Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the green anole lizard,...

10
GENERAL AND COMPARATIVE ENDOCRINOLOGY 77,2462.55 (1990) Endocrine and Behavioral Responses to Aggression and Social Dominance in the Green Anole Lizard, Anolis carolinensis NEIL GREENBERG* AND DAVID CREWS? *Department of Zoology and Life Sciences Graduate Program in Ethology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37996, and tlnstitute of Reproductive Biology, Department of Zoology, University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712 Accepted April 18, 1989 Adult males of the small arboreal iguanid lizard, Anolis carolinensis, will fight and form social dominance hierarchies when placed in habitats with limited resources. The relation- ships between time since initial aggressive interaction, relative social dominance, reproduc- tive activity, and corticosterone and androgen levels were determined for 34 pairs of lizards. A discriminant analysis established a “dominance index” which indicated that over 90% of the difference between individuals who had won or lost aggressive interactions (putative social dominants and subordinates) was attributable to a single discriminant function re- flecting altered body color, perch site selection, and circulating androgen. Animals that had darker body color also selected lower perch sites and had depressed rates of courtship relative to winners of tights and were thus designated as social subordinates. These animals also had levels of circulating androgen significantly lower than that of dominants, but cir- culating corticosterone was not significantly affected. Winners of tights showed a dramatic surge in circulating androgen at 1 hr but returned to near control values by 1 week; losers, however, showed depressed circulating androgen levels at 1 week. o 1990 Academic PW, IK. Aggressive behavior, social dominance, and reproductive activity have been associ- ated with adrenal and other physiological responses to environmental stressors in many vertebrates, including several reptiles (Greenberg and Wingfield, 1987). The inter- actions between these responsive systems and behavior indicate that their underlying physiological substrate involves hormones that have multiple effects. For example, hormones associated with activation of the adrenal axis can also enhance acquisition and delay extinction of active avoidance behavior (Bohus and de Wied, 1980) and hormones associated with gonadal activa- tion can directly affect sensory-perceptual functions (Beach, 1974; Gandelman, 1983) as well as affecting attention either directly (Andrew, 1972) or by means of interactions with adrenocortical hormones (Oades, 1979). One of the primary ambitions of ex- perimental behavioral endocrinology is to determine the manner in which such multi- ple effects are integrated and coordinated with each other and the environment. The green anole lizard, Anolis carolinen- sis, is uniquely suited to the investigation of the manner in which environmental stimuli and physiological variables are integrated in the control of agonistic behavior. The dermal chromatophores of this species have no direct sympathetic innervation (Kleinholz, 1938), but are responsive to cir- culating hormones associated with physio- logical stress such as melanotropin (Green- berg et al., 1986) and epinphrine, thus per- mitting body color to be utilized as a relatively direct indication of an endocrine response to environmental stressors (Greenberg and Crews, 1983). Studies on A. carolinensis have documented body color changes in both agonistic interactions and long-term relationships (Greenberg et al., 1984). For example, the males that ini- 246 0016~6480&O $1.50 Copyright 0 1990 by Academic Press, Inc. AU rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

Transcript of Endocrine and behavioral responses to aggression and social dominance in the green anole lizard,...

GENERAL AND COMPARATIVE ENDOCRINOLOGY 77,2462.55 (1990)

Endocrine and Behavioral Responses to Aggression and Social Dominance in the Green Anole Lizard, Anolis carolinensis

NEIL GREENBERG* AND DAVID CREWS?

*Department of Zoology and Life Sciences Graduate Program in Ethology, University of Tennessee, Knoxville, Tennessee 37996, and tlnstitute of Reproductive Biology, Department of Zoology, University of

Texas, Austin, Texas 78712

Accepted April 18, 1989

Adult males of the small arboreal iguanid lizard, Anolis carolinensis, will fight and form social dominance hierarchies when placed in habitats with limited resources. The relation- ships between time since initial aggressive interaction, relative social dominance, reproduc- tive activity, and corticosterone and androgen levels were determined for 34 pairs of lizards. A discriminant analysis established a “dominance index” which indicated that over 90% of the difference between individuals who had won or lost aggressive interactions (putative social dominants and subordinates) was attributable to a single discriminant function re- flecting altered body color, perch site selection, and circulating androgen. Animals that had darker body color also selected lower perch sites and had depressed rates of courtship relative to winners of tights and were thus designated as social subordinates. These animals also had levels of circulating androgen significantly lower than that of dominants, but cir- culating corticosterone was not significantly affected. Winners of tights showed a dramatic surge in circulating androgen at 1 hr but returned to near control values by 1 week; losers, however, showed depressed circulating androgen levels at 1 week. o 1990 Academic PW, IK.

Aggressive behavior, social dominance, and reproductive activity have been associ- ated with adrenal and other physiological responses to environmental stressors in many vertebrates, including several reptiles (Greenberg and Wingfield, 1987). The inter- actions between these responsive systems and behavior indicate that their underlying physiological substrate involves hormones that have multiple effects. For example, hormones associated with activation of the adrenal axis can also enhance acquisition and delay extinction of active avoidance behavior (Bohus and de Wied, 1980) and hormones associated with gonadal activa- tion can directly affect sensory-perceptual functions (Beach, 1974; Gandelman, 1983) as well as affecting attention either directly (Andrew, 1972) or by means of interactions with adrenocortical hormones (Oades, 1979). One of the primary ambitions of ex- perimental behavioral endocrinology is to

determine the manner in which such multi- ple effects are integrated and coordinated with each other and the environment.

The green anole lizard, Anolis carolinen- sis, is uniquely suited to the investigation of the manner in which environmental stimuli and physiological variables are integrated in the control of agonistic behavior. The dermal chromatophores of this species have no direct sympathetic innervation (Kleinholz, 1938), but are responsive to cir- culating hormones associated with physio- logical stress such as melanotropin (Green- berg et al., 1986) and epinphrine, thus per- mitting body color to be utilized as a relatively direct indication of an endocrine response to environmental stressors (Greenberg and Crews, 1983). Studies on A. carolinensis have documented body color changes in both agonistic interactions and long-term relationships (Greenberg et al., 1984). For example, the males that ini-

246 0016~6480&O $1.50 Copyright 0 1990 by Academic Press, Inc. AU rights of reproduction in any form reserved.

RESPONSES TO AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE 247

tiate almost all sexual activity are charac- teristically completely green while subordi- nate, sexually inactive males are typically brown.

This study was undertaken to determine the association between the outcome of an agonistic interaction, subsequent social dominance, reproductive behavior, and an- drogen and corticosterone levels in the liz- ard, Anolis carolinensis, at different points in time after an initial interaction.

METHODS

Adult A. carolinensis were commercially supplied by the Snake Farm, LaPlace, Louisiana. When re- ceived, animals were placed in habitats under condi- tions known to initiate and maintain gonadal recrudes- cence throughout the year (Crews and Garrick, 1970; Crews et al., 1974; Licht, 1967, 1971). The stimulatory regimen had a photic cycle of 14 hr light:10 hr dark, and provided corresponding daily temperature fluctu- ations of 32:22”. Temperature is known to be a signif- icant variable in the responses of lizard adrenal to ACTH (Licht and Bradshaw, 1969), on spermatogen- esis (Licht, 1971), and in the production and action of androgens (Pearson er al., 1976). Humidity varied in- versely with temperature ranging from 70 (day) to 90% (night) RH.

Behavioral techniques. Prior to behavioral experi- mentation, 68 adult males (minimum size, 61 mm snout-to-vent) were individually housed for a mini- mum of 3 weeks in glass vivaria (25 x 25 x 30 cm) provided with 20-cm diagonal wooden perches and a substrate of sphagnum moss. Food consisted of com- mercially supplied l-cm crickets which maintained alertness in the lizards when provided on alternate days and allowed to hide in the moss. Handling was strictly minimized to eliminate this potential stressor (Meier et al., 1973).

Each lizard was briefly exposed to individual recep- tive females and intruding males to determine disposi- tion to court or to manifest territorial aggression; tests were halted in 5 min or if copulation or a tight seemed imminent. Each was also characterized along two di- mensions associated with physiological stress or social status: body color and perch-site selection (Greenberg et al., 1984). The range of variability manifest for each of these dimensions was divided into several clearly delineated units and was represented on an ordinal scale based upon their sequence of expression in an agonistic situation (Table 1). Because the expression of body color is also sensitive to ambient light reflec- tance and temperature, habitats were provided with uniform illumination levels, background color, and

temperature in order to minimize direct, nonsocial sources of variability in the responses of chromato- phores to circulating hormones.

Pairs of individuals randomly selected to be subjects were introduced to each other by removing the opaque divider separating two connecting vivaria. The distinc- tive displays exchanged between subjects (Greenberg, 1977), including body color changes indicative of adre- nal activation (Greenberg, 1983), were documented; the pairs were left together in the enclosure and body color and perch-site selection were recorded twice daily for up to 1 week.

The terms “winner” or “loser” designate subjects that (1) engaged in an agonistic encounter and mani- fested a postorbital “eyespot,” indicating adrenal epi- nephrine release (Greenberg, 1983), and (2) performed displays that included sagittal expansion of the body profile, a posture unique to conspecific aggression (Greenberg, 1977). Losers were identified by (1) their cessation of display and (2) exclusion from higher perch sites, the adoption of a head-down or cryptic posture, or attempts at flight. The terms “dominant” and “subordinate” are used to designate subjects dur- ing the period of cohabitation subsequent to the initial agonistic encounter. Relative social dominance was determined by daily observations of staged feeding or female introduction episodes in which subjects mani- fested priority of access to food or females in at least 75% of observations. Further, submissiveness was in- dicated by observations of supplantation from perch site, or withdrawal from an arena of activity when the other lizard displays or approaches. Subordinate be- havior was manifest in all instances of exposure to a display and approximately 25% of the observed in- stances of the other lizard’s approach. Dominant indi- viduals, unlike subordinates, will not withdraw or be supplanted from a perch site when approached. In the conditions provided, winners of tights invariably be- come social dominants.

Daily observations of body color and perch-site se- lection were recorded on a hand-held Hewlett-Packard 51B computer. Staged agonistic and courtship epi- sodes were documented with an electromechanical event recorder (Esterline Angus 20-channel recorder); observations of feeding and social dominance interac- tions were recorded by pen. Data were then trans- ferred or transcribed onto an IBM 5 150 computer and subjected to analysis using SAS. Sexual behavior after the introduction of a receptive female was character- ized by rapid nodding (Table 1), a key element of courtship behavior (Crews, 1975; Greenberg, 1977). Observer effects on subjects were minimized by means of closed-circuit color television surveillance.

Hormone assays. Blood samples were taken by rapid decapitation at 1 hr, 1 day, or 1 week following the agonistic interaction. The procedure was per- formed between 1400 and 1600, times at which corti-

248 GREENBERG AND CREWS

TABLE 1 SCORING PROTOCOL FOR BODY COLOR, SITE SELECTION, AND SOCIAL DISPLAY BEHAVIOR IN GREEN ANOLE

LIZARDS, ANOLIS CAROLINENSM

Color index: An ordinal scale of body colors progressively observed during agonistic encounters or manifested in experimentally stressed lizards.

1. (G) all visible pigmented body surfaces a light to moderate green. 2. (GB) part green/part brown. 3. (B) all visible pigmented body surfaces a light to moderate brown. 4. (DB) more than 50% of the body surface is dark brown. 5. (DB/ES) dark brown with an eyespot (ES), a black spot near the posterior margin of the orbit. 6. (G/ES) all visible pigmented body surfaces moderate to dark green with eyespot. 7. (G/B/ES) body color is blotchy, irregularly pigmented dark green and dark brown, with eyespot.

Sire selection: An ordinal scale of perching sites, ranging from optimal predatory surveillance sites through degree of displacement in response to aggression or social dominance behavior by another male.

1. top of perch, favored predatory/social surveillance site. 2. upper half of perch. 3. lower half of perch. 4. on substrate; sustained presence, not foraging for prey. 5. covert perching; hiding; squirreled around perch, in crevice, or out of line of sight of other lizard. 6. burrowed into substrate; frequently with head only above surface.

Display behavior: Units of male behavior observed in contexts of nonspecific disturbance, intrusion or activity of a conspecific males, or intrusion or activity of a female.

AD pushup/dewlap only (vertical movements of anterior body with brief extension of red dewlap; constitutes the 2- to 4-set “assertion” display, response to a broad spectrum of perturbations)

ET extended throat (unique element of “threat” display, sustained expansion of the throat profile, response to possible conspecific intrusion)

SE sagittal expansion (unique element of “challenge” display, sustained posture incorporating ET and displaying expanded lateral profile, response to active conspecific intruder)

RN rapid nod of head (unique feature of “courtship” response to female; typically followed by approach and attempt at copulation)

D Instantaneous scan or 30-set indent sampling of three 4-hr time bins during photoperiod (first and last hours not counted); dyad tests conducted for a minimum of 5 min.

costerone levels are known to be relatively high (Green and Greenberg, unpublished data). Total time betwen the initial disturbance and decapitation was less than 20 sec. Total androgen (ANDR) and cortico- sterone (CS) were chromatographically separated and their circulating levels were then determined by radio- immunoassay using established procedures (Moore et al., 1985; Whittier et al., 1987). The intraassay coefft- cient of variation (CV) and the interassay CV were 6.0 and 7.0%, respectively, for total androgens, and 3.3 and 13.0% for corticosterone. Accuracy values aver- aged 95.9% for androgens and 86.2% for corticoste- rone.

Statistical procedures. Analyses of the contribution of the variables documented to the differences be- tween groups (“winners/dominants” and “losers/ subordinates”) employed multiple discriminant analy- sis (Pimental and Frey, 1978). Discriminant scores were used as a continuous measure of dominance, the

rationale being that those characters most effective in distinguishing between or characterizing behaviors of animals in two conditions will be most heavily weighted by the multiple analysis. This procedure ef- fectively maximizes the distances between two popu- lations (winner/loser or dominant/subordinate) for the variables documented. A dominance index (DI) was derived by multiplying each variable documented by its respective canonical coefftcient (color-dif, + 0.17; site-dif, + 1.04; log CS, -0.09; log ANDR, -0.58).

The behavioral variables, body color and perch-site selection, were represented as the difference between the average pre- and postencounter values to minimize the variability between individuals. The signitlcances of hormonal differences attributable to outcomes of encounters and social dominance were inferred by contrasting the sampled hormone levels manifest in each group with that of a control group randomly se- lected from the same pool of subjects prior to experi-

RESPONSES TO AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE 249

mentation. The endocrine variables, nanograms per milliliter of plasma ANDR and CS, were log trans- formed for statistical analysis. The significance of dif- ferences between color and site changes and the hor- mone levels of the winners and losers at the three sam- pling intervals was analyzed by Welch’s modified t test. The significance of hormone changes following the initial interaction was determined by MANOVA; courtship data was analyzed by Fisher’s exact proba- bility test.

RESULTS

Body color and perch-site selection were immediately and obviously affected by fighting; circulating levels of ANDR, but not of CS, were also affected as revealed by the hormone assays.

Dominance index. A multiple discrimi- nant analysis indicated that 91% of the dif- ference between groups could be accounted for by a single discriminant function (ca- nonical variable). This represents an index of relative social dominance (DI) and re- flects the confidence with which the con- stituent variables of the index could be used to distinguish a dominant from a subordi- nate individual. The significance of the in- dex (Z’ < 0.0001, Roy’s greatest root) is at- tributable primarily to the contributions of three of the four variables: perch height change, body color change, and circulating ANDR levels; CS levels did not contribute significantly to the index.

Color differences. Lizards that won fights showed little or no change in color from that seen before the encounter. Los- ers, on the other hand, showed some vari- ability in the first hour following a fight, but generally were darker in body color than in their preencounter period (Fig. 1). Because of their initial variability, the body color scores of losers were significantly lower (indicating darker) than their prefight scores only at 1 day and 1 week. The dif- ference in body color between winners and losers, however, was significant at all times (1 hr, F = 6.21, prob > F = 0.022; 1 day, F = 10.21, prob > F = 0.007; 1 week, F = 10.61, prob > F = 0.0035). The correlation

h-l 0 Dominant 0.2

- Subordinate LIGHTER

0.0

-0.2

-0 4

-0 6 DARKER

I . I

ONEHOUR ONE DAY ONE WEEK

FIG. 1. Changes in body color scores (see Table 1) following aggressive interaction in cohabiting green anole lizards A. cnrolinensis sampled at 1 hr (n = 12 pairs), 1 day (n = 10 pairs), and 1 week (n = 15 pairs) following an aggressive interaction. Positive changes indicate lighter color, negative changes indicate darker.

between the extent of color change and the dominance index (the first discriminant function) was 0.62.

Site differences. Following fights, changes in site-selection scores (site-dif) were evident as winners adopted higher perch sites and losers adopted lower ones (Fig. 2). The difference in perch-site scores between winners and losers was significant at all sampling intervals (1 hr, F = 20.18, prob > F = 0.0002; 1 day, F = 8.30, prob > F = 0.0129; 1 week, F = 35.07, prob > F = 0.0001); however, the change from prefight levels was significant for winners only at 1 hr and 1 day. The postfight perch-

.” L- I

0.5 HffiHER

0.0

-0 5

-1 .o

-1.5 LOWER

-2.0 ! I ONE HOUR ONE DAY ONEWEEK

FIG. 2. Changes in perch-site selection scores (see Table 1) following aggressive interaction in cohabiting green anole lizards A. carolinensis sampled at 1 hr (n = 12 pairs), 1 day (n = 10 pairs), and 1 week (n = 15 pairs) following an aggressive interaction. Positive changes indicate higher perch selected, negative changes indicate lower.

250 GREENBERG AND CREWS

site scores of losers indicated that the adop- tion of perch sites was significantly lower than their prefight sites at 1 day and 1 week. The correlation between the extent of site change and the dominance index was 0.93, indicating a large contribution of this vari- able to the distinction between losers and winners following a fight.

Reproductive activity. Both winners and losers recovered some courtship activity af- ter fighting as indicated by the number of displays incorporating rapid nods, but win- ners had higher levels than losers at all times tested, attaining sexual activity levels comparable to their prefight levels by 1 week (Fig. 3). This was significantly higher than the activity of losers (Fisher’s exact test, P = 0.026).

At 1 hr postfight, winners displayed 66% of their prefight level of courtship activity while none of the losers courted. One day following a fight, winners and losers courted at 77 and 15% of that group’s pre- tight levels, respectively. One week follow- ing a fight, winners manifested 100% of their pretight levels of courtship head nods, while only 12 of the losers performed this display at prefight levels in response to in- troduced females.

Androgen levels. The mean level of cir- culating total ANDR in winners was higher than that found in control subjects at all sampling times, but the elevation was sig- nificant only 1 hr (P = 0.003 Roy’s greatest

100 SUBORDINATE

ii *O 2 60

B g 40

20

ONE WR ONE DAY ONEWEEK

FIG. 3. Percentage of control values of courtship behavior shown by subjects in each group during the hour (n = 9 pairs), day (n = 10 pairs), or week (n = 15 pairs) following an aggressive interaction.

root). The mean levels of circulating andro- gen in losers, while slightly depressed, were not significantly lower at any sam- pling time from that of control subjects. However, because of this slight depression in losers, the difference in mean hormone levels between winners and losers was sig- nificantly different at 1 hr and 1 week after combat, but not at 1 day (Table 2). The overall correlation between ANDR levels and the dominance index was -0.66.

Corticosterone levels. The mean levels of circulating CS in either winning or losing subjects were not significantly different from those of control subjects at any of the sampling times. Further, there were no sig- nificant differences between winners and losers at different times (Table 2). The overall correlation between CS and the dominance index was -0.12.

DISCUSSION

Following an initial aggressive interac- tion, winning and losing green anole lizards manifested significant differences in body color, perch-site selection, and circulating levels of androgen, but not in the circulat- ing concentrations of CS. These differences were generally sustained through the sub- sequent period of cohabitation and were as- sociated with the relative social dominance of individual lizards.

Behavior. The agonistic interactions be- tween adult male green anole lizards in- volve characteristic exchanges of mutual stalking, display, and body color change (Greenberg, 1977). The changes in color are relatively rapid and indicate fluctuating lev- els or availability of several chroma- totrophic hormones, all of which are also associated with effects on behavior as well as physiological response to environmental stressors (Greenberg and Crews, 1983). Lizards that lost agonistic interactions manifested body color darkening, fre- quently several seconds before any obvious action indicated the outcome (Sigmund,

RESPONSES TO AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE 251

TABLE 2 RELATIVE HORMONE LEVELS OF CORTICOSTERONE (CS) AND ANDROGEN (ANDR) IN WINNERS AND LOSERS

OF AGONISTIC ENCOUNTERS

Variable

Winners/dominants Losers/subordinates

Levels (n) Levels (n) F” F’rob > F

Corticosterone (% of control level)* 1 hr 1 day 1 week

Androgen (% of control levels)’ 1 hr 1 day 1 week

124% (11) 149% (11) 0.02 0.89 172% (8) 131% (8) 0.33 0.57 107% (13) 109% (12) 0.49 0.49

470% (12) 81% (10) 8.78 0.008 127% (8) 94% (9) 0.13 0.73 104% (14) 60% (12) 8.46 0.008

’ rig/ml log transformed before F statistic calculated. b Mean control level of CS = 6.82 r&ml (SEM = 1.45) (n = 9). c Mean control level of ANDR = 1.64 r&ml (SEM = 0.55) (n = 9).

1978). Compared to their prelight behavior, losers were also found at lower perch sites, an indication of reduced social dominance (Greenberg et al., 1984).

The darker body color manifest by sub- ordinate A. carolinensis was associated in earlier studies (Greenberg et al., 1984) with a physiological stress response. This dark- ening is probably attributable to increased availability of the pituitary peptide, melan- otropin (MSH), closely related to ACTH and known to parallel its release in re- sponse to CRF (Proulx-Ferland et al., 1982). Recently, the level of circulating MSH in A. carolinensis has been found to be sensitive to social and environmental stressors (Greenberg and Chen, 1987). It is reduced in response to the acute stress as- sociated with aggression, possibly because of catecholamine suppression of its release from the pituitary, but significantly ele- vated in long-term social subordination (Greenberg et al., 1986).

Courtship behavior was also markedly reduced in losers at all sampling intervals following an interaction; winners showed a slight decrease following a fight but at- tained prefight levels of activity within 1 week. Females were receptive, as indicated by their expression of slow head nods (Greenberg, 1977), even in response to sub-

ordinates. There was no indication that fe- males discriminated males on the basis of body color or were differentially responsive to dominants and subordinates, but they did appear more responsive to more active males, typically the dominant.

Androgen levels. The most dramatic dif- ference between groups was the high circu- lating androgen levels in winners sampled at 1 hr. The levels of circulating androgen were also significantly greater in winners than in losers at 1 week postencounter. This finding recalls the short-term fluctua- tions in the ANDR levels associated with changes in behavior following the aggres- sive encounters of male songbirds (Wing- field and Ramenofsky, 1987; Wingfield and Moore, 1987), which are most apparent during the period in which social relation- ships are being established (Wingfield et al., 1987). In male mountain spiny lizards, Sceloporus jarrovi, on the other hand, al- though behavior following agonistic inter- actions is altered, Moore (1988) found no detectable changes in circulating levels of testosterone. The studies of S. jarrovi, however, were different from those of A. carolinensis in several significant details apart from the species difference. For ex- ample, in the study of S. jarrovi, territorial aggression was elicited from free-living

252 GREENBERG AND CREWS

males in the field by tethered intruders that were removed immediately after the inter- action.

It appears that at least in some contexts, physiological stress and a depressed hypo- thalamic-pituitary-gonadal axis are not in- variably linked. In song sparrows, CS treat- ment does not affect plasma levels of LH, and gonads are maintained in a near- functional state, enabling a rapid resump- tion of reproductive activity as soon as the adrenal stress response abates (Wingfield and Silverin, 1986). Such a response might be of considerable utility to social subordi- nates in a community where mortality of dominants is high, possibly due to their more conspicuous body color and perch site selection.

Interestingly, there is an apparent corre- spondence between androgen and perch heights at which lizards are found. At the time androgen is highest, winners are found at significantly higher perches. Subordi- nates, on the other hand, show the lowest perch height selection scores at 1 week, the time at which androgen is most depressed. This observation is consistent with others on this species: isolated castrated males, fe- males, and juveniles will spontaneously perch at lower heights than isolated intact males (Greenberg and Hake, 1990; Green- berg, unpublished data) as will castrated males when they win aggressive interac- tions (Greenberg et al., 1984). Such obser- vations indicate that endocrine factors may be as significant a variable in microhabitat selection as is social experience.

Previous workers found that increased testis weight and elevated plasma testoster- one levels were associated with dominance behavior in three of six green anoles (Pear- son et al., 1976). In long-term (4 month) undisturbed hierarchies, however, the an- drogen levels of dominants were not higher than those of subordinates (cited in Pearson et al., 1976). In the present study, ANDR levels of dominants at 1 week were similar to levels seen in the control group, but at

this time the subordinates showed a level of circulating ANDR well below that of con- trols. The control value of ANDR (mean, 1.64; SEM, 0.55) is lower than that seen in another study of captive Anolis (Crews et al., 1974), but in the earlier study, the males were long-term experimental animals sub- jected to daily tests of courtship and aggres- sion. While the results indicate that social experience affects levels of circulating ANDR, we cannot discount the possibility that ANDR was highly elevated in future winners of fights. The control values of ANDR, however, were determined in indi- viduals randomly selected from the same pool of subjects as those that interacted so- cially.

Subordinate green anoles were also re- productively inactive, perhaps correspond- ing to reduced ANDR levels, although there is no direct evidence on how low an- drogen levels must be before reproductive dysfunctions are manifest. An elevated cir- culating androgen level is often positively correlated with reproductive activity (Crews and Silver, 1980) and its depression is associated with physological stress (Greenberg and Wingfield, 1987). This could be due to a cross-system feedback inhibition of gonadotropin attributable to adrenal androgen (Kime et al., 1980; and see Christian, 1980) secondary to increased adrenal activity, a classic indicator of the physiological stress response. Alterna- tively, there could be an altered sensitivity of the testes to gonadotropin.

Corticosterone levels. There was a slight but comparable elevation in CS levels in both winners and losers. This is in contrast to an earlier study (Greenberg et al., 1986) in which CS levels were significantly ele- vated in subordinate males cohabiting with a dominant following an initial aggressive encounter. There are several possible rea- sons for the present finding of relative “stability” of CS levels within and between the groups at varying times and in previous research: First, in the present study, no

RESPONSES TO AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE 253

more than 1 week had passed before blood sampling, while the subordinates in the ear- lier study were sampled after at least 3 weeks. In a study of rats, the response of CS to chronic environmental stressors was constant for the first 2 weeks but increased markedly in subsequent weeks (Vogel and Jensch, 1988). Second, short-term CS fluc- tuations might be masked by already mildly elevated levels in both groups as a function of captivity (Grassman and Crews, 1987). In baboons, comparable elevations in the levels of glucocorticoids are shown by both dominants and subordinates in response to stress, although dominants have lower basal concentrations (Sapolsky, 1986). Third, in the present study, lizards were subject to the perturbation of courtship tests during the period of cohabitation; in the earlier study in which a CS increase in subordinates was observed, individuals were undisturbed for the entire period of cohabitation.

An inhibitory effect of CS on male ag- gressive behavior and testicular function was observed by Tokarz (1987) in a conge- ner, A. sagrei. The effective subordination of A. carolinensis in the absence of a dis- tinct CS change in the present study, how- ever, indicates that the CS-related inhibi- tion of aggression seen by Tokarz is part of a more complex ensemble of adaptive re- sponses to a specific form of acute stress.

The CS response of A. carolinensis, while slight, might have been sufficient to activate a mechanism similar to that de- scribed in squirrel monkeys. When mon- keys were allowed to establish social rela- tions, plasma cortisol levels rose in all sub- jects, but at 24 hr (but not 3 hr or 1 week), only dominant males showed increases in testosterone (Coe et al., 1982). In the first hour following the acute stress of rapid cap- ture and immobilization, high-ranking male baboons showed an increase in ANDR lev- els while subordinate males responded by declines (Sapolsky, 1986). In both cases, luteinizing hormone concentrations were

similarly suppressed, indicating a periph- eral mechanism for the effect of acute stress on dominants. This was attributed in part to reduced sensitivity of tastes of tes- tes of high-ranking males to glucocorticoid suppression. However, Sapolsky’s (1986) findings of attenuated transient ANDR rise in dominants following stress-induced cate- cholamine blockade indicate that sympa- thetic epinephrine and norepinephrine are acting peripherally to increase ANDR con- centrations, possibly by means of altered blood circulation through the testes and/or direct stimulation of ANDR release. In pigs, exogenous ACTH can stimulate a sig- nificant and relatively rapid but transient elevation in testicular testosterone secre- tion by means of a mechanism that does not involve pituitary gonadotropins (Fenske et al., 1981).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We gratefully acknowledge the expert technical help of Yuki Morris with RIA procedures, the advice on statistics of Dr. Ralph O’Brien, and the laboratory as- sistance of Donna Layne. This work was made possi- ble by NSF Grant BNS-8406028 and a University of Tennessee Faculty Research Award to N. Greenberg and MH41770 and RSA MH00135 to D. Crews.

REFERENCES

Andrew, R. J. (1972). Recognition processes and be- havior with special reference to effects of testos- terone of persistence. In “Advances in the Study of Behavior” (D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, and E. Shaw, Eds.), Vol. 4, pp. 175-208. Academic Press, New York.

Beach, F. A. (1974). Behavioral endocrinology and the study of reproduction. Biol. Reprod. 10, 2-18.

Bohus, B., and de Wied (1980). Pituitary-adrenal sys- tem hormones and adaptive behavior. In “Gen- eral, Comparative, and Clinical Endocrinology of the Adrenal Cortex” (I. Chester Jones and I. W. Henderson, Eds.), Vol. 3, pp. 256-347. Academic Press, New York.

Christian, J. J. (1980). Endocrine factors in population regulation. In “Biosocial Mechanisms of Popula- tion Regulation” (M. N. Cohen, R. S. Malpass, and H. G. Klein, Eds.), pp. 55-115. Yale Univ. Press, New Haven.

254 GREENBERG AND CREWS

Coe, C. L., Frankhn, D., Smith, E. R., and Levine, S. (1982). Hormonal responses accompanying fear and agitation in the squirrel monkey. Physiol. Be- hav. 29, 1051-1057.

Coe, C. L. , Mendoza, S. P., and Levine S. (1979). Social status constrains the stress response in the squirrel monkey. Physiol. Behav. 23, 633-638.

Crews, D. (1975). Inter- and intraindividual variation in display patterns in the lizard, Anolis carolinen- sis. Herpetologica 31, 37-47.

Crews, D., and Garrick, L. D. (1970). Methods of in- ducing reproduction in captive reptiles. In “Re- productive Biology and Diseases of Captive Reptiles” (J. B. Murphy and J. T. Collins, Eds.), pp. 4%70. Sot. Stud. Amphib. Rept., Miami Uni- versity, Oxford, Ohio.

Crews, D., Rosenblatt, D. S., and Lehrman, D. S. (1974). Effects of unseasonal environmental regi- men, group presence, group composition, and male’s physiological state on ovarian recrudes- cence in the lizard, Anolis carolinesis. Endocri- nology 94, 541-547.

Crews, D., and Silver, R. (1980). Reproductive phys- iology-behavior interactions in nonmammalian vertebrates. In “Handbook for Behavioral Neurobiology” (R. W. Coy and D. W. Pfaff, Eds.). Plenum, New York.

Fenske, M., Holtz, W., Pitzel, L., and Konig, A. (1981). Effects of ACTH-induced testosterone re- lease on the secretion of pituitary gonadotrophin and prolactin release in male pigs. In “Gonadal Steroids and Brain Function, Exp. Brain Res. Suppl. 3” (W. Wuttke and R. Harowski, Eds.), pp. 342-343. Springer-Verlag, New York.

Gandelman, R. (1983). Gonadal hormones and sensory function. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 7, l-17.

Grassman, M., and Crews, D. (1987). Dominance and reproduction in an all-female lizard species. Be- hav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 21, 141-147.

Greenberg, N. (1977). A neuroethological study of dis- play behavior in the lizard Anolis carolinensis (Reptilian, Lacertilia, Iquanidae). Amer. Zool. 17, 191-201.

Greenberg, N. (1983). Central and autonomic aspects of aggression and dominance in reptiles. In “Ad- vances in Vertebrate Neuroethology” (J.-P. Ewert, R. R. Capranica, and D. J. Ingle, Eds.), pp. 1135-l 144. Plenum, New York.

Greenberg, N., and Chen, T. (1987). Aggression and social submissiveness alter melanotropin (MSH) in the lizard. Amer. Zool. 27(4), 49A. [Abstract]

Greenberg, N., and Crews, D. (1983). Physiological ethology of aggression in amphibians and rep- tiles. In “Hormones and Aggressive Behavior” (B. Svare, Ed.), pp. 46%506. Plenum, New York.

Greenberg, N., and Hake, L. (1990). Hatching and

neonatal behavior of the lizard, Anolis carolinen- sis. J. Herpetol. in press.

Greenberg, N., and Wingfield, J. C. (1987). Stress and reproduction: Reciprocal relationships. In “Hor- mones and Reproduction in Fishes, Amphibians, and Reptiles” (D. 0. Norris and R. E. Jones, Eds.), pp. 461-503. Plenum, New York.

Greenberg, N., Chen, T., and Crews, D. (1984). Social status, gonadal state, and the adrenal stress re- sponse in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Horm. Behav. 18, l-l 1.

Greenberg, N., Chen, T., and Vaughan, G. (1986). Melanotropin is altered by acute and chronic so- cial stress in lizards. Sot. Neurosci. Abstracts 2, 834. [Abstract]

Hadley, M. E., and Goldman, J. M. (1969). Physiolog- ical color changes in reptiles. Amer. Zool. 9(2), 489-504.

Kime, D. E., Vinson, G. P., Major, P. W., and Kil- patrick, R. (1980). Adrenal-gonadal relationships. In “General, Comparative, and Clinical Endocri- nology of the Adrenal Cortex” (I. Chester Jones and I. W. Henderson, Eds.), Vol. 3, pp. 133-264. Academic Press, New York.

Kleinholz, L. H. (1938). Studies in reptilian color change. III. Control of light phase and behavior of isolated skin. J. Exp. Zool. 15, 492-499.

Leshner, A. I., and Politch, J. A. (1979). Hormonal control of submissiveness in mice: Irrelevance of the androgens and relevance of the pituitary adre- nal hormones. Physiol. Behav. 22, 531-534.

Licht, P. (1967). Environmental control of annual tes- ticular cycles in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. In- teraction of light and temperature in the initiation of testicular recrudescence. J. Exp. Zool. 165, 505616.

Licht, P. (1971). Regulation of the annual testis cycle of photoperiod and temperature in the lizards, An- olis carolinensis. Ecology 52, 240-252.

Meier, A. H., Trobec, T. N., Haymaker, R., Mac- Gregar, R., III, and Russo, A. C. (1973). Daily variations in the effects of handling on fat storage and testicular weight in several vertebrates. J. Exp. Zool. 184, 281-288.

Moore, M. C., Whittier, J. M., and Crews, D. (1985). Sex steroid hormones during the ovarian cycle of an all-female, parthenogenetic lizard and their correlation with pseudosexual behavior. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 60, 144-153.

Nie, N. H., Hull, C. H., Jenkins, J. G., Steinbrenner, K., and Bent, D. H. (1975). “Statistical Package for the Social Sciences,” 2nd ed. McGraw-Hill, New York.

Oades, R. D. (1979). Search and attention: Interac- tions of the hippocampal-septal axis, adrenocorti- cal and gonadal hormones. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 3, 31-48.

RESPONSES TO AGGRESSION AND DOMINANCE 255

Pearson, A. K., Tsui, H. W., and Licht, P. (1976). Effect of temperatures on spermatogenesis, on the production and action of androgens and on the ultrastructure of gonadotropic cells in the lizard Anolis carolinensis. J. Exp. Zool. 195, 291-304.

Pimentel, R. A., and Frey, D. F. (1978). Multivariate analysis of variance and discriminant analysis. In “Quantitative Ethology” (P. W. Colgan, Ed.), pp. 247-274. Wiley, New York.

Proulx-Ferland, L., Labrie, F., Dumont, D., and Cote, J. (1982). Corticotropin-releasing factor stimulates secretion of melanocyte-stimulating hormone from the rat pituitary. Science 217, 62- 64.

Sandman, C. A., Miller, L. H., Kastin, A. J., Shally, A. V., and Kendall, J. W. (1973). Neuroendo- crine responses to physical and psychological stress. .I. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 34, 386-390.

Sapolsky, R. M. (1986). Stress-induced elevation of testosterone concentrations in high ranking ba- boons: Role of catecholamines. Endocrinology 118(4), 1630-1635.

Sigmund, W. R. (1978). The analysis of visual displays

in the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Indiana University, Blooming- ton. 102 pp.

Tokarz, R. R. (1987). Effects of corticosterone treat- ment on male aggressive behavior in a lizard (An- olis sagrez]. Horm. Behav. 21, 358-370.

Vogel, W. H., and Jensh, R. (1988). Chronic stress and plasma catecholamine and corticosterone lev- els in male rats. Neurosci. Lett. 87, 183-188.

Whittier, J. M., Mason, R. T., and Crews, D. (1987). Plasma steroid hormone levels of female red-sided garter snakes, Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis: Re- lationship to mating and gestation. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 67, 3343.

Wingfield, J. C., and Silverin, B. (1986). Effects of corticosterone on territorial behavior of free- living male song sparrows, Melospiza melodul. Horm. Behav. 20, 405417.

Wingtield, J. C., Ball, G. F., Dufty, A. M., Jr., Heg- ner, R. E., and Ramenofsky, M. (1987). Testos- terone and aggression in birds. Amer. Sci. 75, 602608.