Students' Perceptions of Teacher Feedback and Physical Competence in Physical Education Classes:...

22
36 Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 2006, 25, 36-57 © 2006 Human Kinetics, Inc. Nicaise and Cogérino are with the Laboratory “Center of Research and Innovation on Sport” E.A. 647, UFRAPS—Université Claude Bernard, Lyon, France; Julien Bois is with the “Laboratoire dʼAnalyse de la Performance Sportive” at the University of Pau, France. Anthony J. Amorose is in the School of Kinesiology and Recreation at Illinois State University, Bloomington, IL. Students’ Perceptions of Teacher Feedback and Physical Competence in Physical Education Classes: Gender Effects Virginie Nicaise and Geneviève Cogérino University of Lyon, France Julien Bois University of Pau, France Anthony J. Amorose Illinois State University Feedback is considered a critical teaching function, and researchers in sport peda- gogy have shown interest in verifying its importance in physical education. Many observational studies have found that boys receive more attention and feedback, particularly praise, criticism, and technical information, than girls. Nevertheless, little is known about studentsʼ perceptions of teacher–student interactions. The aim of this study was to investigate whether studentsʼ perceptions of teacher feed- backs are gender-differentiated in physical education, as well as to determine how perceived feedback is related to studentsʼ perceptions of competence. French high school students (N = 450: 200 boys, 250 girls) completed questionnaires assessing their perceptions of their teachersʼ feedback and their perceptions of competence. Results indicated gender differences in the set of variables. Furthermore, the influ- ence of teacher feedback on girlsʼ perceptions of competence was strong, whereas little relationship was found for boys. These findings are then discussed in terms of teaching effectiveness. Key words: French high school students, student–teacher interactions, perceived competence. The most important function in contemporary physical education in France is to prepare children for lifetime physical activity (French Physical Education 04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM 36

Transcript of Students' Perceptions of Teacher Feedback and Physical Competence in Physical Education Classes:...

36

Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 37

Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 2006, 25, 36-57© 2006 Human Kinetics, Inc.

Nicaise and Cogérino are with the Laboratory “Center of Research and Innovation on Sport” E.A. 647, UFRAPS—Université Claude Bernard, Lyon, France; Julien Bois is with the “Laboratoire dʼAnalyse de la Performance Sportive” at the University of Pau, France. Anthony J. Amorose is in the School of Kinesiology and Recreation at Illinois State University, Bloomington, IL.

Students’ Perceptions of TeacherFeedback and Physical Competence

in Physical Education Classes:Gender Effects

Virginie Nicaise and Geneviève CogérinoUniversity of Lyon, France

Julien BoisUniversity of Pau, France

Anthony J. AmoroseIllinois State University

Feedback is considered a critical teaching function, and researchers in sport peda-gogy have shown interest in verifying its importance in physical education. Many observational studies have found that boys receive more attention and feedback, particularly praise, criticism, and technical information, than girls. Nevertheless, little is known about students ̓perceptions of teacher–student interactions. The aim of this study was to investigate whether students ̓perceptions of teacher feed-backs are gender-differentiated in physical education, as well as to determine how perceived feedback is related to students ̓perceptions of competence. French high school students (N = 450: 200 boys, 250 girls) completed questionnaires assessing their perceptions of their teachers ̓feedback and their perceptions of competence. Results indicated gender differences in the set of variables. Furthermore, the influ-ence of teacher feedback on girls ̓perceptions of competence was strong, whereas little relationship was found for boys. These findings are then discussed in terms of teaching effectiveness.

Key words: French high school students, student–teacher interactions, perceived competence.

The most important function in contemporary physical education in France is to prepare children for lifetime physical activity (French Physical Education

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM36

36

Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 37

Programmes, 2000). Regular participation in physical activity for young people can contribute to the enhancement of physical, psychological, and social well-being (Biddle, Sallis, & Cavill, 1998). Teachers play many important roles in helping students in their classes to learn. One of the teacherʼs functions is to monitor students ̓progress and provide feedback. By providing appropriate feedback to the student, teachers hope that performance will be modified and the student will learn the material. The general purpose of this study is to explore male and female students ̓perceptions of their physical education teachers ̓feedback patterns and to determine the relationship between the various types of feedback and the students ̓perceptions of ability in physical education.

Feedback in Physical Education

Given the significance of feedback in the teaching–learning process (Cloes, Premuzak, & Piéron, 1993; Silverman, 1994), the role of feedback provided by physical education teachers has received a considerable amount of attention. A variety of feedback characteristics have been examined, such as the absolute and relative frequency of feedback, the timing of feedback, and the content of feedback (see Magill, 1993), and many researchers have developed multidimensional obser-vation systems to assess these different dimensions of feedback (e.g., Cheffers & Mancini, 1978). Based on these observational systems, a number of studies reveal that teachers ̓feedback is an effective means of increasing student achievement in physical education; the findings, however, are sometimes inconsistent and do not always support feedback as an essential element of learning (for review, see Lee, Keh & Magill, 1993).

One possible reason for the lack of effect found in these observational stud-ies is that students ̓ perceptions of their teachers ̓ feedback typically have not been considered. Students ̓perceptions of instruction do serve as mediating links between teacher behavior and student learning and performance (Doyle, 1977; Lee et al., 1993). Furthermore, there is not always congruence between what a teacher actually says and how different students perceive and interpret the information. Martinek (1988), for example, conducted student interviews to determine whether their perceptions of the teachers ̓ feedback were consistent with coded dyadic interaction. Results showed differences between observed and perceived feedback intended as praise, to correct skills, and to correct behavior. Similar findings have been reported in sport settings with coaches and their athletes (Smoll & Smith, 2002). Although there are clearly cases in which students and teacher have similar perceptions (Tjeerdsma, 1997), focusing on students ̓thoughts and feelings might significantly enhance our understanding of the teaching–learning process and how various instructional behaviors, such as feedback, affect students ̓achievement-related outcomes (Lee & Solmon, 1992).

Feedback and Perceptions of Competence

Research and theory have highlighted the critical function of perceived com-petence on students ̓ achievement-related behaviors, cognitions, and affective responses (Harter, 1998). Perceived competence is defined as how an individual perceives her or his own ability to perform a specific task or a role-appropriate

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM37

38 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 39

behavior. Given the importance of perceived competence, a key question involves asking about the sources of information individuals use to evaluate or determine their level of ability. One important factor is performance-related feedback from parents, teachers, and coaches (Horn & Amorose, 1998). What adults say or do not say in response to a child or an adolescentʼs performance attempt can positively or negatively influence their perceptions of competence.

Although specific research examining how various types of feedback provided by physical education teachers are related to students ̓perceived competence is lacking, considerable research has been done in the area of coaching (Horn, 2002; Smoll & Smith, 2002). Despite some contextual differences between organized sport participation and physical education classes, the content, purpose, and function of performance-related feedback from coaches and teachers are very similar. As such, the literature on coaching (Chelladurai, 1993; Horn; Smoll & Smith) might begin to help us understand the types of feedback that are more or less effective for facilitating positive perceptions of ability in physical education.

Perceived Coaching Feedbackand Perceptions of Competence

Consistent with the majority of social cognitive theories describing the influ-ence of significant others on various achievement-related outcomes (e.g., Harter, 1998; Roberts, 2001), the majority of the coaching feedback research has adopted the perspective that athletes ̓perceptions of coachʼs behavior have direct influence on the athletes ̓psychological responses (Horn, 2002). For instance, the research based on Smoll and Smithʼs Mediational Model of Leadership has found strong support for the influence of perceived coaching behavior on athletes ̓experiences in sport (Smoll & Smith, 2002). Their line of research began with the development of a coding system based on extensive observations of coaches ̓behaviors in practice and game settings (Smith, Smoll, & Hunt, 1977). According to their classification, known as the Coaching Behavior Assessment System (CBAS), coaches respond to athletes ̓desirable performances with either reinforcement or nonreinforcement and react to errors or mistakes with encouragement, technical instruction, punish-ment, punitive technical instruction, or by ignoring the mistake. Using this system to assess observed and perceived coaching feedback, researchers have identified a pattern of behaviors related to positive psychological outcomes (see Horn, 2002; Smoll & Smith). In general, these studies have found that athletes whose coaches provide higher frequencies of positive reinforcement after performance successes, as well as encouragement and technical instruction after mistakes, reported more positive sport experiences. Other studies (Allen & Howe, 1998; Amorose & Horn, 2000; Black & Weiss, 1992), with 10- to 20-year-old athletes, have used slightly different measures of perceived coaching feedback (e.g., the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire; CFQ); the general pattern of results, however, has been relatively consistent. Specifically, athletes who perceived receiving more positive and informational feedback, less criticism or punishment, and perceived that coaches ignored their performances less often tended to report more positive achievement-related outcomes, including higher levels of perceived competence. Given that physical education teachers ̓performance-related responses is similar in nature to athletic coaches, it would be informative to determine whether this same pattern

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM38

38 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 39

of “effective” feedback found in sport leads to positive experiences for physical education students as well.

Feedback and Student’s Gender

Another important consideration when attempting to understand feedback in physical education involves the gender of the student. Clear evidence indicates that teachers tend to interact differently with male and female students in elementary school (Bailey, 1993), junior high (Heller & Parsons, 1981), and high school (Smith, 1992) classrooms. Generally, male students receive more attention in class from teachers than do female students (Duffy, Warren, & Walsh, 2001). Furthermore, the type of attention boys receive varies. Drudy and Úi Chatháin (2002), for instance, demonstrated that boys received more praise, acceptance, and questions from their teachers compared with girls. Similar results have been reported in physical education classrooms (Griffin, 1981; Solomons, 1977). Dunbar and OʼSullivan (1986), for example, reported that interactions between teachers and students in coeducational elementary physical education classes were inequitable and favored boys. Compared with girls, boys were asked questions more often and received more positive and corrective feedback from their teacher.

Although the pattern of differential treatment of male and female students is fairly consistent, some scholars have attributed the greater number of teacher interactions directed toward male students to the fact that male students initiated interactions with their teachers more often (Altermatt, Jovanovic, & Perry, 1998). There is some support for this notion because the study by Drudy and Úi Chatháin (2002), for instance, reported that boys tended to initiate more interactions with their teachers across a variety of subjects (e.g., language, math, science, humani-ties). Other studies, however, have not necessarily found that teachers interact more frequently with boys as a result of male students having initiated more frequent direct verbal interactions with teachers (Duffy et al., 2001). Thus, in exploring the differential treatment of boys and girls in classrooms, future research should at least consider the possible effect of the student initiating communication with the teacher.

Effect of Teacher’s Feedback

Understanding the differential treatment of boys and girls in physical education has important consequences in that these experiences are likely to influence student outcomes. As noted, the research (Dart & Clarke, 1988) suggests that boys have a typically disproportionate share of contributions in whole class interaction, partly as a result of the fact that they are selected by the teacher more frequently than girls but also because of their ability to create conditions (positive and negative) that attract teachers ̓attention and that facilitate their participation and dominant position in classroom interaction. Girls have lower expectations for themselves in classrooms in which they are treated in a qualitatively different manner from the boys (Eccles & Blumenfeld, 1985). In physical sciences, for instance, Jones and Wheatley (1990) reported that boys demand and get more of the teacherʼs attention. This conversationally focal position of the boys provides them with an important learning experience, the confidence of being listened to, and stimulates

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM39

40 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 41

them to participate more (Howe, 1997). Thereby, girls might be unintentionally pushed out of the center of the action, learning to expect a lower participation level than boys.

Over the years many researchers have attempted to identify processes that might influence gender differences in performance and achievement (Eisenberg, Martin, & Fabes, 1996), and beliefs about the self have become a dominant theme. The issue becomes more complicated as we learn more about feelings of competence and the development of competency beliefs, with gender arising as one variable that seems to make a difference.

Althogh the evidence reviewed above clearly indicates that teachers treat male and female students differently, most of this information comes from observational studies. Less is known, however, about how students perceive their teachers ̓com-munications, and whether boys and girl think their instructors provide different types and amounts of feedback. No attempt has been made to incorporate data from the students ̓perspective regarding perceived differential teacher treatment in physical education classes (McBride, 1990), and as noted, most social cogni-tive models describing the influence of significant others argue that the effects of feedback are dependent on how the one receiving the information perceives the feedback, not on what is said actually from an objective standpoint (Harter, 1998; Roberts, 2001). Thus, examining how students perceive their teachers ̓behavior is of critical importance and is therefore one of the primary aims of this study.

The Present Study

The main purpose of this study was to investigate students ̓perceptions of teacher feedback in physical education classes. Specifically, the study examined whether gender differences appeared relative to teacher feedbackbecause many observational studies indicate that boys seem to be advantaged over girls in interac-tions with teachers. It was expected that students ̓perceptions of teachers ̓feedback would be clearly gender differentiated. Boys should perceive that they receive more frequent feedback, such as praise, criticism, technical information, and encourage-ment, and they are likely to believe that they have initiated more communication with teachers compared with girls. The second purpose of the study is to see whether boys ̓and girls ̓perceptions of feedback were linked with their perceived physical competence. It was expected that teachers ̓perceived feedback would be related to students ̓self-perceptions of competence, with praise, encouragement, and infor-mational feedback showing a positive relationship with the students ̓perceptions of ability, whereas criticism and a lack of performance feedback were expected to show a negative relationship. No a priori hypothesis was made concerning gender differences in these relationships.

Method

Participants and Setting

A sample of 450 eighth-, ninth- and tenth- grade students (200 boys and 250 girls) with a mean age of 16 years (SD = 0.85) participated in the study.1 The stu-dents were recruited from three different high schools from two French cities, each

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM40

40 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 41

with a population of 15,000 to 30,000 inhabitants. The sample consisted primarily of Caucasian French middle- to upper-class families.

Questionnaires were administered at the end of the school year in 15 class-rooms. The classrooms were taught by a total of 12 physical education teachers (six men and six women). The questionnaire took approximately 10–15 minutes to complete. The teachers were asked to leave the area while the students answered the questions. Students were assured that their answers would remain confidential and their teacher would not see their answers. The researcher answered any questions from the students and collected the surveys once they had been completed.

Permission to conduct the study was obtained from the university Institutional Review Board, the school administration, and teachers before the start of the study. Students and their parents provided written informed consent before students par-ticipated in the study.

Measures

Perceived Teacher Feedback. Although several questionnaires have been developed for studying dimensions of the physical education learning environment or climate (e.g., Papaioannou, 1994), a number of unanswered question remain. Most relevant to this study, the research conducted so far has not adequately dealt with students ̓perceptions of performance-related feedback provided by the teach-ers. Given the lack of measures available, we decided to adopt an existing version of a reliable and valid scale used to assess perceived coaching feedback. Specifi-cally, we modified the Coaching Feedback Questionnaire (CFQ; Allen & Howe, 1998; Amorose & Horn, 2000). The CFQ, which is based on the original CBAS (Smith et al., 1977), asked respondents to indicate how frequently their high school or college coach responded to their performance attempts with one of 10 specific types of feedback. Four of the categories represent behavioral responses to ath-letes ̓good performances or efforts (praise only, praise combined with information, nonverbal praise, and no response). The other six categories represent behavioral responses to poor performances or errors (encouragement only, corrective infor-mation, no response, criticism, nonverbal criticism, and criticism with corrective information).

Each of these possible responses is directly relevant in a physical education teaching setting, and therefore, in our versions of the scale, we modified the instruc-tion set to ask the students how frequently they perceived their teachers responding in the ways just mentioned. Example items from each of the categories used in our modified version of the CFQ, which we labeled the Perceived Teacher Feedback Questionnaire (PTFQ), are presented in Table 1.

Given that the PTFQ was newly developed for the current study, we conducted a pilot study to determine whether the assessment was reasonable to use in a physical education setting. The frequency and types of feedback provided by two physical education teachers during 10 different physical education lessons were recorded. Based on these observations, our analysis indicated that the feedback categories identified in the PTFQ were commonly used by the teachers, thus providing some initial support for the validity of the scale.

Beyond the 10 feedback categories that were based on the CFQ, we also included some items on the PTFQ to determine the degree to which the students

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM41

42 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 43

Table 1 Items of the Perceived Teacher Feedback Questionnaire (PTFQ)

Categories Items

How often does your physical education teacher respond to your good performance with…?

praise “Nice move. Excellent!”praise + information “Well done! You put the ball into good space

for her to move onto.”nonverbal praise Teacher smiles, claps enthusiastically, pats

you on the back.no response Teacher doesnʼt say anything about your

good practice or effort.student-initiated communication—success

You often show your success to the teacher.

How often does your physical education teacher respond to your poor performance or mistake with…?

encouragement “Hang in there! Better luck next time.”technical/corrective information “Stay goal-side of your check.”criticism “That was a bad pass!”criticism + corrective information “I told you to stay near her/him! Next time

call the ball in a same move!”nonverbal criticism Teacher rolls her/his eyes or looks to the sky,

shakes his/her head, or looks away, looks angry.

no response Teacher ignores your mistakes.student-initiated communication—unsuccessful

You often ask for help from your teacher to correct a mistake.

Note. Items were scored on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher scores reflecting more frequent behavior.

initiated communication with their teachers. As noted, previous studies (Altermatt et al., 1998; Bailey, 1993) suggested that the greater number of teacher interactions directed toward male students might be the result of male students initiating more interactions. Therefore, we included items to reflect the frequency of student-initi-ated communication after both performance successes and failures. The items for these new categories were generated by the authors and the physical education teachers. Examples of these items are presented in Table 1. Initial evidence of the face validity and reliability of the items was provided in a pilot study (Nicaise, 2003) with 30 physical education students (alpha coefficients: student-initiated communication–success = .85 and student-initiated communication–unsuccessful = .87), as well as in this study (see Table 2).

In all, there were a total of 12 perceived teacher feedback categories included in the PTFQ, each of which was represented by three or four items. The response to

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM42

42 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 43

Tab

le 2

F

acto

r S

tru

ctu

re o

f th

e P

erce

ived

Tea

cher

Fee

db

ack

Qu

esti

on

nai

re (

PT

FQ

)

Fac

tor

Load

ing

Item

s1

23

45

67

8

Res

pons

e to

goo

d pe

rfor

man

ce00

0000

0000

0000

00Pr

aise

1.8

7Pr

aise

2.7

3Pr

aise

3.7

4Pr

aise

4.7

7N

onve

rbal

pra

ise

1.5

8N

onve

rbal

pra

ise

2.5

0N

onve

rbal

pra

ise

3.5

8Pr

aise

+ c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n 1

.50

Prai

se +

cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

2.6

5Pr

aise

+ c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n 3

.74

No

resp

onse

1.6

8N

o re

spon

se 2

.67

No

resp

onse

3.7

1St

uden

t-ini

tiate

d co

mm

unic

atio

n 1

.80

Stud

ent-i

nitia

ted

com

mun

icat

ion

2.6

7St

uden

t-ini

tiate

d co

mm

unic

atio

n 3

.71

(con

tinue

d)

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM43

44 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 45

Res

pons

e to

poo

r pe

rfor

man

ce/m

ista

ke00

0000

000

Tech

nica

l/cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

1.6

6Te

chni

cal/c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n 2

.71

Tech

nica

l/cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

3.7

3Te

chni

cal/c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n 4

.70

Enco

urag

emen

t 1.5

5En

cour

agem

ent 2

.41

.48

Enco

urag

emen

t 3.5

9En

cour

agem

ent 4

.60

Crit

icis

m 1

.45

Crit

icis

m 2

.76

Crit

icis

m 3

.59

Crit

icis

m 4

.60

Crit

icis

m +

cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

1.4

8C

ritic

ism

+ c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n 2

.40

.45

Crit

icis

m +

cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

3.6

6N

onve

rbal

crit

icis

m 1

.59

Non

verb

al c

ritic

ism

2.5

7N

onve

rbal

crit

icis

m 3

.57

Non

verb

al c

ritic

ism

4.4

2N

o re

spon

se 1

.58

No

resp

onse

2.7

5N

o re

spon

se 3

.62

Stud

ent-i

nitia

ted

com

mun

icat

ion

1.8

1St

uden

t-ini

tiate

d co

mm

unic

atio

n 2

.85

Stud

ent-i

nitia

ted

com

mun

icat

ion

3.8

2C

ronb

achʼ

s α

.87

.71

.76

.79

.76

.80

.60

.88

Eige

nval

ue5.

150

2.11

02.

090

3.53

01.

890

3.91

01.

900

2.63

0Pe

rcen

t var

ianc

e ex

plai

ned

.13

.05

.05

.10

.06

.10

.05

.06

Not

e. F

acto

r loa

ding

bel

ow .4

0 w

ere

elim

inat

ed fo

r sim

plic

ity.

Tab

le 2

(c

on

tin

ued

)

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM44

44 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 45

each of the 43 items was rated on 7-point Likert scale from (1) never to (7) always, reflecting the perceived frequency of teacher feedback.2

Self-Perceptions of Competence. Harter s̓ (1985) self-perception of competence measure was modified for the physical education context to assess the students ̓perceptions of competence. Specifically, the items asked about students ̓perceptions of ability in school physical education class, ability to learn sport skills in physical education, and confidence in the physical education environment. The specific items included: a) How good do you think you are in physical education? (not good at all to very good); b) Are you satisfied with your performances in physical education? (not at all to very much); c) How skilled do you think you are compared with your class friends? (not good at all to very good); and d) Are you self-confident in physi-cal education? (not at all to very much). Responses were scored on a 7-point scale, with higher scores reflecting a higher perception of competence. The responses from the four items were averaged, and the mean score was used as an indicator of the girls ̓and boys ̓self-perceptions of competence. These items possessed strong face validity and demonstrated acceptable levels of reliability in the current study, with an alpha coefficient of .89.

Background Information. Each participant was asked to complete demographic questions assessing their gender, age, and practice outside of school. Practice outside of school was determined by asking students to indicate whether he or she practiced more than just during physical education lessons. Specifically, students were to respond yes or no to the question, “Do you practice sport outside, in addition to physical education hours?”

Physical Education Performance. To obtain an indicator of the students ̓ability, we had the physical education teachers rate each of the students. These evaluations were represented by a score (0 to 20 points). The teacher based her or his evaluation on physical performance and student engagement. Specifically, the teacher s̓ evalu-ation consisted of test performances, as well as their judgment of each studentʼs participation during physical education classes. Although we acknowledged the potential biases involved in teacher ratings of performance, Trouilloud, Sarrazin, Martinek, and Guillet (2002) found a correlation of .65 between the studentʻs physical tests and the final evaluation by their physical education teacher. Thus, we believe that the teacherʼs assessment of the students is a reasonable indicator of the students ̓actual performance.

The variables reflecting practice outside of school and physical education performance were assessed because they are likely to affect the studentʼs percep-tions of competence. Therefore, we wanted to account for these variables when attempting to predict the students ̓perceptions of competence from their perceptions of their teachers ̓feedback.

Data Analysis

Before testing the main research questions, a principal component analysis was conducted on the PTFQ scale to provide factorial validity evidence for the newly created measure. Descriptive statistics, alpha coefficients, and correlations among all study variables were then examined. All variables were standardized

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM45

46 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 47

within PE classes.3 After these preliminary analyses, we tested the main study pur-poses. First, potential gender differences were explored using a MANOVA. Next, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the influence of perceived teacher feedback on male and female studentʼs percep-tions of competence

ResultsPreliminary Analyses

A principal component analysis was conducted to establish initial factorial validity for PTFQ. The number of factors that constitute a solution for PTFQ was based on several criteria. Consideration included the number of eigenvalues that exceed 1.0, the percentage of variance accounted for by the factors, inspection of the screen plot to determine when the eigenvalues begin to level off, and the cohesiveness of the items within identified factors. After the number of factors had been established, a direct oblimin rotation was used to simplify interpretation of the factors. Factor loadings of at least .40 were considered to be significant to include the items in a factor or subscale (Stevens, 1986). The final solution was an eight-factor structure including 43 of the original items (see Table 2).

Factor 1, labeled Praise and was composed of 10 items representing the teacher s̓ feedback after a successful performance. The items reflected positive information from the teacher, either verbal or nonverbal, and with or without technical instruc-tion. Factor 2 represented failures to respond to a good performance and, therefore, was labeled No Response–Success (three items). Factor 3 represented students ̓attempts to initiate communication with their teacher after a successful performance attempt. This factor was labeled Student-Initiated Communication—Success. Factor 4 represented feedback presenting technical information describing how to correct a mistake. This factor, which contained four items, was labeled Technical/Corrective Information. Factor 5 represents four items reflective of encouragement after a poor performance. Factor 6 included 11 items that represented a negative response to a performance error or mistake. These items included both verbal and nonverbal feedback, as well as items with or without technical/corrective information. Con-sequently, this factor was labeled Criticism. Factors 7 and 8 were also composed of items reflective of unsuccessful performance attempts. Factor 7 included three items representing no response on the part of the teacher, and thus was labeled No Response—Unsuccessful. Factor 8, on the other hand, represented three items in which the focus was on the student asking for help from their teacher. This factor was labeled Student-Initiated Communication-Unsuccessful.

All of the factors demonstrated an acceptable level of internal consistency (see Table 2) based on Cronbachʼs alpha coefficient (Cronbach, 1951), with the exception of Factor 7: No Response—Unsuccessful (α = .60). This factor was, therefore, dropped from all other analyses.

Descriptive StatisticsMeans, standard deviations, and correlations among all the variables of the

study are presented in Table 3. In order to determine the relationships between the among perceived teacherʼs feedback, physical competence, and gender, Pearsonʼs

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM46

46 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 47

Tab

le 3

D

escr

ipti

ve S

tati

stic

s an

d C

orr

elat

ion

s A

mo

ng

Stu

dy

Var

iab

les

(N =

430

)

Vari

able

s1

23

45

67

89

10

1. G

ende

r1.

000

2. P

erce

ived

com

pete

nce

–.35

01.

000

3. P

hysi

cal e

duca

tion

perf

orm

ance

–.24

0.3

91.

000

4. P

rais

e.0

20.2

9.0

7 1

.00

5. N

o re

spon

se—

succ

ess

–.11

00–.

100

.02

–.57

1.00

6. S

tude

nt-in

itiat

ed c

omm

unic

atio

n—

succ

ess

.110

.10

–.05

0 .

26–.

191.

00

7. T

echn

ical

/cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

.230

–.06

0–.

150

.41

–.37

.16

1.00

8. E

ncou

rage

men

t.2

40–.

040

–.10

0 .

53–.

41 .

26 .

661.

009.

Crit

icis

m–.

1400

–.11

0–.

18–.

03 .

17 .

07 .

00 .

041.

0010

. Stu

dent

-initi

ated

com

mun

icat

ion—

u

nsuc

cess

ful

0.13

0.0

7–.

090

.28

–.23

.52

.28

.36

.00

1.00

M

4.03

013

.150

3.29

3.2

02.

334.

413.

783.

012.

70SD

1.37

03.

011.

19 1

.40

1.48

1.23

1.29

.97

1.58

Not

e. G

ende

r (m

ale

= 1,

fem

ale

= 2)

; all

corr

elat

ions

> |.

10| a

re si

gnifi

cant

at p

< .0

5.

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM47

48 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 49

product moment correlation was used. Examination of the correlations suggested that multicollinearity (r ≥ .70) was not an issue (Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996).

Main Analyses

Purpose 1: Gender Differences. The first purpose was to determine whether gender differences existed concerning the perceptions of teachers ̓ feedback. A MANOVA indicated that there were significant gender differences in the set of feedback variables: Wilkʼs lambda = .89, F(8,440) = 6.74, p < .001. As seen in Table 4, significant effects were followed by appropriate univariate post-hoc com-parisons. Effect sizes were calculated to determine the meaningfulness or strength of the differences that emerged. Consistent with the recommendations of Thomas, Salazar, and Landers (1991), effect sizes less than .41 were considered small in magnitude, those between .41 and .70 were considered medium, and those greater than .70 were considered large.4 Significant gender differences were found for No Response–Success, Student-Initiated Communication–Success, Student-Initiated Communication–Unsuccessful, Technical/Corrective Information, Encouragement, and Criticism. Specifically, female students perceived a higher frequency of cor-rective information and encouragement after errors, as well as indicating they were more likely to initiate communication with their teachers after successes and failures. Conversely, female students reported a lower frequency of criticism and indicated their teachers were less likely to ignore them after errors. There were also significant gender differences in terms of perceived competence and physical education performance (see Table 4). Specifically, girls reported lower perceptions of competence and were rated as performing lower in class by their teachers.

Purpose 2: Predicting Perceptions of Competence. Separate hierarchical regression analyses predicting perceptions of competence were conducted for male and female students. We attempted to control for the effects of age, physical education performance, and practice by entering these variables on Step 1 of the hierarchical regressions. We then added the set of perceived feedback variables on step two. In both analyses, the set of variables significantly predicted the students ̓perceptions of competence—girls: F(7, 232) = 10.25, p < .01; boys, F(7, 181) = 6.89, p < .01)—although the addition of the teacher feedback categories did not contribute above and beyond the effect of age, physical education performance, and practice for both groups. A summary of the results is presented in Table 5.

For girls, results revealed that physical education initial performance (β = .30) and practice outside of school (β = .18) significantly predicted their self-perception of competence, F(3, 235) = 8,74, p < .01, R² = .13. The inclusion of the perceived teachers ̓feedback on Step 2, however, explained an additional 18% of the vari-ance. Praise (β = .33) and criticism (β = –.17) were each significant predictors of perceived competence. Based on squared semipartial correlations, the amount of unique variance in the girls ̓perceived physical competence explained by praise was 10% (sr² = .10) and by criticism was 5% (sr² = .05). These findings indicate that female students ̓perceptions of competence were higher for those who perceived that their teachers provided them with more frequent praise and less frequent criti-cism. Further, higher perceptions of competence were associated with practicing outside of physical education class and the teacherʼs rating of the studentʼs physical education performance.

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM48

48 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 49

Tab

le 4

F

ollo

w-U

p U

niv

aria

te A

nal

yses

of

Var

ian

ce, M

ean

s, a

nd

Sta

nd

ard

Dev

iati

on

s fo

r S

tud

y V

aria

ble

s b

y G

end

er

Mal

esFe

mal

es

Vari

able

sP

rinc

ipal

eff

ects

: ge

nder

MS

DM

SD

Eff

ect

size

Teac

herʼs

feed

back

prai

se0.

263.

311.

203.

261.

16 0

.04

no re

spon

se 5

.45*

3.32

1.40

3.11

1.40

0.1

5st

uden

t-ini

tiate

d co

mm

unic

atio

n–su

cces

s 5

.47*

2.24

1.40

2.40

1.47

–0.1

1te

chni

cal/c

orre

ctiv

e in

form

atio

n

31.0

1***

4.05

1.24

4.69

1.13

–0.5

4en

cour

agem

ent

24

.10*

**3.

661.

243.

871.

32–0

.16

criti

cism

8.9

**3.

110.

962.

920.

97 0

.19

stud

ent-i

nitia

ted

com

mun

icat

ion–

unsu

cces

s 7

.1**

2.53

1.52

2.83

1.61

–0.1

9O

ther

var

iabl

espe

rcei

ved

phys

ical

com

pete

nce

66

.61*

**4.

601.

283.

581.

27 0

.80

phys

ical

edu

catio

n pe

rfor

man

ce

16.8

4***

14.2

002.

8412

.400

2.89

0.6

2

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM49

50 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 51

Tab

le 5

S

um

mar

y o

f R

esu

lts

fro

m t

he

Hie

rarc

hic

al R

egre

ssio

n A

nal

ysis

Pre

dic

tin

g P

erce

pti

on

s o

f C

om

pet

ence

Fem

ales

(n =

232

)M

ales

(n =

181

)

Vari

able

R2

βp

sr2

R2

βp

sr2

Step

1.1

3.2

2ag

e .

05.5

7 .

001

.24

.00

.05

7ph

ysic

al e

duca

tion

perf

orm

ance

.30

.00

.09

.30

.00

.09

prac

tice

.18

.06

.03

2 .

27.0

0 .

072

Step

2.3

1.2

7pr

aise

.33

.00

.10

.01

.90

2.10

–4

stud

ent-i

nitia

ted

com

mun

icat

ion–

succ

ess

.06

.43

.00

3 .

10.3

09.

10–3

tech

nica

l/cor

rect

ive

info

rmat

ion

–.25

.14

.01

.05

.82

4.10

–4

enco

urag

emen

t .

08.6

7 .

000

–.08

.72

9.10

–4

criti

cism

–.17

.02

.05

–.06

.47

.00

5no

resp

onse

–suc

cess

–.04

.54

.00

5–.

14.1

6.0

3st

uden

t-ini

tiate

d co

mm

unic

atio

n–un

succ

essf

ul .

12.1

2 .

011

–.01

.85

4.1

0–10

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM50

50 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 51

For boys, results revealed that age (β = .24), physical education performance (β = .30), and practice (β = .27) were significantly linked with self-perceptions of competence, F(3, 184) = 10.3, p < .01, R² = .22. The change in R2 with the addition of the teacher feedback ratings on Step 2, however, was nonsignificant. In other words, the teachers ̓feedback was not significantly associated with boys ̓perceived competence after controlling for age, practice, and physical education performance. Results did indicate that older boys who practiced outside of class and were rated as performing better in physical education class by their teacher tended to report higher perceptions of competence.

Discussion

There were two main goals in this study: 1) investigate possible gender differences in students ̓ perceptions of feedback provided by teachers, and 2) examine whether perceived feedback from teachers affected students ̓perceived competence.

Purpose 1: Gender Differences

With regard to the first purpose of this study, the goal was to see whether boys and girls expressed differences in the way they perceived their teachers responded to them in class. Based on the previous observational research (e.g., Dunbar & OʼSullivan, 1986), we expected that boys would receive a higher frequency of all forms of feedback from their teachers. Interestingly, our results did not provide strong support for this prediction. Results of this study showed no significant gender differences concerning perceived teacher praise. Further, girls perceived receiving more encouragement and technical information. The only findings consistent with our expectations were that girls reported less criticism and lack of responses from their teachers than boys.

Only one study (McBride, 1990) has examined perceived teacher feedback, and results showed little evidence of gender typing in the physical education setting. These results are unlike most observational studies, such as Dunbar and OʼSullivan (1986), who reported that boys received more positive feedback than girls. Concerning technical information and encouragement, girls felt as if they were more supported by their physical education teacher than boys did. These last two variables, however, were not correlated with perceived physical competence. These results disconfirmed our hypothesis that boys would be more favored with praise, technical information, and encouragement than girls. Another result, how-ever, showed that boys perceived receiving significantly more criticism than girls. This result corroborated findings of observational studies, but as Drudy and Úi Chatháin (2002) specified, criticism tends to represent the lowest proportion of all student–teacher interactions. Moreover, the teacherʼs perceived criticism did not link or affect boys ̓perceptions of physical competence.

Gender differences are often found on expectancy measures particularly for activities stereotyped by gender role such as sport (Eccles & Harold, 1991). Unfor-tunately, gender-role stereotypes are not value-free. Extensive evidence indicates that women are stereotyped as less physically competent than men. Girls remain largely passive and invisible in class, particularly in physical education (Stanworth,

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM51

52 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 53

1983). In physical sciences, Jones and Wheatley (1990) showed that boys demand and get more of the teacherʼs attention. Because boys create conditions (posi-tive and negative) to attract teachers ̓attention, they gain an important learning experience, as well as the confidence of being listened to, thus stimulating them to participate more (Howe, 1997). But our results were contrary to this predic-tion. Perhaps girls were more indulgent, or they learned to be satisfied with the teacherʼs investment in physical education because they are less attracted by sport and they have internalized the gender-role stereotype (Eccles & Harold, 1991). Moreover, boys place more importance on demonstrating their physical competence in school than girls.

Finally, it was expected that boys should initiate more interactions (e.g., ask questions or show their success) with their teachers than girls. But, girls said they initiated teacher communication more often. They often asked for help from their teachers to correct mistakes, and they called him or her for showing their good performance. These results are not entirely consistent with previous studies such as Altermatt et al. (1998), who have suggested that male students initiate more interactions than girls. But Duffy et al. (2001) showed there was no significant difference in the number of interactions initiated by male versus female students in mathematics and literature classes.

Based on these results, there appears to be some inconsistencies in the pattern of results depending on whether observational methods are used to code teacher feedback (e.g., Dunbar & OʼSullivan, 1986) versus asking the students about their perceptions of teacher feedback patterns (e.g., McBride, 1990). Consequently, future research should attempt to explore these patterns more closely using a combination of data collection techniques.

Similarly, it is suggested that future studies explore other reasons for the unexpected finding that girls tended to perceive more frequent interactions with their teachers. For instance, the current study was conducted in French schools, whereas the majority of the previous research has been conducted in North America. Perhaps there are cultural differences in how teachers treat boys versus girls in physical education classes around the world. Furthermore, we might consider the changing beliefs regarding the acceptability of female participation in sport and physical activity. With increasing participation and acceptance of girls in these activities, we might expect to see teachers treating boys and girls in a more equal fashion rather than adhering to the typical gender stereotypes in which sport and physical activity is only appropriate for boys. In other words, providing feedback such as praise, encouragement, and technical instruction to girls might seem more reasonable to teachers because their female students demonstrate increasing interest and competence in sport and physical activity.

Finally, we need to consider a host of factors that might influence how students perceive teachers ̓feedback. Scholars examining coaching feedback patterns have identified a host of personal and situational factors that might influence the way in which an individual perceives and interprets the behaviors and feedback patterns of their coaches (see Horn, 2002). For instance, an individualʼs current perceptions of ability or achievement goal orientation might lead him or her to interpret what their teachers say in different ways (Vallerand & Ratelle, 2002). Similarly, the gender of the feedback provider, in this case the teacher, might have an effect on

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM52

52 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 53

both actual and perceived feedback. Studies have found, for example, that male teachers interact more frequently with female students, whereas the opposite is true for female teachers (e.g., Davis, 2000; Weiller & Doyle, 2000). Given our limited sample of teachers, we were not able to adequately test for this, yet the gender of the teacher might be an important moderating factor. Further, it has been sug-gested that feedback received from an opposite sex provider might influence how the recipient perceives and interprets the information (Amorose & Weiss, 1998). Whereas these factors might help to explain the inconsistencies in our findings, future research is needed to specifically test how gender is related to feedback patterns in physical education classes.

Purpose 2: Predicting Perceptions of Competence

The second purpose of this study was to determine whether perceived feedback was related to girls ̓and boys ̓perceived physical competence. It was expected that teachers ̓perceived feedback would be related to students ̓self-perceptions of competence. However, no a priori hypothesis was made concerning the gender-differentiated relationships for these variables. Results of the regression analyses indicated that girls seemed to be more influenced by teacher feedback than boys. Indeed, perceived feedback explained 18% of variance above and beyond that accounted for by age, practice, and performance in class. That is, girls seem more sensitive to teachers ̓communications than boys, and girls use this feedback as a salient source of competence information (Horn, Glenn, & Wentzell, 1993). Based on these perceptions of teacher feedback, we could say that higher frequencies of praise after performance successes and less criticism after mistakes were associated with higher self-perception of competence in female physical education students. Silverman, Tyson, and Krampitz (1992) found that positive feedback is related to high achievement. They argue that positive feedback motivates students to con-tinue to practice, permitting a greater number of total appropriate practice trials. This was interesting given that the girls perceived receiving an equal amount of praise compared with boys, and they perceived receiving less criticism. Based on these results, positive discrimination in favor of girls could be envisaged to improve girls ̓self-perceptions in physical education. Interventions are needed that raise girls ̓confidence in their ability to do well in sport activities and in physical education classes.

Contrary to expectations, teacher feedback was not related to boys ̓percep-tions of physical competence. The relatively weak influence of perceived teacher feedback might be a result of the use of other sources of information for judging their physical competence. Lee et al. (1993) said that in some situations, movement skills can be learned without any teacher feedback at all. External feedback can be essential for some, whereas others prefer to use internal feedback. For instance, boys might use more peer feedback and comparisons or some other sources of competence information rather than their teachers ̓feedback. Research on sources of competence information provides some support for this in that boys tend to place less importance on significant others as cues of ability compared with girls (Horn & Amorose, 1998).

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM53

54 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 55

Implications and Conclusions

Based on the results of this study, we can propose some implications for teacher education programs. For example, teachers need to be aware of engaging in gender-differentiated behaviors in their classrooms. Most gender equality projects propose to develop critical self-awareness of teachers through a process of self-observation of their own classroom interaction patterns. In other words, teachers should try to become aware of how they interact with each of their students in dif-ferent situations. Although it will be difficult for teachers to control how students perceive their behaviors, trying to provide effective feedback (e.g., high frequencies of positive and informational feedback) to all students, whether male or female, should lead to higher levels of achievement and promote more positive classroom experiences for everyone.

Although special attention was given to the methodology used in this study, some limitations have to be acknowledged. For instance, the cross-sectional nature of the data necessitates caution when interpreting the direction of the relationship between teacherʼs feedback and students ̓physical competence. Only experimental studies can provide a strong inference of causality because such designs control for situational variables. Further, given the recent development of the PTFQ, the questionnaire used in this study, additional testing of the scaleʼs psychometric properties is warranted. Even though establishing the reliability and validity of any measure is an ongoing process, the results of the current investigation provide some encouraging results. Nevertheless, until further support for the measure is provided, our findings should be taken with some caution.

In conclusion, little attention has been paid to students ̓perspectives regarding perceived differential teacher treatment. Our findings differ from previous studies because most of them were based on observations of classroom interaction pat-terns. Recognizing the importance of student thoughts, researchers have just begun exploring student cognitions during physical education lessons (Martinek, 1988), and our study is one of the only ones we are aware of that specifically explores gender differences in perceived feedback. Despite a range of provisional findings (see review, Coffey & Delamont, 2000), the research evidence on teacher–pupil interactions is not as robust as it might be or as it is often assumed to be. More-over, the implications of such findings are not completely clear. Bossert (1982) summarized the results of a number of American studies and concluded: “These results leave us in a muddle. Teachers do treat girls and boys differently, but the extensiveness of this differential treatment, whether it is perceived by students, and how it might affect their sex role behavior and attitudes is unknown” (p. 173). Instruction in equity issues is almost nonexistent in the teacherʼs training curricula in many countries (Bullock, 1997), yet seems critical given the potential effects on students.

ReferencesAllen, J.B., & Howe, B.L. (1998). Player ability, coach feedback, and female adolescent

athletes ̓perceived competence and satisfaction. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychol-ogy, 20, 280-299.

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM54

54 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 55

Altermatt, E.R., Jovanovic, J., & Perry, M. (1998). Bias or responsivity? Sex and achieve-ment-level effects on teachers ̓classroom questioning practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 516-527.

Amorose, A.J., & Horn, T.S. (2000). Intrinsic motivation: Relationships with collegiate athletes ̓gender, scholarship status, and perceptions of their coaches ̓behavior. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 22, 63-84.

Amorose, A.J., & Weiss, M.R. (1998). Coaching feedback as a source of information about perceptions of ability: A developmental examination. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 395-420.

Bailey, S.M. (1993). The current status of gender equity research in American schools. Educational Psychologist, 28, 321-339.

Biddle, S., Sallis, J., & Cavill, N. (1998). Young and active? Young people and health-enhancing physical activity: Evidence and implications. London: Health Education Authority.

Black, S.J., & Weiss, M.R. (1992). The relationship among perceived coaching behaviors, perceptions of ability, and motivation in competitive age-group swimmers. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 14, 309-325.

Bossert, S. (1982). Understanding sex differences in childrenʼs classroom experiences. In W. Doyle & T.L. Good (Eds.), Focus on teaching (pp.172-190). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Bullock, L.D. (1997). Efficacy of a gender and ethnic equity in science education curriculum for preservice teachers. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 34, 1019-1038.

Cheffers, J., & Mancini, V. (1978). Teacher-student interaction. In W. Anderson & G. Bar-rette (Eds.), Whatʼs going on in gym: Descriptive studies of physical education classes (pp. 39-50). Motor Skills: Theory into Practice. Monograph 1.

Chelladurai, P. (1993). Leadership. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 647-671). New York: Macmillan.

Cloes, M., Premuzak, J., & Piéron, M. (1993). Effectiveness of a video training programme used to improve error identification and feedback processes by physical education teachers. International Journal of Physical Education, 32, 4-9.

Coffey, A., & Delamont, S. (2000). Feminism and the classroom teacher: Research, praxis and pedagogy. London: Routledge/Falmer.

Cronbach, L.J. (1951). Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrica, 16, 297-334.

Dart, B., & Clarke, J. (1988). Sexism in schools: A new look. Educational Review, 40, 41-49.

Davis, K.L. (2000). Qualitative study of gender interactions between teachers and students in selected high school physical education classes. Dissertation Abstracts International, 61(04), 1338. (UMI No. 9969878).

Doyle, W. (1977). Classroom effects. Theory into Practice, 18, 138-144. Drudy, S., & Úi Chatháin, M. (2002). Gender effects in classroom interaction: Data collection,

self-analysis, and reflection. Evaluation and Research in Education, 16, 34-50.Duffy, J., Warren, K., & Walsh, M. (2001). Classroom interactions: Gender of teacher, gender

of student, and classroom subject. Sex Roles, 45, 579-593.Dunbar, R., & OʼSullivan, M. (1986). Effects of intervention on differential treatment of

boys and girls in elementary physical education lessons. Journal of Teaching in Physi-cal Education, 5, 166-175.

Eccles, J.S., & Blumenfeld, P. (1985). Classroom experiences and student gender: Are there differences and do they matter? In L.C. Wilkinson & C.B. Marrett (Eds.), Gender influ-ences in classroom interaction (pp. 79-114). Orlando, FL: Academic.

Eccles, J.S., & Harold, R.D. (1991). Gender differences in sport involvement: Applying the Eccles ̓expectancy-value model. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 7-35.

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM55

56 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 57

Eisenberg, N., Martin, C.L., & Fabes, R.A. (1996). Gender development and gender effects. In D.C. Berliner & R.C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 358-396). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.

French Physical Education Programmes (31 August 2000). High school general and tech-nologic programmes. Hors series, Official Bulletin, 6.

Griffin, P. (1981). One small step for personkind: Observations and suggestions for sex equality in coeducational physical education classes. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 1, 7-12.

Harter, S. (1985). Manual for the self-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver Press.

Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (pp. 553-618). New York: Wiley & Sons.

Heller, K.A., & Parsons, J.E. (1981). Sex differences in teacher s̓ evaluative feedback and stu-dents ̓expectancies for success in mathematics. Child Development, 52, 1015-1019.

Horn, T.S. (2002). Coaching effectiveness in the sport domain. In T.S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (pp. 309-354). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Horn, T.S., & Amorose, A.J. (1998). Sources of competence information. In J.L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise psychology measurement (pp. 49-64). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology.

Horn, T.S., Glenn, S.D., & Wentzell, A.B. (1993). Sources of information underlying personal ability judgments in high school athletes. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5, 263-274.

Howe, C. (1997). Gender and classroom interaction. Edinburgh: The Scottish Council for Research in Education.

Jones, M.G., & Wheatley, J. (1990). Gender differences in student-teacher interaction. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 27, 861-874.

Lee, A.M., Keh, N.C., & Magill, R.A. (1993). Instructional effects of teacher feedback in physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 12, 228-243.

Lee, A.M., & Solmon, M.A. (1992). Cognitive conceptions of teaching and learning motor skills. Quest, 44, 57-71.

Magill, R.A. (1993). Augmented feedback in skill acquisition. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 193-212). New York: Macmillan.

Martinek, T.J. (1988). Confirmation of a teacher expectancy model: Student perceptions and causal attributions of teaching behaviors. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 59, 118-126.

McBride, R.E. (1990). Sex-role stereotyping behaviours among elementary, junior, and senior high school physical education specialist. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 9, 249-261.

Nicaise, V. (2003). Students ̓perceptions of teachers ̓feedback in physical education class. Unpublished master thesis. /aq/

Papaioannou, A. (1994). Development of a questionnaire to measure achievement orientations in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 65, 11-20.

Roberts, G.C. (2001). Understanding the dynamics of motivation in physical activity: The influence of achievement goals and motivational processes. In G.C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 1-50). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Silverman, S. (1994). Communication and motor skill learning: What we learn from research in the gymnasium. Quest, 46, 345-355.

Silverman, S., Tyson, L.A., & Krampitz, J. (1992). Teacher feedback and achievement in physical education: Interaction with student practice. Teaching and Teacher Educa-tion, 8, 333-344.

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:13 AM56

56 Nicaise et al. Perceptions of Teacher Feedback 57

Smith, D. (1992). A description of classroom interaction and gender disparity in secondary business education instruction. The Delta Phi Epsilon Journal, 34, 183-193.

Smith, R.E., Smoll, F.L., & Hunt, E. (1977). A system for the behavioral assessment of athletic coaches. Research Quarterly, 48, 401-407.

Smoll, F.L., & Smith, R.E. (2002). Coaching behavior research and interventions in youth sport. In F.L. Smoll & R.E. Smith (Eds.), Children and youth in sport (pp. 211-231). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.

Solomons, H. (1977). Sex role mediated achievements behaviors and interpersonal dynam-ics of fifth grade co-educational physical education classes. Dissertation Abstracts International, 37, 5445A. (UMI No. 7706538).

Stanworth, M. (1983). Gender and schooling. London: Hutchinson.Stevens, J.P. (1986). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.Tabachnick, B.G., & Fidell, L.S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd ed.). New York:

Harper & Collins.Thomas, J.R., Salazar, W., & Landers, D.M. (1991). What is missing in p less than .05?

Effect size. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62, 344-348.Tjeerdsma, B.L. (1997). A comparison of teacher and student perspectives of tasks and

feedback. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 16, 388-400.Trouilloud, D.O., Sarrazin, P.G., Martinek, T.J., & Guillet, E. (2002). The influence of

teacher expectations on student achievement in physical education classes: Pygmalion revisited. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 591-607.

Vallerand, R.J., & Ratelle, C.F. (2002). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: A hierarchical model. In E.L. Deci & R.M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 37-64). Rochester, NY: The University of Rochester Press.

Weiller, K.H., & Doyle, E.J. (2000) Teacher–student interaction: An exploration of gender differences in elementary physical education. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 71, 43-45.

Notes1In France, physical education classes are compulsory and typically coeducational. Coedu-

cational classes in and of themselves, however, do not necessarily ensure equal treatment of boys and girls. The French physical education profession itself has been largely silent on issues of gender equality. In todayʼs modern society, it might be expected that male and female students would receive equal attention in school, yet solid evidence of this is lacking.

2The complete version of the PTFQ is available from the first author.3There were missing data because a few students werenʼt evaluated by their teachers. These

subjects (n = 20) were deleted from all analyses.4Effect sizes were calculated using the following equation from Thomas, Salazar, and Land-

ers (1991): M1 – M2 / SDpooled. In this equation M1 and M2 represent the means of two comparison groups (e.g., males vs. females) and the SDpooled represents the pooled standard deviation between the two groups

04Nicaise(36) 12/11/05, 8:14 AM57