FEMALE MATE CHOICE IN NONHUMAN MAMMALS

22
In: Animal Behavior: New Research ISBN 978-1-60456-782-3 Editors: E. A. Weber and L. H. Krause, pp 35-56 © 2008 Nova Science Publishers, Inc. First page affiliation: State/Country missing Chapter 2 FEMALE MATE CHOICE IN NONHUMAN MAMMALS Benjamin D. Charlton Department of Psychology, School of Life Sciences, University of Sussex, BN1 9QH ABSTRACT Until now definite studies of mate choice have typically focused on non-mammal animal species where short life spans and gestation periods make reproductive success quicker to determine, and in which the females show preferences for clearly defined morphological or behavioural male traits. Mammals, which are invariably larger, have longer life spans and inter-birth intervals and are behaviourally more complex, are less suited to this type of experimentation. However, notwithstanding these difficulties, many studies of mate choice have been conducted on mammals and the current literature reveals that the females of several mammal species do appear to choose their mates, and through this potentially gain important direct and indirect fitness benefits. Here I review this body of work to reveal that the majority of female mammal mate choice studies conducted so far, particularly on large mammals, fail to: 1) determine the actual male phenotypic trait(s) of female preference (which is crucial to identifying male characteristics under sexual selection); 2) take into account other environmental and social factors that may affect female responses to male sexual signals, as well as intrinsic factors such as female hormonal state and breeding status; and 3) quantify the fitness benefits to discriminating females. I go on to give suggestions for future research and emphasise the need for a combination of carefully designed experimental and field studies. Experimental setups allow us to isolate specific male traits from other aspects of the male phenotype, determine female hormonal state and control for other competing male and female mating strategies that may affect female behaviour. Field observations of female mating behaviour can then be conducted to determine whether behavioural responses reported using experimental setups translate to actual copulations in natural conditions, and hence affect the reproductive success of individuals. Only this integrative approach will allow us to gain an appreciation of how inter-sexual selection is generated

Transcript of FEMALE MATE CHOICE IN NONHUMAN MAMMALS

In: Animal Behavior: New Research ISBN 978-1-60456-782-3 Editors: E. A. Weber and L. H. Krause, pp 35-56 © 2008 Nova Science Publishers, Inc.

First page affiliation: State/Country missing

Chapter 2

FEMALE MATE CHOICE IN NONHUMAN MAMMALS

Benjamin D. Charlton Department of Psychology, School of Life Sciences,

University of Sussex, BN1 9QH

ABSTRACT

Until now definite studies of mate choice have typically focused on non-mammal animal species where short life spans and gestation periods make reproductive success quicker to determine, and in which the females show preferences for clearly defined morphological or behavioural male traits. Mammals, which are invariably larger, have longer life spans and inter-birth intervals and are behaviourally more complex, are less suited to this type of experimentation. However, notwithstanding these difficulties, many studies of mate choice have been conducted on mammals and the current literature reveals that the females of several mammal species do appear to choose their mates, and through this potentially gain important direct and indirect fitness benefits. Here I review this body of work to reveal that the majority of female mammal mate choice studies conducted so far, particularly on large mammals, fail to: 1) determine the actual male phenotypic trait(s) of female preference (which is crucial to identifying male characteristics under sexual selection); 2) take into account other environmental and social factors that may affect female responses to male sexual signals, as well as intrinsic factors such as female hormonal state and breeding status; and 3) quantify the fitness benefits to discriminating females. I go on to give suggestions for future research and emphasise the need for a combination of carefully designed experimental and field studies. Experimental setups allow us to isolate specific male traits from other aspects of the male phenotype, determine female hormonal state and control for other competing male and female mating strategies that may affect female behaviour. Field observations of female mating behaviour can then be conducted to determine whether behavioural responses reported using experimental setups translate to actual copulations in natural conditions, and hence affect the reproductive success of individuals. Only this integrative approach will allow us to gain an appreciation of how inter-sexual selection is generated

Benjamin D. Charlton 36

on specific aspects of the male phenotype, and ultimately enable us to understand the link between mating preferences, female mate choice and reproductive success in mammals.

INTRODUCTION Sexual selection theory predicts that female mammals should preferentially mate with

high quality males, choosing the sire of their offspring for direct benefits, that increase female survival or fecundity, or indirect (genetic) benefits that increase offspring viability and/or attractiveness (Andersson 1994). Traditionally, the role of female mate choice, especially in polygynous mammals, has been considered negligible compared with male-male competition. However, on the basis of the relatively higher level of maternal to paternal investment and generally lower potential rates of reproduction the females of most mammal species could be expected to select their mating partners (Trivers 1972; Clutton-Brock and Vincent 1991). Although the fitness payoffs for discriminating females (and hence the evolutionary trajectories through which female preferences arise) are still much debated (Kirkpatrick and Ryan 1991; Jennions and Petrie 1997; reviewed by Cordero and Eberhard 2003; Kokko et al. 2003), the adaptive value of female mate choice in mammal species, where males differ in their reproductive value to females, is now generally accepted (Andersson 1994; Paul 2002).

Studies of female choice in mammals are mostly hindered by the confounding influence of other male and female mating strategies. Female mating strategies may fulfill non-procreative roles, for example, multi male mating may reflect the need to confuse paternity in mammals where infanticide is common (Wolff and Macdonald 2004) or to maintain the resources and support of male social group members (Gagneux et al. 1999). In addition, male strategies may limit a female to mating with only one male whatever her preference criteria (Soltis 1999), making it imperative to make the distinction between female mate choice and female mating preferences (Halliday 1983). Interpreting patterns of mate choice in terms of variation in mating preferences is problematic as female mating preferences, referring only to internal motivation, are just one component of female mate choice. Moreover, variation in female mate choice can be due to variability in sampling tactics, which are in turn affected by external factors (risk of predation, male harassment), as well as preference functions (Jennions and Petrie 1997). Hence, in natural conditions mating preferences are expressed in a field of environmental and social constraints imposed on mate searching females. For these reasons, female mate choice is best described “as the differential mating by females as a result of the interaction of environmental conditions, mating preferences and sampling strategies” (Wagner 1998). However, it must also be noted that females may even be passively attracted to a more conspicuous signal and not, after sampling several potential mates, actively choosing an individual at all (Forrest and Raspet 1994). Finally, mate choice is not just restricted to active discrimination between potential mating partners but can also be indirect via any other behaviour that increases or decreases the chances of mating with a specific subset of males (Wiley and Poston 1996). Any female mating strategies that incite male-male competition, e.g. reproductive synchrony (Chism and Rogers 1997) or female promiscuity (Preston et al. 2003), so that only the highest quality males can gain access to females, may represent examples of indirect mate choice.

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 37

Female mate choice, whether direct or indirect, is constrained by the environment and can only be realistically observed under natural conditions. However, in natural conditions male-male competition can generate the same assortative mating patterns as would female choice and it becomes hard to separate the two processes of selection (Andersson 1994). To determine female mating preferences appropriate experimental setups are required that control for male-male competition and isolate the male trait of preference from other aspects of the male phenotype. A further complication of mate choice studies lies in showing that male traits of female preference are positively correlated to male reproductive success in natural conditions i.e. that they influence reproductive outcomes, such as the viability and/or attractiveness of the offspring (Searcy and Andersson 1986). To do this requires genetic paternity analysis studies that determine the reproductive success of different male phenotypes and their subsequent offspring.

Hence, to gain a complete understanding of female mate choice in any given species requires a combination of field and experimental studies. Notwithstanding these problems, many studies of mate choice have been conducted on mammals and important insights gained. The current literature reveals that the females of several mammal species do choose their mating partners and through this potentially gain important direct and indirect fitness benefits.

FEMALE MATE CHOICE STUDIES

Choice for Direct Benefits Evidence that female mammals choose males for resources that directly increase their

fecundity or reduce reproductive costs is less abundant than it is in birds (Lampe and Espmark 2003) and insects (Thornhill 1976) but does exist. Female mate choice in some highly social mammals for more familiar males may be based on complex delayed preferences for individuals that have proved themselves better able to provision their females over time, as has been suggested in spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) (East and Hofer 1991). However, conjecture aside, few studies actually show that female mammals choose mates for direct benefits or indicate what criteria are selected.

There is some evidence to suggest female choice based on territory quality occurs in certain mammal species. A study on a facultatively monogamous population of Pikas (Ochotona princeps) reports female choice of mate based primarily on nest quality and location (Brandt 1989). Females preferred single entrance dens located close to food resources for their nests, factors also positively correlated to reproductive success. In this case, males did not actively interfere with the mating attempts of other individuals and hence females may have been able to exercise relatively free choice. However, the importance of other male phenotypic characteristics not considered in this study, such as vocal displays (that could be used by females to discriminate between males) is unknown. It has also been suggested that the females of some monogamous primates may prefer males that are better able to defend a territory (Smuts 1987). However, it can be difficult in any territorial species to determine whether females are attracted to individual males per se or the territory they currently occupy. As only the highest quality males are able to acquire and hold on to the

Benjamin D. Charlton 38

‘preferred’ territories, any findings are probably confounded by choice for male phenotype. A further complication in quantifying female choice for territory is that it may simply be the result of females copying the preferences of other individuals (Brooks 1998) or appearing to due to the incidental consequence of some other adaptive behaviour e.g. females congregating together to reduce predation risk or harassment (Clutton-Brock and McComb 1993).

Iberian red deer males (Cervus elaphus hispanicus) compete for and defend territories that females are subsequently attracted too (Carranza 1995). Although male phenotype would still appear to be important for attracting females, as some males maintained higher attraction rates than others on the same territories, male reproductive success in this species seems to be largely dependant on the quality and/or position of territory held. Some other studies on polygynous ungulates have suggested female mate choice based at least partly on territory at leks (Apollonio et al. 1990; Balmford et al. 1992a). These mating systems are traditionally thought to provide only indirect benefits to females, however, female choice driven at least in part for direct benefits would go some way to solving what has become known as the 'lek paradox' (Borgia 1979; Reynolds and Gross 1990).

Other studies have suggested that female mammals obtain direct benefits in the form of reduced harassment when selecting to mate with males on leks (Balmford 1991; Clutton-Brock et al. 1992; Nefdt and Thirgood 1997). However, this is also disputed by work reporting higher rates of harassment for females on leks (Bro-Jorgensen 2002). In any case, harassment by other courting males can impose serious fitness costs which mate searching females would be expected to avoid (Reale et al. 1996). It would appear that certain female mating tactics have evolved to reduce male harassment costs (Galimberti et al. 2000) and females may also be expected to have evolved preferences for males with certain characteristics that can reduce disturbances from other males. This appears to be the case in South American sea lions (Otaria flavescens: Cassini and Fernandez-Juricic 2003) and possibly Sumatran orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii: Fox 2002) where female association with adult males, to reduce harassment costs, may be a factor influencing mate choice. Female attraction to males with higher roaring rates in red deer (Cervus elaphus) may also reflect a choice for high quality males that are better able to protect females from harassment and hence a choice based partly on direct benefits to the female (McComb 1991).

In mammals where parental care limits female reproductive success females could be expected to show a choice for males that demonstrate infant care abilities. Some male primates occasionally carry infants and, as carrying may reduce the risks of infanticide and hence be of direct benefit to the female, this could be seen as a form of parental care (Palombit et al. 1997). However, this care may simply be a male mating strategy if females mate preferentially with males that have previously demonstrated infant care (Keddy-Hector 1992). In male lions (Panthera leo) mane darkness is correlated to serum testosterone and nutrition, reflecting good fighting ability and short-term health of the bearer (West and Packer 2002). Thus, females could gain direct benefits from preferring males with darker manes that would be more adept at protecting their cubs from infanticide and providing food for the pride. These predictions remain untested and currently there appears to be no documented cases of female choice based on parental care abilities in mammals. However, some monogamous mammals could be expected to exhibit bi-parental care, as certainty of paternity for males would be higher than it is in polygynous mammals. More research is required to determine whether or not this is the case. Lastly, there may also be direct incentives for females to avoid parasitized males in mating systems where the female appears to gain

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 39

nothing more from than a male’s sperm from copulation (Kirkpatrick and Ryan 1991). Effects of parasite infection on mate choice may therefore reflect the avoidance of direct costs, if fecundity is affected by parasite infection, rather than Hamilton-Zuk style indicator systems.

Choice for Indirect (Genetic) Benefits In mating systems where males contribute little but their genes to their offspring it may

be that females are choosing males for the indirect benefits of ‘good genes’ (Darwin 1871; Maynard Smith 1991). The expression of a specific morphological trait of preference in the offspring whether it advertises ‘genetic quality’ or is arbitrarily ‘attractive’ to females will yield higher lifetime reproductive success via increased longevity or mating opportunities for the bearer (Kokko et al. 2002). Female mate choice for male phenotype is undoubtedly important in lekking mammals (Clutton-Brock et al. 1989; Balmford 1991; Bro-Jorgensen 2002) but the exact criteria on which female preferences are based in these mating systems is still fairly unclear. The majority of mate choice studies conducted so far on mammals claim to demonstrate female preferences for dominant individuals. As high status may indicate high genetic quality, this suggests that choice for ‘good genes’ may be an important selective force in mammals.

A recent study investigating the relationship between male dominance ranks, female mate choice and reproductive success in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) reports female choice for dominant males (Klinkova et al. 2005). However, in this case, the positive correlation between male dominance rank and reproductive success observed would appear to be generated predominantly by rank related differences in female solicitations and competitive ability. Indeed, with a social climate characterized by male coercion, in which females are unlikely to be able to exercise free choice, it may be impossible to determine whether any choice reflects true underlying female preferences. Dominant males will often monopolize access to females during the breeding season or at peak conception time (Soltis 1999; Preston et al. 2003), especially in mating systems where female mating tactics incite male-male competition (Semple 1998; Wolff 1998). Other researchers investigating mammal mate choice in natural conditions, propose that females choose dominant or more vigorous males on the basis that subordinates are rejected: reindeer (Rangifer tarandus: Hirotani 1989); bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis canadensis: Hogg 1987); pronghorn deer (Antilocapra americana: Byers et al. 1994) and bison (Bison bison: Wolff 1998) or because female solicitation rates are higher to dominant individuals: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes: Matsumoto-Oda 1999; Vorobieva et al. 2004); Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata: Soltis et al. 1997); ring-tailed mouse lemurs (Lemur catta: Parga 2002) and vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops: Keddy 1986). However, the findings of the above studies cannot be interpreted as evidence for active female choice of dominant males when in actual fact the female’s ability to express preferences is likely to be constrained by the social environment. In addition, avoidance of young or subordinate males by females is common in several mammal species (Cox and Leboeuf 1977; Clutton-Brock et al. 1982; Miura 1984; Hirotani 1989; Komers et al. 1999) and hence, whether females are directly choosing dominant individuals remains speculative.

When other competing males are around there are likely be other reasons for associating with older or more dominant individuals who may be expected to have greater abilities to

Benjamin D. Charlton 40

acquire and defend resources as well as provide increased protection from sexual coercion, harassment or infanticide (Pope 1990; Pereira and Weiss 1991). Indeed, female capuchins cease to prefer the dominant male when he loses his dominance status, suggesting that they are not selecting these individuals for their ‘good genes’ (Janson 1984). Moreover, dominant male mammals do not always consistently attain high reproductive or even mating success (Takahata et al. 1999). In any case, it is imperative to control for male competition so that female mate choice can be studied free of constraints and mating preferences can be determined.

Experimental Studies of Female Preferences in Mammals

Until now definite experimental studies of mate choice have typically focused on non-mammal animal species e.g. insects (Klappert and Reinhold 2003), frogs (Murphy and Gerhardt 2000) and birds (Mountjoy and Lemon 1996) where short life spans and gestation periods make reproductive success quicker to determine and in which the females show preferences for clearly defined morphological or behavioural male traits. Mammals, which are invariably larger, have longer life spans and inter-birth intervals and are behaviourally more complex, are less suited to this type of experimentation. Notwithstanding these problems, some studies on small mammals reporting direct female mate choice for dominant males have controlled for male competition using laboratory experiments: house mouse (Mus musculus: Rolland et al. 2003; Tomihara 2005) and field experiments: golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus: Lisk et al. 1989); brown lemmings (Lemmus-Trimucronatus: Huck and Banks 1982); voles (Microtus ochrogaster and Microtus montanus: Shapiro and Dewsbury 1986); bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus: Horne and Ylonen 1996) and brown capuchins (Cebus apella: Janson 1984; Welker et al. 1990). However in all these cases, the exact male phenotypic trait(s) of female preference are not known.

A recent experiment on captive grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) employed a two-way mate choice design in which females could choose between two separately caged males, hence controlling for male-male competition (Craul et al. 2004). The authors report a consistent spatial and/or behavioural female preference, during oestrus, for the male with the higher trill call activity. However, as the authors report, higher trill rate is also positively correlated to male dominance rank and hence, it is impossible to determine whether females are actually attracted to calls with higher trill rates or if this is simply a by-product of this correlation. In addition, only 4 copulations (out of 12 females) were observed and hence whether these preferences translate to increased mating, or more importantly, reproductive success is unknown i.e. whether mating with males with higher trill call activity actually increases offspring viability. Indeed, an earlier study on the same species of mouse lemur reports that dominant males only sired half the offspring in a captive environment in which they may be expected to be able to monopolise females (Radespiel et al. 2002). Another study on the wild guinea pig (Galea musteloides), controlling for male competition, reports female preferences for heavier and more frequently courting males (Hohoff et al. 2003). However, in this case the high degree of promiscuity shown by the females suggests that sperm competition may be of greater importance than pre-copulatory mate choice. In this species, as with several primates, mating with larger more dominant males may fulfil other non-procreative functions and again, evidence that it results in more viable offspring is lacking.

In theory, females may also be expected to mate with older males for genetic benefits because viability selection leads to older males with higher genetic quality than those that are

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 41

younger (Trivers 1972; Kokko and Lindstrom 1996). This theory is backed up to some extent by studies reporting female preferences for older males in birds and insects (Simmons and Zuk 1992; Grahn and Vonschantz 1994). Although no empirical evidence exists in mammals, older males may also be preferred where age attests to viability in the current environment. In spotted hyenas (Crocuta crocuta) it has been suggested that male tenure may serve as an index of intrinsic genetic quality, although male fertility may also decline with extreme old age (East et al. 2003). Indeed, a recent theoretical model by Beck & Powell (2000) suggests that preferences for older males are unlikely to evolve in mating systems in which the male provides only sperm. In any case, empirical evidence for increased offspring viability would be required to determine whether females are actually choosing older males for their ‘good genes’. In fact, it appears that the only demonstration of increased progeny viability due to free female mate choice in mammals has been carried out on house mice (Mus musculus) (Drickamer et al. 2000). In this study, females that were allowed to choose their mates produced offspring of greater viability than those mated at random. However, although this represents empirical evidence of female mate choice for ‘good genes’, the actual male phenotypic trait(s) of female preference are again not known.

Indeed, identifying the actual trait of female preference, which is crucial to identifying male characteristics under sexual selection, can be extremely difficult. In addition, female preferences in many mammals may only emerge at specific times such as oestrus (Poole 1989; Matsumoto-Oda 1999; Pillay 2000) or in particular contexts (Qvarnstrom 2001) and in any case, seem likely to be based on multiple cues to male quality, of different or the same modalities (Candolin 2003). It follows therefore that in order to gain a foothold in this immensely complex subject, we must use appropriate experimental setups that control adequately for all other extrinsic and intrinsic factors that affect female behaviour, and in which specific phenotypic traits are isolated from other aspects of the male phenotype. Female responses to variation in specific signal components can then be assessed and an appreciation of how inter-sexual selection is generated on specific aspects of the male phenotype can be gained.

Female Preferences for Specific Phenotypic Traits Signalling Male Genetic Quality

Preliminary evidence that females of some mammal species may choose males for specific phenotypic traits comes from experimental set ups which are able to isolate specific male traits from other aspects of the male phenotype. However, conclusively demonstrating ‘attraction’ to an isolated trait variant, and hence inferring that mate choice decisions could be made based on them, is difficult.

Among mammals, primates possess the most brilliant secondary sexual colouration that may serve to advertise the bearer’s quality to potential mates. Examples include the brightly coloured faces of male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) (Setchell and Dixson 2001) and scrotal colour in vervets (Cercopithecus aethiops) (Gerald 2001). Whether female mandrills or vervet monkeys attend to these colour differences between males, however, is unknown. Rhesus macaque females (Macaca mulatta) gaze for longer at computer-manipulated red over pale versions of male faces (Waitt et al. 2003). In this case, male coloration might provide a cue to male quality but to determine whether these visual preferences indicate sexual preferences rather than constituting a more general response requires further research. Female preferences for darker manes have been demonstrated in lions (Panthera leo) (West and Packer 2002). Darker manes increase heat load for the male lions and may represent an honest indicator of

Benjamin D. Charlton 42

‘good genes’ (handicap theory: Zahavi 1975), as males with darker manes are presumably better able to withstand heat-related costs. Indeed, the study reports that males with the darkest manes had superior survival and competitive abilities resulting in increased offspring fitness, but that mane darkness was not heritable. It appears that male colouration, by indicating genetic quality, may play a part in adaptive female mate choice in mammals. Designing appropriate mate choice experiments to answer this question remains a challenge for the future.

Olfactory cues are relatively simple to isolate and present to females in controlled conditions and some of the best evidence of mate choice for ‘good genes’ in mammals has been generated in this way. Female mice (Mus domesticus) and rats (Rattus rattus) appear able to discriminate between parasitized and non-parasitized males based on urine odours (Willis and Poulin 2000; Kavaliers et al. 2003) and display increased stress responses to the urine odours of parasite-infected males (Kavaliers et al. 1998). In addition, female house mice mate preferentially with non-parasitized individuals (Ehman and Scott 2002), possibly securing resistant, and hence, ‘good’ genes for their offspring (Hamilton and Zuk 1982). Female odour preferences for resident males are also reported in rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) (Reeceengel 1988) however, if these males actually achieve higher reproductive success in natural conditions remains unknown.

Scent over-marking is a common form of competitive advertisement among many mammal species and some work has suggested that scent frequency and placement may play a role in mate choice, possibly for dominant males that mark over subordinates (Johnston et al. 1997; Johnston and Barot 2004) . The Johnston et al. (1997) study on meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus) reports female preferences for familiar over novel males even when their scents have been over marked by novel males; however, the ability of these females to discriminate between either novel or familiar male scent markings placed on top of each other could be disputed. Indeed, studies on prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) in which mate choice based on these criteria had been hypothesised but subsequently disproved, suggests that scent quality may be more important than scent frequency and placement for mate attraction (Thomas 2002; Mech et al. 2003).

Vocal displays can also be reproduced accurately and isolated from other aspects of the male phenotype using playback experiments. Several previous studies on mammals have suggested that male calls may function directly to advertise male quality and attract females without testing experimentally whether or not this is the case: old world monkeys (Gautier and Gautier 1977); pikas (Ochotona princeps) (Conner and Whitworth 1985); hammerhead bats (Hypsignathus monstrosus) (Bradbury 1977); and some ungulates (Kiley 1972; McElligott et al. 1999). The structurally complex songs of lone humpback (Megaptera novaeangliae) and finback whales (Balaenoptera physalus) and the underwater acoustic displays of male harbour seals (Phoca vitulina) are also thought to function in broadcasting male quality and attracting females (Tyack 1981; Hanggi and Schusterman 1994; Croll et al. 2002). More detailed work on humpback whales, although confirming that these acoustic displays mediate approach and avoidance response, reports no female attraction to male song (Tyack 1983; Mobley 1988). However, the possibility that the female humpbacks use male vocal displays to assess potential mates, only allowing certain males to approach them, cannot be excluded. Similarly, although the long calls of male orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) were originally thought to function in mate attraction, playback experiments to specifically test this

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 43

did not yield the predicted results (Mitani 1985). This further highlights how crucial it is to test predictions about mate choice using systematic playback experiments.

Recent work on fur seals (Arctocephalus spp.) suggests that the divergence in bark calls (which are only given during male-female interactions) could be due to female choice but not full threat calls directed at other males (Page et al. 2002). Indeed, although female mate choice is very likely to be a significant selection force on the calls of many male mammals, currently, the only mate choice studies conducted on any mammal that systematically demonstrate a female preference for a vocal characteristic are in red deer (Cervus elaphus) (McComb 1991; Charlton 2006; Charlton et al. 2007b). The McComb (1991) study demonstrated a female preference for male roars delivered at higher rates. As male roaring is energetically costly to produce this may represent an honest signal of genetic quality e.g. a handicap (Zahavi 1975; Grafen 1990) and hence a choice for good genes; however, roaring rate seems more likely to reflect current short-term condition or motivation rather than long-term genetic quality. More recent work using an experimental setup showed that oestrous female red deer preferred male roars in which lower formant frequencies simulated larger caller’s (Charlton et al. 2007b). To date, this remains the only female mate choice study in mammals to demonstrate a mating preference for an isolated male phenotypic trait signalling ‘good genes’ (i.e. larger body size) whilst controlling for female hormonal state and male-male competition.

Choice for Compatible Genes Another aspect of female choice for male genetic ‘quality’ concerns genetic compatibility

(Trivers 1972; Neff and Pitcher 2005) with an increasing body of empirical studies to suggest that females should benefit, by increasing offspring fitness, through choosing genetically dissimilar males (Mays and Hill 2004). Indeed, choice for unfamiliar or novel partners as suggested in several mammals may function to increase offspring genetic heterozygosity and subsequent fitness (Manson 1995; Amos et al. 2001). However, in these cases females should choose in a relative rather than an absolute sense and prefer males with genotypes that best complement their own rather than for ‘good genes’ per se. This method of mate selection does not lead to genetic fixation of male traits, a problem for good genes and runaway selection models, as females should not be congruent in their choice for particular male phenotype i.e. a male that is best for one female may not be best for another (Brown 1997). Choice for compatible genes may have evolved to avoid the reproductive costs associated with inbreeding and distant outbreeding. Female white-footed mice (Peromyscus leucopus) gain maximum reproductive success when mated with males of intermediate genetic similarity (Keane 1990) and may represent an example of this type of mate choice, but how the mating preferences observed in this study are mediated is unclear. Indeed, the major problem facing the theory of female choice based on genetic compatibility is in how females actually recognise genetically incompatible mates.

There is good evidence to suggest that the genes of the major histocompatibility complex (MHC), that play a key role in the immune system, function as cues to mate choice for genetically compatible males (reviewed in Penn and Potts 1999). Generally, the mating preference seems to be MHC-disassortative i.e. males with dissimilar major histocompatibility complex alleles are preferred (Penn 2002). This mating pattern leads to

Benjamin D. Charlton 44

more outbred and heterozygous offspring, which has been suggested to increase offspring fitness (Brown and Eklund 1994; Grob et al. 1998). Indirect evidence for MHC mediated inbreeding avoidance comes from Potts et al. (1991) who, having found significantly fewer MHC-homozygous offspring than expected, suggested that female mice (Mus domesticus) sought extra-territorial copulations with MHC dissimilar males. However, the results of this study may be the product of simple non-MHC mediated inbreeding avoidance. Convincing evidence for MHC-disassortative mating preferences in mammals has been generated by work on captive mice (Mus domesticus) where discrimination between different MHC genotypes seems to be mediated through specific odour cues (reviewed by Jordan and Bruford 1998). MHC-disassortative mating preferences may increase the resistance of offspring to infectious diseases (Penn 2002) and/or enable individuals to recognize closely related kin and function to avoid inbreeding (Yamazaki et al. 2000). Since MHC genes influence resistance to many infectious diseases, females who prefer disease resistant males may confer these resistant alleles on their offspring (Hamilton and Zuk 1982), however, it remains to be shown whether males with attractive displays carry resistant MHC alleles. Indeed, it is not clear whether the main function of MHC-disassortative mating preferences is enhanced progeny immunocompetance or inbreeding avoidance. Either way, there are potential indirect fitness benefits available to discriminating females.

MHC-dependant preferences may exist across several mammal species. For example, MHC variation is related to antler development and body mass in white tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus) and hence indicates ‘good genes’ (Ditchkoff et al. 2001). Interestingly, although MHC variation in soay sheep (Ovis aries L.) is associated with resistance to a parasitic nematode (Paterson et al. 1998) no evidence for MHC-disassortative mating preferences exists in this species (Paterson and Pemberton 1997). In this case, any female preference may have been masked by intense male-male competition but nevertheless demonstrates the need to test these predictions. Theoretically, female preferences for common MHC alleles, and hence genetic similarity at these loci, could also arise where an evolutionary ‘arms race’ type scenario exists between hosts and parasites that have not yet adapted to recently common alleles (Hamilton and Zuk 1982). Female striped mice (Rhabdomys pumilio) prefer ‘homotype’ males from their own population than ‘heterotype’ males, a preference for genetically similar males possibly mediated by MHC alleles (Pillay 2000). However, inbreeding avoidance would appear to offer the highest potential fitness benefits to females (Penn 2002). A recent study on wild house mice (Mus domesticus) reported that inbred males sired only one fifth as many surviving offspring as outbred males (Meagher et al. 2000). Female choice for unfamiliar or novel partners is observed in several mammals (Manson 1995) and may in some cases be mediated by MHC gene complexes in mammal species at risk of inbreeding (Clarke and Faulkes 1999; Valsecchi et al. 2002). While there is much evidence that MHC-dependant mating preferences exist in house mice further research is required to determine their generality to other mammal species.

Post-copulatory Choice Post-copulatory or ‘cryptic’ mate choice, in which females selectively abort the sperm or

offspring of certain males, can take two forms; directional post-copulatory choice in which females bias sperm use after copulation towards certain male phenotypes and non-directional

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 45

in which females favour the sperm of males with compatible genotypes regardless of their phenotype.

The copulation calls of some female primates that, by encouraging mate guarding and minimizing sperm competition, increase the chances of fertilization by preferred males may represent an example of directional post-copulatory mate choice in mammals (Maestripieri and Roney 2005). This is a compelling argument and contrary to previous suggestions that these copulation calls function to increase sperm competition (Oconnell and Cowlishaw 1994; Semple 1998). Indeed, an earlier study on barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) also reported increased mate guarding by consort males in response to copulation calls, which again is consistent with the post copulatory choice hypothesis (Todt et al. 1995).

Non-directional post-copulatory mate choice can take place via mechanisms that have evolved to prevent females from being fertilized by incompatible sperm (Zeh and Zeh 1996; Zeh and Zeh 1997). There is evidence in mice (Mus domesticus) that MHC-derived proteins expressed on sperm may influence the likelihood that a given spermatozoan will fertilise the egg, with MHC-similar pregnancies spontaneously aborted (Rulicke et al. 1998). Conversely, female grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) actively delay pregnancies more when housed with unfamiliar males suggesting post-copulatory choice for familiar males (Radespiel and Zimmermann 2003). Whatever the preference, post-copulatory mate choice may represent a powerful selective force, especially in mating systems dominated by male coercion and in which females are unlikely to be able to exercise pre-copulatory mate choice.

CONCLUSION Female mate choice in mammals represents a challenging area of research with many

questions still to be answered. While it appears that female mammals are provided with important direct and indirect benefits through mate choice, the functions and evolutionary consequences of such choice remain unclear. To answer these questions, definite evidence of how mate choice for particular male phenotypes impacts on female fecundity and reproductive success is required. If direct benefits are available, they are likely to have a much greater effect on female fitness than indirect benefits, and selection for males providing such benefits is easy to understand (Price et al. 1993). Surprisingly, although direct benefits may provide a possible explanation for some female mammal mating tactics (Wolff and Macdonald 2004), empirical evidence for mating preferences based on them is distinctly lacking in mammals, although direct fecundity benefits may underlie some mating preferences on leks. This apparent omission may arise because the majority of mate choice studies so far have been conducted on polygynous mammals in which males appear to provide little, if any, direct benefits to females and subsequently where female preferences based on them would be harder to observe. Mate choice for indirect benefits, because it is generally assumed that it will result from a genetic association between male attractiveness and offspring fitness, has always been controversial and considered insufficient to explain the evolution of female mating preferences alone (Kirkpatrick 1996). However, a recent theoretical model by Moller & Jennions (2001) suggests that direct benefits are only slightly more important than indirect ones arising from female choice. In reality, female mate choice in most mammal species will almost certainly be influenced to some extent by both direct and

Benjamin D. Charlton 46

indirect benefits. A study on puku (Kobus vardoni) and topi antelopes (Damaliscus lunatus) in which male phenotype, predation risk and territory quality were all related to female presence appears to confirm this (Balmford et al. 1992b). However, because putative mate choice cues are often interrelated, their relative importance for mate choice becomes difficult to ascertain.

A major problem with the majority of mammal studies is in how female preferences are quantified. Many experimental studies rely on indirect methods to assay female preferences such as increased attention towards an isolated male trait, which does not necessarily indicate attraction. For example, greater attention by red deer hinds to roars simulating males of sub-adult body size than to those simulating a large adult male, whilst making no sense from a mate choice perspective, is likely to indicate an aversive response to facilitate earlier detection and avoidance of young stags that are known to harass them (Charlton 2006; Charlton et al. 2008b). Observational studies often use high levels of interaction with certain individuals, which may be due to social and/or environmental constraints or for non-procreative reasons, to suggest a mating preference. In addition, many of these studies only record mating success, as individuals gaining copulations or proximity to members of the opposite sex, and do not necessarily record the reproductive success of individuals. It is essential to conduct genetic paternity analysis studies, to determine the reproductive success of trait bearing individuals and their offspring in natural environments, and hence determine any reproductive benefits to choice. Moreover, realistic assessments of the costs of female mate choice need to be carried out under approximately natural conditions when food supplies are limited and predators and competitors are present. These limitations on obtaining accurate measures of the costs and benefits of mate choice, as well as interacting and often competing male and female mating strategies, make it difficult to test theoretical predictions on mammals.

Some theoretical models of female mate choice behaviour suggest that females should be flexible in their mate choice decisions (Real 1990; for a review see Jennions and Petrie 1997) and recent empirical data shows plasticity of female preferences in animals (Moore and Moore 2001; Veen et al. 2001; Welch 2003; Chaine and Lyon 2008) even during a single reproductive cycle (Lynch et al. 2005). Studies of female choice in mammals, in which female preferences may only emerge during the period of peak conception, also emphasize the importance of considering the timing of female ovulation (Matsumoto-Oda 1999; Stumpf and Boesch 2005). Recent work on oestrous female red deer demonstrated a mating preference for male roars simulating larger caller’s (Charlton et al. 2007b), a trait positively correlated to male reproductive success in this species (Reby and McComb 2003). Importantly, by using an experimental setup, this study not only controlled for female hormonal state but was also able to remove other environmental and social factors that may otherwise obscure female mating preferences. Indeed, studies on other animals without observable female mate choice in natural conditions have illustrated that otherwise hidden female preferences do in fact exist and emerge under appropriate experimental conditions (Gould et al. 1999).

Future work should follow this type of experimental approach and concentrate on demonstrating female preferences for specific male traits using setups that control for the effects of male and female mating strategies and female hormonal state. Experimental investigation of female preferences, because they are highly dependant on female hormonal state, should be conducted during and outside peak conception times. Comparisons of female

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 47

reproductive behaviour during and outside peak conception times has yielded interesting results in tungara frogs (Physalaemus pustulosus) (Lynch et al. 2005) and house mice (Mus musculus) (Rolland et al. 2003) and may similarly provide important insights into female mate choice in large mammals. Female preferences are also likely to vary geographically due to evolved genetic/environmental differences (Qvarnstrom 2001). Therefore, interesting comparisons could also be made of the female mating preferences of a given mammal species in different environmental conditions. Moreover, experiments using repeated measures designs would provide more complete information about the form of female preferences in mammals, enabling preference functions (how the strength of a female preference varies according to the expression of the male trait) to be built up for individual females as well as for populations (Wagner 1998). In addition, as mate choice studies in general have paid less attention to variation between individual females than males (Jennions and Petrie 1997) the examination of both within and between-female variation in preferences may prove to be a productive area of research in mammals.

Mate-searching females are also likely to assess multiple cues when making mate choice decisions (Candolin 2003; van Doorn and Weissing 2004), yet no mammal mate choice studies have explicitly examined the relative importance of different male traits to female choice. To do this would require experiments in which different male phenotypic trait values within a stimulus are independently varied and presented to females in order to ascertain their relative importance for mate choice. In particular, this approach would allow us to examine how male traits interact to influence female mating decisions. Such interactions undoubtedly have important implications for sexual display evolution in animals’ generally e.g. satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus: Patricelli et al. 2003) and appear to have important effects on female mate choice behaviour in some anuran (Hyla gratiosa: Poole and Murphy 2007) and insect species (Gryllus campestris: Scheuber et al. 2004), but as yet represent an unexplored avenue of research in mammals. A recent study on red deer independently manipulated components of the male roar, the fundamental frequency and the formant frequencies (an acoustic cue to body size), in order to examine female looking responses to calls characterized by different combinations of these acoustic components (Charlton et al. 2008a). The findings of this study suggested that the fundamental frequency of the male roar did not affect female perception of size-related formant information but also reinforced the idea that formants, being a long-term indicator of male body-size and hence genetic quality, are more important to red deer hinds as acoustic cues for mate assessment during the breeding season. Future mammal studies should follow this approach to examine the combined effect of variation in two separate components of a male mammals sexual display, using experimental setups in which the effects of competing male and female mating strategies, female hormonal state and female sampling strategies are controlled for.

In conclusion, female mate choice in mammals remains a complex research area requiring new and innovative experimental paradigms to elucidate what are often transient female preferences. Technological advances in digital sound production and playback equipment make female preferences for vocal characteristics, which can easily be isolated, a possible area for advancing our knowledge of female mate choice. Playback experiments can be used to present females with re-synthesized male calls, in which a vocal characteristic of interest is manipulated, to test their ability to perceive cues to male quality in mammal calls (Charlton et al. 2007a), and investigate whether they use these cues to assess male quality in a mate choice context (Charlton et al. 2007b.). Other technological advances now allow digital

Benjamin D. Charlton 48

images to be realistically modified to test for visual preferences (Waitt et al. 2003) or to test the ability of females to link acoustic stimuli to visual representations of different male phenotypes (Ghazanfar et al. 2007). This experimental work should be followed up by fieldwork where mating behaviour can be observed and paternity analyses conducted under natural conditions to realistically determine the reproductive outcomes of male and female mating strategies. By combining knowledge gained from experimental and observational studies both in the field and laboratory we may ultimately hope to understand the link between mating preferences, female mate choice and reproductive success in mammals. Only a combination of carefully designed field and laboratory experiments will enable us to achieve this goal.

REFERENCES Amos W, Wilmer JW, Kokko H (2001) Do female grey seals select genetically diverse

mates? Animal Behaviour 62:157-164. Andersson M (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton, PRINCETON UNIVERSITY PRESS. Apollonio M, Festabianchet M, Mari F, Riva M (1990) Site-Specific Asymmetries in Male

Copulatory Success in a Fallow Deer Lek. Animal Behaviour 39:205-212. Balmford A (1991) Mate Choice on Leks. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 6:87-&. Balmford A, Albon S, Blakeman S (1992a) Correlates of Male Mating Success and Female

Choice in a Lek- Breeding Antelope. Behavioral Ecology 3:112-123. Balmford A, Rosser AM, Albon SD (1992b) Correlates of Female Choice in Resource-

Defending Antelope. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 31:107-114. Beck CW, Powell LA (2000) Evolution of female mate choice based on male age: Are older

males better mates? Evolutionary Ecology Research 2:107-118. Borgia G (1979) Sexual selection and the evolution of mating system. In: Sexual selection

and reproductive competition (Eds Blum M, Blum A), pp 19-80. New York: Academic Press.

Bradbury J (1977) Lek mating behavior in the hammer–headed bat. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 45:225-255.

Brandt CA (1989) Mate Choice and Reproductive Success of Pikas. Animal Behaviour 37:118-132.

Bro-Jorgensen J (2002) Overt female mate competition and preference for central males in a lekking antelope. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99:9290-9293.

Brooks R (1998) The importance of mate copying and cultural inheritance of mating preferences. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 13:45-46.

Brown JL (1997) A theory of mate choice based on heterozygosity. Behavioral Ecology 8:60-65.

Brown JL, Eklund A (1994) Kin Recognition and the Major Histocompatibility Complex - an Integrative Review. American Naturalist 143:435-461.

Byers JA, Moodie JD, Hall N (1994) Pronghorn Females Choose Vigorous Mates. Animal Behaviour 47:33-43.

Candolin U (2003) The use of multiple cues in mate choice. Biological Reviews 78:575-595.

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 49

Carranza J (1995) Female Attraction by Males Versus Sites in Territorial Rutting Red Deer. Animal Behaviour 50:445-453.

Cassini MH, Fernandez-Juricic E (2003) Costs and benefits of joining South American sea lion breeding groups: testing the assumptions of a model of female breeding dispersion. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 81:1154-1160.

Chaine AS, Lyon BE (2008) Adaptive plasticity in female mate choice dampens sexual selection on male ornaments in the lark bunting. Science 319:459-462.

Charlton BD (2006) Female perception and use of size-related formant information in the roars of red deer stags (Cervus elaphus). PhD Thesis, University of Sussex.

Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K (2007a) Female perception of size-related formant shifts in red deer, Cervus elaphus. Animal Behaviour 74:707-714.

Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K (2007b) Female red deer prefer the roars of larger males. Biology Letters 3:382-385.

Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K (2008a) Effect of combined source (F0) and filter (formant) variation on red deer hind responses to male roars. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 123 (5): 2936-2943.

Charlton BD, Reby D, McComb K (2008b) Free-ranging red deer hinds use formant frequencies as acoustic cues to male body size and maturity. Ethology 114: 1023-1031.

Chism J, Rogers W (1997) Male competition, mating success and female choice in a seasonally breeding primate (Erythrocebus patas). Ethology 103:109-126.

Clarke FM, Faulkes CG (1999) Kin discrimination and female mate choice in the naked mole-rat Heterocephalus glaber. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 266:1995-2002.

Clutton-Brock TH, Albon S, Guinness FE (1982) Red Deer: Behavior and Ecology of Two Sexes. Chicago, UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS.

Clutton-Brock TH, Hiraiwahasegawa M, Robertson A (1989) Mate Choice on Fallow Deer Leks. Nature 340:463-465.

Clutton-Brock TH, McComb K (1993) Experimental Tests of Copying and Mate Choice in Fallow Deer (Dama-Dama). Behavioral Ecology 4:191-193.

Clutton-Brock TH, Price OF, Maccoll ADC (1992) Mate Retention, Harassment, and the Evolution of Ungulate Leks. Behavioral Ecology 3:234-242.

Clutton-Brock TH, Vincent ACJ (1991) Sexual Selection and the Potential Reproductive Rates of Males and Females. Nature 351:58-60.

Conner DA, Whitworth MR (1985) The Ontogeny of Vocal Communication in the Pika. Journal of Mammalogy 66:756-763.

Cordero C, Eberhard WG (2003) Female choice of sexually antagonistic male adaptations: a critical review of some current research. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 16:1-6.

Cox CR, Leboeuf BJ (1977) Female Incitation of Male Competition - Mechanism in Sexual Selection. American Naturalist 111:317-335.

Craul M, Zimmermann E, Radespiel U (2004) First experimental evidence for female mate choice in a nocturnal primate. Primates 45:271-4.

Croll DA, Clark CW, Acevedo A, Tershy B, Flores S, Gedamke J, Urban J (2002) Bioacoustics: Only male fin whales sing loud songs - These mammals need to call long-distance when it comes to attracting females. Nature 417:809-809.

Darwin C (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London, MURRAY.

Benjamin D. Charlton 50

Ditchkoff SS, Lochmiller RL, Masters RE, Hoofer SR, Van Den Bussche RA (2001) Major-histocompatibility-complex-associated variation in secondary sexual traits of white-tailed deer (Odocoileus virginianus): Evidence for good-genes advertisement. Evolution 55:616-625.

Drickamer LC, Gowaty PA, Holmes CM (2000) Free female mate choice in house mice affects reproductive success and offspring viability and performance. Animal Behaviour 59:371-378.

East ML, Burke T, Wilhelm K, Greig C, Hofer H (2003) Sexual conflicts in spotted hyenas: male and female mating tactics and their reproductive outcome with respect to age, social status and tenure. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270:1247-1254.

East ML, Hofer H (1991) Loud Calling in a Female-Dominated Mammalian Society.2. Behavioral Contexts and Functions of Whooping of Spotted Hyaenas, Crocuta-Crocuta. Animal Behaviour 42:651-669.

Ehman KD, Scott ME (2002) Female mice mate preferentially with non-parasitized males. Parasitology 125:461-466.

Forrest TG, Raspet R (1994) Models of Female Choice in Acoustic Communication. Behavioral Ecology 5:293-303.

Fox EA (2002) Female tactics to reduce sexual harassment in the Sumatran orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus abelii). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52:93-101.

Gagneux P, Boesch C, Woodruff DS (1999) Female reproductive strategies, paternity and community structure in wild West African chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 57:19-32.

Galimberti F, Boitani L, Marzetti I (2000) Female strategies of harassment reduction in southern elephant seals. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 12:367-388.

Gautier JP, Gautier A (1977) Communication in Old World primates. In: How Animals Communicate (Ed Sebeok TA), pp 890-964. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

Gerald MS (2001) Primate colour predicts social status and aggressive outcome. Animal Behaviour 61:559-566.

Ghazanfar AA, Turesson HK, Maier JX, Van Dinther R, Patterson RD, Logothetis NK (2007) Vocal-Tract Resonances as Indexical Cues in Rhesus Monkeys. Current Biology 17:425-430.

Gould JL, Elliot SL, Masters CM, Mukerji J (1999) Female preferences in a fish genus without female mate choice. Current Biology 9:497-500.

Grafen A (1990) Biological Signals as Handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144:517-546.

Grahn M, Vonschantz T (1994) Fashion And Age In Pheasants - Age-Differences In Mate Choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 255:237-241.

Grob B, Knapp LA, Martin RD, Anzenberger G (1998) The major histocompatibility complex and mate choice: Inbreeding avoidance and selection of good genes. Experimental and Clinical Immunogenetics 15:119-129.

Halliday T (1983) The Study of Mate Choice. In: Mate Choice (Ed. By PPG Bateson), pp 3-32. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hamilton WD, Zuk M (1982) Heritable True Fitness and Bright Birds - a Role for Parasites. Science 218:384-387.

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 51

Hanggi EB, Schusterman RJ (1994) Underwater acoustic displays and individual variation in male harbour seals, Phoca vitulina. Animal Behaviour 48:1275-1283.

Hirotani A (1989) Social Relationships of Reindeer Rangifer-Tarandus During Rut - Implications for Female Choice. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 24:183-202.

Hogg JT (1987) Intrasexual Competition and Mate Choice in Rocky-Mountain Bighorn Sheep. Ethology 75:119-144.

Hohoff C, Franzen K, Sachser N (2003) Female choice in a promiscuous wild guinea pig, the yellow- toothed cavy (Galea musteloides). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53:341-349.

Horne TJ, Ylonen H (1996) Female bank voles (Clethrionomys glareolus) prefer dominant males; But what if there is no choice? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 38:401-405.

Huck UW, Banks EM (1982) Differential Attraction of Females to Dominant Males - Olfactory Discrimination and Mating Preference in the Brown Lemming (Lemmus-Trimucronatus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 11:217-222.

Janson CH (1984) Female Choice and Mating System of the Brown Capuchin Monkey Cebus-Apella (Primates, Cebidae). Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie-Journal of Comparative Ethology 65:177-200.

Jennions MD, Petrie M (1997) Variation in mate choice and mating preferences: A review of causes and consequences. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 72:283-327.

Johnston RE, Barot S (2004) Sexual selection, scent over-marking, and mate choice in golden hamsters. Hormones and Behavior 46:113-113.

Johnston RE, Sorokin ES, Ferkin MH (1997) Female voles discriminate males' over-marks and prefer top- scent males. Animal Behaviour 54:679-690.

Jordan WC, Bruford MW (1998) New perspectives on mate choice and the MHC. Heredity 81:239-245.

Kavaliers M, Colwell DD, Choleris E (1998) Parasitized female mice display reduced aversive responses to the odours of infected males. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 265:1111-1118.

Kavaliers M, Fudge MA, Colwell DD, Choleris E (2003) Aversive and avoidance responses of female mice to the odors of males infected with an ectoparasite and the effects of prior familiarity. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 54:423-430.

Keane B (1990) The Effect of Relatedness on Reproductive Success and Mate Choice in the White-Footed Mouse, Peromyscus-Leucopus. Animal Behaviour 39:264-273.

Keddy AC (1986) Female Mate Choice in Vervet Monkeys (Cercopithecus-Aethiops- Sabaeus). American Journal of Primatology 10:125-134.

Keddy-Hector AC (1992) Mate Choice in Nonhuman-Primates. American Zoologist 32:62-70.

Kiley M (1972) The vocalisations of ungulates, their causation and function. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 31:171-222.

Kirkpatrick M (1996) Good genes and direct selection in evolution of mating preferences. Evolution 50:2125-2140.

Kirkpatrick M, Ryan MJ (1991) The Evolution of Mating Preferences and the Paradox of the Lek. Nature 350:33-38.

Benjamin D. Charlton 52

Klappert K, Reinhold K (2003) Acoustic preference functions and sexual selection on the male calling song in the grasshopper Chorthippus biguttulus. Animal Behaviour 65:225-233.

Klinkova E, Hodges JK, Fuhrmann K, de Jong T, Heistermann M (2005) Male dominance rank, female mate choice and male mating and reproductive success in captive chimpanzees. International Journal of Primatology 26:357-384.

Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J (2003) The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270:653-664.

Kokko H, Brooks R, McNamara JM, Houston AI (2002) The sexual selection continuum. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 269:1331-1340.

Kokko H, Lindstrom J (1996) Evolution of female preference for old mates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 263:1533-1538.

Komers PE, Birgersson B, Ekvall K (1999) Timing of estrus in fallow deer is adjusted to the age of available mates. American Naturalist 153:431-436.

Lampe HM, Espmark YO (2003) Mate choice in Pied Flycatchers Ficedula hypoleuca: can females use song to find high-quality males and territories? Ibis 145:E24-E33.

Lisk RD, Huck UW, Gore AC, Armstrong MX (1989) Mate Choice, Mate Guarding and Other Mating Tactics in Golden- Hamsters Maintained under Seminatural Conditions. Behaviour 109:58-75.

Lynch KS, Rand AS, Ryan MJ, Wilczynski W (2005) Plasticity in female mate choice associated with changing reproductive states. Animal Behaviour 69:689-699.

Maestripieri D, Roney JR (2005) Primate copulation calls and postcopulatory female choice. Behavioral Ecology 16:106-113.

Manson JH (1995) Do Female Rhesus Macaques Choose Novel Males. American Journal of Primatology 37:285-296.

Matsumoto-Oda A (1999) Female choice in the opportunistic mating of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Mahale. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 46:258-266.

Maynard Smith J (1991) Theories of Sexual Selection. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 6:146-151.

Mays HL, Hill GE (2004) Choosing mates: good genes versus genes that are a good fit. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19:554-559.

McComb KE (1991) Female Choice for High Roaring Rates in Red Deer, Cervus elaphus. Animal Behaviour 41:79-88.

McElligott AG, O'Neill KP, Hayden TJ (1999) Cumulative long-term investment in vocalization and mating success of fallow bucks, Dama dama. Animal Behaviour 57:1159-1167.

Meagher S, Penn DJ, Potts WK (2000) Male-male competition magnifies inbreeding depression in wild house mice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97:3324-3329.

Mech SG, Dunlap AS, Wolff JO (2003) Female prairie voles do not choose males based on their frequency of scent marking. Behavioural Processes 61:101-108.

Mitani JC (1985) Sexual Selection and Adult Male Orangutan Long Calls. Animal Behaviour 33:272-283.

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 53

Miura S (1984) Social-Behavior and Territoriality in Male Sika Deer (Cervus- Nippon Temminck 1838) During the Rut. Zeitschrift Fur Tierpsychologie-Journal of Comparative Ethology 64:33-73.

Mobley JR (1988) Responses of wintering humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to playback recordings of winter and summer vocalizations and of synthetic sound. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 23:211-233.

Moller AP, Jennions MD (2001) How important are direct fitness benefits of sexual selection? Naturwissenschaften 88:401-415.

Moore PJ, Moore AJ (2001) Reproductive aging and mating: The ticking of the biological clock in female cockroaches. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98:9171-9176.

Mountjoy DJ, Lemon RE (1996) Female choice for complex song in the European starling: A field experiment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 38:65-71.

Murphy CG, Gerhardt HC (2000) Mating preference functions of individual female barking treefrogs, Hyla gratiosa, for two properties of male advertisement calls. Evolution 54:660-669.

Nefdt RJC, Thirgood SJ (1997) Lekking, resource defense, and harassment in two subspecies of lechwe antelope. Behavioral Ecology 8:1-9.

Neff BD, Pitcher TE (2005) Genetic quality and sexual selection: an integrated framework for good genes and compatible genes. Molecular Ecology 14:19-38.

Oconnell SM, Cowlishaw G (1994) Infanticide Avoidance, Sperm Competition and Mate Choice - the Function of Copulation Calls in Female Baboons. Animal Behaviour 48:687-694.

Page B, Goldsworthy SD, Hindell MA, McKenzie J (2002) Interspecific differences in male vocalizations of three sympatric fur seals (Arctocephalus spp.). Journal of Zoology 258:49-56.

Palombit RA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL (1997) The adaptive value of 'friendships' to female baboons: experimental and observational evidence. Animal Behaviour 54:599-614.

Parga J (2002) Male dominance rank reversals during the breeding season in ringtailed lemurs (Lemur catta): changes resulting from female mate choice. American Journal of Physical Anthropology:123-123.

Paterson S, Pemberton JM (1997) No evidence for major histocompatibility complex-dependent mating patterns in a free-living ruminant population. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 264:1813-1819.

Paterson S, Wilson K, Pemberton JM (1998) Major histocompatibility complex variation associated with juvenile survival and parasite resistance in a large unmanaged ungulate population (Ovis aries L.). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:3714-3719.

Paul A (2002) Sexual selection and mate choice. International Journal of Primatology 23:877-904.

Penn DJ (2002) The scent of genetic compatibility: Sexual selection and the major histocompatibility complex. Ethology 108:1-21.

Penn DJ, Potts WK (1999) The evolution of mating preferences and major histocompatibility complex genes. American Naturalist 153:145-164.

Pereira ME, Weiss ML (1991) Female Mate Choice, Male Migration, and the Threat of Infanticide in Ringtailed Lemurs. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 28:141-152.

Benjamin D. Charlton 54

Pillay N (2000) Female mate preference and reproductive isolation in populations of the striped mouse Rhabdomys pumilio. Behaviour 137:1431-1441.

Poole JH (1989) Mate Guarding, Reproductive Success and Female Choice in African Elephants. Animal Behaviour 37:842-849.

Poole KG, Murphy CG (2007) Preferences of female barking treefrogs, Hyla gratiosa, for larger males: univariate and composite tests. Animal Behaviour 71:513-524.

Pope TR (1990) The Reproductive Consequences of Male Cooperation in the Red Howler Monkey - Paternity Exclusion in Multimale and Single- Male Troops Using Genetic-Markers. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27:439-446.

Potts WK, Manning CJ, Wakeland EK (1991) Mating patterns in seminatural populations of mice influenced by MHC genotype. Nature 352:619-621.

Preston BT, Stevenson IR, Wilson K (2003) Soay rams target reproductive activity towards promiscuous females' optimal insemination period. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270:2073-2078.

Price T, Schluter D, Heckman NE (1993) Sexual Selection When the Female Directly Benefits. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 48:187-211.

Qvarnstrom A (2001) Context-dependent genetic benefits from mate choice. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 16:5-7.

Radespiel U, Dal Secco V, Drogemuller C, Braune P, Labes E, Zimmermann E (2002) Sexual selection, multiple mating and paternity in grey mouse lemurs, Microcebus murinus. Animal Behaviour 63:259-268.

Radespiel U, Zimmermann E (2003) The influence of familiarity, age, experience and female mate choice on pregnancies in captive grey mouse lemurs. Behaviour 140:301-318.

Real L (1990) Search Theory and Mate Choice.1. Models of Single-Sex Discrimination. American Naturalist 136:376-405.

Reale D, Bousses P, Chapuis JL (1996) Female-biased mortality induced by male sexual harassment in a feral sheep population. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 74:1812-1818.

Reby D, McComb K (2003) Anatomical constraints generate honesty: acoustic cues to age and weight in the roars of red deer stags. Animal Behaviour 65:519-530.

Reeceengel C (1988) Female Choice of Resident Male Rabbits Oryctolagus-Cuniculus. Animal Behaviour 36:1241-1242.

Reynolds JD, Gross MR (1990) Costs and Benefits of Female Mate Choice - Is There a Lek Paradox. American Naturalist 136:230-243.

Rolland C, MacDonald DW, De Fraipont M, Berdoy M (2003) Free female choice in house mice: Leaving best for last. Behaviour 140:1371-1388.

Rulicke T, Chapuisat M, Homberger FR, Macas E, Wedekind C (1998) MHC-genotype of progeny influenced by parental infection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 265:711-716.

Scheuber H, Jacot A, Brinkhof MWG (2004) Female preference for multiple condition-dependant components of a sexually selected signal. Proceedings Of The Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 271:2453-2457.

Searcy WA, Andersson M (1986) Sexual Selection and the Evolution of Song. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 17:507-533.

Semple S (1998) The function of Barbary macaque copulation calls. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 265:287-291.

Female Mate Choice in Nonhuman Mammals 55

Setchell JM, Dixson AF (2001) Arrested development of secondary sexual adornments in subordinate adult male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). American Journal of Physical Anthropology 115:245-252.

Shapiro LE, Dewsbury DA (1986) Male-Dominance, Female Choice and Male Copulatory-Behavior in 2 Species of Voles (Microtus-Ochrogaster and Microtus-Montanus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 18:267-274.

Simmons LW, Zuk M (1992) Variability In Call Structure And Pairing Success Of Male Field Crickets, Gryllus-Bimaculatus - The Effects Of Age, Size And Parasite Load. Animal Behaviour 44:1145-1152.

Smuts BB (1987) Sexual competition and mate choice. In: Primate Societies (Eds Smuts BB, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM, Wrangham RW, Struhsaker TT), pp 385-399. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Soltis J (1999) Measuring male-female relationships during the mating season in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Primates 40:453-467.

Soltis J, Mitsunaga F, Shimizu K, Nozaki M, Yanagihara Y, DomingoRoura X, Takenaka O (1997) Sexual selection in Japanese macaques.2. female mate choice and male-male competition. Animal Behaviour 54:737-746.

Stumpf RM, Boesch C (2005) Does promiscuous mating preclude female choice? Female sexual strategies in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of the Tai National Park, Cote d'Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57:511-524.

Takahata Y, Huffman MA, Suzuki S, Koyama N, Yamagiwa J (1999) Why dominants do not consistently attain high mating and reproductive success: A review of longitudinal Japanese macaque studies. Primates 40:143-158.

Thomas SA (2002) Scent marking and mate choice in the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster. Animal Behaviour 63:1121-1127.

Thornhill R (1976) Sexual Selection and Nuptial Feeding-Behavior in Bittacus- Apicalis (Insecta-Mecoptera). American Naturalist 110:529-548.

Todt D, Hammerschmidt K, Ansorge V, Fischer J (1995) The vocal behaviour of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus): Call features and their performance in infants and adults. In: Current Topics in Primate Vocal Communication (Eds Zimmermann E, Newman JD, Jurgens U, pp 141-160. New York: Plenum Press.

Tomihara K (2005) Selective approach to a male and subsequent receptivity to mounting comprise mate-choice behavior of female mice. Japanese Psychological Research 47:22-30.

Trivers RL (1972) Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Sexual selection and the descent of man (Ed Campbell B), pp 136-179. Chicago: Aldine Press.

Tyack P (1981) Interactions between Singing Hawaiian Humpback Whales and Conspecifics Nearby. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 8:105-116.

Tyack P (1983) Differential response of humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae, to playback of song or social sounds. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 13:49-55.

Valsecchi P, Razzoli M, Choleris E (2002) Influence of kinship and familiarity on the social and reproductive behaviour of female Mongolian gerbils. Ethology Ecology & Evolution 14:239-253.

van Doorn GS, Weissing FJ (2004) The evolution of female preferences for multiple indicators of quality. American Naturalist 164:173-186.

Benjamin D. Charlton 56

Veen T, Borge T, Griffith SC, Saetre GP, Bures S, Gustafsson L, Sheldon BC (2001) Hybridization and adaptive mate choice in flycatchers. Nature 411:45-50.

Vorobieva E, Hodges K, Heistermann M (2004) The relative importance of male dominance rank and female mate choice on male mating and reproductive success in male chimpanzees. Hormones and Behavior 46:124-124.

Wagner WE (1998) Measuring female mating preferences. Animal Behaviour 55:1029-1042. Waitt C, Little AC, Wolfensohn S, Honess P, Brown AP, Buchanan-Smith HM, Perrett DI

(2003) Evidence from rhesus macaques suggests that male coloration plays a role in female primate mate choice. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 270:S144-S146.

Welch AM (2003) Genetic benefits of a female mating preference in gray tree frogs are context-dependent. Evolution 57:883-893.

Welker C, Hohmann H, Schaferwitt C (1990) Significance of Kin Relations and Individual Preferences in the Social-Behavior of Cebus-Apella. Folia Primatologica 54:166-170.

West PM, Packer C (2002) Sexual selection, temperature, and the lion's mane. Science 297:1339-1343.

Wiley RH, Poston J (1996) Perspective: Indirect mate choice, competition for mates, and coevolution of the sexes. Evolution 50:1371-1381.

Willis C, Poulin R (2000) Preference of female rats for the odours of non-parasitised males: the smell of good genes? Folia Parasitologica 47:6-10.

Wolff JO (1998) Breeding strategies, mate choice, and reproductive success in American bison. Oikos 83:529-544.

Wolff JO, Macdonald DW (2004) Promiscuous females protect their offspring. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 19:127-134.

Yamazaki K, Beauchamp GK, Curran M, Bard J, Boyse EA (2000) Parent-progeny recognition as a function of MHC odortype identity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 97:10500-10502.

Zahavi A (1975) Mate Selection - Selection for a Handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53:205-214.

Zeh JA, Zeh DW (1996) The evolution of polyandry I: Intragenomic conflict and genetic incompatibility. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 263:1711-1717.

Zeh JA, Zeh DW (1997) The evolution of polyandry II: Post-copulatory defenses against genetic incompatibility. Proceedings Of The Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 264:69-75.