Frontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complex level parallels spatial memory...

12
Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complex level parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Behav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043 ARTICLE IN PRESS G Model BBR 9565 1–12 Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Brain Research jou rn al hom epage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr Research report Frontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complex level parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze Bharanidharan Shanmugasundaram a Q1 , Ajinkya Sase a , András G. Miklosi a , Fernando J. Sialana a,d , Saraswathi Subramaniyan a , Yogesh D. Aher a , Marion Gröger b , Harald Höger c , Keiryn L. Bennett d , Gert Lubec a,a Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of Vienna, Währinger Gürtel 18, 1090 Vienna, Austria b Core Facility, Medical University of Vienna, Lazarettegasse 14, A-1090 Vienna, Austria c Core Unit of Biomedical Research, Division of Laboratory Animal Science and Genetics, Medical University of Vienna, Brauhausgasse 34, A-2325 Himberg, Austria d CeMM Research Center for Molecular Medicine of the Austrian Academy of Science, Lazarettgasse 14, AKH BT 25.3, A-1090 Vienna, Austria h i g h l i g h t s GluA1, GluA2 & GluN2A are modulated in frontal cortex in spatial memory training. GluN2B & DAT-ph (Thr 53 ) of hippocampus are involved in spatial memory training. Whereas, GluN1, D1 & nAChR-7 are modulated both in hippocampus and frontal cortex. D1 and GluN1 receptors can form complex. a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 10 April 2015 Received in revised form 21 April 2015 Accepted 23 April 2015 Available online xxx Keywords: Radial arm maze Neurotransmitter receptor complex Blue native PAGE Spatial memory D1-GluN1 complex a b s t r a c t Several neurotransmitter receptors have been proposed to be involved in memory formation. However, information on receptor complexes (RCs) in the radial arm maze (RAM) is missing. It was therefore the aim of this study to determine major neurotransmitter RCs levels that are modulated by RAM training because receptors are known to work in homo-or heteromeric assemblies. Immediate early gene Arc expression was determined by immunohistochemistry to show if prefrontal cortices (PFC) and hippocampi were activated following RAM training as these regions are known to be mainly implicated in spatial memory. Twelve rats per group, trained and untrained in the twelve arm RAM were used, frontal cortices and hippocampi were taken, RCs in membrane protein were quantified by blue-native PAGE immunoblot- ting. RCs components were characterised by co-immunoprecipitation followed by mass spectrometrical analysis and by the use of the proximity ligation assay. Arc expression was significantly higher in PFC of trained as compared to untrained rats whereas it was comparable in hippocampi. Frontal cortical levels of RCs containing AMPA receptors GluA1, GluA2, NMDA receptors GluN1 and GluN2A, dopamine receptor D1, acetylcholine nicotinic receptor alpha 7 (nAChR-7) and hippocampal levels of RCs containing D1, GluN1, GluN2B and nAChR-7 were increased in the trained group; phosphorylated dopamine trans- porter levels were decreased in the trained group. D1 and GluN1 receptors were shown to be in the same complex. Taken together, distinct RCs were paralleling performance in the RAM which is relevant for interpretation of previous and design of future work on RCs in memory studies. © 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. Abbreviations: AMPA, -amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid; NMDA, N-methyl-D-aspartic acid; PAGE, Polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis. Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 1 40400 3215; fax: +43 1 40400 6065. E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Lubec). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043 0166-4328/© 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34

Transcript of Frontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complex level parallels spatial memory...

B

R

Fl

BQ1

FHa

b

c

Ad

h

••••

a

ARRAA

KRNBSD

h0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

33

34

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBR 9565 1–12

Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research

jou rn al hom epage: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /bbr

esearch report

rontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze

haranidharan Shanmugasundarama, Ajinkya Sasea, András G. Miklosia,ernando J. Sialanaa,d, Saraswathi Subramaniyana, Yogesh D. Ahera, Marion Grögerb,arald Högerc, Keiryn L. Bennettd, Gert Lubeca,∗

Department of Pediatrics, Medical University of Vienna, Währinger Gürtel 18, 1090 Vienna, AustriaCore Facility, Medical University of Vienna, Lazarettegasse 14, A-1090 Vienna, AustriaCore Unit of Biomedical Research, Division of Laboratory Animal Science and Genetics, Medical University of Vienna, Brauhausgasse 34, A-2325 Himberg,ustriaCeMM Research Center for Molecular Medicine of the Austrian Academy of Science, Lazarettgasse 14, AKH BT 25.3, A-1090 Vienna, Austria

i g h l i g h t s

GluA1, GluA2 & GluN2A are modulated in frontal cortex in spatial memory training.GluN2B & DAT-ph (Thr53) of hippocampus are involved in spatial memory training.Whereas, GluN1, D1 & nAChR-�7 are modulated both in hippocampus and frontal cortex.D1 and GluN1 receptors can form complex.

r t i c l e i n f o

rticle history:eceived 10 April 2015eceived in revised form 21 April 2015ccepted 23 April 2015vailable online xxx

eywords:adial arm mazeeurotransmitter receptor complexlue native PAGEpatial memory1-GluN1 complex

a b s t r a c t

Several neurotransmitter receptors have been proposed to be involved in memory formation. However,information on receptor complexes (RCs) in the radial arm maze (RAM) is missing. It was therefore the aimof this study to determine major neurotransmitter RCs levels that are modulated by RAM training becausereceptors are known to work in homo-or heteromeric assemblies. Immediate early gene Arc expressionwas determined by immunohistochemistry to show if prefrontal cortices (PFC) and hippocampi wereactivated following RAM training as these regions are known to be mainly implicated in spatial memory.Twelve rats per group, trained and untrained in the twelve arm RAM were used, frontal cortices andhippocampi were taken, RCs in membrane protein were quantified by blue-native PAGE immunoblot-ting. RCs components were characterised by co-immunoprecipitation followed by mass spectrometricalanalysis and by the use of the proximity ligation assay. Arc expression was significantly higher in PFC oftrained as compared to untrained rats whereas it was comparable in hippocampi. Frontal cortical levelsof RCs containing AMPA receptors GluA1, GluA2, NMDA receptors GluN1 and GluN2A, dopamine receptor

D1, acetylcholine nicotinic receptor alpha 7 (nAChR-�7) and hippocampal levels of RCs containing D1,GluN1, GluN2B and nAChR-�7 were increased in the trained group; phosphorylated dopamine trans-porter levels were decreased in the trained group. D1 and GluN1 receptors were shown to be in the samecomplex. Taken together, distinct RCs were paralleling performance in the RAM which is relevant forinterpretation of previous and design of future work on RCs in memory studies.

© 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V.

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

Abbreviations: AMPA, �-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid; NMD∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +43 1 40400 3215; fax: +43 1 40400 6065.

E-mail address: [email protected] (G. Lubec).

ttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043166-4328/© 2015 Published by Elsevier B.V.

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

A, N-methyl-D-aspartic acid; PAGE, Polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis.

ING ModelB

2 ioural

1Q2

iaailf(sofaeilec

uaos[ttatpNfiFmt[bihmnnpDdt

siufedhmAtptp

iuqc

35

36

37

38

39

40

41

42

43

44

45

46

47

48

49

50

51

52

53

54

55

56

57

58

59

60

61

62

63

64

65

66

67

68

69

70

71

72

73

74

75

76

77

78

79

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

91

92

93

94

95

96

97

98

99

100

101

102

103

104

105

106

107

108

109

110

111

112

113

114

115

116

117

118

119

120

121

122

123

124

125

126

127

128

129

130

131

132

133

134

135

136

137

138

139

140

141

142

143

144

145

146

147

148

149

150

151

152

153

154

155

ARTICLEBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behav

. Introduction

Spatial memory is a very essential cognitive component for both,ndividual and species survival. In rodents spatial memory is widelyssessed in the Morris water maze [1] and in the land-based radialrm maze (RAM) [2]. The involvement of neurotransmitter systemsn cognitive functions can be studied using these different spatialearning tasks [3,4]. In RAM the spatial reference memory (identi-ying the position of rewarded arms) and spatial working memoryremembering which arms were visited earlier in the trial) can beimultaneously assessed [5–7]. The gradual decrease in numberf WM errors along the training period signifies that while per-orming the memory task during a session the animal also learns

strategy to perform the task better in the long run that leads toventual improvement in performance during the course of train-ng [8]. Repeated training in the RAM paradigm implies that theearning strategies are consolidated into long term memory afterach training and consolidation encompasses persistent chemicalhanges in the synapse [9,10].

Several neurotransmitter receptors were identified to be mod-lated during memory formation. Ionotropic glutamate receptorsre required and well-studied in the context of learning and mem-ry. Using genetic manipulation studies it was shown that AMPAubunit knockout animals show deficient spatial WM capabilities11–13] and AMPA activation is necessary for the consolida-ion/retention processes [14]. NMDA receptors are important forriggering learning-related plasticity. It has been suggested that thectivation of the NMDA receptor is required for long-term poten-iation (LTP) in the hippocampus [15]. Both, lesion studies andharmacological manipulations in animal models suggest that theMDA-receptor system is important in the induction of memory

ormation [16]. In the RAM task hippocampal NMDA receptors arenvolved in encoding and retrieval processes of spatial WM [14].urthermore, cholinergic receptor systems are important for spatialemory processes. There is significant work on nicotinic receptors

hat can modulate memory and are critical for memory function17–20]. Metabotropic dopamine receptors are also involved inoth, working memory and long term memory processes: Block-

ng receptor activity by pharmacological and knockout approachesave shown that in rodents the D1 receptor in the PFC plays aajor role in spatial memory whereas D2 receptor blockade had

o effect [21,22]. The dopamine transporter (DAT) that pumps theeurotransmitter dopamine back from the synaptic cleft into there-synapse cytosol also plays a major role in WM; inhibition ofAT by a benztropine analog improves WM performance in a PFC-ependent delayed-alteration task [23]. However, its role in longerm spatial memory is not clearly known.

RAM training involves both, spatial reference memory andpatial working memory [24]. Much of the evidence shows themportance of the hippocampus in mediating foraging behavioursing spatial cues [25,26]. Studies also emphasized a role of pre-rontal cortex (PFC) in spatially based foraging behaviours in mazesspecially in the delayed version of RAM [27]. Impairments onelayed spatial tasks after lesions to either the PFC [28] or theippocampal formation [29] suggest that these two brain regionsay interact when an animal is performing spatial memory tasks.part from its role in foraging behaviour of animals in spatial tasks,

he PFC and hippocampus are also involved in long-term memoryrocesses [30,31]. However, information on what major neuro-ransmitter RCs are modulated during spatial memory formationost training in RAM is missing.

Therefore, the aims of the current study were first to find out

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

f PFC and hippocampus are activated following RAM trainingsing Arc expression analysis. Secondly it was tried to answer theuestion which major neurotransmitter RCs and DAT transporteromplexes rather than isolated subunits are paralleling spatial

PRESS Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

memory performance of the rat in the RAM using working memoryerrors (for working memory) and latencies (for reference memory)as parameter.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Animal housing

Male Sprague Dawley rats, aged between 10 and 14 weeks wereused in this study. All rats were purchased and housed in the CoreUnit of Biomedical Research, Division of Laboratory Animal Sci-ence and Genetics, Medical University of Vienna (Himberg, Austria)and maintained in cages made of makrolon and filled with auto-claved woodchips. An autoclaved standard rodent diet (Altromin®,Germany) and water in bottles was available ad libitum. The roomwas illuminated with artificial light from 5:00 h to 19:00 h at anintensity of about 200 lx positioned in 2 m distance. All behaviourexperiments were performed between 8:00 h and 14:00 h.

All procedures were carried out according to the guidelinesof the Ethics committee, Medical University of Vienna, and U.K.Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act, 1986 and associated guide-lines, the European Communities Council Directive of 24 November1986 (86/609/EEC) and were approved by the Federal Ministryof Education, Science and Culture, Austria (BMWF-66.009/0267-II/3b/2012). All efforts were made to minimize animal sufferingand to reduce the number of animals used.

2.2. Radial arm maze

For biochemical analysis twelve rats per group (trained anduntrained) and for Arc expression profiling seven rats per groupwere used. Rats were trained in the twelve arm radial maze. Thesetup is made of black plastic and kept at an elevation of 80 cmabove the floor in a room with visual cues placed distally on allsides. The central platform has a diameter of 50 cm and twelve arms(12 cm × 60 cm) which projects radially outward. A plastic cylinderis used to restrict the movement of rats to the centre before startof the training. The lifting of the cylinder is controlled by a pulleysystem from far end of the room. Food is placed in the arm 1 cmfrom the distal end. Out of twelve arms, eight arms were baitedduring the training and four remained un-baited. The amount offood provided was restricted for five days prior to the experimentto reduce the body weight to 85% to maintain a lean, healthy bodyand also to motivate the rats for foraging behaviour during train-ing. The rats were handled for 30 min/day during these five daysfor adaptation to the experimenter. Water was provided ad libitumduring the whole training.

Before the start of the actual training, rats were given habitu-ation session for two days 5 min each in which some food pelletswere placed scattered all over the maze and rats were allowed toexplore the maze and let consume the food. During the trainingsession, the arms were baited for each rat only once at the begin-ning of each session to assess WM, while the other four arms wereleft un-baited to test reference memory. The pattern of baited andun-baited arms was consistent throughout testing for each rat butdiffered among rats. Each trial began by placing the rat in the cen-tral platform, after 10 s the cylinder was slowly lifted. A sessionlasts eight minutes or until all eight baited arms were entered. Sec-ond time and thereafter entry into a baited arm was counted as aWM error, whereas any entry into an un-baited arm was noted as areference memory error. The rats were given one training sessions

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

per day over a period of ten days.Untrained controls were placed in the maze to run the same

amount of time as their trained counterparts. Animals wereexposed to the same spatial cues, but without reward, therefore

156

157

158

159

ING ModelB

ioural

ra

2

2

2i3aplBtimttTNnWf

2

rfsf(UerTpitfflcs(iwG

2

mmakmtdJiwbBsp3b

160

161

162

163

164

165

166

167

168

169

170

171

172

173

174

175

176

177

178

179

180

181

182

183

184

185

186

187

188

189

190

191

192

193

194

195

196

197

198

199

200

201

202

203

204

205

206

207

208

209

210

211

212

213

214

215

216

217

218

219

220

221

222

223

224

225

226

227

228

229

230

231

232

233

234

235

236

237

238

239

240

241

242

243

244

245

246

247

248

249

250

251

252

253

254

255

256

257

258

259

260

261

262

263

264

265

266

267

268

269

270

271

272

273

274

275

276

277

ARTICLEBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behav

ats did not develop an association between the extra-maze cuesnd the reward.

.3. Histochemical studies

.3.1. Animal perfusion and brain fixationFor Arc immunofluorescence staining, rats were anesthetized

h after the last session of training on day ten with 0.3 ml/kgntraperitoneal injection of sodium penthorbital (Release00 mg/ml, WDT-Wirtschaftsgenossenschaft deutscher Tier-erzte eG) and perfused intracardially with ice-cold PBS (0.1 Mhosphate buffered saline, pH 7.2) containing 0.2% heparin fol-

owed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) at a pH of 7.4 in 0.1 M PBS.rain samples were postfixed in 4% PFA for 24 h at 4 ◦C and thenransferred into 30% sucrose solution (in 0.1 M PBS) for 72 h. Formmunofluorescence fixed brains were embedded with Tissue-Tek

edia (OCT compound, Sakura Finetek Europe, The Netherlands)hen frozen at -20 ◦C and sectioned at 50 �m with a cryostat. Forhe Proximity Ligation Assay fixed brains were embedded withissue-Tek media (OCT compound, Sakura Finetek Europe, Theetherlands) then immersed in isopentane cooled with liquiditrogen and sectioned at 30 �m with a cryostat (Leica CM 3050S,etzlar, Germany). Sections were stored in PBS sodium azide until

urther used.

.3.2. Immunofluorescence stainingFree-floating immunofluorescence staining was performed on

at PFC and hippocampus coronal sections. Samples were washedor 10 min in 0.1 M PBS, then blocked with 10% normal donkeyerum in 0.3% Triton X-100 PBS for 30 min at room temperatureollowed by incubation with anti-Arc rabbit polyclonal antibodyArc/Arg3.1, 1:200, Santa Cruz Biotechnology, Santa Cruz, SA,SA) for 24 h at 4 ◦C. After washing in PBS two times for 5 minach, sections were incubated with secondary antibody (anti-abbit IgG labelled Alexa Fluor 555, 1:1000 dilution, Cell Signalingechnology, Boston, MA, USA) for 1 h in the dark at room tem-erature. Slices were subsequently washed two times 5 min each

n PBS, counterstained by incubation with DAPI (1:1000 dilu-ion, 4′,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole, Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA)or 10 min followed by washing for 5 min then mounted withuorescence mounting medium (DAKO, Glostrup, Denmark) andoverslipped. Images were acquired with Zeiss Observer.Z1 micro-cope (Carl Zeiss GmbH, Jena, Germany) equipped with TissueFAXSTissue Gnostics GmbH, Vienna, Austria) at 20x magnification keep-ng all acquisition settings even through all samples. Arc expression

as analysed and quantified with TissueQuest software (Tissuenostics GmbH, Vienna, Austria).

.3.3. In-situ proximity ligation assay (PLA)The PLA was performed according to the protocol given by the

anufacturer (O-LINK Bioscience, Uppsala, Sweden) with slightodifications. Free floating brain slices were blocked for 30 min

t room temperature using blocking buffer supplied with the PLAit. After blocking brain slices were incubated with diluted mouseonoclonal antibody against the N-methyl D-aspartate recep-

or 1 (GluN1, 1:100, Abcam, Cambridge, UK) and polyclonal antiopamine D1 receptor primary antibodies (1:100, Alomone Labs,

erusalem, Israel) for 48 h at 4 ◦C on a rocking platform. Follow-ng incubation with primary antibodies slices were washed with

ashing buffer A (O-LINK Bioscience, Uppsala, Sweden) then incu-ated with rabbit PLUS and mouse MINUS probes (1:40, O-LINKioscience, Uppsala, Sweden) for 2 h at 37 ◦C with gentle orbital

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

haking. Ligation was performed according to the manufacturer’srotocol with the exception of a 45 min incubation time instead of0 min mentioned. DNA polymerase was diluted 1:20 and incu-ated for 120 min at 37 ◦C. The rest of the amplification steps

PRESS Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 3

remained unchanged. After amplification slices were washed with1x then 0.01x wash buffer B prepared according to the recipe sup-plied in the kit manual. Finally brain slices were transferred toglass slides, mounted with Duolink in Situ Mounting Medium withDAPI (O-LINK Bioscience, Uppsala, Sweden). Images were acquiredwith a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal laser scanning microscope (Carl ZeissGmbH, Jena, Germany) at 20x magnification keeping all acquisitionsettings even through all samples [32,33].

2.4. Biochemical studies

Frontal cortices (FC) and hippocampi was quickly dissected(following a micro dissection procedure described in the book“Neuroproteomics”, chapter “Dissection of Rodent Brain Regions”[34]) from rat brain six hours after the last session of RAM train-ing on day ten by deeply anaesthetizing the animal with CO2 andanimals were killed by neck dislocation. The tissue was stored at−80 ◦C for biochemical analysis.

2.4.1. Crude synaptosome preparationAll procedures were carried out at 4 ◦C. FC and hippocampal tis-

sues were homogenized in ice-cold homogenization buffer [10 mMHEPES, pH 7.5, 300 mM sucrose, one complete protease inhibitortablet (Roche Molecular Biochemicals, Mannheim, Germany) per50 ml] by Ultra-Turrax (IKA, Staufen, Germany). The homogenatewas centrifuged for 10 min at 1000 × g and the pellet was dis-carded. The supernatant was centrifuged at 50,000 × g for 30 minin an ultracentrifuge (Beckman Coulter Optima-L-90 K). The pelletwas re-suspended in washing buffer (homogenization buffer with-out sucrose), kept on ice for 1 h and centrifuged at 50,000 × g for30 min to obtain membrane fraction of crude synaptosome extractas pellet.

2.4.2. Receptor protein extractionAll procedures were carried out at 4 ◦C. An extraction buffer con-

taining 1.5 M 6-aminocaproic acid, 300 mM Bis–Tris, pH 7.0 and1% n-Dodecyl �-d-maltoside (DDM) was added to the membranepellets and incubated for 1 h by gently vortexing every 10 min.Following solubilisation, samples were centrifuged at 15,000 × gfor 60 min. The pellet was discarded. The protein concentrationof the supernatant was estimated using the BCA protein assay kit(Pierce, Rockford, IL, USA). Extracted proteins were then aliquotedand stored at −80 ◦C until used.

2.4.3. Blue native polyacrylamide gel electrophoresis (BN PAGE)and BN PAGE western blot procedure

Equal amount of proteins (30 �g) from trained and untrainedsamples were loaded in the wells and the RCs were separated on5–13% of blue native PAGE gels and the western blot procedurewas carried out using the procedure described previously [35]. Thedetails of antibodies used are given in the supplementary dataTable 1. Immunoreactive bands were quantified by the softwareImage J (NIH). Coomassie blue R-350 stained membranes were usedas loading control and normalized with the western blot densito-metric values [36,37]

2.4.4. Co-immunoprecipitationThe FC crude synaptosome membrane fraction pellet prepared

as described previously was suspended in lysis buffer containing1% DDM, 150 mM NaCl, 1 mM EDTA, 50 mM Tris–HCl (pH 8.0),10 mM NaF, 10 mM Na3VO4, 10 mM Na4O7P2 and protease inhibitorcocktail (Roche, Mannheim, Germany) on a rotation shaker 1 h

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

at 4 ◦C. After centrifugation at 15,000 × g, at 4 ◦C for 20 min, thesupernatant was incubated with affinity-purified goat anti-bodyagainst NMDA receptor subunit GluN1 (GluN1; NMDA�1; sc-1467,Santa Cruz Biotechnology) and dopamine receptor subunit D1

278

279

280

281

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

4 B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

Table 1Showing BN PAGE western blot mean densitometry values (arbitrary units) after normalization and statistical t-test p-values of the individual receptor complexes.

Receptor subunits (apparentmol. wt. in BN PAGE)

FC Hippocampus

mean ± SD p value mean ± SD p value

Trained Untrained Trained Untrained

Glu A1 (∼700 kDa) 1.179 ± 0.186 0.809 ± 0.233 0.0003 1.018 ± 0.239 0.887 ± 0.337 0.2874Glu A2 (∼700 kDa) 0.991 ± 0.101 0.778 ± 0.128 0.0002 1.727 ± 0.299 1.569 ± 0.556 0.3681Glu A2 (∼550 kDa) 0.624 ± 0.492 0.495 ± 0.212 0.4189 – – –Glu A3 (∼700 kDa) 6.059± 1.826 5.242 ± 1.770 0.2783 1.427 ± 0.592 1.424 ± 0.524 0.9886Glu A3 (∼550 kDa) 1.773 ± 0.754 1.383 ± 0.736 0.2136 2.946 ± 1.663 1.968 ± 0.851 0.1008Glu A4 (∼700 kDa) 1.649 ± 0.638 1.697 ± 0.326 0.8219 3.962 ± 1.569 4.800 ± 1.168 0.1532Glu A4 (∼550 kDa) 0.652 ± 0.215 0.786 ± 0.368 0.2926 – – –Glu N1 (∼480 kDa) 0.698 ± 0.159 0.510 ± 0.331 0.0949 4.369 ± 1.508 2.839 ± 0.695 0.0059Glu N1 (∼300 kDa) 0.949 ± 0.595 0.461 ± 0.337 0.0241 – – –Glu N2A (∼700 kDa) 0.910 ± 0.291 0.444 ± 0.137 0.0001 0.736 ± 0.220 0.647 ± 0.244 0.3585Glu N2A (∼500 kDa) – – – 1.141 ± 0.236 1.173 ± 0.363 0.8152Glu N2B (∼700 kDa) – – – 1.237 ± 0.925 0.751 ± 0.303 0.1066Glu N2B (∼500 kDa) 0.682 ± 0.197 0.611 ± 0.132 0.3110 0.930 ± 0.240 0.615 ± 0.143 0.0010nAChR-�7 (∼700 kDa) 1.515 ± 0.331 1.016 ± 0.304 0.0009 0.893 ± 0.272 0.503 ± 0.130 0.0004nAChR-�7 (∼480 kDa) – – – 1.125 ± 0.330 0.554 ± 0.182 0.0001D1 (∼480 kDa) 0.544 ± 0.157 0.431 ± 0.104 0.0510 2.989 ± 0.687 1.893 ± 0.649 0.0006D1 (∼300 kDa) 0.984 ± 0.323 0.647 ± 0.242 0.0090 – – –D2 (∼480 kDa) 0.930 ± 0.287 1.055 ± 0.457 0.4532 – – –D2 (∼300 kDa) 2.541 ± 1.151 2.303 ± 1.106 0.6195 1.460 ± 0.595 1.323 ± 0.323 0.4934DAT (∼500 kDa) 1.344 ± 0.356 1.275 ± 0.473 0.6932 0.530 ± 0.274 0.776 ± 0.413 0.1018

(apabtmPaeda(d1g

2

ts5b3two(a5b4tbwtrr

282

283

284

285

286

287

288

289

290

291

292

293

294

295

296

297

298

299

300

301

302

303

304

305

306

307

308

309

310

311

312

313

314

315

316

317

318

319

320

321

322

323

324

325

326

327

328

329

330

331

332

333

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

341

342

343

344

345

346

347

348

349

350

351

DAT (∼300 kDa) 1.772 ± 0.555 1.898 ± 0.576

DAT-ph (∼500 kDa) 3.915 ± 1.157 3.561 ± 0.670

DAT-ph (∼300 kDa) 2.098 ± 0.634 2.101 ± 0.894

D1DR Antibody, sc-14001; Santa Cruz Biotechnology) in two sep-rate reactions overnight at 4 ◦C and subsequently incubated withrotein-G agarose beads (GE Healthcare, Uppsala, Sweden) for 4 ht 4 ◦C with gentle rotation. After five times of washing with lysisuffer, proteins bound were denatured with sample buffer con-aining 125 mM Tris (pH 6.8), 4% SDS, 20% glycerol, 10% beta-

ercaptoethanol, 0.02% bromophenol blue at 95 ◦C for 3 min. SDS-AGE was performed using 5% stacking and 8% separating gel withn initial current of 50 V for 1 h and then 150 V for 1 h [38]. West-rn blotting was carried out following the procedure as previouslyescribed [39] except the details of the antibodies used in this workre as follows. Membrane blotted with primary antibody anti D1Anti-Dopamine Receptor D1 antibody, ab78021, abcam) 1:1000ilution and anti GluN1 (Anti-NMDAR1 antibody ab134308, abcam):2000 dilution and the secondary antibody used was HRP conju-ated Goat Anti-Mouse IgG H&L (ab97040, abcam) 1:5000 dilution.

.4.5. In-gel digestion of proteinsProtein bands from silver-stained SDS-PAGE gels that were iden-

ified by the corresponding antibodies against the D1 and GluN1ubunits were put into a 1.5 mL tube. Gel pieces were washed with0 mM ammonium bicarbonate and then two times with washinguffer (50% 100 mM ammonium bicarbonate/50% acetonitrile) for0 min each with vortexing. 100 �L of 100% acetonitrile was addedo the tube and the mixture was incubated for 10 min. Gel piecesere dried completely using a SpeedVac concentrator. Reduction

f cysteine residues was carried out with a 10 mM dithiothreitolDTT) solution in 100 mM ammonium bicarbonate pH 8.6 for 60 mint 56 ◦C. After discarding the DTT solution, the same volume of a5 mM iodoacetamide (IAA) solution in 100 mM ammonium bicar-onate buffer pH 8.6 was added and incubated in darkness for5 min at 25 ◦C to alkylate the cysteine residues. The IAA solu-ion was replaced by washing buffer (50% 100 mM ammoniumicarbonate/50% acetonitrile) and washed twice for 15 min each

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

ith vortexing. Gel pieces were washed and dried in 100% ace-onitrile followed by dryness in SpeedVac. Dried gel pieces weree-swollen with 12.5 ng/�L trypsin (Promega, Germany) solutioneconstituted with 25 mM ammonium bicarbonate. Gel pieces were

0.5888 – – –0.3714 0.407 ± 0.201 0.606 ± 0.169 0.01610.9940 0.804 ± 0.376 0.952 ± 0.291 0.2956

incubated for 16 h at 37 ◦C. The supernatant was transferred tonew 0.5 mL tubes, and peptides were extracted with 50 �L of 0.5%formic acid/20% acetonitrile for 20 min in a sonication bath. Thisstep was repeated two times. Samples in extraction buffer werepooled in 0.5 mL tubes and evaporated in a SpeedVac concentrator.The peptides were reconstituted in 5% formic acid and analysed byLC-MS/MS [35].

2.4.6. LC-MS/MS analysisNano-LC-ESI-MS/MS was performed on a linear trap quadrupole

(LTQ) Orbitrap Velos (Thermoscientific, Walthan, MA, USA) cou-pled to an Agilent 1200 HPLC nanoflow system comprised of adual pump with one precolumn and one analytical column (Agi-lent Biotechnologies, Palo Alto, CA, USA) (Bennett et al., 2011).Data were acquired using Xcalibur version 2.1.0. HPLC solventswere as follows: solvent A consisted of 0.4% formic acid in waterand solvent B consisted of 0.4% formic acid in 70% methanol and20% isopropanol. From a thermostated microautosampler, 8 �Lof the peptide mixture were automatically loaded onto a trapcolumn (Zorbax 300SB-C18 5 �m, 5 × 0.3 mm, Agilent Biotechnolo-gies) with a binary pump at a flow rate of 45 �L/min using 0.1%TFA for loading and washing the precolumn. After washing, thepeptides were eluted by back-flushing onto a 16 cm fused silicaanalytical column with an inner diameter of 50 �m packed with aC18 reversed phase material (ReproSil-Pur 120 C18-AQ, 3 �m, Dr.Maisch GmbH, Ammerbuch-Entringen, Germany). Peptides wereeluted from the analytical column with a 27 min gradient rangingfrom 3 to 30% solvent B, followed by a 25 min gradient from 30 to70% solvent B and, finally, a 7 min gradient from 70 to 100% solventB at a constant flow rate of 100 nL/min. The analyses were per-formed in a data-dependent acquisition mode using a top 15 CIDmethod. Dynamic exclusion for selected ions was 60s. A single lockmass at m/z 445.120024 was employed. Maximal ion accumulationtime allowed in MS and MSn mode was 500 and 50ms, respectively.

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

Automatic gain control was used to prevent overfilling of the iontrap and was set to 106 ions and 5000 ions for a full Fourier trans-form mass spectrometry scan and MSn, respectively. Peptides weredetected in MS mode at a resolution of 60,000 (at m/z 400).

352

353

354

355

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 5

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

num

ber o

f WM

E

training sessio ns (days)

*

0

2

4

6

8

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

num

ber o

f RM

E

training sessio ns (days)

a b

0

50

100

150

200

250

300

350

400

450

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

late

ncy

(sec

)

training sessio ns (days)

*

c

F – latd in RMd n in th

AuaUstiacsaFpr

3

3

mmTtudWsi

356

357

358

359

360

361

362

363

364

365

366

367

368

369

370

371

372

373

374

375

376

377

378

379

380

381

382

383

384

385

386

387

388

389

390

391

392

393

394

395

396

397

398

399

400

401

402

403

404

ig. 1. a – Working memory error (WME), b – reference memory error (RME) and cay-8, 9 and 10 compared to day-1 (*P < 0.001, F = 4.949). Slightly decreasing trenday-3 onwards compared to day-1 (*P < 0.0001, F = 13.23). Mean and SEM are show

Proteins were identified using Proteome Discoverer version 1.4. spectral peptide peak list was extracted from the raw files andsing both, Mascot and SequestHT, the peak list was matchedgainst a rat protein database (9,595 protein sequence entries fromniProtKB/Swiss-Prot downloaded on March 2013). The MSMS ion

earch parameters were as follows: one missed cleavage site, massolerances of 10 ppm and 0.6 Da for the precursor and fragmentons. Dynamic modifications were: methionine oxidation, serinend threonine phosphorylation. Static modifications were cysteinearbamidomethylation. Matched peptides were filtered as follows:ignificant threshold below 0.05 for Mascot and 0.1 for SEQUEST,nd a 1% false discovery rate cut-off after target decoy search.iltered peptides from the two search engines were merged androtein identifications requiring at least two unique peptides wereeported.

. Results

.1. Radial arm maze

The total number of working memory errors (WME), referenceemory errors (RME) and latency to finish a trial made by the ani-als with respect to the training sessions were shown in Fig. 1.

rained rats showed significant reduction of latency to finish arial and WME over the course of training. Data were analysedsing repeated measurements ANOVA. WME became statistically

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

ifferent from day-8 onwards (F = 4.949, p < 0.0001), on day-10ME were about threefold lower than on day-1. There was a

lightly decreasing trend for RME however, not statistically signif-cant. Latency to finish a trial showed a decreasing trend and was

ency to finish a trial of RAM behaviour test. WMEs were significantly decreased onE curve was not statistically significant. Latency was significantly decreased frome graph. The data were analysed using repeated measurements ANOVA.

significantly different from day-3 onwards compared to day-1(F = 13.23, p < 0.0001).

3.2. Arc expression analysis in PFC and hippocampus

Immunohistochemical analysis (Fig. 2) showed that the imme-diate early gene Arc in PFC was expressed significantly higherin trained animals when compared to the untrained animals. Intrained animals the bright pink spots representing arc expressionin different regions of PFC, ACd (dorsal anterior cingulate area), FR2(frontal cortex area 2), IL (infralimbic area) and PL (prelimbic area)were significantly more abundant, compared with unpaired Stu-dent’s t-test (p < 0.05). The sub-regions of PFC were identified usingthe reference [40]. In hippocampal regions there was no statisticaldifference in Arc expression between trained and untrained groups(data not shown).

3.3. BN PAGE western blot quantification of membrane receptorcomplexes

The RCs were quantified using BN PAGE western blotting. InFC the RCs containing subunits GluA1 at around 700 kDa, GluA2(∼700 kDa), D1 (∼300 kDa), GluN1 (∼300 kDa), GluN2A (∼700 kDa)and nAChR-�7 (∼700 kDa) were significantly increased in thetrained group. In hippocampus RCs containing GluN1 (∼480 kDa),GluN2B (∼500 kDa), D1 (∼480 kDa), and nAChR-�7 (∼700 &

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

∼480 kDa) were significantly increased in trained groups whencompared to untrained rats. In hippocampi the phosphorylated,activated form of DAT (DAT-ph) (∼500 kDa) showed lower arbi-trary units of optical density in the trained group as compared by

405

406

407

408

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

6 B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

Fig. 2. Expression analysis of IEG Arc protein in PFC region of trained and untrained rats by Immunohistochemistry. The bright pink spots in the image represent the expresseda orsal

a d mors hown

uiTml

3

fcFr

3b

aGea

cToplv

i

409

410

411

412

413

414

415

416

417

418

419

420

421

422

423

424

425

426

427

428

429

430

431

432

433

434

435

436

437

438

439

440

441

442

443

444

445

446

447

448

449

450

451

452

453

454

455

456

457

458

459

460

461

462

rc protein. The image also shows the different regions of PFC. Abbreviations: ACd, drea. Scale-bar in the image represents 500 �m. Arc protein is significantly expresseignificance of difference in unpaired Student’s t-test (*P < 0.05). Mean and SD are s

npaired Student’s t-test. No change in the electrophoretic mobil-ty of the RCs between trained and untrained groups was observed.he mean densitometric values of BN PAGE western blot after nor-alization and statistical t-test p-values of the individual RCs are

isted in the Table 1 (Figs. 3 and 4).

.4. Proximity ligation assay of D1 and GluN1 receptors

Fig. 5 shows the PLA data of D1 and GluN1 receptors in dif-erent regions of rat PFC were presented with red dots indicatingo-localization and proximity of D1 and GluN1 receptors in ACd,R2, IL and PL regions which in turn proposes that D1 and GluN1eceptor subunits may be complex components of the same RCs.

.5. Co-immunoprecipitation of D1 and GluN1 receptor followedy mass spectrometrical identification of receptor complexes

Co-immunoprecipitation was carried out with an immobilizedntibody against D1 as well as an immobilized antibody againstluN1. Western blot analysis shown in Fig. 6 confirmed the pres-nce of both, D1 and GluN1 receptors in both elutions, i.e. thentibody against D1 detected GluN1 and vice versa.

Mass spectrometrical analysis (MS) shows that the immunopre-ipitate using an antibody against D1 contained the GluN1 subunit.otal sequence coverage indicated in Table 2 indicates unambigu-us MS identification of these receptor subunits. The individualeptides obtained from proteolytic cleavage followed by MS are

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

isted in supplementary Table 2. Representative spectra are pro-ided in supplementary Fig. 1.

This finding suggests that the D1 and GluN1 receptors can existn the same complex.

anterior cingulate area; FR2, frontal cortex area 2; IL, infralimbic area; PL, prelimbice in trained group when compared to the untrained rats. Asterisks indicate level of

in the graph.

4. Discussion

Herein, following RAM training in rats, the neuronal activity inPFC and hippocampus were identified using Arc expression analysisand levels of major neurotransmitter RCs in these regions werecompared between trained and untrained animals.

Behavioural results reveal that the animals had learned the taskshowing gradually decreasing latency to finish a session statisti-cally significant from day-3 onwards compared to the first day.Appetite is the motivation for the foraging behaviour of mildlystarving rats in RAM. Animals identify the spatial location of thearm containing food pellets using available distal cues [41]. In orderto retrieve the food efficiently with minimal efforts when a baitedarm is visited for food, the rat has to avoid re-entry. This learn-ing strategy involves working memory and the gradual decreaseof WME over the training days signifies that the learning strate-gies are consolidated to long term memory after each training day[10]. Thus, since the proteomic analysis has been done at the end oftraining, changes in levels of neurotransmitter RCs may be associ-ated with the persistent chemical changes of the neuronal synapsesas a result of RAM training.

Activity regulated cytoskeleton associated protein (Arc) is oneof the Immediate Early Genes (IEG), expressed in the brain regionswhen there is neuronal activation [42–44]. Therefore, IEG expres-sion has been widely used as a marker to identify brain regionsthat are activated in response to learning [45–47]. Own resultsshow that Arc is expressed significantly higher in the trained groupin areas of PFC, ACd, FR2, IL and PL, suggesting that all of these

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

areas are activated following RAM training, whereas the hippocam-pal regions do not show any difference in Arc expression betweentrained and untrained groups. The PFC is known for its involve-ment in guiding the animal for goal-directed behaviour and the

463

464

465

466

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 7

F luA2w ce in

m

hpbsiRIamitmcgwsmtrtt

467

468

469

470

471

472

473

474

475

476

477

478

479

480

481

482

483

484

485

486

487

488

489

490

491

492

493

494

495

496

497

498

499

500

501

502

503

504

ig. 3. BN PAGE western quantification of receptor complexes in FC tissue. GluA1, Ghen compared to untrained rats. Asterisks indicate level of significance of differenean and SD error bars.

ippocampus is critical for spatial navigation and spatial memoryrocessing. These two structures interact through complex circuitsased on the functional demand and their coordination is neces-ary for optimal performance [48–50]. From the current study its understood that both, hippocampus and PFC are required in theAM paradigm. Inhibition of the activity-dependent expression of

EGs causes impairment of short term memory consolidation [51]nd therefore plays a critical role in the formation of long termemory [52,53]. This implies that Arc is more likely expressed

n brain regions that are involved in memory consolidation. Fromhis perspective, elevated levels of Arc expression in trained ani-

als in PFC signifies that PFC neurons are subjected to memoryonsolidation after the last session, because the learning strate-ies are consolidated as the latency to complete the task decreasesith training. Based upon Arc expression results herein, it may be

uggested that the memory consolidation process in RAM trainingay involve only PFC and not hippocampus. It is known, however,

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

hat the hippocampus is essential for acquisition, consolidation andetention of spatial and non-spatial memories [50,54,55]. Own con-rasting result in the hippocampus may propose that after repeatedraining in the RAM, hippocampal neurons do not undergo any

, GluN1, GluN2A, D1, and nAChR-�7 were significantly increased in trained groupsunpaired Student’s t-test (*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, #P < 0.005). The bar graph shows the

memory consolidation and therefore do not express Arc but proba-bly the hippocampus has been involved in the beginning of training,although the statement has to be reconsidered as significant RCslevel changes were observed.

Previous reports have been already addressing the involvementof neurotransmitter RCs rather than individual receptor subunits inspatial memory formation [37,56–58] but work on the RAM has notbeen published so far to the best of our knowledge. Determinationof RCs is relevant as receptor function is carried out as homo-orheteromeric assemblies of receptors.

Glutamate, the major excitatory neurotransmitter exerts itsaction by binding to glutamate receptors. Ionotropic glutamatereceptors are made up of different types of subunits and basedon the subunit composition the biochemical and electrophysio-logical properties of the RCs vary [59]. Specific AMPA receptorsubunits are delivered to synaptic sites following learning and con-tributes to experience-dependent synaptic strengthening [60,61].

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

AMPA receptors play a vital role in WM mechanisms [62]: GluA1subunit deletion in the whole brain results in strong spatial WMdeficits [63]. The molecular mechanism of AMPA receptor subunitfunction in spatial WM is not clearly understood but it has been

505

506

507

508

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

8 B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

F plex

t ecreasS SD er

pttirktsittTsutc

509

510

511

512

513

514

515

516

517

518

519

520

521

522

523

524

525

526

527

528

529

530

531

532

533

534

535

536

ig. 4. BN PAGE western quantification of receptor complexes in hippocampus. Comrained groups when compared to untrained rats whereas the DAT-ph levels were dtudent’s t-test (*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, #P < 0.005). The bar graph shows the mean and

ostulated that it includes the activation of AMPA receptors con-aining the GluA1 subunit [63] and consequently during plasticityhe GluA1/GluA2 containing RCs are added to synapses and dur-ng long term memory formation GluA2/GluA3-containing AMPAeceptors constitutively replace synaptic AMPA receptors whileeeping the synaptic strength constant [64,65]. There is evidencehat GluA1-mediated AMPA receptor signalling is essential forpatial memory tasks, particularly in hippocampus [61,66]. It isntriguing that no differences between hippocampal AMPA recep-or containing RCs levels were observed and this finding is of impor-ance to elucidate different memory mechanisms in hippocampus.hese data emphasize the importance of NMDA receptors in acqui-

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

ition of spatial memory processes. Since on the last day the rats stillndergo training, NMDA receptor subunits may be modulated afterhe last session. Own results show that in FC GluN2A and GluN1ontaining complexes were increased whereas in hippocampus

containing GluN1, GluN2B, D1, and nAChR-�7 levels were significantly increased ined in trained group. Asterisks indicate level of significance of difference in unpairedror bars.

NR2B and GluN1 containing complexes were elevated in trainedanimals, pointing to different mechanism in hippocampus and FCin NMDA-mediated signalling in performance in the RAM. Pharma-cological manipulation studies in an experimental animal modelsuggest that the NMDA-receptor system may be important in theacquisition of memory, but not for the maintenance [16], hencethe changes in NMDA receptor subunits in the current study couldbe due to the effect of the last training session.nAChR-�7 contain-ing RCs levels were significantly higher in the synaptic membranefraction in trained groups. This result is in agreement with a recentbehavioural study indicating that acetylcholine alpha 7-containingRCs are modulated during memory retrieval in mice tested in the

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

multiple-T-maze [67]. Depletion of acetylcholine in dorsolateralPFC markedly impaired WM [17] whereas selective stimulation ofthe neuronal nAChR-�7 receptor improved spatial WM in nonhu-man primates [68] and in rodents [69].

537

538

539

540

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontal cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Behav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 9

Fig. 5. “In Situ” Proximity ligation assay for detection of co-localized D1 and GluN1 receptors in rat PFC. Representative images showing the D1-GluN1 receptor complex withDAPI counterstained nuclei. Red dots as shown by the arrow indicates the co-localization of D1 and GluN1 receptor. Abbreviations: ACd, dorsal anterior cingulate area; FR2,frontal cortex area 2; IL, infralimbic area; PL, prelimbic area. Negative control is done in parallel with no primary antibody added. Scale-bar in the figure represents 10 �m.

ARTICLE IN PRESSG ModelBBR 9565 1–12

10 B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behavioural Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

Table 2Data of mass spectrometrical identification of GluN1 co-immunoprecipitated with D1.

Uniprot accession number Enzyme digestion Gene name �coverage �# Unique Peptides �# Peptides �# PSMs

G3V933D(1A) dopamine receptor

Trypsin Drd1 23.99 8 8 197

P35439-5Glutamate receptor ionotropic,NMDA 1

Trypsin Grin1

Fig. 6. Western blot showing the detection of D1 and GluN1 receptor subunitsimmunoprecipitated with Anti-D1 and Anti-GluN1 antibody. This shows the D1 andGs

wsLaiiicasDraf

atamtbsoso(Dtrp[mUcscuc

ing in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 1984;11:47–60.

541

542

543

544

545

546

547

548

549

550

551

552

553

554

555

556

557

558

559

560

561

562

563

564

565

566

567

568

569

570

571

572

573

574

575

576

577

578

579

580

581

582

583

584

585

586

587

588

589

590

591

592

593

594

595

596

597

598

599

600

601

602

603

604

605

606

607

608

609

610

611

612

613

614

615

616

617

618

619

620

621

622

623

624

625

626

627

luN1 remains in a complex and are co-immuno precipitated. Cntrl is the total crudeynaptosome membrane fraction protein prepared from rat’s FC tissue.

The involvement of the dopaminergic system in memory isell-studied in hippocampus. The dopamine receptor subtype D1

timulates the onset of protein synthesis-dependent late phase ofTP in the hippocampus [70] and facilitates the persistent stor-ge of hippocampus-dependent memories [71]. Apart from its rolen long-term memory formation, the dopaminergic system is alsonvolved in WM processes. The D1 subtype within the PFC has beenmplicated in normal functioning of WM and its alterations canause WM impairments like schizotypal personality disorder [72]nd schizophrenia [73]. D1 receptor blockade using antagonistshowed dose- and delay-dependent impairments in WM whereas2 receptor blockade had no effect [74]. In the current study it is

evealed that levels of a D1 containing complex in the trained groupre higher and D2 containing RCs levels were unchanged in both,rontal cortex and hippocampus.

Complexes containing D1 and GluN1 were co-migrating atround 300 kDa on BN-PAGE which indicates, that these recep-ors could exist in the same complex. It is well known that D1nd NMDA receptors have profound importance in cognition (likeemory and executive functions) and in synaptic plasticity and

here is evidence that D1 and glutamate receptors can co-exist inrain regions. Sarantis et al. examined the effects of the in-vitrotimulation of D1 and D2 receptors on the phosphorylation statef NMDA and AMPA receptor subunits in rat PFC and hippocampushowing that D1 receptor activation elicits a significant increasef the phosphorylation state of NMDA (GluN1, GluN2B) and AMPAGluA1) receptor subunits [75]. In another study it was shown that1 and NR1 receptors interact through protein-protein interac-

ions in single pyramidal neurons and inter-neurons in the adultat PFC [76]. D1 and NMDA (GluN1, GluN2B) receptors form a com-lex through the carboxyl tails and can be co-immunoprecipitated77]. In electrophysiological studies it was demonstrated that D1

odulates NMDA currents in hippocampal neuron cultures [77].sing PLA herein it was observed that all regions in PFC showedo-localisation of D1 and GluN1 receptor subunits. Since the PLA

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

hows proximity and does not confirm physical interactions, theomplex formation between D1 and GluN1 was further examinedsing co-immunoprecipitation experiments that revealed that D1o-eluted with an antibody against GluN1 and vice versa, moreover,

5.76 5 5 5

mass spectrometrical analysis of the immunoprecipitate using anantibody against D1 identified the presence of GluN1. Nai andco-workers explored the effects of the D1–GluN1 interaction onNMDA receptor-dependent LTP and WM reporting that uncou-pling the D1–GluN1 interaction led to impaired WM and decreasedGluN1–CaMKII association and CaMKII activity in rat hippocampus[78].

The DAT is a presynaptic membrane-spanning protein knownto mediate dopamine re-uptake. Reuptake inhibition has beenalready used for cognitive enhancement in the treatment of ADHD.Schmeichel et al. observed improved performance in rats using aPFC-dependent delayed-alternation task of spatial working mem-ory by the administration of a well-characterized benztropineanalog, AHN 2-005 [23]. However, not much is known about itsinvolvement in long term memory formation. Own results showthat levels of DAT containing complexes were comparable betweentrained and untrained animals in frontal cortex and hippocampuswhereas levels of complexes containing the phosphorylated activeform of DAT (phosphorylated at amino acid position Thr53) werelower in trained animals in hippocampus but not in FC.

The presence and modulation of a high molecular weight com-plex containing the activated form of DAT in spatial memoryperformance in the current work points to the probable involve-ment of a large transporter complex that remains to be furthercharacterised by subsequent studies.

Taken together, RCs changes paralleling spatial memory withreference and working memory components in the PFC as well asin hippocampus at the end of the RAM testing were revealed, con-firming the participation of both regions in spatial and workingmemory mechanisms. Moreover, a complex was shown to containD1 and GluN1 and a complex containing phosphorylated, i.e. acti-vated DAT in FC was decreased in rats trained in the RAM probablyindicating a role for dopamine reuptake in memory formation. Thiswork may be relevant for the interpretation of previous work on sig-naling and design of future studies on neurotransmitter receptorsand dopamine transport in spatial memory.

Acknowledgements

The skilful technical assistance of Sabine Rauscher from theimaging core facility is highly appreciated. We acknowledge com-ments from Prof. Dr. Volker Korz, Univ. Vienna. We appreciate thefinancial assistance from the Medical University of Vienna to carryout this work.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, inthe online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

References

[1] Morris R. Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learn-

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

[2] David S, Olton, Robert J, Samuelson. Remembrance of places passed: spatialmemory in rats. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1976;2:97–116.

[3] Levin ED. Psychopharmacological effects in the radial-arm maze. NeurosciBiobehav Rev 1988;12:169–75.

628

629

630

631

ING ModelB

ioural

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

632

633

634

635

636

637

638

639

640

641

642

643

644

645

646

647

648

649

650

651

652

653

654

655

656

657

658

659

660

661

662

663

664

665

666

667

668

669

670

671

672

673

674

675

676

677

678

679

680

681

682

683

684

685

686

687

688

689

690

691

692

693

694

695

696

697

698

699

700

701

702

703

704

705

706

707

708

709

710

711

712

713

714

715

716

717

718

719

720

721

722

723

724

725

726

727

728

729

730

731

732

733

734

735

736

737

738

739

740

741

742

743

744

745

746

747

748

749

750

751

752

753

754

755

756

757

758

759

760

761

762

763

764

765

766

767

768

769

770

771

772

773

774

775

776

777

778

779

780

781

782

783

784

785

786

787

788

789

790

791

ARTICLEBR 9565 1–12

B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behav

[4] Butelman ER. The effect of NMDA antagonists in the radial arm maze task withan interposed delay. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 1990;35:533–6.

[5] Buresová O, Bures J. Radial maze as a tool for assessing the effect of drugs onthe working memory of rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1982;77:268–71.

[6] Levin ED, Timofeeva OA, Yang L, Petro A, Ryde IT, Wrench N, et al. Earlypostnatal parathion exposure in rats causes sex-selective cognitive impair-ment and neurotransmitter defects which emerge in aging. Behav Brain Res2010;208:319–27, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.11.007.

[7] Spritzer MD, Daviau ED, Coneeny MK, Engelman SM, Prince WT,Rodriguez-Wisdom KN. Effects of testosterone on spatial learn-ing and memory in adult male rats. Horm Behav 2011;59:484–96,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.01.009.

[8] McNamara DS, Scott JL. Working memory capacity and strategy use. Mem Cog-nit 2001;29:10–7.

[9] Mizumori SJ, Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Long-term working memoryin the rat: effects of hippocampally applied anisomycin. Behav Neurosci1985;99:220–32.

10] Mizumori SJ, Channon V, Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Short- and long-termcomponents of working memory in the rat. Behav Neurosci 1987;101:782–9.

11] Zamanillo D, Sprengel R, Hvalby O, Jensen V, Burnashev N, Rozov A, et al. Impor-tance of AMPA receptors for hippocampal synaptic plasticity but not for spatiallearning. Science 1999;284:1805–11.

12] Reisel D, Bannerman DM, Schmitt WB, Deacon RMJ, Flint J, Borchardt T,et al. Spatial memory dissociations in mice lacking GluR1. Nat Neurosci2002;5:868–73, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn910.

13] Sanderson DJ, Good MA, Seeburg PH, Sprengel R, Rawlins JNP,Bannerman DM. The role of the GluR-A (GluR1) AMPA receptor sub-unit in learning and memory. Prog Brain Res 2008;169:159–78,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(07)00009-X.

14] Yoshihara T, Ichitani Y. Hippocampal N-methyl-d-aspartatereceptor-mediatedencoding and retrieval processes in spatial working memory: Delay-interposed radial maze performance in rats. Neuroscience 2004;129:1–10,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.07.030.

15] Izquierdo I. Pharmacological evidence for a role of long-term potentiation inmemory. FASEB J Off Publ Fed Am Soc Exp Biol 1994;8:1139–45.

16] Izquierdo I. Role of NMDA receptors in memory. Trends Pharmacol Sci1991;12:128–9.

17] Croxson PL, Kyriazis DA, Baxter MG. Cholinergic modulation of a spe-cific memory function of prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2011;14:1510–2,http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn.2971.

18] Hidaka N, Suemaru K, Kato Y, Araki H. Involvement of �4�2 nico-tinic acetylcholine receptors in working memory impairment induced byrepeated electroconvulsive seizures in rats. Epilepsy Res 2013;104:181–5,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2012.09.017.

19] Yang Y, Paspalas CD, Jin LE, Picciotto MR, Arnsten AFT, Wang M.Nicotinic �7 receptors enhance NMDA cognitive circuits in dorsolat-eral prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110:12078–83,http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1307849110.

20] Deiana S, Platt B, Riedel G. The cholinergic system and spatial learning. BehavBrain Res 2011;221:389–411, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.036.

21] Xing B, Guo J, Meng X, Wei S, Li S. The dopamine D1 but not D3receptor plays a fundamental role in spatial working memory and BDNFexpression in prefrontal cortex of mice. Behav Brain Res 2012;235:36–41,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.035.

22] Wan P, Wang S, Zhang Y, Lv J, Jin QH. Involvement of dopamine D1 receptorsof the hippocampal dentate gyrus in spatial learning and memory deficits in arat model of vascular dementia. Pharmazie 2014;69:709–10.

23] Schmeichel BE, Zemlan FP, Berridge CW. A selective dopamine reuptakeinhibitor improves prefrontal cortex-dependent cognitive function: poten-tial relevance to attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropharmacology2013;64:321–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.005.

24] Olton DS, Collison C, Werz MA. Spatial memory and radial arm maze per-formance of rats. Learn Motiv 1977;8:289–314, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(77)90054-6.

25] Olton DS, Paras BC. Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsycholo-gia 1979;17:669–82, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(79)90042-3.

26] Jarrard LE. On the role of the hippocampus in learning and memory in the rat.Behav Neural Biol 1993;60:9–26.

27] Seamans JK, Floresco SB, Phillips AG. Functional differences between theprelimbic and anterior cingulate regions of the rat prefrontal cortex. BehavNeurosci 1995;109:1063–73.

28] Kesner RP. Retrospective and prospective coding of information: role of themedial prefrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res 1989;74:163–7.

29] Dunnett SB. Role of prefrontal cortex and striatal output systems in short-term memory deficits associated with ageing, basal forebrain lesions, andcholinergic-rich grafts. Can J Psychol 1990;44:210–32.

30] Blumenfeld RS, Ranganath C. Prefrontal cortex and long-term memoryencoding: an integrative review of findings from neuropsychology andneuroimaging. Neuroscientist 2007;13:280–91, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

1073858407299290.31] Ramírez-Amaya V, Balderas I, Sandoval J, Escobar ML, Bermúdez-Rattoni F. Spa-

tial long-term memory is related to mossy fiber synaptogenesis. J Neurosci OffJ Soc Neurosci 2001;21:7340–8.

[

[

PRESS Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx 11

32] Trifilieff P, Rives M-L, Urizar E, Piskorowski RA, Vishwasrao HD, Castrillon J,et al. Detection of antigen interactions ex vivo by proximity ligation assay:endogenous dopamine D2-adenosine A2A receptor complexes in the striatum.BioTechniques 2011;51:111–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.2144/000113719.

33] Balasuriya D, Stewart AP, Edwardson JM. The �-1 receptor interacts directlywith GluN1 but not GluN2A in the GluN1/GluN2A NMDA receptor. J Neurosci2013;33:18219–24, http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI. 3360-13.2013.

34] Sabine S. Dissection of rodent brain regions. In: Neuroproteomics. HumanaPress; 2011. p. 13–26.

35] Kang S-U, Fuchs K, Sieghart W, Pollak A, Csaszar E, Lubec G. Gel-basedmass spectrometric analysis of a strongly hydrophobic GABAA-receptor sub-unit containing four transmembrane domains. Nat Protoc 2009;4:1093–102,http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2009.92.

36] Welinder C, Ekblad L. Coomassie staining as loading control in Westernblot analysis. J Proteome Res 2011;10:1416–9, http://dx.doi.org/10.1021/pr1011476.

37] Sase S, Khan D, Sialana F, Höger H, Russo-Schlaff N, Lubec G. Modafinil improvesperformance in the multiple T-Maze and modifies GluR1, GluR2, D2 and NR1receptor complex levels in the C57BL/6J mouse. Amino Acids 2012;43:2285–92,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00726-012-1306-y.

38] Falsafi SK, Deli A, Höger H, Pollak A, Lubec G. Scopolamine administra-tion modulates muscarinic, nicotinic and NMDA receptor systems. PloS One2012;7:e32082, http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032082.

39] Ganesan MK, Jovanovic M, Secerov B, Ignjatovic M, Bilban M, And-jus P, et al. Radiation protection from whole-body gamma irradiation(6.7 Gy): behavioural effects and brain protein-level changes by an aminoth-iol compound GL2011 in the Wistar rat. Amino Acids 2014;46:1681–96,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00726-014-1728-9.

40] Heidbreder CA, Groenewegen HJ. The medial prefrontal cortex in the rat: evi-dence for a dorso-ventral distinction based upon functional and anatomicalcharacteristics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2003;27:555–79, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.09.003.

41] Mazmanian DS, Roberts WA. Spatial memory in rats under restrictedviewing conditions. Learn Motiv 1983;14:123–39, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(83)90001-2.

42] Abraham WC, Mason SE, Demmer J, Williams JM, Richardson CL, Tate WP, et al.Correlations between immediate early gene induction and the persistence oflong-term potentiation. Neuroscience 1993;56:717–27.

43] Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Worley PF. Environment-specificexpression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensem-bles. Nat Neurosci 1999;2:1120–4, http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/16046.

44] Montag-Sallaz M, Welzl H, Kuhl D, Montag D, Schachner M. Novelty-inducedincreased expression of immediate-early genes c-fos and arg 3.1 in the mousebrain. J Neurobiol 1999;38:234–46.

45] Zhang Y, Fukushima H, Kida S. Induction and requirement of gene expres-sion in the anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex forthe consolidation of inhibitory avoidance memory. Mol Brain 2011;4:4,http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1756-6606-4-4.

46] Link W, Konietzko U, Kauselmann G, Krug M, Schwanke B, Frey U, et al. Soma-todendritic expression of an immediate early gene is regulated by synapticactivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1995;92:5734–8.

47] Lyford GL, Yamagata K, Kaufmann WE, Barnes CA, Sanders LK, CopelandNG, et al. Arc, a growth factor and activity-regulated gene, encodes a novelcytoskeleton-associated protein that is enriched in neuronal dendrites. Neuron1995;14:433–45.

48] Hasselmo ME. A model of prefrontal cortical mechanisms for goal-directedbehavior. J Cogn Neurosci 2005;17:1115–29, http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/0898929054475190.

49] Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex func-tion. Annu Rev Neurosci 2001;24:167–202, http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167.

50] Vertes RP. Interactions among the medial prefrontal cortex, hippocam-pus and midline thalamus in emotional and cognitive processing in therat. Neuroscience 2006;142:1–20, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.06.027.

51] Guzowski JF, Lyford GL, Stevenson GD, Houston FP, McGaugh JL, WorleyPF, et al. Inhibition of activity-dependent arc protein expression in the rathippocampus impairs the maintenance of long-term potentiation and theconsolidation of long-term memory. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2000;20:3993–4001.

52] Robertson HA. Immediate-early genes, neuronal plasticity, and memory.Biochem Cell Biol Biochim Biol Cell 1992;70:729–37.

53] Kaczmarek L. Molecular biology of vertebrate learning: is c-fos a newbeginning? J Neurosci Res 1993;34:377–81, http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jnr.490340402.

54] Vnek N, Rothblat LA. The hippocampus and long-term object memory in therat. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 1996;16:2780–7.

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

55] Jarrard LE. Selective hippocampal lesions and behavior: effects of kainicacid lesions on performance of place and cue tasks. Behav Neurosci1983;97:873–89.

56] Keihan Falsafi S, Ghafari M, Pollak A, Höger H, Lubec G. Hippocampal AMPA-type receptor complexes containing GluR3 and GluR4 are paralleling training

792

793

794

795

796

ING ModelB

1 ioural

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

797

798

799

800

801

802

803

804

805

806

807

808

809

810

811

812

813

814

815

816

817

818

819

820

821

822

823

824

825

826

827

828

829

830

831

832

833

834

835

836

837

838

839

840

841

842

843

844

845

846

847

848

849

850

851

852

853

854

855

856

857

858

859

860

861

862

863

864

865

866

867

868

869

ARTICLEBR 9565 1–12

2 B. Shanmugasundaram et al. / Behav

in the Multiple T-Maze. Neurochem Int 2012;60:425–30, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuint.2012.01.005.

57] Ghafari M, Falsafi SK, Hoeger H, Lubec G. Hippocampal levels of GluR1and GluR2 complexes are modulated by training in the multiple T-Mazein C57BL/6J mice. Brain Struct Funct 2012;217:353–62, http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00429-011-0335-8.

58] Saroja SR, Kim E-J, Shanmugasundaram B, Höger H, Lubec G. Hip-pocampal monoamine receptor complex levels linked to spatial mem-ory decline in the aging C57BL/6J. Behav Brain Res 2014;264:1–8,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.01.042.

59] Collingridge GL, Olsen RW, Peters J, Spedding M. A nomenclature for ligand-gated ion channels. Neuropharmacology 2009;56:2–5, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.063.

60] Mitsushima D, Ishihara K, Sano A, Kessels HW, Takahashi T. Contextuallearning requires synaptic AMPA receptor delivery in the hippocam-pus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2011;108:12503–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1104558108.

61] Rumpel S, LeDoux J, Zador A, Malinow R. Postsynaptic receptor traf-ficking underlying a form of associative learning. Science 2005;308:83–8,http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1103944.

62] Bannerman DM. Fractionating spatial memory with glutamate recep-tor subunit-knockout mice. Biochem Soc Trans 2009;37:1323–7,http://dx.doi.org/10.1042/BST0371323.

63] Freudenberg F, Marx V, Seeburg PH, Sprengel R, Celikel T. Circuit mechanisms ofGluA1-dependent spatial working memory. Hippocampus 2013;23:1359–66,http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hipo.22184.

64] Joels G, Lamprecht R. Interaction between N-ethylmaleimide-sensitive factorand GluR2 is essential for fear memory formation in lateral amygdala. J Neurosci2010;30:15981–6, http://dx.doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI. 1872-10.2010.

65] Shi S, Hayashi Y, Esteban JA, Malinow R. Subunit-specific rules governingAMPA receptor trafficking to synapses in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Cell2001;105:331–43.

66] Malinow R, Malenka RC. AMPA receptor trafficking and synaptic plastic-ity. Annu Rev Neurosci 2002;25:103–26, http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.

Please cite this article in press as: Shanmugasundaram B, et al. Frontlevel parallels spatial memory performance in the radial arm maze. Beh

neuro.25.112701.142758.67] Subramaniyan S, Heo S, Patil S, Li L, Hoger H, Pollak A, et al. A hippocam-

pal nicotinic acetylcholine alpha 7-containing receptor complex is linked tomemory retrieval in the multiple-T-maze in C57BL/6j mice. Behav Brain Res2014;270:137–45, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.05.012.

[

PRESS Brain Research xxx (2015) xxx–xxx

68] Castner SA, Smagin GN, Piser TM, Wang Y, Smith JS, Christian EP, et al.Immediate and sustained improvements in working memory after selec-tive stimulation of �7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Biol Psychiatry2011;69:12–8, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.006.

69] Boess FG, De Vry J, Erb C, Flessner T, Hendrix M, Luithle J, et al. The novel alpha7nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist N-[(3R)-1-azabicyclo[2.2.2]oct-3-yl]-7-[2-(methoxy)phenyl]-1-benzofuran-2-carboxamide improves working andrecognition memory in rodents. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2007;321:716–25,http://dx.doi.org/10.1124/jpet.106.118976.

70] Huang Y-Y, Kandel ER. Age-related enhancement of a protein synthesis-dependent late phase of LTP induced by low frequency paired-pulsestimulation in hippocampus. Learn Mem Cold Spring Harb N 2006;13:298–306,http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.166906.

71] Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LRM, Izquierdo I, Medina JH, Cammarota M. Dopaminecontrols persistence of long-term memory storage. Science 2009;325:1017–20,http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.1172545.

72] Thompson JL, Rosell DR, Slifstein M, Girgis RR, Xu X, Ehrlich Y, et al. Pre-frontal dopamine D1 receptors and working memory in schizotypal personalitydisorder: a PET study with [(11)C]NNC112. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2014,http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-014-3566-6.

73] Kebir O, Tabbane K. Working memory in schizophrenia: a review. L’Encéphale2008;34:289–98, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.encep.2006.12.008.

74] Aultman JM, Moghaddam B. Distinct contributions of glutamate and dopaminereceptors to temporal aspects of rodent working memory using a clinicallyrelevant task. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2001;153:353–64.

75] Sarantis K, Matsokis N, Angelatou F. Synergistic interactions of dopamineD1 and glutamate NMDA receptors in rat hippocampus and prefrontalcortex: Involvement of ERK1/2 signaling. Neuroscience 2009;163:1135–45,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.07.056.

76] Kruse MS, Prémont J, Krebs M-O, Jay TM. Interaction of dopamine D1 withNMDA NR1 receptors in rat prefrontal cortex. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol2009;19:296–304, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2008.12.006.

77] Lee FJS, Xue S, Pei L, Vukusic B, Chéry N, Wang Y, et al. Dualregulation of NMDA receptor functions by direct protein-proteininteractions with the dopamine D1 receptor. Cell 2002;111:219–30,

al cortex and hippocampus neurotransmitter receptor complexav Brain Res (2015), http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.043

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0092-8674(02)00962-5.78] Nai Q, Li S, Wang S-H, Liu J, Lee FJS, Frankland PW, et al. Uncoupling the D1-

N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor complex promotes NMDA-dependentlong-term potentiation and working memory. Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:246–54,http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.011.

870

871

872

873

874