The effects of child sexual abuse.

48
MacIntyre, D. & Carr, A. (1999).The effects of child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Centred Practice, 6, (1), 87-126.

Transcript of The effects of child sexual abuse.

MacIntyre, D. & Carr, A. (1999).The effects of child sexual abuse. Journal of Child Centred Practice, 6, (1), 87-126.

Prevention of CSA

36

THE EFFECTS OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE

A substantial body of empirical evidence now shows that child sexual abuse has

profound effects on the psychological adjustment of children (Kendall-Tackett,

Williams & Finkelhor, 1993) and these effects in some instances continue on into

adulthood (Beitchman, Zucker, Hood, Da Costa & Akman, 1991). A wide range

of factors mediate the impact of abuse on adjustment (Spacarelli, 1994). In this

chapter the impact of sexual abuse on children and adults will be addressed with

reference to the empirical literature in the field and the implications of this for

prevention considered.

DOMAINS AFFECTED BY SEXUAL ABUSE

Sexual abuse can affect children's functioning in the following domains:

behavioural adjustment; attachment; intrapsychic integration and dissociation;

and physiological functioning.

Effects of CSA

37

Behavioural adjustment

Sexually abused children exhibit more behavioural problems compared with

their non-abused counterparts (e.g. Cohen & Mannarino, 1988; Gomes-Schwartz

et al, 1990; Tong, Oates & McDowell, 1987; Kendall-Tackett, Williams &

Finkelhor, 1993). Sexually abused children typically become either more

withdrawn, depressed, disinterested in school and other activities or they become

angry, aggressive and disruptive. That is, they may display both internalising and

externalising behaviour problems (Achenbach, 1984). In addition a proportion

show inappropriate sexual behaviour. (e.g. Gale, Thompson, Moran & Sack;

1988, Friedrich & Leucke, 1988). This is uniquely related to the process of

premature sexualisation inherent in the abuse. Explicit sexual behaviours among

non-abused children are rare but are exhibited by a significant proportion of child

sexual abuse victims. Sexually abused children tend to display compulsive

sexual behaviours and may also engage in sexual behaviour imitative of adult

sexual activity (Friedrich, Grambsch, Damon et al, 1992). In adulthood,

psychosexual difficulties are common among survivors of child sexual abuse

(e.g. Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Courtois, 1988).

Attachment disruption

Children victimised at an early age by their primary caregivers develop insecure

attachments (Carlson, Cicchetti, Barnett & Braunwald, 1989). These early

attachment difficulties lay the foundation for relationship difficulties at later

stages of development (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993). There is substantial evidence

that difficulties in interpersonal relationships are reported by both children

(Friedrich, Beilka & Urquiza, 1987) and adults who have been sexually abused

(Courtois, 1988; Elliot, 1994). Insecure attachments may also predate and

increase vulnerability to abuse or previously secure relationships may be

disrupted by the trauma of abuse. For instance, the reaction of parents to a

Prevention of CSA

38

disclosure may destroy trust and compound the child's sense of betrayal and the

effects of the abuse (e.g. Conte & Schuerman, 1987b; Elliot & Briere, 1994;

Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990).

Alexander (1992) has identified rejection; premature parentification; and

the integenerational transmission of trauma as three attachment-related processes

common in abusive families. The process of rejection results in avoidant

attachment and is often determined by parents' own impoverished experiences of

attachment and care. One critical consequence of rejection is that parents do not

respond appropriately to children's distress. Children then learn that these

feelings fall outside the realm of shareable experiences and so are apt to deny

their own feelings of distress and related needs for safety and security. This

dynamic explains the increased vulnerability of such children to revictimisation

(e.g. Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990). The second attachment-related process

described by Alexander (1992) is premature parentification. This role reversal

process is commonly observed in sexually abusive families (e.g. Levang, 1989).

Again, this premature parentification deprives the child of the support and

nurturance they need before and after the trauma of sexual abuse and leads them

to construct maladaptive internal working models for relationships which persist

into other developmental stages. The multigenerational transmission of fear and

unresolved trauma evident in abusive families is the third attachment-related

process identified by Alexander (1992). Here the attachment history of the parent

is recapitulated in their relationship with the child. This dynamic is consistent

with empirical evidence on risk factors which contribute to sexual abusive

behaviour (Friedrich, 1990).

Intrapsychic integration and dissociation

Summit (1983) described how sexually abused children trapped in secrecy and

helplessness adapt to abuse by engaging in denial and dissociation. Dissociation

Effects of CSA

39

is typically used by children as a defense mechanism to protect themselves from

overwhelming anxiety. Dissociation reduces anxiety by psychologically

negating the pain of the immediate impact of the abuse. In order to cope with

sexual abuse, children may split themselves off from an awareness of what is

happening by dissociating from abuse-related physical sensations or they may

cope by pretending the lower part of their body does not exist. Prolonged abuse

forces the child to routinely rely upon dissociation and so a dissociative response

pattern becomes entrenched (Putnam, 1990). Children victimised during the pre-

school period are particularly likely to use dissociation as a defence mechanism

and to develop a chronic pattern of dissociation (Kirby, Chu & Dill, 1993). These

same defence mechanisms which facilitate psychic survival in childhood are

serious obstacles to effective psychological integration in later life. Dissociation

may present in adulthood as psychic numbing, depersonalisation,

disengagement, amnesia for the abuse, borderline personality disorder, and

multiple personality disorder (Briere & Conte, 1993; Elliot & Briere, 1995;

Williams, 1994; Herman & Schatzow, 1987; Barnard & Hirsh, 1985; Bryer et al,

1987; Bliss, 1984; Coons & Milstein, 1986; Anderson, Yasenik & Ross, 1993).

Multiple personality disorder, the most dysfunctional outcome of a chronic

pattern of dissociation, is associated with severe forms of victimisation such as

co-occurring sexual and physical abuse prior to age eight (Kluft, 1990).

Physiological functioning

Severe chronic sexual abuse may have long-term effects on neuroendocrine

functioning (Herman & Van Der Kolk, 1989; Pitman, Orr, Forgue, De Jong &

Claiborn, 1987). Putnam and Trickett (1993) found that abnormal levels of

cortisol occur in preadolescent sexually abused girls. Neuroendocrine

Prevention of CSA

40

abnormalities have also been detected in pre-pubescent boys who were victims of

physical and sexual abuse (Jensen, Pease, Ten Bensel & Garfinkel, 1991).

Sexual abuse may also lead to earlier onset of puberty (Herman-Giddens, Sandler

& Friedman, 1988; Trickett & Putnam, 1993).

THE DEVELOPMENTAL COURSE OF SYMPTOMS

In the immediate aftermath of victimisation between one fifth and two fifths of

abused children seen by clinicians show significant psychological adjustment

problems (e.g. Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990; Mian, Marton & Le Baron, 1996;

Tufts, 1984). As adults, between one fifth a quarter of victims of sexual abuse

experience considerably more impairment than non-victims (e.g. Kendall-Tackett

et al, 1993; Finkelhor et al, 1990; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986). Extreme long-

term effects are not inevitable and a majority of victims appear to recover and

function adequately (Bentovim, Von Elberg & Boston, 1988; Gomes-Schwartz et

al, 1990; Lanktree & Briere, 1995).

Fluctuating patterns of symptoms following sexual abuse have also been

observed. Mannarino et al's (1989) found a significant reduction in the

internalising but not externalising behaviour problems over time and Friedrich

and Reams (1987) reported a rise in sexual preoccupation among preadolescents.

Some long-term effects may be delayed and only appear when the individual

matures or life events precipitate the emergence of abuse-related

symptomatology. This sleeper effect occurred in a study by Kinzl and Biebl

(1992) who found that the onset of severe psychological problems in female

survivors of child sexual abuse was precipitated when their own children reached

the age at which they were first victimised.

Sexual abuse may have different effects on children at different

developmental stages (Kendall-Tackett et al, 1993). Abuse related depression for

instance, may be expressed as clingy behaviour in a pre-schooler, as disruptive

behaviour during pre-adolescence, and as a major mood disorder with suicidal

Effects of CSA

41

ideation and self-injurious behaviour in adolescence. Abuse related symptom

patterns common among pre-schoolers, pre-adolescents and adolescents deserve

particular mention and will be outlined below.

Pre-schoolers

While some early studies of sexually abused pre-schoolers appeared to indicate

they were less behaviourally disturbed than older children (Adams-Tucker, 1982;

Gomes-Schwartz, Horowitz & Sauzier, 1985), subsequent findings have not

borne this out (Friedrich et al, 1986; Mian et al, 1986). The short-term effects

documented as most characteristic of sexually abused pre-school children include

inappropriate sexual behaviour (Friedrich, Beilke & Urquiza, 1988; Friedrich &

Luecke, 1988; Gale, Thompson, Moran & Sack, 1988; Jampole & Weber, 1987)

and excessive internalizing behaviour problems such as tearfulness, nightmares,

clinginess and anxiety (Fagot, Hagan, Youngblade & Potter, 1989; Friedrich,

Urquiza & Beilke, 1986).

Pre-adolescent children

Behavioural and academic problems at school are commonly reported symptoms

among preadolescent children with a history of sexual abuse (Adams-Tucker,

1982; Black, Dubowitz & Harrington, 1994; Elwell & Ephross, 1987; Johnston,

1979; Tong, Oates, & McDowell, 1987). For instance, Tong et al (1987) found

that teachers rated sexually abused children as performing significantly less well

in school work than other non-abused children. However, it is possible that

cognitive deficits may pre-date the trauma of the abuse since the presence of

Prevention of CSA

42

learning difficulties and developmental delays are risk factors for sexual

victimisation (Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1985; Senn, 1988).

Increased internalizing behaviour problems including depression,

anxiety, poor self-esteem, and post-traumatic stress disorder have also been noted

in this age group (Mannarino, Cohen, Smith & Moore-Motily, 1991; McLeer,

Deblinger, Atkins, Foa & Ralphe, 1988) ). Sexually inappropriate aggressive

behaviour while less manifest in this age group than in sexually abused pre-

schoolers or adolescents is still significantly more frequent among preadolescent

victims than among any non-abused controls (Hewitt & Friedrich, 1990;

Friedrich, 1993; Friedrich, Grambasch, Broughton , Kuiper & Beilke, 1991).

Adolescents

Sexually abused adolescents show a wide range of internalizing behaviour

problems including depression, anxiety, low self-esteem, suicidal ideation, self

injurious behaviour, post-traumatic stress disorder and eating disorders such as

bulimia (Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1985; Sansonnet-Hayden, Haley, Marriage &

Fine, 1987: Lindberg & Distad, 1985; Hibbard et al, 1990; Kendall-Tackett et al,

1993). Higher levels of all these symptoms are reported even in non-clinical

samples of sexually abused adolescents (Gidycz & Koss, 1989). Externalizing

behaviour problems such as running away, truanting and substance abuse are

frequently reported sequelae of sexual abuse among adolescents (Briere & Runtz,

1987; Burgess, Hartman & McCormack, 1987; Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990;

Lindberg & Distad, 1985).

Increased sexual behaviour among adolescents is manifested as promiscuity

(Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1985; Chandy, Blum & Resnick, 1996), involvement in

prostitution or gender identity disturbance (Bagley & Young, 1987) and sexual

confusion (Friedrich, Beilke & Urquiza, 1987; Sansonnet-Hayden et al, 1987).

Particularly among adolescent males, abuse-related sexualized behaviour may be

Effects of CSA

43

expressed as sexually abusive behaviour towards younger children (Gil &

Johnson, 1993; Green, 1993)

EFFECTS OF CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE ON ADULTS

At least a fifth of sexually abused children suffer serious long-term

psychological harm (Browne & Finkelhor, 1986). However, the effects of

childhood sexual abuse on adult adjustment are far from uniform. In adults,

childhood sexual abuse has been found to lead to a wide variety of adjustment

problems including anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and

dissociative states; depression and self-injurious behaviour; relationship and

sexuality difficulties; antisocial behaviour and sexual offending; and parenting

problems (Beitchman et al, 1992; Polusny & Follette, 1995).

Anxiety

Anxiety is one of the most common responses to the trauma of child sexual abuse

(Donaldson & Gardner, 1985; Peters, 1988). Sexual abuse survivors in the

general population are more likely than non-victims to suffer from agoraphobia,

panic disorder, obsessive compulsive disorder and social phobia (Ernst, Angst &

Foldenyi, 1993). The extent to which the experience of child sexual abuse

predisposes adult survivors to chronic anxiety is shown by the finding that they

are five times more likely to be diagnosed with an anxiety disorder than their

non-abused peers (Saunders, Villeponteaux, Lipovsky & Kilpatrick, 1992a; Stein

et al, 1988). Furthermore, in clinical samples, chronic or severe anxiety is

Prevention of CSA

44

correlated with repeated violent sexually abusive experiences (Briere & Runtz,

1987; Herman & Schatzow, 1987).

Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)

Anxiety and fear stemming from the trauma of early sexually abuse may also

manifest itself in the development of post-traumatic stress disorder (e.g. Briere,

1992; Briere & Runtz, 1991; Rowan, Foy, Rodriquez & Ryan, 1994). Child

sexual abuse has been shown to result in PTSD in as many as 36% of adult

survivors and is particularly prevalent in victims reporting contact sexual abuse

and child rape ((Donaldson & Gardner, 1985; Saunders et al, 1992a, 1992b).

While not all victims of child sexual abuse go on to develop full blown PTSD,

the experience of both intrusive traumatic memories and attempts at avoidance of

these memories are common (Elliot & Briere, 1995). A history of concurrent

physical and sexual abuse during childhood may increase the risk of PTSD (e.g.

Rodriguez, Ryan & Foy, 1992).

Dissociation

Dissociation occurs commonly among adult survivors of sexual abuse (Briere &

Runtz, 1987; Chu & Dill, 1990; Kluft, 1990; Putnam, 1990). As has already been

noted, dissociation presents as psychic numbing, depersonalisation,

disengagement, amnesia for the abuse, borderline personality disorder, or

multiple personality disorder. A significant proportion of child sexual abuse

victims report partial or complete amnesia for their abuse experiences (Briere &

Conte, 1993; Elliot & Briere, 1995; Williams, 1994). This phenomenon was also

noted by Groth and Burgess (1979) among sex offenders asked about childhood

abuse. Many did not deny the experience but were not sure if something had

happened. Dissociation of abuse related memories appears to be correlated with

chronic and violent maltreatment with an early age of onset (Briere & Conte,

Effects of CSA

45

1993; Herman & Schatzow, 1987). An association between borderline personality

disorder and child sexual abuse has been reported by several authors (Barnard &

Hirsh, 1985; Bryer et al, 1987). Multiple personality disorder, the most extreme

manifestation of dissociation, is associated with extremely severe and concurrent

physical and sexual abuse (Bliss, 1984; Coons & Milstein, 1986; Anderson,

Yasenik & Ross, 1993).

Depression and self-injurious behaviour

Depression is consistently reported as the most common long-term effect of child

sexual abuse and has been documented in a variety of clinical and non-clinical

studies (Briere & Runtz, 1988; Browne & Finkelhor, 1986; Mullen et al, 1996;

Russell, 1986; Silverman, Reinherz & Giacona, 1996; Wyatt & Powell, 1988;

Beitchman et al,1992). Stein et al (1988) in a large scale community survey

found that sexual abuse victims have as much as a fourfold greater lifetime risk

for major depression than non-victims. Victims of sexual abuse are more likely to

be identified as requiring treatment, usually for depression (Mullen, Romans-

Clarkson, Walton and Herbison, 1988) and the degree of depression is directly

related to the severity of the abuse (Peters, 1988). In studies of clinical

populations higher rates of depression have been found in female patients with

histories of sexual abuse compared to psychiatric control groups (Margo &

McLees, 1991; Pribor & Dinwiddie, 1992). Recovery from a major depressive

episode is also negatively affected by a history of child sexual abuse. For

instance, Zlotnik, Ryan, Miller and Keitner (1995) reported that women without a

history of child sexual abuse were three times more likely to have recovered from

depressive episodes by 12 months. Given the strength of the association between

a history of child sexual abuse and depression, it is not surprising that increased

suicidal ideation and self-injurious behaviours have also been linked to sexual

abuse in studies of both community and clinical populations (Bagley & Ramsay,

Prevention of CSA

46

1986; Briere & Runtz, 1986; Bryer, Nelson, Miller & Krol, 1987; Silverman et al,

1996; Briere & Zaidi, 1989; Ernst et al, 1993).

Interpersonal relationships and sexuality

Women who have been sexually abused in childhood report a greater fear of both

men and women (Briere & Runtz, 1988). They report difficulty trusting others

and are more likely to have a history of failed relationships or marriages than

non-abused women (deYoung, 1982; Russell, 1986). They are more likely to

have fewer friends (Gold, 1986), experience less satisfaction in their relationships

and have more maladaptive interpersonal patterns (Elliot, 1994). Sexual

maladjustment has been found to be associated with a childhood history of sexual

abuse in both clinical (e.g. Briere & Runtz, 1988) and non-clinical samples (e.g.

Saunders et al, 1992b). Saunders et al (1992b) found that 32% of sexually

abused subjects, compared to 16% of non abused subjects, met diagnostic criteria

for a psychosexual disorder at the time of interview. Briere and Runtz (1988)

reported that 45% of sexual abuse survivors, compared to 15% of non-abused

clients at a walk-in counselling centre reported sexual problems.

In adulthood, victims of child sexual abuse have been found to engage in

more promiscuous or indiscriminate sexual behaviour (Briere & Runtz, 1991;

Herman, 1981; Courtois, 1988). Such promiscuous behaviour is associated with

higher rates of HIV infection among adult survivors of child sexual abuse

(Zierler, Feingold et al, 1991). Wyatt et al (1993) concluded that the severity of

sexual abuse experiences contributes to the higher frequency of sexual activity

observed during adulthood thus increasing the chance of exposure to HIV

infection and other sexually transmitted diseases.

Another form of high risk behaviour associated with a history of sexual

victimisation in childhood is prostitution which is even more common among

male victims than females (Silbert & Pines, 1981; Zierler, Feingold et al, 1991).

Effects of CSA

47

Just as sexual abuse increases the risk of subsequent sexual victimisation for

children (Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990) adult victims of child sexual abuse also

show a greater vulnerability to revictimisation, both physical and sexual later in

life (Wind & Silvern, 1992). In summary, child sexual abuse appears to have a

devastating effect on adult sexuality for a significant proportion of victims, these

effects include impaired sexual satisfaction and functioning, promiscuous

behaviour, higher rates of HIV infection, prostitution and a greater vulnerability

for revictimisation which may involve sexual assaults or physical violence.

Anger, antisocial behaviour and sexual offending

Chronic irritability, uncontrollable rage and fear of their own anger are common

complaints among adult survivors of child sexual abuse (Briere & Runtz, 1987;

Donaldson & Gardner, 1985). Survivors are nearly twice as likely to have

problems with anger than non-victims (Briere & Runtz, 1988). The rage

experienced by former victims of sexual abuse can intensify when it is

restimulated by interpersonal events which parallel the original experience of

child abuse (e.g. Kinzl & Biebl, 1992). This anger may be internalised as self-

injurious behaviour (e.g. Courtois, 1988) or externalised as antisocial behaviours

including the perpetration of abuse (e.g. Briere & Runtz, 1988; Herman, 1988).

A small proportion of victims externalise their rage by sexually abusing others.

Approximately 30% of offenders report having been sexually abused as children

(Murphy & Smith, 1996) and there is some evidence to suggest that this group of

offenders have higher overall levels of sexual deviancy and increase levels of

psychological disturbances (Langevin, Wortzman, Wright & Handy, 1989). One

hypothesis is that the victim who becomes a victimiser is identifying with the

aggressor and by taking on the abusive role. This identification with the aggressor

may have a number of pay-offs including reducing the sense of powerlessness

associated with the original trauma.

Prevention of CSA

48

Parenting

Women who were sexually abused as children, particularly victims of incest, may

have difficulty parenting their own children and creating a safe and secure home

environment for their offspring Cole and Woolger (1989) found that female

incest survivors, compared to survivors of extrafamilial abuse, had excessive

expectations of autonomy for their children; were less accepting or supportive of

their children; and also reported greater conflict with their children. Children of

mothers who experienced incest have been found to be at risk for maltreatment

(Cole, Woolger, Power & Smith, 1992). Also, the children of adult victims of

child sexual abuse have a higher risk for intrafamilial sexual victimisation

(Cohen, 1995; Gelinas, 1983).

MEDIATING VARIABLES

A variety of variables mediate the effects of sexual abuse and these include age

of onset of the abuse; the frequency and duration of the abuse; the degree of

violence involved in the abuse; the nature of the abusive acts; the relationship to

the abuser; the gender of the victim; the child's cognitive appraisal and coping

style; family support; and the context of the disclosure

Age of onset.

Results of empirical studies on the impact of age of onset of abuse on later

psychological adjustment are far from clear-cut (Beitchman et al, 1992 ; Kendall-

Tackett et al, 1993). For example, in studies of children Zivney, Nash and Hulsey

(1988) found that those victims with early age of onset were most likely to show

psychological adjustment problem. However, findings from other studies show

Effects of CSA

49

that children abused during pre-teenage and teenage years show the greatest

levels of disturbance (Adams-Tucker, 1982; Sirles, Smith & Kusama, 1989). The

picture emerging from studies of adult survivors is also equivocal. While some

studies have reported a clear association between younger age and greater

adjustment problems (e.g. Courtois, 1979; Meiselman, 1978: Ussher &

Dewberry, 1995; Sedney & Brooks, 1984; Murphy, Kilpatrick et al, 1988), other

have found no significant association (Finkelhor, 1979; Langmade, 1983;

Russell, 1986). Bagley and Ramsay (1986) reported that when the traumatic

effect of penetrative abuse was controlled for, the effect of age of onset of abuse

was no longer significantly associated with the degree of later maladjustment.

The major difficulty in assessing the independent contribution of age of

onset to outcome is that this variable is connected with several other abuse

specific variables. Older children and adolescents are more likely to be

subjected to penetrative abuse (e.g. Murphy, Kilpatrick et al, 1988) while

younger children are more likely to be abused by a father or stepfather (e.g.

Russell, 1986). Both these variables are associated with high levels of trauma and

harm. Age of onset may also be correlated with duration of abuse which again is

associated with severity of impact. Furthermore, because the full extent of the

aftermath of the abuse may not be evident when children are first assessed the

effects of the abuse may be underestimated (Gomes - Schwartz et al, 1990) .

It is also probable that severe and repeated sexual, has a more detrimental

effect on psychological adjustment if it begins when the child is very young

because it disrupts the child's sense of safety and security and may lead the child

to adopt dissociation as a defence mechanism (Cole & Putnam, 1992). In contrast

it is also probable that less severe forms of abuse, particularly abuse that does not

involve physical contact such as exhibitionism or voyeurism has a less marked

effect on younger children since they are less likely to appraise such abuse as

particularly threatening, whereas older children may perceive it as a major threat

to their well being. Clearly, questions remain to be answered about the

relationship of age of onset of abuse to later psychological adjustment.

Prevention of CSA

50

Frequency and duration

The frequency and duration of child sexual abuse is associated with both short-

term and long-term psychological adjustment problems (Beitchman et al, 1992,;

Kendall-Tackett et al, 1993). Briere and Runtz (1985) and Conte and Schuerman

(1987a) in studies of clinical populations of adults and children respectively,

found that the duration of abuse was the factor most strongly associated with

degree of maladjustment. The findings of several non-clinical studies all support

the increased traumatising effect of abuse of extended duration (Bagley &

Ramsay 1986; Peters, 1988; Russell, 1986; Tsai, Feldman-Summers & Edgar,

1979) . These results are particularly persuasive because they are based on

community samples of adults and so represent a wide spectrum of frequency and

severity of abuse. Sexual abuse by multiple perpetrators has also been associated

with more severe outcome in both children and adults (e.g. Friedrich et al, 1986;

Wind & Silvern, 1992).

Violence

In studies of sexually abused children, sexual abuse involving violence has

consistently been found to lead to more profound psychological problems (e.g.

Conte & Schuerman, 1987a; Cohen and Mannarino, 1988; Elwell & Ephross,

1987). This finding parallels the trends reported in studies of adult survivors of

sexual abuse (e.g. Gold, 1986; Mullen et al, 1988; Peters, 1988; Russell, 1986).

In Elwell and Ephross’ (1987) sample of school-age sexual abuse victims,

the most severe symptoms were associated with physical injury to the child, the

use of force by the perpetrator and sexual abuse involving penetration. Cohen

and Mannarino (1988) found that children who experienced abuse involving

physical force or vaginal penetration subsequently engaged in a high level of

aggression and externalising behaviour problems. This suggests that more

Effects of CSA

51

violent assaults may result in more subsequent aggressive behaviour by the

victim.

Beitchman (1992) in his review of long-term effects of child sexual abuse,

asserts that the interaction of violence and aggression with sexual abuse may

produce effects specific to this combination which do not occur or are rarely

associated with the presence of either aggression or sexual abuse alone. These

effects include Multiple Personality Disorder and suicidality. Studies such as

those by Mullen et al (1996) and Gold (1986) indicate that the concurrent

experience of multiple forms of abuse produce higher levels of psychopathology

and poorer outcome patterns among survivors.

Type of sexually abusive act

While the literature is clear on the impact of violent sexual assault during

childhood, whether the type of abusive act consistently influences trauma is less

certain (e.g. Finkelhor, 1979). The results of some studies which focused on

short-term effects of abuse, such as those by Elwell and Ephross (1987) and

Mian, Marton and Le Baron (1996), suggest that experiences which involve

penetration do result in more severe symptomatology in childhood. Most of the

available research on adult survivors of sexual abuse also implicate more serious

sexual abuse, including anal or vaginal penetration as a major factor in producing

serious long-term psychological problems (e.g. Bagley & Ramsay, 1986; Russell,

1986; Wind & Silvern, 1992). Studies which failed to find an association between

the severity of the abusive acts and degree of adjustment problems (e.g.

Finkelhor, 1979; Fromuth, 1986) had very low rates of actual or attempted

intercourse among sample participants, limiting the scope of these studies in

terms of assessing this particular variable. The available evidence from studies

where abuse involving penetration was frequent enough to permit valid

conclusions to be drawn, suggests that more invasive abuse is associated with

Prevention of CSA

52

more traumatic impact in the short-term (e.g. Black, Dubowitz & Harrington,

1994) and in adulthood (e.g. Mullen et al,1988).

Relationship to Offender

The most devastating violation of trust occurs when the perpetrator is closely

related to the victim (e.g. Kendall -Tackett et al, 1993; Beitchman et al, 1992).

Studies investigating the relationship between the identity of the perpetrator and

victim distress have consistently found that abuse by a father or stepfather is

associated with the most serious sequelae in both children and adults (e.g.

Adams-Tucker, 1982; Friedrich et al, 1986; Mian et al, 1996; Peters, 1988;

Russell, 1986; Sirles et al, 1989; Ussher & Dewberry, 1995). Few studies have

examined the differential impact of abuse by fathers compared with stepfathers

but Gomes-Schwartz et al (1990) found that children abused by a parent

substitute (e.g. step-father or mother’s live-in partner) were the most severely

distressed. These children also had the least supportive mothers. These findings

highlight the significance of family intactness and the interaction of maternal

support with the identity of the offender in some cases of sexual abuse.

The consequences for the child and family are likely to be particularly

traumatic when intrafamilial rather then extrafamilial abuse is disclosed. Unlike

extrafamilial abuse, intrafamilial abuse may lead to family break-up and

imprisonment of the abuser or disbelief and scapegoating of the abused child.

The frequency and duration of the abuse is also likely to be of greater

significance when the perpetrator is a family member compared with cases of

extrafamilial abuse (e.g. Bagley & Ramsay,1986). Thus, the association of abuse

by a close family member, particularly a father or father figure, with the most

severe outcomes is complex since other variables are involved including

frequency and duration of abuse; anticipated consequences of disclosure; and

actual consequences of disclosure.

Effects of CSA

53

Gender of the victim

The gender of the victim is associated with both the nature of the sexual abuse

experienced and subsequent psychological adjustment problems. Pierce and

Pierce (1985) in a chart review of 15 male and 180 female victims of child sexual

abuse found that boys were more likely to be abused by a stepfather and girls

were more likely to be abused by their natural fathers. According to these

authors force and violence were more likely to be used against male victims.

Other studies have found that the co-occurrence of physical abuse is more

common in male victims (e.g. Sansonnet-Hayden et al, 1987; Sirles et al, 1989).

While Adams-Tucker (1982) reported that girls were more likely to suffer

multiple sexual victimisations and abuse of longer duration. These studies show

that the experience of abuse may differ for males and females.

Briere et al (1988) and Seidner, Calhoun and Kilpatrick (1985) found

higher levels of maladjustment in male victims as compared with female victims.

Chandy, Blum and Resnick (1996) examined gender differences in outcomes of

sexually abused youngsters and found that female adolescents with a history of

child sexual abuse engaged in more internalising behaviour problems while males

displayed a tendency to show more externalising behaviour problems. Males

were at higher risk than females of poor school performance, delinquent activities

and sexual risk taking. Conversely female adolescents were more likely to

develop suicidal ideation, self-injurious behaviour and eating disorders. Among

the sequelae specifically associated with males are sexual preoccupations and

compulsiveness (Singer et al, 1989), gender identity confusion and sexual

orientation confusion (Finkelhor, 1979). Finkelhor (1979) found that males who

had been sexually abused prior to age 13 by an older male were four times more

likely to be homosexually active. The high rate of child sexual abuse histories

among sex offenders (approximately 30%) demonstrates the long-term impact of

child sexual abuse on adult male sexual adjustment (e.g. Becker, 1988).

Prevention of CSA

54

Cognitive appraisal and coping

Victims' appraisals of abusive experiences, their attribution of blame and their

understanding of the social and interpersonal meaning of abuse, as well as the

coping strategies they use to deal with trauma account for some of the individual

differences in the impact of similar traumatic experiences on different victims

(Spaccarelli, 1994). Younger children’s lack of understanding may buffer them

from the immediate impact of trauma, although realisation in retrospect can have

very negative consequences (Gelinas, 1983). Higher levels of cognitive

functioning are correlated with greater reported distress (Shapiro, Leifer, Marton

& Kassem, 1990). This may be because older children or those with more

sophisticated cognitive functioning are more aware of the implications of the

abuse. Changing cognitive appraisals may also effect willingness to disclose

abuse (Finkelhor, 1995). For instance, boys become more reluctant to disclose

abuse as they approach puberty due to an increased awareness of the stigma

related to homosexuality ( Urquiza & Keating 1990; Watkins & Bentovim, 1992).

Among the abuse related cognitions reported by children are self-blame,

perceptions of the self as different to peers, and reduced interpersonal trust

(Mannarino, Cohen & Berman, 1994 ). It is not surprising that many adult

survivors of abuse continue to experience abuse-related cognitions in the form of

self-blame, low self-esteem, lack of a sense of self-efficacy, feelings of

helplessness and perceptions of life as threatening or hopeless (Gold, 1986; Jehu,

1988). These negative appraisals are associated with impaired psychological

functioning in adults and adolescents (Drauker, 1989; Morrow, 1991). Greater

distrust in children is associated with a global, stable and internal attributions for

abusive experiences reflecting a sense of self-blame as constant across time and

situations (Wolfe, Gentile & Wolfe, 1989) .

The coping strategy which appears to be associated with the most severe

symptomatology is avoidant coping, represented by denial, dissociation and

suppression (e.g. Johnson & Kenkel, 1991; Leitenberg, Greenwald & Cado,

Effects of CSA

55

1992). Active coping strategies such as disclosure and support seeking are

associated with better long-term adjustment, although purposeful disclosure may

be associated with increased distress initially (Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990;

Wyatt & Newcomb, 1990). However the positive impact of active coping

strategies is likely to be mediated by the response to disclosure of significant

members of the child's network and the amount of support available, particularly

from non-abusing parents and family members.

Family factors

A number of specific features of the family environment are risk factors for

sexual abuse (Beitchman et al, 1991). These include family breakdown and

dysfunction (Mian et al, 1986); parental psychopathology and violence

(Friedrich & Leucke, 1988); and parental substance abuse (Burgess et al, 1987).

Significantly, a history of sexual abuse in the mothers of sexually abused children

has been reported in a number of studies (Friedrich & Reams, 1987; Sansonnet-

Hayden et al, 1987; Smith & Israel, 1987). This implies the possibility of an

inter-generational transmission of sexual abuse.

In addition to increasing the likelihood of intrafamilial sexual abuse,

family dysfunction may also exacerbate the effects of the abuse (Alexander,

1992; Courtois, 1988). Higher levels of distress are found in children whose

families are characterised by communication problems, organizational

difficulties, high levels of conflict and low levels of cohesion (Friedrich &

Reams, 1987; Mian et al, 1996).

Maternal support and belief in the child’s disclosure of sexual abuse and

warmth toward the child promotes recovery and helps to mitigate the impact of

trauma (Conte & Schuerman, 1987b; Everson, Hunter, Runyan, Edelson &

Coulter, 1989; Peters, 1988). Maternal support is compromised most often when

the perpetrator is a step-father or boyfriend (Elliot & Briere, 1994; Gomes-

Schwartz et al, 1990). Children who lack maternal support are more likely to

Prevention of CSA

56

recant an abuse accusation (Elliot & Briere, 1994) and to experience an out-of-

home placement (Hunter, Coulter, Runyan & Everson, 1990).

The context of disclosure

The way disclosure occurs and the context of the revelation are important

predictors of both short and long-term consequences for the victims of child

sexual abuse (Everson et al, 1989; Gomes-Schwartz et al, 1990; Sauzier, 1989;

Wyatt & Mickey, 1988; Russell,1986). Most sexual abuse is never disclosed

(Finkelhor, 1990; Russell, 1986) and only a minority of abuse is ever reported to

authorities subsequent to disclosure (e.g. Saunders et al, 1992a; Wyatt & Mickey,

1988). Among adult survivors of child sexual abuse, nondisclosure of abuse has

consistently been found to be associated with more profound psychological

adjustment problems in adulthood (e.g. Wyatt & Mickey, 1988; Wyatt &

Newcomb, 1990). For children the lowest levels of distress are found among

those who did not disclose abuse but whose abuse was discovered accidentally

(e.g. Sauzier, 1989). Taken together, these findings suggest that disclosure is

inherently stressful and results in increased levels of symptomatology in the

short-term but may lead to enhanced recovery in the longer-term.

The levels of stress and trauma associated with disclosure are strongly

associated with family dynamics and the anticipated and actual reactions of

caretakers and others to the revelation of the abuse. (Sauzier, 1989; Lawson &

Chaffin, 1992; Sorenson & Snow, 1991). As mentioned previously, support from

non-abusing parents and their belief in the child’s disclosure is a critical factor in

the child’s adaptation to sexual abuse (Everson et al, 1989; Gomes-Schwartz et

al, 1990; Wyatt & Mickey ,1988). Children who are well supported show fewer

adjustment problems.

Believing a child’s disclosure, offering protection and support and

refraining from blame or pressure are considered crucial to the child’s recovery.

However, as Lawson and Chaffin (1992) point out, parental attitude to disclosure

Effects of CSA

57

is not a discrete post-disclosure event but may be explicitly or covertly manifest

prior to the disclosure itself. It may therefore influence not only post-disclosure

adjustment but also the child’s decision to reveal the abuse. Lawson and Chaffin

(1992) studied 18 children who were not previously suspected of being sexually

abused, and for whom there were only physical indicators of possible abuse (i.e.

sexually transmitted disease). They found that children whose caretakers

accepted the possibility of sexual abuse disclosed at a rate 3.5 times that of those

whose caretakers denied any possibility of abuse.

Professional interventions following disclosure and the nature of the legal

process in which the child participates may influence the impact of abuse on the

child's psychological adjustment. For example, multiple interviews by different

personnel are associated with greater psychological adjustment problems

(Tedesco & Schnell, 1987), although repeated interviews by the same person may

not increase distress. The specific effects of court involvement and testimony

have been investigated in a small number of longitudinal studies. For example,

Runyan et al (1988) reported that poorer recovery was associated with

participation in protracted legal proceedings. Williams (1991) demonstrated that

some of the adverse effects of court testimony could be mitigated by giving

videotaped evidence or using closed-circuit televisions systems. Overall, while

certain types of court proceedings appear to delay recovery, these aspects are

open to modification and consequently, much of the negative impact associated

with legal proceedings is preventable.

CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE ACCOMODATION SYNDROME

Summit (1983) argues that child sexual abuse, its effects and the dynamics of

unsuccessful disclosure may be conceptualized as an accommodation syndrome

with five distinctive features: secrecy; helplessness; entrapment and

accommodation; delayed and unconvincing disclosure; and retraction.

Prevention of CSA

58

Secrecy

CSA is predicated upon secrecy. The secrecy surrounding the sexually abusive

incident serves both as a source of fear and a promise of safety. The child is

terrified of the implied or explicit consequences of the secret being discovered

and so is intimidated into silence. The child also experiences a sense of safety

from reassurances given by a perpetrator that, as long as the secrecy is

maintained, the child will be all right. So the abused child rarely discloses. The

power of secrecy is demonstrated by retrospective surveys which indicate the

majority of victims never told anyone during their childhood. Summit concludes

that children must be helped to expect a supportive, nonpunitive response to the

disclosure before they will be able to share the secret.

Helplessness

Summit points out that a child is three times more likely to be molested by a

recognized trusted adult than by a stranger. Given the authority of the adult and

the dependence of the child, the victim is easily rendered helpless. It is this

terrified helplessness which is often misconstrued as consent, or compliance. The

child is unable to protest, unable to resist and frequently unable to escape. Adults

may despise helplessness because it runs counter to the cherished adult sense of

free will and are quick to attribute blame to children who submit to abuse.

Summit contends that children need to have their helplessness and noncomplicity

acknowledged and their innocence affirmed if they are not to become filled with

guilt and self-hate for somehow inviting the sexual victimization.

Entrapment and accommodation

Effects of CSA

59

Constrained by the demands of secrecy and rendered helpless by the power

imbalance in their relationship, the sexually abused child has little choice but to

accept the situation in an attempt to survive. The child must find a way to

accommodate to the experience which often entails not only intrusive sexual

demands, but also a need to view the perpetrator as an idealized authority. That

accommodation may entail the child assuming a sense of power and control over

the situation, such that the child accepts responsibility and blame for the sexually

abusive relationship. The normal moral order is reversed. Disclosure becomes a

sin greater than the abuse. Accommodation mechanisms used by children to cope

with this way of viewing their abusive situation include splitting of reality,

altered consciousness, hysterical phenomena, delinquency and self-mutilation.

Delayed, conflicted and unconvincing disclosure

The form which disclosure takes and the factors which trigger it can mitigate

against the youngster being believed. Confronted with an angry and rebellious

adolescent, many adults assume that a disclosure is a revolutionary response

against a parent’s attempt to achieve reasonable control and discipline. Denial

also operates strongly when a child or adolescent who has demonstrated no

outward conflict or behaviour problems discloses sexual abuse. The fact that the

child did not talk before now and showed no signs of abuse can be used to

invalidate the disclosure, or to attribute responsibility for its occurrence to the

victim. Summit concludes that specialists must help skeptics overcome their

disbelief.

Retraction

Prevention of CSA

60

Retraction is described by Summit as an escape route often taken by children and

adolescents in the chaotic aftermath of disclosure. Children can feel revictimised

by the process surrounding validation. Their families may be fragmented and the

perpetrator’s threats may appear to be fulfilled. Retraction restores the family's

equilibrium. The child bears the burden of either preserving or destroying the

family. In the absence of immediate support and strategic interventions to force

the offender to take responsibility for the abuse and its aftermath, the child will

usually retract, thereby undermining the credibility of the original disclosure.

Summit's model highlights that the very dynamics of the Sexual Abuse

Accommodation Syndrome reinforce the victimization of children and society's

complacency and denial of its destructiveness. Only by challenging and

interrupting the accommodation process can the chain of abuse be broken.

Summit's model is an important framework for understanding some of the effects

of child sexual abuse and the dynamics surrounding chronic abuse and

unsuccessful disclosure. The model highlights the adaptive role of some of the

effects of abuse (e.g., splitting, self-mutilation) and disclosure related difficulties

(e.g. retraction). as being important for the victim’s survival from the victim's

perspective.

TRAUMAGENIC DYNAMICS MODEL

Like Summit, Finkelhor and Browne (1985) have developed a conceptual

framework within which to conceptualize the many effects of CSA. They argue

that sexual abuse traumatises children through four distinctive mechanisms,

which in turn, account for the variety of outcomes experienced by victims. The

four traumagenic dynamics are (a) traumatic sexualisation, or experience of

developmentally inappropriate sexual behaviours; (b) powerlessness, or feelings

resulting from the contravention of the child’s will and domination by the

abuser; (c) stigmatisation, or the shame, self-blame and negative connotations of

the abuse which the victim internalises; and (d) betrayal , or the shattering of the

child’s belief that trusted adults will protect and not harm them. These key

dynamics, alter a child’s cognitive or emotional orientation to the world and

Effects of CSA

61

distort the child’s self-concept, world view or affective capacities, leading to the

psychological and behavioural problems characteristic of child victims and adult

survivors of sexual abuse.

Traumatic Sexualisation

Traumatic sexualisation is the process involved in the inappropriate conditioning

of the child’s sexual responsiveness and the socialisation of the victim into

distorted beliefs and assumptions about sexual behaviour. Several distinct

processes mediate the dynamic of traumatic sexualisation. These include the

manner in which sexual behaviour, inappropriate to the child’s level of maturity,

is rewarded and the fact that parts of the child’s body become fetishised by the

abuser and thus acquires distorted importance and meaning for the victim. The

distorted views of sexual behaviour and morality transmitted by the offender also

contribute to traumatic sexualisation as do other frightening aspects of the

abusive experience which may become strongly associated with sexual activity.

Traumatic sexualisation increases the salience of sexual issues (e.g. Gale

et al, 1988) results in confusion about sexual norms or identity (e.g. Sansonnet-

Hayden et al, 1987) and accounts for abuse outcomes such as sexual precocity,

compulsive sexuality and sexual aversion or dysfunction (e.g. Burgess, Hartman

& McCormack, 1987; Courtois, 1988), as well as aggressive sexual behaviours

(Friedrich & Leucke, 1988). The impact of traumatic sexualisation is manifest in

the symptomatic behaviour of pre-schoolers in their sexualised play, compulsive

masturbation and age-inappropriate sexual knowledge (e.g. Gale, Thompson,

Moran & Sack, 1988; Friedrich, Grambsch, Broughton, Kuiper & Beilke, 1991).

In older pre-adolescent children, sexual preoccupation, sexual aggression and

other inappropriate behaviours are again more common in sexually abused

children than their non abused counterparts (e.g. Deblinger, McLeer, Atkins,

Ralphe & Foa, 1989; Friedrich & Leucke, 1988). Adolescents may manifest the

impact of this dynamic through promiscuity, prostitution or sexual aggression

Prevention of CSA

62

(e.g. Burgess, Hartman & McCormack, 1987; Sansonnet-Hayden et al, 1987).

Gender identity problems are also found in adolescent victims (e.g. Johnson &

Shrier, 1985; Sansonnet-Hayden et al, 1987). In adulthood, traumatic

sexualization is manifested as sexual dysfunction or sexual offending (e.g.

Courtois, 1979; Gelinas, 1983).

Betrayal

The traumagenic dynamic of betrayal arises from the child’s realisation that a

trusted person has used, manipulated and failed to protect them. It may involve

the shattering of the child’s confidence in the most fundamental of relationships,

with their parents and thus may destroy or impair their subsequent ability to trust

others (e.g. Mannarino et al, 1994). This betrayal may occur in a variety of ways:

directly, at the hands of the abuser, when the child realises the callous disregard

inherent in the sexual abuse; or betrayal may involve the realisation that others

have colluded with or failed to protect them from the abuser. It may also stem

from the reactions of others to the disclosure of abuse. Children who are

disbelieved, blamed, or ostracised, experience a double betrayal, in the sense that

the impact of the abuse is compounded by the negative reaction to disclosure

(Finkelhor & Browne, 1985).

The dynamic of betrayal is linked to feelings such as grief, depression,

heightened dependency, mistrust and anger. It is associated with clinging

behaviour, vulnerability to further abuse, intimacy problems and conduct

problems, all well documented sequelae among child victims and adult survivors

of child sexual abuse (Kendall-Tackett et al, 1993; Beitchman et al,1992 ). The

prevalence of depression in survivors of child sexual abuse (e.g. Peters, 1985)

may reasonably be seen as an attenuated grief reaction in the wake of betrayal.

The heightened dependency seen in some young victims (e.g. Mian et al, 1996)

may emerge as a constant search for a redeeming relationship in adulthood which

increases their vulnerability to physical, sexual and psychological revictimisation

Effects of CSA

63

(e.g. Wind & Silvern, 1992; Wyatt & Newcomb, 1990). Difficulty in intimacy

also operates as a form of protection (e.g. Elliot, 1994). At the opposite end of

the spectrum of reactions to betrayal, are hostility and anger rather than

dependence. The anger and hostility shown by a significant proportion of victims

may be a primitive attempt to protect the self from further betrayal and distress

(e.g. Briere & Runtz, 1988; Donaldson & Gardner, 1985 ). Antisocial behaviour

and offending behaviour are forms of retaliation for the original betrayal (e.g.

Herman, 1988; Gil & Johnson, 1993).

Stigmatisation

The dynamic of stigmatisation relates to the isolation and sense of differentness

experienced by many victims. Keeping the secret of the abuse increases the

victims sense of isolation (e.g. Wyatt & Newcomb, 199o) and diminishes their

self-esteem (e.g. Mannarino et al, 1991; Zivney et al, 1988) leading to negative

self-evaluations. Stigmatisation covers all of the mechanisms which operate to

undermine the child’s positive self image; the sense of shame that is instilled; the

accompanying self-blame; the ostracism suffered; and the negative stereotypes

acquired from society (Kendall-Tackett et al, 1993). These are linked to sequelae

such as social marginalism , for example involvement in drug addiction or

criminal activities or prostitution (Bagley & King, 1990; Silbert & Pines, 1981).

At its most extreme, the dynamic of stigmatisation leads to self-injurious

behaviour and suicide (Briere & Runtz, 1987 ; Saunders et al, 1992b).

Powerlessness

The traumagenic dynamic of powerlessness is related to a constellation of effects,

including fear, anxiety and aggression (Finkelhor & Browne, 1985). Symptoms

Prevention of CSA

64

of fear and anxiety, such as nightmares, phobias and somatic complaints which

reflect powerlessness are among the most common reactions noted in sexually

abused children (Adams-Tucker 1981; Tufts, 1984; Kolko, Moser & Weldy,

1988). Anxiety is also common in adults along with eating disorders (e.g.

Palmer, Chaloner & Oppenheimer, 1992) and dissociation (Briere & Runtz,

1987). The experience of powerlessness impairs the child’s sense of mastery,

competence and their coping skills (Black et al, 1994; Silverman et al, 1996). For

example, Silverman et al (1996) found that sexually abused adolescents showed

an impaired sense of self-efficacy, learning problems, school difficulties,

depression and anxiety. Studies of adult survivors of child sexual abuse also

support this association. For instance, the high rates of revictimisation reported

in several studies (e.g. Russell, 1986; Sorenson, Siegel, Golding & Stein, 1991)

suggest that for some victims the feelings of powerlessness associated with the

original abuse persist across time, situations, and relationships and render them

vulnerable to further abuse. Aggression may occur as a compensatory reaction to

the experience of powerlessness. A need to control and dominate typical of male

victims (Silverman et al, 1996) which results in antisocial behaviour (e.g. Bagley

& King, 1990) or at its most extreme, sexual offending (e.g. Gil & Johnson,

1993) arises from the dynamic of powerlessness.

From the preceding account of Finkelhor and Browne’s (1985)

traumagenic dynamics model of child sexual abuse it can be seen that this

conceptualisation provides a comprehensive and multifaceted description of the

multiple processes involved in traumatisation.

CONCLUSIONS

From this discussion it may be concluded that child sexual abuse has a

devastating effect on behaviour, attachment, intrapsychic integration and

physiological functioning which persists throughout childhood and into

adulthood for a significant proportion of victims. Broadly speaking, this review

Effects of CSA

65

shows that with respect to behaviour, child sexual abuse may lead to an increase

in internalizing and externalizing behaviour problems and an increase in

inappropriate sexualized behaviour. At an interpersonal level, sexual abuse may

disrupt parent-child attachment and this in turn may compromise children's

capacity to make and maintain supportive relationships with peers and other

adults. At an intrapsychic level, children may cope with sexual abuse through

dissociation and subsequently develop a dissociative response set which

compromises psychological integration. In the most severe cases this may lead to

the development of multiple personality disorder. At a physiological level, sexual

abuse may lead to abnormal neuroendocrine functioning.

The impact of child sexual abuse shows a developmental trajectory in that

both the nature and severity of symptoms vary with age. Developmentally

specific problems following sexual abuse occur in pre-school children,

preadolescent children, and adolescents. For example, pre-schoolers may show

clingyness, tantrums and frequent masturbation; preadolescents may show

underachievment at school, nightmares and peer relationship problems; while

adolescents may engage in truanting, drug abuse, sexual abuse of others and

prostitution. In adults sexual abuse has been found to lead to anxiety disorders

including PTSD, dissociative states, depression, self-injurious behaviour,

psychosexual adjustment difficulties, antisocial behaviour, sexual offending and

parenting skills deficits.

The impact of sexual victimisation during childhood is mediated by the

age of onset of the abuse; the frequency and duration of the abuse; the co-

occurrence of violence; the nature of the abusive act; the relationship of the

victim to the perpetrator and the gender of the victim. Broadly speaking, poorer

psychological adjustment occurs in boys and is associated with intrafamilial,

frequent, chronic abuse which began at an early age and involved penetration

and violence.

The impact of child sexual abuse is also mediated by the types of coping

strategies used, the availability of social support, and the way in which the family

and professionals respond to disclosure. Better psychological adjustment occurs

Prevention of CSA

66

in cases where children do not attribute the cause of the abuse to themselves;

cope with the abuse by disclosing it to a trusted peer or adult; receive support and

warmth from the non-abusing parent and other members of their social network;

and engage in a brief minimally intrusive legal process. Poorer psychological

adjustment occurs where children blame themselves for the abuse; use denial,

dissociation and avoidant strategies to cope with the abuse and other life

difficulties; and do not disclose the abuse to adults. In cases where children make

disclosures about the abuse, poorer adjustment occurs where children find that

the non-abusing parent or others disbelieve them and are forced to engage in

multiple forensic interviews with different interviewers and participate in

protracted legal proceedings.

Finkelhor and Browne's (1985) traumagenic dynamics model offers a

parsimonious yet comprehensive framework for conceptualising the complex and

multi-dimensional nature of the effects of child sexual abuse. Within this model

the traumatization process in cases of sexual abuse may be conceptualized as

being underpinned by four distinct mechanisms: traumatic sexualization,

powerlessness, stigmatization and betrayal. Traumatic sexualization accounts for

the inappropriate sexual behaviour shown by victims of sexual abuse. The

dynamic of betrayal accounts for subsequent attachment and relationship

difficulties. Negative self-evaluation, depression and self-injurious behaviour are

accounted for by the dynamic of stigmatization. Excessive anxiety on the one

hand and excessive anger on the other are accounted for the dynamic of

powerlessness within Finkelhor and Browne's model.

The devastating effects of child sexual abuse provide grounds for the

development of and evaluation child abuse prevention programmes. The

multifaceted nature of abuse the and factors which mediate its impact suggest that

prevention programmes should be multisystemic and target not only children but

also significant members of their social networks including parents and teachers.

That is, prevention programmes should aim to help children develop safety skills.

However such programmes should also aim to help parents and teachers create a

Effects of CSA

67

supportive context within which children's use of safety skills is encouraged and

children's disclosures of abuse are believed.

REFERENCES

Achenbach, T. (1984). Assessment And Taxonomy of Child And Adolescent

Psychopathology . Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Adams-Tucker, C. (1981). A socioclinical overview of 28 sex-abused children.

Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 361-367.

Adams-Tucker, C. (1982). Proximate effects of sexual abuse in childhood: A

report on 28 children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 1252-1256.

Alexander, P.C. (1992). Application of attachment theory to the study of sexual

abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 185-195.

Anderson, G., Yasenik, L. & Ross, C. (1993). Dissociative experiences and

disorders among women who identify themselves as sexual abuse

survivors. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 677-686.

Bagley, C. & King, K. (1990). Child Sexual Abuse: The Search For Healing.

London: Tavistock Routledge.

Bagley, C. & Ramsay, R. (1986). Sexual abuse in childhood: Psychosocial

outcomes and implications for social work practice. Journal of Social

Work and Human Sexuality, 4,33-47.

Bagley, C. & Young, L. (1987). Juvenile prostitution and child sexual abuse: A

controlled study. Canadian Journal of Community Mental Health, 6, 5-26.

Barnard, C.P. & Hirsch, C. (1985). Borderline personality and victims of incest.

Psychological Reports, 57, 715-718.

Becker, J.V. (1988). The effects of child sexual abuse on adolescent sexual

offenders. In G.E. Wyatt & G.J. Powell (Eds.), Lasting Effects Of Child

Sexual Abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Prevention of CSA

68

Beitchman, J.H., Zucker, K.J., Hood, J.E., da Costa, G.A. & Akman, D. (1991).

A review of the short-term effects of childhood sexual abuse. Child Abuse

and Neglect, 15, 537-556.

Beitchman, J.H., Zucker, K.J., Hood, J.E., da Costa, G.A., Akman, D. &

Cassavia, E. (1992). A review of the long-term effects of child sexual

abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 101-118.

Bentovim, A., van Elberg, A.. & Boston, P. (1988). The results of treatment. In

A. Bentovim, A. Elton, J. Hildebrand, M. Tranter & E. Vizard (Eds.), Child

Sexual Abuse Within The Family: Assessment And Treatment. London:

Wright.

Black, M., Dubowitz, H. & Harrington, D. (1994). Sexual abuse: Developmental

differences in children’s behavior and self-perception. Child Abuse and

Neglect, 18, 85-95.

Bliss, E.L. (1984). A symptom profile of patients with multiple personalities,

including MMPI results. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 172,

197-202.

Briere, J. (1984). The effects of childhood sexual abuse on later psychological

functioning: Defining a post-sexual abuse syndrome. Paper presented at

the Third National Conference on Sexual Victimisation of Children,

Washington, DC.

Briere, J. (1992). Child Abuse Trauma: Theory And Treatment of The Lasting

Effects. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Briere, J. & Conte, J. (1993). Self-reported amnesia for abuse in adults molested

as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6, 21-32.

Briere, J. & Runtz, M. (1985). Symptomatology associated with prior sexual

abuse in a non-clinical sample. Paper presented at the annual meeting of

the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles.

Briere, J. & Runtz, M. (1986). Suicidal thoughts and behaviours in former sexual

abuse victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 18, 413-423.

Briere, J. & Runtz, M. (1987). Post-sexual abuse trauma: Data and implications

for clinical practice. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 367-379.

Effects of CSA

69

Briere, J. & Runtz, M. (1988). Multivariate correlates of childhood

psychological and physical maltreatment among university women. Child

Abuse and Neglect, 12, 331-341.

Briere, J. & Runtz, M. (1991). The long-term effects of sexual abuse: A review

and synthesis. New Direction For Mental Health Services, 51, 3-13.

Briere, J. & Zaidi, L.Y. (1989). Sexual abuse histories and sequelae in female

psychiatric emergency room patients. American Journal of Psychiatry,

148, 55-61.

Browne, A. & Finkelhor, D. (1986). Impact of child sexual abuse: A review of

the research. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 66-77.

Bryer, J.B., Nelson, B.A., Miller, J.B. & Krol, P.A. (1987). Childhood sexual and

physical abuse as factors in adult psychiatric illness. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 144, 1426-1430.

Burgess, A.W., Hartman, C.R. & McCormack, A. (1987). Abused to Abuser:

Antecedents of socially deviant behaviors. American Journal of

Psychiatry, 144, 1431-1436.

Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D. & Braunwald, K. (1989).

Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants.

Developmental Psychopathalogy, 25, 525-531.

Chandy, J.M., Blum, R.W. & Resnick, M.D. (1996). Gender-specific outcomes

for sexually abused adolescents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 1219-1232.

Chu, J.A. & Dill, D.L. (1990). Dissociative symptoms in relation to childhood

physical and sexual abuse. American Journal of Psychiatry, 147, 887-892.

Cicchetti, D. & Lynch, M. (1993). Toward an ecological/transactional model of

community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for child

development. In D. Reiss, J.E. Richters & M. Radke-Yarrow. (Eds.),

Children and Violence. New York: Guilford Press.

Cohen, J. & Mannarino, A.P. (1988). Psychological symptoms in sexually

abused girls. Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 571-577.

Cohen, T. (1995). Motherhood among incest survivors. Child Abuse and

Neglect, 19, 1431-1442.

Prevention of CSA

70

Cohn, A. (1986). Preventing adults from becoming child molesters. Child Abuse

and Neglect, 10, 559-562.

Cole, P., Woolger, C., Power, T. & Smith, K. (1992). Parenting difficulties

among adult survivors of father-daughter incest. Child Abuse and Neglect,

16, 239-250.

Cole, P.K. & Woolger, (1989). Incest survivors: The relation of their perceptions

of their parents and their own parenting behavior. Child Abuse and

Neglect, 13, 1-8.

Cole, P.M. & Putnam, F.W. (1992). Effects of incest on self and social

functioning: A developmental psychopathological perspective. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 174-184.

Conte, J.R. & Schuerman, J.R. (1987a). Factors associated with an increased

impact of child sex abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 201-211.

Conte, J.R. & Schuerman, J.R. (1987b). The effects of sexual abuse on children:

A multidimensional view. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2, 380-390.

Coons, P.M. & Milstein, V. (1986). Psychosexual disturbances in multiple

personality: Characteristics, etiology, and treatment. Journal of Clinical

Psychiatry, 47, 106-110.

Courtois, C.A. (1979). The incest experience and its aftermath. Victimology, 4,

337-347.

Courtois, C.A. (1988). Healing the Incest Wound: Adult Survivors in Therapy.

New York: Norton.

de Young, M. (1988). The good touch/bad touch dilemma. Child Welfare, 67,

60-68.

Deblinger, E., McLeer, S.V., Atkins, M.S., Ralphe, D.L. & Foa, F. (1989). Post-

traumatic stress in sexually abused, physically abused and non-abused

children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 403-408.

Donaldson, M.A. & Gardner, R. (1985). Diagnosis and treatment of traumatic

stress among women after childhood incest. In C. R. Figley (Ed.). Trauma

And Its Wake: The Study And Treatment Of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder

In Children And Adults. New York: Brunner/Mazel.

Effects of CSA

71

Drauker, C.B. (1989). Cognitive adaptation of female incest survivors. Journal

of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 668-670.

Elliot, D.M. (1994). Impaired object relations in professional women molested as

children. Psychotherapy, 31, 79-86.

Elliot, D.M. & Briere, J. (1994). Forensic sexual abuse evaluations: Disclosures

and symptomatology. Behavioural Sciences and the Law, 12, 261-277.

Elliot, D.M. & Briere, J. (1995). Post-traumatic stress associated with delayed

recall of sexual abuse: A general population study. Journal of Traumatic

Stress Studies, 8, 629-648.

Elwell, M.E. & Ephross, P.H. (1987). Initial reactions of sexually abused

children. Social Casework, 68, 109-116.

Ernst, G., Angst, J. & Foldenyi, M. (1993). The Zurich study: XCII. Sexual

abuse in childhood: Frequency and relevance for adult morbidity, data of a

longitudinal epidemiological study. European Archives of Psychiatry and

Clinical Neuroscience, 242, 293-300.

Everson, M.D., Hunter, W.M., Runyan, D.K., Edelsohn, G.A. & Coulter, M.L.

(1989). Maternal support following disclosure of incest. American Journal

of Orthopsychiatry, 59, 197-207.

Fagot, B.I., Hagan, R., Youngblade, L.M. & Potter, L. (1989). A comparison of

the play behaviors of sexually abused, physically abused and non-abused

school children. Topics in Early Childhood special Education, 9, 88-100.

Finkelhor, D. (1979). Sexually Victimized Children. New York: Free Press.

Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child Sexual Abuse: New Theory and Research. New York:

Free Press.

Finkelhor, D. (1990). Early and long-term effects of child sexual abuse: An

Update. Professional Psychology, 21, 325-330.

Finkelhor, D. (1995). The victimization of children: A developmental

perspective. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 65, 177-193.

Finkelhor, D. & Browne A. (1985). The traumatic impact of child sexual abuse:

A conceptualization. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 55, 530-541.

Prevention of CSA

72

Friedrich, W.N. (1990). Psychotherapy Of Sexually Abused Children And Their

Families. New York: Norton.

Friedrich, W.N. (1993). Sexual victimization and sexual behavior in children: A

review of recent literature. Child Abuse and Neglect, 17, 59-66.

Friedrich, W.N., Beilke, R.L. & Urquiza, A.J. (1987). Children from sexually

abusive families: A behavioral comparison. Journal of Interpersonal

Violence, 2, 21-28.

Friedrich, W.N., Beilke, R.L. & Urquiza, A.J. (1988). Behavior problems in

young sexually abused boys: A comparison study. Journal of Interpersonal

Violence, 3, 21-28.

Friedrich, W.N., Grambsch, P., Broughton, D., Kuiper, J. & Beilke, R.L. (1991).

Normative sexual behavior in children. Pediatrics, 88, 456-464.

Friedrich, W.N., Grambsch, P., Damon, L., Koverola, C., Hewitt, S.K., Lang,

R.A. & Broughton, D. (1992). Child sexual behaviour inventory:

Normative and clinical comparisons. Psychological Assessment, 4, 303-

311.

Friedrich, W.N. & Luecke, W.J. (1988). Young school-age sexually aggressive

children. Professional Psychology Research and Practice, 19, 155-164.

Friedrich. W.N. & Reams, R.A. (1987). Course of psychological symptoms in

sexually abused young children. Psychotherapy, 27, 160-170.

Friedrich, W.N., Urquiza, A.J. & Beilke, R.L. (1986). Behavior problems in

sexually abused young children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 11, 47-

57.

Fromuth, M.E. (1986). The relationship of childhood sexual abuse with later

psychological and sexual adjustment in a sample of college women. Child

Abuse and Neglect, 10, 5-15.

Gale, J., Thompson, R., Moran. T. & Sack, W. (1988). Sexual abuse in young

children: Its clinical presentation and characteristic patterns. Child Abuse

and Neglect, 12, 163-170.

Gelinas, D. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46 312-

332.

Effects of CSA

73

Gidycz, C.A. & Koss, M.P. (1989). The impact of adolescent sexual

victimization: Standardized measures of anxiety, depression and behavioral

deviancy. Violence and Victims, 4, 139-149.

Gil, E. & Johnson, T.C. (1993). Sexualized Children: Assessment And Treatment

Of Sexualized Children And Children Who Molest. Rockville, MD:

Launch.

Gold, E. (1986). Long-term effects of sexual abuse in childhood: An

attributional approach. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,

471-475.

Gomes-Schwartz, B., Horowitz, J., Cardarelli, A. & Sauzier, M. (1990). Child

Sexual Abuse: The Initial Effects. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Gomes-Schwartz, B., Horowitz, J.M. & Sauzier, M. (1985). Severity of

emotional distress among sexually abused preschool, school-age, and

adolescent children. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 36, 503-508.

Green, A.H. (1993). Child sexual abuse: Immediate and long-term effects and

intervention. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent

Psychiatry, 32, 890-902.

Groth, A. & Burgess, A. (1979). Sexual trauma in the life histories of rapists and

child molesters. Victimology, 4, 10-16.

Herman, J. & Harvery, M. (1993). The False Memory Debate: Social Science

Or Social Backlash? Harvard Mental Health Letter.

Herman, J. (1981). Father-Daughter Incest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University

Press.

Herman, J. (1988). Considering sex offenders: A model of addiction. Signs:

Journal of Women in Culture and Society, 13, 695-724.

Herman, J. (1992). Trauma and Recovery. New York: Basic Books.

Herman, J. & Hirschman, L. (1981). Families at risk for father-daughter incest.

American Journal of Psychiatry, 138, 967-970.

Herman, J. & Schatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of memories of

child sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4, 1-14.

Prevention of CSA

74

Herman, J. & Van der Kolk, B.A. (1989). Childhood trauma in borderline

personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 490-495.

Herman-Giddens, M.E., Sandler, A.D. & Friedman, N.E. (1988). Sexual

precocity in girls: an association with sexual abuse? American Journal of

Diseases of the Child, 142, 431-433.

Hewitt, S.K. & Friedrich, W.N. (1991). Effects of probable sexual abuse on

preschool children. In M.Q. Patton (Ed.), Family Sexual Abuse. Newbury

Park, CA: Sage.

Hibbard, R.A., Ingersoll, G.M. & Orr, D.P. (1990). Behavior risk, emotional

risk, and child abuse among adolescents in a non-clinical setting.

Pediatrics, 86, 896-901.

Hunter, W.M., Coulter, M.L., Runyan, D.K. & Everson, M.D. (1990).

Determinants of placement for sexually abused children. Child Abuse and

Neglect, 14, 407-418.

Jampole, L. & Weber, M.K. (1987). An assessment of the behavior of sexually

abused and non-sexually abused children with anatomically correct dolls.

Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 187-192.

Jehu, D. (1988). Beyond Sexual Abuse: Therapy With Women Who Were

Childhood Victims. Chichester, UK: Wiley.

Jensen, J.B., Pease, J.J., ten Bensel, R. & Garfinkel, B.D. (1991). Growth

hormone response patterns in sexually or physically abused boys. Journal

of the American Academy of Child Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 784-790.

Johnson, B.K. & Kenkel, M.B. (1991). Stress, coping and adjustment in female

adolescent incest victims. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 293-305.

Johnson, R.I. & Shrier, D.K. (1985). Sexual victimization of boys. Journal of

Adolescent Health Care, 6, 372-376.

Johnston, M.S.K. (1979). The sexually mistreated child: Diagnostic evaluation.

Child Abuse and Neglect, 3, 943-951.

Kendall-Tackett, K.A., Willliams, L.M. & Finkelhor, D. (1993). Impact of sexual

abuse on children: A review and synthesis of recent empirical studies.

Psychological Bulletin, 113, 164 - 180.

Effects of CSA

75

Kinzl, J. & Biebl. W. (1992). Long-term effects of incest: Life events triggering

mental disorders in female patients with sexual abuse in childhood. Child

Abuse and Neglect, 16, 567-573.

Kirby, J.S., Chu, J. & Dill, D.L. (1993). Correlates of dissociative

symptomatology in patients with physical and sexual abuse histories.

Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34(4), 258-263.

Kluft, R.P. (1990). Incest Related Syndromes of Adult Psychopathology.

Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Kolko, D.J., Moser, J.T. & Weldy, S.R. (1988). Behavioral/emotional indicators

of sexual abuse in child psychiatric in-patients. A controlled comparison

with physical abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 12, 529-541.

Langevin, R., Wortzman, G., Wright, D. & Handy, L. (1989). Studies of brain

damage and dysfunction in sex offenders. Annals of Sex Research, 2, 163-

179.

Langmade, C.J. (1983). The impact of pre- and post-pubertal onset of incest

experiences in adult women as measured by sex anxiety, sex guilt, sexual

satisfaction and sexual behavior. Dissertation Abstracts International, 44,

917B (University Microfilms No. 3592).

Lanktree, C. & Briere, J. (1995). Outcome of therapy for sexually abused

children: A repeated measures study. Child Abuse and Neglect, 19, 1145-

1155.

Lawson, L. & Chaffin, M. (1992). False negatives in sexual abuse disclosure

interviews: Incidence and influence of caretakers’ belief in cases of

accidental abuse discovery by diagnosis of STD. Journal of Interpersonal

Violence, 7, 532-542.

Leitenberg, H., Greenwald, E. & Cado, S. (1992). A retrospective study of long-

term methods of coping with having been sexually abused during

childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, 16, 399-407.

Levang, C.A. (1989). Interactional communication patterns in father/daughter

incest families. Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 1, 53-68.

Prevention of CSA

76

Lindberg, F. & Distad, L. (1985). Survival responses to incest: Adolescents in

crisis. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 521-526.

Mannarino, A.P., Cohen, J.A. & Berman, S.R. (1994). The children’s

attributions and perceptions scale: A new measure of sexual abuse-related

factors. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 23, 204-211.

Mannarino, A.P., Cohen, J.A. & Gregor, M. (1989). Emotional and behavioural

difficulties in sexually abuse girls. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 4,

437-451.

Mannarino, A.P., Cohen, J.A., Smith, J.A. & Moore-Motily, S. (1991). Six and

twelve month follow-up of sexually abused girls. Journal of Interpersonal

Violence, 6, 494-511.

Margo, G.M. & McLees, E.M. (1991). Further evidence for the significance of a

childhood abuse history in psychiatric in-patients. Comprehensive

Psychiatry, 32, 362-366.

McLeer, S.V., Deblinger, E., Atkins, M.S., Foa, E.B. & Ralphe, D.L. (1988).

Post-traumatic stress disorder in sexually abuse children. Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 650-654.

Meiselman, K.C. (1978). Incest: A Psychological Study Of Causes And Effects

With Treatment Recommendations. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Mian, M., Marton, P. & Le Baron, D. (1996). The effects of sexual abuse on 3-

to 5-year old girls. Child Abuse and Neglect, 20, 731-745.

Mian, M., Wehrspann, W., Klajner-Diamond, H., LeBaron, D. & Winder, C.

(1986). Review of 125 children 6 years of age and under who were

sexually abused. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 223-229.

Morrow, K.B. (1991). Attributions of female adolescent incest victims regarding

their molestation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 15, 477-483.

Mullen, P.E., Martin, J.L., Anderson, J.C., Romans, S.E. & Herbison, G.P.

(1996). The long-term impact of the physical, emotional and sexual abuse

of children: A community study.

Effects of CSA

77

Mullen, P.E., Romans-Clarkson, S.E., Walton, V.A. & Herbison, G.P. (1988).

Impact of sexual and physical abuse on women's mental health. Lancet, 1,

841-845.

Murphy, S.M., Kilpatrick, D.G., Amick-McMullan, A., Veronen, L.J.,

Paduhovich, J., Best, C.L., Villeponteau, L.A. & Saunders, B.E. (1988).

Current psychological functioning of child sexual assault survivors.

Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 3, 55-79.

Murphy, W.D. & Smith, T.A. (1996). Sex offenders against children: empirical

and clinical issues. In J.Briere, L. Berliner, J. Bulkley, C. Jenny & T. Reid

(Eds.), The APSAC Handbook On Child Maltreatment. London: Sage.

Palmer, R.L., Chaloner, D.A. & Oppenheimer, R. (1992). Childhood sexual

experiences with adults reported by female psychiatric patients. British

Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 261-265.

Peters, S.D. (1985). Child sexual abuse and later psychological problems. Paper

presented at the American Psychological Association meeting, Los

Angeles.

Peters, S.D. (1988). Child sexual abuse and later psychological problems. In

G.E. Wyatt & G.J. Powell (Eds.), Lasting Effects Of Child Sexual Abuse.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Pierce, R. & Pierce, L.H. (1985). The sexually abused child: A comparison of

male and female victims. Child Abuse and Neglect, 9, 191-199.

Pitman, R.K., Orr, S.P., Forgue, D.F., de Jong, J.B. & Claiborn, J.M. (1987).

Psychophysiologic assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in

Vietnam combat veterans. Archives of General Psychiatry, 44, 970-975.

Polusny, M. & Folette, V. (1995). Long-term correlates of child sexual abuse:

Theory and review of the empirical literature. Applied and Preventive

Psychology, 4, 143-166.

Pribor, E.F. & Dinwiddie, S.H. (1992). Psychiatric correlates of incest in

childhood. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 52-56.

Prevention of CSA

78

Putnam, F.W. (1990). Disturbances of "self" in victims of childhood sexual

abuse. In R. Kluft (Ed.), Incest-Related Syndromes Of Adult Psycho-

Pathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Putnam, F.W. & Trickett, P.K. (1993). Child sexual abuse: A model of chronic

trauma, in D. Reiss, J.E. Richters & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Children

And Violence. New York: Guildford Press.

Rodriguez, N., Ryan, S.W. & Foy, D.W. (1992). Tension reduction and P.T.S.D.

adult survivors of sexual abuse. Paper presented at the annual meeting of

the International Society of Traumatic Stress Studies, Los Angeles.

Rowan, A.B., Foy, D.W., Rodriguez, N. & Ryan, S. (1994). Post-traumatic stress

disorder in a clinical sample of adults sexually abused as children. Child

Abuse and Neglect, 18, 51-61.

Runyan, D.K., Everson, M.D., Edelsohn, G.A., Hunter, W.M. & Coulter, M.L.

(1988). Impact of intervention on sexually abused children. Journal of

Pediatrics, 113, 647-653.

Russell, D. (1986). The Secret Trauma: Incest In The Lives Of Girls And

Women. New York: Basic Books

Sansonnet-Hayden, H., Haley, G., Marriage, K. & Fine, S. (1987). Sexual abuse

and psychopathology in hospitalized adolescents. Journal of the American

Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23¸ 225-229.

Saunders, B.E., Kilpatrick, D.G, Resnick, H.S., Hanson, R.A. & Lipovsky, J.A.

(1992a). Epidemiological characteristics of child sexual abuse: Results

from wave II of the national women’s study. Paper presented at the San

Diego Conference on Responding to Child Maltreatment, San Diego.

Saunders, B.E., Villeponteaux, L.A., Lipovsky, J.A. & Kilpatrick, D.G.(1992b).

Child sexual assault as a risk factor for mental disorder among women: A

community survey. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7, 189-204.

Sauzier, M. (1989). Disclosure of child sexual abuse: For better or worse.

Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 455-469.

Effects of CSA

79

Sedney, M.A. & Brooks, B. (1984). Factors associated with a history of

childhood sexual experience in a nonclinical female population. Journal of

the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 23, 215-218.

Seidner, K.I., Calhoun, K.S. & Kilpatrick, D.G. (1985). Childhood and/or

adolescent sexual experiences: Predicting variability in subsequent

adjustment. Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psychological

Association, Los Angeles

Senn, C.Y. (1988). Vulnerable: Sexual Abuse And People With An Intellectual

Handicap. Downsville, Ontario: G. Allan Roeher Institute.

Shapiro, J.P., Leifer, M., Marton, M.W. & Kassem, L. (1990). Multimethod

assessment of depression in sexually abused girls. Journal of Personality

Assessment, 55, 234-248.

Silbert, M.H. & Pines, A.M. (1981). Sexual child abuse as an antecedent to

prostitution. Child Abuse and Neglect, 5, 407-411.

Silverman, A., Reinherz, H. & Giacona, R. (1996). The long-term sequelae of

child and adolescent abuse: A longitudinal community study. Child Abuse

and Neglect, 20, 709-723.

Singer, M.I., Petchers, M.K. & Hussey, D. (1989). The relationship between

sexual abuse and substance abuse among psychiatrically hospitalized

adolescents. Child Abuse and Neglect, 13, 319-325.

Sirles, E.A., Smith, J.A. & Kusama, H. (1989). Psychiatric status of intrafamilial

child sexual abuse victims. Journal of the American Academy of Child and

Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 225-229.

Smith, H. & Israel, E. (1987). Sibling incest: A study of the dynamics of 25

cases. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 101-108.

Sorenson, S.B., Siegel, J.M., Golding, J.M. & Stein, J.A. (1991). Repeated

sexual victimization. Victims and Violence, 91, 299-308.

Sorenson, T. & Snow, B. (1991). How children tell: The process of disclosure in

child sexual abuse. Child Welfare League of America, 70, 3-15.

Spaccarelli, S. (1994). Stress, appraisal and coping in child sexual abuse: A

theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 240-362.

Prevention of CSA

80

Stein, J.A., Golding, J.M., Siegel, J.M., Burnam, M.A. & Sorenson, S.B. (1988).

Long-term psychological sequelae of child sexual abuse: The Los Angeles

epidemiologic catchment area study. In G.E. Wyatt & G.J. Powell (Eds.),

Lasting Effects Of Child Sexual Abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage

Summit, R. (1983). The child sexual abuse accommodation syndrome. Child

Abuse and Neglect, 7, 177-193.

Tedesco, J.F. & Schnell, S.V. (1987). Children’s reactions to sex abuse

investigation and litigation. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 267-272.

Tong, L., Oates, K. & McDowell, M. (1987). Personality development following

sexual abuse. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 371-383.

Trickett, P.K. & Putnam. F.W. (1993). Impact of child sexual abuse on females:

Toward a developmental psychobiological integration. Psychological

Science, 4, 81-87.

Tsai, M., Feldman-Summers, S. & Edgar, M. (1979). Childhood molestation:

Variables related to differential impacts on psychosexual functioning in

adult women. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 407-417.

Tufts New England Medical Center, Division of Child Psychiatry. (1984).

Sexually Exploited Children: Service And Research Project (Final report

for the Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention).

Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.

Urquiza, A.J. & Keating, L.M. (1990). The prevalence of sexual victimization of

males. In M. Hunter (Ed.), The Sexually Abused Male, Vol I: Prevalence,

Impact And Treatment. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.

Ussher, J.M. & Dewberry, C. (1995). The nature and long-term effects of

childhood sexual abuse: A survey of adult women survivors in Britain.

British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 177-192.

Watkins, B. & Bentovim. A. (1992). The sexual abuse of male children and

adolescents: A review of current research. Journal of Child Psychology

and Psychiatry, 33, 197-248.

Effects of CSA

81

Williams, L. (1991). The impact of court testimony on young children: Use of

protective strategies in day care cases. Unpublished manuscript, Family

Research Laboratory, University of New Hampshire.

Williams, L. (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of

women’s memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 62, 1167-1176.

Wind, T.W. & Silvern, L. (1992). Type and extent of child abuse as predictors of

adult functioning. Journal of Family Violence, 7, 261-281.

Wolfe, V.V., Gentile, C. & Wolfe, D.A. (1989). The impact of sexual abuse on

children: A P.T.S.D. formulation. Behavior Therapy, 20, 215-228.

Wyatt, G.E. & Mickey, M.R. (1988). Mediating factors to outcomes for children.

In G.E. Wyatt and G.J. Powell (Eds.), Lasting Effects Of Child Sexual

Abuse. CA: Sage.

Wyatt, G.E. & Newcomb, M. (1990). Internal and external mediators of

women’s sexual abuse in childhood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 58, 758-767.

Wyatt, G. E., Newcomb, M. & Riederle, M. H. (1993). Sexual Abuse And

Consensual Sex: Women's Developmental Patterns And Outcomes.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Wyatt, G.E. & Powell, G.J. (Eds.), (1988). Lasting Effects Of Child Sexual

Abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Zierler, S, Feingold, L., Laufer, D., Velentgas P., Kantrowitz-Gordon, I. &

Mayer, K. (1991). Adult survivors of childhood sexual abuse and

subsequent risk of H.I.V. Infection. American Journal of Public Health,

81, 572-575.

Zivney, O.A., Nash, M.R. & Hulsey T.L. (1988). Sexual abuse in early versus

late childhood: Differing patterns of pathology as revealed on the

Rorschach. Psychotherapy, 25, 99-106.

Zlotnick, C., Ryan, C.E., Miller, I.W. & Keitner, G.I. (1995). Childhood abuse

and recovery from major depression. Child Abuse and Neglect, 19, 1513-

1516.

Prevention of CSA

82