Social factors modulating attention patterns in carrion crows

18
Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 brill.com/beh Social factors modulating attention patterns in carrion crows Claudia A.F. Wascher a,, Jose W. Valdez b , Cristina Núñez Cebrián a , Vittorio Baglione a and Daniela Canestrari c a Departamento de Ciencias Agro-Forestales, University of Valladolid, Valladolid, Spain b State University of New York College of Environmental Science and Forestry, Syracuse, NY, USA c Unidad Mixta de Investigación en Biodiversidad, Campus de Mieres, University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain * Corresponding author’s current address: Departamento de Ciencias Agro-Forestales Campus La Yutera, Avenida de Madrid 44, 34004 Palencia, Spain, e-mail: [email protected] Accepted 20 October 2013; published online 7 December 2013 Abstract A benefit of group living is the opportunity for individuals to gain valuable information from others, for example about predators, food sources or mate quality. However paying attention to conspecifics also induces costs such as time constraints. In order to optimize information-gaining processes individuals are expected to be selective with regards to whom and what they pay attention to. This selectivity may depend on factors like context, sex, age and social status of both the observing individual and the model. In the present study, we investigated the attention patterns in carrion crows (Corvus corone corone). Since carrion crows live in complex societies where the use of social information is likely to be highly beneficial, we expect attention patterns to be strongly affected by social factors. Nineteen captive crows were presented with either familiar or unfamiliar and kin or non-kin model individuals. We quantified the duration and frequency each observer watched the model in each experimental session. Familiarity with the model did neither affect watching duration nor frequency. However, there was an effect of kinship on individual attention, with male crows observing non-kin at higher frequency than kin, whereas female observers showed no preference. We also found an effect of the percentage of group members watching within a session and attentiveness of each observer. Overall, our study shows that social factors affect attention patterns in crows. Keywords attentiveness, social stimuli, familiarity, carrion crow, Corvus corone corone. © 2014 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003148

Transcript of Social factors modulating attention patterns in carrion crows

Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 brill.com/beh

Social factors modulating attention patterns incarrion crows

Claudia A.F. Wascher a,∗, Jose W. Valdez b, Cristina Núñez Cebrián a,

Vittorio Baglione a and Daniela Canestrari c

a Departamento de Ciencias Agro-Forestales, University of Valladolid, Valladolid, Spainb State University of New York College of Environmental Science and Forestry,

Syracuse, NY, USAc Unidad Mixta de Investigación en Biodiversidad, Campus de Mieres,

University of Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain*Corresponding author’s current address: Departamento de Ciencias Agro-Forestales

Campus La Yutera, Avenida de Madrid 44, 34004 Palencia, Spain,e-mail: [email protected]

Accepted 20 October 2013; published online 7 December 2013

AbstractA benefit of group living is the opportunity for individuals to gain valuable information fromothers, for example about predators, food sources or mate quality. However paying attention toconspecifics also induces costs such as time constraints. In order to optimize information-gainingprocesses individuals are expected to be selective with regards to whom and what they pay attentionto. This selectivity may depend on factors like context, sex, age and social status of both theobserving individual and the model. In the present study, we investigated the attention patternsin carrion crows (Corvus corone corone). Since carrion crows live in complex societies where theuse of social information is likely to be highly beneficial, we expect attention patterns to be stronglyaffected by social factors. Nineteen captive crows were presented with either familiar or unfamiliarand kin or non-kin model individuals. We quantified the duration and frequency each observerwatched the model in each experimental session. Familiarity with the model did neither affectwatching duration nor frequency. However, there was an effect of kinship on individual attention,with male crows observing non-kin at higher frequency than kin, whereas female observers showedno preference. We also found an effect of the percentage of group members watching withina session and attentiveness of each observer. Overall, our study shows that social factors affectattention patterns in crows.

Keywordsattentiveness, social stimuli, familiarity, carrion crow, Corvus corone corone.

© 2014 Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden DOI:10.1163/1568539X-00003148

556 Social attention in crows

1. Introduction

A benefit of living in a group is the possibility to obtain valuable informa-tion from other group members (Galef & Laland, 2005; King & Cowlishaw,2007) with regards to predators (Macdonald, 1983; Mineka & Cook, 1988;Griffin & Evans, 2003), food sources (van Schaik, 1983; Fernández-Juricic& Kacelnik, 2004), territory or mate quality (Danchin & Wagner, 1997; Val-one & Templeton, 2002). However paying attention to conspecifics inducescosts (Giraldeau et al., 2002), in the form of time constraints (Pollard, 2010)or incorrect decision-making, leading for example to energetic costs whenvisiting low quality food patches (Rieucau & Giraldeau, 2011). Therefore, inorder to optimize information-gaining processes, individuals must be selec-tive to whom and what they attend to (Blumstein et al., 2004). This selectivitymay depend on factors like context, sex, age and social status of the ob-server, i.e., the individual watching as well as the model, i.e., the individualbeing watched (Range & Huber, 2007; Scheid et al., 2007). An individual’sdecisions about whether and how long to watch a model individual mightstrongly depend on an individual’s quality, e.g., aggressive strength, matequality, reliability of information provided (Valone, 2007) and the relation-ship between the observer and model individual (kinship: Valsecchi et al.,1996; affiliation status: Scheid et al., 2007). It has been previously shownthat the choice of interaction partner is an important mechanism in complexsocial systems in order to increase efficiency for example in cooperative in-teractions (Noe & Hammerstein, 1994) or social learning (Laland, 2004) andin order to be able to make accurate partner choice decisions animals needinformation about available partners, e.g., reliability, efficiency (Blumsteinet al., 2004; Melis et al., 2006). Furthermore, the social organisation of aspecies influences attention patterns in a way that individuals are less spe-cific in respect to which group members to pay attention to and from whomto learn in highly egalitarian societies, whereas they are more specific indespotic societies (Coussi-Korbel & Fragaszy, 1995).

The present study investigated attention patterns in the carrion crow(Corvus corone corone), a species living in a highly complex and flexiblesocial organization. In most European populations carrion crows form non-breeder flocks up to adulthood and afterwards split up in territorial breedingpairs (Glutz von Blotzheim, 1985). In northern Spain however, crows livein cooperatively breeding groups of up to nine individuals, consisting ofthe breeding pair and retained offspring as well as mostly male, immigrants

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 557

(Baglione et al., 2003). In the case of cooperative breeding, crows mostly in-teract with kin, for example in helping interactions at the nest, e.g., feedingof nestlings (Baglione et al., 2003). Crows are despotic (sensu Coussi-Korbel& Fragaszy, 1995) and form a linear and stable dominance hierarchy withinbreeding groups, with the breeding male being the most dominant individualfollowed by immigrant males, male retained offspring, the breeding female,female retained offspring and female immigrants (Chiarati et al., 2010).Dominant males share food nepotistically with their offspring rather thanwith less related immigrants (Chiarati et al., 2011). Subordinate individualswithin these breeding groups have been shown to benefit from exploratorydominants, who approached novel, potentially dangerous food first (Chiaratiet al., 2012). In closely related corvid species, i.e., members of the genusCorvus, e.g., ravens or rooks, individuals are known to form valuable re-lationships, not only between reproductive partners but especially juvenilesform short- to long-term coalitions and alliances (Heinrich, 2011; Fraser &Bugnyar, 2012). Within such valuable relationships, individuals support eachother in agonistic encounters (Emery et al., 2007; Fraser & Bugnyar, 2012)and share information and resources (Bugnyar et al., 2001; de Kort et al.,2006; Fraser & Bugnyar, 2012). Outside such valuable relationships, ravensand other corvids act competitively, applying various tactics in order to out-perform conspecifics (Bugnyar & Heinrich, 2005, 2006).

Here, we question whether social information is relevant for carrion crowsand if they show selective attention patterns depending on familiarity withthe model, kinship, sex, social behaviour of the observer (i.e., tendency toshow affiliative, agonistic behaviours) and the kind of social relationshipwith the model, quantified by looking at dyadic social interactions duringstandard behavioural observations in the group. In order to quantify the rele-vance of social information, we measured how long (duration in seconds perminute of session duration) and how often (frequency per minutes of sessionduration) carrion crows observed a conspecific model individual. Similar toresults in other social species, we predict crows to show significant inter-est in social information provided by the model individual. Furthermore, thecomplexity of corvid social organisation leads to the prediction that individ-uals should pay selective attention depending on the identity of the modelindividual. On the one hand, crows could potentially gain social informa-tion from unknown birds (e.g., aggressive potential, mate quality), but on

558 Social attention in crows

the other hand they may obtain information on how conspecifics (both fa-miliar and unknown) deal with a novel situation. If individuals value socialinformation from unknown individuals more, we expect crows to be moreattentive to unfamiliar birds than to familiar birds, while if information on anovel situation is preferred, individuals should pay (i) equal attention to bothfamiliar and unfamiliar models, or (ii) more attention to familiar models, asthey may provide more reliable information. In addition, because socialityin the study population is mainly based on kin relationships (Baglione et al.,2003), and social bonds with kin are likely to be more valuable than socialbonds with familiar non-kin individuals outside the reproductive context weexpect individuals to pay more attention to kin than to familiar non-kin. Withrespect to sex, on the one hand males may be expected to pay more attentionto model stimuli than females, because both social relationships and coop-erative behaviour in this population are male-biased (Baglione et al., 2002a;Canestrari et al., 2005), and therefore social information, e.g., aggressive po-tential of an individual, may be more valuable for males than for females. Onthe other hand, however, it has been shown that in the context of foraging andexploration subordinate individuals, which includes females, heavily rely onexploration behaviour of dominant males (Chiarati et al., 2011), suggest-ing that females could be more attentive to conspecifics than males. Also,male model stimuli should be of larger interest for both sexes than femalemodels, due to males’ exploratory behaviour, and a positive social relation-ship between the observer and the model should increase the attention of theformer. Finally, we expect a social facilitation effect, where individuals over-come their neophobia and are more attentive to the model in sessions whenmore individuals in the group are watching the model.

2. Methods

2.1. Ethics statement

All bird manipulations were authorized by Junta de Castilla y León(EP/LE/359/2009). Experiments were conducted by an experienced animalcaretaker, which ensured that no animals were harmed by the experimen-tal procedure, e.g., handling model individuals. Model individuals had adlibitum access to drinking water and food and were observed by an experi-enced animal caretaker to ensure they were not chronically stressed duringthe entire experimental procedure. In fact, after catching and handling crows

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 559

calmed down quite fast and then displayed ‘normal’ behaviour, e.g., feed-ing, preening, resting. Observer individuals remained in captivity before andafter completion of the present study, wild model crows have been releasedback in their territory. In previous studies it has been shown that captureand manipulation of birds has no negative effects on crows (Baglione et al.,2002b, 2010).

2.2. Study subjects

The subjects, which served as observing individuals, were 19 carrion crowshoused in a large outdoor aviary in Solanilla, Spain. Birds were kept in threeseparate groups, reflecting different forms of social organisations found incrows in the wild. The ‘flock’ contained seven non-breeding juveniles (fourfemales and three males) and composed of three pairs of siblings (one pairof female siblings, one pair of male siblings, and one mixed pair of siblings)and one unrelated individual. All individuals in the flock hatched betweenApril and May 2008 and were hand-raised in captivity. The other two groupsrepresented cooperative breeding families, one family was composed of abreeding pair, a one-year old male helper (likely their offspring), and theirthree juvenile offspring (two females and one male), the other family com-posed of a breeding pair and their four juvenile offspring (two females andtwo males). Adult birds and the helper were captured in their territory inSobarriba, Spain during the chick feeding period, when the offspring wasapproximately 15–20 days old in June 2008. Their parents and the helpersubsequently raised nestlings in captivity.

The aviary (30 × 12 × 6 m) was separated in four major compartments(12 × 6 × 3 m), and a testing compartment in the middle (3 × 3 × 3 m),through which each aviary compartment could be entered. The three groupswere visually but not acoustically separated from each other and the testingcompartment. The aviary was equipped with wooden perches, natural vege-tation, rocks, leaves, and feeders, which provided ad libitum access to foodpellets. An enriched diet consisting of meat, apple, pear, cheese, and eggswas provided during the weekdays. Water was available ad libitum for bothdrinking and bathing.

2.3. Experimental procedure

The experiments were conducted in June–August 2009. The test stimuli, i.e.,model individuals consisted of five group members (familiar stimuli) and five

560 Social attention in crows

wild-caught crows (unfamiliar stimuli). Familiar crows were caught from theaviaries in the evening before the trial day using a large mesh bird net andthen placed in the experimental cage (119 × 46 × 46 cm). In the flock, fa-miliar individuals could be either kin or non-kin, as the group consisted ofthree sibling pairs and one individual without siblings, whereas in the fami-lies all familiar individuals were related. The wild crows were captured usinga walk-in crow trap (90 × 45 × 45 cm) divided in half, with one side hous-ing a live decoy crow and the other side with uncooked liver as bait. The trapwas placed in known crow territories before dawn and checked around 1000.If no crow was caught until then, trapping was reattempted the followingmorning. To avoid recapturing the same individual twice or capturing relatedindividuals, all wild crows (N = 6) came from distinctly different territo-rial areas. The captured crows were body measured (e.g., weight, tarsus, billlength). The respective age of each crow was estimated by the internal colourof the upper mandible (Svensson, 1992). Afterwards they were placed in theexperimental cage and left overnight for habituation. After successful con-clusion of the experiment (6–8 days), the crows were released back in theirterritory. Experiments have been performed twice daily, in the morning be-tween 05:30 and 11:10 h and the late afternoon 18:00 and 21:00 h. Familiarbirds were presented to the group in two consecutive sessions (morning andafternoon), unfamiliar birds were presented in four sessions, taking place ontwo different days with a three days break in between. Sessions lasted for78–118 min, depending on the length of the videotape and were recorded onvideo (Sony DCR-TRV270E).

During the test session, the experimental cage with the model bird wasput on the ground of the test compartment adjacent to the focal group. Themodel bird was provided with food and water in the test cage. Opaque coversblocked view into the experimental room. Three holes, 2 cm in diameter,20 cm above ground and 25 cm apart from each other, through which theobserver birds could peak in the cage with stimulus bird, when standingdirectly in front of the holes. Three holes were provided in order to avoidpossible competition for watching opportunities within the groups.

The camera was placed on a tripod inside the experimental group’s com-partment approximately three meters away from the observation holes. An-other camera on a tripod was placed inside the experimental compartmentapproximately 1.5 m from the experimental cage (Figure 1).

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 561

Figure 1. Sketch of the experimental setup. The grey squares show the location of the cagewith the model individual during experimental sessions. The little blue squares display thelocation of the video cameras and the three black dots indicate the three watching holes. Thisfigure is published in colour in the online edition of this journal, which can be accessed viahttp://booksandjournals.brillonline.com/content/1568539x.

Video footage was analysed by J.W.V. and C.A.F.W. An observation eventstarted, when an individual approached within ten cm of an observation holeand was clearly using an eye to peek through the hole. We measured thefrequency of observation events and the observing duration in seconds. Ob-servation duration and frequency was corrected for actual observation time(seconds watching/minutes observation time; observation events/minutes ob-servation time). Individuals of the group, which did not observe in a specifictest session, were indicated as zero. For each observation session the per-centage of birds from the group engaging in at least one observation boutwas calculated.

2.4. Behavioural observations

Between October 2008 and January 2009 we recorded 18 to 30 focal pro-tocols (mean ± SD = 22.052 ± 2.914) per individual of the captive group,which later on served as observers in the presented experiment. These ob-servations were performed in an un-manipulated group context in orderto describe the social behaviour of each individual as well as the socialrelationships between those individuals that later were tested in the atten-tion experimental setting. Each protocol lasted for five minutes and wasvideo recorded for later analysis by C.A.F.W. Duration and frequency of

562 Social attention in crows

the following behavioural parameters were measured: identity of the nearestneighbour, distance to the nearest neighbour in meters, vocalizations, preen-ing, shaking, head wish, wing quivering, approach non-aggressive, agonisticbehaviour (threat, chase flight, fight) and affiliative behaviour (contact sit, al-logrooming, demand to be groomed). Identity of involved individuals as wellas of the initiating and receiving individual was noted. To characterize socialbehaviour, we calculated for each individual the average nearest neighbourdistance and the frequency of non-aggressive approaches, aggression won,aggression lost and affiliative interactions with (1) any other member of thesame group; (2) every individual from the same group that acted as model inthe experiments (76 dyads in total).

2.5. Statistical analysis

We ran two generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) with linear errordistribution and an identity link function. Response variables were watchingfrequency and duration per minute of session duration. In order to account forrepeated measures for each observer individual, the individual identity wasincluded as random factor. Number of session, group type (family, flock),breeding status of the observer, sex of the observer, sex of the model, famil-iarity of the model, relatedness between model and observer and percentageof group watching in each session were included as fixed factors, as wellas two-way interactions between number of session, group type, breedingstatus, observer sex, model sex, familiarity of the model and relatedness.We selected the final model through a backward stepwise selection eliminat-ing non-significant factors from the model in order to reach the best model.GLMM analyses were performed in SPSS 19.0.

Affiliative and agonistic behavioural parameters were analysed separatelybecause dyadic interaction data were only available for birds belonging to thesame group and obviously did not include the crows caught in the wild. Sam-ple size was therefore smaller than that of previous analyses, making a mul-tivariate approach unsuitable. We therefore used Spearman rank correlationtest to analyse the relationship between watching duration/frequency withthe frequency of social interactions that each individual performed with anyother group member (approach non-aggressive, aggression won, aggressionlost, affiliative interactions given, affiliative interactions received, mean near-est neighbour distance) during behavioural observations in a standard groupcontext (see previous section). For experimental sessions where a familiar

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 563

individual served as model, we also correlated watching duration/frequencywith the frequency of dyadic social interactions (approach non-aggressive,aggression won, aggression lost, affiliative interactions, mean neighbour dis-tance) between observer and model. After Bonferroni correction, alpha wasset at 0.008 to account for multiple testing.

3. Results

Overall, in 110 sessions and 180 h 8 min 33 s of total session duration, crowsobserved the model for 5 h 20 min 42 s in 1321 watching bouts. This resultsin a mean ± SD of 0.116 ± 0.131 s of watching and 0.033 ± 0.033 boutsper min session duration.

We did find significant differences between breeders and non-breeders inwatching duration (GLMM: F = 9.102, df1 = 1, df2 = 651, p = 0.003) andfrequency (GLMM: F = 8.509, df1 = 1, df2 = 652, p = 0.004). Breederswatched (mean ± SD: duration: 0.024 ± 0.082 s/min, frequency: 0.004 ±0.011 bouts/min) significantly less compared to non-breeders (mean ± SD:duration: 0.118 ± 0.025 s/min, frequency: 0.018 ± 0.241 bouts/min).

Watching behaviour significantly decreased over the sessions (GLMM:duration: F = 3.518, df1 = 3, df2 = 651, p = 0.015; frequency: F = 4.273,df1 = 3, df2 = 652, p = 0.005; Figure 2).

Familiarity with the model did not affect watching duration (GLMM:F = 0.53, df1 = 1, df2 = 651, p = 0.467; mean ± SD: familiar: 0.1 ±

Figure 2. Mean (±SD) watching duration in seconds (A) and mean (±SD) watching fre-quency (B) per minute of observation, in different experimental sessions. Birds received twosessions with each familiar model individual and four sessions with each unfamiliar model.Sessions 1 and 2 include both familiar and unfamiliar model individuals, sessions 3 and 4only unfamiliar model crows.

564 Social attention in crows

0.21 s/min, unfamiliar: 0.097 ± 0.225 s/min) and frequency (GLMM: F =0.815, df1 = 1, df2 = 652, p = 0.366; mean ± SD: familiar: 0.013 ±0.019 bouts/min, unfamiliar: 0.016 ± 0.025 bouts/min). No differences werefound in attention patterns between family (mean ± SD: duration: 0.093 ±0.254 s/min, frequency: 0.011 ± 0.019 bouts/min) and flock (mean ± SD:duration: 0.107 ± 0.141, frequency: 0.022 ± 0.029 bouts/min) individuals(GLMM: duration: F = 0.18, df1 = 1, df2 = 651, p = 0.671; frequency:F = 0.001, df1 = 1, df2 = 652, p = 0.978). Watching duration did not differbetween males (mean ± SD: 0.1 ± 0.233 s/min) and females (duration:0.096 ± 0.209 s/min; duration: GLMM: F = 0.041, df1 = 1, df2 = 651,p = 0.839), but in the flock females watched more often than males andin the families this pattern was reversed (group × observer sex interaction:frequency: GLMM: F = 12.267, df1 = 1, df2 = 652, p < 0.001).

The sex of the model individual did not affect watching duration (GLMM:F = 0.841, df1 = 1, df2 = 651, p = 0.36; mean ± SD: female models:0.1 ± 0.188 s/min; male models: 0.1 ± 0.229 s/min) and frequency (GLMM:F = 0, df1 = 1, df2 = 652, p = 0.997; mean ± SD: female models: 0.011 ±0.017 bouts/min; male models: 0.016 ± 0.02 bouts/min). We found male ob-servers to watch kin less often than non-kin (mean ± SD: male observers:kin: 0.003 ± 0.007 bouts/min, non-kin: 0.019 ± 0.02 bouts/min), whereasfemales watched kin and non-kin with similar frequency (mean ± SD: fe-male observers: kin: 0.044 ± 0.027 bouts/min, non-kin: 0.052 ± 0.036bouts/min; kinship × observer sex: GLMM: F = 5.006, df1 = 1, df2 =652, p = 0.003; Figure 3). Watching duration was not affected by kinship(GLMM: F = 0.811, df1 = 1, df2 = 651, p = 0.372).

We found a positive effect of the percentage of individuals from a groupwatching in a session and watching duration (GLMM: F = 51.344, df1 = 1,df2 = 651, p < 0.001; Figure 4) and frequency (GLMM: F = 88.222, df1 =1, df2 = 652, p < 0.001).

The mean number of non-aggressive approaches per behavioural observa-tion session, which was recorded in an un-manipulated group situation out-side the experimental context (see methods), was negatively correlated withwatching duration (Spearman’s rho test r2 = −0.622, p = 0.004, N = 19)and frequency (Spearman’s rho test r2 = −0.529, p = 0.02, N = 19; notsignificant (n.s.) after Bonferroni correction) and mean number of aggressiveinteractions lost was positively correlated with watching duration (Spear-man’s rho test: r2 = 0.558, p = 0.013, N = 19; n.s. after Bonferroni) but

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 565

Figure 3. Mean ± SD watching frequency per minute of observation, depending on kinshipto the model individual and sex of the observer individual.

not watching frequency (Spearman’s rho test: r2 = 0.261, p = 0.28, N =19). The frequency of other behaviours shown by an individual during be-havioural observations did not correlate with watching duration in our exper-iment (Spearman’s rho test: aggression won: r2 = 0.271, p = 0.262, N = 19;nearest neighbour distance: r2 = 0.406, p = 0.084, N = 19; affiliation ac-tive: r2 = 0.058, p = 0.812, N = 19; affiliation received: r2 = −0.237,

Figure 4. Watching duration in seconds per minute of session duration, depending on howmany group-members are watching within one session. The x-axis displays the percentage ofgroup members watching; the y-axis displays mean watching duration per individual. Zeromeans nobody of the group is watching, 100 means every group member was watching themodel individual. Each dot represents an observation session.

566 Social attention in crows

p = 0.33, N = 19) and frequency (Spearman’s rho test: aggression won:r2 = 0.187, p = 0.443, N = 19; nearest neighbour distance: r2 = 0.413,p = 0.079, N = 19; affiliation active: r2 = −0.146, p = 0.551, N = 19;affiliation received: r2 = −0.177, p = 0.468, N = 19). We also did not findany significant correlations between the social relationship between observerand model (quantified on the base of dyadic interactions occurred during be-havioural observations in a standard group context) and watching duration(Spearman’s rho test: aggression lost: r2 = −0.1, p = 0.933, N = 76; ag-gression won: r2 = −0.018, p = 0.876, N = 76; approach non-aggressive:r2 = 0.006, p = 0.958, N = 76; nearest neighbour distance: r2 = 0.21, p =0.069, N = 76; affiliation: r2 = −0.1, p = 0.391, N = 76) and frequency(Spearman’s rho test: aggression lost: r2 = 0.069, p = 0.556, N = 76; ag-gression won: r2 = 0.31, p = 0.789, N = 76; approach non-aggressive:r2 = −0.15, p = 0.897, N = 76; nearest neighbour distance: r2 = 0.211,p = 0.067, N = 76; affiliation: r2 = −0.135, p = 0.246, N = 76).

4. Discussion

In this study we investigated social factors affecting attention patterns in car-rion crows. We found that watching behaviour did not differ depending onthe familiarity to the model. This contrasts with our prediction that differ-ences in social relevance would result in differences in attention patterns. Wewould have expected crows to show more interest in unfamiliar individualsif social information about aggressive tendencies or strength but also matequality was more important than information on familiar individuals dealingwith a new situation. Our results suggest that social information gained ina novel situation is important for crows, independent of whether the infor-mation is acquired from a familiar or unfamiliar individual. Most previousstudies investigating attention patterns in complex social systems did not testfor responses to unfamiliar individuals (Range & Huber, 2007; Scheid et al.,2007; Range et al., 2009). A previous study in guppies showed individualsto preferably shoal with familiar individuals during foraging (Lachlan et al.,1998), whereas studies in Norway rats shows no effect of demonstrator fa-miliarity on foraging decisions (Galef et al., 1998). Our results show thatthe effects of familiarity on social attention patterns are not as clear as ex-pected and further study on the use of social information in complex socialorganisations is needed.

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 567

Furthermore, when presented with familiar birds, male observers, unlikefemales watched non-kin models more frequently than kin. This is surprisingsince kinship is known to have a facilitating effect on social learning inter-actions (Valsecchi et al., 1996; Schwab et al., 2008a). Nonetheless, in thissociety where individuals live year round in enlarged families that share oneall purpose territory, the opportunities for interacting with kin are virtuallyubiquitous, so that crows may extract more valuable social information fromwatching non-kin when the circumstances allow this. Also, non-kin individ-uals could probably be of interest as a potential pair-partner and thereforemore interesting than kin individuals. However, we did not find an effect ofmodel sex on attention. If finding a mate played a pivotal role in our experi-mental setting, we would have expected males to watch female models more,but this was not the case, although we cannot exclude that the lack of a sexeffect was due to small sample size. It has previously been suggested thatthe social organisation and foraging ecology of species might strongly affectattention pattern. For example ravens have been shown to be more attentiveto affiliated compared to non-affiliated individuals, whereas affiliation sta-tus and kinship did not affect attention patterns in jackdaws (Scheid et al.,2007; Schwab et al., 2008b). Although direct comparisons of studies have tobe viewed with caution as experimental designs differ (e.g., session length,engagement of model individual in different behaviours), our present resultsagree with previous findings in jackdaws (Corvus monedula) but contrastpatterns in common ravens (Corvus corax). This is interesting as our studypopulation consists of cooperatively breeding carrion crows, which live incohesive kin groups (Baglione et al., 2003). This is different from the socialorganisation of crows in the rest of Europe, where crows breed in territorialpairs and form fission–fusion societies (Glutz von Blotzheim, 1985). As thesocial organisation of crows and ravens is very similar in most parts of Eu-rope, we always expected cognitive and social features of these two speciesto be more similar than between species with a different social organisation,e.g., rooks, jackdaws, which are colonial and typically forage in large groups.The present study diverges from this prediction, which might be attributed tocooperative breeding in this population of crows. To draw final conclusionsabout the effect of social organisation on corvid attention patterns there isneed of a study investigating attention patterns in non-cooperatively breed-ing crows. If those crows behaved more like ravens than jackdaws, this would

568 Social attention in crows

strongly support our previous assumptions about the underlying functions ofattention patterns to social stimuli (Scheid et al., 2007; Schwab et al., 2008a).

Depending on group type, we did find a slight effect of observer sex onwatching frequency. In the families, males watched more often than females,which is to be expected as in crows an effect of sex on exploratory behaviourhas been described previously, with dominant males (breeders) approachinga novel food source faster than other individuals of the group (Chiarati etal., 2012) and males being dominant over females and having a more sta-ble hierarchy (Chiarati et al., 2010). Also, mostly male individuals serve ashelpers in the study population (Canestrari et al., 2005). In the flock this pat-tern was reversed and female individuals watched more often. However asthe effect is not very strong, our sample size is small and, as already men-tioned, our study design allows only limited amount of social informationto be gained from the model individual we should be careful with the inter-pretation of this result. Similarly, we found adult breeders watch less oftenand for shorter durations than non-breeders, but this result also needs to betaken with caution because of the small number of breeders sampled and apotential confounding effect of age.

We did find watching behaviours of crows to be socially facilitated. Insessions where more individuals of the group paid attention to the modelindividual, individuals observed for longer durations and more often. So-cial facilitation has been suggested as important psychological mechanismin social learning processes (Coussi-Korbel & Fragaszy, 1995). It helps in-dividuals in overcoming neophobia (Addessi & Visalberghi, 2001) and in-creases foraging efficiency (Ryer & Olla, 1992; Soma & Hasegawa, 2004),which might also be the main benefits of social facilitation in carrion crows.However, from the present dataset we cannot conclude about a causal linkbetween number of individuals of a group watching and attention patterns.Possibly, the model stimulus was more interesting in sessions where more in-dividuals of the group watched and therefore observers watched longer andmore often.

Lastly, we investigated how the social behaviour of an individual in a stan-dard group situation is correlated with its attentiveness and whether the socialrelationship between the model and the observer individual affected attentionpatterns. We found that individuals who approached conspecifics in a non-aggressive manner were less attentive towards the model individuals in theexperiment. This could be another indicator that in our experiment, being

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 569

‘less attentive’ did not necessarily mean ‘less interested’ in social informa-tion. We cannot say what watching duration or how many watching bouts arenecessary to gain all the necessary information about a model individual. In-dividuals may even differ in their ability to gain social information and moresocial individuals might be more efficient in gaining information than moreaggressive ones (Taborsky & Oliveira, 2012).

The relationship between the observer and model individuals (frequencyof affiliative or agonistic contacts) did not affect attention patterns. In ourstudy, affiliative behaviours, e.g., contact sitting, allogrooming, were exclu-sively shared between related individuals. Therefore in the group of juvenilesiblings, each crow shared affiliative contacts only with its sibling. The onlyexception was the one crow without a sibling, who was affiliative with twoother individuals at low rates. Anyhow, in the family groups all group mem-bers were also related to each other and therefore did share a varying numberof affiliative contacts with different individuals (family 1: 0–36; family 2:0–17 affiliative interactions recorded: Wascher, unpublished data). When itcame to aggressive behaviours, we would have expected observers to avoidmodel individuals against whom they lose in aggressive encounters, but ourdata showed no indication in this direction. The lack of a relationship couldeasily be due to the small sample size. Also, behavioural protocols wererecorded from November 2008 to January 2009, whereas the attention exper-iment was performed between July–August 2009. We cannot fully excludethat social dynamics changed over that period, although social relationshipsin corvids are generally known to be stable over time (Boeckle & Bugnyar,2012; Loretto et al., 2012).

In conclusion, our results showed that attention patterns in carrion crowswere affected by kinship, especially in males, in a way similar to attentionpatterns reported in jackdaws but not in ravens. This can probably be at-tributed to the social organisation in carrion crows in this specific populationin Northern Spain, where individuals interact consistently and repetitivelywith kin in cohesive cooperative groups that live year round in all-purposeterritories (Baglione et al., 2005) and have probably more information togain by watching non-kin when the circumstances allow. A direct compari-son with other European crow populations with a different social system isa promising follow up of this study and is likely to provide further impor-tant insights to understand attention patterns in animals living in complexsocieties.

570 Social attention in crows

Acknowledgements

We thank Shaun Atsbury for help in the field and Corina Logan and ananonymous reviewer for helpful comments on the manuscript. This studywas financially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Compet-itively, project CGL2011-27260, to V.B.

References

Addessi, E. & Visalberghi, E. (2001). Social facilitation of eating novel food in tufted ca-puchin monkeys (Cebus apella): input provided by group members and responses affectedin the observer. — Anim. Cogn. 4: 297-303.

Baglione, V., Canestrari, D. & Marcos, M. (2002a). History, environment and social be-haviour: experimentally induced cooperative breeding in the carrion crow. — Proc. Roy.Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 269: 1247-1251.

Baglione, V., Marcos, M., Canestrari, D. & Ekman, J. (2002b). Direct fitness benefits of groupliving in a complex cooperative society of carrion crows, Corvus corone corone. — Anim.Behav. 64: 887-893.

Baglione, V., Canestrari, D., Marcos, J.M. & Ekman, J. (2003). Kin selection in cooperativealliances of carrion crows. — Science 300: 1947-1949.

Baglione, V., Canestrari, D., Chiarati, E., Vera, R. & Marcos, J.M. (2010). Lazy group mem-bers are substitute helpers in carrion crows. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 277:3275-3282.

Blumstein, D.T., Verneyre, L. & Daniel, J.C. (2004). Reliability and the adaptive utility ofdiscrimination among alarm callers. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 271: 1851-1857.

Boeckle, M. & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Long-term memory for affiliates in ravens. — Curr. Biol.22: 801-806.

Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2005). Ravens, Corvus corax, differentiate between knowledge-able and ignorant competitors. — Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci. 272: 1641-1646.

Bugnyar, T. & Heinrich, B. (2006). Pilfering ravens, Corvus corax, adjust their behaviour tosocial context and identity of competitors. — Anim. Cogn. 9: 369-376.

Bugnyar, T., Kijne, M. & Kotrschal, K. (2001). Food calling in ravens: are yells referentialsignals? — Anim. Behav. 61: 949-958.

Canestrari, D., Marcos, J. & Baglione, V. (2005). Effect of parentage and relatedness on theindividual contribution to cooperative chick care in carrion crows Corvus corone corone.— Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 116: 422-428.

Chiarati, E., Canestrari, D., Vera, R., Marcos, J.M. & Baglione, V. (2010). Linear and stabledominance hierarchies in cooperative carrion crows. — Ethology 116: 346-356.

Chiarati, E., Canestrari, D., Vila, M., Vera, R. & Baglione, V. (2011). Nepotistic access tofood resources in cooperatively breeding carrion crows. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 65:1791-1800.

C.A.F. Wascher et al. / Behaviour 151 (2014) 555–572 571

Chiarati, E., Canestrari, D., Vera, R. & Baglione, V. (2012). Subordinates benefit from ex-ploratory dominants: response to novel food in cooperatively breeding carrion crows. —Anim. Behav. 83: 103-109.

Coussi-Korbel, S. & Fragaszy, D.M. (1995). On the relation between social dynamics andsocial learning. — Anim. Behav. 50: 1441-1453.

Danchin, E. & Wagner, R.H. (1997). The evolution of coloniality: the emergence of newperspectives. — Trends Ecol. Evol. 12: 342-347.

De Kort, S.R., Emery, N.J. & Clayton, N.S. (2006). Food sharing in jackdaws, Corvus mon-edula: what, why and with whom? — Anim. Behav. 72: 297-304.

Emery, N.J., Seed, A.M., Von Bayern, A.M.P. & Clayton, N.S. (2007). Cognitive adaptationsof social bonding in birds. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 362: 489-505.

Fernández-Juricic, E. & Kacelnik, A. (2004). Information transfer and gain in flocks: theeffects of quality and quantity of social information at different neighbour distances. —Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 55: 502-511.

Fraser, O.N. & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Reciprocity of agonistic support in ravens. — Anim.Behav. 83: 171-177.

Galef, B.G. & Laland, K.N. (2005). Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theo-retical models. — BioScience 55: 489-499.

Galef, B.G., Rudolf, B., Whiskin, E.E., Choleris, E., Mainardi, M. & Valsecchi, P. (1998).Familiarity and relatedness: effects on social learning about foods by Norway rats andMongolian gerbils. — Anim. Learn. Behav. 26: 448-454.

Giraldeau, L.-A., Valone, T.J. & Templeton, J.J. (2002). Potential disadvantages of usingsocially acquired information. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 357: 1559-1566.

Glutz von Blotzheim, U.N. (1985). Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas. — Aula, Wiesbaden.

Griffin, A.S. & Evans, C.S. (2003). Social learning of antipredator behaviour in a marsupial.— Anim. Behav. 66: 485-492.

Heinrich, B. (2011). Conflict, cooperation, and cognition in the common raven. — Adv. Stud.Behav. 43: 189-237.

King, A.J. & Cowlishaw, G. (2007). When to use social information: the advantage of largegroup size in individual decision making. — Biol. Lett. 3: 137-139.

Lachlan, R., Crooks, L. & Laland, K. (1998). Who follows whom? Shoaling preferences andsocial learning of foraging information in guppies. — Anim. Behav. 56: 181-190.

Laland, K.N. (2004). Social learning strategies. — Learn. Behav. 32: 4-14.

Loretto, M., Fraser, O.N. & Bugnyar, T. (2012). Ontogeny of social relations and coalitionformation in common ravens (Corvus corax). — Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 25: 180-194.

Macdonald, D.W. (1983). The ecology of carnivore social behaviour. — Nature 301: 379-384.

Melis, A.P., Hare, B. & Tomasello, M. (2006). Chimpanzees recruit the best collaborators. —Science 311: 1297-1300.

Mineka, S. & Cook, M. (1988). Social learning and the acquisition of snake fear in monkeys.— In: Social learning: psychological and biological perspectives (Zentall, T.R. & Galef,B.G.J., eds). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, p. 51-73

572 Social attention in crows

Noe, R. & Hammerstein, P. (1994). Biological markets: supply and demand determine theeffect of partner choice in cooperation, mutualism and mating. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.35: 1-11.

Pollard, K.A. (2010). Making the most of alarm signals: the adaptive value of individualdiscrimination in an alarm context. — Behav. Ecol. 22: 93-100.

Range, F. & Huber, L. (2007). Attention in common marmosets: implications for social-learning experiments. — Anim. Behav. 73: 1033-1041.

Range, F., Horn, L., Bugnyar, T., Gajdon, G.K. & Huber, L. (2009). Social attention in keas,dogs, and human children. — Anim. Cogn. 12: 181-192.

Rieucau, G. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2011). Exploring the costs and benefits of social informationuse: an appraisal of current experimental evidence. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 366:949-957.

Ryer, C.H. & Olla, B.L. (1992). Social mechanisms facilitating exploitation of spatiallyvariable ephemeral food patches in a pelagic marine fish. — Anim. Behav. 44: 69-74.

Scheid, C., Range, F. & Bugnyar, T. (2007). When, what, and whom to watch? Quantifyingattention in ravens (Corvus corax) and jackdaws (Corvus monedula). — J. Comp. Psychol.121: 1851-1857.

Schwab, C., Bugnyar, T., Schloegl, C. & Kotrschal, K. (2008a). Enhanced social learningbetween siblings in common ravens, Corvus corax. — Anim. Behav. 75: 501-508.

Schwab, C., Bugnyar, T. & Kotrschal, K. (2008b). Preferential learning from non-affiliatedindividuals in jackdaws (Corvus monedula). — Behav. Proc. 79: 148-155.

Soma, M. & Hasegawa, T. (2004). The effect of social facilitation and social dominance onforaging success of budgerigars in an unfamiliar environment. — Behaviour 141: 1121-1134.

Svensson, L. (1992). Identification guide to European Passerines. — Naturhistoriska Riks-muset, Stockholm.

Taborsky, B. & Oliveira, R.F. (2012). Social competence: an evolutionary approach. —Trends Ecol. Evol. 27: 679-688.

Valone, T.J. (2007). From eavesdropping on performance to copying the behavior of others:a review of public information use. — Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 62: 1-14.

Valone, T.J. & Templeton, J.J. (2002). Public information for the assessment of quality:a widespread social phenomenon. — Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 357: 1549-1557.

Valsecchi, P., Choleris, E., Moles, A., Guo, C. & Mainardi, M. (1996). Kinship and famil-iarity as factors affecting social transfer of food preferences in adult Mongolian gerbils(Meriones unguiculatus). — J. Comp. Psychol. 110: 243-251.

van Schaik, C.P. (1983). Why are diurnal primates living in groups? — Behaviour 87: 120-144.