The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects

11
The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects Department of Psychology, University of Jena, Germany Nadine Kloth Department of Psychology, University of Jena, Germany Stefan R. Schweinberger Recent ndings demonstrate that the perception of other peoples eye gaze direction can be dramatically biased by previous adaptation to that gaze direction. Here, we further investigated this aftereffect by examining its development over time, with particular attention to the potential role of the ambiguity of the test stimulus. Following adaptation to gaze to one direction, participantsability to correctly classify gaze to the adapted direction was severely impaired, both for ambiguous and relatively unambiguous test stimuli. Of particular importance, this aftereffect decreased over time but remained measurable up to 7 minutes post adaptation, with its decline following an exponential decay function. The implications of the present ndings are discussed with respect to both coding mechanisms involved in gaze perception and a potential role of adaptation effects in real life situations. Keywords: face perception, high-level adaptation, gural adaptation, gaze perception, neural habituation, time course Citation: Kloth, N., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects. Journal of Vision, 8(11):4, 111, http://journalofvision.org/8/11/4/, doi:10.1167/8.11.4. Introduction Visual adaptation has long been investigated in order to gain insight into the processing mechanisms of the human visual system. Traditionally, visual aftereffects have been studied and reported for early stages of processing and for relatively simple stimulus characteristics such as lumi- nance, contrast (Chen, Zhou, Gong, & Liang, 2005), color, or motion (Antal et al., 2004). One of the most popular and extensively investigated examples is the motion aftereffect (MAE) whose first mention is ascribed to Aristotle (Parva Naturalia). Here, the prolonged viewing of a downward-moving stimulus subsequently leads to the illusionary perception of an upward motion in a static image (Purkinje, 1820; see Anstis, Verstraten, & Mather, 1998, for a review). This phenomenon has been explained to result from a disequilibrium between motion detectors tuned to opposite directionsVwith neural fatigue (Barlow & Hill, 1963) and reciprocal inhibition processes (Culham et al., 1999; Tootell et al., 1995) being discussed as the underlying neural mechanisms. The observation of this MAE (as one example of a well-established visual aftereffect) therefore revealed detailed and non-invasive insight into the organization of the neural system processing vertical motionVwith one subsystem detecting upward motion and a second subsystem detecting down- ward motion. While adaptation to simple stimulus attributes has been known for literally hundreds of years, a striking novel discovery within the last few years was that adaptation is also of central importance for the perception of very complex visual stimuli. Webster and MacLin (1999) reported the so-called face distortion aftereffect (FDAE)Va figural aftereffect affecting the perception of face configurations. They found that adaptation to distorted (e.g., contracted) faces subse- quently led to an altered perception of normal faces: Participants perceived normal test faces as distorted in the direction opposite to adaptation (e.g., expanded). Similar high-level adaptation processes with contrastive after- effects have also been reported for other face-related processes such as the perception of identity (Leopold, O’Toole, Vetter, & Blanz, 2001), gender (Kova ´cs et al., 2006), viewpoint (Fang, Ijichi, & He, 2007), ethnicity, and emotional expression (Webster, Kaping, Mizokami, & Duhamel, 2004). As in the investigation of adaptation effects for simple stimulus characteristics, figural high- level adaptation experiments with face stimuli allow a valuable insight into the mechanisms and functional organization of face perception: Webster and MacLin (1999), for example, reported the FDAE to be asymmetric, i.e., adaptation to distorted but not to undistorted (“normal”) faces showed a clear effect on the perception of subsequently presented test faces. This is in line with the theory of a so-called “face–space” (Valentine, 1991) which suggests face representations to be organized in a multi-dimensional space with an average face prototype as the center (see also Leopold et al., 2001). Recent studies suggest that the perception of eye gaze direction in human faces can also be altered by adaptation (Jenkins, Beaver, & Calder, 2006; Seyama & Nagayama, 2006). This is an interesting finding as the directional content in eye gaze is an important social signal. Eye gaze is able to evoke attentional shifts in observers (Driver Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 111 http://journalofvision.org/8/11/4/ 1 doi: 10.1167/8.11.4 Received November 19, 2007; published August 6, 2008 ISSN 1534-7362 * ARVO

Transcript of The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects

The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effectsDepartment of Psychology, University of Jena,

GermanyNadine Kloth

Department of Psychology, University of Jena,GermanyStefan R. Schweinberger

Recent findings demonstrate that the perception of other people’s eye gaze direction can be dramatically biased byprevious adaptation to that gaze direction. Here, we further investigated this aftereffect by examining its development overtime, with particular attention to the potential role of the ambiguity of the test stimulus. Following adaptation to gaze to onedirection, participants’ ability to correctly classify gaze to the adapted direction was severely impaired, both for ambiguousand relatively unambiguous test stimuli. Of particular importance, this aftereffect decreased over time but remainedmeasurable up to 7 minutes post adaptation, with its decline following an exponential decay function. The implications of thepresent findings are discussed with respect to both coding mechanisms involved in gaze perception and a potential role ofadaptation effects in real life situations.

Keywords: face perception, high-level adaptation, figural adaptation, gaze perception, neural habituation, time course

Citation: Kloth, N., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). The temporal decay of eye gaze adaptation effects. Journal of Vision,8(11):4, 1–11, http://journalofvision.org/8/11/4/, doi:10.1167/8.11.4.

Introduction

Visual adaptation has long been investigated in order togain insight into the processing mechanisms of the humanvisual system. Traditionally, visual aftereffects have beenstudied and reported for early stages of processing and forrelatively simple stimulus characteristics such as lumi-nance, contrast (Chen, Zhou, Gong, & Liang, 2005), color,or motion (Antal et al., 2004). One of the most popularand extensively investigated examples is the motionaftereffect (MAE) whose first mention is ascribed toAristotle (Parva Naturalia). Here, the prolonged viewingof a downward-moving stimulus subsequently leads to theillusionary perception of an upward motion in a staticimage (Purkinje, 1820; see Anstis, Verstraten, & Mather,1998, for a review). This phenomenon has been explainedto result from a disequilibrium between motion detectorstuned to opposite directionsVwith neural fatigue (Barlow& Hill, 1963) and reciprocal inhibition processes (Culhamet al., 1999; Tootell et al., 1995) being discussed as theunderlying neural mechanisms. The observation of thisMAE (as one example of a well-established visualaftereffect) therefore revealed detailed and non-invasiveinsight into the organization of the neural systemprocessing vertical motionVwith one subsystem detectingupward motion and a second subsystem detecting down-ward motion. While adaptation to simple stimulusattributes has been known for literally hundreds of years,a striking novel discovery within the last few years wasthat adaptation is also of central importance for theperception of very complex visual stimuli. Webster and

MacLin (1999) reported the so-called face distortionaftereffect (FDAE)Va figural aftereffect affecting theperception of face configurations. They found thatadaptation to distorted (e.g., contracted) faces subse-quently led to an altered perception of normal faces:Participants perceived normal test faces as distorted in thedirection opposite to adaptation (e.g., expanded). Similarhigh-level adaptation processes with contrastive after-effects have also been reported for other face-relatedprocesses such as the perception of identity (Leopold,O’Toole, Vetter, & Blanz, 2001), gender (Kovacs et al.,2006), viewpoint (Fang, Ijichi, & He, 2007), ethnicity, andemotional expression (Webster, Kaping, Mizokami, &Duhamel, 2004). As in the investigation of adaptationeffects for simple stimulus characteristics, figural high-level adaptation experiments with face stimuli allow avaluable insight into the mechanisms and functionalorganization of face perception: Webster and MacLin(1999), for example, reported the FDAE to be asymmetric,i.e., adaptation to distorted but not to undistorted(“normal”) faces showed a clear effect on the perceptionof subsequently presented test faces. This is in line withthe theory of a so-called “face–space” (Valentine, 1991)which suggests face representations to be organized in amulti-dimensional space with an average face prototype asthe center (see also Leopold et al., 2001).Recent studies suggest that the perception of eye gaze

direction in human faces can also be altered by adaptation(Jenkins, Beaver, & Calder, 2006; Seyama & Nagayama,2006). This is an interesting finding as the directionalcontent in eye gaze is an important social signal. Eye gazeis able to evoke attentional shifts in observers (Driver

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 http://journalofvision.org/8/11/4/ 1

doi: 10 .1167 /8 .11 .4 Received November 19, 2007; published August 6, 2008 ISSN 1534-7362 * ARVO

et al., 1999; Friesen & Kingstone, 1998; Schuller &Rossion, 2001) at least when processed in foveal vision(Burton, Bindemann, Langton, Schweinberger, & Jenkins,in press), and humans are very efficient in detecting evensmall (G1- of visual angle) relative changes in the gazedirection of others (Symons, Lee, Cedrone, & Nishimura,2004). Jenkins et al. (2006) found that adaptation to gazedirected into a certain direction dramatically impairedparticipants’ subsequent ability to perceive eye gaze in theadapted direction. They presented participants with aseries of adaptation stimuli showing faces with gazeuniversally directed 25- to either the left or right side.Following adaptation, participants typically perceived eyegaze into the adapted direction as looking straight at them.This effect was found to be symmetric, i.e., the influenceof adaptation on the perception of gaze into the adapteddirection was equivalent for left and right adaptationdirection. In a similar study, using a two-alternativeforced-choice response setup with “left” and “right” asresponse options, Seyama and Nagayama (2006) foundthat after adaptation to eye gaze in one directionparticipants classified straight eye gaze or gaze directedinto the adapted direction more often as pointing into thedirection opposite to adaptation. Seyama and Nagayama(2006) also found symmetric results for the adaptation toleft and right eye gaze direction. Both Jenkins et al. (2006)and Seyama and Nagayama (2006) conducted a furtherexperiment in order to rule out that the reported after-effects were exclusively due to low-level adaptationprocesses (e.g., related to the pupil position). In a thirdexperiment, both groups excluded the hypothesis that theobserved gaze adaptation effects were due to generalspatial adaptation phenomena independent of eye gaze,suggesting that higher cortical mechanisms selectivelycode the perception of human gaze directions. Despitethese similarities between studies, there are also somedifferences in the reports by both groups concerning thenature of gaze direction aftereffects on stimuli gazing inthe unadapted direction: In addition to effects of adaptationon eye gaze directed in the adapted direction, Seyama andNagayama (2006) reported more “left” classifications forstraight gaze following adaptation to right gaze and more“right” classifications for straight gaze following adaptationto left gaze (relative to a neutral adaptation condition). Bycontrast, in their basic experiment, Jenkins et al. (2006) didnot report effects of adaptation on the perception of straightgaze or gaze to the unadapted direction. Evidence foraftereffects of gaze adaptation on the perception of unad-apted gaze direction is therefore somewhat mixed at present.High-level face adaptation effects have been detected

only in the past few years, and their time course is only justbeginning to be studied.Most recently, the effects of differentadaptation durations on certain aspects of face perceptionhave been investigated. In a functional magnetic resonanceimaging (fMRI) study, Fang,Murray, and He (2007) showedthe nature of face adaptation effects to depend on adaptationduration. They reported increasing durations to gradually

amplify “viewpoint-tuned” adaptation effects in face-specificareas and found different face-related brain regions to bedifferentially sensitive to short- and long-term adaptation.Similarly, Kovacs, Zimmer, Harza, and Vidnyanszky (2007)showed that short-term (500 ms) adaptation effects on facegender perception were invariant to the position of the teststimulus relative to the preceding adaptation stimulus,whereas long-term adaptation (5000 ms) led to facialaftereffects that were characterized by both a positioninvariant and a position-specific component. Again, thesefindings suggest that the systematic variation of adaptationdurations can be a tool to study selective adaptation ofdifferent neural mechanisms of shape-specific coding.There is relatively little knowledge regarding the nature of

the decay of high-level figural aftereffects, whereas thetiming parameters of low-level adaptation have been widelyinvestigated and discussed. In general, three importantinfluences on the time course can be distinguished. Theseaspects are the presentation duration of the adaptor, thepresentation duration of the test stimulus, and the timeinterval between the adaptor and test stimulus. The empiricalfindings vary considerably across adapted visual properties,the methodological details of the respective experiments, andeven across participants. However, generally speaking, themagnitude of most aftereffects increases as a function ofadaptation time, and decreases as a function of presentationduration of the test stimulus. For tilt aftereffects, for example,Magnussen and Johnsen (1986) report buildup and decay tobe logarithmic functions of time. The duration of motionaftereffects was reported to increase as a power function ofthe presentation time of the moving adaptation stimulus,while the decline was described by an exponential decayfunction (e.g., Hershenson, 1989, 1993; see also Petersik,2002, for a report of an exponential decay for three-dimensional MAEs).In a first study on the temporal characteristics of high-level

face-related adaptation, Leopold, Rhodes, Muller, andJeffery (2005) examined the effects of varying presentationdurations of the adaptation (1000 ms to 16000 ms) and teststimuli (200 ms to 1600 ms) on face identity aftereffects.They reported that these aftereffects increase logarithmi-cally as a function of adaptation duration and decreaseexponentially as a function of presentation duration of thetest stimulus. In a later study, similar findings were obtainedfor face aftereffects at an even higher (i.e., size-invariant)level (Rhodes, Jeffery, Clifford, & Leopold, 2007).The present study is motivated in part by the fact that

although the influences of adaptor duration and teststimulus duration have been investigated in a few studies,there is only little insight into the influence of the timeinterval between the presentation of the adaptation andtest stimulus on face adaptation effects. Previous studiestypically used an adaptor-test interval of just a fewhundred milliseconds (Jenkins et al., 2006; Kovacs et al.,2006; Leopold et al., 2005; Rhodes et al., 2004; Websteret al., 2004; Webster & MacLin, 1999). The idea that faceadaptation effects can survive an interval in the range of

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 2

minutes received preliminary support by a brief report(Carbon & Leder, 2006) on a face distortion aftereffect.As a limitation, this study only used one single face (ofMona Lisa) and did not track the decay of the aftereffect.To our knowledge, no study to date precisely evaluatedthe time course of face adaptation effects as a function ofthe adaptor-test interval.In a previous study on the neural correlates of eye gaze

adaptation (Schweinberger, Kloth, & Jenkins, 2007), weinvestigated the effects of adaptation to left and right gazedirections in two consecutive blocks with block ordercounter-balanced across participants. Although separatedby a break that offered a number of intervening visualstimuli (e.g., contact with experimenter, new instructionscreen), our data appeared to suggest an influence fromgaze adaptors in the first adaptation block on performancein the second block several minutes later. This was anaccidental finding that was unrelated to the aims of thatstudy, and that was therefore not reported. As eye gazedirection often changes quickly in real-life situations ofhuman communication, a finding of relatively long-persisting effects of eye gaze adaptation might be some-what unexpected. In the current paper, we report twoexperiments specifically designed to gain a systematicevaluation of the time course of eye gaze adaptationeffects. In Experiment 1, we measured participants’ abilityto correctly identify straight gaze and gaze directed 5- leftor right, before and directly after adaptation to eye gazestrongly (25-) diverted to right direction. In order tomonitor the decrease of gaze adaptation aftereffects overtime, a series of several further post-adaptation phases wasrun within approximately ten minutes. As prior research(Jenkins et al., 2006; Seyama & Nagayama, 2006)suggested that the magnitude of gaze adaptation effectspartly depends on the ambiguity of the test stimulus, wefurther explored the role of the ambiguity of test stimuli forthe persistence of the adaptation effects: In Experiment 2,we followed the same procedure as in Experiment 1, butusing more distinct gaze deviations (10-) in the test stimuli.

Experiment 1

MethodsParticipants

Twenty-five naıve participants (18 to 30 years,M = 21.6 years, 3 men) contributed data and receivedcourse credit for their participation. They all gave theirinformed consent and reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Stimuli

Test faces were color photographs of 6 male and 6female young adults as in previous studies (Jenkins et al.,

2006). Each model posed at gaze angles of 5- left (L05),straight (S00), and 5- right (R05; all directions from theobserver’s point of view). Photos of the same 12 modelsgazing 25- right (R25) were used as adaptation stimuli.All faces were presented in a black elliptical mask. Testfaces measured 13.0 � 7.5 cm and adaptation facesmeasured 19.0 � 11.0 cm. Stimuli were presented at aviewing distance of È87 cm which was kept constantusing a chin rest.

Procedure

The experiment began with a pre-adaptation phase inorder to determine the general ability to accuratelyperceive the gaze direction of faces. Using right index,middle, and ring fingers on three response keys, partic-ipants indicated if a test face showed left, straight, orright gaze direction, respectively. Thirty-six test faces(12 identities� 3 gaze directions) were presented in randomorder. For each trial, a question mark was first presented(800 ms), was then replaced by the test face (400 ms), andfollowed by a blank screen (2250 ms) during whichparticipants responded. One pre-adaptation trial thereforetook 3450 ms, leading to a pre-adaptation phase blockduration of two minutes and four seconds (124.2 s).The pre-adaptation phase was followed by an adapta-

tion phase in which participants were presented with twoconsecutive runs with twelve adaptation stimuli each,presented in randomized order. These stimuli showed eyegaze averted 25- in right direction and were passivelyviewed by participants. Exposure duration was 3500 msfor each adaptation stimulus, with an inter-stimulusinterval of 200 ms. Adaptation stimuli were presented atabout 150% the size of test stimuli so that the eye regionsin adaptation and test stimuli were non-overlapping. Theadaptation block had a total duration of one minute and29 seconds (88.8 s).The adaptation phase was immediately followed by a

series of five post-adaptation phases during whichparticipants were again asked to determine the directionof eye gaze. In general, post-adaptation phases wereequivalent to the pre-adaptation phase. The first post-adaptation phase, however, was characterized by slightdifferences in design and duration. Here, each teststimulus was preceded by two consecutive top-up adapta-tion displays (3500 ms each) presented before the questionmark (1000 ms) and the test face (400 ms) to ensuremaximal adaptation effects during the whole first testblock. To avoid any potential effects of immediate facialidentity repetitions (Schweinberger, Huddy, &Burton, 2004),neither of the two top-up adaptation stimuli carried the sameidentity as the following test face. A single trial in the firstpost-adaptation block had a duration of 10650 msVthecompletion of the whole first post-adaptation phase thereforetook 6 minutes and 23 seconds (383.4 s).Participants were then presented with four further post-

adaptation phases that did not contain top-up adaptation

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 3

stimuli. The timing parameters of these blocks wereequivalent to those of the pre-adaptation phase, with asingle trial duration of 3450 ms and a total duration of twominutes and four seconds (124.2 s) for each of the fourpost-adaptation phases. The consecutive post-adaptationphases were separated by standardized breaks of 30 seach, the first of which was presented immediately afterthe first post-adaptation phase. At the onset of the firstbreak, participants were informed in writing via themonitor about break duration and were instructed toclassify the gaze direction of each face in the followingpart (as there were no more top-up adaptation stimuli fromthis point on). All subsequent breaks simply informed theparticipants about the duration of the break and, 10 sbefore the end of the break, informed them that theywould have to continue with their task shortly. For a shortoverview of the procedure, please see Figure 1.The assessment of the temporal persistence of gaze

direction aftereffects was a central aim of the currentstudy, and our considerations concerning the analysis oftiming were as follows: First, by using top-up adaptationstimuli before each test stimulus, the first post-adaptationphase was designed to capture the maximal adaptationeffect in the context of the present study (cf. Jenkins et al.,2006). The starting point in time, relative to which we

tracked the decay of aftereffects, therefore coincided withthe end of the first post-adaptation phase (see Figure 2).As practical considerations (limited number of partici-pants, randomized presentation of stimuli in differentconditions) prevented an assessment of aftereffects on atrial-to-trial basis, we used relatively short test blocks withonly 12 test stimuli for each gaze direction in the secondto fifth post-adaptation phase. To determine a time courseof adaptation effects, we then defined the average timeacross all test faces within a given phase (relative to theend of the first post-adaptation phase) as a time point ofmeasurement. Similarly, we took the average performanceacross all test faces within a given phase as an indicatorfor the residual magnitude of aftereffects at this time.Figure 2 illustrates the resulting time scale.

Data analysis

An analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the factorsADAPTATION PHASE (A00, A01, A02, A03, A04, andA05) and gaze DIRECTION of test stimulus (left, right,and straight) was conducted to analyze the “straight”responses, in analogy to Jenkins et al. (2006). Whereappropriate, we performed epsilon corrections for hetero-geneity of covariances throughout (Huynh & Feldt, 1976).

Figure 1. Schematic example for trial procedure in the pre-adaptation (first row), adaptation (second row), and 1st post-adaptation (thirdrow) phases. Please note that test stimuli were actually presented at a smaller size than adaptors (see Procedure for details) and that the2nd to 5th post-adaptation trials were equivalent to the pre-adaptation trials.

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 4

All post hoc t-tests were corrected with the Bonferroni-procedure (! = .05).

Results

In the pre-adaptation test, participants were fairlyaccurate at discerning eye gaze directions (70.3 T 22.8%,71.8 T 18.9%, and 74.7 T 18.5% mean correct responses Tstandard deviations for left, right, and direct gaze,respectively). As expected, the ability to correctly per-ceive right gaze direction was strongly reduced afteradaptation to that direction (9.8 T 18.1% correct). Thecorrect classification of straight gaze, however, wasrelatively unaffected (75.3 T 15.6% correct responses)whereas correct classifications as “left gaze” appeared toincrease after adaptation (e.g., to 83.4 T 23.2% in the firstpost-adaptation phase). For the detailed response patternobtained in the different adaptation conditions (pre-adaptation test, first to fifth post-adaptation test), pleasesee Table 1.The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of both

ADAPTATION PHASE (F(5,120) = 11.42, p G .001) andDIRECTION (F(2,48) = 93.93, p G .001), as well as asignificant interaction (F(10,240) = 32.45, p G .001).Whereas the frequency of incorrect “straight” responses to

left vs. right test stimuli was comparable in the pre-adaptation phase, there was a dramatic increase inincorrect straight responses in the first post-adaptationphaseVbut only to right gazing stimuli. In subsequentpost-adaptation phases, this pattern gradually returned tonear-baseline levels. Conversely, there was some decreasein incorrect straight responses to left gazing stimuli in thefirst post-adaptation phase, a pattern that also graduallyreturned to near-baseline levels subsequently. Theseobservations were further evaluated by post hoc t-tests,which revealed that whereas the number of incorrect“straight” responses did not differ for left and right gazingstimuli during the pre-adaptation phase (p 9 .6), it clearlydid so after adaptation: “Straight” responses were signifi-cantly more frequent to right as compared to left gazingstimuli from the first post-adaptation phase up to thethird one (t(24) = 9.74, p G .001; t(24) = 5.25, p G .001;and t(24) = 3.36, p G .01, for first, second and third post-adaptation phases, respectively). This difference wasmainly due to a strong increase of “straight” responsesto test stimuli showing right gaze direction after adapta-tion. Compared to the pre-adaptation phase, there weresignificantly more “straight” responses to right gazingtest stimuli in the first (t(24) = 11.87, p G .001), second(t(24) = 6.95, p G .001), and third post-adaptation phase(t(24) = 4.10, p G .001), with a strong trend into the samedirection in the fourth post-adaptation phase (puncorrected = .03).

Direction

Response

Left gaze direction Right gaze direction Straight gaze direction

“l” “r” “s” “l” “r” “s” “l” “r” “s”

A00 70.3 T 4.6 1.0 T 0.7 28.7 T 4.3 1.7 T 1.0 71.8 T 3.8 26.5 T 3.5 15.1 T 3.5 10.2 T 2.6 74.7 T 3.7A01 83.4 T 4.6 0.4 T 0.4 16.2 T 4.6 4.2 T 2.6 9.8 T 3.6 86.0 T 4.3 22.9 T 3.0 1.8 T 0.7 75.3 T 3.1A02 76.4 T 3.8 0.4 T 0.4 23.2 T 3.8 2.4 T 1.7 42.0 T 5.2 55.6 T 5.3 17.9 T 2.9 1.3 T 0.8 80.8 T 2.8A03 70.9 T 3.7 1.0 T 0.6 28.1 T 3.7 2.3 T 1.2 52.3 T 5.4 45.3 T 5.1 11.6 T 3.0 4.1 T 1.9 84.2 T 3.7A04 70.1 T 3.5 0.7 T 0.5 29.2 T 3.5 1.0 T 0.7 62.4 T 5.0 36.6 T 4.8 11.7 T 2.3 7.0 T 2.1 81.3 T 2.7A05 70.7 T 4.1 0.3 T 0.3 29.0 T 4.1 0.7 T 0.5 67.3 T 3.4 32.0 T 3.4 12.1 T 2.4 5.1 T 1.6 82.7 T 2.7

Table 1. Mean percentages (TSEM) of left (“l”), right (“r”) and straight (“s”) responses to test stimuli depending on the actual gaze directionof the stimuli (left gaze direction, right gaze direction, straight gaze direction) and the test phase. Please note that the pre-adaptationphase is indicated by “A00,” the first to fifth post-adaptation phases are indicated by the abbreviations “A01” to “A05,” respectively.

Figure 2. Schematic illustration of time scale. Each box represents one test phase, with “A00” used as an abbreviation for “pre-adaptationphase” and A01 to A05 as abbreviations for the first to fifth post-adaptation phases, respectively. Times stated in the boxes representdurations of the respective phases. The end of phase A01 is considered to be t = 0. Durations printed in blue represent the time elapsed atthe end of the respective test phases. Durations printed in black represent the average time elapsed since t = 0 across all test faces withina given phase.

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 5

Incorrect “straight” responses to test stimuli showing leftgaze exhibited a weaker pattern of aftereffects in theopposite direction: Following adaptation to right gaze,incorrect “straight” responses to left gazing test stimulisignificantly decreased compared to pre-adaptation level inthe first post-adaptation phase only (t(24) = j3.01, p G .01).Although some degree of gradual return to pre-adaptationlevel could also be observed in the data on left gazingstimuli (see Table 1 and Figure 3), differences to pre-adaptation level were not significant in the other post-adaptation phases.Concerning the classification of stimuli showing straight

gaze direction, there was a very small increase in correctresponses after adaptation (see Table 1), which onlyreached a level of significant difference from the pre-adaptation phase in the third post-adaptation phase(t(24) = 2.73, p G .05). This enhancement was accompaniedby a significant decrease in incorrect “right” classificationsof straight gazing test stimuli in the first (t(24) = j3.68,p G .01), second (t(24) = j3.53, p G .01), and third(t(24) = j2.83, p G .01) post-adaptation phases.Figure 3 depicts the mean percentage of “straight”

responses to test stimuli gazing left and right against thetime elapsed since adaptation. We found that the develop-ment of the aftereffect over time for left and right gazingtest stimuli could be well defined by exponential functionsof the form

f ðxÞ ¼ ðY0 j PlateauÞ*eðjk*xÞ þ Plateau: ð1ÞPlease see Table 2 for an overview of the parameters ofthe functions fitted for left and right gazing stimuli,respectively.There was a high goodness of fit of the exponential

functions for both the increase in incorrect “straight”classifications of test stimuli showing left gaze (R2 = 0.99)and the decrease in incorrect “straight” classifications oftest stimuli showing right gaze (R2 = 0.97).

Discussion

The results of Experiment 1 replicate recent studies oneye gaze adaptation effects (Jenkins et al., 2006; Seyama& Nagayama, 2006; Schweinberger et al., 2007): Pro-longed adaptation to faces showing eye gaze directed tothe right subsequently led to an altered perception of eyegaze direction. Most strikingly, adaptation biased theclassification of eye gaze to the adapted direction to beperceived as direct gaze. This aftereffect decreased overtime but remained significant until the fourth post-adaptation phase, corresponding to about 385 s afteradaptation (please see Figure 2 for details on the timecourse). The time course of the aftereffect was wellmodeled by an exponential decay function (see Figure 3).As this is the first study to systematically determine the

effects of the adaptor-test-interval in high-level faceadaptation, it is difficult to relate our findings to those ofearlier studies. However, the comparison of the currentdata with findings on the decay of MAEs suggests acomparatively long-lived nature of gaze adaptation after-effects. While the exponential nature of the decay functionhas typically been reported for low-level effects as well(e.g., Hershenson, 1989; Petersik, 2002; Tootell et al.,1995), Hershenson (1989) reported that motion after-effects induced by extensive adaptation durations as longas 15 minutes almost completely decayed only 80 s afteradaptation. We note that whereas the combined effects ofadaptor duration and test stimulus duration have beenaddressed by two previous studies (cf. Leopold et al.,2005; Rhodes et al., 2007), the combined effects of allthree factors remain an important issue for future research.In Experiment 2, we aimed at replicating and extending

these findings. We conducted an analogous experiment,however, using less ambiguous test stimuli. As previousstudies (Jenkins et al., 2006; Seyama & Nagayama, 2006)showed reduced levels of aftereffects for less ambiguoustest stimuli, we hypothesized that those effects might alsobe subject to faster decay. We therefore determined thedecay of adaptation effects for these test stimuli andcompared them to those obtained in Experiment 1.

Figure 3. Exponential functions fitted on the percentages of“straight” responses to test stimuli showing left (blue) and right(red) gaze direction.

Parameter

Gaze direction of test face

Left Right

Y0 (in %) 16.17 T 0.23 84.97 T 4.76Plateau (in %) 29.28 T 0.18 34.87 T 3.84k (in 1/s) 0.01011 T 0.00060 0.00954 T 0.00309

Table 2. Parameter estimations for exponential functions fittingpercentages of “straight” responses to test stimuli gazing into theleft and right direction, respectively. Y0 represents the intercept(the response at time x = 0), and the plateau represents theasymptote (the response at time x = V). K is the rate constantwhich is expressed in reciprocal of the X axis time units.

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 6

Experiment 2

MethodsParticipants

Twenty-five new participants (19 to 32 years old,M = 21.4 years, 5 men) contributed data to this study andreceived course credit for their participation. They allgave their informed consent and reported normal orcorrected-to-normal vision.

Stimuli

Test faces were color photographs of the same twelveindividuals as in Experiment 1. The models posed at thegaze angles 10- left (L10), straight (S00), and 10- right(R10) leading to a much more obvious deviance fromstraight gaze in the “left gaze” and “right gaze” conditionsas compared to Experiment 1. As before, photos of thesame 12 models gazing 25- right were used as adaptationstimuli. Stimuli were of the same size as in the firstexperiment and a constant viewing distance of È87 cmwas ensured by using a chin rest.

Procedure

The procedure was equivalent to the one in Experiment 1.

Data analysis

An analysis of variance analogous to the one inExperiment 1 was conducted.

Results

Participants showed very good accuracy in discriminat-ing left (M = 92.9 T 14.8%), right (M = 91.3 T 16.4%), andstraight (M = 88.8 T 12.3%) gaze directions. Theseaccuracies are much higher than those obtained inExperiment 1 (cf. Table 1), reflecting lower task difficulty.However, even the classification of these relatively unam-biguous stimuli was severely altered as a consequence of

adaptation, as can be seen in the response pattern depictedin Table 3.As before, adaptation to stimuli gazing 25- to the right

direction severely disrupted the ability to correctlyperceive eye gaze into that directionVleading to incorrect“straight” responses in 72.5% (SD = 17.9%) of the trials inthe first post-adaptation phase. However, this aftereffectseemed to recover more quickly than in Experiment 1, asincorrect “straight” responses to right test stimuli werealready strongly decreased in the second post-adaptationphase (M = 18.9 T 25.2%). The ANOVA revealed asignificant main effect of ADAPTATION PHASE(F(5,120) = 74.95, p G .001), a significant main effectof gaze DIRECTION of test stimulus (F(2,48) = 821.63,p G .001), and a significant interaction (F(10,240) = 69.62,p G .001). Bonferroni-corrected comparisons showed thatthe mean percentages of incorrect “straight” responses didnot differ for left and right test stimuli during this pre-adaptation phase (p 9 .6). Following adaptation, however,there were significantly more “straight” responses to rightas compared to left gazing test stimuli in the first (t(24) =16.13, p G .001) and second post-adaptation phase (t(24) =3.62, p G .01). These differences between responses to leftand right test stimuli were due to a strong increase of“straight” responses to test stimuli showing right gazedirection. Compared to the pre-adaptation phase, therewere significantly more “straight” responses to right teststimuli in both the first (t(24) = 19.18, p G .001) and thesecond post-adaptation phase (t(24) = 3.02, p G .05). Fromthe third post-adaptation phase on, no such significantpost-adaptation effects could be discovered (all ps 9 .10).The percentages of “straight” responses to test stimulishowing left gaze did not differ across adaptation phases(all ps 9 .09).As in Experiment 1, we plotted the mean percentage of

straight responses against the time elapsed since adapta-tion. The development of the aftereffect over time forright gazing test stimuli could be well defined by anexponential decay function of the form

f ðxÞ ¼ ðY0 j PlateauÞ�eðjk�xÞ þ Plateau; ð2Þwith Y0 (in %) = 72.54 (T1.29), Plateau (in %) = 8.99(T0.76), and k (in 1/s) = 0.02396 (T0.00191). We refrained

Direction

Response

Left gaze direction Right gaze direction Straight gaze direction

“l” “r” “s” “l” “r” “s” “l” “r” “s”

A00 92.9 T 3.0 2.7 T 2.3 4.4 T 1.6 2.3 T 1.7 91.3 T 3.3 6.3 T 1.9 8.1 T 2.1 3.1 T 0.9 88.8 T 2.5A01 88.0 T 4.8 3.7 T 3.3 8.3 T 3.1 0.7 T 0.5 26.8 T 3.7 72.5 T 3.6 9.6 T 2.5 0.3 T 0.3 90.1 T 2.4A02 91.0 T 4.2 3.7 T 3.7 5.3 T 2.4 1.3 T 1.0 79.7 T 5.5 18.9 T 5.0 4.3 T 1.2 1.3 T 0.8 94.3 T 1.3A03 90.0 T 4.7 4.3 T 4.0 5.7 T 2.9 3.0 T 3.0 86.2 T 4.9 10.8 T 3.3 4.7 T 1.5 2.0 T 1.0 93.3 T 2.0A04 92.7 T 4.1 4.0 T 3.6 3.4 T 1.4 2.3 T 2.3 89.2 T 4.7 8.4 T 3.6 4.0 T 1.5 1.3 T 0.6 94.7 T 1.7A05 92.2 T 4.3 4.0 T 4.0 3.8 T 1.9 2.2 T 1.8 89.7 T 4.4 8.1 T 3.0 3.7 T 1.0 1.3 T 0.8 95.0 T 1.4

Table 3. Mean percentages (TSEM) of left (“l”), right (“r”), and straight (“s”) responses to test stimuli depending on the actual gaze directionof the stimuli (left, right, straight) and the test phase. Abbreviations as in Table 1.

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 7

from fitting the responses to test stimuli showing left gaze,as these did not differ significantly across adaptationphases (see above). In order to allow for a directcomparison with Experiment 1, the functions of bothexperiments were plotted together in Figure 4. Theexponential decay function fit on the “straight” classifica-tions to test stimuli gazing into the right direction almostperfectly matched the empirical data (R2 = 0.99).As hypothesized, the direct comparison of the results

obtained in Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 suggests amuch steeper initial decay in the 10- as compared to the5- experiment.

Discussion

Using less ambiguous test stimuli, the adaptationprocedure in Experiment 2 led to a qualitatively similarbut reduced perceptual bias in the classification of gazedirections. Immediately after adaptation, test stimuli weremost often misjudged as looking straight at the observerVeven when in reality showing a substantial 10- gazedeviation into the adapted direction. However, this illu-sionary aftereffect was only measurable in the first andsecond post-adaptation phases. Based on the time scale ofthis study (for details, please see Figure 2), this means thatthe aftereffect lasted about 77 s. The comparison of theexponential decay functions fitted on the empirical datareveals that apart from the higher initial level of illusions inExperiment 1, there is also a more gradual decrease of thesemisperceptions in Experiment 1 than in Experiment 2.

General discussion

While the time interval between the adaptor and teststimulus has been previously demonstrated to affect the

magnitude of aftereffects following adaptation to simplestimulus characteristics such as motion (e.g., Kanai &Verstraten, 2005), the present study is the first to system-atically describe the course of high-level face adaptationeffects over timeVfrom their first maximal level untiltheir decay to insignificance. Although aftereffects causedby adaptation to eye gaze direction showed a systematicdecay in post-adaptation phases, a remarkable finding ofthe present study is that such aftereffects were stillmeasurable several minutes post-adaptation. In bothexperiments, post-adaptation effects (i.e., incorrect“straight” classifications of eye gaze into the adapteddirection) were maximal immediately following adapta-tion, and had decayed to near-baseline levels in the lastpost-adaptation phase, approximately after ten minutes. Inline with previous research (Jenkins et al., 2006; Seyama& Nagayama, 2006), we found the initial level of gazeaftereffects to be smaller for less ambiguous test stimuli.At the same time, adaptation effects on less ambiguoustest stimuli were also subject to faster decay.Previous studies of face adaptation typically used an

adaptor-test interval of just a few hundred milliseconds(Jenkins et al., 2006; Kovacs et al., 2006; Leopold et al.,2005; Rhodes at al., 2004; Webster et al., 2004; Webster& MacLin, 1999; but see Carbon and Leder, 2006, andLeopold et al., 2001, for informal reports of diminishedbut still measurable face identity aftereffects after anadaptor-test interval of 2400 ms, and Schweinberger et al.,2008, for novel evidence of auditory adaptation in voiceperception lasting for several minutes).We show that adaptation continues to cause biased

gaze perception over several minutes, suggesting thatthese effects may not be limited to very specificconditions in the laboratory. Instead, it seems likely that,under appropriate conditions, adaptation may bias thesocial perception of eye gaze in real-life situations. Faceadaptation effects have been investigated for a multitudeof social signals, some of which are typically subject torelatively rapid changes (e.g., eye gaze direction,expression, facial speech), whereas others (e.g., gender,identity) tend to be more stable over time. Invariant vs.changeable aspects of human faces are thought to beprocessed in different neural systems (Haxby, Hoffmann,& Gobbini, 2000). A plausible but yet unexploredhypothesis would be that the recalibration processesevident in adaptation effects are faster for those systemscoding changeable aspects of facial information. Whilerecent research has begun to demonstrate differentmechanisms for short- and long-term adaptation (Fanget al., 2007; Kovacs et al., 2007), an important questionfor future research will be whether both mechanisms andtime courses of adaptation effects can be dissociated fordifferent social signals in faces. Tentative support for thisassumption might be seen in a comparison of our findingswith those by Carbon et al. (2007) who reported residualaftereffects of adaptation to distorted faces to be stillmeasurable 24 hours after adaptation. These authors

Figure 4. Exponential functions fitted on the percentages of“straight” responses to test stimuli showing left (blue) and right(red) gaze direction obtained in Experiment 1 (solid lines) andExperiment 2 (dotted lines).

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 8

measured effects only at one point in time, and a precisetracking of the decay of aftereffects to invariant vs.changeable aspects of faces will therefore be required fora more thorough comparison.A few studies of adaptation have begun to address the

question of contrastive vs. multichannel coding of faces,with some authors reporting evidence for contrastivecoding (Leopold et al., 2001; Robbins, McKone, &Edwards, 2007) and others reporting evidence for multi-channel coding (Calder, Jenkins, Cassel, & Clifford,2007). When considering that those studies investigatedadaptation to different social signals in faces, thosefindings are not necessarily in contradiction. The presentresults might be reconciled with the idea of contrastive(two-channel) coding of horizontal eye gaze direction. Weconcede that, on the basis of the present data, it is difficultto completely exclude a multi-channel system withseparate channels for distinct gaze directions (in the mostsimple case, left, direct, and right), at least when assumingrelatively broad tuning curves of individual channels.However, two aspects of the current data seem to be morein line with the idea of contrastive coding of gazedirection: First, multichannel models predict adaptationeffects to mainly arise for test stimuli close to the adaptor(Robbins et al., 2007). Our data, however, show thatadaptation to eye gaze deviating by 25- causes substantialand more persistent aftereffects for the perception of 5-test stimuli as compared to 10- test stimuli. Second, unlikethe basic results of Jenkins et al. (2006), but similar toSeyama and Nagayama (2006), adaptation to right gaze inthe present study not only impaired the observer’sperception of gaze in the adapted direction, but alsoimproved observer’s perception of gaze in the oppositedirection, again supporting the idea that adaptation effectsto eye gaze reflect the coding of gaze direction in acontrastive manner.

Conclusion

Based on a recently established paradigm that demon-strated dramatic impairments in the perception of eye gazeto an adapted direction, the present study demonstrates,for the first time, that such aftereffects continue to bemeasurable for several minutes after adaptation. Thedecay of adaptation almost perfectly followed an expo-nential function, with more ambiguous test stimuliproducing both a larger initial illusion, and a more gradualdecay over time. We conclude that the investigation of thetime course of adaptation effects for different types ofstable and changeable facial signals may provide impor-tant insights into the respective coding mechanismsinvolved in face perception.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Nida Bajwa, Lars Buder, KristinEisengraber-Pabst, Jessika Golle, Julia Gorschewski,Helen Helms, Michaela Litzmann, Moritz Nickel, MarenPatzwahl, Claudia Penseler, Stephanie Trost, and LennartVictor for their help at various stages of this research.Further, we thank two anonymous reviewers for theirhelpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.We are particularly grateful to Markus Junghofer for hissupport with the data analysis.

Commercial relationships: none.Corresponding author: Nadine Kloth.Email: [email protected]: Department of Psychology, Friedrich-Schiller-University of Jena, Am Steiger 3/ Haus1, 07743 Jena,Germany.

References

Anstis, S., Verstraten, F. A. J., & Mather, G. (1998). Themotion aftereffect. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2,111–117.

Antal, A., Varga, E. T., Nitsche, M. A., Chadaide, Z.,Paulus, W., Kovacs, G., et al. (2004). Direct currentstimulation over MT+/V5 modulates motion aftereffectin humans. Neuroreport, 15, 2491–2494. [PubMed]

Barlow, H. B., & Hill, R. M. (1963). Evidence for aphysiological explanation of the waterfall phenomenonand figural after-effects. Nature, 200, 1345–1347.[PubMed]

Burton, A. M., Bindemann, M., Langton, S. R. H.,Schweinberger, S. R., & Jenkins, R. (in press). Gazeperception requires focussed attention: Evidence froman interference task. Journal of Experimental Psy-chology: Human Perception and Performance.

Calder, A. J., Jenkins, R., Cassel, A., & Clifford, C. W. G.(2007). Adaptation reveals that the visual represen-tation of eye gaze is coded by a non-opponentmultichannel system. Paper presented at the jointmeeting of the Experimental Psychology Society andthe Psychonomic Society, Edinburgh, UK.

Carbon, C. C., & Leder, H. (2006). The Mona Lisa effect:Is ‘our’ Lisa fame or fake? Perception, 35, 411–414.[PubMed]

Carbon, C. C., Strobach, T., Langton S. R., Harsanyi, G.,Leder, H., & Kovacs, G. (2007). Adaptation effectsof highly familiar faces: Immediate and longlasting. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1966–1976.[PubMed]

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 9

Chen, A. H., Zhou, Y., Gong, H. Q., & Liang, P. J.(2005). Luminance adaptation increased the contrastsensitivity of retinal ganglion cells. Neuroreport, 16,371–375. [PubMed]

Culham, J. C., Dukelow, S. P., Vilis, T., Hassard, F. A.,Gati, J. S., Menon, R. S., et al. (1999). Recovery offMRI activation in motion area MT following storageof the motion aftereffect. Journal of Neurophysiology,81, 388–393. [PubMed] [Article]

Driver, J., Davis, G., Ricciardelli, P., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E.,& Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggersreflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6,509–540.

Fang, F., Ijichi, K., & He, S. (2007). Transfer of the faceviewpoint aftereffect from adaptation to different andinverted faces. Journal of Vision, 7(13):6, 1–9, http://journalofvision.org/7/13/6, doi:10.1167/7.13.6.[PubMed] [Article]

Fang, F., Murray, S. O., & He, S. (2007). Duration-dependent FMRI adaptation and distributed viewer-centered face representation in human visual cortex.Cerebral Cortex, 17, 1402–1411. [PubMed]

Friesen, C. K., & Kingstone, A. (1998). The eyes have it!Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictivegaze. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 490–495.

Haxby, J. V., Hoffman, E. A., & Gobbini, M. I. (2000). Thedistributed human neural system for face perception.Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 223–233. [PubMed]

Hershenson, M. (1989). Duration, time constant, anddecay of the linear motion aftereffect as a functionof inspection duration. Perception & Psychophysics,45, 251–257. [PubMed]

Hershenson, M. (1993). Linear and rotation motion after-effects as a function of inspection duration. VisionResearch, 33, 1913–1919. [PubMed]

Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1976). Estimation of the Boxcorrection for degrees of freedom from sample data inrandomized block and split-plot designs. Journal ofEducational Statistics, 1, 69–82.

Jenkins, R., Beaver, J. D., & Calder, A. J. (2006). Ithought you were looking at me: Direction-specificaftereffects in gaze perception. Psychological Sci-ence, 17, 506–513. [PubMed]

Kanai, R., & Verstraten, F. A. (2005). Perceptualmanifestations of fast neural plasticity: Motion pri-ming, rapid motion aftereffect and perceptual sensi-tization. Vision Research, 45, 3109–3116. [PubMed]

Kovacs, G., Zimmer, M., Banko, E., Harza, I., Antal, A., &Vidnyanszky, Z. (2006). Electrophysiological corre-lates of visual adaptation to faces and body parts inhumans. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 742–753. [PubMed][Article]

Kovacs, G., Zimmer, M., Harza, I., & Vidnyanszky, Z.(2007). Adaptation duration affects the spatial selec-tivity of facial aftereffects. Vision Research, 47,3141–3149. [PubMed]

Leopold, D. A., O’Toole, A. J., Vetter, T., & Blanz, V.(2001). Prototype-referenced shape encoding revealedby high-level aftereffects. Nature Neuroscience, 4,89–94. [PubMed]

Leopold, D. A., Rhodes, G., Muller, K. M., & Jeffery, L.(2005). The dynamics of visual adaptation to faces.Proceedings of the Royal Society B: BiologicalSciences, 272, 897–904. [PubMed] [Article]

Magnussen, S., & Johnsen, T. (1986). Temporal aspects ofspatial adaptation. A study of the tilt aftereffect.Vision Research, 26, 661–672. [PubMed]

Petersik, J. T. (2002). Buildup and decay of a three-dimensional rotational aftereffect obtained with athree-dimensional figure. Perception, 31, 825–836.[PubMed]

Purkinje, J. (1820). Beitrage zur naheren Kenntniss desSchwindels aus hautognostischen Daten. Medicini-sche Jahrbucher des Kaiserlich-Koniglichen Osterr-reichischen Staates, 6, 79–125.

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Clifford, C. W., & Leopold,D. A. (2007). The timecourse of higher-level faceaftereffects. Vision Research, 47, 2291–2296[PubMed]

Rhodes, G., Jeffery, L., Watson, T. L., Jaquet, E.,Winkler, C., & Clifford, C. W. (2004). Orientation-contingent face aftereffects and implications forface-coding mechanisms. Current Biology, 14,2119–2123. [PubMed] [Article]

Robbins, R., McKone, E., & Edwards, M. (2007).Aftereffects for face attributes with different naturalvariability: Adapter position effects and neuralmodels. Journal of Experimental Psychology:Human Perception and Performance, 33, 570–592.[PubMed]

Schuller, A. M., & Rossion, B. (2001). Spatial attentiontriggered by eye gaze increases and speeds up earlyvisual activity. Neuroreport, 12, 2381–2386.[PubMed]

Schweinberger, S. R., Casper, C., Hauthal, N., Kaufmann,J. M., Kawahara, H., Kloth, N., et al. (2008).Auditory adaptation in voice perception. CurrentBiology, 18, 684–688. [PubMed]

Schweinberger, S. R., Huddy, V., & Burton, A. M. (2004).N250r: A face-selective brain response to stimulusrepetitions. Neuroreport, 15, 1501–1505. [PubMed]

Schweinberger, S. R., Kloth, N., & Jenkins, R. (2007).Are you looking at me? Neural correlates of gazeadaptation. Neuroreport, 18, 693–696. [PubMed]

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 10

Seyama, J., & Nagayama, R. S. (2006). Eye directionaftereffect. Psychological Research, 70, 59–67.[PubMed]

Symons, L. A., Lee, K., Cedrone, C. C., & Nishimura M.(2004). What are you looking at? Acuity for triadic eyegaze. Journal of General Psychology, 131, 451–469.[PubMed]

Tootell, R. B., Reppas, J. B., Dale, A. M., Look, R. B.,Sereno, M. I., Malach, R., et al. (1995). Visual motionaftereffect in human cortical area MT revealed byfunctional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature, 375,139–141. [PubMed]

Valentine, T. (1991). A unified account of the effects ofdistinctiveness, inversion, and race in face recogni-tion. Quarterly Journal of Experimental PsychologyA: Human Experimental Psychology, 43, 161–204.[PubMed]

Webster, M. A., Kaping, D., Mizokami, Y., & Duhamel, P.(2004). Adaptation to natural facial categories. Nature,428, 557–561. [PubMed]

Webster, M. A., & MacLin, O. H. (1999). Figuralaftereffects in the perception of faces. PsychonomicBulletin & Review, 6, 647–653. [PubMed]

Journal of Vision (2008) 8(11):4, 1–11 Kloth & Schweinberger 11