Providing Consumers with Sale Information: Evidence from a ...

44
Providing Consumers with Sale Information: Evidence from a Field Experiment in Online Supermarket Shopping. Kr Eliaz y , Orli Oren-Kolbinger z and Sarit Weisburd x June 1, 2018 Abstract Does providing consumers with information about discounts help them re- alize more savings? We address this question using data from a eld experiment on a website for online grocery shopping. Our results illustrate the di¢ culty in using information provision to steer shoppers towards cheaper alternatives (of equal or higher quality than their substitutes). We nd that providing (treat- ment) shoppers with promotional information on sale categories increases the probability of purchasing within the category. This e/ect is driven by an increase in purchasing rates for both the reduced priced items and regularly priced substitutes. Our analysis focuses on understanding how item place- ment, promotional information, and the way promotional information is dis- played impact consumer choices in a multi-product environment where prices are changing. Keywords: Limited attention, Salience, Information processing, Supermarket shopping. Financial support from the The Pinhas Sapir Center for Development at Tel-Aviv University and the Michigan Institute for Teaching and Research in Economics is greatfully acknowledged. We are in debt to Benny Wang for his tremendous help with this project. y School of Economics, Tel-Aviv University, [email protected]. z School of Law, Villanova University, [email protected] x School of Economics, Tel-Aviv University, [email protected].

Transcript of Providing Consumers with Sale Information: Evidence from a ...

Providing Consumers with Sale Information:Evidence from a Field Experimentin Online Supermarket Shopping.�

K�r Eliazy, Orli Oren-Kolbingerzand Sarit Weisburdx

June 1, 2018

Abstract

Does providing consumers with information about discounts help them re-alize more savings? We address this question using data from a �eld experimenton a website for online grocery shopping. Our results illustrate the di¢ culty inusing information provision to steer shoppers towards cheaper alternatives (ofequal or higher quality than their substitutes). We �nd that providing (treat-ment) shoppers with promotional information on sale categories increases theprobability of purchasing within the category. This e¤ect is driven by anincrease in purchasing rates for both the reduced priced items and regularlypriced substitutes. Our analysis focuses on understanding how item place-ment, promotional information, and the way promotional information is dis-played impact consumer choices in a multi-product environment where pricesare changing.Keywords: Limited attention, Salience, Information processing, Supermarketshopping.

�Financial support from the The Pinhas Sapir Center for Development at Tel-Aviv Universityand the Michigan Institute for Teaching and Research in Economics is greatfully acknowledged. Weare in debt to Benny Wang for his tremendous help with this project.

ySchool of Economics, Tel-Aviv University, k�[email protected] of Law, Villanova University, [email protected] of Economics, Tel-Aviv University, [email protected].

1 Introduction

A consumer in the modern market place is faced with an overwhelming amount

of information about a multitude of consumption alternatives. There is an ocean of

substitutes for almost every product type, each product type is o¤ered by a myriad of

suppliers, and there are many frequent promotions that cause prices across suppliers

to �uctuate almost continuously. Since consumers may have only a limited amount

of time to devote to price comparisons, they may end up missing bargains that can

help them save money. In light of this, are consumers more likely to buy discounted

items when provided with information about sales? Addressing this question can

improve our understanding of how promotional information a¤ects demand. The

answer is important for both policy makers interested in helping consumers realize

potential savings, and for retailers interested in using discounts to steer consumers

to particular items.

To tackle the above question, we analyze data from a �eld experiment conducted

on a website for online grocery shopping. The experiment consisted of o¤ering tem-

porary discounts on select items and providing information about these discounts to

a (randomly selected) subset of shoppers via weekly emails. The discounted items

were selected such that each had an obvious, more expensive substitute of equal or

lower perceived quality (e.g., organic fruits were priced lower than their conventional

counterparts). Our goal is to understand how information provision impacts the

likelihood of choosing the discounted item over its more expensive (and often lower

perceived quality) substitute, and how this likelihood is a¤ected by product display

(whether a discounted item appears next to its substitute), the amount of infor-

mation provided (e.g. listing categories of items with the largest discounts versus

adding more detailed information on the types of items on sale), and by shoppers�

"attentiveness" (as measured by a proxy we propose).

Our analysis provides a number of key observations. While some of these obser-

vations are intuitive, our study permits us to quantify their magnitudes and their

e¤ect on savings.

Searching for substitutes is costly, and the likelihood of realizing savings from

discounts decreases with search costs. The likelihood of buying a discounted item

(of weakly higher quality) versus its substitute increases by 31 percentage points

(s.e. 14) when the discounted item and its substitute appear adjacent on the screen

(the average pre-discount relative purchase rate is roughly 24 percent).1 The e¤ect

of the sale is much smaller and more noisily measured for items that are farther

apart from their substitutes (an increase of 7.5 percentage points s.e. 10). On

average, purchasing a discounted item versus its substitute resulted in savings of 88

cents relative to the non-discount price (which averaged $2.99). This means that

displaying substitutes next to each other (which then does not require shoppers to

search for the di¤erent substitutes of a particular product) can lead to a 24 percent

increase in the savings uptake, or 21 cents per item for the average shopper. It is

important to keep in mind that the average American supermarket carries close to

40,000 items, so that even if only one percent of these items is on sale, the placement

of these items could result in signi�cant changes to consumer surplus.2

Providing shoppers with information on sales does not necessarily lead to savings.

Notifying shoppers of food categories with discounts increases their probability of

making a purchase within these categories by roughly 200 percent relative to the

control group (an increase of 1.6 percentage points (s.e. 0.6) at an average purchase

rate of 0.8 percent (s.d. 9)).3 During each week of the experiment discounts were

o¤ered in roughly 13 food categories, such that shoppers could save a total of $11 if

they switched to each discounted item from its more expensive substitute in every

category. However, the bene�t of the sale only averaged $2, as 70 percent of the

increase in purchases made by treatment shoppers was due to an increase in the

purchase rate of the more expensive substitute item.

The framing of information impacts choices. The largest di¤erence between the

treatment and control groups is observed for items in the discounted food category

that was listed �rst in the treatment email. During most weeks the top listed cate-

gory consisted of items with the smallest discounts, and while control shoppers were

less likely to purchase discounted items in this category, treatment shoppers were

more likely to purchase. Thus, the di¤erence in purchasing rates between the treat-

ment and control group was 3 times larger in the �rst item category with treatment

1Whether a pair of substitutes are displayed next to each other is independent of their prices,or of the di¤erence between their prices. Buying a substitute item was on average 28 percent moreexpensive than the on-sale target item for non-neighboring items, and 25 percent more expensivethan the on-sale target item for neighboring items.

2The Food Marketing Institute, a trade group, reported that the average number of items carriedin a US supermarket in 2016 was 38,900.

3Speci�cally, treatment shoppers were told what are the four categories with the biggest sales,and what is the largest discount (in percentage terms) in each of these categories.

shoppers increasing their purchase rate of the discounted item by 1.8 percentage

points (s.e. 0.8) more than the control group.

The impact of information provision depends on the �attentiveness�of a shopper.

Each week all shoppers (both treatment and control) were noti�ed via email of a

weekly promotion o¤ering an immediate rebate (applied at check-out) that would be

awarded for buying at least one unit of an eligible item on the site.4 A shopper may

be described as being in an �attentive state� if he purchases the rebate-awarding

item whenever the rebate is higher than the item�s price (this was true in most

weeks, and in some weeks the di¤erence was at least $9). Treatment shoppers in

an attentive state are almost twice as likely to purchase the discounted item (an

increase of 10 percentage points (s.e. 3) ) relative to control shoppers who increase

their purchase rate by 5.2 percentage points (s.e. 4.8). Furthermore, treatment

shoppers in an attentive state did not increase their demand for the more expensive

substitute. In contrast, treatment shoppers in an inattentive state are 10 percentage

points (s.e. 5.9) more likely than control shoppers in an inattentive state to buy the

more expensive item.

The above observations suggest that search-costs or limited-attention introduce

frictions that may prevent shoppers from realizing savings opportunities even when

they are noti�ed of these opportunities. In particular, coarse information - specifying

only the product categories with the biggest sales - can also lead to higher purchases

of the items that are not on sale.

The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. Section 2 discusses related

literature. The design of the randomized control trials is explained in Section 3.

Summary statistics on the sample are provided in Section 4 and the results are

discussed in Section 5. Section 6 discusses the responses to a post-experiment survey

regarding consumer preferences. Section 7 concludes.

2 Related literature

Our paper is motivated by recent theoretical analysis of the e¤ect of salience on

consumer behavior. Most notably, Bordalo, Gennaioli and Shleifer (2016) propose a

model of how one product attribute may be more salient than another. In our data,

4To avoid situations where customers only bought a single item of the eligible good, the rebatewas also conditional on spending at least $20.

salience plays a role in terms of which discounts are more prominent (e.g., appear on

the same line on the computer screen).

A number of studies provide experimental evidence on how individuals manage

limited attention. Gabaix, Laibson, Moloche and Weinberg (2006) provide evidence

that laboratory behavior of subjects in experiments where instrumental information

was costly to acquire (either �nancially or because time was scarce) matches the

predictions of a boundedly rational model where individuals use only approximate

option-value calculations. Caplin and Dean (2013) use a laboratory design to test

a behavioral property of the rational inattention model. In contrast to our work,

these studies have been performed on students in laboratory settings. A recent

exception is Bartos, Bauer, Chytilová and Majt¼eka (2016), who provide evidence

from �eld experiments in rental housing and job applications showing that suppliers

in these markets do not acquire all the available information (they do not view the

resumes of all the applicants), but rather focus their attention only on a select group

of applicants, based on stereotypical attributes of that group (employers are more

likely to view resumes of majority candidates while landlords are more likely to view

resumes of minority candidates).

The prevalence of limited attention also suggests that �nudges�or reminders that

make some information salient may help individuals make better decisions. In our

study consumers in the treatment group were sent emails alerting them to the item

categories with the biggest sales. We �nd that this nudge increased the probability

of purchasing within a category but not necessarily the on-sale item.

There are only a few studies that present empirical evidence on the implications

of limited attention on consumer behavior. The main obstacle in providing such

evidence is the di¢ culty in obtaining data on the information that consumers paid

attention to. One line of inquiry investigated the impact of making associated fees

salient to consumers. Hossain and Morgan (2006) conduct a �eld experiment on

eBay and �nd that when two auctions o¤er the same e¤ective total price, more

bidders are attracted to the auction with a lower opening price and higher shipping

price. Chetty, Looney and Kroft (2009) provide evidence from a �eld experiment in a

grocery store showing that posting prices that include taxes reduces demand. Blake,

Moshary, Sweeney and Tadelis (2017) use data from a �eld experiment carried out

by an online retailer to show that up-front display of the total cost of each available

item, including all fees, (as opposed to displaying only the listed prices and adding

the fees at check out) a¤ects not only the likelihood of purchase but also the quality

of the items purchased.

A second strand of literature focused on the extent to which consumers search for

the best prices. De los Santos, Hortacsu, and Wildenbeest (2012) use a large data set

on web browsing and purchasing behavior to test whether consumers are searching in

accordance to various classical search models. Helmers, Krishnan and Patnam (2015)

use a unique data set from an online retailer to show that consumers are more likely

to buy products that receive a saliency shock when they are recommended by new

products.5 Clerides and Courty (2015) use scanner data from a supermarket chain

to show that during periods in which the price of a discounted pack of detergent

was lower than the corresponding price of a larger (�economy-size�) pack (of the

same product), consumers still bought the larger, and more expensive pack. This

�nding suggests that some consumers are either not comparing all prices, or are

not computing (or computing erroneously) the price per-unit when making their

purchasing decisions.

This study adds to the existing literature by examining how consumers�shopping

decisions are a¤ected by the relative prices of substitute goods and how di¤erent

levels of information provisions a¤ect these choices. Our experimental design sheds

new light on the di¢ culty consumers face in allocating their attention and in making

optimal choices even when the information is readily available on a single webpage.

Importantly, as supermarket shopping involves repeat purchases of goods over short

time periods, we are able to verify the robustness of our results when examining the

same individual making choices over the same products at varying prices.

3 Experimental design

The platform. We partnered with a website that o¤ers a purchase and next day

delivery service from a large U.S. supermarket in a University city. The website

includes roughly 3,000 items that are sold in the supermarket store. These items

are divided into several categories to help shoppers perform an intuitive search (e.g.

produce, dairy, etc.). Shoppers can also search the website for any item they would

like to purchase using a search command. Shoppers need to add the items that they

5The salience shock was created by a group of items that appeared below each product with thetitle "You May Also Like."

would like to purchase to their basket, and at checkout they pay for the products plus

a �at delivery fee of $2.99 for each order. During the period of the experiment there

was no option to re-order previous baskets or to add items from previous orders. In

addition, all prices were �xed and there were no promotional sales. Shoppers are

required to choose a delivery date and a two-hour delivery window. The cuto¤ time

for next day delivery is midnight every day. These shoppers are mainly students

(80 percent) with some professors (10 percent). Only 10 percent of shoppers are

una¢ liated with the University.6

The website was interested in encouraging its registered customers to increase

the frequency and volume of their purchases, and to learn how di¤erent promotional

tactics a¤ect shopping behavior. To achieve this goal, it planned to conduct a series

of randomized control trials, and agreed to allow us to in�uence the design of these

trials in a way that would also enable us to address our questions. Hence, the

experimental design was somewhat constrained by the objectives of the website.

Temporary discounts. The experiment was conducted over a period of 13 weeks

during which the website o¤ered temporary discounts so that the prices of some

select items �uctuated, dropping during the sale and rising when the sale expires.

Discounted items were marked on the website with two asterisks (**), and a footnote

at the bottom of the screen explained that the marked item was on sale and the

original higher price was speci�ed. This method of marking discounts was used

because of the following reasoning. First, we did not want discounts to be too salient

so there would be an advantage for receiving an email that provided information on

which items were discounted. Second, we wanted to allow any shopper who accessed

the website to �nd out about the temporary sale if he exerted some e¤ort in noticing

�ne details.7

The experiment focused on items in 28 categories that were popular with shoppers

in the pre-experiment period (see Table (1)).8 All of these categories include at

least 2 items that could be considered substitutes. Each month a di¤erent set of6This information was obtained from responses to an optional survey conducted at checkout

during the �rst month of the experiment period. Eighty percent of the shoppers who made apurchase during the experiment period responded to the survey.

7We operated under the constraint that all shoppers must face the same exact set of prices.8The 28 item categories are: bananas, kiwis, lemons, raspberries, apples, bulk apples, blueberries,

pineapples, avocados, broccoli, cucumbers, kale, onions, green onions, peppers, lettuces, limes,tomatoes, bread, organic bread, eggs, brown eggs, organic eggs, milk, bulk milk, organic milk,yogurt, and water.

categories were discounted so that a discount on an item was valid for one month.

The items whose prices were manipulated during the experiment were de�ned as

target items, and their alternatives were de�ned as substitute items. During the

period with the lowest relative discounts (in percentages) on target items, the highest

discount was 25%, while during the period with the highest relative discounts, the

maximal discount was 75%. See Tables (2-3) for a full list of the relevant target and

substitute items as well as the discounts given during the experiment period.

These discounted target items fell into four general categories: (i) organic and

conventional items, (ii) same items that are o¤ered in di¤erent sizes (e.g. jumbo

avocado and regular avocado) or bulk quantities (e.g. apples, that are o¤ered as

single units or 3lb bags, or milk that is o¤ered in 0.5gal and 1gal) (iii) brand names

vs. generic store brand (e.g. Aunt Millie�s breads vs. generic supermarket whole

wheat bread), and (iv) two competing brands of the same exact product (e.g., Dasani

vs. Ice Mountain mineral water in bottles of the same size).

There are two motivating factors behind the choice of target items. First, we tried

to select target items that had �almost perfect�substitutes and which had low levels

of brand loyalty. Recent evidence suggests that consumers display relatively low

brand loyalty to supermarket items as compared to clothing and appliances (Nielsen

(2013)), and their choice of food brands is most a¤ected by price considerations

(Byron (2008)). Within the food and beverage category, consumers tend to exhibit

more brand loyalty to breakfast cereals, carbonated drinks and snacks (Chidmi and

Lopez (2007), Nielsen (2013)). None of these were included as target items in the

experiment, hence, we assume that price sensitivity is stronger than brand loyalty in

deciding between a target item and its substitute.9

The second motivating factor is the public perception of organic items. Studies

have indicated that consumers generally express positive attitudes toward organic

foods, perceiving them as tastier and kinder to the environment (Roddy et al. (1996),

Magnusson et al. (2001), Perkovic and Orquin (2017)). While there may be disagree-

ment among researchers whether this perception is backed by scienti�c evidence (see

Baransky et al. (2014) for a meta-analysis that claims there are healthier aspects of

9In a post study questionnaire of the participants, 80% of 55 individuals who responded answeredthat they would switch brands for a discount of 20%. We found a similar response when surveyingan additional 378 US respondents in the same age and education categories. See Section 6 for moredetail.

organic food), what is important for the current study is the public perception.10

An important feature of the discounted items was the variation in their display:

Some close substitutes (where one was discounted and the other was not) appeared

next to each other on the screen, while others appeared in di¤erent rows and re-

quired scrolling down to notice both items. Whether a target item and its substitute

appeared on the same row was independent of their prices relative to other products.

We will use the variation in location as a proxy for the cost involved in comparing

the price of a target item with its substitutes.

Rebates. In weekly emails, shoppers were o¤ered an immediate rebate (applied

at the time of checkout) if they spent at least $20 and also bought at least one unit

of an item from a given group of eligible items (which changed every week). During

the �rst three weeks of the study, the rebate was equal to the �at delivery fee of

$2.99 (it was presented to shoppers as free delivery), and in the last three weeks it

was raised to $10.11 Between the fourth and the eleventh week the rebate was $2.99

for the control group and $10 for the treatment group (the di¤erence between these

two groups is explained below).

Table (4) lists the rebate item o¤er for each week as well as both the price of

the item and bene�t of purchase for individuals in both the treatment and control

groups. In almost all weeks the rebate o¤er was strictly higher than the price of a

single unit of an eligible item. This meant that a consumer who noticed this o¤er

could save a non-negligible amount of money by buying a single unit of an eligible

item even if he did not consume it after (as evident from Table (4) a single unit of

an eligible item was often a piece of fruit or vegetable so that it did not require extra

storage). As explained in the next section, we can use this fact as a proxy for how

much attention the shopper paid to the consumption problem.

Treatment and control. The 355 shoppers who had made purchases in the sec-

ond half of 2015 were randomly divided into two groups � 178 in treatment and

177 in control.12 The di¤erence between these groups was that treatment shoppers

10In our post study questionnaire, 91% of 55 responders said they would buy an organic item ifits price was weakly cheaper than a conventional version of the same item. This result also held inan additional survey follow up with 378 participants. See Section 6.11Starting o¤ with free delivery before moving to the high rebate was also meant to give some

credibility to this promotional o¤er.12While we have data on shoppers beginning in December 2014 (over a year before the experiment

was run) we only include shoppers who had made a purchase within the previous 6 months when

received additional information on discounted items in the weekly email. In order

to separately measure the e¤ect of the email contents from a general salience e¤ect

or compliance e¤ect, both groups were sent weekly promotional emails. However,

during the entire period of the study the email to the control group did not mention

any price discounts.

In contrast, the email to the treatment group displayed the following: four product

categories (e.g., milk, eggs, fruits, bread) that were on (temporary) sale that month,

the item with the biggest discount in each category (expressed in percentage points)

and a link to the relevant page of each category. The treatment group was also

informed that discounted items were marked by �**�.

During the second half of the study (from the sixth week on), shoppers in the

treatment group began to receive a more detailed weekly email. For these weeks

the email included a line alerting shoppers to the fact that many organic items were

now on sale and even cheaper than non-organic items. Additionally, those who had

purchased a substitute item in a category that is now on sale received a personalized

email alerting them to this fact (e.g. "Don�t forget to consider some alternatives to

your last purchase of eggs that we have on sale this month"). Figure (1) and (2)

depict examples of the email formats for both the treatment and control group.

4 The Data

This paper analyzes purchasing decisions made by 355 shoppers over the 13 weeks

of the experiment in 28 product categories (177 shoppers were assigned to control

and 178 to treatment).13 For each of these 355 shoppers, we track their decision

of whether or not to make a purchase in each category over the duration of the

experiment (129,220 observations). In total, 1,046 category purchases were made

over 338 shopping trips by 130 shoppers during the experiment period. 167 by 66

shoppers in the control group and 171 by 64 shoppers in the treatment group.

Table (5) provides summary statistics in the pre-experiment period (December

de�ning the original treatment and control groups. We expected these shoppers to be the mostlikely to make purchases during the period of the experiment.13The 28 item categories are: bananas, kiwis, lemons, raspberries, apples, bulk apples, blueberries,

pineapples, avocados, broccoli, cucumbers, kale, onions, green onions, peppers, lettuces, limes,tomatoes, bread, organic bread, eggs, brown eggs, organic eggs, milk, bulk milk, organic milk,yogurt, and water.

2014 - January 2016) for both the full sample and a subset of 305 shoppers who

had a history of purchasing in at least one of the 28 item categories (152 in control

and 153 in treatment). This subset is important as it turns out that past purchases

within the category are a very strong predictor of current purchases with di¤erential

e¤ects between those allocated to the control and treatment groups. Not surprisingly,

since individuals were randomly allocated to treatment and control, there are no

signi�cant di¤erences in shopping trends between the treatment and control groups.

Generally, shoppers had shopped on the site 5 times prior to the experiment with

trips averaging roughly $70. Importantly, when conditioning on shoppers who made

purchases of either the target or substitute items, the control and treatment groups

continue to look very similar. Generally, the substitute items were purchased much

more frequently than the target items.

Recall that when a shopper browses through items, some discounted target items

are displayed right next to their substitutes (or in the same row), while others may

require scrolling down. In light of this, we say that a target item and its substitute

are �neighbors�if they appear on the same line on the website. Figure (4) illustrates

this by displaying a screenshot from the website. The target item that is shown,

organic bananas, was on sale for $0.24 per unit (regular price $0.49), while the two

corresponding - and adjacent - substitutes are �banana ripe� and �banana mild

green�whose prices remained constant at $0.39 per unit. Six out of the twenty-eight

item categories are neighbors (avocados, bananas, kiwis, lemons, raspberries, and

water).14 These neighboring categories make up roughly a quarter of purchases of

target items and almost a third of substitute item purchases. If comparing prices

among neigboring items is simpler, we would expect shoppers to be more likely

(not) to purchase a discounted target item when its neighboring substitute is more

expensive (cheaper).

5 Findings

This section examines how all shoppers respond to exogenous price changes, and

then measures the impact of information on this response by di¤erentiating between

14The 22 non-neighboring item categories are: apples, bulk apples, blueberries, pineapples, broc-coli, cucumbers, kale, onions, green onions, peppers, lettuces, limes, tomatoes, bread, organic bread,eggs, brown eggs, organic eggs, milk, bulk milk, organic milk, yogurt. See a detailed explanation inTables (2) & (3).

shoppers in the treatment and control groups. Surprisingly, our results suggest that

the treatment group responds by purchasing more of the higher priced (weakly lower

quality) substitute items. In order to understand whether increased information is

the factor driving this outcome, we move on to examine the di¤erential responses of

the treatment and control groups across speci�c item categories and across di¤erent

types of shopping trips.

We �rst focus on categories that appeared at the top of the email sent to the

treatment group.15 If making information more accessible to the treatment group

is what drives the di¤erence in outcomes between treatment and control, we would

expect the di¤erence to be sharpest for the most salient categories. We next estimate

the extent to which providing additional detail in the treatment email regarding sales

on organic items with shopper speci�c suggestions impacted outcomes. If information

is the mechanism driving our results, we would expect a stronger di¤erences in on-

sale purchasing rates between the treatment and control group during weeks when

the treatment group received more detailed sale information. Finally, we focus on

purchases in the rebate item category. While only treatment shoppers were noti�ed

of sales, both treatment and control shoppers were encouraged to buy in the rebate

item category. This provides an opportunity to di¤erentiate between a salience shock

that shoppers in both the treatment and control group received and an information

shock that was received only by treatment shoppers.

Are shopping trips heterogeneously impacted by information? Throughout the

experiment, shoppers in both the treatment and control groups were given the op-

portunity to save a signi�cant amount of money on their shopping trip if they pur-

chased a rebate item. This provides an opportunity to di¤erentiate between shoppers

making purchases on the site in an attentive state (purchased the rebate item) and

inattentive state (missed out on savings from rebate item). We show that this classi-

�cation plays an important role in predicting the impact of information on shopping

decisions.

5.1 The E¤ect of a Sale on Purchasing Decisions

Figure (3) graphs the fraction of shoppers purchasing the target item versus the

substitute in the same category over time. This �gure focuses on the fraction of

15In the �rst 3 weeks of the experiment this line focused on the vegetables category, weeks 4 and5 focused on eggs, weeks 6-9 focused on yogurt, and weeks 10-13 focused on milk.

shoppers buying within a category (which includes target and substitute items) who

choose a target item and how this ratio changes during the sale period. Each item

category is mapped to 1 during the �rst two weeks of it going on sale, and 2 for the

last two weeks of it remaining on sale. The purchase rate at time 0 is the purchase

rate in the two weeks leading up to the sale, and the purchase rate in time 3 refers

to the purchase rate 2 weeks after the sale. Thus, prior to the sale, shoppers selected

the target item roughly 35 percent of the time. During the sale period this number

increases to roughly 50 percent of purchases and then returns to the prior purchase

level after the sale. While this di¤erence is large in size, the estimate is noisy.

Table (6) illustrates that the largest and most signi�cant e¤ect relates to the

choice between a substitute and target item that appear on the same line on the

webpage. Thus, while generally the probability of purchasing a target item instead

of a substitute item increases by 13 percentage points (s.e. 7.9) during the sale

period, for those items that appear on the same line as their substitute, the e¤ect is

a 30 percentage point increase (s.e. 14.2). This suggests that inattention may have

played a role, leading some shoppers to choose dominated items (lower perceived

quality and more expensive).

Table (7) further illustrates the change in purchasing trends created by the tar-

get item sale. This analysis di¤erentiates between 3 di¤erent outcomes for the full

sample of 355 shoppers over the 13 weeks of the experiment in each of the 28 product

categories. The �rst 3 columns of Table (7) examine the extent to which the sale

increased purchases within the item category. In other words, did the sale primarily

cause shoppers to shift their purchase from the substitute to target item, or alterna-

tively, draw shoppers to purchase in an additional category. The second 3 columns

under the title "Buy Target" examine the e¤ect on the on-sale target item. The last

3 columns of the table under the title "Buy Substitute" examine the e¤ect of the

target item sale on the substitute item. The analysis includes individuals in the sam-

ple who did not make a shopping trip during that week. For these shoppers: make

purchase, buy target, and buy substitute are equal to zero for all product categories

in that week.

All speci�cations in Table (7) include week, item and shopper �xed e¤ects. The

average purchase rate within item categories is 0.8 percent (s.d. 9) with a rate of

0.2 percent (s.d. 5) buying target and 0.5 percent (s.d. 7.4) buying the substitute.

We observe a general increase in purchasing rates within product categories during a

target item sale. This increase is driven by shoppers roughly tripling their purchase

rate of the target item during the sale period (increasing their probability of purchase

by 0.6 percentage points (s.e. 0.2)). Table (7) illustrates that the e¤ect of the sale

is concentrated among shoppers purchasing in a category where they have a history

of shopping previously. Despite these signi�cant responses to the sale, Figure (6)

still illustrates that while some shoppers (15 percent) move from purchasing the

substitute to the target item when it goes on sale, many (50 percent) pay the same

price or more to remain with the substitute item.

5.2 The Role of Promotional Materials

Why did a signi�cant proportion of shoppers choose apparently dominated alterna-

tives on their shopping trips (more expensive and of lower quality)? One plausible

explanation may be that shoppers were not fully attentive to all available discounts.16

If we focus on treatment shoppers who received additional information on the sale -

how much do they alter their shopping decisions relative to the control group?

Before discussing the results of this analysis it is important to clarify the random

assignment that occurred between the treatment and control groups. Shoppers in the

sample were randomly allocated to treatment (receiving a weekly promotional email

with sale information and a rebate item) and control (receiving a weekly promotional

email with a rebate item). Thus, examining shopping outcomes based on allocation

to treatment and control group provides "intention to treat" results. We therefore

focus primarily on these outcomes as opposed to limiting the sample to shoppers

who made purchases or read the promotional email which could introduce selection

concerns.17

Shoppers in the treatment group were much more a¤ected by sales than shoppers

in the control (see Table (8)). Speci�cally, in item categories where they had made

purchases in the past, they increased their purchase rate by 1.2 percentage points

(s.e. 0.4) at an average purchase rate of 0.8 percent in the sample. If anything,

shoppers with a history of purchases in the control group decrease their purchase

16An alternative hypothesis may be that many shoppers suspected that an item on sale was oflower quality (close to expiration date). However, in our post-study questionnaire only 3 out of 27respondents said they did not buy an item on sale because they thought it was of lower quality orclose to its expiration date.17While we attempted to collect information on email opens, there are concerns about the accu-

racy of this measure. We discuss this further in Subsection 5.4.

rate during this period by -0.4 percentage points (s.e. 0.4). Some of this e¤ect on

purchasing rates is a result of increased purchases of the target item (an increase of

0.8 percentage points (s.e. 0.3)) that is driven by item categories where the target

and substitute appear on the same line (an increase of 1.1 percentage points (s.e.

6)). This suggests that a consumer who considers a particular option is more likely

to notice �neighboring�options than options that require scrolling down.

The largest sale e¤ect measured in Table (8) relates to the purchase of substitute

items (see column (9)). Shoppers in the treatment group who have a history of pur-

chasing in a category are 1.5 (s.e. 0.5) percentage points more likely than shoppers

in the control group to purchase a substitute item during the period when the alter-

native target item was on sale. The measured di¤erence is three times smaller and

not statistically signi�cant for neigboring items.

Why would receiving information on category sales increase the probability of

purchasing a substitute item? One possible explanation is that the email to the

treatment group impacted two separate shopping decisions. The �rst is what item

categories to purchase, and the second, is whether to purchase the substitute or target

item. In other words, receiving an email that noti�es you that vegetables are on-sale

may increase the probability of purchasing vegetables on the site. This increase could

be driven by your interest in the sale and/or a salience reminder that you would like

to buy vegetables. This salience reminder is unique to the treatment group and could

lead to an increase in purchases of the substitute item. Purchasing the substitute

may be especially likely in categories where this item is visually separated from the

on-sale target item. Shoppers who have a history of buying in a given category are

more likely to be familiar with the substitute items, which were purchased 3-4 times

more frequently than the target items in the pre experiment period.

An alternative explanation to the di¤erential information explanation that we

just described is one of di¤erential incentives. Recall that the size of the rebate

ranged between $2.99 and $10 over the di¤erent weeks of the experiment. We control

for rebate size in all speci�cations, as there were weeks where the treatment group

received a $10 o¤er, while control shoppers received a $2.99 o¤er. In order to make

sure that our results are not driven by a selection issue where certain types respond

to a $2.99 versus $10 rebate o¤er we re-run our analysis from Table (8) including

only weeks when the treatment and control group received the same rebate o¤er.

Table (9) illustrates that the observed di¤erences in behavior between the treatment

and control groups cannot be explained by di¤erential incentives.

5.3 Does Content Matter?

If the saliency of item categories is driving the purchasing patterns observed in Table

(8), then we might expect this phenomenon to be strongest in item categories that

appeared �rst in the treatment email. It is speci�cally in these item categories

where the treatment group received the largest salience shock (assuming they are

impacted more by information they see �rst) and the control group did not receive

any information. We focus our comparison on non-neighboring item categories, as

there are weeks where their broader category (vegetables) was on sale and appeared

in the �rst line of the email but not all sub categories (e.g. tomatoes) were on sale.

This allows us to separately identify the e¤ect of appearing in the �rst sale category in

the treatment email from actually being on sale. We also only include the purchasing

decisions of shoppers in item categories where they have made a purchase in the pre-

experiment period. Table (8) illustrates that these are the decisions that are being

most impacted by the target sales.

Table (10) compares the purchasing decisions of the treatment and control groups

for general item categories versus �rst-item categories. Purchases of the target item

increased on average by 0.5 percentage points (s.e. 0.3) during the sale period for

shoppers in the control group in categories outside of the �rst item categories. This

increase was primarily driven by "switchers" as these shoppers decreased their pur-

chase rate of the substitute item by 0.7 percentage points (s.e. 0.4). While, the

treatment group similarly increased their purchase rate of the target items in these

categories by 0.6 percentage points (s.e. 0.3) they also increased their purchase rate of

the substitute items by 0.3 percentage points (s.e. 0.4). Thus, in these sale categories

that are less salient to the treatment group, we observe fewer people purchasing the

discounted item, but a similar number of substitute item purchases to that observed

among all categories in Table (8).

The e¤ect of the sale on target purchases of the control group was smaller for

the �rst-item categories (and was even below regular purchase rates). During most

weeks these �rst item categories were those with the smallest discounts which may

explain the lower interest in these items. However, the treatment group behaves

signi�cantly di¤erently in regard to these �rst item categories. They increase their

purchasing rate of the on-sale target items by an additional 1.8 percentage points

(s.e. 0.8) relative to the control group at an average purchase rate of 0.8 percent

(s.d. 9.1). The treatment group also increases their purchase rate of the substitute

items by 1.6 percentage points (s.e. 1.1) relative to the control group at an average

purchase rate of 2.1 percent (s.d. 14.5). Thus, the di¤erences between the treatment

and control group are largest for these more salient �rst item categories.

In Table (11) we consider the impact of more detailed emails on the treatment

group. During this period, the treatment email always included a line alerting shop-

pers to the fact that many organic items are on sale and in some cases even cheaper

than the non-organic alternative. Additionally, if the shopper in the treatment group

had purchased a substitute item in his/her previous trip, these personalized emails

included the line "you may want to consider some alternatives to your last purchase

in category � that are now on sale."

During the non-detail weeks, the signi�cant observed change in shopping behavior

is that shoppers in the treatment group increase their purchase rate of non-organic

items (substitutes) by 1.5 percentage points (s.e. 0.6) in categories that include an

on-sale organic item (target). This e¤ect is unique to non-neighboring categories,

as treatment shoppers if anything decrease their purchase rate of substitutes by 0.9

percentage points (s.e. 1.7) in neighboring categories. During this same period, the

control group increase their purchase of the substitute item by 0.2 percentage points

(s.e. 0.7). This e¤ect is much smaller in size and more noisily measured.

During the detail email weeks, shoppers in the treatment group increase their

purchase rate of on-sale organic target items by 0.9 percentage points (s.e. 0.5)

and are much less likely to purchase the substitute items in these categories than

they were during the non-detail weeks (a measured increase of 0.2 percentage points

(s.e. 0.5)). However, since we observe a similar increase of 0.7 percentage points

(s.e. 0.4) in purchase of the target item among control shoppers during these detail

weeks, we cannot attribute this e¤ect to an information e¤ect.18 Thus, the e¤ect of

more detailed information was primarily a reduction in purchasing "mistakes" of the

substitute item for treatment shoppers.

In order to further examine the source of the increased purchasing rate of substi-

tute items during a sale period, we di¤erentiate between the general e¤ect of a sale

18This di¤erence between detail and non detail weeks could be driven by di¤erences in the targetitems that were on sale or in the size of the discount that was o¤ered.

and the e¤ect of a sale in the rebate item category in Table (12). This comparison

is important because shoppers in both the treatment and control group received a

salience reminder to purchase in the rebate item category, whereas only the treat-

ment group received a summary of the other item categories where sales were taking

place. Table (12) illustrates that shoppers in the treatment group are much more

likely to purchase a target item in the rebate category than the control group when

the target and substitute items are "neighbors" (an increase of 2.8 percentage points

(s.e. 1.4) relative to an average purchase rate of 1.6 percent (s.d. 12.7). This e¤ect

does not appear for other items.

For categories outside of the rebate item, the treatment group increased their

purchase rate of substitute items relative to the control group during the target item

sale (an increase of 1 percentage point (s.e. 0.4)). Note, that while the increase

in treatment purchases of the target item was driven by neighboring categories, the

increase of substitute purchases is driven by non-neighboring categories. This e¤ect

remains in the rebate item categories, but is much more noisily measured (a 1.3

percentage point increase (s.e. 1).

Our results so far suggest that the way information was presented in the treatment

email impacted shopper choices. The di¤erence in purchase rates of the on-sale target

items between treatment shoppers and control shoppers was largest in categories that

appeared �rst in the email and in situations where both groups received a saliency

shock to the category (rebate item categories). However, the observed increase in

substitute item purchases for the control group is fairly similar across these di¤erent

salience categories. While we do observe a decrease in this "mistake" during detailed

email weeks, this detail did not signi�cantly increase the likelihood of treatment

shoppers purchasing the on-sale target item. In other words, it seems information

helped shoppers to avoid these "mistakes" by simply not purchasing in the category,

as opposed to purchasing the on-sale item. In the next subsection we explore whether

a proxy for "shopping inattention" can explain the observed increase in purchasing

rates of the substitute items among treatment shoppers during a target sale period.

5.4 Heterogenous E¤ects of Promotional Materials on Pur-

chasing Decisions

Is it possible that the information provided in the email was more helpful to some

consumers than others? If we could categorize shoppers in both the treatment and

control group as being in an attentive or inattentive state, would they be di¤erentially

e¤ected by the information provided in the email? During the experiment period, all

shoppers were awarded either free shipping (with a value of $2.99) or an immediate

rebate of $10 if they purchased the weekly rebate item and spent $20 or more. The

price of the rebate item, during most weeks, was lower than $2.99, and more than 95%

of purchases in this period were above $20. Hence, conditional on paying attention

to the promotional email, adding the rebate item to the basket (in most weeks) is

the rational response to these o¤ers (see Table(4)). However, if a shopper did not

pay close attention to the email, then he might miss or misunderstand the rebate

o¤er and not take advantage of it. 19 We can use this information to characterize the

state of a shopper as attentive if they purchased the rebate item during their trip

and inattentive otherwise.

Table (13) focuses on shopping trips conducted during weeks when the rebate

was higher than the price of the rebate item. The reason for this is that we are only

able to classify a shopper as attentive or inattentive if they made a purchase during

a week when adding the rebate item was the rational response to paying attention

to the email. This di¤ers from our previous analysis, where we included all shoppers

in all weeks of the experiment. It is important to clarify that shoppers in both the

treatment and control group were o¤ered the rebate on their shopping trip as long

as they purchased in the rebate item category (which included both a target and

substitute item). The average purchase rate of the target (substitute) items during

these conducted shopping trips is 10.1 percent (s.d. 30.1) (25 percent (s.d. 43.3)).

Shoppers in the control group in an attentive state (who purchased the rebate

item) increased their purchase rate of on-sale target items by 5.2 percentage points

19One may be concerned that some shoppers did not respond to the rebate o¤er because theysuspected a hidden "catch". However, in our post study questionnaire only two out of 44 responderssaid they did not take advantage of the rebate o¤er because they thought it was not a good deal.Roughly 39% of the responders stated that they did not have time to consider the rebate o¤er,while 50% said they did not need the eligible item. Note, however, that in many cases shopperscould save on average $6 by simply adding a single piece of a vegetable or fruit. See Section 6 forfurther discussion of this.

(s.e. 4.8). This e¤ect was much larger and more precisely measured for those in the

treatment group who increased their purchase rate of these items by 9.9 percentage

points (s.e. 2.9). For these treatment shoppers in an attentive state, the sale had

almost no e¤ect on the purchase rate of substitute items in the categories with on-

sale target items (a change of -0.4 percentage points s.e. (4.8)). However, shoppers

who fail to purchase the rebate item behave very di¤erently.

Shoppers in the control group in an inattentive state (who did not purchase the

rebate item) increased their purchase rate of on-sale target items by 1.5 percentage

points (s.e. 3.1). This e¤ect is larger but still very noisy for those in the treatment

group (5.7 percentage points (s.e. 4.3)). It is shoppers in the treatment group in

an inattentive state who show a large increase in the purchase of substitute items

relative to the control group during a target sale in that category. Speci�cally these

shoppers in the treatment group in an inattentive state are 10 percentage points

(s.e. 5.9) more likely to add a substitute item to their shopping cart than the control

group.

In our discussion so far, we have labeled these shoppers as being in an attentive

or inattentive state, but is it possible that they are compliers versus non-compliers?

Where compliers read their email and are aware of the target sale if they are in

the treatment group and non-compliers are not. In order to address this concern

we collected information on whether or not the weekly email was opened for all

individuals in the sample. Generally, the email was opened 38 percent of the time

for shoppers in both the treatment and control groups. While this can provide an

indicator of compliance it is not exact due to concerns regarding the accuracy of this

measure.20 Selection is an additional concern with using this compliance information.

Generally, we may be concerned that only more careful shoppers in the treatment

group will be compliers which would bias our results. In this way our experiment

is unique in that we are also able to focus on compliers in both the treatment and

control groups. When we constrain the sample in Table (13) to shoppers who opened

their weekly email, we are left with 752 observations on shoppers in an attentive

20There are a few issues that lower the accuracy of this measure. First, if emails are forwardedto a di¤erent account it will be marked in our data as unopened. Additionally, even if a treatmentshopper does not open emails every week, after the email was opened once this shopper will bebetter informed about sales than a control shopper (due to the added information on sales beingmarked with asteriks). There is also the opposite concern as some spam detectors will result inemails being marked as opened in our database regardless of consumer behavior. We are thereforecareful in applying this information to our analysis.

state and 503 by shoppers in an inattentive state. This increases the variance of our

estimates substantially as only 461 of the 672 item purchases analyzed in Table (13)

were conducted by shoppers who were tagged as opening their email.

Table (14) summarizes the results for shoppers in both the treatment and control

groups for weeks where we observed them opening their emails. Generally, shoppers

who opened their email were more likely to purchase the target item in both the

treatment and control groups (compared to estimates from Table (13)). This result

is not surprising as more careful shoppers may be more likely to read emails. Thus,

highlighting the importance of being able to identify these "complier" types in both

the treatment and control groups. It is important to clarify that the analysis in Tables

(13) and (14) only includes shoppers during weeks when they made a purchase on

the site. Therefore, this result cannot be explained by shoppers reading the email

because they plan to go shopping and subsequently being more likely to make a

purchase.

Excluding shoppers who did not open their email from the analysis in Table

(13) shrinks the reported di¤erence in purchase rates of the target item between

treatment and control shoppers in attentive and inattentive states. Shoppers in

the control group in an inattentive state, increased their purchase rate of the on-

sale target items by 5.3 percentage points (s.e. 5.9). This e¤ect is larger and more

precisely measured for those in the treatment group (9.7 percentage points (s.e. 5.3)).

Shoppers in the control group in an attentive state, increased their purchase of the

target item by 7.4 percentage points (s.e. 5.8). The e¤ect on the treatment group

shoppers is larger, an increase of 11.1 (s.e. 3.2) on their purchase rate of target items.

Table (14) shows similar estimated di¤erences in comparing the purchase rates of

substitute items between treatment and control shoppers in attentive and inattentive

states to those shown in Table (13). While these estimates are much more noisily

measured, they continue to suggest that treatment shoppers in an attentive state

behave di¤erently than they do in an inattentive state. Treatment shoppers in an

attentive state decrease their purchase rate of the substitute items relative to the

control group, the opposite seems to be occur in an inattentive state.

6 Discussion

Our interpretation of the data relies on the following key assumptions:

1. Shoppers are willing to purchase an item they do not need if this leads to

signi�cant savings.

2. Shoppers prefer organic items if they are not more expensive than their non-

organic counterparts.

3. Shoppers would switch brands if a competing brand is reduced to or below the

price of the regular brand they usually purchase.

In order to ensure the validity of these assumptions, we conducted two follow up

surveys. The �rst was sent only to the participants of our study and had a response

rate of only 24 percent (55 shoppers). 91 percent of the responders answered that

they would choose an organic item if it was weakly cheaper than it�s non-organic

alternative. 80 percent of the responders reported that they would switch brands for

a discount of 20 percent.

Because of the low response rate of our �rst follow up survey, we conducted an

additional survey using the Qualtrics platform on 378 US participants with ages

ranging between 18 and 30 and with at least some college education. 76 (84) percent

of participants answered that they would purchase a 50 cent item they did not need

in order to receive a $3 ($10) discount on their supermarket purchase. Their response

shrunk to 62 (83) percent when the question focused on a $2.50 item that they did

not need in order to receive a $3 ($10) discount. Over 70 percent of respondents

reported that they would choose organic if it was the same price as the non-organic

alternative for prices ranging between $1.00-$3.50. This percent climbs to close to 90

percent when organic is cheaper than the non-organic alternative. Lastly, 68 percent

of respondents replied that they would switch brands if the alternative brand was

discounted to the same price as the item they usually buy. This percent climbs to 80

percent when the discounted alternative becomes cheaper than the item they usually

buy.

These survey results provide some support to our underlying assumptions, and

hence, lend support to our interpretation of the data as re�ecting shopping behavior

under limited attention. The reason is that the behavior of our participants stand

in stark contrast to the vast majority of the survey responses.

7 Concluding remarks

Comparing prices across a large variety of products is a non-trivial task, especially

when prices are constantly changing. Much of the economic analysis is based on the

premise that individuals are attuned to all price �uctuations and perfectly process

signals about these price changes. In contrast, the results of our �eld experiment

show that individuals can miss opportunities to save and tend to focus on price

comparisons that are more salient. Moreover, a signi�cant proportion of individuals

forego opportunities to save that are brought to their attention. Indeed, a surprising

conclusion that arises from our �ndings is that it is not straightforward to draw

individuals� attention to price changes that can help them save, even when they

are provided with personalized messages. Our analysis suggests that advertising

sales can end up increasing purchase rates of all items in the category where a sale is

taking place. Speci�cally, information on within category sales can increase purchase

rates of both the on-sale item and other alternatives in that category. If indeed,

promotional materials increase "mistakes" among consumers, �rms could pro�t by

increasing prices of "substitutes" while advertising "target" discounts.

Our data hints at the existence of a typology of shopping trips where one type

is more focused on processing information on savings opportunities. Speci�cally,

shoppers in a "high ability/attention" state could bene�t while shoppers in a "low

ability/attention" state may not. Interestingly, Heiss et al. (2016) �nd a similar

result in a much more complex environment. Their study examines the mechanisms

driving inertia in consumer�s choice of health insurance plans over time. They also

�nd that while certain consumers pro�t when forced to pay attention to di¤erences

between plans, others are worse o¤. Our �nding, that this di¤erence exists even when

information is clearly summarized in a single email and risk aversion should play a

much smaller role, suggests a promising direction for future research. To improve

consumer welfare it is imperative to understand both the characteristics of both more

and less "attentive" states, as well as the strategies that can improve information

processing across all consumers.

References

[1] Baranski, Marcin et al.(2014): �Higher Antioxidant and Lower Cadmium Con-

centrations and Lower Incidence of Pesticide Residues in Organically Grown

Crops: A Systematic Literature Review and Meta-analyses.�British Journal of

Nutrition 112, 794�811.

[2] Bartos, Vojt¼ech, Michal Bauer, Julie Chytilová and Filip Majt¼eka (2016): �At-

tention Discrimination: Theory and Field Experiments with Monitoring Infor-

mation Acquisition,�American Economic Review 106(6), 1437-75.

[3] Blake, Tom, Sarah Moshary, Kane Sweeney and Steve Tadelis (2017): �Price

Salience and Product Choice,�Working Paper.

[4] Byron, Ellen (2008): �At the Supermarket Checkout, Frugality Trumps Brand

Loyalty,�Wall Street Journal, Nov. 6.

[5] Bordalo, Pedro, Nicola Gennaioli, and Andrei Shleifer (2016): �Competition for

Attention,�Review of Economic Studies 83(2), 481-513.

[6] Caplin, Andrew and Mark Dean (2013): �The Behavioral Implications of Ratio-

nal Inattention with Shannon Entropy,�Working Paper, New York University.

[7] Chidmi, Benaissa and Rigoberto A. Lopez (2007): �Brand-Supermarket Demand

for Breakfast Cereals and Retail Competition,�American Journal of Agricul-

tural Economics 89(2), 324-337.

[8] Chetty, Raj, Adam Looney and Kory Kroft (2009): �Salience and Taxation:

Theory and Evidence,�The American Economic Review 99(4), 1145-1177.

[9] Clerides, Sofronis. and Pascal Courty (2015): �Sales, Quantity Surcharge, and

Consumer Inattention.�Review of Economics and Statistics, forthcoming.

[10] Costa, Dora L. and Matthew E. Kahn (2013): �Energy Conservation �Nudges�

and Environmentalist Ideology: Evidence from a Randomized Residential Elec-

tricity Field Experiment,� Journal of European Economic Association 11(3),

680-702.

[11] De Clippel, Geo¤roy, K�r Eliaz and Kareen Rozen (2014): �Competing for

Consumer Inattention,�Journal of Political Economy 122(6), 1203-1234.

[12] De los Santos, Babur, Ali Hortaçsu and Matthijs R. Wildenbeest (2012). �Test-

ing Models of Consumer Search Using Data on Web Browsing and Purchasing

Behavior,�American Economic Review Vol. 102, Number 6, pp. 2955-2980.

[13] Gabaix, Xavier, David Laibson, Guillermo Moloche and Stephen Weinberg

(2006): �Costly Information Acquisition: Experimental Analysis of a Bound-

edly Rational Mode,�The American Economic Review 96(4), 1043-1068.

[14] Heiss, Florian, Daniel McFadden,Joachim Winter, Amelie Wuppermann, and

Bo Zhou (2016): "Inattention and Switching Costs as Sources of Intertia in

Medicare Part D," NBER Working Paper 22765.

[15] Helmers, Christian, Pramila Krishnan and Manasa Patnam (2015). �Attention

and Saliency on the Internet: Evidence from an Online Recommendation Sys-

tem,�CEPR Discussion Paper No. 10939.

[16] Hossain, Tanjim and John Morgan (2006): �... Plus Shipping and Handling:

Revenue (Non) Equivalence in Field Experiments on eBay,� B.E. Journal of

Economic Analysis and Policy: Advances in Economic Analysis and Policy 6(2),

1-27.

[17] Iyengar, Sheena (2010): The Art of Choosing. Twelve, Hachette Book Group.

[18] Karlan, Dean, Margaret McConnell, Sendhil Mullainathan and Jonathan Zin-

man (2016): �Getting to the Top of Mind: How Reminders Increase Saving,�

Management Science 62(12), 3393�3411.

[19] K½oszegi, Botond and Matthew Rabin (2006): "A Model of Reference-Dependent

Preferences," Quarterly Journal of Economics, 121, 1133-1165.

[20] Magnusson, Maria, Anne Arvola, Ulla-Kaisa Koivisto Hursti, Lars Åberg and

Per-Olow Sjödén (2001): �Attitudes towards organic foods among Swedish con-

sumers,�British Food Journal 103(3), 209-227.

[21] Manzini, Paola and Marco Mariotti (2014): �Stochastic Choice and Considera-

tion Sets,�Econometrica 82 (3): 1153-1176.

[22] Nielsen Global Report of Loyalty Sentiment (2013).

[23] Perkovic, Sonja and Jacob Lund Orquin (2017): �Implicit Statistical Learning

in Real World Environments Behind Ecologically Rational Decision Making,�

forthcoming in Psychological Science.

[24] Rayner, Keith (1998): �Eye Movements in Reading and Information Processing:

20 Years of Research,�Psychological Bulletin 124(3), 372-422.

[25] Roddy, Gerardine, Cathal A. Cowan and W George Hutchinson (1996): �Con-

sumer Attitudes and Behavior to Organic Foods in Ireland,�Journal of Inter-

national Consumer Marketing 9(2), 41-63.

[26] Sallee, James M. and Joel Slemrod (2012): �Car notches: Strategic automaker

responses to fuel economy policy,�Journal of Public Economics 96, 981-999.

[27] Sims, Christopher (2003): �Implications of Rational Inattention,� Journal of

Monetary Economics 50(3), 665�690.

[28] Sims, Christopher (2010): �Rational Inattention and Monetary Economics,�

Chapter 4 of Handbook of Monetary Economics, edited by Ben Friedman and

Michael Woodford for Elsevier, 155-181.

Table 1: Purchasing Frequency of Target and Substitute Items Prior to Experiment

Product Name Quantity Purchased

Bananas 357Bananas (Organic) 72Onions 191Onions (Organic) 42Kroger: Bread 139Aunt Millie's Bread 56Kroger: Eggs ­ 12ct 134Egg­Lands Best: Cage Free Large Brown Eggs ­ 12ct 14Kroger: Grade A Large Brown Eggs ­ 12ct 19Simple Truth: Natural Cage Free Large Brown Eggs ­ 12ct 78Kroger: Milk (1gal) 114Kroger: Milk (0.5gal) 96Horizon: Organic Milk (0.5gal) 22Simple Truth Organic: Milk (0.5gal) 43Apple (Lg) 103Apple (Organic) 69Apple Bag ­ 3 lb bag 65Bell Pepper 99Bell Pepper (Organic) 15Blueberries 94Blueberry (Organic) 11Avocado 76Jumbo Avocado 28Cucumber 75Cucumber (Organic) 15Ice Mountain: Water ­ 24pk 74Kroger: Purified Drinking Water ­ 24pk 11Dasani: Water ­ 24pk 20Aquafina ­ 24pk 11Chobani: Greek Yogurt 71Fage: Greek Yogurt 55Raspberries 62Raspberries (Organic) 10Roma Tomato 41Roma Tomato (Organic) 4Romaine Lettuce 33Romaine Lettuce (Organic) 3Broccoli, Kiwi, Lime, Kale, Pineapple, and Lemon were excluded from this table for lack ofspace.

Table 2: Target and Substitute Produce Items

Weeks Target Item Price SalePrice

Substitute Item Price

1­5 Organic Banana 0.49 0.39 Regular Banana 0.391­5 Organic Blueberries 5.49 4.99 Regular Blueberries 4.991­5 Organic Kiwi 0.99 0.79 Regular Kiwi 0.791­5 Organic Apple (Fuji) 1.49 1.25 Regular Apple (Fuji) 1.251­5 Organic Apple (Gala) 1.49 1.25 Regular Apple (Gala) 1.251­5 Organic Apple (Granny Smith) 1.49 1.25 Regular Apple (Granny Smith) 1.251­5 Organic Lime 1.29 0.89 Regular Lime 0.891­5 Organic Broccoli 3.49 3.25 Regular Broccoli 3.251­5 Organic Romaine Lettuce 3.29 2.59 Regular Romaine lettuce 2.591­5 Organic Cucumber 1.89 0.99 Regular Cucumber 0.991­5 Jumbo Ripe Avocado 2.25 1.49 Jumbo Unripe Avocado 2.256­9 Organic Tomato 0.79 0.59 Regular Tomato 0.596­9 Organic Red Bell Pepper 2.79 2.59 Regular Red Bell Pepper 2.596­9 Organic Onion 2.59 1.99 Regular Sweet Onion 1.996­9 Organic Kale 2.19 1.99 Regular Kale 1.996­9 Organic Green Onion 0.99 0.95 Regular Green Onion 0.956­9 Apples 3 lb bag (~4 ct.) 5.39 4.49 Regular Apple 1.256­9 Organic Lemon 1.49 1.29 Regular Lemon 1.296­9 Organic Pineapple 6.49 5.49 Regular Pineapple 5.4910­13 Organic Banana 0.49 0.24 Regular Banana 0.3910­13 Organic Blueberries 5.49 4.00 Regular Blueberries 4.9910­13 Organic Apple 1.49 1.00 Regular Apple 1.2510­13 Organic Apple (Fuji) 1.49 1.00 Regular Apple 1.2510­13 Organic Raspberries 5.49 3.89 Regular Raspberries 3.9910­13 Organic lemon 1.49 0.99 Regular Lemon 1.2910­13 Organic Broccoli 3.49 2.00 Regular Broccoli 3.2510­13 Organic Cucumber 1.89 0.75 Regular Cucumber 0.9910­13 Roma Tomato Organic 0.79 0.20 Regular Tomato 0.5910­13 Red Bell Pepper Organic 2.79 1.99 Regular Red Bell Pepper 2.5910­13 Sweet Onion Organic 2.59 1.00 Regular Sweet Onion 1.9910­13 Organic Green Onion 0.99 0.50 Regular Green Onion 0.95

Table 3: Target and Substitute Dairy, Egg, and Durable Items

Dairy

Weeks Target Item Price Sale Price Substitute Item Price1­5 Kroger: Milk (0.5gal) 2.99 1.75 Kroger: Milk (1gal) 3.991­5 Horizon Organic:  0%  fat

free Milk (0.5gal))5.45 4.49 Simple Truth Organic: Fat

Free Milk4.49

1­5 Fage: 0% and 2% fatYogurt (plain and cherry)

1.89 1.50 Chobani: Yogurt,Fage: Yogurt (Other)

1.89

6­9 Fage: 0% and 2% fatYogurt (plain and cherry)

1.89 1.50 Chobani: Yogurt,Fage: Yogurt (Other)

1.89

10­13 Simple Truth Organic:Milk (0.5gal)

4.49 2.99 Horizon Organic: Milk 5.45

Eggs

Weeks Target Item Price Sale Price Substitute Item Price1­5 Kroger: Grade A large

Brown Eggs­12ct3.69 2.89 Kroger Grade A Large

Eggs­12ct2.99

1­5 Egg­Land's Best: CageFree Large Brown Eggs­12ct

5.49 4.35 Simple Truth: NaturalCage Free Grain FedLarge Brown Eggs­12ct

4.45

10­13 Kroger: Grade A LargeBrown Eggs­12ct

3.69 1.89 Kroger Grade A LargeEggs­12ct

2.99

10­13 Simple Truth: NaturalCage Free Grain FedLarge Brown Eggs­12ct

4.45 2.50 Kroger Grade A LargeEggs­12ct

2.99

Durables

Weeks Target Item Price Sale Price Substitute Item Price6­9 Kroger: Multigrain Bread 2.59 1.99 Kroger: 100% Whole

Wheat Bread2.59

6­9 Kroger: Wheat Bread 2.45 1.99 Kroger: Buttermilk Bread 2.196­9 Dasani: Water 6.99 5.49 Ice mountain: Water

Aquafina: WaterKroger: WaterNiagara: Water

5.996.995.495.99

12­13 Aunt Millie's Bread:100% Whole Wheat

3.65 2.19 Aunt Millies: 12 WholeGrain, Honey & CrunchOat, Honey Wheat, MultiGrainKroger 100% WholeWheat

3.65

2.59

12­13 Aunt Millie's Bread:Butter Top White

3.65 2.19 Kroger: Buttermilk Bread,Wheat Bread

2.45

12­13 Aunt Millie's Bread:Whole Grain White

3.65 2.19 Aunt Millies: ItalianKroger: White, Italian

3.652.19

Table 4: O¤ered Rebate Items By Week

Week Rebate Item Rebate Item Price(in $'s)

Rebate ItemRefund

Rebate ItemRefund

Control Group Treat Group

1 Bananas 0.39 2.99 2.99

2 Blueberries 4.49 2.99 2.99

3 Apples 1.25 2.99 2.99

4 Broccoli 3.25 2.99 10

5 Bananas, Blueberries, Apples, orBroccoli

See Prices Above 2.99 10

6 Tomatoes 0.59 2.99 10

7 Red bell pepers 2.59 2.99 10

8 Bread 1.99 2.99 10

9 Yogurt 1.5 2.99 10

10 Bananas 0.24 2.99 10

11 Apples 1 10 10

12 Bread 2.19 10 10

13 Eggs 2.49 10 10

Table 5: Sample Characteristics in Pre Experiment Period

Controla Treata Diffb Controla Treata Diffb

Number of Shopping Trips 4.373 4.264 ­0.097 4.829 4.732 ­0.097(5.814) (5.678) (0.693) (6.122) (5.988) (0.693)

Number of Items Purchased 12.544 13.039 0.856 13.529 14.385 0.856(7.157) (8.553) (0.883) (7.017) (8.337) (0.883)

Number of Target Items Purchased: 2.198 2.758 0.65 2.559 3.209 0.65(28 Categories) (4.856) (6.372) (0.689) (5.153) (6.769) (0.689)

           Neigboring Categories: 0.599 0.702 0.103 0.697 0.817 0.120          (6 Categories) (1.683) (2.397) (0.220) (1.798) (2.569) (0.254)

           Non­Neighboring Categories: 1.599 2.056 0.457 1.862 2.392 0.530          (22 Categories) (3.900) (4.989) (0.475) (4.151) (5.308) (0.546)

Number of Substitute Items Purchased: 8.565 8.360 ­0.205 9.974 9.725 ­0.248(28 Categories) (11.585) (12.901) (1.302) (11.929) (13.433) (1.455)

           Neigboring Categories: 2.904 2.427 ­0.477 3.382 2.824 ­0.558          (6 Categories) (6.555) (5.125) (0.624) (6.961) (5.428) (0.714)

           Non­Neighboring Categories: 5.661 5.933 0.272 6.592 6.902 0.310          (22 Categories) (7.341) (8.624) (0.850) (7.525) (8.937) (0.946)

Number of Categories Purchased 4.260 4.500 0.240 4.961 5.235 0.275(3.587) (3.690) (0.386) (3.390) (3.462) (0.392)

Total $ Amount Spent on Purchase 66.186 65.198 ­0.988 70.957 70.166 ­0.791(38.556) (40.119) (4.177) (38.403) (39.833) (4.481)

Number of Shoppers 177 178 152 153aStandard deviations are presented in parenthesisbStandard errors are presented in parenthesis

Full Sample Target or Substitute History

Our analysis focuses on 28 item categories.  Six of these items are charectorized as NeighborCategories ­ item categories where the substitute and target items appear on the same line of thewebpage (avocados, bananas, kiwis, lemons, raspberries, and water).  The remaining 22 non­neighboring categories are the following: apples, bulk apples, blueberries, pineapples, broccoli,cucumbers, kale, onions, green onions, peppers, lettuces, limes, tomatoes, bread, organic bread,eggs, brown eggs, organic eggs, milk, bulk milk, organic milk, yogurt.

Table 6: The Probability of Purchasing a Target (vs. Substitute) ItemAll Items Neighbors Non­ Neigbors

(1) (2) (3)

Pre Sale ­0.033 0.003 ­0.042(Weeks ­5 & ­6) (0.048) (0.087) (0.064)

Pre Sale ­0.045 ­0.071 ­0.068(Weeks ­3 & ­4) (0.048) (0.063) (0.065)

Sale Event 0.129 0.307** 0.075(Weeks 1 & 2) (0.079) (0.142) (0.100)

Sale Event 0.136 0.322** 0.091(Weeks 3 & 4) (0.115) (0.139) (0.139)

Post Sale ­0.133 ­0.058 ­0.172(Weeks 1 & 2) (0.117) (0.150) (0.155)

Post Sale ­0.008 0.234 ­0.015(Weeks 3 & 4) (0.145) (0.247) (0.136)

Item Fixed Effects Yes Yes YesShopper Fixed Effects Yes Yes Yes

Number of Items 914 266 648

Standard errors are presented in parenthesis and clustered at the shopper level.An observation is defined as a purchase of either a target or substitute item.Neighbors refers to item categories where the substitute and target items appear on thesame line of the website.The 2 weeks prior to the sales event are excluded from the analysis, so that these estimatesshow the change in the purchasing rate of the target item relative to these excluded weeks.*Significant at 10%;  **significant at 5%;  ***significant at 1%

Table7:Shopper�sResponsetoaSaleontheTargetItem

Targ

et S

ale

0.00

1**

0.00

3***

0.00

10.

001*

**0.

002*

**0.

001*

*0.

0000

30.

001

­0.0

002

(0.0

004)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

005)

(0.0

005)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

003)

(0.0

003)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

004)

Targ

et S

ale 

X Hi

stor

y0.

003

0.00

40.

002

0.00

5**

0.00

60.

004*

*­0

.001

­0.0

00­0

.001

(0.0

03)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

03)

Hist

ory 

of P

urch

ase

0.02

8***

0.03

7***

0.02

4***

0.00

6***

0.00

7**

0.00

5***

0.02

2***

0.02

9***

0.01

8***

in C

ateg

ory 

 (0/1

)(0

.004

)(0

.006

)(0

.003

)(0

.001

)(0

.003

)(0

.001

)(0

.003

)(0

.006

)(0

.003

)

High

 Reb

ate 

Wee

k0.

005*

**0.

007*

*0.

004*

**0.

001

0.00

10.

001

0.00

4***

0.00

6***

0.00

3**

(0/1

)(0

.002

)(0

.003

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)

Item

 Cat

egor

y FE

'sX

XX

XX

XX

XX

Shop

per &

 Wee

k FE

'sX

XX

XX

XX

XX

N It

emsx

Wee

ks12

9,22

027

,690

101,

530

129,

220

27,6

9010

1,53

012

9,22

027

,690

101,

530

Mea

n of

 Dep

ende

nt0.

008

0.01

30.

007

0.00

20.

004

0.00

20.

005

0.00

90.

005

Varia

ble:

[0.0

90]

[0.1

15]

[0.0

81]

[0.0

50]

[0.0

64]

[0.0

45]

[0.0

74]

[0.0

94]

[0.0

67]

Stan

dard

 err

ors a

re p

rese

nted

 in p

aren

thes

is a

nd cl

uste

red 

at th

e sh

oppe

r lev

el. S

tand

ard 

devi

atio

ns a

ppea

r in 

brac

kets

.

Sam

e lin

e ca

teto

ries r

efer

 to n

eigb

ors w

here

 the 

subs

titut

e an

d ta

rget

 item

s app

ear o

n th

e sa

me 

line 

of th

e w

ebsi

te.

Hist

ory 

of P

urch

ase 

is e

qual

 to 1

 if th

e sh

oppe

r pur

chas

ed e

ither

 a su

bstit

ute 

or ta

rget

 item

 in th

is ca

tego

ry in

 the

pre­

expe

rimen

t per

iod.

 A h

igh 

reba

te w

eek 

refe

rs to

 wee

ks w

hen 

the 

reba

te w

as $

10 fo

r pur

chas

ing 

the 

reba

te it

em.

High

 reba

te w

eek=

0 w

hen 

the 

reba

te w

as $

2.99

.*S

igni

fican

t at 1

0%;  

**si

gnifi

cant

 at 5

%;  

***s

igni

fican

t at 1

%

An o

bser

vatio

n is

 def

ined

 by 

a sh

oppe

r, w

eek,

 and

 item

 cate

gory

.

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

ne

Purc

hase

 Item

 in C

ateg

ory

Buy 

Targ

etBu

y Su

bstit

ute

All

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

neAl

lSa

me

Line

Diff

 Line

All

Table8:DoTreatmentShoppersRespondDi¤erentlytoaSaleontheTargetItem?

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e­0

.001

­0.0

002

­0.0

01­0

.000

1­0

.000

2­0

.000

1­0

.001

­0.0

001

­0.0

01(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e X 

Hist

0.01

7***

0.01

10.

018*

**0.

005

0.00

70.

004

0.01

2***

0.00

50.

015*

**(0

.006

)(0

.012

)(0

.006

)(0

.004

)(0

.011

)(0

.003

)(0

.005

)(0

.011

)(0

.005

)

Targ

et S

ale 

(TS)

0.00

1**

0.00

3**

0.00

10.

001*

*0.

002*

*0.

001

0.00

030.

001

0.00

01(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.000

5)(0

.001

)(0

.000

4)(0

.000

4)(0

.001

)(0

.000

4)

Targ

et S

ale 

X Hi

stor

y­0

.006

­0.0

01­0

.008

*0.

002

0.00

20.

002

­0.0

07**

­0.0

03­0

.009

**(0

.004

)(0

.008

)(0

.005

)(0

.003

)(0

.009

)(0

.002

)(0

.003

)(0

.009

)(0

.004

)

High

 Reb

ate 

Wee

k (0

/1)

0.00

5***

0.00

7**

0.00

5***

0.00

10.

001

0.00

10.

004*

**0.

006*

**0.

003*

**(0

.002

)(0

.003

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)

Cate

gory

, Sho

pper

 & W

eek 

FE's

XX

XX

XX

XX

X

N It

emsx

Wee

ks12

9,22

027

,690

101,

530

129,

220

27,6

9010

1,53

012

9,22

027

,690

101,

530

Mea

n of

 Dep

ende

nt0.

008

0.01

30.

007

0.00

20.

004

0.00

20.

005

0.00

90.

005

Varia

ble:

[0.0

90]

[0.1

15]

[0.0

81]

[0.0

50]

[0.0

64]

[0.0

45]

[0.0

74]

[0.0

94]

[0.0

67]

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Tre

atm

ent G

roup

 Dur

ing 

Sale

 Per

iod 

(With

 His

tory

 in C

ateg

ory)

:TS

 + H

isto

ry x

 TS 

+0.

012*

**0.

012

0.01

1***

0.00

8***

0.01

1*0.

007*

*0.

005

0.00

30.

005

  Tre

at X

 TS 

+ Tr

eat X

 TS 

X H

(0.0

04)

(0.0

10)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

03)

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Con

trol

 Gro

up D

urin

g Sa

le P

erio

d (W

ith H

isto

ry in

 Cat

egor

y):

TS +

 His

otry

 x T

S­0

.004

0.00

1­0

.007

0.00

30.

004

0.00

3­0

.007

**­0

.001

­0.0

09**

(0.0

04)

(0.0

08)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

04)

*Sig

nific

ant a

t 10%

;  **

sign

ifica

nt a

t 5%

;  **

*sig

nific

ant a

t 1%

Stan

dard

 err

ors a

re p

rese

nted

 in p

aren

thes

is a

nd cl

uste

red 

at th

e sh

oppe

r lev

el. S

tand

ard 

devi

atio

ns a

re p

rese

nted

 inbr

acke

ts. A

n ob

serv

atio

n is

 def

ined

 by 

a sh

oppe

r, w

eek,

 and

 item

 cate

gory

. Sam

e lin

e ca

teto

ries r

efer

 to n

eigb

ors

whe

re th

e su

bstit

ute 

and 

targ

et it

ems a

ppea

r on 

the 

sam

e lin

e of

 the 

web

site

. His

tory

 of P

urch

ase 

is e

qual

 to 1

 if th

esh

oppe

r pur

chas

ed e

ither

 a su

bstit

ute 

or ta

rget

 item

 in th

is ca

tego

ry in

 the 

 pre

­exp

erim

ent p

erio

d. A

 hig

h re

bate

 wee

kre

fers

 to w

eeks

 whe

n th

e re

bate

 was

 $10

 for p

urch

asin

g th

e re

bate

 item

 (oth

erw

ise 

it w

as w

as se

t at $

2.99

). Ad

ditio

nal

cont

rols

 incl

ude:

 an 

indi

cato

r var

iabl

e fo

r whe

ther

 or n

ot th

e sh

oppe

r has

 a h

isto

ry o

f pur

chas

e in

 cate

gory

 and

 this

varia

ble 

inte

ract

ed w

ith tr

eatm

ent.

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

ne

Purc

hase

 Item

 in C

ateg

ory

Buy 

Targ

etBu

y Su

bstit

ute

All

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

neAl

lSa

me

Line

Diff

 Line

All

Table9:DoTreatmentShoppersRespondDi¤erentlytoaSaleontheTargetItem

(IdenticalRebateWeeks)?

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e­0

.001

0.00

2­0

.002

­0.0

001

0.00

1­0

.000

3­0

.001

0.00

04­0

.001

*(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e X 

Hist

0.02

1**

0.01

70.

021*

*0.

007

0.01

30.

005

0.01

4**

0.00

60.

016*

*(0

.008

)(0

.016

)(0

.009

)(0

.005

)(0

.015

)(0

.005

)(0

.007

)(0

.018

)(0

.008

)

Targ

et S

ale 

(TS)

0.00

10.

001

0.00

20.

0004

0.00

10.

0001

0.00

1­0

.000

30.

001*

(0.0

01)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

01)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

01)

Targ

et S

ale 

X Hi

stor

y­0

.010

­0.0

12­0

.009

0.00

20.

001

0.00

3­0

.010

**­0

.010

­0.0

11*

(0.0

06)

(0.0

12)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

05)

(0.0

15)

(0.0

06)

High

 Reb

ate 

Wee

k (0

/1)

­0.0

04*

­0.0

08*

­0.0

02­0

.001

­0.0

00­0

.001

­0.0

04**

­0.0

09**

*­0

.003

*(0

.002

)(0

.004

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.001

)(0

.002

)(0

.004

)(0

.001

)

N It

em C

ateg

orie

sxW

eeks

59,6

4012

,780

46,8

6059

,640

12,7

8046

,860

59,6

4012

,780

46,8

60

Mea

n of

 Dep

ende

nt0.

008

0.01

30.

007

0.00

30.

005

0.00

20.

005

0.00

90.

004

Varia

ble:

[0.0

90]

[0.1

15]

[0.0

81]

[0.0

52]

[0.0

68]

[0.0

46]

[0.0

72]

[0.0

93]

[0.0

66]

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Tre

atm

ent G

roup

 Dur

ing 

Sale

 Per

iod 

(With

 His

tory

 in C

ateg

ory)

:TS

 + H

isto

ry x

 TS 

+0.

011*

*0.

008

0.01

2**

0.00

9***

0.01

5*0.

008*

*0.

004

­0.0

040.

005

  Tre

at X

 TS 

+ Tr

eat X

 TS 

X H

(0.0

05)

(0.0

12)

(0.0

05)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

08)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

04)

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Con

trol

 Gro

up D

urin

g Sa

le P

erio

d (W

ith H

isto

ry in

 Cat

egor

y):

TS +

 His

otry

 x T

S­0

.008

­0.0

11­0

.008

0.00

30.

001

0.00

3­0

.009

*­0

.010

­0.0

10(0

.006

)(0

.012

)(0

.007

)(0

.004

)(0

.013

)(0

.003

)(0

.005

)(0

.014

)(0

.006

)

*Sig

nific

ant a

t 10%

;  **

sign

ifica

nt a

t 5%

;  **

*sig

nific

ant a

t 1%

Stan

dard

 err

ors a

re p

rese

nted

 in p

aren

thes

is &

 clus

tere

d at

 the 

shop

per l

evel

. Sta

ndar

d de

viat

ions

 are

 pre

sent

edin

 bra

cket

s. A

n ob

serv

atio

n is

 def

ined

 by 

a sh

oppe

r, w

eek,

 and

 item

 cate

gory

. All 

spec

ifica

tions

 incl

ude 

cate

gory

fixed

 eff

ects

, sho

pper

 fixe

d ef

fect

s, a

nd w

eek 

fixed

 eff

ects

. Onl

y w

eeks

 whe

n th

e tr

eatm

ent a

nd co

ntro

l gro

upre

ceiv

ed th

e sa

me 

reba

te o

ffer

 are

 incl

uded

 in a

naly

sis.

 Sam

e lin

e ca

teto

ries r

efer

 to n

eigb

ors w

here

 the

subs

titut

e an

d ta

rget

 item

s app

ear o

n th

e sa

me 

line 

of th

e w

ebsi

te. H

isto

ry o

f Pur

chas

e is

 equ

al to

 1 if

 the

shop

per p

urch

ased

 eith

er a

 subs

titut

e or

 targ

et it

em in

 this

 cate

gory

 in th

e  p

re­e

xper

imen

t per

iod.

 A h

igh 

reba

tew

eek 

refe

rs to

 wee

ks w

hen 

the 

reba

te w

as $

10 fo

r pur

chas

ing 

the 

reba

te it

em (o

ther

wis

e it 

was

 was

 set a

t $2.

99).

Addi

tiona

l con

trol

s inc

lude

: an 

indi

cato

r var

iabl

e fo

r whe

ther

 or n

ot th

e sh

oppe

r has

 a h

isto

ry o

f pur

chas

e in

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

ne

Purc

hase

 Item

 in C

ateg

ory

Buy 

Targ

etBu

y Su

bstit

ute

All

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

neAl

lSa

me

Line

Diff

 Line

All

Table 10: Is the Sale E¤ect Di¤erent for First Line Categories?

Treat X Target Sale (TS) 0.010 0.001 0.010*(0.006) (0.004) (0.006)

Treat X TS X First­Item (FI) 0.026* 0.017** 0.006(0.015) (0.008) (0.013)

Target Sale (TS) ­0.002 0.005* ­0.007*(0.005) (0.003) (0.004)

First­Item X (TS) ­0.018 ­0.015** ­0.000(0.013) (0.007) (0.010)

First­Item Category 0.0004 0.003 ­0.004(0.009) (0.004) (0.007)

Treat X First­Item (FI) ­0.008 ­0.002 ­0.004(0.011) (0.004) (0.009)

Item Category Fixed Effects X X XShopper & Week Fixed Effects X X X

N ItemsxWeeks 15,093 15,093 15,093

Mean of Dependent 0.030 0.008 0.021Variable: [0.172] [0.091] [0.145]

Change in Purchase Rate of Treatment Group During Sale Period:TS+ Treat x TS 0.008** 0.006** 0.003

(0.004) (0.003) (0.004)

First Item: TS + Treat x TS + 0.016* 0.008 0.009     FI x TS + Treat x TS x FI (0.009) (0.007) (0.008)

Change in Purchase Rate of Control Group During Sale Period:First Item: TS +  TS x First­Item ­0.020* ­0.010* ­0.007

(0.011) (0.005) (0.008)

*Significant at 10%;  **significant at 5%;  ***significant at 1%

Buy SubstituteMake Purchase Buy Target

Additional controls include: an indicator for a high rebate week (a rebate of $10 versus $2.99) as wellas a control for the number of times this shopper purchased in this category during the pre­experiment period.

Standard errors are presented in parenthesis and clustered at the shopper level. Standarddeviations are presented in brackets. First Item refers to the item category that appeared first in theemail to the treatment group.An observation is defined by a shopper, week, and item category. For each shopper, we only includenon­neighboring item categories where this shopper had a history of purchasing either the target orsubstitute item during the pre­experiment period.  We focus on non­neighboring item categories,as there are weeks where their broader category (vegetables) was on sale and appeared in the firstline of the email but not all sub categories (e.g. tomatoes) were on sale that week. This allows us toseparate out the effect of a first­item category from an on­sale first­item category.

Table11:IstheSaleE¤ectDi¤erentwhentheTreatmentGroupReceivedMoreDetailedSaleInformation?

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e (T

S)0.

020*

­0.0

330.

039*

*0.

008

­0.0

060.

007

0.01

3­0

.032

0.03

2***

(0.0

12)

(0.0

31)

(0.0

15)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

11)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

21)

(0.0

11)

Trea

t X T

S X 

Deta

il­0

.011

0.04

8­0

.032

*­0

.006

0.00

9­0

.006

­0.0

040.

046

­0.0

25*

(0.0

14)

(0.0

35)

(0.0

19)

(0.0

10)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

12)

(0.0

30)

(0.0

15)

Targ

et S

ale 

(TS)

0.00

050.

033

­0.0

15­0

.001

0.01

3­0

.002

0.00

20.

023

­0.0

12(0

.008

)(0

.021

)(0

.011

)(0

.005

)(0

.012

)(0

.007

)(0

.007

)(0

.016

)(0

.008

)

Deta

il X 

Targ

et S

ale 

(TS)

0.00

1­0

.034

0.01

60.

008

­0.0

020.

008

­0.0

09­0

.035

*0.

005

(0.0

11)

(0.0

22)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

10)

(0.0

19)

(0.0

12)

N It

emsx

Wee

ks14

,443

4,53

79,

906

14,4

434,

537

9,90

614

,443

4,53

79,

906

Mea

n of

 Dep

ende

nt0.

036

0.05

00.

030

0.01

10.

016

0.00

90.

025

0.03

40.

021

Varia

ble:

[0.1

87]

[0.2

18]

[0.1

70]

[0.1

04]

[0.1

25]

[0.0

93]

[0.1

56]

[0.1

81]

[0.1

43]

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Tre

atm

ent G

roup

 Dur

ing 

Sale

 Per

iod:

TS+ 

Trea

t x T

S0.

020*

*­0

.001

0.02

4*0.

007

0.00

80.

005

0.01

5**

­0.0

090.

020*

*(0

.009

)(0

.02)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

11)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

08)

Deta

il:   T

S+ T

reat

 x T

S  +

0.00

90.

013

0.00

70.

009*

0.01

50.

007

0.00

20.

002

0.00

1   D

etai

l x T

S + 

Trea

t x T

S x 

D(0

.008

)(0

.018

)(0

.008

)(0

.005

)(0

.012

)(0

.005

)(0

.005

)(0

.014

)(0

.005

)

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Con

trol

 Gro

up D

urin

g Sa

le P

erio

d:De

tail:

TS+ 

Deta

ilxTS

0.00

1­0

.001

0.00

10.

007*

0.01

10.

006

­0.0

07­0

.012

­0.0

07(0

.006

)(0

.012

)(0

.008

)(0

.004

)(0

.01)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

05)

(0.0

1)(0

.006

)

Stan

dard

 err

ors a

re p

rese

nted

 in p

aren

thes

is a

nd cl

uste

red 

at th

e sh

oppe

r lev

el. A

n ob

serv

atio

n is

 def

ined

 by 

a sh

oppe

r,w

eek,

 and

 item

 cate

gory

.  De

tail 

refe

rs to

 wee

ks w

here

 the 

trea

tmen

t em

ail s

peci

fical

ly m

entio

ned 

that

 som

e or

gani

cite

ms a

re o

n sa

le.  

This

 ana

lysi

s is r

un o

n a 

subs

et o

f 20 

out o

f 28 

of th

e ite

m ca

tego

ries f

or w

hich

 the 

targ

et it

em is

orga

nic.

 For

 eac

h sh

oppe

r, w

e on

ly in

clud

e ite

m ca

tego

ries w

here

 this

 shop

per h

ad a

 his

tory

 of p

urch

asin

g ei

ther

 the

targ

et o

r sub

stitu

te it

em d

urin

g th

e pr

e­ex

perim

ent p

erio

d. A

ll sp

ecifi

catio

ns in

clud

e sh

oppe

r, ite

m ca

tego

ry, a

nd w

eek

fixed

 eff

ects

. Add

ition

al co

ntro

ls in

clud

e: a

n in

dica

tor v

aria

ble 

for w

heth

er o

r not

 this

 was

 a d

etai

led 

emai

l wee

k, a

s wel

las

 this

 var

iabl

e in

tera

cted

 with

 trea

tmen

t, an

 indi

cato

r for

 a h

igh 

reba

te w

eek 

(a re

bate

 of $

10 v

ersu

s $2.

99) a

s wel

l as a

cont

rol f

or th

e nu

mbe

r of t

imes

 this

 shop

per p

urch

ased

 in th

is ca

tego

ry d

urin

g th

e pr

e­ex

perim

ent p

erio

d. *

Sign

ifica

nt a

t10

%;  

**si

gnifi

cant

 at 5

%;  

***s

igni

fican

t at 1

%

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

ne

Purc

hase

 Item

 in C

ateg

ory

Buy 

Targ

etBu

y Su

bstit

ute

All

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

neAl

lSa

me

Line

Diff

 Line

All

Table12:IstheSaleE¤ectDi¤erentforRebateItem

Categories?

Trea

t X T

arge

t Sal

e (T

S)0.

014*

**0.

008

0.01

5***

0.00

4­0

.001

0.00

40.

010*

*0.

007

0.01

1**

(0.0

05)

(0.0

15)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

14)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

04)

(0.0

12)

(0.0

05)

Trea

t X T

S X 

Reba

teIte

m­0

.005

0.00

7­0

.008

0.00

10.

029*

­0.0

11­0

.004

­0.0

130.

002

(0.0

11)

(0.0

26)

(0.0

11)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

10)

(0.0

24)

(0.0

10)

Targ

et S

ale 

(TS)

­0.0

04­0

.001

­0.0

050.

003

0.00

90.

002

­0.0

07**

­0.0

08­0

.007

**(0

.004

)(0

.010

)(0

.004

)(0

.003

)(0

.011

)(0

.002

)(0

.003

)(0

.011

)(0

.004

)

Reba

te It

em (R

I) X 

(TS)

0.02

0***

0.01

30.

018*

*0.

005

­0.0

120.

011*

0.01

3**

0.02

00.

006

(0.0

07)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

05)

(0.0

11)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

06)

(0.0

19)

(0.0

06)

N It

emsx

Wee

ks20

,215

5,12

215

,093

20,2

155,

122

15,0

9320

,215

5,12

215

,093

Mea

n of

 Dep

ende

nt0.

037

0.05

50.

030

0.01

00.

016

0.00

80.

026

0.03

70.

021

Varia

ble:

[0.1

88]

[0.2

28]

[0.1

72]

[0.1

02]

[0.1

27]

[0.0

91]

[0.1

58]

[0.1

90]

[0.1

45]

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Tre

atm

ent G

roup

 Dur

ing 

Sale

 Per

iod:

TS+ 

Trea

t x T

S0.

010*

*0.

007

0.00

9***

0.00

7**

0.00

80.

006*

*0.

004

­0.0

010.

004

(0.0

04)

(0.0

11)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

02)

(0.0

03)

(0.0

07)

(0.0

03)

Reba

te: T

S+ T

reat

 x T

S +

0.02

5***

0.02

70.

019*

*0.

013*

*0.

024*

**0.

006

0.01

30.

006

0.01

2   R

I x T

S + 

Trea

t x T

S X 

RI(0

.009

)(0

.021

)(0

.009

)(0

.005

)(0

.012

)(0

.005

)(0

.008

)(0

.017

)(0

.008

)

Chan

ge in

 Pur

chas

e Ra

te o

f Con

trol

 Gro

up D

urin

g Sa

le P

erio

d:Re

bate

: TS+

 Tre

at x

 TS 

+0.

016*

*0.

012

0.01

30.

008

­0.0

040.

013*

0.00

70.

012

­0.0

01   R

I x T

S + 

Trea

t x T

S X 

RI(0

.008

)(0

.018

)(0

.008

)(0

.006

)(0

.011

)(0

.006

)(0

.006

)(0

.020

)(0

.007

)St

anda

rd e

rror

s are

 pre

sent

ed in

 par

enth

esis

 and

 clus

tere

d at

 the 

shop

per l

evel

. Sta

ndar

d de

viat

ions

 are

 pre

sent

ed in

brac

kets

. An 

obse

rvat

ion 

is d

efin

ed b

y a 

shop

per, 

wee

k, a

nd it

em ca

tego

ry. F

or e

ach 

shop

per, 

we 

only

 incl

ude 

item

cate

gorie

s whe

re th

is sh

oppe

r had

 a h

isto

ry o

f pur

chas

ing 

eith

er th

e ta

rget

 or s

ubst

itute

 item

 dur

ing 

the 

pre­

expe

rimen

tpe

riod.

  All 

spec

ifica

tions

 incl

ude 

shop

per, 

item

 cate

gory

, and

 wee

k fix

ed e

ffec

ts. R

ebat

e Ite

m is

 def

ined

 as t

he it

emca

tego

ry th

at re

sulte

d in

 a d

isco

unt (

reba

te) i

f pur

chas

ing 

is e

ither

 a ta

rget

 or s

ubst

itute

 item

 with

in th

isca

tego

ry. A

dditi

onal

 cont

rols

 incl

ude:

 an 

indi

cato

r var

iabl

e fo

r a h

igh 

reba

te w

eek 

(a re

bate

 of $

10 v

ersu

s $2.

99) a

s wel

las

 a co

ntro

l for

 the 

num

ber o

f tim

es th

is sh

oppe

r pur

chas

ed in

 this

 cate

gory

 dur

ing 

the 

pre­

expe

rimen

t per

iod.

*Sig

nific

ant a

t 10%

;  **

sign

ifica

nt a

t 5%

;  **

*sig

nific

ant a

t 1%

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

ne

Purc

hase

 Item

 in C

ateg

ory

Buy 

Targ

etBu

y Su

bstit

ute

All

Sam

eLi

neDi

ff Li

neAl

lSa

me

Line

Diff

 Line

All

Table 13: What "Types" of Shoppers React to Information?

Treat X Target Sale 0.037 0.046 0.042 0.029 0.009 0.100*(0.042) (0.056) (0.050) (0.044) (0.068) (0.059)

Target Sale (TS) 0.040 0.052 0.015 ­0.015 ­0.013 ­0.050(0.036) (0.048) (0.031) (0.037) (0.047) (0.044)

Prior Purchases 0.022*** 0.023*** 0.020*** 0.028*** 0.027*** 0.029***in Category (0.004) (0.005) (0.005) (0.006) (0.007) (0.008)

High Rebate Week 0.022 ­0.017 0.028 0.035 ­0.009 0.068(0/1) (0.033) (0.059) (0.033) (0.041) (0.055) (0.080)

Item Category FE's X X X X X XShopper & Week FE's X X X X X X

N ItemsxWeeks 1,891 963 928 1,891 963 928

Mean of Dependent 0.101 0.119 0.082 0.250 0.250 0.249Variable: [0.301] [0.324] [0.274] [0.433] [0.433] [0.433]

Change in Purchase Rate of Treatment Group During Sale Period:Target Sale + Treat x TS 0.077*** 0.099*** 0.057 0.014 ­0.004 0.050

(0.026) (0.029) (0.043) (0.024) (0.048) (0.041)Standard errors are presented in parenthesis and clustered at the shopper level.Standard deviations are presented in brackets.

*Significant at 10%;  **significant at 5%;  ***significant at 1%

A high rebate week refers to weeks when the rebate was $10 for purchasing the rebateitem. This indicator is equal to zero when the rebate amount was set at $2.99.

PurchaseRebate

MissRebate

Buy Target Buy Substitute

AllPurchaseRebate

MissRebate All

An observation is defined by a shopper, week, and item category. For each shopper,we only include item categories where this shopper had a history of purchasing eitherthe target or substitute item during the pre­experiment period.  We only includeweeks when the rebate item cost less than the rebate and the shopper made apurchase. During these weeks we are able to differentiate between "attentive" and"inattentive" shoppers.  An "attentive" shopper is defined as one that purchased therebate item, an "inattentive" shopper is defined as someone who made a shoppingtrip but missed the rebate savings he would have received for making a purchase inthe rebate item category.

Table 14: What "Types" of Shoppers React to Information (conditioning on openingemail)?

Treat X Target Sale 0.026 0.038 0.044 ­0.018 ­0.031 0.081(0.058) (0.065) (0.075) (0.062) (0.077) (0.089)

Target Sale (TS) 0.072 0.074 0.053 0.017 0.014 ­0.023(0.052) (0.058) (0.059) (0.053) (0.049) (0.084)

Prior Purchases 0.019*** 0.020*** 0.017*** 0.030*** 0.026*** 0.035***in Category (0.004) (0.004) (0.005) (0.007) (0.006) (0.009)

High Rebate Week 0.050 0.016 0.020 0.011 0.028 0.053(0/1) (0.045) (0.053) (0.051) (0.062) (0.057) (0.091)

Item Category Fixed Effects X X X X X XShopper & Week FE's X X X X X X

N ItemsxWeeks 1,255 752 503 1,255 752 503

Mean of Dependent 0.107 0.121 0.085 0.257 0.243 0.276Variable: [0.309] [0.326] [0.280] [0.437] [0.429] [0.448]

Change in Purchase Rate of Treatment Group During Sale Period:Target Sale + Treat x TS 0.098*** 0.111*** 0.097* ­0.001 ­0.017 0.058

(0.026) (0.032) (0.053) (0.033) (0.059) (0.044)Standard errors are presented in parenthesis and clustered at the shopper level.Standard deviations are presented in brackets.

*Significant at 10%;  **significant at 5%;  ***significant at 1%

A high rebate week refers to weeks when the rebate was $10 for purchasing the rebateitem. This indicator is equal to zero when the rebate amount was set at $2.99.

Buy Target Buy Substitue

AllPurchaseRebate

MissRebate All

PurchaseRebate

MissRebate

An observation is defined by a shopper, week, and item category. For each shopper, weonly include item categories where this shopper had a history of purchasing either thetarget or substitute item during the pre­experiment period.  We only include weekswhen the shopper opened his email,  the rebate item cost less than the rebate, and theshopper made a purchase. During these weeks we are able to differentiate between"attentive" and "inattentive" shoppers. An "attentive" shopper is defined as one thatpurchased the rebate item after opening the email, an "inattentive" shopper is definedas someone who opened the promotional email and made a shopping trip but missedthe rebate savings he would have received for making a purchase in the rebate item

Figure 1: Examples of Email Format During Basic Weeks

Control (email title: Free Shipping on ­­­­ if you Buy a Banana!!!)

, your local grocery delivery service!­­­­Greetings from

Got a banana? Get a one­time refund on shipping for a purchase of over $20 if you buy one bananaor more!1(Click here)1 Offer valid on all bananas. Use this email address when placing your purchase and a refund of$2.99 will be applied within 24 hours of purchase. Valid until ­­­

Treatment (email title: Free Shipping on ­­­­ if you Buy a Banana!!!)

cal grocery delivery service!, your lo­­­­Greetings from

Got a banana? Get a one­time refund on shipping for a purchase of over $20 if you buy one bananaor more!1 (Click here)

…   and  if  that’s  not  enough,  make  sure  you  check  our  discounts  for  the  month  of  February(discounted items are marked by **).

Our biggest discounts are in the following categories:

1. Vegetables –up to 45% off select items (Click here)

2. Milk –up to 40% off select items (Click here)

3. Fruits –up to 30% off select items (Click here)

4. Eggs –up to 20% off select items (Click here)1 Offer valid on all bananas. Use this email address when placing your purchase and a refund of$2.99 will be applied within 24 hours of purchase. Valid until ­­­

Figure 2: Examples of Email Format During Detailed Weeks

Control: (email title: Click for $10 off your ­­­­ purchase!!)

Greetings from ­­­­, your local grocery delivery service!

Got apples? Get a $10 refund by simply purchasing at least one apple and inserting the couponcode dcash at checkout! 1 (Click here)

1 Offer valid on all apples. Use this email address and the dcash coupon code when placing your purchase and youwill receive a $10.00 one­time refund on your purchase of $20 or more. The refund will be applied within 24 hours.Valid until ­­­.

Treatment: (email title: Click for $10 off your ­­­­ purchase!!)

Greetings from ­­­­, your local grocery delivery service!

We are devoted to helping our customers get the "best bang for the buck".

So don't miss out on our April discounts! Our April sale prices are so low that organic sale itemsare often even cheaper than the non­organic alternative! (discounted items are marked by **)

Don't forget to consider some alternatives to your last purchase of eggs that we have on salethis month.

To use your $10 refund ­ simply click on one of the links below to the site, purchase at least oneapple and insert the coupon code found below.

Our biggest discounts are on the following products:

1. Milk – up to 33% off select items (Click here)

2. Eggs – up to 49% off select items (Click here)

3. Fruit – up to 51% off select items (Click here).

4. Vegetables – up to 75% off select items (Click here)

Make sure to purchase one or more apples and enter coupon­code dcash at checkout!1

1 Offer valid on all apples. Use this email address and the dcash coupon code when placing your purchase and youwill receive a $10.00 one­time refund on your purchase of $20 or more. The refund will be applied within 24 hours.Valid until ­­­.

Figure 3: The Fraction of Shoppers Purchasing a Target vs. Substitute Item

0.2

.4.6

.8Pr

obab

ility 

of P

urch

asin

g Ta

rget

 Rel

ativ

e to

 Sub

stitu

te

­3 ­2 ­1 0 1 2 3 4Weeks of Sale

The Effect of a Sale on Target Item Purchases

Figure 4: Example of Target versus Substitute Item During Sale Period