Download - An Experimental Examination of Stress Reactivity in Adolescents and Young Adults With Asthma

Transcript

This article was downloaded by: [Oklahoma State University]On: 20 February 2012, At: 11:01Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Children's Health CarePublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hchc20

An Experimental Examinationof Stress Reactivity inAdolescents and Young AdultsWith AsthmaAngelica R. Eddington a , Larry L. Mullins a , JenniferByrd-Craven a & John M. Chaney aa Department of Psychology, Oklahoma StateUniversity, Stillwater, OK

Available online: 17 Feb 2012

To cite this article: Angelica R. Eddington, Larry L. Mullins, Jennifer Byrd-Craven& John M. Chaney (2012): An Experimental Examination of Stress Reactivity inAdolescents and Young Adults With Asthma, Children's Health Care, 41:1, 16-31

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02739615.2012.643287

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up todate. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liablefor any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages

whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith or arising out of the use of this material.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

Children’s Health Care, 41:16–31, 2012

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 0273-9615 print/1532-6888 online

DOI: 10.1080/02739615.2012.643287

An Experimental Examination of StressReactivity in Adolescents and Young

Adults With Asthma

Angelica R. Eddington, Larry L. Mullins,Jennifer Byrd-Craven, and John M. Chaney

Department of Psychology, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater, OK

Despite advancements in treatment for asthma, the adolescent and young adult

(AYA) population continues to face a host of psychological challenges when com-

pared to healthy controls. In addition, unusually high mortality and morbidity rates

for asthma are present in the AYA population. Some researchers have suggested an

examination of stress reactivity to shed light on these differences. Using a learned

helplessness task, this study investigated the differences in cortisol elevation in

a sample of college students with asthma and their healthy controls. Results

supported an initial elevation of cortisol in asthma participants not evident in

the healthy control population. Notably, neither college students with asthma nor

healthy controls perceived the learned helplessness task to be stressful. Findings

suggest that college students with asthma may be more reactive to novel envi-

ronments when compared to healthy controls, as evidenced by increased cortisol

levels. Future studies on the differences in stress reactivity in individuals with

asthma are warranted.

Asthma, the most common chronic illness in the United States, is a chronic

inflammatory disorder of the airway in which individuals experience recurrent

episodes of wheezing, breathlessness, chest tightness, and cough (National Heart,

Lung, and Blood Institute [NHLBI], 2008). Traditionally viewed as an illness of

younger children, recent studies have clearly demonstrated high asthma preva-

Correspondence should be addressed to Angelica R. Eddington, Department of Psychology,

Oklahoma State University, 116 North Murray Hall, Stillwater, OK 74078. E-mail: angelica.

[email protected]

16

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 17

lence rates in older children and young adolescents aged 11 to 17 (Akinbami,

Moorman, Garbe, & Sondik, 2009). Although some individuals report that

they experience a reduction in symptoms as they enter adolescence, studies

have documented that this decrease does not happen as often as once thought

(Carpentier, Mullins, & Van Pelt, 2007).

Unfortunately, little research has focused on psychosocial aspects of asthma

in the adolescent and young adult (AYA) population, although studies have

shown that, as a group, they still experience significant life challenges (e.g.,

Hommel et al., 2003; Mullins, Chaney, Pace, & Hartman, 1997; Perez-Yarza,

1996). Adults and adolescents with asthma have been documented to be at

greater risk for engaging in substance abuse (Forero, Bauman, Young, Booth, &

Nutbeam, 1996), experience high rates of school absenteeism (American Lung

Association, 2007), and struggle with unemployment (Taitel, Allen, & Creer,

1998) when compared to same-aged peers. Notably, a significant number of

college students are hospitalized each year for reasons secondary to their asthma

(Jolicoeur, Pharm, Boyer, Reeder, & Turner, 1994). At the same time, 40% of

college students with asthma report that they do not seek medical attention even

if they feel that their symptoms are severe enough for medical care (Jolicoeur

et al., 1994). Thus, it would appear that older AYAs continue to face a host

of challenges related to the experience of asthma. As a result, researchers have

increasingly turned to the examination of factors that place these individuals

at risk.

In this vein, a number studies have investigated cognitive deficits in function-

ing that potentially result from the unpredictable nature of asthma. One line of

research has utilized “learned helplessness theory” as a means of understanding

the impact of the unpredictability of asthma and the stress that accompanies

this disease (Chaney et al., 1999; Mullins et al., 1997). Using an experimentally

induced learned helplessness paradigm, Chaney et al. provided evidence that

older adolescents with childhood-onset asthma, who had experienced a non-

contingent computer (i.e., non-controllable/unsolvable) concept formation task,

demonstrated more performance deficits on a subsequent anagram task than age-

matched healthy controls. In addition, a pre–post examination of causal attribu-

tions revealed an increase in internal attributions among college students with

asthma in the uncontrollable condition, whereas healthy controls displayed more

external attributions for failure (Chaney et al., 1999). Such results suggest that

the unpredictable and variable nature of asthma may indeed lead to an increased

cognitive vulnerability to helplessness under conditions of non-contingency or

stress. It is interesting to note that little research, to date, has focused on the

psycho-physiologic aspects of stress reactivity in youth with asthma, particularly

within an experimental context. Thus, it remains to be seen whether individuals

with asthma also respond differentially in terms of their physiological responses

to stress compared to healthy controls.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

18 EDDINGTON ET AL.

To date, stress reactivity and associated cortisol elevations have witnessed

limited research in youth with chronic illnesses. Although stress reactivity may

include many complex patterns, its most basic definition applies to the difference

between baseline arousal and stress-induced arousal (Bauer, Quas, & Boyce,

2002). Although still developing, research suggest that stress has been linked

to the onset, development, and morbidity of asthma in children (Mrazek et al.,

1999; Randolph & Fraser, 1999; Wade et al., 1997), as well as exacerbation of

symptoms (Joachim et al., 2003). In recent years, researchers have increasingly

utilized salivary cortisol, a biomarker of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA)

axis activity often initiated by psychosocial stress (Aardal-Eriksson, Karlberg,

& Holm, 1998; Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994; Masharani, et al., 2005), as

a measure of stress reactivity. The degree of activation depends, in part, on

the unpredictable, uncontrollable, or novel nature of a situation, anticipation,

or ego involvement (Kudielka, Hellhammer, & Kirschbaum, 2007). Vedhara

et al. (2007) hypothesized that early childhood illness could have a long-lasting

effect on activity of the HPA axis in adulthood, and increased activity of the

HPA axis following stressors. It is interesting to note that they found that

childhood respiratory illnesses were associated with reduced HPA axis activity

later in adulthood. Recently, cortisol secretion has been studied in populations of

individuals with asthma. Masharani et al. found support for the normal diurnal

pattern of cortisol secretion in a sample of adults with varying asthma severity.

In addition, after collecting salivary cortisol samples from the participants 30

min and then 12 hr after awakening, they found significantly higher cortisol

levels in the morning samples. They also investigated the impact of prescribed

glucocorticoid from inhaled, nasal, or oral usage on cortisol secretion. The results

showed that external glucocorticoid use suppressed salivary cortisol secretion,

especially in the 30-min-after-awakening samples. Such results suggest that indi-

viduals with asthma demonstrate similar cortisol patterns, as healthy controls and

glucocorticoid use is effective, as should be, in suppressing cortisol elevations.

Hence, increases in cortisol among individuals with asthma should be attributed

to other factors.

Wolf, Nicholls, and Chen (2008) investigated cortisol levels, salivary ˛-

amylase (an enzyme that reflects stress-responsive sympatho-adrenal medullary

[SAM] axis activity) levels, and stress in healthy children and adolescents

and children and adolescents with asthma. In healthy children, chronic stress

was associated with flatter cortisol slopes. Higher chronic stress among chil-

dren with asthma was associated with lower daily ˛-amylase, thus indicat-

ing lower sympathetic activity and implying increased susceptibility to symp-

tom exacerbations. In addition, their study supported a reversed secretion pat-

tern of cortisol and ˛-amylase, and that only salivary ˛-amylase patterns dif-

fered between children with asthma and healthy children, with asthmatic chil-

dren showing lower levels. Wolf et al. hypothesized that increased sympathetic

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 19

system activity could potentially protect against asthma symptom exacerba-

tions.

Researchers have suggested that the use of self-report inventories alone may

limit the conclusions that we can draw concerning the psychological effects of

having asthma (Gillaspy, Hoff, Mullins, Van Pelt, & Chaney, 2002; Mullins,

Chaney, Balderson, & Hommel, 2000); therefore, the use of salivary cortisol to

investigate psycho-physiological outcomes appears to be warranted. Indeed, in

the context of asthma, a better understanding of stress reactivity would yield

important information about the potential long-term physical and psychological

outcomes, especially because psychological risk factors have been postulated to

account for increased morbidity and mortality rates (Kang & Fox, 2000). Thus,

the primary aim of this study was designed to extend the work of Chaney et al.

(1999) in a sample of college students with asthma utilizing salivary cortisol as

a measure of stress reactivity.

In this study, we used a learned helplessness paradigm task with either a

controllable (contingent) or uncontrollable (non-contingent) condition. Cortisol

levels were taken at three different time points (prior to the manipulation [T1],

immediately after manipulation [T2], and 15 min post manipulation [T3]). Given

that cortisol is a biomarker of psychosocial (Kirschbaum & Hellhammer, 1994)

and uncontrollable stress (de Boer, de Beun, Slangen, & Van der Gugten, 1990;

Henry, 1986; Huether, 1998; Vogel, 1985), we hypothesized different cortisol

elevation patterns for participants with asthma and their healthy controls for each

collection point during the learned helplessness task (Chaney et al., 1999) as a

function of the experimental manipulation. Because stress has been associated

with the development of asthma and maintenance of symptoms (Randolph &

Fraser, 1999; Wade et al., 1997), we hypothesized, at the first collection point

(T1), that college students with childhood-onset asthma would exhibit higher

cortisol levels than healthy controls in both experimental conditions. In the non-

contingent condition, we predicted that cortisol levels at T2 would increase in

both college students with childhood-onset asthma and their healthy controls in

response to the uncontrollable stress (Vogle, 1985), with steeper elevations in

participants with asthma given that healthy controls have been found to exhibit

lower cortisol levels when exposed to stress than individuals with asthma (Wolf

et al., 2008). We expected this increase in both participant groups in the non-

contingent condition to decrease at T3, as research has supported a decline of

cortisol within 10 to 15 min after a stressor (Sapolsky, Romero, & Munck,

2000).

In the contingent (controllable) condition, we predicted that cortisol levels of

both college students with childhood-onset asthma and healthy controls would

not show increases at T2, assuming that this condition will elicit less stress or

learned helplessness in the participants (see Chaney et al., 1999). In addition,

in the contingent condition, we expected T2 and T3 cortisol levels for both

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

20 EDDINGTON ET AL.

participants with asthma and healthy controls to remain fairly consistent with

T1 cortisol levels, also assuming that this condition will elicit less stress and,

hence, be less cortisol reactive (de Boer et al., 1990; Henry, 1986; Huether,

1998; Vogel, 1985).

METHOD

Participants

College students with and without a history of asthma were recruited from a

Midwestern university through psychology courses or the health center. Partic-

ipants in the asthma group were 30 college students between the ages of 18

and 24 (M D 20:00, SD D 1:39), who self-identified as having childhood-

onset asthma (prior to the age of 12). Using a self-report, 7-point Likert scale

of asthma severity based on NHLBI (2008) guidelines, 46.7% of participants

with asthma reported having intermittent symptoms, 23.3% reported having mild

symptoms, 10% reported having moderate symptoms, and 5% reported having

severe symptoms associated with asthma. In addition, 83.3% of the chronic

population reported currently taking medication for their asthma, and 46.7%

reported currently receiving medical treatment from a physician. Most of the

sample (90%) reported that their symptoms were controllable. The group with

asthma included 18 women (60.0%) and 12 men (40.0%). The majority of

participants self-identified as Caucasian (83.3%; n D 25), followed by Native

American (13.3%; n D 4).

Participants in the control group were 30 college students between the ages

of 18 and 24 (M D 19:70, SD D 1:34), who denied any history of asthma or

major chronic illnesses. The control group included 18 women (60.0%) and 12

men (40.0%). The majority of participants self-identified as Caucasian (80.0%;

n D 24), followed by Native Americans (10.0%; n D 3) and Hispanics/Latinos

(6.7%; n D 2). The distribution of ethnicities reflects the demographic makeup

of the larger college population at this particular institution.

Materials

Demographic form. A demographic questionnaire was designed for this

study that inquired about the participant’s gender, age, ethnicity, number of

years of education completed, household income, marital status, current chronic

illness, and medication history.

Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS; Zung, 1971). The SAS is a

20-item (e.g., “I feel afraid for no reason at all,” and “I can feel my heart

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 21

beating fast”), self-report measure that assessed anxiety. Each question infers

about anxious behavior, and is scored on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (none

or a little of the time), 2 (some of the time), 3 (a good part of the time),

to 4 (most of the time). The overall score from the SAS was used to assess

participants’ levels of anxiety, with higher scores reflecting greater anxiety.

Scores range from 20 to 80, with 20 through 44 indicating normal anxiety

levels, 45 through 59 indicating mild to moderate anxiety levels, 60 through

74 indicating marked to severe anxiety levels, and 75 through 80 indicating

extreme anxiety levels. This measure has been found to have high internal

consistency in community samples (.79; Knight, Waal-Manning, & Spears, 1983)

and depressed patients (.88; Gabreys & Peters, 1985). Cronbach’s alpha for this

sample was .82.

Center for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression (CES–D) Scale (Radloff,1977). The CES–D is a 20-item, self-report measure that assesses depressive

symptoms and behaviors during the past week. For each item, the respondents

rate how much they experienced depressive and non-depressive feelings during

the week (e.g., “I did not feel like eating; my appetite was poor,” and “I felt

I was just as good as other people”). All 20 items were summed to reflect a

total score. The total score is used to assess participants’ levels of depression,

with higher scores being indicative of greater levels of depression. A score > 16

indicates a clinically significant level of psychological distress. The measure has

demonstrated high internal consistency estimates (.85–.90) in general and patient

populations (Radloff, 1977). This sample had a Cronbach’s alpha of .89.

Saliva collection materials and sampling. All salvia collections were

taken between 12:00 p.m. and 5:00 p.m. to control the influence of the natural

diurnal pattern (Gibson et al., 1999). Because it is well-established that cortisol

levels can peak and decline within 10 to 15 min (Sapolsky et al., 2000), saliva

samples were taken at three different time points (T1 D prior to experimental

task, T2 D immediately following the experimental task, and T3 D 15 min after

the experimental task) during the first session of the study. The participants were

given three oral swabs, each located inside one of the three swab storage tubes

(from Salimetrics Testing Services, Inc., State College, PA). The participants

were instructed to saturate the oral swabs with saliva while inside their mouths.

Each storage tube had the participant’s subject number and the abbreviation for

when to swab their mouths (T1, T2, and T3). A timer was used to time 15 min

between T2 and T3 collection points.

Oral swab samples were stored at �20ıC in an on-site freezer until analysis.

After all samples had been taken, samples were packed on dry ice and shipped

to Salimetrics and assayed in singulate using enzymatic immunoassay (EIA)

following standard procedures outlined by Salimetrics. The EIA kit included

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

22 EDDINGTON ET AL.

a microtitre plate with monoclonal antibodies to cortisol, and was designed to

capture 0.0003 to 3.00 �g/dL of cortisol. Standard cortisol levels from Sali-

metrics and study samples competed with cortisol linked horse radishperoxidase

for the antibody binding sites. The cortisol standards, controls, and samples

were then pipetted into appropriate wells on the microtitre plate. Assay diluents

were used to serve as the zero for comparison purposes. A dilution of the

enzyme conjugate was immediately pipetted into each well using a multichan-

nel pipette. After mixing the plate, it was incubated at room temperature for

55 min.

After washing unbound components, the substrate tetramethylbenzidine

(TMB) was added, and the solution was mixed on a plate rotator for 5 min

at 500 rpm and placed in the dark at room temperature for an additional 25 min.

Fifty �L of solution was added to each well to stop the enzymatic reaction, and

then mixed on a plate rotator for 3 min. The plate was placed on a plate reader

within 10 min of adding the stop solution. Bound cortisolperoxidase was then

measured by optical density color differences produced by the reaction of the

peroxidase enzyme on the substrate TMB.

Referral sheet. Because the tasks and measures in the study could have

elicited some distress in the participants, a list of available psychological support

services was issued to each person.

Procedure

Institutional review board approval for the protection of human participants was

obtained, and the study was advertised online through a chronic health screener

for enrolled undergraduate students, as well as through flyers. All participants

voluntarily consented to the study via e-mail. Verbal and written informed

consent was obtained at the beginning of the first session after explaining the

purpose of the study. All participants were above the age of 18; therefore, the

protocol for obtaining consent for minors was not necessary. Participants were

matched by age and gender after data collection. Participants received course

credit and monetary compensation. The study consisted of two sessions, with

the first lasting 1 1

2hr and the second lasting approximately 1 hr. In the first

session, after filling out the consent form, the first saliva sample was collected

(T1). Then, participants were assigned (before arrival) by the experimenter to

either a contingent or non-contingent condition for the computerized experi-

mental (learned helplessness) task. Participants were read a script of examples

of the tasks and how they work. After the examples, a computer presented

the participants with a concept-formation task involving 40 stimulus patterns

grouped into four sets of 10 problems (for a complete description, see Chaney

et al., 1999). The experimental manipulation involved one-half of the participants

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 23

receiving positive feedback from the computer screen, which allowed successful

completion of the task (contingent or controllable condition); and the other

half of the participants receiving random, negative feedback from the computer

screen, which prevented successful completion of the task (non-contingent or

non-controllable condition).

As part of the standardized instructions, all participants were led to believe

that the task was solvable and that determining the correct stimulus dimension

was attainable. However, only participants in the contingent-feedback condition

were given solvable problems with response-contingent correct and incorrect

feedback about their performance. In addition, after the computer task, research

assistants verbally commented with similar positive feedback or random, nega-

tive feedback, depending on the experimental condition, to the participant about

his or her performance. As soon as the feedback was given, the participant was

asked to give saliva sample T2, and the experimenter started the timer for 15 min.

Following the administration of the computer task, participants were asked to

complete an anagram task containing 20 anagrams with five letters per anagram,

and were given 100 sec to solve each anagram. The program was conducted on

the same computer as the experimental task. The purpose of this task was to

measure changes in performance and motivation following experiencing non-

contingency in the earlier task. After the task was over, a score was presented

on the screen. The experimenter documented this score. Next, the experimenter

gave the participant the demographic form to fill out (allowing 15 min to pass).

After 15 min passed, the T3 saliva sample was taken. If the participant did not

fill out the demographic form before the 15 min, the experimenter took the saliva

sample, and then allowed the participant to fill out the form afterward.

Following the completion of the experiment, participants were given an

explanation regarding the deceptive aspects of the study and the expected results

to be gained from the research. In the second session, the experimenter re-

introduced the study to the participants. All participants were asked to complete

a series of questionnaires concerning anxiety, depression, and other psychosocial

concepts.

RESULTS

Preliminary Results

Because HPA axis over-activity and more frequent and longer cortisol elevations

have been associated with mental disorders, like depression (Burke, Davis, Otte,

& Mohr, 2005), descriptive statistics were conducted to determine the percentage

of college students with asthma and the healthy controls that met clinically sig-

nificant criteria for depression or scored in the severe to extremely severe range

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

24 EDDINGTON ET AL.

for anxiety. Results revealed that 23.2% of participants with asthma and 30% of

the healthy controls scored in the clinically significant range for depression (> 16

for the CES–D; Radloff, 1977). In addition, 26.7% of participants with asthma

and 46.8% of healthy controls scored in the moderate range of depression (7–10

for the CES–D). For anxiety, 3.3% of college students with asthma and 3.3% of

healthy controls scored in the severe to extreme range (between 60 and 80 on the

SAS; Zung, 1971). Further, 93.3% of college students with asthma and 93.3%

of healthy controls reported anxiety levels in the normal range (between 20 and

44 on the SAS; Zung, 1971). In addition, an independent t test revealed that

college students with asthma did not differ from healthy controls on self-ratings

of anxiety, t.58/ D 0:88, p D :383 (d D 0:15); and depression, t.58/ D �0:03,

p D :977 (d D 0:01). In sum, college students with asthma and their healthy

controls did not self-report significant differences in levels of either depression

or anxiety.

Primary Results

To address the primary hypothesis in this study, a 2 (Illness Status) � 3 (Saliva

Measurement Times) � 2 (Experimental Condition) mixed analysis of variance

design was used to assess the level of cortisol after the experimental ma-

nipulation. Analysis revealed a significant main effect of measurement times,

F.1:54; 84:92/ D 7:53, p D :002 .d D 0:12/. Bonferroni-corrected post hoc

tests showed that measurement time T2 .M D 0:32/ was significantly higher

than measurement time T3 .M D 0:28/; T1 measurements were not significantly

different from T2 .p > :05/ or T3 .p > :05/ measurements. There was no

main effect for illness status, F.1; 55/ D 3:32, p D :74 .d D 0:06/; or

experimental condition, F.1; 55/ D 0:58, p D :45 .d D 0:01/, thus indicating

that neither illness status nor experimental condition alone had an effect on

cortisol levels. There was an interaction between measurement times and illness,

F.1:54; 84:92/ D 3:42, p D :049 .d D 0:06/, indicating that the differences

between measurement times differed by illness group. To break down this

interaction, simple contrasts were conducted to compare the three measurement

times across illness statuses. The contrasts revealed that at T1, cortisol levels

for asthma participants (M D 0:29; SD D 0:18) were significantly higher than

age-gendered healthy controls (M D 0:20, SD D 0:10), F.1; 58/ D 5:94,

p D :018. Among asthma participants and same age-gendered healthy controls,

cortisol levels were not significantly different at T2, F.1; 58/ D 3:16, p D :081;

or at T3, F.1; 58/ D 0:82, p D :370. There was no interaction between

experimental condition and illness, F.1; 55/ D 0:73, p D :40 .d D 0:01/;

between measurement time and experimental condition, F.1:54; 84:92/ D 0:90,

p D :39 .d D 0:02/; or between measurement time, experimental condition,

and illness, F.1:54; 84:92/ D 1:43, p D :24 .d D 0:03/.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 25

FIGURE 1 Cortisol levels of participants with asthma and same age-gendered controls as

a function of the learned helplessness task (color figure available online).

Thus, to summarize, the only significant effects on cortisol levels were mea-

surement times and the interaction of measurement times and illness condi-

tions. The stressor from the learned helplessness task did not account for the

significant elevations in cortisol levels in college students with asthma com-

pared to healthy controls. Overall, T2 cortisol levels were significantly higher

than T3 cortisol levels for both college students with asthma and healthy con-

trols. In addition, the interaction indicated that participants with asthma had

overall higher T1 cortisol levels than same age-gendered healthy controls (see

Figure 1).

DISCUSSION

The primary aim of this study was to determine if college students with asthma

would evidence higher cortisol levels (and, hence, greater stress reactivity)

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

26 EDDINGTON ET AL.

following a helplessness induction task when compared to same age-gendered

healthy controls. With regard to the primary aim, results supported the hypothesis

as it pertains to demonstrating higher cortisol levels in college students with

asthma at T1 in both the contingent and non-contingent condition. However,

although post hoc testing revealed that T2 cortisol was significantly higher than

T3 for both participants with asthma and healthy controls, simple contrasts

suggested that the increase was not due to the experimental manipulation of

the learned helplessness task. In addition, it would appear that T1 levels of

cortisol were initially elevated for college students with asthma and seemed

to decline (see Figure 1); therefore, the results cannot be interpreted that this

elevation in cortisol levels at T2 was a response to the stress task in either

college students with asthma or to healthy controls. Cortisol levels in the non-

contingent condition did decline from T2 to T3 in college students with asthma,

but showed little variability in the healthy controls, as evidenced by remaining

fairly consistent with T1 levels (see Figure 1).

Notably, in the contingent or controllable condition, cortisol levels showed

little variability across the three time points and, hence, did support the hypoth-

esis of cortisol levels remaining fairly consistent from T1 to T2 and T3 in both

the asthma and healthy control groups. More important, it should be noted that

the non-contingent or non-controllable condition of the learned helplessness

task did not elicit increased cortisol secretion in either college students with

childhood-onset asthma or their matched controls, as predicted by previous

research (Chaney et al., 1999). Several factors could have contributed to this

lack of a manipulation effect. A primary explanation for this finding might

be the dated design of the computerized learned helplessness task, which may

not have been perceived by the participants as valid and, hence, non-stressful.

The experimental task used in this study was developed from older studies

from the 1970s (e.g., Benson & Kennelly, 1976; Hiroto & Seligman, 1975;

Levine, 1971) and implemented with 1980 computer software. Participants in

this sample were born between 1985 and 1991, and the older task may have

looked dated to them, contributing to the lack of effective manipulation in both

populations. Second, despite extensive training and modeling, poorly executed

feedback from the computer-generated data and corresponding verbal responses

of the research assistants after the experimental task may be another factor

that could have contributed to the unexpected cortisol pattern. Indeed, a key

factor for the contingent/non-contingent feedback relied on verbal reiteration

from research assistants after the computer task. To the extent that the feedback

was not convincingly delivered, the T2 cortisol level would not be influenced.

Although research has found support for normal diurnal patterns of cortisol

in adults with asthma (Masharani et al., 2005), there has been limited research

comparing cortisol levels among individuals with a chronic illness and their

healthy controls. Also, although this study did show a significant decrease

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 27

in cortisol levels 15 min after the stress task (T3), for college students with

childhood-onset asthma in the non-contingent condition, as mentioned earlier,

the decrease did not appear to be an effect of the non-contingent aspect of

the learned helplessness task. The cortisol levels were significantly elevated at

the start of the experiment, and tapered off toward T3 in college students with

childhood-onset asthma. The observed decrease in cortisol in participants with

asthma in this study is consistent with adaptive reorganization to a stressor (i.e.,

adapting to an unfamiliar event or context, such as being part of an experiment),

which was later reappraised and perceived as non-threatening (i.e., Abramson,

Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978) and, hence, reflects habituation to the environment.

Although speculative, the initial elevation noticed in the participants with asthma

in the contingent and non-contingent conditions could also be the result of

a pre-experimental “anticipation” response. Supporting this proposition, this

anticipation response was not seen in the healthy control population in either the

contingent or non-contingent conditions (see Figure 1). Similarly, these results

are also consistent with the reactivity and then, later, recovery phases of the HPA

axis (McEwen, 1998), and suggests that individuals with asthma may be more

reactive to novel situations. With more research, positive biological reactivity

may be highlighted as a prevented tactic or skill to foster in college students

with asthma.

This study has a number of limitations. First, asthma diagnosis and severity

were subjective, based on self-report, and was not verified by a medical profes-

sional. Furthermore, knowledge about prescribed glucocorticoid from inhaled,

nasal, or oral usage was not analyzed. In addition, most of the individuals with

asthma reported mild severity (see Table 1); thus, these results may not generalize

when compared to populations with more severe asthma. Also, all participants

were college students, and the majority were Caucasian, thereby limiting gen-

eralizability, particularly given that statistics show that Puerto Ricans have the

TABLE 1

Description of Self-Report Asthma Severity Ratings

Description Frequency %

Very mild 14 46.7

Mild 7 23.3

Very moderate 3 5.0

Moderate 3 5.0

Severe 3 5.0

Extremely severe 0 0.0

Respiratory failure 0 0.0

Note. N D 30 college students with asthma.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

28 EDDINGTON ET AL.

highest current asthma prevalence rates, followed by non-Hispanic Blacks and

non-Hispanic American Indians (National Center for Health Statistics, 2006).

The small sample size limits the ability to find significant effects. Finally, salivary

cortisol measurements were not assayed in duplicate for average variability for

each assay. More accurate ratings could have been achieved with duplicate

assays.

Despite these limitations, several strengths should be acknowledged. First,

this study is an initial step in highlighting cortisol differences in individuals

with chronic illnesses. To our knowledge, this study is the first to find signif-

icant differences in cortisol levels of college students with asthma and their

healthy controls. The results provide evidence that stress levels may be initially

elevated in individuals with asthma due to anticipation of novel situations or

environments.

Because the method of stress induction used in this study (i.e., the learned

helplessness task) did not appear to elicit stress, a more effective methodology

should be used in the future. The Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum, Pirke, &

Hellhammer, 1993) has been utilized and validated in cortisol research, and

would mostly likely elicit stress in this population. Further examination of

cortisol levels in individuals with a chronic illness across different illnesses,

genders, and varying ethnicities would be helpful in future research, especially

because several disparities have been documented in mortality, morbidity, and

prevalence rates among individuals with asthma (Centers for Disease Control,

1998). In addition, verification of asthma diagnosis, severity of asthma symp-

toms, and physician visits should be confirmed with additional independent

physician ratings. Including evaluation of salivary ˛-amylase (SAM) would be

quite beneficial to future research (Baum, 1993).

Implications for Practice

Evidence for the peak, then decline, of cortisol levels in individuals with asthma

could have a number of implications for future research and practice. These

data indeed suggest that college students with asthma are more reactive to stress.

More knowledge about the pattern of cortisol levels and its impact on individuals

with asthma would be beneficial for understanding the health and lifestyle of

individuals with asthma or with other chronic illnesses. This information would

be helpful in treatment planning and recommendations for young adults with

asthma. Furthermore, Chaney et al. (1999) documented a pre–post increase in

internal attributions among college students with asthma in the uncontrollable

condition, whereas this study demonstrated a pre–post decline in cortisol levels.

Hommel, Chaney, Wagner, & Jarvis (2006) found more external attributions

after an uncontrollable event in adolescents with juvenile rheumatic disease.

Evaluation of an inverse relation between cortisol levels and internal or external

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 29

attributions may explain physiological components to stress habituation. Indeed,

it would be intriguing to see if attributing stress to internal versus external

sources is linked to a decrease in cortisol levels in individuals with asthma.

Overall, future efforts examining the differences in stress reactivity among

individuals with asthma and healthy controls seems warranted.

REFERENCES

Aardal-Eriksson, E., Karlberg, B. E., & Holm, A. C. (1998). Salivary cortisol—An alternative to

serum cortisoldetermination in dynamic function tests. Clinical Chemistry Laboratory Medicine,

36, 215–222.

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in human:

Critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.

Akinbami, L. J., Moorman, J. E., Garbe, P. L., & Sondik, E. J. (2009). Status of childhood asthma

in the United States, 1980–2007. Pediatrics, 123(Suppl. 3), S131–S145.

American Lung Association. (2007). Trends in asthma morbidity and mortality. Washington, DC:

Author. Retrieved from http://www.lungusa.org

Bauer, A. M., Quas, J. A., & Boyce, W. T. (2002). Associations between physiological reactivity

and children’s behavior: Advantages of a multisystem approach. Journal of Developmental &

Behavioral Pediatrics, 23, 102–113.

Baum, B. J. (1993). Principles of saliva secretion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

694, 17–23.

Benson, J., & Kenelly, K. (1976). Learned helplessness: The result of uncontrollable reinforcements

or uncontrollable aversive stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 138–145.

Burke, H. M., Davis, M. C., Otte, C., & Mohr, D. C. (2005). Depression and cortisol responses to

psychological stress: A meta-analysis. Psychoendocrinology, 30(9), 846–856.

Carpentier, M. Y., Mullins, L. L., & Van Pelt, J. C. (2007). Psychological, academic, and work

functioning in college students with childhood-onset asthma. Journal of Asthma, 44, 119–124.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (1998). Measuring childhood asthma prevalence before

and after the 1997 redesign of the National Health Interview Survey–United States. Morbidity

and Mortality Weekly Report, 49, 908–911.

Chaney, J. M., Mullins, L. L., Uretsky, D. L., Pace, T. M., Werden, D., & Hartman, V. L. (1999).

An experimental examination of learned helplessness in older adolescents and young adults with

long-standing asthma. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 24, 259–270.

De Boer, S. F., de Beun, R., Slangen, J. L., & Van der Gugten, J. (1990). Dynamics of plasma cate-

cholamine and corticosterone concentrations during reinforced and extinguished operant behavior

in rats. Physiology & Behavior, 47, 691–698.

Forero, R., Bauman, A., Young, L., Booth, M., & Nutbeam, D. (1996). Asthma, health behaviors,

social adjustment, and psychosomatic symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Asthma, 33, 157–164.

Gabreys, J. B., & Peters, K. (1985). Reliability, discriminant and predictive validity of the Zung

Self-rating Depression Scale. Psychological Reports, 57, 1091–1096.

Gibson, E. L., Checkley, S., Papadopoulos, A., Poon, L., Daley, S., & Wardle, J. (1999). Increased

salivary cortisol reliably induced by a protein-rich midday meal. Psychosomatic Medicine, 61,

I214–I224.

Gillaspy, S., Hoff, A. H., Mullins, L. L., Van Pelt, J. C., & Chaney, J. M. (2002). Psychological

distress in high risk youth with asthma. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 363–371.

Henry, J. P. (1986). Neuroendocrine patterns of emotional response. In R. Plutchik & M. Kellerman

(Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience (pp. 37–60). New York, NY: Academic.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

30 EDDINGTON ET AL.

Heuther, G. (1998). Stress and the adaptive self-organization of neuronal connectivity during early

childhood. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 16, 297–306.

Hiroto, D. S., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327.

Hommel, K. A., Chaney, J. M., Wagner, J. L., & Jarvis, J. N. (2006). Learned helplessness in

children and adolescents with juvenile rheumatic disease. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,

60, 73–81.

Hommel, K. A., Chaney, J. M., Wagner, J. L., White, M. M., Hoff, A. L., & Mullins, L. L.

(2003). Anxiety and depression in older adolescents with long-standing asthma: The role of

illness uncertainty. Children’s Health Care, 32, 51–63.

Joachim, R. A., Quarcoo, D., Arck, P. C., Herz, U., Renz, H., & Klapp, B. F. (2003). Stress

enhances airway reactivity and airway inflammation in an animal model of allergic bronchial

asthma. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 811–815.

Jolicoeur, L. M., Pharm, D., Boyer, J. G., Reeder, C. E., & Turner, J. (1994). Influence of asthma or

allergies on the utilization of health care resources and quality of life of college students. Journal

of Asthma, 31, 251–267.

Kang, D., & Fox, C. (2000). Neuroendocrine and leukocyte responses and pulmonary function to

acute stressors. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 22, 276–285.

Kirschbaum, C., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1994). Salivary cortisol in psychoneuroendocrine research:

Recent developments and applications. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 57, 460–467.

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., & Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The “Trier Social Stress Test”—A toll for

investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology, 28,

76–81.

Knight, R. G., Waal-Manning, H. J., & Spears, G. F. (1983). Some norms and reliability data for

the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Zung Self-rating Depression Scale. British Journal of

Clinical Psychology, 22, 245–249.

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., & Kirschbaum, C. (2007). Ten years of research with the Trier

Social Stress Test–revisited. In E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Social neuroscience:

Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp. 56–83). New York,

NY: Guilford.

Levine, M. (1971). Hypothesis theory and nonlearning despite ideal S-R-reinforcement contingen-

cies. Psychological Review, 78, 130–140.

Masharani, U., Shiboski, S., Eisner, M. D., Katz, P. P., Janson, S. L., Granger, D. A., & Blanc, P. D.

(2005). Impact of exogenous glucocorticoid use on salivary cortisol measurements among adults

with asthma and rhinitis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 30, 744–752.

McEwen, B. S. (1998). Stress, adaptation, and disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

840, 33–44.

Mrazek, D. A., Klinnert, M., Mrazek, P. J., Bower, A., McCormick, D., Rubin, B., : : : Jones, J.

(1999). Prediction of early-onset asthma in genetically at-risk children. Pediatric Pulmonology,

27, 85–94.

Mullins, L. L., Chaney, J., Balderson, B., & Hommel, K. A. (2000). The relationship of illness

uncertainty, illness intrusiveness, and asthma severity to depression in young adults with long-

standing asthma. International Journal of Rehabilitation and Health, 5, 177–186.

Mullins, L. L., Chaney, J., Pace, T., & Hartman, V. (1997). Illness uncertainty, attributional style, and

psychological adjustment in older adolescents and young adults with asthma. Journal of Pediatric

Psychology, 22, 871–880.

National Center for Health Statistics. (2006). National health interview survey. Atlanta, GA: Centers

for Disease Control and Prevention. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nchs

National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. (2008). What is asthma? Bethesda, MD: Author. Retrieved

from http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health/dci/Diseases/Asthma/Asthma_ Treatments.html

Perez-Yarza, E. G. (1996). Introduction to issues in adolescent asthma. Thorax, 51(Suppl. 1), S1.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2

STRESS REACTIVITY 31

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in general

populations. Applied Psychological Measures, 1, 385.

Randolph, C., & Fraser, B. (1999). Stressors and concerns in teen asthma. Current Problems in

Pediatrics, 29, 82–93.

Sapolosky, R. M., Romero, L. M., & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress

responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine

Review, 21, 55–59.

Taitel, M. S., Allen, L., & Creer, T. L. (1998). The impact of asthma on the patient, family, and

society. In H. Kostes & A. Harver (Eds.), Self-management of asthma (pp. 3–19). New York, NY:

Marcel Dekker.

Vedhara, K., Miles, J., Crown, A., McCarthy, A., Shanks, N., Davies, D., : : : Ben-Shlomo, Y. (2007).

Relationships of early childhood illness with adult cortisol in the Barry Caerphilly Growth (BCG)

cohort. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 32, 865–873.

Vogel, W. H. (1985). Coping, stress, stressors and health consequences. Neuropsychobiology, 13,

129–135.

Wade, S., Weil, C., Holden, G., Mitchell, H., Evans III, R., Kruszon-Moron, D., : : : Wedner, J.

(1997). Psychosocial characteristics of inner-city children with asthma: A description of the

NCICAS psychosocial protocol: National Cooperative Inner-City Asthma Study. Pediatric Pul-

monology, 24, 263–276.

Wolf, J. M., Nicholls, E., & Chen E. (2008). Chronic stress, salivary cortisol, and ˛-amylase in

children with asthma and healthy children. Biological Psychology, 78, 20–28.

Zung, W. W. K. (1971). A rating instrument for anxiety disorders. Psychosomatics, 12, 371–337.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Okl

ahom

a St

ate

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

1:01

20

Febr

uary

201

2