2012 The applicability of the Theory of Planned Behaviour in predicting adherence to ART among a...

10
http://hpq.sagepub.com/ Journal of Health Psychology http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/362 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/1359105311416875 2012 17: 362 originally published online 12 August 2011 J Health Psychol Wylene Saal and Ashraf Kagee ART among a South African sample The applicability of the Theory of Planned Behaviour in predicting adherence to Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com can be found at: Journal of Health Psychology Additional services and information for http://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/362.refs.html Citations: What is This? - Aug 12, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record - Mar 15, 2012 Version of Record >> at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012 hpq.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Transcript of 2012 The applicability of the Theory of Planned Behaviour in predicting adherence to ART among a...

http://hpq.sagepub.com/Journal of Health Psychology

http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/362The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/1359105311416875

2012 17: 362 originally published online 12 August 2011J Health PsycholWylene Saal and Ashraf Kagee

ART among a South African sampleThe applicability of the Theory of Planned Behaviour in predicting adherence to

  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

can be found at:Journal of Health PsychologyAdditional services and information for     

  http://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/17/3/362.refs.htmlCitations:  

What is This? 

- Aug 12, 2011 OnlineFirst Version of Record 

- Mar 15, 2012Version of Record >>

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Journal of Health Psychology17(3) 362 –370© The Author(s) 2011 Reprints and permission: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/1359105311416875hpq.sagepub.com

Adherence to antiretroviral therapy is necessary to ensure positive health outcomes for persons living with HIV and AIDS, specifically in terms of minimizing the progression of the disease and thus reducing mortality (Boulle et al., 2008). Considerable evidence has shown that sub-optimal adherence, for example, failing to attend clinic appointments and take pills at the required times can lead to a low CD4 count, a high viral load, poor quality of life and ulti-mately death (Mannheimer et al., 2005). Poor adherence among patients enrolled in an antiret-roviral treatment programme may also increase patients’ risk of developing resistant strains of HIV, thus requiring the use of more expensive second-line drugs than first-line. As funding for ART programs is limited, ensuring optimal adherence is essential, not only to reduce patient

mortality and improve quality of life, but also to limit the wastage of drugs, clinicians’ time and clinic resources.

Several barriers to ART adherence have been identified, namely structural barriers associated with the environment in which patients live (Coetzee et al., 2010; Kagee and Delport, 2010; Kagee et al., 2010); mental health problems (Nel and Kagee, in press; Simoni et al., 2011); low levels of perceived social support (Holstad et al., 2006); substance abuse (Tucker et al., 2004); and concerns about stigma (Klitzman et al.,

The applicability of the Theory of Planned Behaviour in predicting adherence to ART among a South African sample

Wylene Saal and Ashraf Kagee

AbstractWe sought to determine the extent to which the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) was applicable in predicting medication adherence among South Africans receiving antiretroviral therapy (ART). Regression analyses revealed that the linear combination of attitudes towards adherence, perceived behavioural control and perceived group norms explained 12 percent of the variance in intentions to adhere to ART. We also found a non-significant relationship between intentions to adhere to treatment and self-reported adherence. The results call into question the extent to which TPB is helpful in understanding a health-promoting behaviour such as medication adherence among South Africans receiving ART.

Keywordsantiretroviral therapy, adherence, Theory of Planned Behaviour

Stellenbosch University, South Africa

Corresponding author:Ashraf Kagee, Stellenbosch University, Private bag X1, Stellenbosch 7700, South Africa. Email: [email protected]

416875 HPQXXX10.1177/1359105311416875Saal and KageeJournal of Health Psychology

Article

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Saal and Kagee 363

2004). Key factors associated with adherence that were identified in a meta-analysis included fear of disclosure, substance abuse, forgetfulness, suspicions of treatment, complexity of treat-ment, decreased quality of life and competing demands on time (Mills et al., 2006). Further, in a study testing the relationship between autono-mous regulation and ART adherence, this relationship was confirmed but mediated by self-efficacy (Lynam et al., 2009). Together, these findings bring into focus the need for autonomous motivation in ART users to be encouraged by health care personnel.

Adherence has been defined by the World Health Organisation (2003) as ‘the extent to which a person’s behaviour – taking medica-tion, following a diet, and/or executing lifestyle changes corresponds with agreed recommenda-tions from a health care provider’ (p. 3). A high rate of adherence, which is concerned with the correct dosage taken within the correct time period and in the correct manner, is necessary for treatment to be successful. Dose adherence refers to the number and proportion of doses taken, schedule adherence refers to adherence to doses taken on time and dietary adherence refers to doses taken correctly with food (Schonnesson et al., 2006). Under ideal circumstances all these dimensions of adherence are required. Non-adherence may thus take various forms, for example, not taking the medication at all, taking the medication at the wrong time, taking the wrong dose due to misunderstanding treatment directions, prematurely terminating the medica-tion, or adjusting the regimen to reduce side effects and toxicity without consulting the health provider (Miller, 1997). Preparatory behaviours that impede proper pill-taking adher-ence include not filling prescriptions, self-adjusting the regimen to modulate side effects and toxicities and incorrectly understanding the doctor’s instructions (Chesney, 2003).

Theories of health behaviour seek to explain why individuals engage in health-related behav-iours, which in the present study is ART adher-ence. The Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) postulates that volitional behaviour is

associated with the intentions to engage in a specific behaviour (Rhodes and Courneya, 2004), in this case correct ART adherence. Intentions in turn are influenced by attitudes, perceived behavioural control and perceived group norms about the behaviour (Ajzen 1991; 2002). The TPB has been found to be a useful predictor of health behaviour, including HIV/AIDS health behaviour (Janepanish et al., 2011; White et al., 2010). Its utility in previous research has suggested that it may be similarly applicable, at least in part, in the context of understanding ART adherence. To our knowl-edge, only two previous studies have used the TPB in predicting adherence among South African samples. Fincham and colleagues (2008) used the TPB to predict dietary and fluid adherence among haemodialysis patients and showed that while attitudes and perceived behavioural control (PBC) explained 15.5 per-cent of variance in self-reported adherence, the full model was not optimal in explaining vari-ance in self-reported adherence. Kagee and van der Merwe (2006) used the TPB to predict treat-ment adherence among patients seeking care for diabetes and hypertension at public health clinics in South Africa. These authors showed that the TPB explained 47 percent of the vari-ance intentions to adhere to treatment and that perceived behavioural control was the strongest predictor of intentions to adhere to treatment.

Yet, social cognitive theoretical models such as the TPB have received criticism for being overly focused on individual decision-making and therefore of limited use in non-western cul-tural contexts (Campbell, 2003). To this extent, it was considered useful to determine the extent to which the TPB was applicable in the South African cultural and political context.

There is a considerable body of research from many parts of the world to suggest that perceived stigma is associated with poor ART adherence (Dlamini et al., 2009; Klitzman et al., 2004). Also, it has been noted that patients fear-ing that others would discover their positive sta-tus would often incur considerable expense to travel to clinics far from their homes to avoid

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

364 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

being seen seeking care for their condition and would have to hide their medication to avoid discovery by family members (Coetzee and Kagee, 2010; Hardon et al., 2007). While it has been suggested that the availability of ART may play a role in changing in people’s perceptions of HIV/AIDS from that of a death sentence to a manageable chronic illness (Kagee, 2008), it is simultaneously assumed that perceived stigma is an important barrier to adherence.

In the context of many millions of persons living with HIV gaining increasing access to ART, understanding adherence among this pop-ulation is an important public health and psy-chological concern. The aim of the present study was to determine the extent to which the TPB, together with perceived stigma, could explain adherence behavior among patients receiving antiretroviral care.

Method

Participants

One-hundred-and-one patients receiving anti-retroviral treatment were recruited by means of convenience sampling at a peri-urban public hos-pital in South Africa. The clinics provided treat-ment for HIV including anti-retroviral treatment (ARVs) to patients. The following inclusion cri-teria were applied: (a) only participants aged 18 years of age and older were included in the study; (b) participants were able to understand either spoken English or Afrikaans; (c) participants were physically and psychologically capable of engaging in an interview and completing a bat-tery of self-report instruments. Patients who indi-cated they had been diagnosed with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia or related psychotic dis-orders were excluded from the study.

Procedure

Patients seeking ART services were informed by clinic nurses about the research study. They were told that if they were interested in learn-ing more about the study, they could approach

the researcher in a private room where the study would be explained to them. Patients who agreed to participate in the study were asked to sign an informed consent form, fol-lowing which they were asked to complete a questionnaire battery administered in English, Afrikaans or Xhosa.

Upon completion of the questionnaire, each participant received a supermarket voucher as a token of thanks for their participation. On the recommendation of the clinic director we also offered snacks to all patients, regardless of whether they participated in the study and a supermarket voucher to the clinic nurses for handing out flyers about the study to patients. Ethical clearance was obtained from the Stellenbosch University Health Research Ethics Committee and permission to conduct the research was obtained from the Western Cape Department of Health, the hospital superinten-dent and the clinic director.

Instruments

Attitudes toward treatment adherence. Two sub-scales of the Adherence Attitude Inventory were used to test attitudes toward treatment adherence. The Adherence Attitude Inventory is a 28-item, Likert-type instrument (Lewis and Abell, 2002), that assesses cognitive function-ing, patients/health worker communication, self-efficacy and commitment to treatment adherence. The reliability of the instrument as indicated by Cronbach alpha was 0.75 (Lewis and Abell, 2002). The subscales Commitment to Adherence and Patient-Provider Communi-cation subscales were used in the study.

Perceived subjective norms. An eight-item questionnaire was developed to calculate per-ceived subjective norms (with four Likert scale responses ranging from ‘strongly disa-gree’ to ‘strongly agree’). A similar question-naire was used in a study conducted by Kagee et al. (2008) and the Cronbach alpha was 0.61. Their questionnaire was modified to apply ART users.

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Saal and Kagee 365

Perceived behavioural control (PBC). PBC was measured using an eight-item questionnaire assessing self-efficacy to engage in adherence-related activities. A similar questionnaire used in a study by Kagee and van der Merwe (2006) was adapted for ART users.

Intentions to adhere to treatment. An eight-item, three-response-option Likert-type scale was constructed to measure intentions to engage in various adherence-related activities. A simi-lar questionnaire used by a study conducted by Kagee and van der Merwe (2006) had a Cron-bach alpha of 0.72.

Perceived HIV stigma. The HIV stigma scale was used in the present study to assess per-ceived stigma (Berger et al., 2001). This 40-item scale includes four subscales: Personalized stigma, disclosure concern, negative self-image and concern with public attitudes. The overall Cronbach alpha for the HIV Stigma Scale is 0.96 and the alphas for the subscales ranged from 0.90 to 0.93 (Berger et al., 2001).

Self-reported adherence. The self-reported adher-ence measure was used to assess adherence (Morisky et al., 1986). The scale questions are: Do you ever forget to take your medicine? Are you careless at times about taking your medicine? When you feel better do you sometimes stop tak-ing your medicine? Sometimes if you feel worse when you take the medicine, do you stop taking it? In the original study by Morisky et al. the alpha reliability of the scale was shown to be 0.61.

Data analysis

All statistical procedures were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). Frequencies, percentages, means, stand-ard deviations and ranges were calculated for the various independent variables. Base 10 and natu-ral base logarithmic transformations were per-formed in order to normalise skewed distributions. Multicollinearity between predictor variables was assessed using the Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) statistic. Hierarchical regression analysis

was used to test the linear combination of the TPB variables on intentions to adhere to antiret-roviral medication.

Results

Description of the sample

A total of 121 patients were invited to enrol in the study of which 101 agreed to participate. The mean age was 35 years (SD = 7.05 years). Most of the sample, (82.2%) were female; 66 percent were Black, and 42.6 percent stated they were unemployed. Of the total sample, 45 percent stated they were single, 11 percent stated they were widowed, six percent were separated, nine percent were divorced, and 29 percent were married or living together. In terms of annual family income, 42 percent reported less than ZAR10,000; 17 percent reported between ZAR10,000 and ZAR40,000: 6 percent reported more than ZAR40,000; and 35 percent stated that they did not know their annual family income.

Data screening

Tests of collinearity revealed that this was not a concern as correlations among predictors were less than 0.80 and no Variance Inflation Factor was greater than 10. The sample sizes, means, standard deviations, ranges of the key variables are presented in Table 1. The internal consisten-cies of various instruments as measured by Cronbach’s alpha and the inter-correlations between the variables are reported in Tables 2 and 3, respectively. The measure of self-reported adherence originally had modest internal consist-ency (α = 0.56), which improved to 0.59 when two were removed from the analysis, namely, for-got to take medication during the last two weeks and forgot to take medication over the weekend.

Predicting intentions to adhere to ART

Table 4 presents the summary of the hierarchi-cal regression analysis for the variables that

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

366 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

predicted intentions to adhere. Table 5 presents the regression statistics for the two models that were tested, namely, the TPB vari-ables in predicting intentions at Step 1, and the TPB variables as well as perceived stigma at Step 2. The linear combination of the TPB vari-ables in Step 1 significantly explained 12 per-cent (R2 = 0.12; F (3, 97) = 4.52, p = 0.01) of the variance in intentions to adhere to treatment. Although the addition of perceived stigma explained an additional three percent of the

variance in intentions, this addition was non-significant: F (1, 96) = 3.09, p = 0.08. We also tested the relationship between intentions to adhere and self-reported adherence but this cor-relation coefficient was also non-significant. A further test of the linear combination of the pre-dictor variables in explaining self-reported adherence indicated a non-significant result.

Discussion

The TPB was able to explain 12 percent of the variance in intentions to adhere to antiretroviral treatment, a medium effect size for multivariate models in the social sciences (Cohen, 1988). The correlation between intentions and reported adherence was non-significant, however. These results suggests that the linear combination of attitudes towards antiretroviral treatment, perceived behavioral control and perceived subjective norms was able to account for a significant, albeit modest, proportion of the variance in behavioral intentions.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of key variables.

N M SD Range

Attitude towards adherence 95 38.42 9.55 14–51Perceived subjective norms 96 23.22 4.86 9–32Perceived behavioural control 98 22.52 2.57 8–24Intentions 99 21.49 3.51 8–24Perceived stigma 84 90.78 20.34 46–153Self-report adherence 101 3.61 .93 0–4

Table 2. Cronbach’s alpha of the measures.

Variable Cronbach’s alpha

Intentions .94Attitudes towards treatment .86Perceived subjective norms .75Perceived behavioural control .81Perceived stigma .93Self-reported adherence .59

Table 3. Correlation Matrix of TPB variables and self-reported adherence.

Intentions Attitudes towards adherence

PSN PBC PSTIG Self-report adherence

Intentions 1 Attitudes .21* 1 PSN .15 .05 1 PBC .35** .28** .14 1 PSTIG -.24* .01 .11 -.30** 1 Self-report adherence

-.05 .21* .03 .09 -.05 1

*Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (two-tailed)**Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (two-tailed)

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Saal and Kagee 367

The TPB can be used to understand intentions to engage in adherence to ART. These data add to the growing number of studies showing the utility of TBP variables in predicting adherence to various prescribed health behaviours, for example, dietary and fluid intake (Fincham et al., 2008); medication for diabetes and hyper-tension (Kagee and van der Merwe, 2006); exercise-based therapy in rehabilitation (Yardley and Donovan-Hall, 2007); and exercise – rehabilitation therapy among cardiac patients (Blanchard et al., 2002). The study results

indicate that when considering intervention options to enhance adherence, TPB variables such as attitudes, perceived behavioural control and group norms may need to be taken into account.

We were surprised by the non-significant relationship between perceived stigma and intentions. It is possible that this finding may be peculiar to the sample who had received psy-chosocial services, which included how to deal with stigma, from a non-governmental organi-zation allied with the treatment clinic. It is also possible that a rational construct such as inten-tions was non-orthogonally related with per-ceived stigma, thus accounting for this result. In other words, the TPB and related constructs may be inappropriately applied in the context where the study was conducted. Such a view is echoed in Marks’ (2008) criticism of the TPB, observing that theories such as the TPB may not reflect the complexity and interplay among social, cultural, economic and political factors that influence health behavior. Indeed, social cognitive theoretical models such as the TPB and the Health Belief Model have been criti-cized for being individualistic in focus and of

Table 4. Summary of Hierarchical multiple regression analysis for variables predicting intentions to adhere to treatment (Regression model 1 and 2).

Block R R2 DR2 Std error F df1 df2 p

A 0.35 0.12 0.12 3.31 4.52 3 97 0.01B 0.39 0.15 0.03 3.27 3.09 1 96 0.08

A. Predictors: (Constant), Attitudes towards treatment adherence, Subjective norms, Perceived behavioural control

B. Predictors: (Constant), Attitudes towards treatment adherence, Perceived subjective norms, Perceived behavioural control, Perceived stigma

C. Dependent variable: Intentions to adhere to treatment

Table 5. Parameters for variables predicting intentions to adhere to treatment (N = 101).

Model Unstandardized StandardizedBeta coefficients

t Sig. 95% CI Collinearity statistics

B Std. error LL UL Tolerance VIF

1 Constant 9.76 3.25 3.00 .00 3.31 16.22 Attitudesb .05 .04 .12 1.24 .27 -.03 .12 .92 1.08 PSNc .08 .07 .11 1.13 .26 -.06 .22 .98 1.02 PBCd .36 .14 .26 2.63 .01 .09 .63 .91 1.102 Constant 13.79 3.95 3.50 .00 5.95 21.62 Attitudesb .05 .04 .14 1.40 .17 -.02 .12 .92 1.09 PSNc .10 .07 .13 1.40 .17 -.04 .24 .96 1.04 PBCd .29 .14 .21 2.01 .05 .004 .57 .83 1.21 PSTIGe -.03 .02 -.17 -1.76 .08 -.07 .004 .90 1.11

Note: CI = confidence interval; LL = lower limit, UL = upper limit.a. Dependent variable: Intentions to adhere to treatmentb. Attitudes towards treatment adherencec. Perceived subjective normsd. Perceived behavioural controle. Perceived stigma

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

368 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

limited applicability in countries that make up the global South (Campbell, 2003). It has been argued that individual and intra-psychic factors insufficiently consider the socio-cultural com-plexity needed to foster a health-enabling com-munity within which persons living with HIV can engage in health behaviours such as adher-ence. The unexplained variance in adherence intentions may thus in part be due to the context within which ART-users live. Structural condi-tions specific to resource-constrained envi-ronments such as poor access to affordable transport, long waiting times at clinics, poor interpersonal interaction with health care per-sonnel and stigma (Coetzee et al., 2011; Kagee and Delport, 2010) may exist as additional extra-individual barriers to adherence inten-tions that lie beyond the TPB’s ability to explain. To this extent, contextual factors, such as the nature of the public health care system in South Africa and other resource-constrained countries, may also play an important role in determining adherence intentions. It is at this nexus, between cognitive processes and the contextual factors that create a health enabling environment, that further theoretical, concep-tual and empirical research is needed to under-stand adherence intentions and behaviour. The findings of the study underscore Marks’ (1996) call for placing health psychology theories, of which the TPB is an example, into its sociopo-litical and community context and it is likely that the utility of the theory will have limita-tions if applied in a de-contextualized manner. The data of this study, along with the substantial body of literature critical of the TPB (e.g. Marks, 2008; 1996) call into question the presumed utility of the TPB in resource-constrained contexts.

We also found a non-significant relationship between intentions and self-reported adherence. These findings are similar to those of Fincham et al (2008) and Kagee and van der Merwe (2006). Among possible explanations for this apparent lack of association are recall bias and social desirability bias. Recall bias occurs when a respondent’s answer is influenced by his or her memory of the event rather than actual

observation of the event. Social desirability bias occurs when respondents tailor their answers to questions in a manner that is in keeping with what they think the questioner wishes the answer to be. In the present sample, the self-reported adherence questionnaire had an inter-nal consistency of 0.59 which did not meet the criterion of 0.70, recommended by Field (2000). After the necessary items were deleted from the questionnaire, the internal consistency could still not be improved. It is possible that a con-struct such as self-reported adherence may be vulnerable to the biases mentioned previously and that a construct as complex as adherence may need to be assessed in a more comprehen-sive manner, such as electronic monitoring of pill-container activity (e.g. Lyimo et al., 2011).

In addition to these biases may be the possi-bility that intentions to adhere simply do not correlate with actual self-reported behaviour, signaling the possibility that the model may not be applicable beyond the point of intentions in the context of ART adherence. Additional research is necessary to evaluate this possibility by, for example, conducting assessment of actual adherence in real time through the use of electronic diaries, cellular technology, or daily assessment so as to minimize the likelihood of recall bias.

The study was part of a longer cross-sectional investigation into the correlates of poor adher-ence to antiretroviral treatment. The study ques-tionnaire took on average almost one hour to complete, introducing the possibility of respond-ent fatigue. As a consequence, participants may have concentrated less well on questions occur-ring at the tail end of the questionnaire. Also, as is the case in many public health clinics in sub-Saharan Africa, the sample consisted mainly of women and may have limited generalizability to male ART users.

Adherence is assumed by some theorists to result from rational social-cognitive decision-making processes (Ogden, 2000). Rational deci-sion-making models, of which the TPB is an example, often do not take into consideration the role of personality, unconscious processes,

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Saal and Kagee 369

mental status and socio-political context. These factors could potentially play an important role in furthering understanding of adherence. To this extent the role of these factors in explaining ART non-adherence awaits further investigation.

ReferencesAjzen I (1991) The theory of planned behavior.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50: 179–211.

Ajzen I (2002) Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psy-chology 32(4): 665–683.

Boulle A, Bock P, Osler M, Cohen K, Channing L, Hilderbrand K, et al. (2008) Antiretroviral ther-apy and early mortality in South Africa. Bulletin of the World Health Organisation 86: 678–687.

Berger BE, Ferrans CE and Lashley FR (2001) Mea-suring stigma in people with HIV: Psychometric assessment of the HIV stigma scale. Research in Nursing and Health 24: 518–529.

Blancherd CM, Courneya KS, Rodgers WM, Daub B and Knapkik G (2002) Determinants of exercise intention and behaviour during and after phase 2 cardiac rehabilitation: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Rehabilitation Psychology 47: 308–323.

Campbell C (2003) Letting them Die: How HIV/AIDS Prevention Programmes Often Fail. Wet-ton, South Africa: Double Storey Books.

Chesney M (2003) AIDS patient care and STDs. 17: 169–177.

Coetzee B, Kagee A and Vermeulen N (2011) Structural barriers to adherence to antiretroviral therapy in a resource-constrained setting. The perspectives of health care providers. AIDS Care 23: 146–151.

Cohen J (1988) Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Science (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Dlamini PS,Wantland D, Makoae LN, Chirwa M, Kohi TW, Greeff M, et al. (2009) HIV stigma and missed medications in HIV-positive people in five African countries. AIDS Patient Care & STDs 23(5): 377–387.

Field A (2000) Discovering Statistics: Using SPSS for Windows. London: Sage.

Fincham D, Kagee A and Moosa R (2008) Dietary and fluid adherence among haemodialysis patients attending public sector hospitals in the

Western Cape. South African Journal of Clinical Nutrient 21(2): 00-00.

Hardon AP, Akurut D, Comoro C, Ekezie C, Irunde HF, Gerrits, T, et al. (2007) Hunger, waiting time and transport costs: time to confront challenges to ART adherence in Africa. AIDS Care 19(5): 658–665.

Holstad MK, Pace JC, De AK and Ura DR (2006) Factors associated with adherence to antiretrovi-ral therapy. Journal of the Association of Nurses in AIDS Care 17: 4–15.

Janepanish P, Dancy BL and Park C (2011) Consistent condom use among Thai heterosexual adult males in Bangkok, Thailand. AIDS Care 23: 460–466.

Kagee A and Delport T (2010) Barriers to adherence to antiretroviral treatment: The perspectives of patient advocates. Journal of Health Psychology 15: 1001–1011.

Kagee A and van der Merwe M (2006) Predicting treatment adherence among patients attending primary health care clinics: The utility of the Theory of Planned Behavior. South African Jour-nal of Psychology 36(4): 699–714.

Kagee, A. (2008). Symptoms of depression and anx-iety among a sample of South African patients living with a chronic illness. Journal of Health Psychology, 13, 541–549.

Kagee A, Remien RH, Berkman A, Hoffman S, Campos L and Swartz L (2010) Structural barri-ers to ART adherence: Challenges and potential ways forward. Global Public Health 1–15.

Klitzman RL, Kirshenbaum SB, Dodge B, Remien RH, Ehrhardt AA, Johnson MO, et al. (2004) Intricacies and inter-relationships between HIV disclosure and HAART: A qualitative study. AIDS Care 16: 628–640.

Lewis SJ and Abell N (2002) Development and evaluation of the Adherence Attitude Inventory. Research on Social Work Practice 12: 107–123.

Lyimo RA, van den Boogaard J, Msoka E, Hospers HJ, van der Ven A, Mushi D, et al. (2011) Mea-suring adherence to antiretroviral therapy in northern Tanzania: feasibility and acceptability of the Medication Event Monitoring System. BMC Public Health 11: 92.

Lynam I, Catley D, Goggin K, Rabinowitz JL, Gerkovich MM, Williams K, et al. (2009) Auton-omous regulation and locus of control as pre-dictors of antiretroviral medication adherence. Journal of Health Psychology 14: 578–586.

Mannheimer SB, Matts J, Telzak E, Chesney M, Child C, Wu AW, et al. (2005) Quality of life in

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from

370 Journal of Health Psychology 17(3)

HIV-infected individuals receiving antiretroviral therapy is related to adherence. AIDS Care 17: 10–22.

Marks DF (1996) Health psychology in context. Journal of Health Psychology 1: 7–21.

Marks DF (2008) The quest for meaningful theory in health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology 13: 977.

Miller NH (1997) Compliance in treatment regimens in chronic asymptomatic diseases. American Journal of Medicine 102: 43–49.

Mills EJ, Nachega JB, Buchan I, Orbinski J, Atta-ran A, Singh S, et al. (2006) Adherence to anti-retroviral therapy in sub-Saharan Africa and North America: A meta-analysis. JAMA 296: 679–690.

Morisky DE, Green LW and Levine DM (1986) Con-current and predictive validity of a self-reported measure of medication adherence. Medical Care 24: 67–74.

Nel A and Kagee A (in press) Common mental health problems and ART adherence. AIDS Care.

Ogden J (2000) Health Psychology: A Textbook. Groot Brittanje: Biddles.

Rhodes RE and Courneya KS (2004) Differentiating motivation and control in the theory of planned behavior. Psychology, Health, and Medicine 9: 205–215.

Schonnesson LN, Diamond PM, Ross MW, Wil-liams M and Bratt G (2006) Baseline predictors of three types of antiretroviral therapy (ART) adherence: A two year follow up. AIDS Care 18: 406–414.

Simoni JM, Safren SA, Manhart LE, Lyda K, Gross-man CI, Rao D, et al. (2011) Challenges in addressing depression in HIV research: Assess-ment, cultural context, and methods. AIDS and Behavior 15: 376–388.

Tucker JS, Orlando M, Burnam MA, Sherbourne CD, Kung FY and Gifford AL (2004) Psycho-social mediators of antiretroviral nonadherence in HIV-positive adults with substance use and mental health problems. Health Psychology 23: 363–370.

White KM, Terry DJ, Troup C, Rempel LA and Norman P (2010) Predicting the consumption of foods low in saturated fats among people diag-nosed with Type 2 diabetes and cardiovascular disease: The role of planning in the theory of planned behaviour. Appetite 55: 348–354.

World Health Organization (2003) Adherence to Long-term Therapies: Evidence for Action. Geneva: Author.

Yardley L and Donovan-Hall M (2007) Predicting adherence to exercise-based therapy in rehabili-tation. Rehabilitation Psychology 52: 56–64.

at University of Stellenbosch on April 8, 2012hpq.sagepub.comDownloaded from