benight, C. C., & Cieslak, R. (2009). Self-efficacy and health-

related outcomes of collective trauma: A systematic review. European

Psychologist, 14(1), 51–62. Retrieved from http://www.efpa.eu/journal

Lutz, C. (1985). Depression and the translation of emotional worlds. In A. Kleinman

& B. J. Good (Eds.), Culture and depression: Studies in the anthropology and

cross-cultural psychiatry of affect and disorder (pp. 63–100). Berkeley:

University of California Press.

Luxemburger, C., White, N. J., ter Kuile, F., Singh, H. M., Allier-Frachon, I., Ohn,

M., … Nosten, F. (2003). Beri-beri: The major cause of infant mortality in Karen

refugees. Transactions of the Royal Society of Tropical Medicine and Hygiene,

97(2), 251–255. Retrieved from http://www.elsevier.com/wps/find/

journaldescription.cws_home/681019/description#description

MacFarlane, A., Glynn, L. G., Mosinkie, P. I., & Murphy, A. W. (2008). Responses

to language barriers in consultations with refugees and asylum seekers: A

telephone survey of Irish general practitioners. BioMed Central Family Practice,

9, 68–73. doi: 10.1186/1471-2296-9-68

Macleod, C., & Bhatia, S. (2008). Postcolonialism and psychology. In C. Willig & W.

Stainton-Rogers (Eds.), The Sage handbook of qualitative psychology (pp. 576–

589). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mae Tao Clinic. (2011). Health services: Surgical services (OPD/IPD). Retrieved 29

September, 2011, from http://maetaoclinic.com/services/health-services/

surgical-service-opdipd.html

Maher, S. (2006). Community adult education based settlement programs for newly

arrived African women on Humanitarian and Women at Risk visas. (Churchill

Trust Fellowship Report). Retrieved 3 March, 2011, from www.churchilltrust.

com.au/site_media/fellows/MAHER_Sara_2006.pdf

Mahn, M., Maung, C., Shwe Oo, E. K., Smith, L., Lee, C. I., Whichard, E., … Lee, J.

J. (2008). Multi-level partnerships to promote health services among internally

displaced in eastern Burma. Global Public Health, 3(2), 165–186. doi:

10.1080/17441690801942821

355

Malinowski, B. (1922). Argonauts of the Western Pacific. London, England:

Routledge.

Manderson, L., & Allotey, P. (2003). Cultural politics and clinical competence in

Australian health services. Anthropology & Medicine, 10(1), 71–85. doi:

10.1080/13648470301266

Manicas, P. (1998). A realist social science. In M. Archer, R. Bhaskar, A. Collier, T.

Lawson & A. Norrie (Eds.), Critical realism: Essential readings (pp. 313–408).

London, England: Routledge.

Manson, K. (2010). Bibliography of Karen linguistics. Retrieved 22 February, 2011,

from http://latrobe.academia.edu/KenManson/Papers/144394/A_bibliography

_of_Karen_linguistics

Mares, P., & Allotey, P. (2003). Controlling compassion: The media, refugees, and

asylum seekers. In P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of refugees: Public health

perspectives from crisis to settlement (pp. 212–227). Melbourne, Australia:

Oxford University Press.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for

cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/rev/index.aspx

Marmot, M. (2005). Social determinants of health inequalities. The Lancet, 365

(9464), 1099–1104. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)71146-6

Marsella, A. J. (1994). Ethnocultural diversity and international refugees: Challenges

for the global community. In A. J. Marsella, T. Bornemann, S. Ekblad & J. Orley

(Eds.), Amidst peril and pain: The mental health and well-being of the world’s

refugees (pp. 341–364). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Marsella, A. J., Friedman, M. J., Gerrity, E. T., & Scurfield, R. M. (1996).

Ethnocultural aspects of PTSD: Some closing thoughts. In A. J. Marsella, M. J.

Friedman, E. T. Gerrity & R. M. Scurfield (Eds.), Ethnocultural aspects of

posttraumatic stress disorder: Issues, research and clinical applications (pp.

529–538). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Marsella, A. J., Friedman, M. J., & Spain, E. H. (1996). Ethnocultural aspects of

PTSD: An overview of issues and research directions. In A. J. Marsella, M. J.

Friedman, E. T. Gerrity & R. M. Scurfield (Eds.), Ethnocultural aspects of

posttraumatic stress disorder: Issues, research, and clinical applications (pp.

105–129). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

356

Marshall, G. N., Schell, T. L., Elliott, M. N., Berthold, S. M., & Chun, C. A. (2005).

Mental health of Cambodian refugees two decades after resettlement in the

United States. Journal of the American Medical Association, 294(5), 571–579.

doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.571

Marshall, H. I. (1997). The Karen people of Burma: A study in anthropology and

ethnology. Bangkok, Thailand: White Lotus.

Marshall, M. G., & Gurr, T. R. (2003). A global survey of armed conflicts, self-

determination movements, and democracy. In Peace and Conflict Series.

University of Maryland, Center for International Development and Conflict

Management, Integrated Network for Societal Conflict Research. Retrieved 18

April, 2012, from http://www.systemicpeace.org/peace.htm

Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York, NY: Harper.

Mayer, J. (2007). “In God I have put my trust”: Refugees and religion. Refugee

Survey Quarterly, 26(2), 6–10. Retrieved from http://rsq.oxfordjournals.org

Mayou, R., Ehlers, A., & Hobbs, M. (2000). Psychological debriefing for road traffic

victims: Three-year follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. British Journal of

Psychiatry, 176(6), 589–593. doi: 10.1192/bjp.176.6.589

McCabe, R., & Priebe, S. (2004). Explanatory models of illness in schizophrenia:

Comparison of four ethnic groups. British Medical Journal, 185(1), 25–30. doi:

10.1192/bjp.185.1.25

McFarlane, A. C. (2004). Assessing PTSD and comorbidity: Issues in differential

diagnosis. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken spirits: The treatment of

traumatized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture victims (pp. 81–103). New

York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

McKay, S. (Ed.). (2009). Refugees, recent migrants and employment: Challenging

barriers and exploring pathways. Abingdon, VA: Routledge.

McKeon, P., & Carrick, S. (1991). Public attitudes to depression: A national survey.

Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 8(2), 116–121. Retrieved from

http://www.irishpsychiatry.ie/Members/MembersInformationTools/irishjournalof

psychologicalmedicine.aspx

McLellan, J. (2004). Cambodian refugees in Ontario: Religious identities, social

cohesion and transnational linkages. Canadian Ethnic Studies Journal 36(2),

101–118. Retrieved from http://umanitoba.ca/publications/ces

357

McMichael, C., Gifford, S. M., & Correa-Velez, I. (2011). Negotiating family,

navigating resettlement: Family connectedness amongst resettled youth with

refugee backgrounds living in Melbourne, Australia. Journal of Youth Studies,

14(2), 179–195. doi: 10.1080/13676261.2010.506529

McMichael, C., & Manderson, L. (2004). Somali women and well-being: Social

networks and social captial among immigrant women in Australia. Human

Organization, 63(1), 88–99. Retrieved from http://www.sfaa.net/ho

McPherson, J. (2012). Does narrative exposure therapy reduce PTSD in survivors of

mass violence? Research on Social Work Practice, 22(1), 29–42. doi:

10.1177/1049731511414147

Mead, G. H. (1917). Scientific method and the individual thinker. In J. Dewey (Ed.),

Creative intelligence: Essays in the pragmatic (pp. 176–227). New York, NY:

Henry Holt.

Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa. New York, NY: Morrow.

Meadow, A. (1982). Psychopathology, psychotherapy, and the Mexican-American

patient. In E. E. Jones & S. J. Korchin (Eds.), Minority mental health (pp. 331–

361). New York, NY: Praeger.

Meara, N. M., & Day, J. D. (2000). Epilogue: Feminist visions and virtues of ethical

psychological practice. In M. M. Brabeck (Ed.), Practicing feminist ethics in

psychology. (pp. 249-268). Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Melville, A., & Faye, S. (2003). Psychosocial interventions: Evaluations of UNICEF

supported projects (1999–2001). Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from United Nations

International Children’s Emergency Fund website: http://www.unicef.

org/evaldatabase/files/IDS_2003_008.pdf

Merali, N. (2004). Individual assimilation status and intergenerational gaps in

Hispanic refugee families. International Journal for the Advancement of

Counselling, 26(1), 21–32. doi: 10.1023/B:ADCO.0000021547.83609.9d

Merriam, S. B., Johnson-Bailey, J., Lee, M.-Y., Kee, Y., Ntseane, G., & Muhamad,

M. (2001). Power and positionality: Negotiating insider/outsider status within and

across cultures. International Journal of Lifelong Education, 20(5), 405–416. doi:

10.1080/02601370120490

358

Michultka, D., Blanchard, E. B., & Kalous, T. (1998). Responses to civilian war

experiences: Predictors of psychological functioning and coping. Journal of

Traumatic Stress, 11(3), 571–577. doi: 10.1023/A:1024412931068

Miller, K. E. (2004). Beyond the frontstage: Trust, access, and the relational context

in research with refugee communities. American Journal of Community

Psychology, 33(3–4), 217–227. Retrieved from http://www.springer.com/

psychology/community+psychology/journal/10464

Miller, K. E., Kulkarni, M., & Kushner, H. (2006). Beyond trauma-focused

psychiatric epidemiology: Bridging research and practice with war-affected

populations. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(4), 409–422. doi:

10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.409

Miller, K. E., Omidian, P., Quraishy, A. S., Quraishy, N., Nasiry, M. N., Nasiry, S.,

… Yaqubi, A. A. (2006). The Afghan symptom checklist: A culturally grounded

approach to mental health assessment in a conflict zone. American Journal of

Orthopsychiatry, 76(4), 423–433. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.4.423

Miller, K. E., & Rasco, L. M. (2004). An ecological framework for addressing the

mental health needs of refugee communities. In K. E. Miller & L. M. Rasco

(Eds.), The mental health of refugees: Ecological approaches to healing and

adaptation (pp. 1–66). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Miller, K. E., & Rasmussen, A. (2010). War exposure, daily stressors, and mental

health in conflict and post-conflict settings: Bridging the divide between trauma-

focused and psychosocial frameworks. Social Science and Medicine, 70(1), 7–16.

doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.09.029

Minas, I. H. (1990). Mental health in a culturally diverse society. In J. Reid & P.

Trompf (Eds.), The health of immigrant Australia: A social perspective (pp. 250–

282). Sydney, Australia: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

Mirza, M. (1998). 'Same voices, same lives': Revisiting black feminist standpoint

epistemiology. In P. Connolly & B. Troyna (Eds.), Researching racism in

education: Politics, theory and practice (pp. 79–94). Buckingham, England:

Open University Press.

Mitchell, J., Jentes, E., Ortega, L., Sucosky, M. S., Jefferies, T., Bajcevic, P., …

Painter, J. (2012). Lead poisoning in United States-bound refugee children:

Thailand-Burma border, 2009. Pediatrics, 129(2), 392–399. doi:

10.1542/peds.2011-1218

359

Modvig, J., & Jaranson, J. M. (2004). A global perspective of torture, political

violence, and health. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken spirits: The

treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture victims (pp.

33–52). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Modvig, J., Pagaduan-Lopez, J., Rodenburg, J., Salud, C. M. D., Cadigon, R. V., &

Panelo, C. I. A. (2000). Torture and trauma in post-conflict East Timor. The

Lancet, 356(9243), 1763. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)03218-9

Mollica, R. F. (2001). The trauma story: A phenomenological approach to the

traumatic life experiences of refugee survivors. Psychiatry, 64(1), 60–63. doi:

10.1521/psyc.64.1.60.18242

Mollica, R. F., Caspi-Yavin, Y., Bollini, P., Truong, T., Tor, S., & Lavelle, J. (1992).

The Harvard Trauma Questionnaire: Validating a cross-cultural instrument for

measuring torture, trauma, and posttraumatic stress disorder in Indochinese

refugees. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 180(2), 111–116. Retrieved

from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Mollica, R. F., Cui, X., McInnes, K., & Massagli, M. P. (2002). Science-based policy

for psychosocial interventions in refugee camps: A Cambodian example. Journal

of Nervous and Mental Disease, 190(3), 158–166. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Mollica, R. F., McInnes, K., Pham, T., Smith Fawzi, M. C., Murphy, E., & Lin, L.

(1998). The dose-effect relationships between torture and psychiatric symptoms

in Vietnamese ex-political detainees and a comparison group. Journal of Nervous

and Mental Disease, 186(9), 543–553. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Mollica, R. F., McInnes, K., Pool, C., & Tor, S. (1998). Dose effect relationships of

trauma to symptoms of depression and post-traumatic stress disorder among

Cambodian survivors of mass violence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173(6),

482–488. doi: 10.1192/bjp.173.6.482

Mollica, R. F., Wyshak, G., & Lavelle, J. (1987). The psychological impact of war

trauma and torture on Southeast Asian refugees. American Journal of Psychiatry,

144(12), 1567–1572. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.

org/journal.aspx?journalid=13

360

Mollica, R. F., Wyshak, G., Lavelle, J., Truong, T., Tor, S., & Yang, T. (1990).

Assessing symptom change in southeast Asian refugee survivors of mass

violence and torture. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147(1), 83–88. Retrieved

from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/journal.aspx?journalid=13

Moodley, R., & West, W. (2005). Introduction. In R. Moodley & W. West (Eds.),

Integrating traditional health practices into counseling and psychotherapy (pp.

xv–xxvii). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Morse, J. M. (1997). “Perfectly healthy, but dead”: The myth of inter-rater reliability.

Qualitative Health Research, 7(4), 445–447. doi: 10.1177/

104973239700700401

Morse, J. M., & Richards, L. (2002). Readme first for a user’s guide to qualitative

methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Muller, H. J. (1994). Women in urban Burma: Social issues and political dilemmas.

Women’s Studies International Forum, 17(6), 609–920. doi: 10.1016/0277-

5395(94)00068-9

Mumtaz, Z., & Salway, S. (2009). Understanding gendered influences on women’s

reproductive health in Pakistan: Moving beyond the autonomy paradigm. Social

Science and Medicine, 68(7), 1349–1356. doi: 10.1016/

jsocscimed.2009.01.25

Murdock, G. P., Wilson, S. F., & Frederick, V. (1978). World distribution of theories

of illness. Ethnology, 17(4), 449–470. Retrieved from http://www.pitt.edu/

~ethnolog/

Murphy, D., Ndegwa, D., Kanani, A., Rojas-Jaimes, C., & Webster, A. (2002).

Mental health of refugees in inner-London. Psychiatric Bulletin, 26(6), 222–224.

doi: 10.1192/pb.26.6.222

Murphy, H. B. M. (1955). Flight and resettlement. New York, NY: Columbia

University Press.

Murray, K. E., Davidson, G. R., & Schweitzer, R. D. (2008). Psychological wellbeing

of refugees resettling in Australia: A literature review prepared for the

Australian Psychological Society. Retrieved 25 June, 2011, from Australian

Psychological Society website: http://www.psychology.org.au/publications/

statements/refugee/

361

Murray, K. E., Schweitzer, R. D., & Davidson, G. R. (2010). Review of refugee

mental health interventions following resettlement: Best practices and

recommendations. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 80(4), 576–585. doi:

10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01062.x

Nagayama Hall, G. C. (2001). Psychotherapy research with ethnic minorities:

Empirical, ethical, and conceptual issues. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 69(3), 502–510. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/

journals/ccp/index.aspx

Nash, M. (1966). Ritual and ceremonial cycle in upper Burma. In M. Nash (Ed.),

Anthropological studies in Theravada Buddhism (pp. 97–115). In Cultural Report

Series. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Southeast Asia Studies.

Nega, A. (2008). Utilising migrant skills will benefit all. Australian Mosaic, 18, 35–

36.

Neiman, A., Soh, A., & Sutan, E. (2008). Karen cultural profile. Retrieved 22

February, 2011, from Ethnomed website: http://ethnomed.org/culture/

karen/karen-cultural-profile

Neurosis. (2011). In Oxford English Dictionary. Retrieved 26 May, 2011, from

http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/126435?redirectedFrom=neurosis

New South Wales Department of Community Services. (n.d.). What is child abuse?

Retrieved 15 August, 2011, from http://www.community.nsw.gov.au/

preventing_child_abuse_and_neglect/what_is_child_abuse.html

New South Wales Department of Health. (2011). Refugee Health Plan 2011–2016.

[Policy directive]. Retrieved 19 August, 2011, from

http://www.health.nsw.gov.au/policies/pd/2011/PD2011_014.html

New South Wales Department of Health. (2012). Health warning to avoid Burmese

traditional powders. [Media release]. Retrieved 22 April, 2012, from

http://www.health.nsw.gov.au/news/2012/20120414_00.html

Nguyen, P. V. (2008). Perceptions of Vietnamese fathers’ acculturation levels,

parenting styles, and mental health outcomes in Vietnamese American adolescent

immigrants. Social Work, 53(4), 337–346. Retrieved from

http://www.naswpress.org/publications/journals/sw.html

Nguyen, S. D. (1982). The psycho-social adjustment and the mental health needs of

Southeast Asian refugees. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa, 7(1),

26–35.

362

Nicassio, P. M. (1985). The psychosocial adjustment of the Southeast Asian refugee:

An overview of empirical findings and theoretical models. Journal of Cross-

Cultural Psychology, 16(2), 153–173. doi: 10.1177/0022002185016002002

Nicassio, P. M., & Pate, J. K. (1984). An analysis of problems of resettlement of the

Indochinese refugees in the United States. Social Psychiatry, 19(3), 135–141.

doi: 10.1007/BF00595153

Nicholl, C., & Thompson, A. (2004). The psychological treatment of post traumatic

stress disorder (PTSD) in adult refugees: A review of the current state of

psychological therapies. Journal of Mental Health, 13(4), 351–362. doi:

10.1080/09638230410001729807

Nichter, M. (1981). Idiom of distress: Alternatives in the expression of psychosocial

distress: A case from South India. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 5(4), 379–

408. doi: 10.1007/BF00054782

Nickerson, A., Bryant, R. A., Silove, D., & Steel, Z. (2011). A critical review of

psychological treatments of posttraumatic stress disorder in refugees. Clinical

Psychology Review, 31(3), 399–417. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.10.004

Nickerson, A., & Hinton, D. E. (2011). Anger regulation in traumatized Cambodian

refugees: The perspectives of Buddhist monks. Culture, Medicine and

Psychiatry, 35(3), 396–416. doi: 10.1007/s11013-011-9218-y

Niederland, W. G. (1964). Psychiatric disorder among persecution victims: A

contribution to the understanding of concentration camp pathology and its after-

effects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 139(5), 458–474. Retrieved

from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Okello, E. S., & Ekblad, S. (2006). Lay concepts of depression among the Baganda of

Uganda: A pilot study. Transcultural Psychiatry, 53(2), 287–313. doi:

10.1177/1363461506064871

Olesen, V. L. (2005). Early millennial feminist qualitative research: Challenges and

contours. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of

qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 235–278). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Oliver, C. (2011). Critical realist grounded theory: A new approach for social work

research. British Journal of Social Work, 42(2), 371–387. doi: 10.1093/

bjsw/bcr064

Omark, R. C. (1980). Nervous breakdown as a folk illness. Psychological Reports,

47(3), 862. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1980.47.3.862

363

Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Leech, N. L. (2005). On becoming a pragmatic researcher:

The importance of combining quantitative and qualitative research

methodologies. International Journal of Social Research Methdology, 8(5), 375–

387. doi: 10.1080/13645570500402447

Oras, R., de Ezpeleta, S. C., & Ahmad, A. (2004). Treatment of traumatized refugee

children with eye movement desentization and reprocessing in a psychodynamic

context. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 58(3), 199–203. doi:

10.1080/08039480410006232

Ormel, J., VonKorff, M., Ustun, B., Pini, S., Koten, A., & Oldehinkel, T. (1994).

Common mental disorders and disability across cultures: Results for the WHO

collaborative study on psychological problems in general health care. Journal of

the American Medical Association, 272(22), 1741–1748. doi: 10.1001/

jama.1994.03520220035028

Orth, J., Doorschodt, L., Verburgt, J., & Drozdek, B. (2004). Sounds of trauma: An

introduction to methodology in music therapy with traumatized refugees in

clinical and outpatient settings. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek (Eds.), Broken

spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees, war and torture

victims (pp. 443–480). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Pahud, M., Kirk, R., Gage, J. D., & Hornblow, A. R. (2009). The coping processes of

adult refugees resettled in New Zealand. (New Issues in Refugee Research:

Working Paper No. 179). Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from United Nations High

Commissioner for Refugees website: http://www.unhcr.org/4b167d769.html

Palic, S., & Elklit, A. (2011). Psychosocial treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder

in adult refugees: A systematic review of prospective treatment outcomes studies

and a critique. Journal of Affective Disorders, 131(1–3), 8–23. doi:

10/1016/j/jad.2010.07.005

Pargament, K. I., Koenig, H. G., & Perez, L. M. (2000). The many methods of

religious coping: Development and initial validation of the RCOPE. Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 56(4), 519–543. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-4679

(200004)56:4

Park, C. L., Mills, M. A., & Edmondson, D. (2012). PTSD as meaning violation:

Testing a cognitive worldview perspective. Psychological Trauma: Theory,

Research, Practice and Policy, 4(1), 66–73. doi: 10.1037/a0018792

364

Parker, G., Chan, B., & Tully, L. (2006). Recognition of depressive symptoms by

Chinese subjects: The influence of acculturation and depressive experience.

Journal of Affective Disorders, 93(1–3), 141–147. doi: 10/1016/j.jad.2006.

03.002

Patel, V. (1995). Explanatory models of mental illness in sub-saharan Africa. Social

Science and Medicine, 40(9), 1291–1298. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)00231-H

Patel, V. (2000). The need for treatment evidence for common mental disorders in

developing countries. Psychological Medicine, 30(4), 743–746. Retrieved from

doi:http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displayJournal?jid=PSM

Patel, V. (2001). Cultural factors and international epidemiology. British Medical

Bulletin, 57(1), 33–45. doi: 10.1093/bmb/57.1.33

Patel, V., Abas, M., Broadhead, J., Todd, C., & Reeler, A. (2001). Depression in

developing countries: Lessons from Zimbabwe. British Medical Journal,

322(7284), 482–484. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7284.482

Patel, V., Ferrao Simbine, A. P., Soares, I. C., Weiss, H. A., & Wheeler, E. (2007).

Prevalence of severe mental and neurological disorders in Mozambique: A

population-based survey. The Lancet, 370(9592), 1055–1060. doi: 10.1016/

S0140-6736(07)61479-2

Patel, V., Gwanzura, F., Simunyu, E., Lloyd, K., & Mann, A. (1995). The

phenomenology and explanatory models of common mental disorder: A study in

primary care in Harare, Zimbabwe. Psychological Medicine, 25(6), 1191–1199.

doi: 10.1017/S003329170003316X

Patel, V., Musara, T., Butau, T., Maramba, P., & Fuyane, S. (1995). Concepts of

mental illness and medical pluralism in Harare. Psychological Medicine, 25(3),

485–493. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700033407

Patel, V., Pereira, J., & Mann, A. (1998). Somatic and psychological models of

common mental disorder in primary care in India. Psychological Medicine, 28(1),

135–143. Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/action/

displayJournal?jid=PSM

Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods (3rd ed.).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Pedersen, D. (2002). Political violence, ethnic conflict, and contemporary wars: Broad

implications for health and social well-being. Social Science and Medicine, 55(2),

175–190. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00261-1

365

Pedersen, D., Kienzler, H., & Gamarra, J. (2010). Llaki and Nakary: Idioms of

distress and suffering among the Highland Quechua in the Peruvian Andes.

Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry, 34(2), 279–300. doi: 10.1007/s11013-010-

9173-z

Pedersen, M. (2008). Burma’s ethnic minorities: Changing their own path to peace.

Critical Asian Studies, 40(1), 45–66. Retrieved from http://www.tandf.co.uk/

journals/RCRA

Perez, C. R., & Soto, J. A. (2011). Cognitive reappraisal in the context of oppression:

Implications for psychological functioning. Emotion, 11(3), 675–680. doi:

10.1037/a0021254

Perrin, M. (2011, June 23). Art shares stories of Burmese refugees. Wentworth

Courier. Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from http://wentworth-

courier.whereilive.com.au/news/story/art-shares-stories-of-burmese-refugees

Petevi, M., Revel, J. P., & Jacobs, G. A. (2001). Rapid assessment of mental health

needs of refugees, displaced and other populations affected by conflict and post-

conflict situations. A community-oriented assessment. Retrieved from World

Health Organisation, Department of Mental Health and Substance Dependence,

Team of Mental Health Determinants and Populations website:

www.who.int/hac/techguidance/pht/7405.pdf

Petry, N. M., Armentano, C., Kuoch, T., Norinth, T., & Smith, L. (2003). Gambling

participation and problems among South East Asian refugees to the United

States. Psychiatric Services, 54(8), 1142–1148. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.54.8.

1142

Pfanner, D. E. (1966). The Buddhist monk in rural Burmese society. In M. Nash

(Ed.), Anthropological studies in Theravada Buddhism (pp. 77–96). In Cultural

Report Series. New Haven, CT: Yale University, Southeast Asia Studies.

Phan, T., Steel, Z., & Silove, D. (2004). An ethnographically derived measure of

anxiety, depression and somatization: The Phan Vietnamese Psychiatric Scale.

Transcultural Psychiatry, 41(2), 200–232. doi: 10.1177/1363461504043565

Philp, J., & Mercer, D. (1999). Commodification of Buddhism in contemporary

Burma. Annals of Tourism Research, 26(1), 21–54. doi: 10.1016/S0160-

7383(98)00050-4

Pike, K. L. (1954). Language in relation to a unified theory of the structure of human

behavior. Glendale, CA: Summer Institute of Linguistics.

366

Pilcher, J., & Whelehan, I. (2004). 50 key concepts in gender studies. London,

England: Sage.

Pittaway, E., & Bartolomei, B. (2001). Refugees, race and gender: The multiple

discrimination against refugee women. Refuge, 19(6), 21–32. Retrieved from

https://pi.library.yorku.ca/ojs/index.php/refuge/index

Platz, R. (2003). Buddhism and Christianity in competition? Religious and ethnic

identity in Karen communities of Northern Thailand. Journal of Southeast Asian

Studies, 34(3), 473–490. doi: 10.1017/S0022463403000432

Ponce, D. (1974). The Filipinos in Hawaii. In W. S. Tseng, J. F. McDermott & T. W.

Maretzki (Eds.), People and cultures in Hawaii (pp. 34–43). Honolulu:

University Press of Hawaii.

Pope, C., & Mays, N. (1995). Qualitative research: Reaching the parts other methods

cannot reach: An introduction to qualitative methods in health and health services

research. British Medical Journal, 311(6996), 42–45. doi:

10.1136/bmj.311.6996.42

Porter, M., & Haslam, N. (2001). Forced displacement in Yugoslavia: A meta-

analysis of psychological consequences and their moderators. Journal of

Traumatic Stress, 14(4), 817–834. doi: 10.1023/A:1013054524810

Porter, M., & Haslam, N. (2005). Predisplacement and postdisplacement factors

associated with mental health of refugees and internally displaced persons: A

meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 294(5), 602–612.

doi: 10.1001/jama.294.5.602

Power, D. V., Moody, E., Trussel, K., O’Fallon, A., Chute, S., Kyaw, M., … Mamo,

B. (2010, April). Caring for the Karen: A newly arrived refugee group.

Minnesota Medicine. Retrieved from http://www.minnesotamedicine.com/

Home.aspx

Priest, R. G., Vize, C., Roberts, A., Roberts, M., & Tylee, A. (1996). Lay people’s

attitudes to treatment of depression: Results of opinion poll for Defeat Depression

Campaign just before its launch. British Medical Journal, 313(7061), 858–859.

doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7061.858

Psychosocial Working Group. (2003). Psychosocial intervention in complex

emergencies: A framework for practice. (Working Paper). Retrieved 9 February,

2012, from http://www.forcedmigration.org/psychosocial/PWGinfo.

htm/papers/PWGpapers.htm

367

Pupavac, V. (2004). Psychosocial interventions and the demoralization of

humanitarianism. Journal of Biosocial Science, 36(4), 491–504. doi:

10.1017/S0021932004006613

Purdie, N., Dudgeon, P., & Walker, R. (2010). Working together: Aboriginal and

Torres Strait Islander mental health and wellbeing principles and practice.

Canberra, Australia: Commonwealth of Australia.

Rabkin, J. G. (1982). Stress and psychiatric disorders. In L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz

(Eds.), Handbook of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (pp. 566–584). New

York, NY: Free Press.

Raguram, R., Weiss, M. G., Keval, H., & Channabasavanna, S. M. (2001). Cultural

dimensions of clinical depression in Bangalore, India. Anthropology & Medicine,

8(1), 31–46. doi: 10.1080/13648470120063889

Rajah, A. (2002). A ‘nation of intent’ in Burma: Karen ethno-nationalism, nationalism

and narrations of nation. The Pacific Review, 15(4), 517–537. doi: 10.1080/

0951274021000029413

Rao, M. S. (1968). The history of medicine in India and Burma. Medical History,

12(1), 52–61. Retrieved from http://journals.cambridge.org/action/

displayJournal?jid=MDH

Raphael, B., & Wilson, J. (2000). Introduction and overview: Key issues in the

conceptualization of debriefing. In B. Raphael & J. P. Wilson (Eds.),

Psychological debriefing theory, practice and evidence (pp. 1–17). Cambridge,

England: Cambridge University Press.

Rau, V., & Fanselow, M. S. (2007). Neurobiological and neuroethological

perspectives on fear and anxiety. In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M. Barad

(Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical and cultural

perspectives (pp. 21–26). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Razee, H. (2006). ‘Being a good woman’: Suffering and distress through the voices of

women in the Maldives. (Doctoral thesis, University of New South Wales,

Sydney, Australia). Retrieved from http://www.library.unsw.edu.au

Razum, O., Zeeb, H., & Rohrmann, S. (2000). The ‘healthy migrant effect’: Not

merely a fallacy of inaccurate denominator figures. International Journal of

Epidemiology, 29(1), 191–192. doi: 10.1093/ije/29.1.191

Reed, H. E., & Keely, C. B. (2001). Forced migration and mortality. Washington,

DC: National Academy Press.

368

Refugee Council of Australia. (2010). Economic, civic and social contributions of

refugees and humanitarian entrants. A literature review. Retrieved 18 April,

2012, from www.refugeecouncil.org.au/docs/resources/Contributions_of_

refugees.pdf

Refugee Council of Australia. (2011a). Refugee and humanitarian program 2011–12.

Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from http://www.refugeecouncil.org.au/

resources/consultations.html

Refugee Council of Australia. (2011b). Settlement support. Retrieved 24 June, 2011,

from http://www.refugeecouncil.org.au/settlement/support.php#ccs

Refugees International. (2010). Burma. Retrieved 4 December 2010, from

http://www.refintl.org/where-we-work/asia/burma

Reinharz, S. (1992). Feminist methods in social research. New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

Richter, K., & Klippel, A. (2007). Before or after: Prepositions in spatially

constrained systems. In T. Barkowsky, M. Knauff, G. Ligozat & D. R. Montello

(Eds.), Spatial cognition V: Reasoning, action, interaction (pp. 453–469).

Heidelberg, Germany: Springer.

Ritchie, J. (2001). Not everything can be reduced to numbers. In C. Berglund (Ed.),

Health research (pp. 149–173). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

Ritchie, J., Zwi, A. B., Blignault, I., Bunde-Birouste, A., & Silove, D. (2009). Insider-

outsider positions in health-development research: Reflections for practice.

Development in Practice, 19(1), 106–112. doi: 10.1080/

09614520802576526

Rosbrook, B., & Schweitzer, R. D. (2010). The meaning of home for Karen and Chin

refugees from Burma: An interpretative phenomenological approach. European

Journal of Psychotherapy and Counselling, 12(2), 159–172. doi:

10.1080/13642537.2010.488876

Rose, S. C., Bisson, J., Churchill, R., & Wessely, S. (2009). Psychological debriefing

for preventing post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Cochrane Database of

Systematic Reviews 2012(2), 1-47. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD000560

Rosenberg, E., Richard, C., Lussier, M., & Schuldiner, T. (2011). The content of talk

about health conditions and medications during appointments involving

interpreters. Family Practice, 28(3), 317–322. doi: 10.1093/fampra/cmq094

369

Rosenwald, G. C., & Wiersma, J. (1983). Women, career changes and the new self:

An analysis of rhetoric. Psychiatry, 46(3), 213–229. Retrieved from

http://www.psychiatryjournal.co.uk

Rotem, O. (2003). Buddhism. In C. S. Littleton (Ed.), The Sacred East (pp. 54–91).

London, England: Duncan Baird.

Rothbaum, B. O., Foa, E. B., Riggs, D. S., Murdock, T., & Walsh, W. (1992). A

prospective examination of post-traumatic stress disorder in rape victims. Journal

of Traumatic Stress, 5(3), 455–475. doi: 10.1007/BF00977239

Rousseau, C., & Measham, T. (2007). Posttraumatic suffering as a source of

transformation: A clinical perspective. In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M.

Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical and cultural

perspectives (pp. 275–293). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Ryan, D., Doonley, B., & Benson, C. (2008). Theoretical perspectives on post-

migration adaptation and psychological well-being among refugees: Towards a

resource-based model. Journal of Refugee Studies, 21(1), 1–18. doi:

10.1093/jrs/fem047

Sabioncello, A., Kocijan-Hercigonja, D., Rabatic, S., Tomasic, J., Jeren, T.,

Matijevic, L., … Dekaris, D. (2000). Immune, endocrine, and psychological

responses in civilians displaced by war. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62(4), 502–

508. Retrieved from http://www.psychosomaticmedicine.org

Sachs, E., Rosenfeld, B., Lhewa, D., Rasmussen, A., & Keller, A. (2008). Entering

exile: Trauma, mental health and coping among Tibetan refugees arriving in

Dharamsala, India. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21(2), 199–208. doi:

10.1002/jts.20324

Saltsman, A. P. (2009). Contested rights: Subjugation and struggle among Burmese

forced migrants in exile. (Master’s thesis). Available from ProQuest Dissertations

and Theses database. (UMI No. 1470328)

Sapir, E. (2010). The psychology of culture. In R. A. Le Vine (Ed.), Psychological

anthropology: A reader on self in culture (pp. 23–29). Chichester, England:

Wiley-Blackwell.

Scheper-Hughes, N. (1983). The problem of bias in androcentric and feminist

anthropology. Women’s Studies: An Inter-Disciplinary Journal, 10(2), 109–116.

doi: 10.1080/00497878.1983.9978584

370

Schubert, C. C., & Punamaki, R. (2011). Mental health among torture survivors:

Cultural background, refugee status and gender. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry,

65(3), 175–182. doi: 10.3109/08039488.2010.514943

Schuster, M. A., Stein, B. D., Jaycox, L. H., Collins, R. L., Marshall, G. N., Elliott,

M. N., … Berry, S. H. (2001). A national survey of stress reactions after the

September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. The New England Journal of Medicine,

345(20), 1507–1512. Retrieved from http://www.nejm.org

Schwartz, S., & Corcoran, C. (2010). Biological theories of psychiatric disorders. In

T. L. Scheid & T. N. Brown (Eds.), A handbook for the study of mental health:

Social contexts, theories, and systems (2nd ed., pp. 64–88). New York, NY:

Cambridge.

Schweitzer, R. D., Brough, M., Vromans, L., & Asic-Kobe, M. (2011). Mental health

of newly arrived Burmese refugees in Australia: Contributions of pre-migration

and post-migration experience. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 45(4), 299–307. doi: 10.3109/00048674.2010.543412

Schweitzer, R., Greenslade, J., & Kagee, A. (2007). Coping and resilience in refugees

from the Sudan: A narrative account. Australian and New Zealand Journal of

Psychiatry, 41(3), 282–288. doi: 10.1080/00048670601172780

Schweitzer, R., Melville, F., Steel, Z., & Lacherez, P. (2006). Trauma, post-migration

living difficulties, and social support as predictors of psychological adjustment in

resettled Sudanese refugees. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,

40(2), 179–187. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1614.2006.01766.x

Schweitzer, R., Perkoulidis, S. A., Krome, S. L., & Ludlow, C. N. (2005). Attitudes

towards refugees: The dark side of prejudice in Australia. Australian Journal of

Psychology, 57(3), 107–179. doi: 10.1080/00049530500125199

Schweitzer, R., & Steel, Z. (2008). Researching refugees: Methodological and ethical

considerations. In P. Liamputtong (Ed.), Doing cross-cultural research: Ethical

and methodological perspectives (pp. 87–101). In Social Indicators Research

Series. London, England: Springer.

Scott, W. J. (1990). PTSD in DSM-III: A case in the politics of diagnosis and disease.

Social Problems, 37(3), 294–310. Retrieved from

http://ucpressjournals.com/journal.php?j=sp

Sein, D. M. (1958, February). The women of Burma. The Atlantic Monthly, 122–125.

Retrieved from http://www.theatlantic.com

371

Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, development, and death. San

Francisco, CA: Freeman.

Selth, A. (2010). Modern Burma studies: A survey of the field. Modern Asian Studies,

44(2), 401–440. doi: 10.1017/S0026749X08003508

Shalev, A. Y. (2007). PTSD: A disorder of recovery? In L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson

& M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma: Integrating biological, clinical, and

cultural perspectives (pp. 207–223). New York, NY: Cambridge University

Press.

Shan Human Rights Foundation, & Shan Women’s Action Network. (2002). Licence

to rape. The Burmese military regime’s use of sexual violence in the ongoing war

in Shan state. Retrieved February 18, 2011, from Society for Threatened Peoples

website: http://www.gfbv.it/3dossier/asia/burma/burma-shan-en.html

Sheikh-Mohammed, M., MacIntyre, C. R., Wood, N. J., Leask, J., & Isaacs, D.

(2006). Barriers to access to health care for newly resettled sub-Saharan refugees

in Australia. Medical Journal of Australia, 185(11–12), 594–597. Retrieved from

https://www.mja.com.au

Shrestha, N. M., Sharma, B., Van Ommeren, M., Regmi, S., Makaju, R., Komproe, I.

H., … de Jong, J. T. V. M. (1998). Impact of torture on refugees displaced within

the developing world: Symptomatology among Bhutanese refugees in Nepal.

Journal of the American Medical Association, 280(5), 443–448. doi:

10.1001/jama.280.5.443

Sikorski, J. M., Stampfer, H. G., Cole, R. M., & Wheatley, A. E. (1996).

Psychological aspects of chronic low back pain. Australian and New Zealand

Journal of Surgery, 66(5), 294–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1445-2197.1996.

tb01189.x

Silove, D. (1999). The psychosocial effects of torture, mass human rights violations,

and refugee trauma: Toward an integrated conceptual framework. Journal of

Nervous and Mental Disease, 187(4), 200–207. Retrieved from

http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Silove, D. (2003). Mental health of asylum seekers: Australia in a global context. In

P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of refugees: Public health perspectives from crisis to

settlement (pp. 68–82). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

372

Silove, D. (2004). The global challenge of asylum. In J. P. Wilson & B. Drozdek

(Eds.), Broken spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers, refugees,

war and torture victims (pp. 13–31). New York, NY: Brunner-Routledge.

Silove, D. (2007). Adaptation, ecosocial safety signals, and the trajectory of PSTD. In

L. J. Kirmayer, R. Lemelson & M. Barad (Eds.), Understanding trauma:

Integrating biological, clinical and cultural perspectives (pp. 242–258). New

York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Silove, D. (2010). From trauma to survival and adaptation: Towards a framework for

guilding mental health initiatives in post-conflict societies. In D. Ingleby (Ed.),

Forced migration and mental health: Rethinking the care of refugees and

displaced persons (pp. 29–51). New York, NY: Springer.

Silove, D., & Bryant, R. (2006). Rapid assessments of mental health needs after

disasters. Journal of the American Medical Association, 296(5), 576–578. doi:

10.1001/jama.296.5.576

Silove, D., Sinnerbrink, I., Field, A., Manicavasagar, V., & Steel, Z. (1997). Anxiety,

depression and PTSD in asylum-seekers: Assocations with pre-migration trauma

and post-migration stressors. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 170(4), 351–357.

doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.4.351

Silove, D., Steel, Z., & Bauman, A. (2007). Mass psychological trauma and PTSD:

Epidemic or cultural illusion? In J. P. Wilson & C. So-kum Tang (Eds.), Cross-

cultural assessment of psychological trauma and PTSD (pp. 319–336). New

York, NY: Springer.

Simich, L., Beiser, M., & Mawani, F. N. (2003). Social support and the significance

of shared experience in refugee migration and resettlement. Western Journal of

Nursing Research, 25(7), 872. doi: 10.1177/0193945903256705

Singh, B. (1993). Independence and democracy in Burma, 1945–1952: The turbulent

years. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, Center for South and Southeast Asian

Studies.

Skidmore, M. (2002). Menstrual madness: Women’s health and well-being in urban

Burma. Women and Health, 35(4), 81–99. doi: 10.1300/J013v35n04_06

Skidmore, M. (2003). Darker than midnight: Fear, vulnerability, and terror making in

urban Burma (Myanmar). American Ethnologist, 30(1), 5–21. doi: 10.1525/

ae.2003.30.1.5

373

Smith, L. T. (1999). Decolonising methodologies: Research and indigenous peoples.

London, England: Zed Books.

Smith, M. (1994). Ethnic groups in Burma: Development, democracy and human

rights. London, England: Anti-Slavery International.

Smith, S. (2010, October). Caring for one of Minnesota’s newest ethnic groups.

Culture Care Connection. Retrieved 9 February, 2011, from Minnesota Medical

Assocation website: http://www.mnmed.org/News/NewsFullStory/tabid/22

66/ArticleID/3790/CBModuleId?3252/Default.aspx

Smith-Hefner, N. J. (1994). Ethnicity and the force of faith: Christian conversion

among Khmer refugees. Anthropological Quarterly, 67(1), 24–37. Retrieved

from http://aq.gwu.edu

Somasundaram, D. J. S. (2010). Collective trauma in the Vanni: A qualitative inquiry

into the mental health of the internally displaced due to the civil war in Sri Lanka.

International Journal of Mental Health Systems, 4(22), 1–31. doi: 10.1186/1752-

4458-4-22

Somasundaram, D. J. S., & Jamunanantha, C. S. (2002). Psychosocial consequences

of war. In J. T. V. M. de Jong (Ed.), Trauma, war and violence: Public mental

health in socio-cultural context (pp. 205–258). New York, NY: Kluwer

Academic / Plenum Publishers.

Sondergaard, H. P., & Theorell, T. (2003). A longitudinal study of hormonal reactions

accompanying life events in recently resettled refugees. Psychotherapy and

Psychosomatics, 72(1), 49–58. doi: 10.1159/000067185

South, A. (2007). Burma: The changing nature of displacement crises. (RSC Working

Paper No. 39). Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from http://www.rsc.ox.ac.uk/

publications/working-papers-folder_contents/RSCworkingpaper39.pdf

Sparks, E. E., & Park, A. H. (2000). The integration of feminism and

multiculturalism: Ethical dilemmas at the border. In M. M. Brabeck (Ed.),

Practicing feminist ethics in psychology (pp. 203–224). Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

Spiro, M. E. (1969). The psychological function of witchcraft belief: The Burmese

case. In W. Caudill & L. Tsung-Yi (Eds.), Mental health research in Asia and the

Pacific (pp. 245–258). Honolulu, Hawaii: East-West Center.

Spiro, M. E. (1997). Gender ideology and psychological reality: An essay on cultural

reproduction. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

374

Spradley, J. P. (1979). The ethnographic interview. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart

and Winston.

Spradley, J. P. (1980). Participant observation. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart and

Winston.

Spring, M., Westermeyer, J., Halcon, L., Savik, K., Robertson, C., Johnson, D. R., …

Jaranson, J. (2003). Sampling in difficult to access refugee and immigrant

communities. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 191(2), 813–819.

Retrieved from http://journals.lww.com/jonmd/pages/default.aspx

Steel, Z., Frommer, N., & Silove, D. (2004). Part I - The mental health impacts of

migration: The law and its effects. Failing to understand: Refugee determination

and the traumatized applicant. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry,

27(6), 511–528. doi: 10.1016/j.ijlp.2004.08.006

Steel, Z., Silove, D., Bird, K., McGorry, P., & Mohan, P. (1999). Pathways from war

trauma to posttraumatic stress symptoms among Tamil asylum seekers, refugees,

and immigrants. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 12(3), 421–435. doi:

10.1023/A:1024710902534

Steinberg, D. I. (2010). Burma/Myanmar: What everyone needs to know. New York,

NY: Oxford University Press.

Stevens, P. E. (1996). Focus groups: Collecting aggregate-level data to understand

community health phenomena. Public Health Nursing, 13(3), 170–176. doi:

10.1111/j.1525-1446.1996.tb00237.x

Stewart, D. W., & Shamdasani, P., N. (1990). Focus groups: Theory and practice.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory

procedures and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and

procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Strauss, A., Schatzmann, L., Bucher, R., Ehrlich, D., & Sabshin, M. (1964).

Psychiatric ideologies and institutions. New York, NY: Free Press.

Stubbs, P. (2010). Transforming local and global discourses: Reassessing the PTSD

movement in Bosnia and Croatia. In D. Ingleby (Ed.), Forced migration and

mental health: Rethinking the care of refugees and displaced persons (pp. 53–

66). New York, NY: Springer.

375

Suddaby, R. (2006). From the editors: What grounded theory is not. Academy of

Management Journal, 49(4), 633–642. Retrieved from http://journals.

aomonline.org/amj

Sue, S. (1998). In search of cultural competence in psychotherapy and counseling.

American Psychologist, 53(4), 440–448. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.4.440

Sue, S., & Zane, N. (2009). The role of culture and cultural techniques in

psychotherapy: A critique and reformulation. Asian American Journal of

Psychology, 5(1), 3–14. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/aap

Sulaiman, S. O. Y., Bhugra, D., & de Silva, P. (2001). Perceptions of depression in a

community sample in Dubai. Transcultural Psychiatry, 38(2), 201–218. doi:

10.1177/136346150103800204

Sulaiman-Hill, C. M. R., & Thompson, S. C. (2011). Sampling challenges in a study

examining refugee resettlement. BioMed Central International Health and

Human Rights, 11(2): 2. doi: 10.1186/1472-698X-11-2

Summerfield, D. (1999). A critique of seven assumptions behind psychological

trauma programmes in war-affected areas. Social Science and Medicine, 48(10),

1449–1462. Retrieved from http://www.journals.elsevier.com/social-science-and-

medicine

Summerfield, D. (2000). Childhood, war, refugeedom and ‘trauma’: Three core

questions for mental health professionals. Transcultural Psychiatry, 37(3), 417–

433. doi: 10.1177/136346150003700308

Summerfield, D. (2001). The invention of post-traumatic stress disorder and the social

usefulness of a psychiatric category. British Medical Journal, 322(7278), 95–98.

doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7278.95

Summerfield, D. (2002). Effects of war: Moral knowledge, revenge, reconciliation,

and medicalised concepts of “recovery”. British Medical Journal, 325(7372),

1105–1107. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7372.1105

Summerfield, D. (2007). ‘Major depression’ in Ethiopia: Validity is the problem.

British Journal of Psychiatry, 190(4), 362. doi: 10.1192/bjp.190.4.362a

Summerfield, D. (2008). How scientifically valid is the knowledge base of global

mental health? British Medical Journal, 336(7651), 992–994. doi:

10.1136/bmj.39513.441030.AD

376

Summerfield, D. (2010). “My whole body is sick … my life is not good”: A Rwandan

asylum seeker attends a psychiatric clinic in London. In D. Ingleby (Ed.), Forced

migration and mental health: Rethinking the care of refugees and displaced

persons (pp. 97–114). New York, NY: Springer.

Suu Kyi, A. S. (2010). Freedom from fear. London, England: Penguin Books.

Tankink, M., & Richters, A. (2007). Silence as a coping strategy: The case of refugee

women in the Netherlands from South-Sudan who experienced sexual violence in

the context of war. In B. Drozdek & J. P. Wilson (Eds.), Voices of trauma:

Treating psychological trauma across cultures (pp. 191–210). New York, NY:

Springer.

Taylor, C. (2009). The culture of confession from Augustine to Foucault. New York,

NY: Routledge.

Taylor, C., & White, S. (2001). Knowledge, truth and reflexivity. Journal of Social

Work, 1(1), 37. doi: 10.1177/146801730100100104

Taylor, J. (2004). Refugee and social exclusion: What the literature says. Migration

Action, XXVI(2), 16–31.

Tedeschi, R., & Calhoun, L. (2004a). Posttraumatic growth: A new perspective of

psychotraumatology. Psychiatric Times, 21(4), 58-60. Retrieved from

http://www.psychiatrictimes.com/home

Tedeschi, R., & Calhoun, L. (2004b). Posttraumatic growth: Conceptual foundations

and empirical evidence. Psychological Inquiry, 15(1), 1–18. Retrieved from

http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/journal.asp?issn=1047-840X&linktype=44

Terheggan, M. A., Stroebe, M. S., & Kleber, R. J. (2001). Western conceptualizations

and Eastern experience: A cross-cultural study of traumatic stress reactions

among Tibetan refugees in India. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 14, 391–403. doi:

10.1023/A:1011177204593

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2007). Border history. Retrieved 2 March,

2011, from http://www.tbbc.org/camps/history.htm

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2009). Food security programme: Food,

nutrition, and agriculture. Retrieved 2 March, 2011, from

http://www.tbbc.org/whatwedo/whatwedo.htm#food

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2010a). Programme report: January to June

2010. Retrieved 2 March 2011, from http://www.tbbc.org/resources/

resources.htm#reports

377

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2010b). Nine thousand nights. Refugees from

Burma: A people’s scrapbook. Bangkok, Thailand: Thailand Burma Border

Consortium.

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2011a). Programme report: January to June

2011. Retrieved 26 September, 2011, from http://www.tbbc.org/resources/

resources.htm#reports

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (2011b). TBBC’s camp population figures:

August 2011. Retrieved 26 September, 2011, from http://www.tbbc.org/

camps/camps.htm

Thailand Burma Border Consortium. (n.d.). Camp locations. Retrieved 18 April,

2012, from http://www.tbbc.org/camps/camps.htm

Thampy, L., & Buhgra, D. (2004). In-patient care and ethnic minority patients. In P.

Campling, S. Davies & G. Farquharson (Eds.), From toxic institutions to

therapeutic environments: Residential settings in mental health services (pp. 99–

107). London, England: The Royal College of Psychiatrists.

Thawnghmung, A. M. (2008). The Karen revolution in Burma: Diverse voices,

uncertain ends. Washington, DC: East-West Center.

Thoits, P. A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 54(4), 416–423. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/

pubs/journals/ccp/index.aspx

Thomas, W. I., & Thomas, D. (1928). The child in America. New York, NY: A. A.

Knopf.

Thoresen, C. E. (1999). Spirituality and health: Is there a relationship? Journal of

Health Psychology, 4(3), 291–300. Retrieved from http://hpq.sagepub.com

Tilden, V. P., & Galyen, R. D. (1987). Cost and conflict: The darker side of social

support. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 9(1), 9–18. Retrieved from

http://wjn.sagepub.com

Tint Way, R. (1985). Burmese culture, personality and mental health. Australian and

New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 19(3), 275–282. doi: 10.3109/

00048678509158832

Toneatto, T. (2002). A metacognitive therapy for anxiety disorders: Buddhist

psychology applied. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 9(1), 72–78. doi:

10.1016/S1077-7229(02)80043-8

378

Toole, M. J. (2003). The health of refugees: An international public health problem.

In P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of refugees: Public health perspectives from crisis

to settlement (pp. 35–53). Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

Toole, M. J., & Waldman, R. J. (1988). An analysis of mortality trends among

refugee populations in Somalia, Sudan, and Thailand. Bulletin of the World

Health Organization, 66(2), 237–247. Retrieved from http://www.who.int/

bulletin/en/

Toole, M. J., & Waldman, R. J. (1990). Prevention of excess mortality in refugee and

displaced populations in developing countries. Journal of the American Medical

Association, 263(24), 3296–3302. doi: 10.1001/jama.1990.

03440240086021

Tribe, R. (1999a). Bridging the gap or damming the flow? Some observations on

using interpreters/bicultural workers when working with refugee clients, many of

whom have been tortured. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 72(4), 567–576. doi:

10.1348/000711299160130

Tribe, R. (1999b). Therapeutic work with refugees living in exile: Observations on

clinical practice. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 12(3), 233–243. Retrieved

from http://www.tandf.co.uk/journals/titles/09515070.asp

Tribe, R. (2002). Mental health of refugees and asylum-seekers. Advances in

Psychiatric Treatment, 8(4), 240–247. doi: 10.1192/apt.8.4.240

Tribe, R. (2007). Working with interpreters. The Psychologist, 20(3), 159–161.

Retrieved from www.thepsychologist.org.uk Tribe, R. (2009). Working with interpreters in mental health. International Journal of

Culture and Mental Health, 2(2), 92–101. doi: 10.1080/17542860903115976

Tribe, R., & Keefe, A. (2009). Issues in using interpreters in therapeutic work with

refugees. What is not being expressed? European Journal of Psychotherapy and

Counselling, 11(4), 409–424. doi: 10.1080/13642530903444795

Tribe, R., & Lane, P. (2009). Working with interpreters across language and culture in

mental health. Journal of Mental Health, 18(3), 233–241. doi:

10.1080/09638230701879102

379

Tsey, K., Wilson, A., Haswell-Elkins, M., Whiteside, M., McCalman, J., Cadet-

James, Y., & Wenitong, M. (2007). Empowerment-based research methods: A

10-year approach to enhancing Indigenous social and emotional wellbeing. The

Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Psychiatrists, 15(s1), 34–38. doi:

10.1080/10398560701701163

Turnbull, G. J. (1998). A review of post-traumatic stress disorder. Part I: Historical

development and classification. Injury, 29(2), 87–91. doi: 10.1016/S0020-

1383(97)00131-9

United Kingdom Home Office. (2009). Country of origin information report – Burma

(Union of Myanmar). Retrieved 1 December, 2010, from United Nations High

Commissioner for Refugees website:

http://www.unhcr.org/refworld/docid/49b0e08b2.html

United Nations Convention on the Elimination of Discrimination against Women.

(2008). Concluding observations of the committee on the elimination of

discrimination against women: Myanmar. Retrieved 4 December, 2010, from

http://www.unhcr.org/refworld/docid/494ba8d00.html

United Nations Development Programme. (2010). Human development report 2010.

The real wealth of nations: Pathways to human development. Retrieved 26

September, 2011, from http://hdr.undp.org/en/statistics/

United Nations Educational Scientific and Cultural Organization, & International

Institute for Education Planning. (2009). Certification counts: Recognizing the

learning attainments of displaced and refugee students. Paris, France:

International Institute for Educational Planning.

United Nations General Assembly. (2010a). Progress report of the Special

Rapporteur on the situation of human rights in Myanmar, Tomás Ojea Quintana.

Retrieved 18 April, 2012, from

http://www.securitycouncilreport.org/site/c.glKWLeMTIsG/b.2802231/

United Nations General Assembly. (2010b). Situation of human rights in Myanmar.

Retrieved 25 October, 2011, from www.burmalibrary.org/docs09/SRM2010-rep-

A65-368(en).pdf

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2004). UNCHR resettlement

handbook. Retrieved 30 November 2010, from http://www.unhcr.org/

4a2ccf4c6.html

380

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2008). UNHCR handbook for the

protection of women and girls. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.unhcr.org/pages/49c3646c1d9.html

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2010a). 2010 UNHCR country

operations profile – Thailand. Retrieved 4 December 2010, from

http://www.unhcr.org/cgi-bin/texis/vtx/page?page=49e489646

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2010b). Progress report on

resettlement. Retrieved 26 September, 2011, from

http://www.unhcr.org/pages/4a16b1676.html?

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2010c). Statistical yearbook 2009.

Retrieved 26 September, 2011, from http://www.unhcr.org/4ce532ff9.html

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2010d). UNHCR Global Report

2009. Retrieved 21 May, 2012 from http://www.unhcr.org/4c08f2829.html

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2011a). 2011 UNHCR country

operations profile – Myanmar. Retrieved 28 September, 2011, from

http://www.unhcr.org/pages/49e4877d6.html

United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees. (2011b). Text of the 1951

Convention and 1967 Protocol. Retrieved 29 September, 2011, from

http://www.unhcr.org/pages/49da0e466.html

United States Department of State. (2009). 2008 human rights report: Burma.

Retrieved 4 December 2010, from http://www.state.gov/g/drl/rls/hrrpt/2008/

eap/119035.htm

United States Department of State. (2010). 2009 human rights report: Burma.

Retrieved 4 December 2010, from http://www.state.gov/g/drl/rls/hrrpt/2009/

eap/135987.htm

Ussher, J. M., Rae, M., Rhyder-Obid, M., Perz, J., Newman, P., & Wong, W. K. T.

(2011). Reproductive and sexual health needs of culturally and linguistically

diverse communities in Western Australia. Sydney: University of Western

Sydney, School of Psychology.

Vance, C. S. (1991). Anthropology redisovers sexuality: A theoretical comment.

Social Science and Medicine, 33(8), 875–884. doi: 10.1016/0277-

9536(91)90259-F

381

van der Kolk, B. A., & van der Hart, O. (1989). Pierre Janet and the breakdown of

adaptation in psychological trauma. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146(12),

1530–1540. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/journal.aspx?

journalid=13

van de Put, W. A. C. M., & Eisenbruch, M. (2004). Internally displaced Cambodians:

Healing trauma in communities. In K. E. Miller & L. M. Rasco (Eds.), The

mental health of refugees: Ecological approaches to healing and adaptation (pp.

133–160). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Van Ommeren, M. (2003). Validity issues in transcultural epidemiology. The British

Journal of Psychiatry, 182(5), 376–378. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.5.376

van Tubergen, F., & Kalmijn, M. (2009). A dynamic approach to the determinants of

immigrants’ language proficiency: The United States, 1980–2000. International

Migration, 43(3), 519–543. Retrieved from

http://www.blackwellpublishing.com/journal.asp?ref=0020-7985

Vogler, P. (2006). In the absence of the humanitarian gaze: Refugee camps after dark.

(New Issues in Refugee Research: Research Paper No. 137). Retrieved 18 April,

2011, from United Nations High Commissioner for Refugees website:

http://www.unhcr.org/457ff97b2.html

Wakefield, J. C. (2001). The myth of DSM’s invention of new categories of disorder:

Houts’s diagnostic discontinuity thesis disconfirmed. Behaviour Research and

Therapy, 39(5), 575–624. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00071-1

Walton, E., Berasi, K., Takeuchi, D. T., & Uehara, E. S. (2010). Cultural diversity

and mental health treatment. In T. L. Scheid & T. N. Brown (Eds.), A handbook

for the study of mental health: Social contexts, theories, and systems (2nd ed., pp.

439–460). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Ward, J. (2002). If not now, when? Addressing gender-based violence in refugee,

internally displaced and post-conflict settings. A global overview. 2002. New

York, NY: The Reproductive Health for Refugees Consortium.

Warren, C. A. (2000). A fertile grounding: Cultural studies meets women’s health.

National Women’s Studies Association Journal, 12(3), 178–192. Retrieved from

http://www.nwsa.org/

Watters, E. (2010). Crazy like us: The globalization of the American psyche. New

York, NY: Free Press.

382

Weine, S. M., Kulenovic, A. D., Pavkovic, I., & Gibbons, R. (1998). Testimony

psychotherapy in Bosnian refugees: A pilot study. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 155(12), 1720–1726. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.

org/journal.aspx?journalid=13

Weine, S. M., Vojvoda, D., Becker, D. F., McGlashan, T. H., Hodzic, E., Laub, D., …

Lazrove, S. (1998). PTSD symptoms in Bosnian refugees 1 year after

resettlement in the United States. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(4), 562–

564. Retrieved from http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/journal.aspx?

journalid=13

Weiss, M. (1997). Explanatory Model Interview Catalogue (EMIC): Framework for

comparative study of illness. Transcultural Psychiatry, 34(2), 235–263. doi:

10.1177/136346159703400204

Weiss, M. (2001). Cultural epidemiology: An introduction and overview.

Anthropology and Medicine, 8(1), 5–29. doi: 10.1080/13648470120070980

Weiss, M., Doongaji, D. R., Siddhartha, S., Wypij, D., Pathare, S., Bhatawdekar, M.,

… Fernandes, R. (1992). The Explanatory Model Interview Catalogue (EMIC).

Contribution to cross-cultural research methods from a study of leprosy and

mental health. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 160(6), 819–830. doi:

10.1192/bjp.160.6.819

Wertheim-Cahen, T., van Dijk, M., Schouten, K., Roozen, I., & Drozdek, B. (2004).

About a weeping willow, a phoenix rising from its ashes, and building a house ...

Art therapy with refugees: Three different perspectives. In J. P. Wilson & B.

Drozdek (Eds.), Broken spirits: The treatment of traumatized asylum seekers,

refugees, war and torture victims (pp. 419–442). New York, NY: Brunner-

Routledge.

Whittaker, S., Hardy, G., Lewis, K., & Buchan, L. (2005). An exploration of

psychological well-being with young Somali refugee and asylum-seeker women.

Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 10(2), 177–196. doi:

10.1177/1359104505051210

Wierzbicka, A. (1994). Emotion, language, and cultural scripts. Washington, DC:

American Psychological Association.

383

Wig, N. N., Suleiman, M. A., Routledge, R., Srinviasa Murthy, R., Ladrido-Ignacio,

L., Ibrahim, H. H. A., & Harding, T. W. (1980). Community reactions to mental

disorders: A key informant study in three developing countries. Acta Psychiatrica

Scandinavica, 61(2), 111–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1980.tb00571.x

Willgerodt, M. A. (2003). Using focus groups to develop culturally relevant

instruments. Western Journal of Nursing Research, 25(7), 798–814. doi:

10.1177/0193945903256708

Williams, B., & Healy, D. (2001). Perceptions of illness causation among new

referrals to a community mental health term: “Explanatory model” or

“explanatory map”? Social Science and Medicine, 53(4), 465–476. Retrieved

from http://www.journals.elsevier.com/social-science-and-medicine

Willig, C. (1999). Beyond appearances: A critical realist approach to social

constructionist work. In D. J. Nightingale & J. Cromby (Eds.), Social

constructionist psychology: A critical analysis of theory and practice (pp. 36–

51). Buckingham, England: Open University Press.

Wilson, A., Renzaho, A. M. N., McCabe, M., & Swinburn, B. (2010). Towards

understanding the new food environment for refugees from the Horn of Africa in

Australia. Health and Place, 16(5), 969–976. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.

2010.06.001

Wilson, J. P. (1994). The historical evolution of PTSD diagnostic criteria: From Freud

to DSM-IV. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7(4), 681–698. doi: 10.1002/jts.

2490070413

Wilson, J. P. (1995). Traumatic events and PTSD prevention. In B. Raphael & E. D.

Barrows (Eds.), The handbook of preventative psychiatry (pp. 281–296).

Amsterdam, Holland: Elsevier.

Wilson, J. P. (2007). The lens of culture: Theoretical and conceptual perspectives in

the assessment of psychological trauma and PTSD. In J. P. Wilson & C.

So-kum Tang (Eds.), Cross-cultural assessment of psychological trauma and

PTSD (pp. 3–30). New York, NY: Springer.

Wilson, J. P., & Drozdek, B. (2007). Are we lost in translations?: Unanswered

questions on trauma, culture and posttraumatic syndromes and recommendations

for future research. In B. Drozdek & J. P. Wilson (Eds.), Voices of trauma:

Treating psychological trauma across cultures (pp. 367–386). New York, NY:

Springer.

384

Wing, D. M. (1998). A comparison of traditional folk healing concepts with

contemporary healing concepts. Journal of Community Health Nursing, 15(3),

143–154. Retrieved from http://www.tandfonline.com/toc/hchn20/current

Winland, D. N. (1994). Christianity and community: Conversion and adaptation

among Hmong refugee women. The Canadian Journal of Sociology, 19(1), 21–

45. Retrieved from http://www.cjsonline.ca

Woods, P. (1992). Symbolic interactionism: Theory and method. In M. D. LeCompte,

W. L. Millroy & J. Preissle (Eds.), The handbook of qualitative research in

education (pp. 337–404). New York, NY: Academic Press.

Working Lives Research Institute. (2005). Women refugees - from volunteers to

employees: A research project on paid and unpaid work in the voluntary sector

and volunteering as a pathway into employment. Retrieved 23 April, 2012, from

http://www.workinglives.org/research-themes/migrant-workers/refugee-women-

volunteers-lda.cfm

World Bank. (2011). Migration and remittances factbook 2011 (2nd ed.).

Washington, DC: Author.

World Health Organization. (1986). Ottawa Charter for Health Promotion. Geneva:

Author.

World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and

behavioural disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva, Switzerland:

Author.

Wuthnow, R. (1991). Acts of compassion: Caring for others and helping ourselves.

Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Yakushko, O., & Espin, O. M. (2010). The experience of immigrant and refugee

women: Psychological issues. In L. Hope & N. F. Russo (Eds.), Handbook of

diversity in feminist psychology (pp. 535–558). New York, NY: Springer.

Yehuda, R. (2003a). Adult neuroendocrine aspects of PTSD. Psychiatric Annals,

33(1), 30–36. Retrieved from http://www.healio.com/psychiatry

Yehuda, R. (2003b). Changes in the concept of PTSD and trauma. Psychiatric Times,

20(4), 1–5. Retrieved from www.psychiatrictimes.com

Yehuda, R., & McFarlane, A. (1995). Conflict between current knowledge about

posttraumatic stress disorder and its original conceptual basis. American Journal

of Psychiatry, 152(12), 1705–1713. Retrieved from

http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/journal.aspx?journalid=13

385

Ying, Y. W. (1990). Explanatory models of major depression and implications for

help-seeking among immigrant Chinese-American women. Culture, Medicine

and Psychiatry, 14(3), 393–408. doi: 10.1007/BF00117563

Young, A. (1995). The harmony of illusions: Inventing post-traumatic stress disorder.

Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Zarowsky, C. (2004). Writing trauma: Emotion, ethnography, and the politics of

suffering among Somali returnees in Ethiopia. Culture, Medicine and Psychiatry,

28(2), 189–209. doi: 10.1023/B:MEDI.0000034410.08428.29

Ziguras, S., Klimidis, S., Lewis, J., & Stuart, G. (2003). Ethnic matching of clients

and clinicians and use of mental health services by ethnic minority clients.

Psychiatric Services, 54(4), 535–541. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.54.4.535

Zwi, A. B., & Alvarez-Castillo, F. (2003). Forced migration, globalisation, and public

health: Getting the big picture into focus. In P. Allotey (Ed.), The health of

refugees: Public health perspectives from crisis to settlement (pp. 14–34).

Melbourne, Australia: Oxford University Press.

Zwi, A. B., & Ugalde, A. (1989). Towards an epidemiology of political violence in

the Third World. Social Science and Medicine, 28(7), 633–642. doi:

10.1016/0277-9536(89)90210-4

386

Appendices

387

Appendix A: DSM-IV criteria

Diagnostic criteria for posttraumatic stress disorder (American Psychiatric

Association, 2000, pp. 467–468)

A. The person has been exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following

were present:

1. the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events

that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the

physical integrity of self or others

2. the person’s response involved intense fear, helplessness, or horror. Note: In

children, this may be expressed instead by disorganised or agitated behaviour

B. The traumatic event is persistently re-experienced in one (or more) of the

following ways:

1. recurrent and intrusive distressing recollections of the event, including images,

thoughts, or perceptions. Note: In young children, repetitive play may occur in

which themes or aspects of the trauma are expressed.

2. recurrent distressing dreams of the event. Note: In children, there may be

frightening dreams without recognisable content.

3. acting or feeling as if the traumatic event were recurring (includes a sense of

reliving the experience, illusions, hallucinations, and dissociative flashback

episodes, including those that occur upon awakening or when intoxicated).

Note: In young children, trauma-specific re-enactment may occur.

4. intense psychological distress at exposure to internal or external cues that

symbolise or resemble an aspect of the traumatic event

5. physiological reactivity on exposure to internal or external cues that symbolise

or resemble an aspect of the traumatic event

C. Persistent avoidance of stimuli associated with the trauma and numbing of general

responsiveness (not present before the trauma), as indicated by three (or more) of the

following:

1. efforts to avoid thoughts, feelings, or conversations associated with the trauma

2. efforts to avoid activities, places, or people that arouse recollections of the

trauma

388

3. inability to recall an important aspect of the trauma

4. markedly diminished interest or participation in significant activities.

5. feeling of detachment or estrangement from others

6. restricted range of affect (e.g., unable to have loving feelings)

7. sense of a foreshortened future (e.g., does not expect to have a career,

marriage, children, or a normal life span)

D. Persistent symptoms of increased arousal (not present before the trauma), as

indicated by two (or more) of the following:

1. difficulty falling or staying asleep

2. irritability or outbursts of anger

3. difficulty concentrating

4. hypervigilance

5. exaggerated startle response

E. Duration of the disturbance (symptoms in Criteria B, C, and D) is more than one

month.

F. The disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social,

occupational, or other important areas of functioning.

Specify if:

Acute: if duration of symptoms is less than 3 months

Chronic: if duration of symptoms is 3 months or less

Specify if:

With Delayed Onset: if onset of symptoms is at least 6 months after the stressor.

389

Diagnostic criteria for Acute Stress Disorder (American Psychiatric Association,

2000, pp. 471–472)

A. The person has been exposed to a traumatic event in which both of the following

were present:

1. the person experienced, witnessed, or was confronted with an event or events

that involved actual or threatened death or serious injury, or a threat to the

physical integrity of self or others

2. the person’s response involved intense fear, helplessness, or horror

B. Either while experiencing or after experiencing the distressing event, the individual

has three (or more) of the following dissociative symptoms:

1. a subjective sense of numbing, detachment, or absence of emotional

responsiveness

2. a reduction in awareness of his or her surroundings (e.g., “being in a daze”)

3. derealisation

4. depersonalisation

5. dissociative amnesia (i.e., inability to recall an important aspect of the

trauma)

C. The traumatic event is persistently re-experienced in at least one of the following

ways: recurrent images, thoughts, dreams, illusions, flashback episodes, or a sense of

reliving the experience; or distress on exposure to reminders of the traumatic event.

D. Marked avoidance of stimuli that arouse recollections of the trauma (e.g., thoughts,

feelings, conversations, activities, places, people).

E. Marked symptoms of anxiety or increased arousal (e.g., difficulty sleeping,

irritability, poor concentration, hypervigilance, exaggerated startle response, motor

restlessness).

F. The disturbance causes clinically significant distress or impairment in social,

occupational, or other important areas of functioning or impairs the individual’s

ability to pursue some necessary task, such as obtaining necessary assistance or

390

mobilising personal resources by telling family members about the traumatic

experience.

G. The disturbance lasts for a minimum of 2 days and a maximum of 4 weeks and

occurs within 4 weeks of the traumatic event.

H. The disturbance is not due to the direct physiological effects of a substance (e.g., a

drug of abuse, a medication) or a general medical condition, is not better accounted

for by Brief Psychotic Disorder, and is not merely an exacerbation of a pre-

existing Axis I or Axis II disorder.

391

Appendix B: Interview guide for Karen key informants

Demographics

Date:

Name of community spokesperson:

Role of person:

Educational / professional background:

Sex:

Age group:

Length of stay in Australia:

Other info:

General orientation

1. I would like to know a little bit about their current situation. What is life in

Australia like for most of the Karen women that you know?

Note: check English skills, employment, visa status

2. Would you say it is a close community or are there many people who do not

have contact with other Karen?

3. Are there social clubs and other organisations that bring people together? Are

they well attended?

4. What language or languages are regularly spoken at home and at social

functions?

5. How do Karen women view Australian society? What aspects do they like

about Australia? What are the common complaints about Australian culture,

institutions or people?

6. Are there any services or businesses which are used regularly by Karen

women?

Services

I’m interested in how Karen women cope with their stress and emotions. I’m

especially interested in how the family, community and services can do a better job in

giving them the help and support they need.

392

7. In many ways mental and emotional health problems are not much different

from other health and social problems. I was wondering which health services

Karen women use? For example, do they go to particular doctors or clinics, do

they use special ethnic services?

8. What are the good things about the services that they use?

9. What are some of the complaints about these services that you have heard?

10. Are there specific problems that this group face in accessing services when

they are experiencing mental health problems? What are they?

11. In your opinion what are the main mental health issues facing the community?

12. What do you think should be done to help these problems?

13. Would you be willing to help me on this project by helping me get into contact

with people with these problems?

How did / does the community consider and react to mental health problems?

Has this changed in Australia?

Do people ask for emotional support / psychological support when they need it?

If yes, how are they seen by the their community?

Please describe the ways conflicts and disagreements are dealt with by people in the

current situation?

Optional prompt

Give a description of a hypothetical person with psychological symptoms…

(identified in interview).

Name and provide a detailed description of all the locally recognised syndromes such

a person might have.

Name and describe all the problems that these symptoms might represent.

393

Appendix C: Interview guide for service providers

Service background

1. First I would like to learn a little bit more about your service. Would you

please briefly describe your service.

Note:

o How long has it been operating?

o How is it funded and managed?

o How many staff and facilities does it have?

o What services are provided?

o How many people use the service?

o Has the number of people serviced or the type of services offered,

changed in the past 3-4 years? If so, what has been the change and why

did it occur?

2. My project is focusing particularly on Karen refugee women. Does this

service ever see Karen women?

3. Do other refugee communities from Burma use this service?

General questions re. Karen women’s background and experiences

What is life in Australia like for most of the Karen women that you know or are aware

of (note check re. English skills, visas etc.).

1. Are there some general observations that you can make about the differences

between the Karen women attending your service and Anglo-Australians?

And other ethnic groups?

2. Would you say it is a close community or are there many people who do not

have contact with other Karen?

394

3. Are there social clubs and other organisations that bring people together? Are

they well attended?

4. How do Karen women view Australian society? What aspects do they like

about Australia? What are the common complaints about Australian culture,

institutions or people?

5. Are there any services or businesses which are used regularly by Karen

women?

6. I was wondering which health services Karen women use? For example, do

they go to particular doctors or clinics, do they use special ethnic services?

Karen women and this service

Thank you for answering all of these questions. Now I would like to learn about your

experiences with Karen women (or other refugee women from Burma) in terms of

coming to this service.

1. What are the most common reasons Karen women (or other refugee

women) attend your service?

2. How are Karen women (or other refugee women) usually referred to your

service?

3. Which services do you usually offer to Karen women (or other refugee

women)?

4. For which of the services is there the greatest demand?

5. Are there services which you think are desirable, but which are not popular

with clients?

6. Are there services which clients ask for that this service cannot provide?

7. As far as you know, have staff at this service received any training in

cross-cultural issues? If yes, in what ways was the training helpful? If no,

in what ways do you think that such training would be helpful?

395

8. Does the service have any bilingual workers? If yes, what are their

responsibilities? What are the advantages of having them attached to your

service? Are there any disadvantages?

9. Does the service ever use an interpreter service? If yes, have you found

them to be effective? Have there been any problems? If not, how do you

deal with communication problems when they arise?

Mental health

In this project I am interested in how Karen women cope with stress and emotions. I

am especially interested in how the family, community and services can do a better

job in giving them the help and support they need.

10. In the course of regular work, is there ever any contact with Karen women

(or other refugee women) with mental health problems? If yes, what are

some typical examples?

11. In your opinion, what are the main mental health issues facing Karen

women?

12. Do you believe Karen women are more or less likely to experience mental

health problems than other refugee women? Than other Australians?

13. Do you believe that the mental health problems of Karen women are more

likely to go undetected?

14. Do you believe that they mental health problems of Karen women are

more likely to be inadequately treated?

15. Are workers in this service trained to recognise mental health problems. If

yes, what is the content of the training?

16. Is there anyone in your service with counselling or other mental health

skills?

17. Are people who may have mental health problems told about other

services? If yes, which services? Is there regular feedback and

communication between that service and this organisations? If not, are

workers aware of services which are available?

18. What are the good things about the services that they use or that are

available for them to use?

396

19. What are some of the complaints about these services that you have heard?

20. Are there specific problems that this group faces in accessing services

when they are experiencing mental health problems? What are they?

21. What do you think should be done to help these problems?

Would you be willing to help me on this project by helping me get into contact with

organisations or people familiar with these problems and issues?

397

Appendix D: Interview guide for Karen women

398

399

400

401

402

403

Appendix E: Participant information and consent form (English version

only)

HREC 08338

THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES

PARTICIPANT INFORMATION STATEMENT AND CONSENT FORM

A study of the emotional wellbeing and problems experienced by Karen refugees.

Study Description

This research is being conducted to explore how Karen women in Sydney feel about and experience the

impact of displacement. Many Karen people have experienced oppression due to decades of political

conflict in Burma. We hope to learn about the emotional and social impact from the perspective of

Karen people who are refugees in Australia. This study will be important in the sense that it will gather

ideas and expressions that come directly from the Karen people themselves.

You were invited as a possible participant in this study because you are a member of the Karen refugee

community in Sydney.

Taking Part In This Study

Interviews

If you decide to participate, we will ask you to participate in up to two individual or group interviews.

These interviews will take up to approximately two hours. Interviews will be conducted in your chosen

language and an interpreter will be made available.

Confidentiality

All information gathered during this study will be kept confidential. Your name will not be included or

mentioned on any published data.

404

Reimbursement

You will be provided with a $20 food voucher as a token of our appreciation.

Your consent

Your decision whether or not to participate will not prejudice your future relations with the University

of New South Wales. You are free to withdraw your consent and to discontinue participation at any

time without prejudice.

Feedback

When the study is complete you will be provided with a report summarising the results. In addition,

you will be invited to attend a community meeting where the results will be discussed and you may

participate in identifying research recommendations and actions to be taken.

405

THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES

Risks and Benefits

The personal benefits for taking part in this study include the chance to share and discuss your

experiences and knowledge. It is hoped that the information obtained from this study will be of use to

guide policy planning and for developing assistance programs for Karen refugees in Australia. We

cannot and do not guarantee or promise that you will receive any benefits from this study.

You may be asked about stressful situations that you have experienced and it is possible that you may

find discussing these matters distressing or upsetting. Should you feel distressed by this, you will be

assisted in seeking appropriate help.

Complaints

Complaints may be directed to the Ethics Secretariat, The University of New South Wales, Sydney

2052 Australia (phone 9385 4234, fax 9385 6648, email [email protected]). Any complaint you

make will be investigated promptly and you will be informed out the outcome.

Contact

If you have any questions, please feel free to ask us and we will be happy to answer them.

This research is being conducted by Paula Watkins, a doctoral candidate at the University of New

South Wales. She may be contacted at the School of Public Health and Community Medicine, Level 2

and 3, Samuels Building, Faculty of Medicine, UNSW Sydney 2052, Australia. Her telephone numbers

are:

9130 4880 and 0416 180 337, and her email is [email protected].

You will be given a copy of this form to keep.

406

THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES

PARTICIPANT INFORMATION STATEMENT AND CONSENT FORM

(continued)

A study of the emotional wellbeing and problems experienced by Karen refugees.

You are making a decision whether or not to participate. Your signature indicates that, having read the

information provided above, you have decided to participate.

…………………………………………………….…………………………………………………….

Signature of Research Participant Signature of Witness

…………………………………………………….…………………………………………………….

(Please PRINT name) (Please PRINT name)

…………………………………………………….…………………………………………………….

Date Nature of Witness

407

THE UNIVERSITY OF NEW SOUTH WALES

REVOCATION OF CONSENT

A study of the emotional wellbeing and problems experienced by Karen refugees

I hereby wish to WITHDRAW my consent to participate in the research proposal described above and

understand that such withdrawal WILL NOT jeopardise any treatment or my relationship with The

University of New South Wales or any organisations within the Karen Community.

…………………………………………………….…………………………………………………….

Signature Date

……………………………………………………

Please PRINT Name

The section for Revocation of Consent should be forwarded to Dr Husna Razee, School of Public

Health and Community Medicine, Level 2 and 3, Samuels Building, Faculty of Medicine, UNSW

Sydney 2052, Australia.

408

Appendix F: Group interview guide

409

410

411

412

413

414

Appendix G: Conversations with Karen: art exhibition

Media coverage

Media 1: Art shares stories of Burmese refugees (Perrin, 2011).

415

Media 2: Conversations with Karen (“Conversations with Karen, 2011).

416

Photos of event

Photo 1: Exhibition space

Photo 2: Dolls made by Karen women in Sydney were on display. After the

exhibition the dolls were sent to children in orphanages on the Thailand-Burma

border

417

Photo 3: Group of Karen women (faces blurred to protect anonymity) visiting the

exhibition and sharing lunch