Children's emotion regulation across and within nations: A comparison of Ghanaian, Kenyan, and...

17
415 British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2012), 30, 415–431 C 2011 The British Psychological Society The British Psychological Society www.wileyonlinelibrary.com Children’s emotion regulation across and within nations: A comparison of Ghanaian, Kenyan, and American youth Diana Morelen 1 , Janice Zeman 2 , Carisa Perry-Parrish 3 and Ellen Anderson 4 1 University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia, USA 2 College of William & Mary, Williamsburg, Virginia, USA 3 Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, USA 4 Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, USA This research examined national, regional, developmental, and gender differences in children’s reported management of anger and sadness. Participants (8–15 years) were 103 Ghanaian children from a village setting, 142 Ghanaian children from a middle-class urban context, 106 Kenyan children from an impoverished urban context, and 170 children from the United States in lower to middle-class urban areas (58.8% Caucasian). Children completed the Children’s Anger and Sadness Management Scales (Zeman, Shipman, & Penza-Clyve, 2001) to assess emotion management (i.e., effortful control, over control, under control). Comparisons across nations indicated that Ghanaian youth reported more overt anger expression than youth from Kenya and the United States and less anger inhibition than Kenyan youth. U.S. children reported less overt expression and more constraint over sadness than Kenyan and Ghanaian children, although Kenyans reported being calmer when experiencing sadness than Ghanaian and American youth. Comparing Ghanaian regional contexts, village children reported more anger control than urban children. Regardless of nationality, boys reported more control over sadness than girls who reported more under control of sadness and more over control of anger than boys. Future research is needed to build on these descriptive, preliminary findings examining under-studied cross-national contexts. Environmental influences are important to consider when investigating children’s development in general and emotion regulation in particular (Zeman, Cassano, Perry- Parrish, & Stegall, 2006). For example, emotional expression and experience are influenced by societal display rule norms (Saarni, 1999), with such norms shaped by environmental factors (e.g., socioeconomic, cultural norms) that vary across and within nations (Cole, Tamang, & Shrestha, 2006; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992). Although Correspondence should be addressed to Diana Morelen, Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602, USA (e-mail: [email protected]). DOI:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02050.x

Transcript of Children's emotion regulation across and within nations: A comparison of Ghanaian, Kenyan, and...

415

British Journal of Developmental Psychology (2012), 30, 415–431C© 2011 The British Psychological Society

TheBritishPsychologicalSociety

www.wileyonlinelibrary.com

Children’s emotion regulation across and withinnations: A comparison of Ghanaian, Kenyan,and American youth

Diana Morelen1∗, Janice Zeman2, Carisa Perry-Parrish3

and Ellen Anderson4

1University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia, USA2College of William & Mary, Williamsburg, Virginia, USA3Johns Hopkins School of Medicine, Baltimore, Maryland, USA4Indiana University, Bloomington, Indiana, USA

This research examined national, regional, developmental, and gender differences inchildren’s reported management of anger and sadness. Participants (8–15 years) were103 Ghanaian children from a village setting, 142 Ghanaian children from a middle-classurban context, 106 Kenyan children from an impoverished urban context, and 170children from the United States in lower to middle-class urban areas (58.8% Caucasian).Children completed the Children’s Anger and Sadness Management Scales (Zeman,Shipman, & Penza-Clyve, 2001) to assess emotion management (i.e., effortful control,over control, under control). Comparisons across nations indicated that Ghanaian youthreported more overt anger expression than youth from Kenya and the United Statesand less anger inhibition than Kenyan youth. U.S. children reported less overt expressionand more constraint over sadness than Kenyan and Ghanaian children, althoughKenyans reported being calmer when experiencing sadness than Ghanaian and Americanyouth. Comparing Ghanaian regional contexts, village children reported more angercontrol than urban children. Regardless of nationality, boys reported more control oversadness than girls who reported more under control of sadness and more over controlof anger than boys. Future research is needed to build on these descriptive, preliminaryfindings examining under-studied cross-national contexts.

Environmental influences are important to consider when investigating children’sdevelopment in general and emotion regulation in particular (Zeman, Cassano, Perry-Parrish, & Stegall, 2006). For example, emotional expression and experience areinfluenced by societal display rule norms (Saarni, 1999), with such norms shapedby environmental factors (e.g., socioeconomic, cultural norms) that vary across andwithin nations (Cole, Tamang, & Shrestha, 2006; Mesquita & Frijda, 1992). Although

∗Correspondence should be addressed to Diana Morelen, Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602,USA (e-mail: [email protected]).

DOI:10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02050.x

416 Diana Morelen et al.

certain universal components of emotional expression have been documented (e.g.,physiological reactions, certain facial displays; Ekman, 1999; Ekman & Friesen, 1971),more recent research has sought to augment theories of emotion by considering theirsubjective nature and the environmental influences that shape emotional experiences(Markus & Kitayama, 1994). For instance, research indicates that certain aspects ofemotions may be biologically hardwired (Ekman, 1999), yet the contexts in whichemotions are elicited and the meanings associated with these experiences are influencedstrongly by social contexts (Kim-Prieto & Eid, 2004; Markus & Kitayama, 1994). Althoughthere is a general consensus among emotion researchers that both universal andenvironment-specific factors contribute to emotional expression, cross-national researchon emotion regulation in children is in its infancy, particularly in developing nations,highlighting the need to elucidate which emotion regulation patterns are context specificand which may be more universal in nature.

Children’s development in Western countries has been studied for decades, and thefield of emotional development has gained increasing attention in the past two decades(Adrian, Zeman, & Veits, 2011). However, there is a paucity of research examiningcentral developmental processes in children raised in African countries and, to ourknowledge, no research comparing emotion regulation strategies within and acrossAfrican nations. In their paper documenting the existing and potential contributions ofAfrican research on the field of developmental psychology, Super, Harkness, Barry, andZeitlin (2011) argue the importance of studying children from different socio-culturalenvironments. From a ‘developmental-niche’ conceptualization (Super & Harkness,2002), children’s environments are comprised of synergistic subsystems (e.g., physicalenvironment, customs regarding parenting), thus, studying ‘local’ (i.e., specific) Africanenvironments can inform ‘global’ child development research (Super et al., 2011). Asthere are numerous disparities between the social, physical, and economic environmentsfor a child raised in the United States versus a developing country in Africa (e.g., culturalbeliefs, schools, resources), it is plausible that differences in emotion regulation wouldbe present given the strong influence that socio-contextual factors have on children’sdeveloping emotion skills (Cole et al., 2006).

Socioeconomic status (SES) is a particular socio-contextual factor that affects achild’s development. Literature from Western nations provides overwhelming evidenceof the deleterious effects poverty has on children’s overall development, includingsocioemotional functioning (Buckner, Bassuk, Weinreb, & Brooks, 1999; Evans & English,2002; Raver, 2004). Thus, the fact that 50% of individuals in sub-Saharan Africa nationsfall below the poverty level (as estimated based on World Bank Policy of families makingless than $1.25 in U.S. dollars a day; World Bank, 2010) in comparison to 14% ofindividuals in the United States (as assessed in 2009; U.S. Census Bureau, 2010), providesan opportunity to compare the effects of disparate socioeconomic environments onan aspect of development (i.e., emotion regulation) that is known to be sensitive tocontextual effects. Children raised in poverty in the United States are more likely toexperience residential instability, violence in their neighbourhoods, and harsh, punitiveparenting than their middle-income counterparts (McLoyd, 1998). Likewise, an increasedexposure to such environmental stressors has been associated with emotion regulationdifficulties in U.S. youth (Gilliom, Shaw, Beck, Schonberg, & Lukon, 2002; Kliewer,Reid-Quinones, Shields, & Foutz, 2009). Additionally, anthropological research in theUnited States has demonstrated that attitudes about emotions and emotional displaysvary among social classes, suggesting that socioeconomic differences within a nation mayinfluence children’s emotion regulation development (Kusserow, 1999; Miller, 1986).

Emotion regulation across and within nations 417

Thus, one might expect emotional expressivity differences to emerge as disparitiesin socioeconomic contexts increase given the specific social norms associated witheach context. To date, however, there has been no empirical documentation of suchdifferences using samples of children living in African nations. The primary goal of thisstudy, therefore, was to explore differences in children’s report of emotion regulationbehaviours across nations (i.e., Ghana, Kenya, United States) and within a nation (i.e.,rural village and urban area in Ghana).

Emotion regulationAs emotion regulation is an emerging field of study, operational definitions are ofkey importance to how emotion processes are studied and understood within specificenvironmental contexts. In this paper, emotion regulation is defined as ‘[t]he processof initiating, avoiding, inhibiting, maintaining, or modulating the occurrence, form,intensity, or duration of internal feeling states . . . ’ (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004, p. 338).As such, emotion regulation is not simply the suppression or control of emotion, but isa dynamic process that involves the management and alteration of emotional behaviour.The present study focuses on three facets of emotion regulation [i.e., effortful control,over control, under control] because they aptly reflect different aspects of the dynamicnature of emotion management (Zeman, Shipman, & Penza-Clyve, 2001). Effortfulcontrol, as applied to emotion regulation, involves wilful attention to an emotion-eliciting situation in order to respond in an adaptive, thoughtful manner (Rothbart &Bates, 2006). Over control involves the suppression or inhibition of a felt emotion andis more inflexible and rigid than effortful control. In contrast, under control reflectsan impulsive or reactive, overt response to the experience of a particular emotion(Zeman et al., 2001). Using adult samples, methods for managing emotional expressionhave been found to vary in acceptability based on the environment in which oneis raised (Matsumoto, Takeuchi, Andayani, Kouznetsova, & Krupp, 1998). Further,based on the functionalist perspective (Campos, Mumme, Kermoian, & Campos, 1994),emotion regulation processes differ by the type of emotion experienced given thatemotional experience is contextually bound; thus, goals and action tendencies may differbased on the type of emotion experienced. Anger and sadness were chosen for studygiven that they represent common, universal childhood experiences (Ekman & Friesen,1971).

Research in Western nations suggests that patterns of emotion regulation in childrenvary depending on child age and gender. Regarding age, U.S. research findings suggestthat children’s emotion regulation skills improve with development and are simultane-ously and reciprocally influenced by changes in cognitive, biological, and social realms(Saarni, 1999). For example, based on literature examining Caucasian children, as earlyas 3 and 4 years of age, children begin to exert rudimentary control over their overtexpressions of negative emotion (Cole, 1986). This ability increases with age, suchthat older children report expressing the emotions of anger and sadness less oftenthan younger children, particularly to their peers (Zeman & Garber, 1996). Duringmiddle childhood, around age 10, children typically have developed the necessary skillsto manage their emotions deliberately and effectively in response to social demands(Saarni, 1999; Zeman et al., 2006). As children progress into adolescence, they mayengage in more emotion regulation behaviours than in middle childhood in response tothe increasing complexity of social demands inherent in the peer context. For instance,adolescents in the eighth grade reported exerting more control over sadness and anger

418 Diana Morelen et al.

than fifth-grade children because they expected less social support from their peers inresponse to these emotional displays (Zeman & Shipman, 1997).

Specifically, the development of effortful emotion control begins early in childhooddevelopment and is influenced by a variety of intrinsic (biological) and extrinsic(environmental) factors (for a review, see Fox & Calkins, 2003). Intrinsic factors, germaneto the development of effortful control as early as the first year of life, include the ability toregulate attention and engage in inhibitory control. As children age, development of thesecognitive processes facilitates the control of emotion, whereas under development islikely to impede effortful control. Extrinsic influences include caregiver responsiveness,maternal controlling behaviour, and peer acceptance, to name a few (e.g., Gottman,Katz, & Hooven, 1996; Zeman & Shipman, 1997). Further, research on Western sampleshas demonstrated that emotion regulation characterized by under or over control asopposed to effortful control is associated with risk for psychological maladjustmentincluding internalizing and externalizing problems (Bowie, 2010; Eisenberg, Hofer, &Vaughan, 2007; Olson, Sameroff, Lunkenheimer, & Kerr, 2009). What is consideredmaladaptive behaviour in one nation or environment, however, cannot automaticallybe considered maladaptive in another. Within a developmental psychopathology frame-work, multiple pathways can produce a given outcome (e.g., psychopathology) justas one pathway can lead to disparate outcomes depending on contextual factors (e.g.,Rutter & Sroufe, 2000). These contextual influences operate on multiple levels (e.g., fromgenes to culture). Therefore, the pathway(s) between emotional over and under controland psychological symptoms cannot be generalized to non-Western nations becauseresearch does not yet exist to support such a generalization.

In addition to developmental differences, research has indicated that emotionregulation processes differ by gender. For example, Caucasian boys who display sadnessare likely to violate a societal emotion display rule and are considered ‘unmanly’, whereasexpressing sadness is more socially acceptable for girls (Brody, 2000). Other researchhas demonstrated that girls express feelings of sadness and pain more than boys becauseboys anticipate more negative interpersonal consequences (e.g., teasing) when express-ing vulnerable types of emotions (Zeman & Garber, 1996). Interestingly, Caucasian girlsreported more over control of anger than boys, even when both boys and girls reportedfeeling equal amounts of anger (Underwood, Coie, & Herbsman, 1992). It is notable thatboys and girls differ not only in the frequency but also the manner in which they expressspecific negative emotions. For instance, girls are more likely to substitute an exaggerateddisplay of positive emotion for experienced negative emotion, whereas boys are morelikely to substitute neutral expressions (Cole, 1986). Additionally, in Western nations,adolescence is a time when gender norms become more pronounced, resulting in anincrease in gender differences in emotion regulation patterns (Perry-Parrish & Zeman,2011; Zeman & Shipman, 1997). For example, based on both self-report and peer report,adolescent boys were more likely to control sadness expression than adolescent girlswho were more likely to report expressing sadness through crying and other overtbehaviours (Perry-Parrish & Zeman, 2011). Although these gender and developmentaldifferences have been demonstrated within Western samples, it is not known whethersuch trends are universal across nations.

Emotion regulation across and within nationsAn accumulating body of research has described developmental and environmentalinfluences on children’s acquisition and use of emotion regulation skills in Westernnations. Research investigating such phenomena in non-Western nations is increasing

Emotion regulation across and within nations 419

and has demonstrated that children’s emotional development varies both across andwithin nations (e.g., Cole & Tamang, 1998; Raval, Martini, & Raval, 2007). Such findingsillustrate the range of broad (e.g., national) and specific (e.g., ethnic, socioeconomic)environmental influences surrounding children’s experience and management of emo-tion (Lutz, 1988; Wang & Fivush, 2005). There is mounting evidence for universaldevelopmental trends in emotion regulation abilities in some non Western nations (e.g.,Cole, Bruschi, & Tamang, 2002); however, there is very limited emotion research withAfrican children and adults. As an exception, one recent study examined emotion EC inKenyan preschool age children living in a slum who were exposed to political violence(Kithakye, Morris, Terranova, & Myers, 2010). Competent emotion regulation, definedas effortful control, attention focusing, attention shifting, and inhibitory control, wasassessed by the preschool teachers and found to be associated with less-aggressivebehaviour and more prosocial behaviour post conflict.

Recent research has reported significant gender effects in the emotional experienceof children from non Western nations outside Africa. A study of Turkish preschoolersrevealed that girls were rated higher than boys by their teachers and mothers on ameasure of emotion regulation and emotional lability (Yagmurlu & Altan, 2009). Thesedata are consistent with Western literature that suggests the emergence of genderdifferences in emotional management as early as preschool age (e.g., Cole, 1986). Ravalet al. (2007) investigated the regulation of anger, sadness, and pain in Gujarati children,aged 5–9, from two urban communities in India. Results demonstrated significant genderdifferences in emotion regulation behaviours. Specifically, girls reported more attemptsto control their anger compared to boys and displayed sadness more frequently thananger, findings that parallel Western studies (e.g., Underwood et al., 1992; Zeman &Garber, 1996). The authors speculated that because the majority of the children werefrom the middle to upper classes of the Hindu caste system, adherence to social normsmight have been a particularly salient influence on emotion regulation behaviours (Ravalet al., 2007).

In sum, cross-national research is emerging that demonstrates some universality acrossdevelopment and gender concerning the expression and control of anger and sadness.What is absent from this emerging database is the inclusion of research examining thesefactors in African children. The goal of this study was to explore whether developmentaland gender effects in emotion regulation across and within African nations would surface.

Demographic structure of Ghana and KenyaIn order to more fully appreciate the national comparisons being made in this research,a brief overview of the demographic structure of the African nations being studied ispresented. Ghana, a nation in West Africa, has a population of approximately 25 millionpeople (36.5% below the age of 14) and eight major ethnic groups. Originally colonizedby the United Kingdom, Ghana was the first sub-Saharan African nation to gain itsindependence in 1957 and currently operates as a constitutional democracy. The averagegross domestic product (GDP) per capita was estimated to be $1,560 in 2010 with anestimated 29% of the population below the poverty line in 2007.1 Ghana has beenrelatively free of political conflicts since 1981. With an average of 10 years of schooling

1‘National estimates of the percentage of the population falling below the poverty line are based on surveys of sub-groups,with the results weighted by the number of people in each group. Definitions of poverty vary considerably among nations. Forexample, rich nations generally employ more generous standards of poverty than poor nations’ (CIA, 2011b, para. 142)

420 Diana Morelen et al.

for most Ghanaians, the literacy rate is estimated to be 58% of the population over theage of 14 (Central Intelligence Agency [CIA], 2011a).

The Republic of Kenya is an East African nation that gained its independence fromthe United Kingdom in 1963 and has a population of approximately 41 million people(42% below the age of 14) with seven major ethnic groups. The average GDP per capitawas approximately $1,600 in 2010 with an estimated 40% of the population below thepoverty line in 2008. Kenya has had more violent political conflict than Ghana, with themost recent political conflict occurring after the 2007 elections and lasting for 3 monthswith approximately 1,500 deaths. The average number of years completed in school is11 and the literacy rate is estimated to be 85% of the population over the age of 14 (CIA,2011a).

In contrast to Ghana and Kenya, the U.S. population is approximately 313 million(20% below the age of 14), with the GDP per capita estimated to be $47,400 in 2010(CIA, 2011a) and an estimated poverty rate of 14% in 2009 (U.S. Census Bureau, 2010).The average number of years completed in school is 16, and the literacy rate is estimatedto be 99% of the population over the age of 14 (CIA, 2011a).

Children from both Ghana and Kenya were included in order to investigate nationalitydifferences within the African continent, acknowledging that these nations each haveunique languages, cultural practices, and customs. Overall, Kenya has a higher percent-age of people living in poverty and has experienced more recent violent political conflictthan Ghana. Ghana and Kenya do, however, share certain similarities including Englishas the official language and their British colonization history. Further, when comparedto the U.S., both nations have relatively similar GDP and average years of education.Although there are 53 sovereign states in the continent of Africa, the inclusion of bothGhana and Kenya allows comparisons to be made between Western and non-Westerncultures, two independent sub-Saharan nations, and reduces an overgeneralization thatall African nations are comparable. Additionally, sampling from two locations withinGhana allows comparisons between rural and urban regions.

Present studyThe overarching goal of the present research was to examine children’s report of theirmanagement of anger and sadness experience, and how this may vary as a functionof environmental context (i.e., national, regional), age, gender, and type of emotion.To facilitate comparisons across groups, a well-validated measure (Children’s EmotionManagement Scales [CEMS]; Zeman et al., 2001) was used to examine specific facets ofemotion management. Given the lack of existing data on Ghanaian and Kenyan children’semotion regulation, analyses regarding the nature and type of differences in children’semotion management were considered exploratory. Generally, it was expected thatchildren raised in environments characterized by more adversity and economic hardship(i.e., Ghana, Kenya) would display less emotional EC, more over control, and moreunder control than children growing up in more economically secure environments (i.e.,U.S.) because these environments are more laden with stressors (e.g., hunger, poverty,violence) that might affect how children manage their emotions (Gilliom et al., 2002).Given that no research has compared emotion regulation strategies of children in Ghanaand Kenya, within Africa national comparisons were exploratory. Certain similarities indevelopment in children’s reported emotion regulation were anticipated across nationalcontexts. Specifically, EC for anger was expected to be reported more frequently in theolder than the younger age group, whereas the under control of sadness was expected

Emotion regulation across and within nations 421

to decrease with age. Additionally, it was hypothesized that certain gender differenceswould not be influenced by nationality and economic status such that girls would reportmore over control of their anger and more under control of their sadness, whereasboys would report more EC of their sadness. Similar to the exploratory nature of thecross-national comparisons, the hypotheses for the within Ghana regional comparisonwas based on research suggesting that increased environmental stressors and povertymay affect children’s ability to be effortful when managing emotion (e.g., Gilliom et al.,2002). Specifically, it was expected that village children would report less EC, moreover control, and more under control for anger and sadness when compared to urbanchildren.

MethodParticipants

Ghanaian rural village sampleA total of 103 children (54 boys; M age = 11.36 years, SD = 2.61; range: 8–15 years)participated from the rural village, Kpando, which is in the Volta Region of Ghana(approximately 120 miles north of Accra, the capitol of Ghana) and is predominatelycomposed of members of the Ewe tribe. Kpando has a population of approximately113,000 with 51% of the residents under the poverty line (Ministry of Local Governmentand Rural Development and Maks Publications & Media Services [MRDMPS], 2010).Ghanaian children spoke Ewe, a regional language, as well as English, the officialgovernment language. Although Ghana is in the process of providing free publiceducation, the majority of schools are privately owned and have differing fee structures.All children in this sample attended a private school considered to be typical of those forworking class, non-professional, and uneducated families who can afford to pay tuitionto ensure a more stable, quality learning environment than the public schools. Thestudent:teacher ratio was approximately 30:1. The cost for attending this school was55.5 Ghanaian Cedis (approximately $39) per term plus textbooks and materials.

Ghanaian urban sampleParticipants were 142 children (68 boys; M age = 11.07 years, SD = 1.88, range: 8–15 years) who attended a private school in Adenta, a suburb of Accra, Ghana’s capitalcity. Accra has a population of approximately 438,000 people and is predominatelycomprised of members of the Ga and Akan ethnic groups (MRDMPS, 2010). Althoughspecific information about Adenta’s poverty rates is not available, in comparison tourban areas, rural areas in Ghana have significantly more poverty (MRDMPS, 2010).Students spoke English and Ga or Twi, two regional languages. This private school wasconsiderably more affluent in comparison to the village school (e.g., had electricity,indoor plumbing, two computer labs, a cafeteria, school bus). The student:teacher ratiowas approximately 25:1 with tuition costs ranging from 275–315 Cedis ($192–$220) perterm plus school uniforms and supplies.

Kenyan sampleParticipants were 106 children (48 boys; M age = 11.14, SD = 1.74, range: 8–15 years)recruited from two schools in the Mathare slums of Nairobi. Mathare is known forits extreme poverty and brutal gang violence with only 5% of its population legallyemployed (Mathare Roots, 2010). There are approximately 500,000 people living within

422 Diana Morelen et al.

a few square miles resulting in high rates of crime, substance abuse, and individuals livingwith HIV/AIDS (Mathare Roots, 2010). This geographic area was centrally involved in thepost-presidential election violence of January 2008. Both schools, located approximatelyfive miles apart, were operated by the same non-profit organization and administrativestaff and had about a 1:30 teacher:student ratio. Children spoke English, the officialgovernment language of Kenya, and were also fluent in Swahili. Children did not paytuition to attend this school but were required to buy uniforms. Although the school hadrunning water, indoor plumbing, and limited electricity, it was typical for 5–8 childrento share a textbook.

American sampleParticipants included 170 children (95 boys; 41.2% African American, 58.8% Caucasian;M age = 11.07, SD = 1.82, range: 8–15 years) who were recruited from two publicelementary and two middle schools in Virginia and Georgia. The schools were locatedin small urban areas that reflected lower to middle class SES. For brevity, children fromthe United States will be referred to as ‘American.’

MeasuresThe CEMS (Zeman et al., 2001) were used to measure regulation of anger (CAMS)and sadness (CSMS). Previous studies have identified three factors including RegulationCoping, Inhibition, and Dysregulation (Zeman et al., 2001). However, given the differentcultural contexts being assessed in this study, these scales were re-labelled to reflect moreneutral descriptors. Factor analyses were conducted to determine whether the originalfactor structure was retained with this new sample (see Preliminary Analyses below).The final scales used for analyses were based on the 11-item CAMS and 12-item CSMS thatuse a 3-point Likert scale (1 = hardly ever, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often) to assess specificemotion regulation behaviours through three domains. The first factor was labelled ECand assessed deliberate attempts to manage emotional arousal and expressivity (e.g., ‘Istay calm and don’t let sad things get to me’, ‘I try to calmly deal with what is makingme mad’). Using American child samples, EC has been considered an adaptive way tocontrol emotions that is neither too controlled (e.g., suppression) nor lacks control (e.g.,dysregulation). The second factor, over control, assessed inhibition or suppression ofemotions (e.g., ‘I hide my sadness’, ‘I hold my anger in’). On the opposite end of theemotional control spectrum, the third factor assessed overt emotional displays termedunder control and may reflect a lack of regulatory efforts (e.g., ‘I cry and carry on whenI’m sad’, ‘I say mean things to others when I am mad). Modifications were employedto minimize cultural misinterpretations of specific words (e.g., ‘insulting’ as a synonymfor ‘saying mean things’). Black and white drawings of faces were used to depict angerand sadness. Additionally, a visual representation (e.g., circles of differing sizes) of thefrequency scale was used to depict the differing magnitudes of each choice. Under eachcircle was the corresponding verbal option and its numerical value.

ProcedureGhana and Kenya were chosen for study because they are both English-speaking nations,and both in sub-Saharan Africa. The research team had contacts in both nations makingdata collection in the schools and access to a translator more easily arranged. Ineach setting, informed consent by school administrators, parents, and/or guardians

Emotion regulation across and within nations 423

was obtained as well as child verbal assent. Caucasian-American female adults readquestionnaires aloud to the children in English. Ghanaian and Kenyan children spokeEnglish, but given the variation in fluency, a local translator assisted the interviewerby providing clarification if needed. For younger children and all African children,questionnaires were administered individually or in small groups of two to three children.For older American children, questionnaires were administered in a group setting withfive to seven children participating. American and African children received a smallgift (e.g., pencil, eraser, stickers) and participating African schools were provided witheducational materials (e.g., books, paper).

ResultsPreliminary AnalysesIn order to determine whether the factor structure of the CEMS scales remained invariantacross the cultures assessed in this study, factor analyses were conducted using the datafrom the African sample. Factors were retained if they had eigenvalues above 1.0 andfactor loadings above .40 in order to achieve improved accuracy (Stevens, 1996).

AngerPrincipal components analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation extracted three factors thataccounted for 47.28% of the variance. The eigenvalue for the first factor was 2.34; thesecond factor, 1.33; the third factor, 1.06. One item for EC (‘I stay calm and keep my coolwhen I am feeling angry’) did not load on any factor and was excluded from subsequentanalyses. The first factor consisted of the original four over control items and accountedfor 23.36% of the variance. The second factor consisted of the original three undercontrol items and accounted for 13.34% of the variance. The third factor consisted ofthree EC items and accounted for 10.58% of the variance.

SadnessPCA with varimax rotation extracted four factors that accounted for 52.60% of thevariance. The eigenvalue of the first factor was 2.10; the second factor, 1.45; the thirdfactor, 1.56; and the fourth factor, 1.08. One EC item (‘I try to calmly deal with whatis making me sad’) did not load on any factor and was dropped from analyses. The firstfactor consisted of the original four over control items and accounted for 19.10% ofthe variance. The second factor consisted of the three original under control items andaccounted for 13.17% of the variance. The third factor consisted of two EC items thatcorresponded to exerting control over expressions of sadness and accounted for 10.50%of the variance. It was named ‘EC: Constraint’. The fourth factor consisted of two ECitems that corresponded to remaining calm while feeling sad and accounted for 9.82% ofthe variance. This factor was named ‘EC: Calmness’. If a three-factor solution was forced,the items for the Control subscale no longer loaded on any factor. Across all subscales,internal consistencies ranged from .43 to .66 based on two-, three-, or four-item scales.In the following analyses, this revised scale structure was used with both the African andAmerican samples.

Analytic strategyIn order to examine developmental effects, children were placed into younger(ages 8–11) or older (ages 12–15) age groups that reflected elementary versus middle

424 Diana Morelen et al.

Table 1. sample size, mean age, and standard deviation of participant groups

Older Older Total Younger Younger Totalboys girls older boys girls younger

Ghana village 13.48 (1.16) 13.00 (1.22) 13.26 (1.20) 9.52 (1.21) 9.46 (1.23) 9.49 (1.21)n = 25 n = 21 n = 46 n = 29 n = 28 n = 57

Ghana urban 12.87 (.78) 12.97 (.89) 12.92 (.83) 9.74 (1.16) 9.70 (1.13) 9.75(1.01)n = 30 n = 30 n = 60 n = 38 n = 44 n = 82

Kenya-Nairobi 12.68 (.95) 12.84 (1.23) 12.75(1.08) 9.89 (1.01) 9.85 (.78) 9.86 (.87)n = 25 n = 22 n = 47 n = 23 n = 36 n = 59

United States 13.22 (1.02) 12.86 (.61) 13.10 (.92) 9.88 (.98) 9.86 (.93) 9.87 (.95)n = 42 n = 21 n = 63 n = 53 n = 54 n = 107

Total 13.01 (1.01) 9.76 (1.05)n = 216 n = 305

school educational groupings in both Africa and the United States. See Table 1 forage by gender breakdowns. Then, two sets of analyses were conducted. The first setexamined differences as a function of residing in an urban context by nationality (i.e.,Ghana, Kenya, U.S.) and the second set of analyses examined differences within Ghanaas a function of living in an urban versus a village setting. All data were analysed usingMANOVA with Nation or Place, Age Group, and Gender designated as the independentvariables and the scores on the Anger and Sadness CEMS subscales as the dependentvariables. Significant effects were further analysed using one-way ANOVA with Tukey’sHSD post hoc comparisons.

Nationality comparisons

Regulation of angerNationality differences were examined by comparing children from the three urbancontexts of Ghana, Kenya, and the U.S. MANOVA revealed a significant main effect ofNation, Wilks’ � = .86, F(6, 782) = 10.13, p = .001, �p

2 = .07. Tests of between-subjecteffects revealed differences on the over control subscale, F(2, 393) = 15.83, p = .001,�p

2 = .08, and the under control subscale, F(2, 393) = 7.37, p = .001, �p2 = .04. Post hoc

analyses revealed that Kenyan children (M = 2.17, SD = .52) reported inhibiting theirdisplays of anger more frequently than the Ghanaian (M = 1.89, SD = .48) and Americanchildren (M = 1.80, SD = .53), who did not differ from each other, F(2, 410) = 17.78,p = .001. Regarding the under control scale, Ghanaian children (M = 1.95, SD = .52)reported expressing their anger in overt ways more frequently than the Kenyan (M =1.79, SD = .56) and American children (M = 1.71, SD = .51), who did not differ fromeach other, F(2, 407) = 7.76, p = .001.

MANOVA also revealed a significant Gender main effect, Wilks’ � = .98, F(3, 391) =2.81, p = .04, �p

2 = .02. Tests of between-subject effects revealed significant differenceson the over control subscale, F(1, 393) = 5.84, p = .02, �p

2 = .02. Girls reported (M =1.99, SD = .52) inhibiting or suppressing overt expressions of anger more than boys(M = 1.86, SD = .53), t(411) = −2.32, p = .02.

Regulation of sadnessMANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Nation, Wilks’ � = .86, F(8, 784) =7.58, p = .001, �p

2 = .07, that indicated differences for the under control subscale,

Emotion regulation across and within nations 425

F(1, 365) = 10.04, p = .002, �p2 = .03, EC: Calmness, F(1, 365) = 12.09, p = .001, �p

2 =.03, and EC: Constraint subscale, F(1, 365) = 12.09, p = .001, �p

2 = .03. Regardingunder control, post hoc analyses indicated that Ghanaian (M = 1.96, SD = .49) andKenyan children (M = 1.85, SD = .50) reported expressing their sadness overtly morefrequently than American children (M = 1.69, SD = .55), F(2, 410) = 10.08, p = .001.Further, Kenyan children reported remaining calm when feeling sad more frequently(M = 2.32, SD = .61) than Ghanaian (M = 2.06, SD = .55), and American children (M =2.08, SD = .60) who did not differ significantly from each other, F(2, 412) = 7.31, p =.001. Concerning the Constraint subscale, American children reported exerting moreconstraint over their sadness expression (M = 2.31, SD = .51) than Ghanaian (M = 2.01,SD = .49) and Kenyan children (M = 1.99, SD = .55) who did not differ significantlyfrom each other, F(2, 411) = 15.71, p = .001.

MANOVA also yielded a significant main effect of Gender, Wilks’ � = .94, F(4, 392) =6.07, p = .001, �p

2 = .06, with between-subject effects for the under control subscale,F(1, 365) = 10.04, p = .002, �p

2 = .03, and the EC: Constraint subscale, F(1, 365) =6.16, p = .01, �p

2 = .02. Regarding under control, girls (M = 1.93, SD = .52) reportedexpressing their sadness in overt ways more frequently than boys (M = 1.72, SD =.52), t(411) = 4.23, p = .001. Similarly, boys reported controlling or constraining theirsadness displays more frequently (M = 2.21, SD = .57) than girls (M = 2.03, SD = .54),t(412) = 3.27, p = .001.

Certain main effects were qualified by a significant Nation by Gender interaction,Wilks’ � = .95, F(8, 784) = 2.67, p = .01, �p

2 = .03 with between-subjects differenceson the under control subscale, F(1, 365) = 8.20, p = .004, and the over control subscale,F(1, 365) = 8.20, p = .004. These interactions were best explicated by examiningNationality differences within Gender. Regarding under control, Ghanaian (M = 1.93,SD = .49) and Kenyan (M = 1.76, SD = .51) boys reported expressing their sadness inovert ways more than American boys (M = 1.54, SD = .49), F(2, 206) = 12.98, p = .001.There were no significant differences for girls. Regarding the over control subscale, therewere no significant differences among boys but a marginally significant finding emergedfor girls, F(2, 200) = 2.92, p = .057. Kenyan girls reported inhibiting their sadness morefrequently (M = 2.19, SD = .52) than Ghanaian (M = 2.00, SD = .51) and American girls(M = 1.99, SD = .50), who did not differ from each other.

Within Ghana comparisons

Regulation of angerChildren’s data from the village school were compared to the children attending schoolin Accra-Adenta to evaluate within-nation differences. MANOVA revealed a significantmain effect of Place, Wilks’ � = .97, F(3, 234) = 2.81, p = .04, �p

2 = .04, in whichtests of between-subject effects revealed differences for the over control subscale,F(1, 236) = 8.06, p = .005, �p

2 = .03. Village children reported inhibiting their angermore frequently (M = 2.07, SD = .47) than children from Accra-Adenta (M = 1.89, SD =.48), t(242) = 2.92, p = .004.

Regulation of sadnessMANOVA revealed a significant main effect of Age Group, Wilks’ � = .96, F(4, 233) =2.63, p = .04, �p

2 = .04. Tests of between-subject effects revealed a significant differenceon the EC: Calmness subscale, F(1, 236) = 6.97, p = .01, �p

2 = .03, and a marginallysignificant difference on the EC: Constraint subscale, F(1, 236) = 2.85, p = .09,

426 Diana Morelen et al.

�p2 = .01. Older children reported staying calm more frequently when feeling sad (M =

2.20, SD = .55) than the younger children (M = 2.01, SD = .55), t(242) = −2.65, p =.01, and also reported exhibiting more sadness expression constraint than the youngerchildren (older: M = 2.08, SD = .48; younger: M = 1.96, SD = .48), t(242) = −1.73,p = .08.

DiscussionGiven the dearth of cross-national research examining emotional processes in children,particularly in African nations, this research sought to provide a descriptive overviewof national, regional, developmental, and gender differences in children’s self-reportedmanagement of anger and sadness. Examination of the findings revealed five generalthemes.

First, it appears that Ghanaian children report being more emotionally expressivethan Kenyan and American children. Specifically, Ghanaian children reported displayingtheir anger in more overt, under controlled ways than Kenyan and American children.Conversely, Kenyan children reported suppressing their anger more than Ghanaian andAmerican children. Based on these preliminary findings, it appears that Ghanaian youthmay be more expressive with wider fluctuations in their emotionality than children fromthe U.S. and Kenya.

Second, American children tended to report exerting more control over sadnesscompared to both groups of African children. Specifically, American children reportedconstraining their displays of sadness more than African children, although theKenyan children reported responding calmly to their sadness more than Ghanaian andAmerican children. Further, the differences in the under control of sadness betweencontinents was particularly noticeable for boys such that Ghanaian and Kenyan boysreported displaying their sadness in under controlled ways more frequently thanAmerican boys. Perhaps American boys experience more pressure to dampen theirdisplays of sadness than the African boys, consistent with research indicating thatAmerican boys believe they will not receive support for expression of vulnerableemotions to their peers or parents (Zeman & Garber, 1996). Further, Kenyan girlsreported more over control of their sadness than Ghanaian and American girls, althoughthis finding represented a trend and should be interpreted with caution.

Taken together, these findings may reflect a national cultural difference in theacceptability of emotional displays or perhaps even a broader cultural emotional climate(Becht & Vingerhoets, 2002). These findings dovetail the researchers’ observations thatemotional expressions, particularly anger and happiness, were more frequently andpassionately displayed in Ghana than in Kenya (e.g., it was more common to see peoplearguing in the streets loudly in Ghana compared to Kenya, though the researchers areaware of the biases in unsystematic observation). It may be that blatant displays ofnegative emotion in Kenya are sanctioned only in certain circumstances (i.e., outrageover political events), but are not tolerated or accepted in day-to-day life where emotionalcontrol may be more adaptive and protective.

Third, consistent with our hypotheses, regional comparisons within Ghana indicatedthat children from the village reported more over control of anger than children inthe urban region. Future research should investigate whether this difference couldbe related, in part, to the environmental adversity (as indicated by poverty rates andlack of resources) experienced by the village children. Western literature suggests thatpoverty and adversity affect youths’ development, including emotional development

Emotion regulation across and within nations 427

(e.g., Buckner et al., 1999; Kusserow, 1999; Miller, 1986). These discrepancies also mayarise from socialization experiences, in which emotional expressivity is shaped by theexpectations and responses of others to anger expression. Based on observation, albeitnot standardized and likely biased, the majority of children living in the village with theirfamilies often received harsh repercussions (e.g., beatings) for their overt emotionaldisplays (e.g., yelling, crying) by family members, whereas these types of responseswere not observed in the suburban context. Although speculative, it is possible thatthe village children have learned to control their expressions of anger in response tothe expectation that they will receive a punitive response to emotional displays. Asthis research is exploratory in nature, future research needs to be conducted to fullyunderstand the nature of and outcomes associated with emotion socialization processesin this context.

Fourth, as anticipated, robust gender differences emerged across nations such thatgirls reported more over control of anger whereas boys reported more control oversadness expression than girls suggesting that gender differences in emotion regulationmay be more universal than nation specific. This finding is commensurate with researchin both Western and non-Western nations suggesting that compared to boys, girls tendto display anger less frequently and sadness more frequently (e.g., Fischer & Manstead,2000; Raval et al., 2007; Underwood et al., 1992). Future research should investigate thesocialization processes that operate in all three nations to understand whether the samemechanisms are operating to produce this consistent set of findings.

Fifth, consistent with our general hypothesis that some commonalities across nationswould emerge, one developmental differences was found. Specifically, in Ghana,regardless of living environment (rural vs. urban), older children reported controllingtheir sadness more than younger children. Specifically, older youth reported remainingcalm and in control of their sad feelings more frequently than younger children. Thisfinding is consistent with past research using samples from both Western (e.g., Zemanet al., 2001) and non-Western nations (e.g., Cole et al., 2002). It is puzzling that thesefindings did not hold for the samples from Kenya and the U.S. Analyses of the agegroup means by country (including all participants) for sadness control revealed that theGhanaian younger age group sample had the lowest mean scores compared to the othertwo nations, suggesting that perhaps younger Ghanaian children are more expressive ofsadness in general than the other nations.

In sum, the results of this research represent a preliminary investigationinto environmental influences on emotion management through cross-nation andwithin-nation comparisons. To our knowledge, this is a first attempt to conduct suchresearch with a population that has virtually been ignored in developmental literature,namely, children in sub-Saharan Africa. Although the findings from this study makean initial contribution to our understanding of differences and similarities in children’semotion regulation across and within two African nations, there are several limitations ofnote. First, although limited demographic information was obtained, the infrastructurein both Ghana and Kenya did not allow for precise measurement of SES. Thus, estimatesof population size and teacher to student ratios are provided, but future research shouldattempt to find innovative ways to obtain more precise demographic markers. Second,the data relied solely on self-report using measures normed on Western samples. It willbe important for future studies to incorporate multiple reporters (e.g., parents, teachers)and multiple methods (e.g., observational, ethnographic) of assessment given researchdocumenting that such methodological changes can yield differing findings (Zeman et al.,2006). For instance, research has documented that boys and girls may differ on emotional

428 Diana Morelen et al.

constructs depending on whether self-report or observational methods are used (Shields,1991). One of the advantages of the emotion regulation measure used in this study is thatthe items are not placed within a specific social context but rather are asked in a generalmanner (i.e., ‘I hide my sadness’). Despite this advantage, there were several limitationsof the measure. The questionnaires used in the present study did not assess motivationfor emotion regulation (e.g., why children reported managing emotions in a particularmanner) or socialization norms (e.g., how they learned to manage their emotions in aparticular way) or whether certain emotion regulation methods were associated withparticular outcomes (e.g., what modes of emotion regulation were adaptive). Thus,the next studies should build on the current research by assessing such factors thatare known to influence children’s emotional development (Eisenberg, Cumberland, &Spinrad, 1998). Third, female American interviewers interviewed children; thus, socialdesirability may have affected their responses. Lastly, in African cross-cultural research,there is a call to involve more local researchers in order to promote collaboration anddisciplinary development (Marfo, 2011). Thus, future research should seek to incorporateresearchers native to the area being studied.

These preliminary findings can help guide future research to develop additionalquestions and tasks that can address facets of emotion management specific to Africannations. Relatedly, empirical efforts should include measures of socialization in orderto more comprehensively assess the mechanisms through which emotion regulation issocialized in children of different cultural contexts. Future research should also attemptto better understand whether different aspects of emotion management (e.g., undercontrol, over control) are related to maladaptive outcomes, as has been shown to bethe case in Western samples (e.g., Zeman et al., 2006). Overall, this study offers apreliminary investigation regarding the role of nationality, developmental factors, andgender on children’s regulation of their anger and sadness. This study adds to the existingresearch on developing nations and highlights the great need for further cross-nationalresearch to build on and extend the findings from this study.

AcknowledgementsWe would like to acknowledge the extraordinary level of support, enthusiasm, and patienceprovided by the following people involved with this research project: (a) in Kpando, Ghana,Vincentia Atsu and Mawusi Dotse who served as translators, tour guides, and invaluable aidsin making connections with local schools (i.e., Miracle Preparatory School, Delta PreparatorySchool); (b) in Adenta, Ghana, Dan and Pat Ayitiah and the teachers and children at theLight Academy who provided exemplary hospitality and assistance, (c) in Nairobi, Kenya, theHeadmistress Jane, our driver and translator, John, our local facilitators Rebecca and AndyDesRoches, and the children at the Muthare Valley Child Development Centre who openeddoors for us; and (d) in Williamsburg, Virginia, the Reves Center for International Studies forproviding financial assistance and the Munroe Scholars program for providing this invaluableopportunity for William and Mary students.

ReferencesAdrian, M., Zeman, J., & Veits, G. (2011). Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A

35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children. Journal of Experimental ChildPsychology, 110, 171–197.

Becht, M. C., & Vingerhoets, J. J. M. (2002). Crying and mood change: A cross-cultural study.Cognition & Emotion, 16 , 87–101. doi:10.1080/02699930143000149

Emotion regulation across and within nations 429

Bowie, B. (2010). Understanding the gender differences in pathways to social deviancy: Re-lational aggression and emotion regulation. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 24, 27–37.doi:10.1016/j.apnu.2009.04.007

Brody, L. (2000). The socialization of gender differences in emotional expression: Display rules,infant temperament, and differentiation. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Socialpsychological perspectives. Studies in emotion and social interaction (pp. 24–47). New York:Cambridge University Press.

Buckner, J. C., Bassuk, E. L., Weinreb, L., & Brooks, M. (1999). Homelessness and its relation to themental health and behavior of low-income school-age children. Developmental Psychology,35, 246–257. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.35.1.246

Campos, J. J., Mumme, D. L., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. G. (1994). A Functionalist perspectiveon the nature of emotion. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation:Behavioral and biological considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in ChildDevelopment, 59, 284–303.

Central Intelligence Agency. (2011a). The world factbook. Retrieved from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/index.html

Central Intelligence Agency. (2011b). The world factbook: Definitions and notes. Retrievedfrom https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world- factbook/docs/notesanddefs.html?countryName = Ghana&countryCode = gh&regionCode = af#2046

Cole, P. (1986). Children’s spontaneous control of facial expression. Child Development, 57,1309–1321. doi:10.1111/1467–8624.ep7252163

Cole, P. M., Bruschi, C. J., & Tamang, B. L. (2002). Cultural differences in children’s emotional reac-tions to difficult situations. Child Development, 73, 983–996. doi:10.1111/1467–8624.00451

Cole, P. M., & Tamang, B. L. (1998). Nepali children’s ideas about emotional displays in hypotheticalchallenges. Developmental Psychology, 34, 640–646. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.34.4.640

Cole, P. M., Tamang, B. L., & Shrestha, S. (2006). Cultural variations in the socialization ofyoung children’s anger and shame. Child Development, 77, 1237–1251. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2006.00931.x

Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (1998). Parental socialization of emotion.Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli0904_1

Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., & Vaughan, J. (2007). Effortful control and its socioemotional conse-quences. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 287–306). New York, NY:Guilford Press.

Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition. ChildDevelopment, 75, 334–339. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2004.00674.x

Ekman, P. (1999). Facial expressions. In T. Dalgleish & M. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognitionand emotion (pp. 301–320). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.

Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotions. Journalof Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129. doi:10.1037/h0030377

Evans, G. W., & English, K. (2002). The environment of poverty: Multiple stressor exposure,psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Development, 73, 1238–1248. doi:10.1111/1467–8624.00469

Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2000). The relation between gender and emotion in differentcultures. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives(pp. 71–96). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Fox, N., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and extrinsicinfluences. Motivation and Emotion, 27, 7–26. doi:10.1023/A:1023622324898

Gilliom, M., Shaw, D., Beck, J. E., Schonberg, M. A., & Lukon, J. L. (2002). Anger regulation indisadvantaged preschool boys: Strategies, antecedents, and the development of self-control.Developmental Psychology, 38, 222–235. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.38.2.222

Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and theemotional life of families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of FamilyPsychology, 10, 243–268.

430 Diana Morelen et al.

Kim-Prieto, C., & Eid, M. (2004). Norms for experiencing emotions in sub-Saharan Africa. Journalof Happiness Studies, 5, 241–268. doi:10.1007/s10531–004-8787–2

Kithakye, M., Morris, A. S., Terranova, A. M., & Myers, S. S. (2010). The Kenyan politicalconflict and children’s adjustment. Child Development, 81, 1114–1128. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2010.01457.x

Kliewer, W., Reid-Quinones, K., Shields, B. J., & Foutz, L. (2009). Multiple risks, emotion regulationskill, and cortisol in low-income African American youth: A prospective study. Journal of BlackPsychology,35, 24–43. doi:10.1177/0095798408323355

Kusserow, A. (1999). De-homogenizing American individualism: Socializing hard and soft individ-ualism in Manhattan and Queens. Ethos, 27, 210–234. doi:10.1525/eth.1999.27.2.210

Lutz, C. A. (1988). Unnatural emotions: Everyday sentiments on a Micronesian atoll & theirchallenge to Western theory. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Marfo, K. (2011). Envisioning an African child development field. Child Development Perspectives,5, 140–147.

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994).The cultural construction of self and emotion: Implications forsocial behavior. In S. Kitayama & H. R. Markus (Eds.), Emotion and culture: Empirical studiesof mutual influence (pp. 89–130). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Mathare Roots (2010). The slum. Retrieved from http://www.mathareroots.org/index.php?id=50Matsumoto, D., Takeuchi, S., Andayani, S., Kouznetsova, N., & Krupp, D. (1998). The contribution

of individualism-collectivism to cross-national differences in display rules. Asian Journal ofSocial Psychology, 1, 147–165. doi:10.1111/1467–839X.00010

McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psycholo-gist, 53, 185–204. doi:10.1037/0003–066X.53.2.185

Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review. PsychologicalBulletin, 112, 179–204. doi:10.1037/0033–2909.112.2.179

Miller, P. J. (1986). Teasing as language socialization and verbal play in a white working-classcommunity. In B. B. Schieffelin & E. Ochs (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures(pp. 199–212). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Ministry of Local Government and Rural Development and Maks Publications & Media Services(August 2010). Volta Region: Kpando. Retrieved from http://www.ghanadistricts.com/districts/?r=7& =123&sa=2583

Olson, S., Sameroff, A., Lunkenheimer, E., & Kerr, D. (2009). Self-regulatory processes in thedevelopment of disruptive behavior problems: The preschool-to-school transition. In S. L. Olson& A. J. Sameroff (Eds.), Biopsychosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhoodbehavioral problems (pp. 144–185). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Perry-Parrish, C., & Zeman, J. (2011). Relations among sadness regulation, peer acceptance, andsocial functioning in early adolescence: The role of gender. Social Development, 20, 135–153.doi:10.1111/j.1467–9507.2009.00568.x

Raval, V., Martini, T., & Raval, P. (2007). ‘Would others think it is okay to express myfeelings?’ Regulation of anger, sadness and physical pain in Gujarati children in India. SocialDevelopment, 16 , 79–105. doi:10.1111/j.1467–9507.2007.00373.x

Raver, C. C. (2004), Placing emotional self-regulation in sociocultural and socioeconomic contexts.Child Development, 75, 346–353. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2004.00676.x

Rothbart, M. K., & Bates, J. E. (2006). Temperament. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg(Vol. Ed), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional, and personalitydevelopment (6th ed., pp. 105–176). New York: Wiley.

Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges.Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265–296. doi:10.1017/S0954579400003023

Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New York: Guilford Press.Shields, S. A. (1991). Gender in the psychology of emotion: A selective research review. In

K. T. Strongman (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 227–245).New York: Wiley.

Stevens, J. (1996). Multivariate statistics for the social sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Emotion regulation across and within nations 431

Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (2002). Culture structures the environment for development. HumanDevelopment, 45, 270–274. doi:10.1159/000064988

Super, C. M., Harkness, S., Barry, O., & Zeitlin, M. (2011). Think locally, act globally: Contributionsof African research to child development. Child Development Perspectives, 5, 119–125.

Underwood, M. K., Coie, J. D., & Herbsman, C. R. (1992). Display rules for anger andaggression in school-age children. Child Development, 63, 366–380. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1992.tb01633.x

U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). Poverty: Highlights. Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/hhes/www/poverty/about/overview/index.html

Wang, Q., & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother-child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploringthe functions of reminiscing in Euro-American and Chinese families. Social Development, 14,473–495. doi:10.1111/j.1467–9507.2005.00312.x

Yagmurlu, B., & Altan, O. (2009). Maternal socialization and child temperament as predictors ofemotion regulation in Turkish preschoolers. Infant and Child Development, 19, 275–296.doi:10.1002/icd.646

Zeman, J., Cassano, M., Perry-Parrish, C., & Stegall, S. (2006). Emotion regulation inchildren and adolescents. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 155–168.doi:10.1097/00004703–200604000-00014

Zeman, J., & Garber, G. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and pain: It depends on who iswatching. Child Development, 67, 957–973. doi:10.2307/1131873

Zeman, J., & Shipman, K. (1997). Social-contextual influences on expectancies of managing angerand sadness: The transition from middle childhood to adolescence. Developmental Psychology,33, 917–924. doi:10.1037/0012–1649.33.6.917

Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Development and initial validation ofthe children’s sadness management scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 187–205.doi:10.1023/A:1010623226626

Received 16 September 2010; revised version received 6 June 2011