Perceptions of danger: A psychological mediator of neighborhood demographic characteristics

11
American Joumol of Orthopsychiatty, 68(3). July 1998 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER: A Psychological Mediator of Neighborhood Demographic Characteristics Lee Shumow, Ph.D., Deborah Lowe Vandell, Ph.D., Jill Posner, Ph.D. ~~ Associations between neighborhood characteristics and adjustment of 168 urban Jifrh-grade children were explored. High neighborhood risk was associated with parent and teacher reports of child misconduct, and withparent and child reports of child psychological distress, while child and parent perceptions of neighbor- hood dangers appeared to mediate these associations. Specific neighborhood as- pects were also related to child adjustment: higher violent crime rates with high- er child misconduct rates, and lower neighborhood incomes with greater child psychological distress. Implicationsfor research and intervention are discussed. ryant (1985) has suggested that peo- B ple’s perceptions of their neighbor- hoods may serve as a mediating mechanism between objective features of the neighbor- hood and children’s development. This view is consistent with contextual theories of development, which posit that individu- als actively interpret and react to their sur- roundings (Bronfenbrenner, 1992; Jessor, Vanden Bos, Vanderryn, Costa, & Turbin, 1995). The purpose of the current study is to test the hypothesis that perceptions of danger in the neighborhood mediate, in part, associations between more distal de- mographic features of the neighborhood and children’s behavioral problems. Baron and Kenny (1 986) have described a way to examine such a mediational hypo- thesis. First, a relationship is demonstrated between distal factors, such as demogra- phic neighborhood risk indicators, and child adjustment. Second, the distal factor must be related to a psychological process (e.g., parents’ and children’s perceptions of their neighborhood). Third, both the distal factor (neighborhood risk) and the pro- posed mediator (neighborhood percep- tions) are used to predict the outcome vari- ables (e.g., children’s behavior problems). If the contribution of the distal factor to the outcome is reduced and the proposed medi- ator is shown to contribute significantly to the outcome, then it may be concluded that the process examined is a mediator (Baron & Kenny, 1986). This approach was used in the study re- ported here, which also controlled for child gender, race, family structure, maternal ed- Based on apaperpresentedat the 1997 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, D.C. Work MU supported by a grantfrom the Spencer Foundation to the secondand third authors. Authors are at the Department of Educational Psychology, Counseling, and Special Education, Northern Illinois Univer- sity, DeKalb (Shumow); and Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison (Vandell, Posner). 468 (0 1998 American Orthopsychiatric Association, Inc.

Transcript of Perceptions of danger: A psychological mediator of neighborhood demographic characteristics

American Joumol of Orthopsychiatty, 68(3). July 1998

PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER: A Psychological Mediator of Neighborhood

Demographic Characteristics

Lee Shumow, Ph.D., Deborah Lowe Vandell, Ph.D., Jill Posner, Ph.D.

~~

Associations between neighborhood characteristics and adjustment of 168 urban Jifrh-grade children were explored. High neighborhood risk was associated with parent and teacher reports of child misconduct, and with parent and child reports of child psychological distress, while child and parent perceptions of neighbor- hood dangers appeared to mediate these associations. Specific neighborhood as- pects were also related to child adjustment: higher violent crime rates with high- er child misconduct rates, and lower neighborhood incomes with greater child psychological distress. Implications for research and intervention are discussed.

ryant (1985) has suggested that peo- B ple’s perceptions of their neighbor- hoods may serve as a mediating mechanism between objective features of the neighbor- hood and children’s development. This view is consistent with contextual theories of development, which posit that individu- als actively interpret and react to their sur- roundings (Bronfenbrenner, 1992; Jessor, Vanden Bos, Vanderryn, Costa, & Turbin, 1995). The purpose of the current study is to test the hypothesis that perceptions of danger in the neighborhood mediate, in part, associations between more distal de- mographic features of the neighborhood and children’s behavioral problems.

Baron and Kenny (1 986) have described a way to examine such a mediational hypo- thesis. First, a relationship is demonstrated

between distal factors, such as demogra- phic neighborhood risk indicators, and child adjustment. Second, the distal factor must be related to a psychological process (e.g., parents’ and children’s perceptions of their neighborhood). Third, both the distal factor (neighborhood risk) and the pro- posed mediator (neighborhood percep- tions) are used to predict the outcome vari- ables (e.g., children’s behavior problems). If the contribution of the distal factor to the outcome is reduced and the proposed medi- ator is shown to contribute significantly to the outcome, then it may be concluded that the process examined is a mediator (Baron & Kenny, 1986).

This approach was used in the study re- ported here, which also controlled for child gender, race, family structure, maternal ed-

Based on apaperpresentedat the 1997 meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, D.C. Work MU supported by a grantfrom the Spencer Foundation to the secondand third authors. Authors are at the Department of Educational Psychology, Counseling, and Special Education, Northern Illinois Univer- sity, DeKalb (Shumow); and Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison (Vandell, Posner).

468 (0 1998 American Orthopsychiatric Association, Inc.

SHUMOW ET AL 469

ucation, and family income in all analyses. Accounting for these individual and family characteristics when investigating possible contextual effects of neighborhood was deemed important because families that live in riskier neighborhoods are likely to differ on these characteristics from those that live in better neighborhoods, and be- cause such controls help ensure that ob- served neighborhood effects are not at- tributable to family factors.

Research relating neighborhood context to delinquency during adolescence has a long history (Coulton & Pandey, 1992; Maccoby, Johnson, & Church, 1958; Shaw, 1929). Many of these studies found that neighborhood risk contributed to delin- quency, although others did not (Jencks & Mayer, 1990). A number of the studies failed, however, to control for family fac- tors that were confounded with neighbor- hood context. Fewer studies have consid- ered preadolescents, although there are in- dications that these younger children are affected by living in high-risk neighbor- hoods (Henggeler, 1991). In a set of Aus- tralian studies, neighborhood quality was found to be related to the self-reported be- havioral adjustment of 9-1 1-year-olds, con- trolling for family characteristics (Home1 & Burns, 1989). Children who resided in inner-city areas felt more lonely, worried, fearful, angry, and unhappy than children living in other areas. Children, particularly boys from low-income families, in neigh- borhoods with more social problems re- ported the greatest alienation and with- drawal.

Some previous studies have relied only on parental reports, whereas others have gathered ratings from children or teachers. These data sources are likely to have dif- ferent perspectives on behavioral adjust- ment. For example, in a study of children attending an elementary school in a moder- ately violent area of Washington, D.C., Richter and Martinez (1993) found that par- ents reported far fewer internalizing symp- toms for their children than did the children

themselves. Teachers may have a yet dif- ferent perspective on children’s behavior. The current study has extended prior re- search parameters by examining relations between neighborhood demographic fea- tures and fifth-grade urban children’s well- being from the perspective of multiple re- spondents. Both misconduct and psycho- logical distress, as reported by teachers, parents, and children, were considered, and neighborhood demographic factors were expected to be associated with children’s self-reported psychological distress and teachers’ and parents’ reports of misconduct.

Associations between the demographic features of the neighborhood and parents’ and children’s perceptions of their neigh- borhood were also examined. Little is known about how children’s perceptions of their neighborhood relate to actual condi- tions, but research on adult perceptions is extensive. Strong associations exist be- tween objectively defined neighborhood characteristics and subjective ratings by adult residents (Kasl, White, Will, & Mar- cue , 1982). Disintegration, both physical (Taylor, Shumaker, & Gottfiedon, 1985) and social (Rohe & Burby, 1988) have been linked to residents’ perceptions of safety. Families in the present study resided in ei- ther working class or ghetto neighborhoods. Their perceptions were expected to func- tion as mediators of neighborhood charac- teristics and children’s behavioral prob- lems. On the one hand, perceiving the neighborhood as unsafe might lead to anger and increase the likelihood of acting aggressively; on the other, such percep- tions could result in hypervigilance, anxi- ety, and depression.

The psychological importance of paren- tal perceptions has been highlighted in sev- eral recent studies. Adults consider neigh- borhood safety an important aspect of daily life (Caspi, Bolger, & Eckenrode, 1987; Cook, 1988). A survey conducted in Dallas (Warr, 1992) showed that adults’ concerns about their children’s safety predicted pur- chase of weapons by male heads-of-house-

470 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

hold. Findings from a longitudinal study (Kasl et al., 1982; White, Kasl, Zahner, & Will, 1987) of low-income minority moth- ers of ten-year-old children suggest that maternal perceptions of the neighborhood may link neighborhood conditions and children’s psychological adjustment: ob- jective ratings and maternal perceptions of neighborhood conditions were found to predict maternal depression and anger. Other recent research (Silverman, La Gre- ca, & Wasserstein, 1996) has suggested that children, too, worry about their per- sonal safety and that these womes are of- ten intense and related to children’s anxi- ety. Both parent and child perceptions are tested in the study reported here.

The final purpose of the present study was to delineate the components of neigh- borhood risk and determine whether spe- cific features were associated with chil- dren’s misconduct and psychological dis- tress. Prior research and theory suggest that these specific features may contribute to behavioral problems.

Children living in neighborhoods with lower overall income are likely to observe many people in difficult and pressing eco- nomic circumstances. At least one previous study has found that the proportion of low- income neighbors predicted maternal re- ports of externalizing problems in five- year-old children (Ducan, Brooks-Gunn, & Klebanov, 1994); however, because data from only one source was reported, it was unclear whether this relationship was a function of actual externalizing behavior or of maternal perceptions.

The proportion of single-parent families in the neighborhood may also be related to children’s behavioral problems. Consistent with the idea that two-parent families are able to provide more monitoring of neigh- borhood children, Brooks-Gunn, Duncan, Kato, and Sealand (1991) reported that be- havioral problems of young children were predicted by the percentage of single-par-

ent households that were in the neighbor- hood.

A third neighborhood component that may influence children’s school behavior, in particular, is the educational level of ad- ults in the neighborhood. Those who have been relatively successful in attaining an education may serve as positive role mod- els for the neighboring children.

Recent studies report community vio- lence as a neighborhood characteristic spe- cifically related to children’s behavioral ad- justment (Garbarino, Dubrow, Kostelny, & Pardo, 1992). Both case studies and empiri- cal research describe possible psychologi- cal ramifications, such as depression and behavioral problems, for those children who are exposed to neighborhood crime and violence (Garbarino et al., 1992; Kot- lowitz, 1991; Martinez & Richter, 1993; Osofiky, Wewers, Hann,& Fick, 1993).

In sum, perceptions of safety were hy- pothesized as a proximal psychological ex- perience mediating associations between demographic characteristics of urban neigh- borhoods and children’s adjustment. The study reported here: I) examined relations between neighborhood demographic fac- tors and child adjustment, as rated by teach- ers, parents, and children; 2) examined re- lations between neighborhood characteris- tics and parents’ and children’s perceptions of safety, testing the perceptions as a psy- chological mediator between neighbor- hood demographics and child outcomes; and 3) dismantled the neighborhood com- posite variable so as to determine its spe- cific characteristics (median educational level, percent of single parent households, median household income, and rates of vi- olent crimes) and test their association with child adjustment.

METHOD Sample

Families were originally selected* for a longitudinal study of childrens develop-

*Details regarding the selection procedure and attrition have been reported elsewhere (Posner & Vundell, 1994).

SHUMOW ET AL 47 1

ment when the children were in third grade and were an average age of 9.1 years ( S B S). The original sample of 216 children at- tended nine Milwaukee public schools. Average maternal education was 12.8 years (SB2). Wilcoxon sign rank tests comparing study participants to other chil- dren in these schools showed no differ- ences in demographic characteristics, school transfer during the year, or reading achieve- ment scores. More than 98% of the families had a family income below the median in the predominantly working-class city of Milwaukee.

The current study focused on the chil- dren when they were in fifth grade, since the neighborhood perceptions question- naire was only administered during the fifth-grade assessment. Of the 194 children in this data set, complete neighborhood, parent, teacher, and child reports were available for 168, and these formed the sample for the current analyses. Of these 168 children, 49% were male, 46% were African-American, and 49% had single parents. Because of participants’ consider- able geographic mobility, the children at- tended over 40 different schools during the course of the study, and since some fami- lies used post-office boxes, their residences could not always be reliably ascertained. Chi-square and t-tests indicated that there were no significant differences between the 168 children in the current analyses and the other 48 children from the original sample of 2 16 on gender, race, family structure, in- come, neighborhood risk index, maternal education, or reported behavioral problems during third grade.

Neighborhood Characteristics Neighborhoods were defined by census

tracts. Use of this definition has several ad- vantages. I ) Previous studies (Chase-Lam- dale & Gordon, 1996; Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Duncan, Brook-Gunn, & Kle- banov, 1994) have operationalized neighbor- hoods in this way, thus allowing compari- son between studies. 2) Census boundaries

are drawn by local committees charged with identifying locally perceived “neighbor- hoods,” so that using census tracts to ap- proximate neighborhoods has face validity (Hojnacki, 1979). 3) Census tracts common- ly cover from six- to nine-square-block ar- eas in inner city areas of Milwaukee and rarely cross major geographic (rivers) or man-made (street thoroughfares) barriers; thus, they approximated areas that children are likely to think of and experience as their neighborhood (Mathews, 1986).

Children’s fifth-grade addresses, ob- tained during the fall of the 1993 school year, were keyed to census tract. The neighborhoods ranged from working-class to ghetto areas. Information obtained from the 1990 census for each of the 93 tracts in which the children lived was: a) proportion of female-headed households (W.24, S B .18); b) median education for adults (W 11.9 years, S B . 9 ) ; and c) median house- hold income (W$22,530, SB$8,484). In addition, reports of homicide, rape, aggra- vated battery, other battery, and vice crimes were obtained from the Milwaukee Police Department on each tract for the cal- endar year 1993 ending in December of the children’s fifth-grade school year (M= 108.6, SB89.7). Principal-component anal- ysis yielded a single factor, neighborhood risk, that accounted for 76% of the vari- ance. Each child was assigned the principal component score corresponding to their fifth-grade address.

Procedure The person in each family who claimed

primary parenting responsibilities was asked to complete and return question- naires in postage-paid envelopes and to participate in a phone interview. When families did not return the questionnaires or experienced reading difficulties, infor- mation was obtained from interviews. In most cases, questionnaires were completed by the children’s mothers, although oth- ers-five grandmothers, two aunts, seven fathers, and one foster mother-who were

472 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

primary caregivers provided information for 15 of the children. Teachers were asked to complete questionnaires describing the participating children’s adjustment. Mea- sures were administered to small groups of 2-3 children at school. Items were read aloud and children marked their responses.

Measures Parent reports of child misconduct and

distress. Parents completed the 28-item Be- havior Problems Index @ill, 1990), rating behavior manifested by the children during the previous three months (O=never, 1= sometimes or often). The antisocial sub- scale of the index measures children’s mis- conduct on six items (B.27 , SD=.28), such as lying, cruelty, and destructiveness (Cron- bach’s alpha=.70). The anxiety subscale measures their psychological distress on six items (B .38 , SD=.29), such as as ner- vousness, feelings of worthlessness, and sadness (Cronbach’s alpha=.73).

Teacher reports of child misconduct and distress. Teachers completed a behavioral gdjustment rating scale that includes sub- scales on misconduct and distress at school (Santrock & Warshak; 1979). The five mis- conduct items include “This child threatens other children” and “This child hits other children.” The psychological distress scale contained five items, such as “This child is fearful of new things” and “This child is unhappy.” Possible responses range from l=never to 5=always (for misconduct: B 2.02, S B . 8 0 ; for distress: M=2.29; S B .47). Cronbach’s alpha was .92 for the mis- conduct scale and .57 for the distress scale, suggesting the teacher report of distress may not be reliable.

Child report. Using a measure developed by Brown and Clasen (1985), children in- dicated how often in the past month (1 = never to 5=almost every day) they had en- gaged in any of ten types of misconduct, including destroying property, stealing, and fighting (W1.54; SB.48; Cronbach’s alpha=.77). They completed the Reynolds Depression Scale (Reynolds, 1989) to re-

port psychological distress. This 30-item measure (M=1.59, SD.38; Cronbach’s al- pha=.90.) uses a four-point response scale (l=almost never to 4=all the time). Sample items include “I feel sad,” “I feel worried,” and “I feel I am no good.”

Perceptions of Neighborhood Danger A six-item scale assessing children’s

perceptions of neighborhood danger was created for the present study. The items, derived from qualitative reports of low- income urban children’s daily lives, were: “Drug dealers are a problem in my neigh- borhood,” “It is safe in my neighborhood” (reversed), “I am scared of gangs in my neighborhood,” “There are people in my neighborhood who might hurt me,” “I feel safe playing outside at my house” (re- versed), and “I worry about people with guns and knives in my neighborhood.” Five-point ratings ranged from 1 =totally true to 5=totally false (W2.34; SD1.08; Cronbach’s alpha=.82).

An eight-item scale, also created for this study, measured ’ parent perceptions of neighborhood danger. Six of the items cor- respond to those on the children’s scale. The two additional items assess the dangers of walking to the nearest store and of play- ing at the nearest playground. Ratings range from l=totally true to 5=totally false (W2.49, SB.76; Cronbach’s alpha=.84).

RESULTS First, the neighborhood demographic

composite score was used to predict child misconduct and psychological distress as reported by children, parents, and teachers, controlling for individual family character- istics. Second, children’s and parents’ per- ceptions of neighborhood danger were tested as mediators of neighborhood demo- graphic characteristics and children’s be- havior problems. Finally, specific neighbor- hood characteristics were examined as po- tential predictors of behavioral problems.

To test the hypothesis that neighborhood perceptions are psychological experiences

SHUMOW ET AL 473

that mediate relationships between demo- graphic measures of the neighborhood and children's misconduct or psychological distress, statistical procedures described by Baron and Kenny (1986) were followed. Three sets of equations were examined in a series of multiple regressions. First, teacher, parent, and child ratings of chil- dren's behavioral problems were regressed on the neighborhood demographic com- posite score while controlling for individ- ual family characteristics, in order to deter- mine whether the neighborhood factor pre- dicted children's psychological distress or misconduct. Second, child and parent per- ceptions of neighborhood danger were re- gressed on the family and neighborhood demographic composite in order to deter- mine whether the neighborhood demo- graphic factor predicted perceptions by children or parents of neighborhood dan- ger. Finally, reports of child adjustment were regressed on the full model, including neigh- borhood demographic and perception mea- sures, in order to determine whether the sta- tistical significance of the neighborhood de- mographic features was reduced by the in- clusion of neighborhood perceptions. Ac- cording to Baron and Kenny (1986), it is necessary to obtain all three conditions to infer mediation.

Preliminaty Analyses TABLE 1 displays Pearson correlations

between teacher, parent, and child reports of child psychological distress and miscon- duct. As can be seen, there was moderate agreement between respondents regarding child misconduct. Teachers discriminated between misconduct and distress, as indi- cated by the negative correlation between both reports. Parent reports of misconduct were related to their reports of distress, as were the child reports. Teacher reports of distress had small significant correlations with parent reports, but were not related to child reports. Parent reports of distress were modestly but significantly related to children's reported distress.

Table 1 RATINGS OF CHILDREN'S PSYCHOLOGICAL

DISTRESS AND MISCONDUCT: PEARSON CORRELATIONS

DISTRESS MISCONDUCT RESPOND. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Distress

1.T- - 0.26" 0.14 -0.21" 0.05 -0.08 2. Parant - 0.18. 0.03 0.51- 0.21" 3. ChiM - 0.03 0.18 0.33"

4. Teachar - 0.35- 0.33" 5. Parent - 0.37 6. ChiM -

Misconduct

*p.05; "pc.01; "p.001; -p.oOOl.

Neighborhood Demographics and Children's Adjustment

To establish an association between neigh- borhood demographic characteristics and child adjustment, as required in the media- tional model, the measures of children's behavioral problems were regressed on the neighborhood demographic principal com- ponent score and childfamily controls (child gender, race, family income, family struc- ture, and parent's educational level). As TABU 2 shows, children's psychological dis- tress, as reported by parents, children, and teachers, was predicted by the regression model, which included the control vari- ables and the neighborhood demographic composite score. Inspection of individual betas in the regression equations revealed that the neighborhood composite was posi- tively associated with children's psycho- logical distress, as reported by parents w.06) and children (p<.05), over and above the contribution of individual family predic- tors. Misconduct also was predicted by the model. Inspection of individual betas indi- cates that the neighborhood demographic composite score was associated with parent @<.05), teacher @<.09), and child (~6.05) reports of misconduct, even after control- ling for individual family variables.

Neighborhood Demographics and Perceptions of Danger

The next analysis tested whether the neighborhood demographic composite score

474 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

sions are summarized in TABLE 4. Both par- ent and child reports of psychological dis- Table 2

SIMULTANEOUS MULTIPLE REGRESSIONS tress were predicted by the full model. In- PREDICTING CHILDREN'S DISTRESS AND

MlSCONDUCT FROM NEIGHBORHOOD RISK spection of the betas for individual predic- tors revealed that the neighborhood demo-

PREDICTORS Psychological Distress

Gender' Rawb Income Family Structurec Matemal Educationd Neighborhood Risk R' Adjusted R'

Misconduct Gender' Raceb Income Family Structurec Matemal Educationd Neighborhood Risk

REPORT TEACHER PARENT CHILD

-0.14 0.18 0.07

-0.12 -0.16 0.17 0.08'. 0.05"

-0.02 0.23" 0.01

-0.22" -0.07 0.20' 0.08" 0.05"

-0.14- 0.17 0.01

-0.14 -0.02 0.22- 0.08- 0.04"

0.25"" 0.15- 0.26-' -0.14 0.04 0.03 -0.02 -0.08 0.02 -0.10 -0.25" -0.09 -0.11 -0.03 -0.13 0.17' 0.24" 0.05

R' 0.20- 0.20- 0.09- Adjusted @ 0.17- 0.17"- 0.06

Note. Standardized betas are entered in the table. *Gender: l=male. O-%mak. bRace: 1 =Caucasian, O=African-Amrican. CFamily structure: 1 =two-parent, O=sing)e-parent. dMatemal educaticm scale: l=chigh school, 2=high school, 3=postsecondary, 4tBachelor degree, 5=some gradu- ate study, 6=graduate degree. 'w.10: "w.05: "w.01; -w.OOI.

predicted parents' and children's percep- tions of danger in the neighborhood, using regression equations that included the con- trol variables. As can be seen in TABLE 3, the neighborhood demographic composite predicted both parent (8=.62, p<.OOl) and child (8=.57, p<.OOl) perceptions of dan- ger in the neighborhood, as hypothesized.

Perceptiom as Mediators of Neighborhood Demographic Characteritics

The fmal step in establishing mediation involved regressing the child distress (teach- er, parent, and child ratings) and miscon- duct (teacher and parent ratings) measures on the full model composed of the neigh- borhood demographic composite score, fam- ilykhild control variables (child gender, race, family income, single parent status, and maternal education), and both parent and child perceptions of neighborhood danger (Pearson r=.48 between parent and child rating). The results of these regres-

- graphic composite was no longer a signifi- cant predictor of psychological distress af- ter the respondents' perceptions of the neigh- borhood were introduced into the model. Parental perception of danger emerged as a mediator of neighborhood demographic characteristics for parent-reports of child psychological distress and misconduct. Children's perceptions of danger mediated child-reported distress.

Neighborhood Risk and Behavioral Functioning

Simultaneous multiple regression equa- tions were estimated in order to examine whether specific elements of the neighbor- hood were differentially associated with children's distress or misconduct. These re- gressions included the same individual familykhild controls as the previous mod- els (i.e., child gender, race, family income, family structure, maternal education). Indi- vidual demographic components (median adult education, median household in- come, percent female-headed household, and violent crime rate) were substituted for

Table 3 ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN

NEIGHBORHOOD COMPOSITE AND PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

PREDICTOR Gender' Raceb Family Income Family Structurec Matemal Educationd Neighborhood Risk

R' Adiusted @

PERCEPTIONS PARENT CHlL,D

-0.12 -0.05 -0.10 -0.03 -0.03 -0.02 0.06 -0.01

-0.01 0.10 0.62' 0.57' 0.34' 0.33' 0.31' 0.30'

Note. Standardized betas are entered in the table. *Gender: l=male, O=female. bRace: I =Caucasian, O=AfricanAmerican. CFamily structure: l=two-parent, O=single-parent. dMatemal education scale: l=-=high school, 2=high school. 3=postsecondary, 4=Bachelor degree, 5=some graduate study, 6=graduate degree. * ~ . O O O l .

SHUMOW ET AL 475

Table 4 PERCEPTIONS AS MEDIATORS

OF NEIGHBORHOOD RISK AND BEHAVIOR PROBLEMS

PREDICTORS Psychological Distress

GendeP Raceb Income Family structurec Maternal educationd Neighborhood risk Parent perception Child perception Rz Adjusted

Misconduct Gender. Raceb Income Family structurec Maternal educationd Neighborhood risk Parent perception Child perception @ Adiusted @

REPORTS TEACHER PARENT CHILD

-0.13 0.20 0.06

-0.11 -0.14 0.15

-0.04 0.04 0.07 0.02

0.25"' -0.14 -0.03 -0.10 -0.10 0.16 0.04

-0.02 0.19=*** 0.15=-*

0.02 0.20' 0.02

-0.24" 0.07 0.04 0.27"

-0.02 0.13" 0.09"

0.18' 0.01

-0.07 -0.26** -0.03 0.10 0.25"

-0.02 0.25'- 0.21.'"'

-0.12 0.18 0.03

-0.19. -0.03 -0.06 0.04 0.46H*' 0.23"" 0.19""

0.26." 0.03 0.02

-0.11 -0.14 -0.05 0.04 0.16 0.11. 0.06

Note. Standardized betas are entered in the table. .Gender 1 =male, O=female. bRace: 1 =Caucasian, O=AfricanAmerican. CFamily structure: 1 =two-parent. O=single-parent. dMatemal education scale: l=<hQh school, P=high school, 3=postsecondary, 4=Bachelor degree, 5=some gradu- ate, 6=graduate degree. 'pc.05; "p.01; "'pc.001; '~'pc.0001.

the demographic composite score. TABLE 5 displays the standardized betas associated with each of these individual neighborhood components. As can be seen, different as- pects of the neighborhood were associated

with distress and misconduct. Crime rates predicted parent and child reports of mis- conduct; median household incomes pre- dicted parent and teacher reports of child psychological distress and parent reports of misconduct; proportion of female-headed households predicted child-reported dis- tress; median adult education predicted re- ports by teachers, but not by parents or children, of misconduct.

DISCUSSION The present study examined relation-

ships between a composite index of neigh- borhood risk and two types of behavioral problems among urban children. This neigh- borhood measure was found to predict chil- dren's misconduct and psychological dis- tress, even when individual family factors were controlled. Both parent and teacher reports of misconduct were associated with neighborhood demographic characteristics, as were parent and child reports of psycho- logical distress. It should be noted that a full range of neighborhoods was not as- sessed. Rather, these associations were found within a fairly restricted range of ur- ban neighborhoods, from impoverished to working class. It may be that danger is es- pecially salient psychologically in such neighborhoods.

Consistent with earlier findings concern- ing adolescents (Jencks & Mayer, 1990)

Table 5 NEIGHBORHOOD COMPONENTS PREDICTING DISTRESS AND MISCONDUCT

COMPONENT Psychological Distress

Median adult education Median household income Percent female-headed households Crimes against people

Median adult education Median household income Percent female-headed households Crimes against DeODle

Misconduct

TEACHER B @

-0.15 0.05" -0.20**' 0.05" 0.16 0.04"

-0.02 0.02

-0.1 7" 0.17"" -0.13 0.16'*** 0.11 0.16"" 0.04 0.17""

REPORT PARENT

B @ -0.08 0.03 -0.23" 0.05" 0.12 0.03 0.15 0.04.'

-0.11 0.15"" -0.24"' 0.17"" 0.20 0.16*"' 0.18" 0.17""

CHILD

B @ -0.04 0.02 -0.14 0.03 0.28'. 0.05" 0.16 0.03

0.02 0.05..

-0.01 0.05" -0.01 0.05"

0.17. 0.08"' ~~

Note: Standardized betas entered in table from equations: control variables, gender, race, income. family s t r u i i i and maternal education. 'pc.1; * * p . o 5 ; ***p.oi; ****pc.oooi.

476 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

and preschoolers (Duncan et al., 1994)’ parent and teacher reports of misconduct were associated with neighborhood charac- teristics. Living in neighborhoods with higher rates of violent crime, lower in- comes, lower education levels, and more female-headed households was associated with higher rates of child misconduct. The fact that both parents and teachers reported heightened misconduct suggests that the differences are not simply an artifact of parental stress. There was also converging evidence regarding negative effects of poor quality neighborhoods on children’s psy- chological distress. Both parent and child reports of child distress were higher in riskier neighborhoods. Teacher reports of children’s psychological distress, however, were not associated with neighborhood characteristics. It may be that teachers are less aware of children’s distress because it is “quieter” than acting out in the class- room, and the latter distracts them. The low Cronbach’s alpha obtained on teacher re- ports of psychological distress may also be a factor.

Present results showed that perceptions of neighborhood danger were associated with actual conditions for both children and adults. The association between adult perceptions and independent ratings of neighborhood quality has been well estab- lished in the literature (Karl et ul., 1982; Rohe & Burby, 1988; Taylor, Shumakr, & Gotrfehon, 1985). Much less research, however, has focused on children’s percep- tions of their neighborhoods. In this study, children’s as well as parents’ perceptions of danger matched independent demo- graphic indicators of the neighborhoods.

Parental perceptions of neighborhood danger were found to mediate associations between neighborhood demographic char- acteristics and parent reports of child mis- conduct and psychological distress. The re- lationship between demographic character- istics and child-reported distress also ap- peared to be mediated by the children’s perception of neighborhood danger. Nei-

ther parents’ nor children’s fears mediated teacher ratings of misconduct. Interest- ingly, not only were perceptions of danger found to be psychological processes link- ing neighborhood demographics to chil- dren’s mental health, but the variance ac- counted for by the model (including per- ceptions) increased substantially compared to the demographic model, especially in the case of children’s self-reports of psy- chological distress. Thus, these perceptions appear to be important factors in under- standing children’s functioning.

Specific aspects of the neighborhoods that predicted the children’s behavioral problems depended on the source of the ratings. Parent reports of child misconduct were associated with police statistics of criminal activity against persons in the im- mediate area. Duncan, Brooks-Gunn, and Klebanov (1994) suggested that parents in high-risk areas may over-report children’s behavioral problems. The present finding that parent perceptions of danger in the neighborhood mediated their reports of both misconduct and distress suggests some heightened vigilance in monitoring children’s behavioral functioning in such areas. Although children’s misconduct re- ports were not predicted by global neigh- borhood risk, they were marginally associ- ated with the frequency of violent crimes in the neighborhood. This suggests that chil- dren react to their everyday experience by acting out, either in imitation of criminal behavior or in an attempt to protect them- selves from the deviant ecology (Sampson, 1993). Interestingly, the proportion of fe- male-headed households, which is some- times used as a proxy for less available adult monitoring, did not predict any of the three ratings of children’s misconduct. The proportion of female-headed households in the neighborhood was, however, found to relate to children’s depression, suggesting that less adult supervision and support in- fluenced the children.

These findings have implications for pre- vention and intervention programs. First,

SHUMOW ET AL 477

the children manifesting behavioral prob- lems were preadolescent, suggesting that programs need to reach them at an early age. Some evidence suggests that being home alone after school exacerbates beha- vioral problems (Vandel, Posner, Shumow, & Kang, I995), and that high quality after- school programs are protective (Vandell, Shumow, & Posner, 1997). A promising av- enue for prevention programs is the con- nection of children in high-risk areas with formal ongoing support systems (Bryant, 1985). Children in the some of the poorest neighborhoods typically have fewer people on whom they can depend on for help, and children from the lowest socioeconomic strata have only half as many people avail- able for support as do middle-income chil- dren (Garbarino, Burston, Rober, Russell, & Crouter; 1978). Trained youth workers and high-quality after-school programs could serve important supportive functions for these children.

Second, parents’ sensitivity to children’s behavioral problems indicates that they can be recruited as allies in the effort to prevent problems from developing or worsening. The present study does not address the manner in which parents manage chil- dren’s activities in situations of neighbor- hood risk and community crime. Other re- cent research suggests that parenting prac- tices are influenced by neighborhood con- text. Furstenberg (1993)’ for instance, noted that the more resourceful parents in high-risk neighborhoods sought out safe havens for their children. Whether provid- ing developmental opportunities in safe places will contribute to children’s behav- ioral competence is a topic for future re- search.

Third, the finding that perceptions of danger served as psychological mediators of behavioral problems, suggests that ef- forts to increase neighborhood safety may be critically important. These perceptions may serve an important adaptive function in eliciting self-protective strategies, prompt- ing both parents and children to take ad-

vantage of educational personal safety pro- grams. Future research needs to address whether such measures are effective deter- rents to perceptions of vulnerability to dan- ger and to development of behavioral prob- lems. Rohe and Burby (1988) found ade- quacy of safety precautions to be strongly related to decreased fear in urban housing projects.

In summary, a neighborhood risk com- posite predicted children’s misconduct re- ported by teachers and parents and chil- dren’s psychological distress reported by parents and children. These relations ap- peared to be mediated by parental and child perceptions of the danger present in the neighborhood. The findings underscore the importance of the community context in understanding and mitigating children’s behavioral problems.

REFERENCES Baron, R., & Kenny, D. (1986). The moderator-

mediator variable distinction in social psychologi- cal research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1992). Ecological systems the- ory. In R. Vasta (Ed.), Six theories of child devel- opment (pp. 187-249). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Brooks-Gunn, J., Duncan, G., Kato, P., & Sealand, N. (1991, April). Do neighborhoods influence child and adolescent behavior? Paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development bien- nial meeting, Seattle.

Brown, B., & Clasen, D. (1985). Self-Reported Be- havior Index. Unpublished manuscript. Spencer Peer Group Study, Department of Educational Psychology, University of Wisconsin, Madison.

Bryant, B. (1985). The neighborhood walk. Mono- graphs of the Society for research in child devel- opment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Caspi, A,, Bolger, N., & Eckenrode, J. (1987). Link- ing person and context in the daily stress process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 184-195.

Chase-Lansdale, P.L., & Gordon, R. (1996). Eco- nomic hardship and development of five- and six- year-olds: Neighborhood and regional perspec- tives. Child Development, 67, 3338-3385.

Cook, C. (1988). Components of neighborhood satis- faction: Responses from urban and suburban single-parent women. Environment and Behavior,

Connell, J., Spencer, M., & Aber, L. (1994). Educa- tional risk and resilience in African-American

psycho lo^, 51, 1173-1 182.

20, 115-149.

478 PERCEPTIONS OF DANGER

youth: Context, self, action and outcomes in school. Child Development, 65,493-506.

Coulton, C., & Pandey, S. (1992). Geographic con- centration of poverty and risk to children in urban neighborhoods. American Behavioral Scientist, 35,238-257.

Duncan, G., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Klebanov, P. (1994). Economic deprivation and early childhood devel- opment. Child Development, 65, 296-3 18.

Furstenberg, F. (1993). How families manage risk and o p p o ~ i t y in dangerous neighborhoods. In W.J. Wilson (Ed.), Sociology and the public agenda @p. 231-258). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Garbarino, J., Dubrow, N., Kostelny, K., & Pardo, C. (1992). Children in danger: Coping with the con- sequences of community violence. San Francisco:

Garbarino, J., Burston, N., Rober, S., Russell, R., & Crouter. A. (1978). Social maps of children ap- proaching adolescence: Studying the ecology of youth development. Journal of Youth and Adoles- cence, 7,417428.

Henggeler, S . (1991). Multidimensional causal mod- els of delinquent behavior and their implications for treatment. In A. Siege1 & R. Cohen (Eds.), Context and development (pp. 21 1-231). Hills- dale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Hojnacki, W. (1979). What is a neighborhood?. So- cial Policy, I0,47-52.

Homel, R., & Bums, A. (1989). Environmental qual- ity and the well-being of children. Social Indica- tors Research, 21, 133-158.

Jencks, C., & Mayer, S. (1990). The social conse- quences of growing up in a poor neighborhood. In L. Lynn & M McGeary-(Eds.), Inner-city poverty in the United States. Washington DC: National Academy Press.

Jessor, R., Vanden Bos, J., Vandenyn, J., Costa, F., & Turbin, M.S. (1995). Protective factors in ado- lescent problem behaviors: Moderator effects and development. Developmental Psychology, 31,

Kasl, S., White, M., Will, J., & Marcuse, P. (1982). Quality of the residential environment and mental health. In A. Baum & J. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology (pp. 1-30). Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

Kotlowitz, A. (1991). There are no children here. New York: Doubleday.

Maccoby, E., Johnson, J., & Church, R. (1958.) Com- munity integration and social control of juvenile delinquency. Journal of Social Issues, 14,38-5 1.

Martinez, P., & Richter, J. (1993). The NIMH com- munity violence project: Children’s distress symp- toms associated with violence exposure. Psychia-

Matthews, M. (1986). The influence of gender on the

JOSS~Y-B~SS.

923-933.

try, 56,22-35.

environmental cognition of young boys and girls. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 147,295-302.

Osofsky, J., Wewers, S., Hann, D., & Fick, A. (1993). Chronic community violence: What is happening to our children? Psychiatry, 56,3645.

Posner, J.K., & Vandell, D.L. (1994). Low-income children’s after-school care: Are there beneficial effects of after-school programs? Child Develop- ment, 65,440456.

Reynolds, W.M. (1989). ReynolcFF Child Depression Scale. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Re- sources.

Richter, J., & Martinez P. (1993). The NIMH commu- nity violence project: Children as victims of and witnesses to violence. Psychiatry, 56, 7-21,

Rohe, W., & Burby, R. (1988). Fear of crime in public housing. Environment and Behavior, 20, 700-720.

Sampson, R. (1993). The community context of vio- lent crime. In W. J. Wilson (Ed.), Sociology and the public agenda (pp. 259-286). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Santrock, J., & Warshak, R. (1979). Father custody and social development in boys and girls. Journal ofSocialIssues, 35, 112-125.

Shaw, C. (1929). Delinquency areas. Chicago: Uni- versity of Chicago Press.

Silverman, W., La Greca, A., & Wasserstein, S. (1996). What do children worry about? Worries and their relation to anxiety. Child Development, 66,671-686.

Taylor, R., Shumaker, S., & Gottfredson, S. (1985). Neighborhood-level links between physical fea- tures and local sentiments: Deterioration, fear of crime, and confidence. Journal of Architectural Planning Research, 2,261-275.

Vandell, D.L., Posner, J., Shumow, L., & Kang, K.S. (1995, March). Concurrent, short- term, and long- term effects of self-care. Paper presented at bien- nial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN

Vandell, D.L., Shumow, L., & Posner, J. (1997). Children’s after-school programs: Promoting re- siliency or vulnerability? In H. McCubbin, A. Tompson, J. Futrell, & L. McCubbin (Eds), Pro- moting resiliency in families and children at risk: Interdisciplinary perspectives. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Wan, M. (1992). Altruistic fear of victimization in households. Social Sciences Research Quarterly,

White, M., Kasl, S., Zahner, G., & Will, J. (1987). Perceived crime in the neighborhood and mental health of women and children. Environment and Behavior, 19,588-613.

Zill, N. (1990). Behavior problems index based on parent report. Unpublished document. Washing- ton DC: Child Trends, Inc.

73,723-736.

For reprints: Lee Shumow, Ph.D.. Deparhnent of EPCSE, Northern Illinois University, DeKalb, IL 601 15