Do PSAs take a bite out of Shark Week? The effects of juxtaposing environmental messages with...

27
http://scx.sagepub.com/ Science Communication http://scx.sagepub.com/content/36/5/544 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/1075547014547159 2014 36: 544 Science Communication Jessica Gall Myrick and Suzannah D. Evans Environmental Messages With Violent Images of Shark Attacks ? The Effects of Juxtaposing Shark Week Do PSAs Take a Bite Out of Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com can be found at: Science Communication Additional services and information for http://scx.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://scx.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://scx.sagepub.com/content/36/5/544.refs.html Citations: What is This? - Sep 30, 2014 Version of Record >> at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014 scx.sagepub.com Downloaded from at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014 scx.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Transcript of Do PSAs take a bite out of Shark Week? The effects of juxtaposing environmental messages with...

http://scx.sagepub.com/Science Communication

http://scx.sagepub.com/content/36/5/544The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/1075547014547159

2014 36: 544Science CommunicationJessica Gall Myrick and Suzannah D. Evans

Environmental Messages With Violent Images of Shark Attacks? The Effects of JuxtaposingShark WeekDo PSAs Take a Bite Out of

  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

can be found at:Science CommunicationAdditional services and information for    

  http://scx.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://scx.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://scx.sagepub.com/content/36/5/544.refs.htmlCitations:  

What is This? 

- Sep 30, 2014Version of Record >>

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Science Communication2014, Vol. 36(5) 544 –569

© 2014 SAGE PublicationsReprints and permissions:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1075547014547159

scx.sagepub.com

Article

Do PSAs Take a Bite Out of Shark Week? The Effects of Juxtaposing Environmental Messages With Violent Images of Shark Attacks

Jessica Gall Myrick1 and Suzannah D. Evans2

AbstractA between-subjects experiment (N = 531) studied the juxtaposition of programming from the Discovery Channel’s Shark Week with shark conservation public service announcements (PSAs). Cultivation and priming theories provided a conceptual framework for testing how shark-on-human violence paired with different types of PSAs (celebrity endorser present or not) influence audiences’ emotional reactions, threat perceptions, willingness to support conservation, and intentions to seek information. Findings reveal that shark-on-human violence and presence of a PSA influence fear reactions and perceived threat of shark attacks. Findings related to conservation support, information seeking, and the role of previous Shark Week viewing and demographics are also discussed.

Keywordsenvironmental communication, mass communication theory, perception of environment, psychology of communication, quantitative analysis, education

1Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA2University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA

Corresponding Author:Jessica Gall Myrick, The Media School, Indiana University, Ernie Pyle Hall 141, 940 E. Seventh Street, Bloomington, IN 47405, USA. Email: [email protected]

547159 SCXXXX10.1177/1075547014547159Science CommunicationMyrick and Evansresearch-article2014

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 545

television science, marine bioscience, public service announcements, emotions

The Discovery Channel’s Shark Week is one of summer’s perennial television favorites. Every August, millions of Americans tune in to watch hours of programming dedicated to the ocean’s greatest predator. Shark Week presents a compelling mix of educational content about sharks with thrilling stories of shark attack victims and survivors. This combination of documentary and entertainment is increasingly popular, especially on cable television (Metz, 2008. In 2013, Shark Week broke its own viewership records since it pre-miered in 1987: 2.12 million people watched primetime Shark Week pro-gramming and nearly 29 million viewers tuned in overall (De Moraes, 2013). Its 2013 kickoff program, a 2-hour primetime show dedicated to the extinct Megalodon shark, inspired more social media chatter than any other show on broadcast or cable that day, including a professional football game and a doc-umentary on teen phenom Justin Bieber (De Moraes, 2013).

Shark Week is “a pop-culture leviathan” that thrills audiences by suggest-ing that sharks are among the last threats to human dominance on earth (Fetters, 2012). Despite their fearsome reputation, however, sharks very rarely kill humans; each year about four people are killed by sharks (University of Florida, 2013). This figure has been called “ridiculously low” by a shark biologist, especially “when you consider the billions and billions of human hours spent in the water every year” (Lovgren, 2005). Meanwhile, up to 70 million sharks are killed each year by the fishing industry as accidental bycatch and for shark fin soup, a ceremonial dish popular in China (Mahr, 2010). As a result, many shark species face extinction (Denyer, 2013). Still, a content analysis of Shark Week episodes from 2001 to 2012 found that the Discovery Channel relied heavily on presenting audiences with images of sharks as violent killers, even after the channel partnered with ocean conser-vation groups in 2010 (Evans, 2014).

Shark Week presents a conundrum for environmentalists who aim to pro-tect shark species from extinction. Shark Week provides access to audiences who are interested in sharks, yet the image of sharks presented by the Discovery Channel emphasizes their potential violence over their declining numbers. In recent years, the Discovery Channel has run shark conservation public service announcements (PSAs) from organizations like the Pew Charitable Trusts and Oceana that emphasize the opposite message. For example, one PSA states: “We shouldn’t be scared of sharks. We should be scared for sharks” (Oceana, 2010). These PSAs do not ask for donations or specific behavioral change (although the websites they ask viewers to visit

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

546 Science Communication 36(5)

do), but rather appeal for better understanding and awareness of shark con-servation.1 Whether these short PSAs have an effect on the Shark Week audi-ence’s perception of sharks or play a motivating role in engaging viewers with conservation is an empirical question that has remained unanswered.

This study aims to describe the effects of juxtaposing conservation PSAs with Shark Week programming. Specifically, we test if and how different lev-els of shark-on-human violence paired with different types of PSAs, or with no PSA at all, influence audience beliefs, emotions, and willingness to sup-port shark conservation. We do so using an online between-subjects experi-ment with clips from Shark Week and PSAs developed by ocean conservation groups.

Literature Review

Violent Media and Fear Reactions

Fear is the emotion one often feels when facing a threat or danger (Lazarus, 1991), and it is accompanied by bodily reactions such as a pounding heart, raised brows, and wide eyes (Darwin, 2009; Ekman & Rosenberg, 1998; Roseman, Wiest, & Swartz, 1994). Feeling afraid motivates people to be vigi-lant and take so-called fight or flight actions (Lazarus, 1991; Öhman, 2009). Media have long been recognized as having the ability to scare audiences (Cantor, 2009; Janis & Feshbach, 1953; Witte, 1992). In particular, mediated representations of dangers or injuries, distortions of natural forms (e.g., injury-related bodily mutilation), and the others’ experiences of danger and/or fear are especially likely to produce fright reactions in audiences (for a review, see Cantor, 2009). These three types of fear-arousing stimuli are very common in Shark Week programming (Evans, 2014). In its three-decade his-tory, research indicates that Shark Week has left a mark on the public’s per-ceptions of sharks. One survey found that most respondents had heard of Shark Week (58%), more than the number who had actually been snorkeling or diving with sharks (10%; O’Bryhim, 2006). Additionally, most partici-pants greatly overstated their own risk of being attacked by a shark.

This suggests that a marine twist on cultivation theory may be at play: a “mean ocean” effect. Cultivation theory posits that viewers will import atti-tudes about risk of violence from watching television (Gerbner, 1969; Morgan & Shanahan, 2010). Numerous studies have shown a connection between watching crime-related television shows and increased perceptions of risk of being a crime victim (e.g., Goidel, Freeman, & Procopio, 2006; Holbrook & Hill, 2005). In one study, concern about violence in television advertisements was also correlated with overestimating risk about being the victim of assault

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 547

(Hetsroni, 2011). Moreover, heavy television viewing is associated with less willingness to make sacrifices for environmental reasons (Shanahan, Morgan, & Stenbjerre, 1997). Thus, watching the violent images often featured on Shark Week may lead audiences to become overly fearful of being victimized by sharks and/or less willing to make sacrifices in order to protect sharks and their environment.

In addition to longitudinal televised viewing of shark attacks, just one instance of viewing shark-on-human violence might elicit fear. Media prim-ing is “the short-term impact of exposure to the media on subsequent judg-ments or behaviors” (Roskos-Ewoldsen, Roskos-Ewoldsen, & Carpentier, 2009, p. 74). Media priming occurs when exposure to media makes a particu-lar concept (e.g., violence as a solution, failings of particular politicians) more accessible in the mind and can influence how audiences act toward others (e.g., Bushman, 1998) or vote for political candidates, for example (e.g., Moy, Xenos, & Hess, 2006; Pan & Kosicki, 1997). Fear is rather easily primed by visual stimuli thanks to the associative connections in the brain’s neural networks (Esteves, Parra, Dimberg, & Öhman, 1994). Priming of fear has been linked to increased risk perceptions (Lerner & Keltner, 2001) and differences in information seeking about and supported remedies for societal threats (i.e., gun violence, drunk driving; Nabi, 2003). A cultivated and/or primed fear of sharks may influence audiences’ perceptions of sharks and support for shark conservation, leading us to these hypotheses:

Hypothesis 1: Watching more violent video of sharks will be associated with higher levels of fear afterward than will watching video of sharks not attacking humans.Hypothesis 2: Watching more violent video of sharks will be associated with greater perceived threat of shark attacks.Hypothesis 3: Frequent viewing of Shark Week will be associated with greater fear and perceived threat of shark attacks.

Shark Conservation Public Service Announcements

PSAs are a form of advertising meant to promote the messages of nonprofit groups and government institutions instead of commercial products. Since 2010, the Discovery Channel has made a concerted effort to partner with ocean conservation groups during Shark Week and has aired PSAs, featured more scientists in its programming, and shared facts about shark conservation on its website (Eilperin, 2012).2 A content analysis of Shark Week program-ming found that although conservation messaging and statements of low risk of shark attacks did increase since 2010, Shark Week still largely presented

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

548 Science Communication 36(5)

sharks as violent and dangerous predators (Evans, 2014). Additionally, pre-2010 Shark Week footage is still aired regularly and is available on platforms such as Netflix, iTunes, and YouTube. Sharks are a crucial part of a healthy marine ecosystem, and marine scientists are aware of the importance of high-lighting conservation messages about sharks as a critical part of protecting them from extinction (Simpfendorfer, Heupel, White, & Dulvy, 2011). Because shark conservation PSAs have emphasized that sharks very rarely attack humans and, therefore, are not as scary as they are often portrayed in the media, we propose the following hypothesis:

Hypothesis 4: Those who watch a PSA about shark conservation will experience lower levels of fear afterward than will those who do not see a shark conservation PSA.

Additionally, the evocation of fear can sometimes lead to boomerang effects where audiences are angry at the attempt to influence how they should think, feel, or act (Dillard & Shen, 2005). For instance, persuasive messages that attempt to scare audiences into taking action are often met with defensive reactions, such as judgments that the message is overblown or manipulative (Nabi, Roskos-Ewoldsen, & Carpentier, 2008; Shen, 2010). By highlighting the irrationality of being scared of sharks, PSAs about shark conservation may prompt audiences to question the nature of the Shark Week program-ming. As such, we propose another hypothesis:

Hypothesis 5: Participants will report stronger defensive reactions toward Shark Week when a PSA is shown than when one is not.

Types of Public Service Announcements

PSAs use different strategies in their attempts to gain the public’s attention. Using celebrity spokespeople has long been a favored marketing technique for message creators (Erdogan, 1999). Celebrity endorsements can result in increased sales for products by adding value through “meaning transfer,” where the endorsed product or message gains the positive aura of the well-liked celeb-rity (Amos, Holmes, & Strutton, 2008). For example, Steve Irwin was consid-ered an environmental celebrity whose message helped popularize conservation efforts despite his lack of knowledge of biodiversity (Northfield & McMahon, 2010). A review of studies on the use of celebrities in PSAs found that this technique can be helpful in changing attitudes or behaviors such as curbing drunk driving, learning about HIV/AIDS prevention, quitting smoking, and fostering antidrug attitudes among adolescents (Shead et al., 2011).

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 549

While a review of the literature does not reveal many studies about the effects of celebrity PSAs in the environmental realm specifically, the success of celebrity PSAs in other social change–oriented arenas suggests that a simi-lar effect could occur in this context. However, the effectiveness of a celeb-rity endorser may be mitigated by pairing pro-environmental PSAs with compelling content that (indirectly, through its arousal of fear in audiences) contradicts the message that sharks are not dangerous and are, in fact, threat-ened with extinction.

Research Question 1: Will a shark conservation PSA with a celebrity spokesperson be more effective (i.e., engender greater intentions to sup-port shark conservation) than a shark conservation PSA without a celebrity?

Research in marketing and consumer psychology indicates that the con-text within which an advertisement is shown will influence its effectiveness. For instance, Griskevicius et al. (2009) found that advertisements relying on a social proof (e.g., “most popular”) persuasion heuristic were more effective when placed within the context of fear-inducing media (a crime drama), while advertisements using a scarcity (e.g., “limited edition”) approach were more persuasive when displayed within a romantic media context (a romantic comedy). These findings conformed to predictions from evolutionary psy-chology that when scared, people like to stick together, but when fighting for mates they tend to try to stand out from the crowd. Other research demon-strates that arousal can transfer from one mediated context to the next (Cantor, Bryant, & Zillmann, 1974; Zillmann, 1971) as can attention (Anderson, Alwitt, Lorch, & Levin, 1979) and emotional reactions (Cavanaugh, Bettman, Luce, & Payne, 2007; Holbert & Hansen, 2008; Lerner & Keltner, 2000). This body of research leads us to the following research question:

Research Question 2: How will level of violence and the type of PSA influence intentions to support shark conservation?

Moreover, viewer demographics may also predict differences in responses to Shark Week and shark conservation PSAs. Age, gender, and education level have each been linked to support for environmental conservation and related behaviors (e.g., Kim, Jeong, & Hwang, 2013; Stern, Dietz, & Kalof, 1993; Van Liere & Dunlap, 1980). The existing literature linking demo-graphic factors and environmental conservation support lead to the following inquiry regarding support for shark conservation after viewing different por-trayals of sharks and different PSAs:

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

550 Science Communication 36(5)

Research Question 3: Will the effects of violence and type of PSA on intentions to support shark conservation differ based on the demographics of the participants (gender, age, education level)?

A further step in assessing the processes behind the effects of shark-on-human violence in Shark Week and shark conservation PSAs is to test if emotional and cognitive reactions to shark-related content are also predictors of intentions to sup-port shark conservation as well as to seek more information about sharks. Cognition and emotion interact to help shape audience reactions to media mes-sages (Nabi, 1999; Witte, 1992). Both perceived risk (e.g., O’Connor, Bord, & Fisher, 1999) and emotional connections (e.g., Carrus, Passafaro, & Bonnes, 2008; Kollmuss & Agyeman, 2002) have been found to motivate individuals to take action related to the environment. Emotions can also motivate behavior above and beyond what cognitive beliefs alone would predict (Cappella, Romantan, & Lerman, 2002). This is likely because emotions serve a motivating function that helps individuals turn thoughts into actions (Zeelenberg & Pieters, 2006).

Furthermore, the risk information seeking and processing model (RISP; Griffin, Dunwoody, & Neuwirth, 1999) points to the joint importance of cogni-tive factors, like risk perception, and emotions, such as fear, in motivating information seeking. The risk information seeking and processing model (Griffin et al., 1999) has successfully predicted when people seek information about climate change (Kahlor, 2007; Yang & Kahlor, 2013) and other environ-mental risks (Griffin et al., 2008). While stronger risk perceptions are related to a greater likelihood of information seeking, the impact of emotions on motivat-ing information seeking depends on the nature of the emotion felt. For instance, fear can increase information seeking or avoidance, depending on the individ-ual and the situation (Brashers, 2001; Miller, 1987), while anger has been linked to increased seeking (Griffin et al., 2008). This leads to our last inquiry:

Research Question 4: How will emotional reactions (i.e., fear, anger, compassion, and interest) and cognitive reactions (i.e., personal risk per-ceptions and defensive reactions) to viewing Shark Week video clips and PSAs influence participants’ intentions to seek more information about sharks and to support shark conservation efforts?

Method

Sample

Participants were recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk platform (MTurk), an increasingly common and valid source of participants for social scientific research (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). The sample consisted of

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 551

531 U.S. participants ranging in age from 18 to 72 years (M = 36.04, SD = 13.09). The sample included slightly more males (n = 275; 51.8%) than females. More than three quarters were White (77.4%), and 9.8% of the par-ticipants were African American, 9.4% Asian or Pacific Islander, and another 6.8% Hispanic/Latino(a). Most of the participants had at least a high-school education/GED (98.3%), and almost half (44.8%) reported having obtained a 4-year college degree or higher.

Design and Procedure

A questionnaire hosted by Qualtrics housed a between-subjects online exper-iment. After giving consent to participate, participants provided demographic data and then viewed one of the videos assigned randomly by Qualtrics. After viewing the video containing both a Shark Week clip and a clip that the par-ticipants were told was a commercial that ran during Shark Week, participants responded to the remaining dependent measures. The monetary incentive to participate was $0.50 USD. All procedures were approved by an institutional review board.

Stimulus Materials

Video clips from Shark Week as well as two PSAs about shark conservation and a non–shark-related commercial (which served as an attention control) were used as the stimulus materials. Each video included two segments: an approximately 3-minute clip from Shark Week followed by one of three shorter videos: a PSA promoting shark conservation featuring a celebrity, a PSA promoting shark conservation without a celebrity, or an attention control video (see Table 1).

The 15 video versions varied by the level of shark-on-human violence (none, medium level of violence, high level of violence) and presence/type of PSA (PSA present and celebrity spokesperson, PSA present and no celebrity, PSA absent). Additionally, for the medium- and high-violence videos, both live-to-tape and recreated portrayals of shark-on-human violence were included in the design; both types were actual clips from Shark Week. This design allowed for improved ecological validity in the study by using a broader range of actual Shark Week programming. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) procedures revealed no significant differences on dependent mea-sures based on the live-to-tape or recreated nature of the violence conditions (Fs < 1). Therefore, this variable was not used or controlled for in further analyses.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

552 Science Communication 36(5)

Table 1. Description of Stimulus Materials.

Experimental condition Title of clip

Year/episode Description

Length, minutes

High violence, live-to-tape

Anatomy of a Shark Bite

2003/Episode 1

A man watches video from when he was bitten by a shark and the anatomy of his injury is shown. The bloody leg injury is shown in its entirety.

3:21

High violence, reenacted

Top 5 Eaten Alive

2007/Episode 3

A great white shark repeatedly bites a woman swimming off the Australian coast as the water fills with blood and she screams.

3:38

Medium violence, live-to-tape

Surviving Sharks

2008/Episode 2

Two men test if sharks are more aggressive at night or day. After dumping bait into the ocean, a shark bites a chainmail-clad trained diver. No blood or wound is visible.

3:32

Medium violence, recreated

Shark Attack Survivors

2006/Episode 2

A young woman recounts her tail of being attacked by a great white shark while surfing in Bodega Bay, off the coast of California. No blood or wound is visible.

3:10

No violence, live-to-tape

Shark City 2011/Episode 7

Sharks are shown in their natural habitat, as a voice-over explains shark social hierarchies and hunting habits.

4:20

Celebrity PSA Scared for Sharks

2009 Starring actress January Jones, the nonprofit Oceana advocates for shark conservation in this PSA about the ways humans are hurting the shark population while swimming with sharks.

0:36

Noncelebrity PSA Sharks. It’s a Numbers Game

2012 Produced for the Pew Global Shark Conservation initiative, this PSA explains how many sharks are being killed by humans and that sharks need protection.

1:00

Attention control Jury Duty 2013 This is a commercial for Wendy’s pretzel bacon cheeseburger where friends discuss their love for the burger.

0:30

Note. PSA = public service announcement.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 553

Measures

Emotional Reactions. After viewing both video segments, participants were asked to rate their emotional reactions. They were given the following prompt: “After watching the video segments, how do you feel? This video segment made me feel . . .,” followed by a randomized listing of 13 emotional reaction adjectives pre-sented on 7-point scales ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much): fear (fearful, afraid, scared, anxious; α = .93, M = 3.08, SD = 1.83), anger (irritated, annoyed, aggravated, angry; α = .90, M = 2.11, SD = 1.42), compassion (compassionate, sympathetic; r = .68, p < .001, M = 3.84, SD = 1.78), and curiosity (inquisitive, curious, interested; α = .79, M = 4.61, SD = 1.47).

Perceived Personal Threat of Shark Attack. Perceived threat measures were adapted from Witte, Cameron, McKeon, and Berkowitz (1996). The concept of perceived threat is a combination of perceived severity of the threat (three items, e.g., “I believe that shark attacks are severe”) and perceived suscepti-bility of the threat (three items, e.g., “I am at risk for a shark attack if I swim in the ocean”), all measured on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). All six items were summed and divided by six to form an index of perceived threat (α = .78, M = 4.95, SD = 1.09). In addition to dem-onstrating acceptable reliability as a measure of perceived threat, a confirma-tory principal component analysis revealed that all six items loaded adequately onto one factor (all component loadings were .46 or higher).

Defensive Reactions Toward Shark Week. Participants were asked to rate their opinions of how well six adjectives (overdone, distorted, overblown, exag-gerated, exploitative, and manipulative) described the Shark Week segment, specifically, on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The six adjectives were adopted from Nabi et al. (2008), and displayed a high level of internal reliability (α = .92, M = 2.86, SD = 1.51).

Intentions to Seek More Information About Sharks. Three items adapted from Yang (2012) assessed intentions to seek more information from sharks. The items were “When the topic of sharks comes up, I’m likely to tune it out” (reversed); “I am likely to search for information about sharks”; and “I am likely to look for information about sharks to learn more about them” (α = .74, M = 4.12, SD = 1.51). Participants responded to all three items on a scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

Intentions to Support Shark Conservation. Four items adapted from Oliver, Dil-lard, Bae, and Tamul (2012) were used to measure intentions to support shark

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

554 Science Communication 36(5)

conservation. Participants were asked their willingness, from 1 (not at all willing) to 7 (extremely willing), to “donate money to an organization lobby-ing for policies to help protect sharks,” “sign a petition asking your represen-tative to vote for shark conservation legislation,” “discuss the issue of shark conservation with family and/or friends,” and “forward a link about shark conservation to others” (α = .90, M = 3.44, SD = 1.75).

Previous Viewing of Shark Week. Two measures were used to assess previous viewing. The first asked how many years (from 1987-2013) the participants recalled viewing the program (M = 3.15, SD = 4.71). The second asked par-ticipants to recall approximately how many hours they spent watching the most reason season of Shark Week. Responses ranged from zero to 100 hours (M = 1.50, SD = 5.47). However, the 100 hours response was an extreme outlier (next highest response was 40 hours), and therefore removed from analysis in order to improve the normality of the data for accurate analyses (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Additionally, this variable had a substantial positive skew. To correct for this, we used a transformation suggested by Tabachnick and Fidell and took the base 10 log of the variable plus a constant (1). This transformation resulted in a variable that met the assumptions of normality (M = 0.21, SD = 0.32, skew = 1.68, kurtosis = 3.03). Recent and lifetime viewing were significantly correlated (Pearson’s r = .35, two-tailed, p < .001).

Results

Hypotheses 1, 2, 4, and 5 and Research Questions 1 and 2 were tested using ANOVA procedures typical for testing if experimental groupings are associ-ated with significantly different levels of a continuous outcome variable. Hypothesis 3 and Research Questions 3 and 4 were analyzed via hierarchical regression. First, though, a two-item manipulation check asked participants to rate their agreement with two statements on a scale from 1 (strongly dis-agree) to 7 (strongly agree): “The events in the Shark Week clip seem vio-lent” and “The events in the Shark Week clip seem passive” (reverse coded). These two items were combined and averaged to form an index of perceived violence. A one-way ANOVA with the index as the outcome and the violence condition as the factor was significant, F(2, 528) = 54.41, p < .001, ηp

2 = .17. A Tukey honestly significant difference (HSD) post hoc test revealed signifi-cant (p < .001) differences in violence perceptions between the high-violence videos (M = 5.67, SD = 1.10), medium-violence videos (M = 4.85, SD = 1.30), and the no-violence video (M = 4.22, SD = 1.29). Therefore, the exper-imental manipulations did result in different levels of perceived violence.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 555

Hypothesis 1 predicted that watching more violent shark videos would induce greater levels of fear in participants. A significant one-way ANOVA with fear as the outcome and the violence condition as the fixed factor con-firmed this hypothesis, F(2, 528) = 28.34, p < .001, ηp

2 = .10. A Tukey HSD post hoc test revealed significant (p < .001) differences in fear reactions between the high-violence condition (M = 3.61, SD = 1.78), medium-vio-lence condition (M = 3.05, SD = 1.83), and the no-violence condition (M = 2.04, SD = 1.43). Hypothesis 2 predicted that watching more violent shark videos would increase perceptions of personal threat of a shark attack. Another one-way ANOVA with violence condition as the fixed factor, this time with perceived threat as the outcome, was also significant, F(2, 528) = 8.50, p < .001, ηp

2 = .03. A Tukey HSD post hoc test revealed that while there was no difference in threat perceptions between the high- (M = 5.06, SD = 1.02) and medium-violence (M = 5.04, SD = 1.07) conditions, watching any sort of shark-on-human violence in the study led to higher threat perceptions than watching a clip with no shark-on-human violence (M = 4.56, SD = 1.20). Therefore, Hypothesis 2 was partially supported.

Hypothesis 3 posited that frequent viewing of Shark Week would result in greater fear and perceived threat of shark attacks. Two hierarchical regres-sions were run to test this hypothesis. The first, with fear as the dependent variable, placed the experimental manipulations and gender in the first step. The second step included hours spent watching the most recent season of Shark Week and years spent watching Shark Week. The second hierarchical regression utilized the same steps and independent variables to predict per-ceived threat of shark attack (see Table 2). The most noteworthy finding was that after controlling for the variables in Step 1, the recent viewing of Shark Week was a significant predictor of fear reactions (β = .12, p < .05), while years spent viewing the program was not (β = −.04, p = .41). When predicting perceived threat of a shark attack, recent viewing (β = .10, p = .06) approached significance, while longer-term consumption (β = −.07, p = .19) was not a significant predictor. Given the impact of recent viewing of Shark Week on feelings of fear and perceptions of threat, Hypothesis 3 was partially supported.

Hypothesis 4 predicted that watching a PSA about shark conservation would help assuage the fear that could result from watching a violent Shark Week clip. A two-way ANOVA with fear as the outcome and PSA condition and violence condition as the fixed factors was used to test this hypothesis. The violence condition was included as a factor in case the PSAs had differ-ent effects on fear reactions when shown after different types of Shark Week clips. The omnibus ANOVA was significant, F(4, 522) = 7.25, p < .001, ηp

2 =

.10; however, the main effect for PSA condition was not significant, F < 1.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

556 Science Communication 36(5)

Additionally, the interaction between PSA and violence conditions was not significant, F < 1. Therefore, Hypothesis 4 was not supported.

Hypothesis 5 predicted that seeing a PSA about shark conservation would prompt participants to perceive Shark Week content as overdone or exploit-ative. A two-way ANOVA with defensive reactions as the outcome and the PSA condition and violence condition as the fixed factors was not significant, F(8, 522) = 1.24, p = .27, ηp

2 = .02. The interaction between the PSA condi-tion and the violence was also not significant, F < 1. Therefore, Hypothesis 5 was not supported.

Research Questions 1 and 2 asked how level of violence and type of PSA would influence intentions to support shark conservation. A two-way ANOVA was significant, F(8, 522) = 3.02, p < .01, ηp

2 = .04. There was a significant main effect for PSA condition, F(2, 522) = 7.93, p < .001, ηp

2 = .03. A Tukey HSD post hoc test showed that both the celebrity (M = 3.53, SD = 1.77) and noncelebrity (M = 3.74, SD = 1.79) conditions were significantly more effec-tive at promoting intentions to support shark conservation than the condition without a PSA (M = 3.08, SD = 1.64) at p = .05. However, there was no

Table 2. Hierarchical Regressions Predicting Fear and Perceived Threat.

Predictor variables (β)

Models

Fear Threat

1 2 1 2

Step 1 Age −.12** −.10* .07† .09* Education .09* .08* −.11* −.12** Gender .09* .10* .12** .12** Medium-violence condition .28*** .27*** .20*** .20** High-violence condition .43*** .43*** .22*** .21*** Noncelebrity PSA .05 .04 −.04 −.04 Celebrity PSA .06 .06 −.10* −.10*Step 2 2013 Shark Week viewing .12* .10†

Lifetime Shark Week viewing −.04 −.07 R2 .13 .14 .07 .08 ΔR2 .01 .01 F 10.76** 9.10*** 5.48*** 4.67***

Note. PSA = public service announcement. n = 530.†p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 557

difference between the celebrity and noncelebrity conditions. The interaction between violence condition and PSA condition on intentions to support shark conservation only approached significance, F(4, 522) = 2.26, p = .06, ηp

2 = .02. Therefore, the level of shark-on-human violence viewed in the preceding Shark Week clip did not alter the impact of viewing a PSA on participants’ intentions to support shark conservation.

To test Research Question 3, which asked if sample demographics influ-enced intentions to support shark conservation, a hierarchical regression analysis was performed (see Table 3). This regression included demographic variables (gender, age, and education level) in the first block, followed by the experimental conditions (level of violence and type of PSA) in the second block. The results revealed that gender was a significant predictor of inten-tions to support shark conservation in each block of the analysis, with women (M = 3.59, SD = 1.82) more likely than men (M = 3.30, SD = 1.68) to support shark conservation. Age was also a negative predictor of intentions to support shark conservation in the first two blocks. Education did not predict inten-tions to support shark conservation.

To test Research Question 4, which asked how emotional and cognitive reactions to Shark Week would influence intentions to support shark conser-vation as well as intentions to seek more information about sharks, another hierarchical regression analysis was performed (see Table 4). The cognitive variables (perceived risk and defensive reactions) did not predict intentions to support shark conservation. However, adding the emotional reactions to the video content explained an additional portion of the variance (R2 change = .19). Feelings of fear predicted less support for conservation while anger, compassion, and interest all predicted greater support. For information seek-ing, defensive reactions to Shark Week were associated with lower intentions to seek information, while feelings of interest were a strong positive predictor of seeking intentions.

Discussion

The present data revealed numerous consequences of viewing Shark Week. This study demonstrated that participants were more likely to support shark conservation after viewing a PSA, but they were still likely to over-state their own risk of being the victim of a shark attack. We found that participants overstated their own risk and reported more fear whether they were watching Shark Week clips with high or medium levels of violence and that they did not distinguish between live-to-tape and reenacted vio-lence. This suggests that televised shark-on-human violence, whether gory, cartoonish, or realistic, can possibly result in fear and/or overstated

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

558 Science Communication 36(5)

beliefs about personal risk. Research on fear appeals suggests that adding an efficacy component to messages about threats (e.g., “You can take pre-cautions to avoid shark attacks”) could motivate audiences to take adap-tive actions (Witte, 1992, 1998). Future work could test how nature-related programming and/or PSAs that include efficacy information alter the influence of fear on perceptions.

Table 3. Hierarchical Regression Predicting Intentions to Support Shark Conservation.

Predictor variables (β)

Models

1 2 3

Step 1 Age −.16** −.17*** −.16 Education .02 .03 .04 Gender .10* .11** .31**Step 2 Medium-violence condition −.06 −.12 High-violence condition −.07 −.14 Noncelebrity PSA .20*** .33 Celebrity PSA .13** .40*Step 3 Age × medium-violence condition −.05 Age × high-violence condition .00 Age × noncelebrity PSA −.01 Age × celebrity PSA .02 Education × medium-violence condition .17 Education × high-violence condition .17 Education × noncelebrity PSA −.09 Education × celebrity PSA −.27 Gender × medium-violence condition −.11 Gender × high-violence condition −.17†

Gender × noncelebrity PSA −.07 Gender × celebrity PSA −.04 R2 .03 .06 .08 ΔR2 .03 .02 F 5.77** 5.10*** 2.31**

Note. PSA = public service announcement. n = 531.†p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 559

The number of years viewers had watched Shark Week was not a good predictor of fear or perceived threat of shark attacks. Rather, watching the most recent season of Shark Week was a better predictor of feelings of fear and perceptions of shark threat. This is despite the fact that post-2010 Shark Week programs were more likely to include statements of low risk of shark attack than earlier seasons (Evans, 2014). This finding indicates that while the Discovery Channel may be adding more scientific statements such as the actual low risk of shark attack to Shark Week programming, it still presents

Table 4. Hierarchical Regression Predicting Intentions to Support Shark Conservation and Intentions to Seek Shark-Related Information.

Predictor variables (β)

Models predicting conservation support

Models predicting information seeking

1 2 3 4 1 2 3 4

Step 1 Age −.16*** −.17*** −.17*** −.16** −.12** −.13** −.13** −.11** Education .02 .03 .03 −.01 .07 .07 .10* .05 Gender .10* .11** .12** .11** −.04 −.04 −.07 −.07†

Step 2 Medium-

violence condition

−.06 −.06 −.06 −.01 −.04 −.06

High-violence condition

−.07 −.06 −.06 −.06 −.09 −.07

Noncelebrity PSA

.20*** .19*** .13** .07 .07 .05

Celebrity PSA .13** .13** .10* .07 .08† .06Step 3 Perceived

personal threat

−.02 −.06 .14** .09*

Defensive reactions

−.01 .00 −.19*** −.16***

Step 4 Fear −.12* .01 Anger .16*** .04 Compassion .21*** .09†

Interest .30*** .39*** R2 .03 .06 .06 .25 .02 .03 .09 .27 ΔR2 .03 .00 .19 .01 .06 .18 F 5.75** 5.12*** 3.98*** 13.27*** 3.41* 2.02† 5.39*** 14.44***

Note. PSA = public service announcement. n = 530.†p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

560 Science Communication 36(5)

sharks as frightening and potentially violent animals in ways that resonate with audiences. Since recent viewing of Shark Week was a better predictor of fear and threat reactions than the number of years watching the program-ming, it is possible that a priming framework may be a better explanation than cultivation theory as to why viewers are fearful after watching Shark Week. More work is needed to explore the interplay between priming and cultivation with regard to pairing educational PSAs that attempt to “correct” the messaging of popular television content by presenting a contradictory message.

While viewing shark conservation PSAs did increase intentions to support shark conservation, it did not alleviate the fear participants felt after viewing the segments. Additionally, viewing the PSA did not incite defensive reac-tions toward Shark Week. Given the vividness of the images and sounds in its production, it is possible that the arousal from watching the Shark Week seg-ment carried over even after the PSA due to excitation transfer (Zillmann, 1971), possibly overriding the PSA’s message that sharks are not to be feared. Our study does show, however, that even short pro-shark advocacy messages can have desirable effects when paired with a potentially conflicting pro-gramming that vilifies sharks, such as increased intent to support shark con-servation by signing a petition, donating money, or merely talking with a friend about saving sharks. Nonprofit groups depend on grassroots support to help enact policy or social change; moreover, they depend on tangible public engagement in order to be known as leaders in their causes. From our results, it seems partnering with Shark Week, with its large viewership, may be a worthwhile investment for advocacy groups working to protect sharks from extinction.

One interesting finding was that there was no significant difference in the effects of the celebrity and noncelebrity PSAs on intentions to support shark conservation. Advocacy groups considering using a celebrity for a media campaign may find an equally effective public response without the time and funds required for a celebrity spokesperson. A limitation of this study, how-ever, was that it used just one celebrity PSA. It is therefore impossible to generalize a conclusion about the effects of any celebrity on attitudes about sharks. Future research could use multiple celebrities and statistically control for liking of the celebrity to address this concern.

Another intriguing finding was that women and younger participants were more likely to support shark conservation than were men and older partici-pants. These effects did not interact with experimental conditions, meaning that they occurred despite the different amounts of violence and types of commercials presented. Women tend to have higher levels of trait empathy (Lawrence, Shaw, Baker, Baron-Cohen, & David, 2004), and perhaps felt

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 561

more for the sharks than did their male counterparts. Younger people and the more educated are also more likely to be politically liberal (Pew Center for the People & the Press, 2012) and, therefore, may be more likely to support liberal causes like environmental protection. These findings beg further investigation, but they may be helpful for advocacy groups who want to tar-get their messages to friendly audiences.

With regard to audience intentions to support shark conservation and seek information about sharks, factoring in emotional reactions to the shark-related media allowed the models to predict a significantly larger portion of variance than did the models with only demographics, experimental manipu-lations, and cognitive reactions. These results point to the importance of mea-suring discrete emotional reactions to media consumption as predictors of support and information seeking. Grouping emotions into general negative and positive valence categories, as is often done in media effects work, may inadvertently mask the motivational nuances different emotions bring to these contexts (Nabi, 2010). For instance, while fear and anger are both nega-tive emotions, fear was associated with less support while anger was associ-ated with more support for conservation. Additional work could assess the specific referents for these emotional reactions—did viewers feel compas-sion toward sharks or toward victims of shark attacks, and did they feel anger toward shark attack victims who did not take precautions or toward those who are killing sharks?—to provide more insight as to the role of discrete emotions in motivating conservation support and information seeking.

Previous work on information seeking in environmental contexts has also found audience channel beliefs, informational subjective norms, capacity/efficacy, and attribution of responsibility to play important roles in predicting information seeking (e.g., Dietz, Fitzgerald, & Shwom, 2005; Stern et al., 1993). Moreover, the effects sizes on many of our analyses were small, there-fore implying that additional factors can help explain the remaining variance in outcomes. Future work could measure these variables in conjunction with viewership of and cognitive and affective reactions to watching popular con-tent with a contradictory PSA message.

Like any social scientific endeavor, the present study comes with limita-tions. First, it is important to acknowledge that there are many ways to ana-lyze audience responses to media, and the present study utilized only a limited range of theoretical (i.e., cultivation and priming frameworks) and method-ological (i.e., experimental) approaches. Future work could improve on the present study by using qualitative approaches to get a richer analysis of how audiences make sense of popular culture content accompanied by PSAs related to conservation. Additionally, our study captured reactions immedi-ately following viewing. Further ethnographic study or a survey design could

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

562 Science Communication 36(5)

describe attitude change over time and also clarify the attention-getting role of celebrities in PSAs, which may not have been captured in our experimental design showing no difference between the celebrity and noncelebrity PSA. Moreover, our self-report measures of recent and past viewing were signifi-cantly correlated, and future research could rely on more precise measure-ments of media exposure made possible by digital video recorder and cookie technology.

The sample taken from MTurk, while more diverse than a sample com-prising undergraduate university students, may not be representative of Shark Week viewers. Thus, these results may not directly translate into the actual effects of Shark Week on the general public. Also, the stimuli used here were short clips of a multihour, multiyear popular culture phenomenon. While the manipulation check was successful in differentiating the clips based on level of violence, use of more varied and longer Shark Week footage would provide a richer picture of the effects of viewing the program. The emotion measures after the video segments captured general discrete emotional states, but we do not know toward whom the compassion or anger is felt. Future research could use more time- and topic-sensitive emotion measures.

Lastly, intent is not behavior. In addition to providing a branding opportu-nity for nonprofit organizations, running PSAs should result in increased donations and membership, political support, or behavior change, depending on the specific call to action in the PSA. The PSAs used in this study were geared toward general awareness and attitude change about shark conserva-tion with no specific “ask” for donations or behavioral change, making our study of their effectiveness in changing attitudes and intentions limited but appropriate. Studies of PSAs asking for behavioral change may especially benefit from behavioroid measures or a longitudinal design.

Conclusion

The Discovery Channel’s stated mission is “to satisfy curiosity and make a difference in people’s lives by providing the highest quality content, services and products that entertain, engage and enlighten” (Discovery Channel, 2014). This form of infotainment, exemplified by the popular annual televi-sion program Shark Week, does not always align with the goals of environ-mental advocates. The Discovery Channel has been criticized by many scientists and environmentalists for presenting an inaccurate and potentially harmful portrait of sharks as killers, when in fact sharks rarely attack humans and some species are at risk of extinction (Eilperin, 2012).

The channel’s partnership with ocean conservation groups starting in 2010 may be seen as an attempt to respond to those critiques. Partnering with a

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 563

nonprofit organization is a tried and true method for corporations to address controversies and perceived shortcomings (BP, 2012; Ocean Conservancy, 2013). Our findings indicate that this strategy may result in a complex recep-tion for the conservation organization’s message. While Shark Week’s por-trayal of shark-on-human violence resonated with viewers and evoked a fear of sharks, participants were also more likely to support shark conservation after viewing a PSA. In other words, running a PSA may allow a conservation organization to reach its goal of changing some attitudes about conservation, but not all of them, even if the PSA explicitly asked viewers to reject the mes-sage of the popular television content (e.g., “Scared of sharks? We should be scared for sharks”; Oceana, 2010).

This study has implications for advocates who wish to change attitudes about environmental issues as well as for scholars of media effects. The por-trayals of sharks under study here are small parts of a controversial and popu-lar media product. Moreover, the complexity of our results reflects the challenges of measuring effects in a media landscape characterized not by single messages received in an orderly fashion. In reality, audiences consume and make sense of countless (often-conflicting) media messages in their day-to-day lives. Audiences’ negotiation of conflicting mediated messages has been the subject of many productive scholarly approaches, such as cognitive dissonance (Festinger, 1957) and uses and gratification theories (Ruggiero, 2000), and it has proved especially fruitful in the health communications realm (e.g., Rady, McGregor, & Verheijde, 2012). However, the experimental paradigm often struggles to recreate the alternately hybridized, balkanized, and intertexualized global media system in its studies. This study attempted to close some of the gap by examining a persuasive appeal—a PSA asking viewers to support shark conservation—not on its own but contextualized within the actual media content it was paired with when it aired on the Discovery Channel. While this is a simplified version of reality, it is a contri-bution toward our understanding of how audiences negotiate the complex messaging they receive from multiple environmental and entertainment media channels.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Scott Myrick for his assistance in editing the stimulus materi-als for this study.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

564 Science Communication 36(5)

Funding

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publi-cation of this article.

Notes

1. One of the authors (Evans) was employed at Oceana during the production and original airing of these PSAs and observed the intentions of the conservation organizations during this time.

2. One of the authors (Evans) was employed at Oceana from 2007 to 2012, prior to work on the present study. From this author’s experience, it is difficult to assign one reason to the Discovery Channel’s decision to form a partnership with conservation organizations. A new director took over Shark Week in 2010, and the conservation organizations themselves were interested in reaching a large audience through Shark Week. Additionally, the Discovery Channel’s suburban Washington, D.C., location made partnership with the area’s many conservation organizations a logistical possibility. A combination of these factors was most likely at play.

References

Amos, C., Holmes, G., & Strutton, D. (2008). Exploring the relationship between celebrity endorser effects and advertising effectiveness. International Journal of Advertising, 27, 209-234.

Anderson, D. R., Alwitt, L. F., Lorch, E. P., & Levin, S. R. (1979). Watching children watch television. In G. Hale & M. Lewis (Eds.), Attention and cognitive develop-ment (pp. 331-361). New York, NY: Plenum.

BP. (2012). National fish and wildlife foundation projects. Retrieved from http://www.bp.com/en/global/corporate/gulf-of-mexico-restoration/restoring-the-envi-ronment.html

Brashers, D. E. (2001). Communication and uncertainty management. Journal of Communication, 51, 477-497. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2001.tb02892.x

Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6, 3-5. doi:10.1177/1745691610393980

Bushman, B. J. (1998). Priming effects of media violence on the accessibility of aggressive constructs in memory. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 537-545. doi:10.1177/0146167298245009

Cantor, J. R. (2009). Fright reactions to mass media. In J. Bryant & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 287-303). New York, NY: Routledge.

Cantor, J. R., Bryant, J., & Zillmann, D. (1974). Enhancement of humor appreciation by transferred excitation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 30, 812-821. doi:10.1037/h0037543

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 565

Cappella, J. N., Romantan, A., & Lerman, C. (2002). Emotional bases for quitting smoking: Extending the integrated theory of behavior change. Philadelphia: Annenberg Public Policy Center, University of Pennsylvania.

Carrus, G., Passafaro, P., & Bonnes, M. (2008). Emotions, habits and rational choices in ecological behaviours: The case of recycling and use of public trans-portation. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28, 51-62. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.09.003

Cavanaugh, L. A., Bettman, J. R., Luce, M. F., & Payne, J. W. (2007). Appraising the appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 17, 169-173. doi:10.1016/S1057-7409(07)70024-4

Darwin, C. (2009). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. New York, NY: Penguin.

De Moraes, L. (2013). Shark Week nets biggest crowd ever in opening, but some fin-atics attack kickoff’s fake docu. Retrieved from http://www.deadline.com/2013/08/shark-week-nets-biggest-crowd-ever-in-opening-but-some-obses-sives-attack-discovery-over-kickoffs-fake-documentary/

Denyer, S. (2013). In China, victory for wildlife conservation as citizens persuaded to give up shark fin soup. Retrieved from http://www.washingtonpost.com/world/in-china-victory-for-wildlife-conservation-as-citizens-persuaded-to-give-up-shark-fin-soup/2013/10/19/e8181326-3646-11e3-89db-8002ba99b894_story.html

Dietz, T., Fitzgerald, A., & Shwom, R. (2005). Environmental values. Annual Review of Environment and Resources, 30, 335-372. doi:10.1146/annurev.energy.30.050504.144444

Dillard, J. P., & Shen, L. (2005). On the nature of reactance and its role in per-suasive health communication. Communication Monographs, 72, 144-168. doi:10.1080/03637750500111815

Discovery Channel. (2014). Overview and mission. Retrieved from http://corporate.discovery.com/our-company/overview/

Eilperin, J. (2012). Shark Week at 25: Discovery seeks to keep fest relevant, builds conservation ties. Retrieved from http://articles.washingtonpost.com/2012-08-09/lifestyle/35490559_1_air-jaws-shark-week-brooke-runnette

Ekman, P., & Rosenberg, E. L. (1998). What the face reveals: Basic and applied stud-ies of spontaneous expression using the Facial Action Coding System (FACS). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: A literature review. Journal of Marketing Management, 15, 291-314. doi:10.1362/026725799784870379

Esteves, F., Parra, C., Dimberg, U., & Öhman, A. (1994). Nonconscious asso-ciative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. Psychophysiology, 31, 375-385. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02446.x

Evans, S. (2014, March). Shark Week in an era of infotainment. Paper presented at the NEXUS Interdisciplinary Conference, Knoxville, TN.

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Evenston, IL: Row.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

566 Science Communication 36(5)

Fetters, A. (2012). The evolution of Shark Week, pop-culture leviathan. Retrieved from http://www.theatlantic.com/entertainment/archive/2012/08/the-evolution-of-shark-week-pop-culture-leviathan/261063/

Gerbner, G. (1969). Toward “cultural indicators”: The analysis of mass mediated public message systems. AV Communication Review, 17, 137-148. doi:10.1007/BF02769102

Goidel, R. K., Freeman, C. M., & Procopio, S. T. (2006). The impact of television viewing on perceptions of juvenile crime. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 50, 119-139. doi:10.1207/s15506878jobem5001_7

Griffin, R. J., Dunwoody, S., & Neuwirth, K. (1999). Proposed model of the relation-ship of risk information seeking and processing to the development of preventive behaviors. Environmental Research, 80, S230-S245. doi:10.1006/enrs.1998.3940

Griffin, R. J., Yang, Z., ter Huurne, E., Boerner, F., Ortiz, S., & Dunwoody, S. (2008). After the flood: Anger, attribution, and the seeking of information. Science Communication, 29, 285-315. doi:10.1177/1075547007312309

Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2009). Fear and loving in Las Vegas: Evolution, emotion, and persuasion. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 384-395. doi:10.1509/jmkr.46.3.384

Hetsroni, A. (2011). Violence in television advertising: Content analysis and audience attitudes. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 19, 97-112. doi:10.1080/15456870.2011.561170

Holbert, R. L., & Hansen, G. J. (2008). Stepping beyond message specificity in the study of emotion as mediator and inter-emotion associations across attitude objects: Fahrenheit 9/11, anger, and debate superiority. Media Psychology, 11, 98-118. doi:10.1080/15213260701832512

Holbrook, R. A., & Hill, T. G. (2005). Agenda-setting and priming in prime time tele-vision: Crime dramas as political cues. Political Communication, 22, 277-295. doi:10.1080/10584600591006519

Janis, I. L., & Feshbach, S. (1953). Effects of fear-arousing communications. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 78-92. doi:10.1037/h0060732

Kahlor, L. A. (2007). An augmented risk information seeking model: The case of global warming. Media Psychology, 10, 414-435. doi:10.1080/15213260701532971

Kim, S., Jeong, S.-H., & Hwang, Y. (2013). Predictors of pro-environmental behav-iors of American and Korean students: The application of the theory of reasoned action and protection motivation theory. Science Communication, 35, 168-188. doi:10.1177/1075547012441692

Kollmuss, A., & Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: Why do people act environmen-tally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environmental Education Research, 8, 239-260. doi:10.1080/13504620220145401

Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., & David, A. S. (2004). Measuring empathy: Reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychological Medicine, 34, 911-920. doi:10.1017/S0033291703001624

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 567

Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14, 473-493. doi:10.1080/026999300402763

Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146-159. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146

Lovgren, S. (2005). Jaws at 30: Film stoked fear, study of great white sharks. Retrieved from http://news.nationalgeographic.com/news/2005/06/0615_050615_jawssharks. html

Mahr, K. (2010, August). Shark-fin soup and the conservation challenge. Time Magazine. Retrieved from http://content.time.com/time/magazine/article/0,9171, 2021071,00.html

Metz, A. M. (2008). A fantasy made real: The evolution of the subjunctive documen-tary on US cable science channels. Television & New Media, 9, 333-348. doi: 10.1177/1527476408315117

Miller, S. M. (1987). Monitoring and blunting: Validation of a questionnaire to assess styles of information seeking under threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 345-353. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.2.345

Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (2010). The state of cultivation. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 54, 337-355. doi:10.1080/08838151003735018

Moy, P., Xenos, M. A., & Hess, V. K. (2006). Priming effects of late-night comedy. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 18, 198-210. doi:10.1093/ijpor/edh092

Nabi, R. L. (1999). A cognitive-functional model for the effects of discrete negative emotions on information processing, attitude change, and recall. Communication Theory, 9, 292-320. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.1999.tb00172.x

Nabi, R. L. (2003). Exploring the framing effects of emotion. Communication Research, 30, 224-247. doi:10.1177/0093650202250881

Nabi, R. L. (2010). The case for emphasizing discrete emotions in com-munication research. Communication Monographs, 77, 153-159. doi:10.1080/03637751003790444

Nabi, R. L., Roskos-Ewoldsen, D., & Carpentier, F. D. (2008). Subjective knowl-edge and fear appeal effectiveness: Implications for message design. Health Communication, 23, 191-201. doi:10.1080/10410230701808327

Northfield, J. K., & McMahon, C. R. (2010). Crikey! Overstating the conserva-tion influence of the Crocodile Hunter. Science Communication, 32, 412-417. doi:10.1177/1075547010379424

O’Bryhim, J. (2006). Public knowledge, attitudes, and behavior towards sharks and shark conservation (Unpublished master’s thesis). George Mason University, Fairfax, VA.

Oceana. (2010). Scared for sharks—Oceana. Retrieved from http://act.oceana.org/sign/scaredforsharks2013/

Ocean Conservancy. (2013). International coastal cleanup sponsoring partners. Retrieved from http://www.oceanconservancy.org/our-work/our-partners/inter-national-coastal-cleanup.html

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

568 Science Communication 36(5)

O’Connor, R. E., Bord, R. J., & Fisher, A. (1999). Risk perceptions, general envi-ronmental beliefs, and willingness to address climate change. Risk Analysis, 19, 461-471. doi:10.1023/A:1007004813446

Öhman, A. (2009). Of snakes and faces: An evolutionary perspective on the psychol-ogy of fear. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 50, 543-552.

Oliver, M. B., Dillard, J. P., Bae, K., & Tamul, D. J. (2012). The effect of narra-tive news format on empathy for stigmatized groups. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 89, 205-224. doi:10.1177/1077699012439020

Pan, Z., & Kosicki, G. M. (1997). Priming and media impact on the evalua-tions of the president’s performance. Communication Research, 24(1), 3-30. doi:10.1177/009365097024001001

Pew Center for the People & the Press. (2012). Partisan polarization surges in Bush, Obama years. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/2012/06/04/partisan-polarization-surges-in-bush-obama-years/

Rady, M. Y., McGregor, J. L., & Verheijde, J. L. (2012). Mass media campaigns and organ donation: Managing conflicting messages and interests. Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy, 15, 229-241. doi:10.1007/s11019-011-9359-3

Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206-221. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.206

Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., Roskos-Ewoldsen, B., & Carpentier, F. R. D. (2009). Media priming: An updated synthesis. In J. Bryant & M. B. Oliver (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 74-93). New York, NY: Routledge.

Ruggiero, T. E. (2000). Uses and gratifications theory in the 21st century. Mass Communication and Society, 3(1), 3-37. doi:10.1207/S15327825MCS0301_02

Shanahan, J., Morgan, M., & Stenbjerre, M. (1997). Green or brown? Television and the cultivation of environmental concern. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 41, 305-323. doi:10.1080/08838159709364410

Shead, N. W., Walsh, K., Taylor, A., Derevensky, J., & Gupta, R. (2011). Youth gam-bling prevention: Can public service announcements featuring celebrity spokes-persons be effective? International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 9, 165-179. doi: 10.1007/s11469-009-9260-y

Shen, L. (2010). Mitigating psychological reactance: The role of message-induced empathy in persuasion. Human Communication Research, 36, 397-422. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2010.01381.x

Simpfendorfer, C. A., Heupel, M. R., White, W. T., & Dulvy, N. K. (2011). The importance of research and public opinion to conservation management of sharks and rays: A synthesis. Marine & Freshwater Research, 62, 518-527. doi:10.1071/MF11086

Stern, P. C., Dietz, T., & Kalof, L. (1993). Value orientations, gender, and environmental concern. Environment & Behavior, 25, 322-348. doi:10.1177/0013916593255002

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Pearson.

University of Florida. (2013). 2012 U.S. shark attacks highest since 2000. Retrieved from http://news.ufl.edu/2013/02/11/shark-attack-2012/

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Myrick and Evans 569

Van Liere, K. D., & Dunlap, R. E. (1980). The social bases of environmental concern: A review of hypotheses, explanations and empirical evidence. Public Opinion Quarterly, 44, 181-197. doi:10.1086/268583

Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel pro-cess model. Communication Monographs, 12, 329-349.

Witte, K. (1998). Fear as motivator, fear as inhibitor: Using the extended parallel pro-cess model to explain fear appeal successes and failures. In P. A. Andersen & L. K. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion: Research, theory, applications, and contexts (pp. 423-450). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Witte, K., Cameron, K. A., McKeon, J. K., & Berkowitz, J. M. (1996). Predicting risk behaviors: Development and validation of a diagnostic scale. Journal of Health Communication, 1, 317-342. doi:10.1080/108107396127988

Yang, Z. J. (2012). Too scared or too capable? Why do college students stay away from the H1N1 vaccine? Risk Analysis, 32, 1703-1716. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01799.x

Yang, Z. J., & Kahlor, L. (2013). What, me worry? The role of affect in infor-mation seeking and avoidance. Science Communication, 35, 189-212. doi:10.1177/1075547012441873

Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2006). Feeling is for doing: A pragmatic approach to the study of emotions in economic behavior. In M. DeCremer, M. Zeelenberg, & J. K. Murnighan (Eds.), Social psychology and economics (pp. 117-137). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Zillmann, D. (1971). Excitation transfer in communication-mediated aggressive behav-ior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 419-434. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(71)90075-8

Author Biographies

Jessica Gall Myrick (PhD, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill) is an assis-tant professor at the Indiana University Media School. Her research examines the role of emotions in media processes and effects, with a focus on how media use can lead to positive outcomes such as improved public health, environmental conservation, and civic engagement with policy issues.

Suzannah D. Evans (MA, Indiana University) is a doctoral student and the Richard Cole Fellow in the School of Journalism and Mass Communication at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Her research explores the role of voices for social and environmental change in journalism, popular culture, and online life.

at INDIANA UNIV on October 1, 2014scx.sagepub.comDownloaded from